You are on page 1of 13

International Journal of Behavioral Development

http://jbd.sagepub.com/

Mothers' expressive style and emotional responses to children's behavior predict children's prosocial
and achievement-related self-ratings
Julie C. Dunsmore, Isabel S. Bradburn, Philip R. Costanzo and Barbara L. Fredrickson
International Journal of Behavioral Development 2009 33: 253 originally published online 25 February 2009
DOI: 10.1177/0165025408098025

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://jbd.sagepub.com/content/33/3/253

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

On behalf of:

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

Additional services and information for International Journal of Behavioral Development can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://jbd.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://jbd.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://jbd.sagepub.com/content/33/3/253.refs.html

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010


253-264 098025 Dunsmore (Q8D):210 x 280mm 03/04/2009 09:52 Page 253

International Journal of Behavioral Development © 2009 The International Society for the
2009, 33 (3), 253–264 Study of Behavioural Development
http://www.sagepublications.com DOI: 10.1177/0165025408098025

Mothers’ expressive style and emotional responses


to children’s behavior predict children’s prosocial and
achievement-related self-ratings

Julie C. Dunsmorea, Isabel S. Bradburna, Philip R. Costanzob, and Barbara L. Fredricksonc

In this study we investigated whether mothers’ typical expressive style and specific emotional
responses to children’s behaviors are linked to children’s prosocial and competence self-ratings. Eight-
to 12-year-old children and their mothers rated how mothers had felt when children behaved pro-
socially and antisocially, achieved and failed to achieve. Children rated self-descriptiveness of proso-
cial and achievement-related traits. Mothers’ positive expressiveness was associated with children’s
higher achievement-related self-ratings. Mothers’ positive- and negative-dominant expressiveness was
associated with children’s lower prosocial self-ratings. Mothers’ happiness about both children’s
prosocial and achievement-related behavior was associated with children’s higher self-ratings for both
domains. Mothers’ anger about children’s antisocial behavior was related to children’s lower self-
ratings for both domains. When mothers were higher in negative-submissive expressiveness, and
responded with more sadness to children’s failure to achieve, children reported lower achievement
self-ratings. Results support the importance of multidimensional assessment of self-concept and
suggest that parents’ typical expressive style moderates the influence of parents’ specific emotional
responses on children’s self-ratings.

Keywords: emotion; family expressiveness; middle childhood; mother–child interaction; self

Sense of self is associated with a host of important cognitive sections we review literature related to three models of how
and socio-emotional processes, including memory and infor- parents’ emotional communication may influence children’s
mation processing (Markus, 1977; Markus, Crane, Bernstein, self-concepts: (a) general approval, (b) selective parental
& Siladi, 1982; Sui & Zhu, 2005; Wang, 2004), and social socialization, and (c) salience.
judgments, status, and behavior (Caldwell, Rudolph, Troop-
Gordon, & Kim, 2004; Catrambone & Markus, 1987; Cornell
et al., 1990; Egan & Perry, 1998; Fong & Markus, 1982; General approval model
Froming, Nasby, & McManus, 1998; Markus, Smith &
Moreland, 1985). Cognitive-affective representations of the Throughout childhood and adolescence, parental acceptance
self appear to be multidimensional (Harter, 1999; Marsh, or approval is strongly linked to children’s positive self-esteem
Craven, & Debus, 1998) and influenced by both social (Harter, 1999), as is how much positive affect a parent typi-
comparison and social feedback (Cooley, 1902; Harter, 1999; cally expresses at home (Halberstadt, Crisp, & Eaton, 1999;
Mead, 1934). Halberstadt & Eaton, 2003). How well children think they are
An important source of social feedback is children’s parents. living up to their parents’ standards also predicts child self-
Theorists have long proposed that parents’ emotion-laden esteem (Harter, 1990; Higgins, 1991; Moretti & Higgins,
communication is central to children’s understanding of their 1990). Parents’ emotional reactions to their child’s behavior
own behavior and developing sense of self and identity (Dix, inform the child about parental standards for behavior and
1991; Higgins, 1989, 1991; Stern, 1985; Sullivan 1940, 1953; how capable parents think the child is of meeting those stan-
see also Dunsmore & Halberstadt, 1997). Empirical research dards (Barrett & Nelson-Goens, 1997; Higgins, 1991). Indeed,
likewise suggests an important role for parental emotional both positive and negative parental emotional reactions to
communication in children’s self-concepts. In the following children’s behavior are related to children’s experience of self-
reflective emotions, such as pride, shame, and guilt (Alessandri
a Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, USA. b Duke University, & Lewis, 1993; Barrett & Nelson-Goens, 1997; Harter, 1996,
USA. c University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill University, USA. 1999; Sroufe, 1996).

Correspondence should be sent to Dr Julie Dunsmore, Mail Code thank Amy G. Halberstadt and Danny Axsom for comments on an
0436, Blacksburg, VA 24060, USA; e-mail: jdunsmor@vt.edu earlier version of this manuscript. We thank Amanda Bahho, Naureen
The first author conducted this study in partial fulfillment of the Bhullar, Janet Davis, Pam Dixon, Anna Guerin, Maggie Hanson,
requirements for the doctoral degree from Duke University. A Grant- Christy Jackson, Rachel Liccardo, Holland Omar, Sasha Paroff,
in-Aid for Research from Sigma Xi funded this study. Portions of this Milagros Ramirez, Megan Robinson, Seema Shah, Eric Teruel, and
manuscript were presented at the Society for Research in Child Colleen Trzaskos for their assistance with this project. We are grateful
Development, March 1995, Indianapolis, IN and at the Society for to the mothers and children who participated.
Research in Child Development, April 2001, Minneapolis, MN. We

