You are on page 1of 8

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/13836229

How Sleep Deprivation Affects Psychological Variables Related to College


Students' Cognitive Performance

Article in Journal of American College Health · December 1997


DOI: 10.1080/07448489709595597 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

264 8,547

2 authors, including:

June J Pilcher
Clemson University
75 PUBLICATIONS 5,085 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by June J Pilcher on 10 May 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


This article was downloaded by: [Clemson University]
On: 30 August 2011, At: 06:03
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41
Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of American College Health


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vach20

How Sleep Deprivation Affects Psychological Variables


Related to College Students' Cognitive Performance
June J. Pilcher PhD & Amy S. Walters MA

Available online: 24 Mar 2010

To cite this article: June J. Pilcher PhD & Amy S. Walters MA (1997): How Sleep Deprivation Affects Psychological Variables Related to
College Students' Cognitive Performance, Journal of American College Health, 46:3, 121-126

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/07448489709595597

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction,
re-distribution, re-selling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly
forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be
complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be independently
verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or
costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the
use of this material.
How Sleep Deprivation Affects
Psychological Variables Related to College
Students' Cognitive Performance

June J. Pilcher, PhD, and Amy S. Walters, MA


Downloaded by [Clemson University] at 06:03 30 August 2011

Abstract. The effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive perfor- It is well established that sleep deprivation of 24 hours or
mance and on psychological variables related to cognitive perfor- more leads to noticeable decrements in performance lev-
mance were studied in 44 college students. Participants completed els.'.7 The psychological variables behind these decrements,
the Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal after either 24
hours of sleep deprivation or approximately 8 hours of sleep. After however, are less clear. One theory states that decreases in
completing the cognitive task, the participants completed 2 ques- performance are attributable to a decrease in the ability of
tionnaires, one assessing self-reported effort, concentration, and the sleep-deprived person to focus the attention and effort
estimated performance, the other assessing off-task cognitions. As necessary to complete the task Similarly, a
expected, sleep-deprived participants performed significantly number of early sleep-deprivation studies concluded that
worse than the nondeprived participants on the cognitive task.
However, the sleep-deprivedparticipants rated their concentration the detrimental effects of sleep loss on performance result
and effort higher than the nondeprived participants did. In addi- from periods of inattention called Iupl'sr.~."~ Moreover, one
tion, the sleep-deprived participants rated their estimated perfor- early study specifically concluded that sleep loss leads to a
mance significantly higher than the nondeprived participants did. decrease in attention to external stimuli.' None of the earli-
The findings indicate that college students are not aware of the ex- er studies, however, attempted to assess self-reported vari-
tent to which sleep deprivation negatively affects their ability to
complete cognitive tasks. ables that reflect changes in psychological events or
thoughts that may be associated with the observed decre-
Key Words: cognitive performance, psychological variables,
ments in performance.
self-reports, sleep deprivation
The effect of sleep deprivation on psychological vari-
ables associated with performance, such as self-reported es-

V
timates of attention, effort, and performance, have not been
oluntary sleep deprivation is a common occur-
thoroughly investigated. Few studies have examined per-
rence for many college students, who often par-
ceived effort and performance,' and the results from
tially deprive themselves of sleep during the week
those studies have often been contradictory. For example,
and compensate by increasing their sleep time over the
some researchers have suggested that sleep deprivation may
weekend.' This pattern of sleep deprivation and rebound be-
affect the willingness of the individual to put forth the effort
comes more pronounced around examination periods,
to perform well on a task more than the actual ability ofthe
sometimes resulting in 24 to 48 hours of total sleep depri-
individual to perf0rm.I
vation. By depriving themselves of sleep, college students
By contrast, other researchers have concluded that partic-
are not only increasing their feelings of sleepiness during
ipants may recognize their decreased performance levels
the day, thus decreasing their ability to pay attention in
following sleep deprivation and attempt to overcome this
class, but are also negatively affecting their ability to per-
decrease by increasing their effort.Is However, other studies
form on exams.
have shown that a perceived increase in effort does not ap-
pear to overcome the detrimental effects of sleep depriva-
tion. In one study,I3 the participants were given a reward for
June J. Pilcher ciiitl Amy S. Walters Lire with the Depciriinriit of better performance, which resulted in an increase in per-
Psychology ( i t Brridle!, Utiiwrsity iri Peoriu, Illiiiois. ceived effort but no change in actual performance. In addi-