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010


253-264 098025 Dunsmore (Q8D):210 x 280mm 03/04/2009 09:52 Page 254

254 DUNSMORE ET AL. / MOTHER’S EXPRESSIVE STYLE AND EMOTIONAL RESPONSES TO CHILDREN’S BEHAVIOR

The intensity and type of parental affective response may Selective parental socialization model
alter how children interpret and internalize their parents’
responses and so are important to study (Halberstadt et al., Parents actively try to shape their children’s behavior and atti-
1999). Although not focusing on self-concept formation, tudes (Bugental & Grusec, 2006; Maccoby & Martin, 1983;
studies of children’s social competence and achievement Parke & Buriel, 2006), but little is known about how parents’
indicate that moderate to high positive parental expressivity and specific emotional communications may relate to different
mild to moderate negative expressiveness may be most domains of children’s self-concepts. By mid- to late childhood,
beneficial to children’s behavior. For example, mild maternal children have well-differentiated self-concepts that incorporate
anger is associated with preschoolers’ greater social competence multiple schemas pertaining to different aspects of their lives
(Denham & Grout, 1992), and elementary school age children’s (Damon & Hart, 1988; Harter, 1999; Higgins, 1989, 1991;
sympathy is greater when parents report moderate negative- Marsh & Craven, 2006; Moretti & Higgins, 1990, 1999; Ruble
dominant expressivity (e.g., anger, contempt) compared to both & Dweck, 1995). Two aspects are particularly significant for
low and high levels of parental negative-dominant expressivity parents: behavioral conduct and achievement (Harter, 1999).
(Valiente, Eisenberg et al., 2004). Given the centrality of these domains for parents, it seems
Several studies show linear relationships between parental plausible that their reactions to children’s performance in these
positive expressivity and children’s social competence (Halber- areas may be particularly salient for children, but the process
stadt et al., 1999). Parental positive expressivity was linked to is not clear. For example, consider the parent who expresses
a teacher rating of boys’ functioning in late elementary and delight when their child is kind to others, but expresses little
early middle school, including both academic and socio- reaction when their child shows imagination. Will the child rate
emotional functioning (Bronstein, Fitzgerald, Briones, him/herself highly on both kindness and imagination? Or will
Pieniadz, & D’Ari, 1993), and a composite of parental positive he/she focus on his/her kindness, and downplay his/her imagin-
and negative expressivity was unrelated to children’s reading ation? Selective parental expressiveness may highlight specific
achievement but positively associated with a teacher rating of types of behavior to the child, rendering them more salient in
children’s concentration, attentional focus, and ability to the child’s memory (Dunsmore, Halberstadt, Eaton, &
complete schoolwork and with math achievement in early Robinson, 2005) and thus emphasizing them more in his/her
elementary school (Greenberg, Lengua, Coie, Pinderhughes, self-perceptions. Low intensity, overt parental emotion
& the Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group, 1999). communication of either valence may alert a child to the
However, Valiente et al. (2004) found that elementary school importance of a given behavior or interpersonal exchange,
age children’s sympathy is greater when parents report enhancing learning without overwhelming the ability to
moderate positive expressivity compared to both low and high effectively process information (Dunsmore & Halberstadt,
levels of parental positive expressivity. They suggest that high 1997).
levels of both positive and negative expressive intensity may A related way that parents’ specific emotional responses may
lead to children’s over-arousal. influence children’s domain-specific self-concepts is through
The type of parental emotional response also appears signifi- the metric of parental standards. A parent’s joy not only signals
cant. Hostility and contempt – two forms of negative-dominant the importance of the type of behavior, but implies the child
emotions – appear particularly corrosive to personal and family has met a standard that the parent implicitly or explicitly holds
functioning (Gottman, 1994; Hooley, 2004), including self- for the child. Parental disapproval, disappointment or other
esteem (Capaldi, Forgatch, & Crosby, 1994). A high negative- communication of negative emotion about a child’s behavior
dominant parental affective style is associated with elementary indicates the child has violated or fallen short of a standard or
school children’s self-protective use of emotion display rules expectation. Sadness in particular is thought to represent an
(Jones, Abbey, & Cumberland, 1998). Parental expression of acceptance of a disappointing outcome or of irrevocable loss
negative-submissive emotions, such as sadness, disappoint- (Lazarus, 1991; Stein & Jewett, 1986). If directed toward a
ment or guilt, is associated with both positive and negative child in response to his/her behavior, parental sadness may
outcomes. Mothers’ negative-submissive expressivity is associ- convey a sense of inevitability or hopelessness to the child. For
ated with less self-soothing among toddlers (Garner, 1995) instance, parental sadness upon hearing of a child’s conduct or
and less prosocial use of emotion display rules in elementary achievement failure may signal to the child a disappointed
school age children (Jones et al., 1998). However, when “recognition” of the failure as “true” of him/her, and thus the
mothers are moderate to high in negative-submissive expres- child may describe him/herself accordingly low on this trait.
siveness, their elementary school age children’s constructive Parental anger, on the other hand, might signal more of a viola-
coping is less negatively affected by daily stress (Valiente, tion of a standard, with the implication that the child could act
Fabes, Eisenberg, & Spinrad, 2004). differently, thus protecting the child’s sense of competence.
In summary, parental emotional expressiveness with Investigating these specific emotional communications in
children has been linked to global indices of self-worth and relation to children’s multidimensional self-concepts adds
with children’s behaviors that are increasingly related to important information to our understanding of the family
children’s self-concepts by middle childhood. However, we context of children’s developing sense of self.
know little about how parental expressive style at home is
linked to children’s domain-specific self-concepts. Given that
parental expressive style predicts children’s self-esteem and Salience model
social and academic competence, it may be that positive and
negative parental expressiveness have linear and curvilinear Finally, mothers’ typical pattern of emotional expressiveness
effects on children’s general self-concept across domains, anal- may moderate the influence of her specific emotional responses
ogous to a sense of self-worth or “character” (Turiel, 2006). to her child’s behavior on children’s domain-specific self-
The first goal of this study is to test this general approval model. concepts. Set against a backdrop of general parental warmth

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010


253-264 098025 Dunsmore (Q8D):210 x 280mm 03/04/2009 09:52 Page 255

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2009, 33 (3), 253–264 255

and acceptance, high expectations and appropriate reinforce- actions tend to highlight consistent patterns (Metts et al.,
ment of them are associated with high self-esteem and social 1991). Because the theoretical models we are testing empha-
and academic competence in middle-class and Caucasian size consistent patterns of expressivity and of specific
youth and children (Harter, 1999; Maccoby & Martin, 1983). emotional responses to types of behavior, we considered these
However, unrealistically high parental standards or high expec- characteristics of retrospective evaluations to be, if anything,
tations without compensatory warmth are linked to poor self- advantageous for our purpose.
schemas and low self-esteem (Harter, 1999). Both fathers and mothers play important roles in emotion
Specific emotional responses may be more or less salient to socialization. We focus on mothers in this study. Children in
children depending on how consistent they are with parents’ the age range of our sample report both greater expression of
typical expressive style. If parents’ specific emotional response emotion to mothers than fathers and greater expectation of
differs from their typical expressiveness – for example, if they positive response to their emotional expression from mothers
display high levels of negative affect in response to children’s than fathers (Fuchs & Thelen, 1988). We therefore chose to
behavior when they typically express low negative affect at focus exclusively on mothers to maximize the potential that
home – this might especially heighten children’s attention to children would provide rich descriptions of their memories
the behavior, increasing the likelihood that children will about success and failure for prosocial behavior and achieve-
internalize this information as relevant to their sense of self ment when talking with their parent.
(Dunsmore & Halberstadt, 1997). Conversely, parents’ specific Late childhood represents an especially interesting time in
emotional expressions that are consistent with their expressive which to study linkages between parent emotion socialization
style may be more easily interpreted by children. Because they and children’s domain-specific self-ratings. At this time
fit the expressive pattern children are accustomed to, such children’s sense of self has changed from focusing on concrete,
emotional responses may take less effort to process, which may observable characteristics to incorporating more differentiated
facilitate children’s linkage of their behavior to their self- traits into their self-concept (Harter, 1999; Montemayor &
concept. Consistent with this, preschool children’s recognition Eisen, 1977). Children have also become increasingly adept at
of positive emotions is more accurate when their mothers are abstracting connections between single self-descriptors in
high in positive expressiveness (Dunsmore & Smallen, 2001) comparing themselves to their peers (Harter, 1999). Concen-
and their memory is better for events associated with mothers’ trating on late childhood both provides a lens on a develop-
specific emotional expressions that fit their typical expressive mentally important time period in the emergent sense of self
style (Dunsmore et al., 2005). and maximizes the potential for variance in differential self-
concepts (Harter, 1999; Marsh et al., 1998).
We systematically tested the three models described in the
The present study preceding review. The general approval model suggests that
higher maternal typical positive expressiveness will predict
We investigated two dimensions of 8- to12-year-old children’s children’s higher self-ratings for both prosocial and achieve-
self-concepts in relation to maternal emotional expressiveness ment-related traits, regardless of how mothers respond to the
and specific emotional responses to children’s behavior. We specifics of their children’s reported memories. Similarly, we
chose two domains important to parents, prosocial behavioral expected that mothers’ lower typical expression of negative
conduct and achievement. In regard to mothers’ typical affect, especially negative-dominant expressiveness, would
expressiveness at home, we addressed linear and quadratic predict higher prosocial and achievement-related self-ratings.
functions of positive, negative-dominant, and negative-submis- We also tested for quadratic relations of maternal expressive
sive emotions. In regard to mothers’ specific emotional styles to children’s self-ratings.
responses to children’s behavior, we included mothers’ happy, The selective parental socialization model would be supported
angry, and sad reactions to their child’s descriptions of by specific linkages of mothers’ happiness, anger, and sadness
behavior that reflected success and failure in the two domains in response to children’s prosocial behavior with children’s
(prosocial behavior and achievement). prosocial self-ratings, and not with children’s achievement-
Pragmatic and ecological validity concerns precluded related self-ratings, and the parallel pattern for mothers’
attempting to elicit children’s prosocial and achievement- emotional responses to children’s achievement-related
related behavior in the lab in order to observe mothers’ behavior. We expected that greater maternal happiness in
emotional responses. So, we borrowed from the emotion elic- response to prosocial behavior (and achievement) would be
itation (Levenson, Carstensen, Friesen, & Ekman, 1991) and linked with children’s higher self-ratings for prosocial behavior
interpersonal interaction (Burnett, 1991; Metts, Sprecher, & (and achievement). Based on the core relational themes of
Cupach, 1991) literatures by asking children to describe times anger and sadness (e.g., Lazarus, 1991; Stein & Jewett, 1986),
when they had done something nice for someone or done well we predicted that children whose mothers responded with
at something (success in prosocial behavior and achievement) greater sadness to antisocial behavior (and failure to achieve)
and times when they had done something mean to someone or would report lower prosocial (or achievement-related) self-
done badly at something (failure in prosocial behavior and ratings, whereas those whose mothers responded with greater
achievement). This technique has the disadvantage of relying anger to antisocial behavior (and failure to achieve) might
on mothers’ and children’s retrospective evaluation of mothers’ report higher prosocial (or achievement-related) self-ratings.
emotional reactions to children’s behavior. However, it has the Finally, the salience model would be supported by interactions
advantage of allowing children to select personally meaningful between mothers’ specific emotional responses and typical
experiences to discuss with their mothers. Retrospective evalu- expressive styles showing domain-specific linkages to children’s
ations of emotions tend to focus on peak and end moments of prosocial and achievement-related self-ratings. Theoretically,
emotional intensity (Fredrickson, 2000; Fredrickson & we expected that mismatches between mothers’ specific
Kahneman, 1993), and retrospective evaluations of inter- emotional response to the child’s behavior and their typical

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010


253-264 098025 Dunsmore (Q8D):210 x 280mm 03/04/2009 09:52 Page 256

256 DUNSMORE ET AL. / MOTHER’S EXPRESSIVE STYLE AND EMOTIONAL RESPONSES TO CHILDREN’S BEHAVIOR

expressive style would be related to children’s self-ratings. For vividly as possible. Neither mothers nor children read the cards
example, children whose mothers are typically low in negative- out loud so they would focus on the child’s description and not
dominant expressiveness, yet respond with a high degree of on the cues when subsequently completing ratings. Other than
anger when the child fails to achieve, would rate themselves not reading the cards out loud, mothers and children were told
highly in achievement-related traits, because the atypically that they could talk and act as usual, including asking each
intense anger would augment the message that the child had other questions about the memories or making comments. The
done badly at something very important and was capable of first author noted card order and monitored the game to
better. However, because of the empirical research we cited ensure that children’s memory descriptions fit the card and did
earlier, we also thought it possible that matches in mothers’ not overlap categories. In no case was it necessary to redirect
specific emotional response and typical expressive style might the children’s memory descriptions.
be related to children’s self-ratings. After children described each memory, mothers and
children marked 10-centimeter long lines to rate how much
happiness, sadness, and anger mothers had felt when they had
found out about the behavior the child had described. They
Method were reminded to mark all three of the emotion lines each time,
because people can feel more than one emotion at a time.
Participants Rating sheets were placed in boxes when completed, and the
Seventy-two 8- to 12-year-old children (50% girls; age M = person who had just described a memory placed a token on
10.11 years, SD = .81) and their mothers (age M = 39.62 the gameboard, a colorful unfinished picture. Conclusion of
years, SD = 3.78) participated. By parental report, children the memory-cuing game was signaled by placing the final token
were African-American (9.7%), Asian-American (1.4%), on the picture.
European-American (86.1%), and Hispanic American (2.8%).
Ninety percent of the children lived in two-parent households.
Most (67.6%) of the children had one sibling living in their
Materials
home, 23.9% had two siblings living in their home, and Children’s self-schema questionnaire. Children rated, on a 9-
8.5% of the children had no siblings. Parents were generally point Likert scale, the self-descriptiveness and importance of
well educated; all had completed high school, and most (71% five prosocial traits (forgiving, helpful, honest, loving, and nice)
of fathers, 65% of mothers) had college or graduate degrees. and five achievement-related traits (capable, doing well at
Most parents had professional or semi-professional jobs (e.g., things, imaginative, intelligent, and logical).1 Children were
physicians, professors, graphic artists, restaurateurs, retail offered cards with brief developmentally appropriate defi-
sales). Participants were recruited through letters distributed nitions of the traits to aid them in completing the question-
through public and private schools, flyers, advertisements, naire. For example, “imaginative” was defined as “creative; able
and word of mouth. Mothers were given $10 and children to think of new ideas.”
were given a $5 gift certificate to a toy or bookstore for Children’s self-ratings of the descriptiveness of the prosocial
participating. traits were summed and then standardized, as were children’s
self-ratings of the descriptiveness of the achievement-related
traits. Internal consistency of each subscale was good
Procedure (Cronbach’s alpha = .75 for prosocial and .73 for achievement).
The first author conducted all sessions. Participants were given
the choice of coming to the lab or participating in their home. Self-expressiveness in the Family Questionnaire (SEFQ; Halberstadt
Most families (74%) chose to participate in their home. The et al., 1995). Mothers rated, on a 9-point Likert scale (1 =
lab was furnished with toys and appealing posters to provide a not at all to 9 = very), how frequently they express emotions
family-friendly atmosphere. Trained undergraduates assisted in certain situations in the family. This 40-item instrument
with videotaping in the lab. At families’ homes, videotaping was yields three subscales: (a) positive expressiveness (e.g., “Telling
monitored in an unobtrusive manner and any other family family members how happy you are”), (b) negative-dominant
members in the home were occupied so the children and expressiveness (e.g., “Blaming one another for family
mothers could participate with as little interruption as possible. troubles”), and (c) negative-submissive expressiveness (e.g.,
There were no significant differences on any variables accord- “Crying after an unpleasant disagreement”). The SEFQ has
ing to whether families participated in the home or the lab been validated with a variety of other measures (Halberstadt
(all |t|s < 1, all ps > .30). Children and mothers first played et al., 1995; see Halberstadt et al., 1999, for a review). Internal
the memory-cuing game (described below). Children then consistency for each subscale was excellent (Cronbach’s alpha
completed the self-schema questionnaire, and mothers = .90 for positive expressiveness, .86 for negative-dominant
completed a questionnaire packet including the Self- expressiveness, and .79 for negative-submissive expressiveness).
expressiveness in the Family Questionnaire (Halberstadt,
Cassidy, Stifter, Parke, & Fox, 1995). Sessions lasted approxi-
mately one hour.
In the memory-cuing game, mothers and children took turns
drawing cards. Children’s cards provided the four behavioral
cues – prosocial (did something nice); antisocial (did some-
1 Importance ratings were included to explore the self-schema categorization
thing mean); achievement (did well at something); and failure
approach used with adults (Catrambone & Markus, 1987; Fong & Markus, 1982;
to achieve (did badly at something). Cards instructed children Markus, 1977; Markus et al., 1982, 1985). The pattern of results using this
to think of a time they had behaved in the way indicated and categorical approach paralleled that reported here using children’s dimensional
to describe their memory of that event to their mother as self-ratings of trait descriptiveness.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010


253-264 098025 Dunsmore (Q8D):210 x 280mm 03/04/2009 09:52 Page 257

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2009, 33 (3), 253–264 257

Table 1
Descriptive statistics for and correlations among study variables

2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 M ( SD) Range

1 Mothers’ happiness – prosocial .13 .06** .06 .12 .23† .18* –.03*** –.23*** .39*** .27*** 15.59 (5.01) 0–20
2 Mothers’ anger – antisocial — .34** .13 –.17 .13* .11* .04*** .24*** –.10*** .27*** 10.78 (5.10) 0–20
3 Mothers’ sadness – antisocial — .18 –.04 .28* .25* .09*** –.12*** .07*** –.07*** 7.42 (5.32) 0–20
4 Mothers’ happiness – achievement — .15 –.09* .05* –.04*** –.01*** .17*** .23*** 17.41 (3.84) 4.9–20
5 Mothers’ anger – failure to achieve — .08* .02* –.18*** –.11*** .07*** .11*** 8.74 (5.67) 0–19.3
6 Mothers’ sadness – failure to achieve — –.04* .04*** .02*** .10*** –.02*** 9.32 (5.51) 0–20
7 Mothers’ positive expressiveness — .41*** .10*** –.20†** .09*** 7.04 (.98) 3.84–8.79
8 Mothers’ negative submissive-expressiveness — .58*** –.12*** –.07*** 4.94 (1.30) 2.30–8.70
9 Mothers’ negative dominant-expressiveness — –.26*** .00*** 3.76 (1.23) 2.00–7.73
10 Children’s prosocial self-rating — .43*** 7.24 (1.25) 3.80–9
11 Children’s achievement self-rating — 7.27 (1.19) 4.20–8.80

Note. †p < .10; *p < .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001.

Results mothers’ ratings for each emotional response to each behavior


to form six composite variables of interest: mothers’ happiness
Preliminary analyses in response to child prosocial behavior, mothers’ happiness in
response to child achievement, mothers’ sadness in response to
We first calculated correlations of all variables with children’s child antisocial behavior, mothers’ anger in response to child
age. Mothers of older children reported more anger about antisocial behavior, and mothers’ sadness and anger in
children’s antisocial behavior, r = .26, p < .05.Younger children response to child failure to achieve. Descriptive statistics for
rated mothers as happier about their achievement behavior, and correlations among these composite variables, mothers’
r = –.33, p < .01. Mothers of older children reported more typical expressive styles, and children’s self-descriptiveness
negative-dominant expressiveness, r = .24, p < .05. There was ratings are presented in Table 1.
no relation of children’s age with their self-descriptiveness
ratings for prosocial or achievement-related traits.
Research suggests that mother–daughter dyads talk more
about emotions than mother–son dyads (Flannagan & Perese,
Testing the general approval model
1998) and that girls include more affiliative themes in their We conducted a repeated-measures ANOVA to test the general
autobiographical narratives than do boys (Buckner & Fivush, approval model. Repeated criterion variables were children’s
1998). To investigate whether gender differences may influence prosocial and achievement-related self-ratings. Type of self-
our findings, we conducted t tests on mothers’ expressiveness rating was a within-subjects predictor variable. The between-
in the family, ratings of mothers’ emotional reactions, and subjects predictor variables were mothers’ positive,
children’s self-ratings. Mothers of sons reported greater negative-dominant, and negative-submissive expressiveness
positive expressiveness compared to mothers of daughters, and their quadratic functions. We reduced the model using a
t(70) = 2.02, p < .05. Mothers of sons reported greater happi- backwards stepwise approach with α = .05. We report the
ness about their child’s prosocial behavior than mothers of reduced model. We found a main effect for type of self-rating,
daughters, t(70) = 2.78, p < .01. Boys rated their mothers as F(1,67) = 9.92, p < .01. Furthermore, mothers’ positive
angrier about their antisocial behavior than did girls, t(70) = expressiveness interacted with type of rating to predict
2.22, p < .05. There were no gender differences in children’s children’s self-ratings, F(1,67) = 9.35, p < .01, as did mothers’
self-ratings of prosocial and achievement-related traits. negative-dominant expressiveness, F(1,67) = 8.43, p < .01, and
Children’s age and gender were not considered further in the negative-submissive expressiveness, F(1,67) = 4.92, p < .05.
analyses reported below.2 We calculated generalized eta squared (η2G), which is preferred
Finally, because clarity in parental emotional communi- as an effect size statistic for repeated measures designs like ours
cation may be significant in children’s interpretation and (Bakeman, 2005). Effect sizes were small (η2G = .03, .03, and
outcomes (Halberstadt et al., 1999), we thought it important .02 for the interactions of type of rating with mothers’ positive,
to examine concordance in children’s and mothers’ ratings of negative-dominant, and negative-submissive expressiveness,
mothers’ emotional responses. Children’s and mothers’ ratings respectively).
for each emotion in response to each behavior tended to be To depict the findings, we formed three groups for each type
positively correlated (average r = .43, minimum r = .05, of family expressiveness: (a) those whose scores were more
maximum r = .61). For both mothers’ and children’s ratings, than one standard deviation below the mean, (b) those whose
correlations between emotion ratings for exemplifications scores were more than one standard deviation above the mean,
(emotion ratings for prosocial behavior correlated with those and (c) those whose scores were within one standard deviation
for achievement) and violations (emotion ratings for antisocial of the mean (Aiken & West, 1991). We then plotted least-square
behavior correlated with those for failure to achieve) tended to means for children’s self-ratings for each group. These are
be low (average rs = .09 and .14, respectively; minimum r = shown in Figure 1. As Figure 1 shows, mothers’ positive
–.15, maximum r = .31). Thus, we summed children’s and expressiveness was negatively related to children’s prosocial
self-rating and positively related to children’s achievement-
related self-rating. Mothers’ negative-dominant expressiveness
2 Controlling for child age and gender did not alter our pattern of results. was also negatively related to children’s prosocial self-rating

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010


253-264 098025 Dunsmore (Q8D):210 x 280mm 03/04/2009 09:52 Page 258

258 DUNSMORE ET AL. / MOTHER’S EXPRESSIVE STYLE AND EMOTIONAL RESPONSES TO CHILDREN’S BEHAVIOR

0.8

0.6
Children’s self-ratings (z scores)

0.4

0.2
Prosocial behavior
0
Achievement

–0.2

–0.4

–0.6

–0.8

–1
)

)
SD

SD

SD

SD

SD

SD
es

es

es
en

en

en
–1

+1

+1

–1

1
(–

(+
siv

siv

siv
s(

s(
s(

s(
es

es
ss

s
re
es

es
es

es

es
ne
pr

pr
xp
en

en
n

en

en
ex

ex
ive
ive

te
siv

siv
siv

siv
ss
ss
ive

ive
es

es
an

es

es
re
re
sit

iss
pr

pr
r

pr
in
xp
p

xp
om
ex

ex
po

bm
ex

ex
te

te
-d
ive

ive

su
ive

ive
id

an

an
M

e-
sit

iss
sit

iss
in

tiv

in

tiv
m

m
po

bm
po

m
ga
do

ga
do

ub
ne

su
w

igh

ne
e-

e-

s
Lo

e-

e-
id
tiv
H

tiv

id
tiv
M

tiv
M
ga

ga

ga

ga
ne

ne

ne

ne
w

igh

igh
Lo

Lo
H

Figure 1. Children’s self-ratings in relation to mothers’ positive, negative-dominant, and negative-submissive expressiveness.

and not strongly related to children’s achievement-related self- antisocial behavior, F(1,65) = 4.26, p < .05. Effect sizes were
rating. Finally, mothers’ negative-submissive expressiveness small to medium (η2G = .11, .04, and .03 for mothers’ happi-
was positively related to children’s prosocial self-rating and ness in response to children’s prosocial behavior, happiness in
negatively related to children’s achievement-related self-rating, response to children’s achievement, and anger in response to
but only at high levels of negative-submissive expressiveness. children’s antisocial behavior, respectively). A follow-up
univariate analysis predicting the composite of children’s self-
ratings for both prosocial behavior and achievement showed
Testing the selective parental socialization model that children’s self-ratings were higher when mothers
We conducted a repeated-measures ANOVA to test the selec- responded with more happiness to children’s prosocial (β =
tive parental expressiveness model. Repeated criterion vari- .43) and achievement-related behavior (β = .27). Children’s
ables were children’s prosocial and achievement-related self-ratings were lower when mothers responded with more
self-ratings. Type of self-rating was a within-subjects predictor anger to children’s antisocial behavior (β = –.25).
variable. Between-subjects predictor variables were ratings of
mothers’ happiness in response to children’s prosocial
behavior, happiness in response to children’s achievement,
Testing the salience model
sadness in response to children’s antisocial behavior, sadness We conducted two repeated-measures ANOVAs to test the
in response to children’s failure to achieve, anger in response salience model. For both, repeated criterion variables were
to children’s antisocial behavior, and anger in response to children’s prosocial and achievement-related self-ratings and
children’s failure to achieve. A backwards stepwise model type of self-rating was a within-subjects predictor variable.
reduction strategy was used, with α = .05; the reduced model The first ANOVA tested between-subjects predictor variables
is reported below. relevant to children’s prosocial behavior, including interactions
Three between-subjects effects were found. Children’s self- between ratings of mothers’ response to children’s prosocial/
ratings for both prosocial behavior and achievement were antisocial behavior and mothers’ parallel typical expressive
predicted by mothers’ happiness in response to children’s style (e.g., mothers’ anger in response to children’s antisocial
prosocial behavior, F(1,68) = 14.53, p < .001; by mothers’ behavior interacting with mothers’ negative-dominant
happiness in response to children’s achievement, F(1,68) = expressive style).The second ANOVA did the same with predic-
4.97, p < .05; and by mothers’ anger in response to children’s tor variables relevant to children’s achievement-related

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010


253-264 098025 Dunsmore (Q8D):210 x 280mm 03/04/2009 09:52 Page 259

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2009, 33 (3), 253–264 259

0.8

0.6

0.4
Children’s self-ratings (z scores)

Low sadness about failure to achieve –


0.2 prosocial self-rating
Low sadness about failure to achieve –
competence self-rating
0 High sadness about failure to achieve –
prosocial self-rating
High sadness about failure to achieve –
–0.2 competence self-rating

–0.4

–0.6

–0.8

–1
Low negative Mid negative High negative
submissiveness submissiveness submissiveness

Figure 2. Children’s self-ratings as a function of mothers’ sadness in response to failure to achieve and negative-submissive expressiveness.

behavior.3 A backwards stepwise model reduction strategy was mothers who were typically high in expressing negative-
used, with α = .05. The reduced models are reported below. submissive emotion were rated high in sadness in response to
The ANOVA including ratings of mothers’ responses to children’s failure to achieve.4
prosocial/antisocial behavior yielded no new effects. On the
other hand, the ANOVA including ratings of mothers’
responses to achievement/failure to achieve did yield new Discussion
effects. Children’s self-ratings were predicted by the interaction
between the type of self-rating and mothers’ sadness in Both mothers’ typical expressivity in the family and their
response to children’s failure to achieve, F(1,67) = 4.19, p < specific emotional responses to children’s behavior were
.05. This interaction was subsumed within the significant uniquely and additively related to children’s descriptions of
three-way interaction of type of self-rating, mothers’ sadness in themselves as prosocial or achieving. Because we tested both
response to children’s failure to achieve, and mothers’ more distal, background emotional climate and proximal,
negative-submissive expressiveness, F(1,67) = 4.36, p < .05. contingent maternal emotional communications, we were able
The effect size of this three-way interaction was small (η2G = to distill relations between them in connection to children’s
.02). We conducted a median split on mothers’ sadness and a sense of self. Our focus on type as well as intensity of negative
tripartite split on mothers’ negative-submissive expressiveness emotion and our empirical examination of mothers’ and
to depict this interaction in Figure 2. As Figure 2 shows, children’s sending/receiving of affective communication
children’s self-ratings for achievement were lower when (Halberstadt, Denham, & Dunsmore, 2001) represent
substantial contributions called for in a recent review

3 For the first ANOVA, the full list of between-subject predictor variables is

as follows: mothers’ happiness about children’s prosocial behavior, mothers’


anger about children’s antisocial behavior, mothers’ sadness about children’s 4 We conducted one final repeated-measures ANOVA to examine whether

antisocial behavior, mothers’ positive expressiveness, mothers’ negative- the effects in the general approval, selective parental socialization, and salience
dominant expressiveness, mothers’ negative-submissive expressiveness, inter- models remained significant at α = .05 when all effects were included. All did,
action of mothers’ happiness about prosocial behavior with positive except for (a) the interaction between type of self-rating and mothers’ negative-
expressiveness, interaction of mothers’ anger about antisocial behavior with submissive expressiveness (F(1,62) = .18, p > .65), which is subsumed within
negative-dominant expressiveness, and interaction of mothers’ sadness about the three-way interaction in the salience model, and (b) the between-subjects
antisocial behavior with negative-submissive expressiveness. For the second effect for mothers’ anger in response to children’s antisocial behavior (F(1,62) =
ANOVA, the full list of between-subject predictor variables is parallel, with 3.96, p < .06), which retained the same pattern of association despite no longer
achievement/failure to achieve as the focus of mothers’ response. meeting our alpha level.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010


253-264 098025 Dunsmore (Q8D):210 x 280mm 03/04/2009 09:52 Page 260

260 DUNSMORE ET AL. / MOTHER’S EXPRESSIVE STYLE AND EMOTIONAL RESPONSES TO CHILDREN’S BEHAVIOR

(Halberstadt et al., 1999). We discuss how our results address children’s lower social competence (Halberstadt et al., 1999).
the three models we described in the introduction below. The effects may also work the other way: children who are less
prosocial may engender more negative affect in their mothers
at home. We discuss negative-submissive expressivity below,
General approval model because that effect was subsumed within the interaction
We expected that mothers’ reports of their typical expressive- including sadness about child failure to achieve. We note also
ness would predict their children’s ratings across domains, that, though we tested for quadratic relations between maternal
such that high positive and moderate negative expressivity negative-dominant and negative-submissive expressiveness and
would be associated with high self-concept ratings for both children’s self-ratings, these were not substantiated.
prosocial behavior and achievement. Contrary to these expec- In summary, though our findings were not what we expected
tations, mothers’ typical expressivity was differentially related for the general approval model, they do confirm that parents’
to children’s self-ratings according to the type of expressivity typical expressive styles are linked to children’s self-concept.
and domain of self-concept. Mothers’ typical positive and Our findings also suggest that background emotional “climate”
negative-dominant expressivity were negatively related to may operate more specifically or through more proximal
children’s prosocial self-ratings, but positively related (positive channels to influence children’s sense of themselves. One more
expressivity) or relatively unrelated (negative-dominant) to proximal channel is mothers’ specific emotional responses to
children’s achievement-related self-ratings. In contrast, children’s behavior.
maternal typical negative-submissive expressivity negatively
predicted children’s achievement-related self-ratings, but was
positively related to their prosocial self-ratings. The consistent
Selective parental socialization model
discrepancy between linkages to different self-concept domains We had expected domain-specific effects on children’s self-
appears to rule out an explanation based on a global sense of ratings for mothers’ specific responses, yet they appeared to
self-worth that might have elevated or lowered ratings in both have more pervasive effects, showing associations across both
self-concept domains. domains. As predicted, mothers’ happiness in response to
The positive association of mothers’ positive expressivity children’s prosocial behavior and in response to children’s
with children’s achievement-related self-ratings is consistent achievement each uniquely predict children’s higher self-
with extant literature. The negative association between ratings – but in both domains. Though neither happiness
maternal typical positive expressiveness and children’s self- response is significantly correlated with mothers’ typical
rated prosocial traits was unanticipated. Positive family expres- positive expressiveness in the family, it may be that these
siveness has been linked to early elementary school age ratings of mothers’ happiness about children’s prosocial and
children’s prosocial behavior (Halberstadt et al., 1999), though achievement-related behavior are more precise indicators of
these studies did not assess children’s self-descriptions. mothers’ typical positive expressiveness towards her child in
However, an observational study with kindergarteners showed particular. Thus, we are inclined to interpret this finding as
that, with child affect controlled, parent positive affect in same- consistent with the general approval model.
sex parent–child pairs predicted poorer social competence Contrary to our expectation for mothers’ specific anger
(Isley, O’Neil, Clatfelter, & Parke, 1999). Parental positive response based on the core relational theme of anger (Lazarus,
affect that is not directly related to or reciprocal with a child 1991; Stein & Jewett, 1986), but consistent with empirical
may be confusing or alienating to the child (Isley et al., 1999), literature on typical negative-dominant expressivity, mothers’
or may reflect a defensive, Pollyanna quality that denies anger at children’s antisocial behavior (but not failure to
authentic emotional experience, a maternal style that is achieve) predicted children’s lower self-ratings across both
associated with more antisocial behavior in some children domains. This specific link between anger about children’s
(Bowlby, 1980; Weinfield, Sroufe, Egeland, & Carlson, 1999). antisocial behavior and both self-ratings may be consistent with
However, in this case we might expect to have found a curvi- other research showing that academic self-concept and
linear association linking mothers’ moderate positive expressiv- problems are unrelated to global self-esteem (Crocker &
ity and children’s greater prosocial self-ratings compared to Luhtanen, 2003; Marsh & Craven, 2006), whereas social
either extreme (as similar to Valiente et al., 2004). Yet, though problems are so related (Crocker & Luhtanen, 2003).
we tested for such a curvilinear association, it was not Parents’ anger is linked to children’s experience of shame,
supported. which is characterized by global negative evaluations of oneself
Alternatively, the negative associations may paradoxically (Eisenberg, 2000; Ferguson & Stegge, 1995; Lazarus, 1991).
underscore previously found relations between high positive Because antisocial behaviors discussed by parents and children
expressivity and child self-esteem. In other words, these in this study necessarily involved harm to others (doing some-
children may be emotionally secure or mature enough to thing mean), parents might be especially likely to respond with
somewhat leaven their prosocial self-ratings. Replication in anger and children might be especially likely to experience
other samples will help determine what may underlie this shame about their antisocial behavior, which may result in
association. lower overall self-ratings. Alternatively, shame-proneness is
Findings for maternal negative expressiveness varied by type negatively related to perspective-taking and empathy (Tangney,
of negative emotion in interesting ways. Higher rates of 1991). Thus, children who tend toward global negative self-
maternal negative-dominant expressivity at home may be more evaluations may also tend to behave less empathically toward
relevant to children’s views of their prosocial behavior than others, which may elicit parental anger about antisocial
achievement because anger and related affects are so intimately behavior or lack of prosocial behavior.
tied to active engagement in interpersonal relationships
(Bowlby, 1973; Lazarus, 1991). Indeed, previous studies have
found high levels of anger expressed at home associated with

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010


253-264 098025 Dunsmore (Q8D):210 x 280mm 03/04/2009 09:52 Page 261

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2009, 33 (3), 253–264 261

Salience model It will be important in future research to work with cultural


groups with varying display rules and beliefs about emotions
Finally, in the salience model, we expected domain-specific
and about the sense of self. For example, expression of
effects for the combination of maternal-specific emotional
negative-dominant emotions like anger may be more accept-
responses to children’s behavior with mothers’ typical pattern
able in working-class than middle-class American families
of emotional expression. Here, mothers’ negative-submissive
(Miller, Cho, & Bracey, 2005; Miller & Sperry, 1987). Indeed,
expressive style provided a backdrop against which mothers’
within working-class families, dramatized expression of
specific response of sadness about children’s failure to achieve
negative-dominant emotions through personal narratives may
predicted children’s achievement-related self-rating. Earlier we
function both to support the child’s sense of autonomy and
speculated that maternal sadness about specific child actions
resilience and to promote social status (Miller et al., 2005).
might convey a sense of impotence to a child. Achievement
When this is the case, the inverse relation we found between
may involve agency to a greater extent than prosocial behavior;
parental negative-dominant expressiveness and child prosocial
for example, a child may consider passive “niceness” to be
self-ratings may not hold. Instead, within moderate levels of
prosocial. Thus, achievement may be more vulnerable to
negative-dominant expressiveness, there may be positive
depressed self-concept if the child feels the parent does not
associations between parental expressiveness and child proso-
believe in his agency.
cial and achievement-related self-ratings.
Moreover, our results suggest that contingent maternal
Similarly, cultural differences in the sense of self might affect
sadness by itself is not particularly influential. Rather, the
relations between parental emotion socialization and children’s
match between higher maternal sadness about failure to
self-ratings. European-American children’s self-descriptions
achieve and higher typical maternal negative-submissive
include more positive and fewer neutral characteristics
expressivity appears most detrimental for children’s achieve-
compared with Chinese children’s self-descriptions (Wang,
ment-related self-rating. This is roughly consistent with
2004). When the sense of self is less focused on evaluating one’s
previous work showing moderating effects of parental positive
unique characteristics and more focused on describing one’s
expressive style on children’s emotion-recognition accuracy
social interconnections, parental emotional responses to
and memory related to parents’ specific positive emotional
behaviors and expressive styles may be interpreted as less
expressions (Dunsmore et al., 2005; Dunsmore & Smallen,
meaningful for achievement-related self-ratings, thereby atten-
2001). These results suggest that sensitivity, rather than salience,
uating those relations. Relations with prosocial self-ratings may
may be the better name for this model. In fact, when mothers
remain or strengthen because prosocial self-ratings may be
reacted more intensely than typically – that is, they displayed
more relevant for social interconnections than achievement-
relatively high levels of contingent sadness but low levels of
related self-ratings.
negative-submissive emotion at home – children rated them-
We chose to examine three basic maternal emotions, happi-
selves higher on achievement. Thus, it appears that children’s
ness, sadness, and anger, because they are easily understand-
general exposure to higher rates of sadness-related affect in
able for children in the age range of our sample, commonly
their mothers may constitute a risk factor for poor achieve-
experienced and expressed in family life, and parallel the
ment-related sense of self, a risk that becomes amplified by
dimensions of expressive style measured by the SEFQ. Self-
their mothers’ sad reactions to an instance of their achievement
conscious emotions, such as embarrassment or guilt, are also
failure.
commonly reported parental responses to children’s socially
This may not be a passive process; children may have framed
undesirable behavior (Mills & Rubin, 1990). Parental pride,
their narratives to elicit particular emotions from their
guilt, and shame about their children’s behavior may also
mothers, perhaps to reinforce their own self-concepts.
convey salient information to children about the relevance of
Maternal sadness at home may set the stage for children’s
their behavior for their sense of self. The meaning conveyed by
vulnerable sense of achievement-related self, perhaps due to
these self-conscious emotional responses may vary according
lower rates of positive engagement and structuring,
to the family’s cultural context. For example, cultural values in
components important in children’s competence and achieve-
the Netherlands are considered to be more individualistic
ment motivation (Wigfield, Eccles, Schiefele, Roesner, &
compared with cultural values in Spain, which emphasize
Davis-Kean, 2006). Lower sense of achievement-related abili-
family honor and relation. For Dutch children, adolescents and
ties, in turn, may cause the child to act in ways that reinforce
adults, shame is more strongly linked with the self compared
that belief, including provoking maternal emotions of resigna-
with Spanish children, adolescents and adults (Mosquera,
tion and sadness, and intensifying children’s lower sense of
Manstead, & Fischer, 2000). Testing our mechanism models
achievement.
with culturally disparate groups would contribute to under-
standing more about the underlying principles guiding
parental emotion socialization and children’s self-concept in
Strengths, limitations, and future directions late childhood.
Several limitations of the study suggest caution in generalizing Future work examining linkages between fathers’ expressive
the results. Our study included a relatively small, well-off styles and emotional responses with children’s self-ratings will
sample from a university community, in which mothers may also provide an important extension of our results. Different
have higher achievement aspirations for their children, and patterns may be seen with fathers. Fathers report more mini-
children for themselves, than other groups. We do not know mization of children’s sadness than do mothers, and less use of
how consistent children’s perceptions of their own traits might problem-focused coping (Cassano, Perry-Parish, & Zeman,
be with measures of their prosocial behavior and achievement. 2007). Children’s social competence is positively related to
We note that, as with similar samples, mothers’ positive expres- mothers’ positive interactions, and negatively related to fathers’
siveness was rather high, and few children rated themselves controlling behavior (McDowell, Kim, O’Neil, & Parke, 2002).
below the midpoint on any of the items. In African and Caribbean families, fathers have primary

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010


253-264 098025 Dunsmore (Q8D):210 x 280mm 03/04/2009 09:52 Page 262

262 DUNSMORE ET AL. / MOTHER’S EXPRESSIVE STYLE AND EMOTIONAL RESPONSES TO CHILDREN’S BEHAVIOR

responsibility for their children’s academic and behavioral processes that may link parents’ emotion socialization with
competence and mothers have primary responsibility for children’s sense of self. To the extent that parents’ expressive
children’s daily care and emotional functioning (Roopnarine, styles and patterns of emotional responses seen when children
2004; Roopnarine, Krishnakumar, Metindogan, & Evans, are in late childhood are consistent with those present when
2006). Thus, the distinctive role of fathers in socialization of children were in early childhood, and remain consistent across
children’s achievement-related self-perceptions may warrant the transition to adolescence, we expect our findings to gener-
particular attention. However, what may be most important in alize to younger and older age groups. With younger children,
impacting children’s sense of self may not be parent gender per however, linkages of parental emotion socialization with
se but rather the meaning conveyed to children by parents’ children’s sense of general acceptance may be stronger than
emotional responses within the context of parents’ expressive domain-specific associations because the sense of self is less
style and cultural values (Dunsmore & Halberstadt, 1997). differentiated in early childhood than late childhood. With
Perhaps the most important limitation in our method is the adolescents’ increased differentiation, focus on identity
retrospective bias inherent in our measurement of mothers’ development, and sensitivity to evaluation, linkages may be
emotional responses to children’s behaviors. Eliciting even stronger. Future longitudinal research with a more
children’s prosocial and achievement-related behavior in the diverse sample, focused on how differential types of affective
lab in order to observe mothers’ emotional response was communication link to development of self-concept across
impractical, so we elected to ask children to recount past early and middle childhood through adolescence and beyond,
behavior. Because children decided what to tell their mothers, will be fruitful in determining how these patterns of typical
there is a high degree of heterogeneity in the behaviors they and domain-specific emotional communication may be
recounted. For example, some children may have selected rela- related not only to trajectories of self-concept development
tively minor violations, whereas others may have revealed more but also to long-term cognitive and socio-emotional adaptive
serious transgressions. However, this variability may also repre- outcomes.
sent a strength of the study because children were able to select
personally meaningful experiences to discuss with their
mothers. Nonetheless, we remain cognizant that our measure References
of maternal emotional responses is far from isomorphic with
mothers’ actual emotional expression at the time of the Aiken, L.S., & West, S.G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting
behavior. Rather, linkages of mothers’ emotional responses interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Alessandri, S.M., & Lewis, M. (1993). Parental evaluation and its relation to
with children’s self-ratings may well reflect mother–child
shame and pride in young children. Sex Roles, 29, 335–343.
emotional communication when the child was recounting the Bakeman, R. (2005). Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures
experience just prior to completing the self-questionnaire. designs. Behavior Research Methods, 37, 379–384.
Despite these limitations, this study makes an important Barrett, K.C., & Nelson-Goens, G.C. (1997). Emotion communication and the
contribution to understanding the relationship between development of the social emotions. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & K.C. Barrett
(Vol. Ed.), New directions for child development. The communication of emotion:
parental emotion communication and children’s sense of self. Current research from diverse perspectives (No. 77, pp. 69–88). San Francisco:
Whether ratings of mothers’ emotional responses reflect Jossey-Bass Publishers.
current or past emotional communication, they are connected Bowlby, J. (1973). Separation: Anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books.
to children’s self-ratings, as are mothers’ typical expressive Bowlby, J. (1980). Loss: Sadness and depression. New York: Basic Books.
Bronstein, P., Fitzgerald, M., Briones, M., Pieniadz, J., & D’Ari, A. (1993).
styles. Most critically, this study points to considerable linkage
Family emotional expressiveness as a predictor of early adolescent social and
specificity. Type of maternal affective communication, specific psychological adjustment. Journal of Early Adolescence, 13, 448–471.
emotional responses to child behaviors, typical expressive Buckner, J.P., & Fivush, R. (1998). Gender and self in children’s autobiograph-
patterns, and self-concept domain all matter in differential ical narratives. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 407–429.
patterns of association. Maternal negative-submissive emotion, Bugental, D.B., & Grusec, J.E. (2006). Socialization processes. In W. Damon &
R.M. Lerner (Editors-in-Chief), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child
at home and in response to children’s behavior, was most psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed.,
tightly linked to children’s achievement-related self-ratings. pp. 366–428). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons.
Maternal negative-dominant expressiveness was more robustly Burnett, R. (1991). Accounts and narratives. Retrospective self-reports. In
related to children’s prosocial self-ratings, whereas maternal B.M. Montgomery & S. Duck (Eds.), Studying interpersonal interaction
(pp. 121–140). New York: Guilford Press.
positive expressiveness was related to both children’s prosocial
Caldwell, M.S., Rudolph, K.D., Troop-Gordon, W., & Kim, D.Y. (2004).
and achievement self-ratings, with opposing associations. Reciprocal influences among relational self-views, social disengagement, and
Mothers’ specific positive reactions to both prosocial and peer stress during early adolescence. Child Development, 75, 1140–1154.
achievement-related behavior and negative-dominant reactions Capaldi, D.M., Forgatch, M.S., & Crosby, L. (1994). Affective expression in
to prosocial behavior were related to children’s overall self- family problem-solving discussions with adolescent boys. Journal of Adolescent
Research, 9, 28–49.
concept. In sum, our results suggest that the general accept- Cassano, M., Perry-Parish, C., & Zeman, J. (2007). Influence of gender on
ance, selective parental socialization, and sensitization models parental socialization of children’s sadness regulation. Social Development, 16,
are complementary, each examining a different level of 210–231.
analysis. Just as parents’ expressive style may provide a Catrambone, R., & Markus, H. (1987). The role of self-schemas in going beyond
the information given. Social Cognition, 5, 349–368.
backdrop against which domain-specific emotional responses
Cooley, C.H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York: Charles
are more or less salient, children’s sense of general acceptance Scribner & Sons.
may provide a backdrop against which domain-specific self- Cornell, D.G., Pelton, G.M., Bassin, L.E., Landrum, M., Ramsay, S.G., Cooley,
ratings are more or less differentiated. M.R., Lynch, K.A., & Hamrick, E. (1990). Self-concept and peer status
In conclusion, our results strongly support the utility of a among gifted program youth. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82,
456–463.
nuanced, multidimensional approach to investigating maternal Crocker, J., & Luhtanen, R.K. (2003). Level of self-esteem and contingencies of
emotional communication and children’s sense of self. Our self-worth: Unique effects on academic, social and financial problems in
results also provide a tantalizing hint of the developmental college students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 701–712.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010


253-264 098025 Dunsmore (Q8D):210 x 280mm 03/04/2009 09:52 Page 263

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT, 2009, 33 (3), 253–264 263

Damon, W., & Hart, D. (1988) Self-understanding in childhood and adolescence. (Eds.), Self processes and development. The Minnesota symposia on child psychol-
New York: Cambridge University Press. ogy,Vol. 23 (pp. 125–165). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Denham, S.A., & Grout, L.A. (1992). Mothers’ emotional expressiveness and Hooley, J.M. (2004). Do psychiatric patients do better clinically if they live with
coping: Relations with preschoolers’ socio-emotional competence. Genetic, certain kinds of families? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13,
Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 118, 75–101. 202–205.
Dix, T. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: Adaptive and maladap- Isley, S.L., O’Neil, R., Clatfelter, D., & Parke, R.D. (1999). Parent and child
tative processes. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 3–25. expressed affect and children’s social competence: Modeling direct and
Dunsmore, J.C., & Halberstadt, A.G. (1997). How does family emotional expres- indirect pathways. Developmental Psychology, 35, 547–560.
siveness affect children’s schemas? In W. Damon (Series Ed.), & K.C. Barrett Jones, D.C., Abbey, B.B., & Cumberland, A. (1998). The development of display
(Vol. Ed.), New directions for child development. The communication of emotion: rule knowledge: Linkages with family expressiveness and social competence.
Current research from diverse perspectives (No. 77, pp. 45–68). San Francisco: Child Development, 69, 1209–1222.
Jossey-Bass Publishers. Lazarus, R.S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University
Dunsmore, J.C., Halberstadt, A.G., Eaton, K.L., & Robinson, M.L. (2005). Press.
Mothers’ typical and event-specific positive expressions influence children’s Levenson, R.W., Carstensen, L.L., Friesen, W.V., & Ekman, P. (1991). Emotion,
memory for events. Social Development, 14, 339–360. physiology, and expression in old age. Psychology and Aging, 6, 28–35.
Dunsmore, J.C., & Smallen, L.S. (2001). Parents’ expressiveness and young Maccoby, E.E., & Martin, J.A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family:
children’s emotion decoding with parents and unknown adults. Journal of Parent–child interactions. In P.H. Mussen (Series Ed.), & E.M. Hetherington
Genetic Psychology, 162, 478–494. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 4. Socialization, personality, and
Egan, S.K., & Perry, D.G. (1998). Does low self-regard invite victimization? social development (4th ed., pp. 1–101). New York: Wiley.
Developmental Psychology, 34, 299–309. Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self.
Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 35, 63–78.
Review of Psychology, 51, 665–697. Markus, H., Crane, M., Bernstein, S., & Siladi, M. (1982). Self-schemas and
Ferguson, T.J., & Stegge, H. (1995). Emotional states and traits in children: The gender. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 42, 38–50.
case of guilt and shame. In J.P. Tangney & K.W. Fischer (Eds.), Self-conscious Markus, H., Smith, J., & Moreland, R.L. (1985). Role of the self-concept in
emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment and pride (pp. 174–197). the perception of others. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 49,
New York: Guilford Press. 1494–1512.
Flannagan, D., & Perese, S. (1998). Emotional references in mother–daughter Marsh, H.W., & Craven, R.G. (2006). Reciprocal effects of self-concept and
and mother–son dyads’ conversations about school. Sex Roles, 39, 353–367. performance from a multidimensional perspective: Beyond seductive pleasure
Fong, G.T., & Markus, H. (1982). Self-schemas and judgments about others. and unidimensional perspectives. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1,
Social Cognition, 1, 191–204. 133–163.
Fredrickson, B.L. (2000). Extracting meaning from past affective experiences: Marsh, H.W., Craven, R., & Debus, R. (1998). Structure, stability and develop-
The importance of peaks, ends, and specific emotions. Cognition & Emotion, ment of young children’s self-concepts: A multicohort-multioccasion study.
14, 577–606. Child Development, 69, 1030–1053.
Fredrickson, B.L., & Kahneman, D. (1993). Duration neglect in retrospective McDowell, D.J., Kim, M., O’Neil, R., & Parke, R.D. (2002). Children’s
evaluations of affective episodes. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 65, emotional regulation and social competence in middle childhood: The role
45–55. of maternal and paternal interactive style. Marriage & Family Review, 34,
Froming, W.J., Nasby, W., & McManus, J. (1998). Prosocial self-schemas, self- 345–364.
awareness, and children’s prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality & Social Mead, G.H. (1934). Mind, self, and society from the standpoint of a social behaviorist.
Psychology, 75, 766–777. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Fuchs, D., & Thelen, M.H. (1988). Children’s expected interpersonal conse- Metts, S., Sprecher, S., & Cupach, W.R. (1991). Retrospective self-reports. In
quences of communicating their affective state and reported likelihood of B.M. Montgomery & S. Duck (Eds.), Studying interpersonal interaction
expression. Child Development, 59, 1314–1322. (pp. 162–178). New York: Guilford Press.
Garner, P.W. (1995) Toddlers’ emotion regulation behaviors: The roles of social Miller, P.J., Cho, G.E., & Bracey, J.R. (2005). Working class children’s experi-
context and family expressiveness. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 156, 417–430. ence through the prism of personal storytelling. Human Development, 48,
Gottman, J.M. (1994). What predicts divorce? The relationship between marital 115–135.
processes and marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Miller, P., & Sperry, L.L. (1987). The socialization of anger and aggression.
Greenberg, M.T., Lengua, L.J., Coie, J.D., Pinderhughes, E.E., & the Conduct Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 1–31.
Problems Prevention Research Group. (1999). Predicting developmental Mills, R.S.L., & Rubin, K.H. (1990). Parental beliefs about problematic social
outcomes at school entry using a multiple-risk model: Four American behaviors in early childhood. Child Development, 61, 138–151.
communities. Developmental Psychology, 35, 403–417. Montemayor, R., & Eisen, M. (1977). The development of self-conceptions from
Halberstadt, A.G., Cassidy, J., Stifter, C.A., Parke, R.D., & Fox, N.A. (1995). childhood to adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 13, 314–319.
Self-expressiveness within the family context: Psychometric support for a new Moretti, M.M., & Higgins, E.T. (1990). The development of self-system vulner-
measure. Psychological Assessment, 7, 93–103. abilities: Social and cognitive factors in developmental psychopathology. In
Halberstadt, A.G., Crisp, V.W., & Eaton, K.L. (1999). Family expressiveness: A R.J. Sternberg & J. Kolligian, Jr. (Eds.), Competence considered (pp. 286–314).
retrospective and new directions for research. In P. Philippot, R. Feldman, & New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
E. Coats (Eds.), Social context of nonverbal behavior (pp. 109–155). New York: Moretti, M.M., & Higgins, E.T. (1999). Own versus other standpoints in self-
Cambridge University Press. regulation: Developmental antecedents and functional consequences. Review
Halberstadt, A.G., Denham, S.A., & Dunsmore, J.C. (2001). Affective social of General Psychology, 3, 188–223.
competence. Social Development, 10, 79–119. Mosquera, P.M.R., Manstead, A.S.R., & Fischer, A.H. (2000). The role of
Halberstadt, A.G., & Eaton, K.L. (2003). A meta-analysis of family expressive- honor-related values in the elicitation, experience, and communication of
ness and children’s emotion expressiveness and understanding. Marriage & pride, shame, and anger: Spain and the Netherlands compared. Personality &
Family Review, 34, 35–62. Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 833–844.
Harter, S. (1990). Developmental differences in the nature of self-representa- Parke, R.D., & Buriel, R. (2006). Socialization in the family: Ethnic and eco-
tions: Implications for the understanding, assessment, and treatment of logical perspectives. In W. Damon & R.M. Lerner (Series Eds.), & N. Eisen-
maladaptive behavior. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 14, 113–142. berg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and
Harter, S. (1996). Developmental changes in self-understanding across the 5 to personality development (6th ed., pp. 429–504). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons.
7 shift. In A.J. Sameroff & M.M. Haith (Eds.), The five to seven year shift: Roopnarine, J.L. (2004). African-American and African-Caribbean fathers:
The age of reason and responsibility. The John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Levels, quality, and meaning of involvement. In M.E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of
Foundation series on mental heath and development (pp. 207–236). Chicago: the father in child development (4th ed., pp. 58–97). New York: Wiley & Sons.
University of Chicago Press. Roopnarine, J.L., Krishnakumar, A., Metindogan, A., & Evans, M. (2006). Links
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York: between parenting styles, parent–child academic interaction, and early
Guilford Press. academic skills and social behaviors of English-speaking Caribbean immi-
Higgins, E.T. (1989). Continuities and discontinuities in self-regulatory and self- grants. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21, 238–252.
evaluative processes: A developmental theory relating self and affect. Journal Ruble, D.N., & Dweck, C.S. (1995). Self-perceptions, person conceptions, and
of Personality, 57, 407–444. their development. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Social development. Review of person-
Higgins, E.T. (1991) Development of self-regulatory and self-evaluative ality and social psychology (15, pp. 109–139). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE
processes: Costs, benefits, and tradeoffs. In M.R. Gunnar & L.A. Sroufe Publications.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010


253-264 098025 Dunsmore (Q8D):210 x 280mm 03/04/2009 09:52 Page 264

264 DUNSMORE ET AL. / MOTHER’S EXPRESSIVE STYLE AND EMOTIONAL RESPONSES TO CHILDREN’S BEHAVIOR

Sroufe, L.A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R.A., Shepard, S.A., Cumberland, A., &
early years. New York: Cambridge University Press. Losoya, S.H. (2004). Prediction of children’s empathy-related responding
Stein, N.L., & Jewett, J.L. (1986). A conceptual analysis of the meaning of from their effortful control and parents’ expressivity. Developmental Psychology,
negative emotions: Implications for a theory of development. In C.E. Izard & 40, 911–926.
P.B. Read (Eds.), Measuring emotions in infants and children, Vol. 2. Cambridge Valiente, C., Fabes, R.A., Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T.L. (2004). The relations
studies in social and emotional development (pp. 238–267). New York: Cambridge of parental expressivity and support to children’s coping with daily stress.
University Press. Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 97–106.
Stern, D.N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis Wang, Q. (2004). The emergence of cultural self-constructs: Autobiographical
and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books. memory and self-description in European American and Chinese children.
Sui, J., & Zhu, Y. (2005). Five-year-olds can show the self-reference advantage. Developmental Psychology, 40, 3–15.
International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 382–387. Weinfield, N.S., Sroufe, A.L., Egeland, B., & Carlson, E.A. (1999). The nature
Sullivan, H.S. (1940). Conceptions of modern psychiatry. New York: Norton. of individual differences in infant–caregiver attachment. In J. Cassidy &
Sullivan, H.S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New York: Norton. P.R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical
Tangney, J.P. (1991). Moral affect: The good, the bad, and the ugly. Journal of applications (pp. 68–88). New York: Guilford Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 598–607. Wigfield, A., Eccles, J.S., Schiefele, U., Roesner, R.W., & Davis-Kean, P. (2006).
Turiel, E. (2006). The development of morality. In W. Damon & R.M. Lerner Development of achievement motivation. In W. Damon & R.M. Lerner (Series
(Series Eds.), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology:Vol. 3. Eds.), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social,
Social, Emotional, and Personality Development (6th ed., pp. 789–857). emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 933–1002). Hoboken, NJ:
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons. Wiley & Sons.

Downloaded from jbd.sagepub.com by Susana Guimarães on September 4, 2010

You might also like