VOL 46, NOVEMBER 1997 121


COLLEGE HEALTH

tion, studies have shown that increasing amounts of sleep tion that has not been thoroughly investigated is sleep de-
loss do not have a detrimental effect on participants’ self-re- privation.
ported motivation levels.14,’5As these results show, the rela- In sum, our current study addressed three specific issues.
tionships between sleep deprivation and psychological vari- First, does sleep loss lead to changes in self-reported levels
ables associated with performance are not clearly of psychological variables related to actual performance?
understood. As measures of psychological variables, we examined self-
Another method of examining psychological variables reported levels of concentration, effort, and estimated per-
that may be associated with the decrease in performance formance and self-reported off-task cognitions while the
following sleep deprivation is assessment of off-task cogni- participant completed a complex cognitive task. Because
tions. Off-task cognitions are thoughts that are not directed sleep deprivation increases feelings of sleepiness and fa-
to the completion of the task at hand but that intrude upon tigue, we expected the sleep-deprived individuals to report
concentration. These cognitions can include negative evalu- lower levels of concentration, effort, and estimated perfor-
ations of one’s performance on the task, such as “I don’t mance and higher levels of off-task cognitions if they were
know how to do this,” or completely unrelated thoughts, capable of accurately assessing these psychological vari-
such as “I wonder what I should have for lunch today.” Only ables.
one study to date has investigated the effect of sleep depri- The second aim of our study was to determine whether
vation on off-task cognitions,“’ but the participants in that sleep deprivation significantly alters mood states that may
study were specifically selected for their high baseline lev- be related to performance. As specific measures of mood,
Downloaded by [Clemson University] at 06:03 30 August 2011

els of off-task cognitions. Conclusions, therefore, could not we assessed feelings of tension, depression, anger, vigor, fa-
be drawn about the effect of sleep deprivation on off-task tigue, and confusion. On the basis of a previous study that
cognitions independent of baseline levels. used the same mood measures,?’ we expected sleep-de-
Sleep-deprived participants’ current mood state may pro- prived participants to report increased fatigue, confusion,
vide additional information about the ability of the individ- and tension and decreased vigor.
ual to perform following sleep deprivation. One of the best The final purpose of our current study was to determine
documented effects of sleep deprivation and one that would whether sleep deprivation alters peoples’ ability to make an
be expected to decrease complex task-solving ability is an accurate assessment of their concentration, effort, and esti-
increase in self-reported sleepiness and f a t i g ~ e . ’ ~ ’ . ~ ~ . ’ ’ mated performance. To investigate this aspect of sleep de-
Other specific mood states could also influence success- privation, we compared self-reported assessments with ac-
ful task completion. For example, if sleep deprivation has a tual performance levels.
consistent negative effect on tension or anxiety, sleep-de-
prived participants would be expected to have more diffi- METHOD
culty than nondeprived participants in maintaining the nec- Participants
essary attention and effort to complete a complex cognitive
task. Although several studies have reported that sleep de- We solicited study participants from five psychology
privation decreases positive mood states and increases neg- classes, two 100-level courses, one 200-level course, and
ative mood states,3,’4,1x,1y
relatively few studies have exam- two 400-level courses. Of the original 65 volunteers, 44 (26
ined the effect of sleep deprivation on specific mood states. women and 18 men) completed the study. The mean age of
Another important consideration is the effect of sleep de- the respondents, who were given extra credit points as an
privation on an individual’s ability to accurately assess psy- incentive to participate, was 20.5 years ( S D = 4.37).
chological variables, such as concentration, effort, and esti-
Materials
mated performance. Research findings have shown that the
accuracy of self-reports varies, depending upon experimen- We used the Watson-Glaser Critical Thinking Appraisal
tal characteristics surrounding the task. For example, John- (WG; The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX) to
son and colleagues*” found that participants’ self-reports of measure cognitive performance. We chose the WG because
the amount of effort they put into a task corresponded bet- it would be cognitively challenging and similar to normal
ter with performance on a difficult task than on a very easy testing conditions for college students in that it is a linguis-
task. The researchers also found that the amount of report- tic task that requires mental but no physical effort. The WG
ed effort, but not necessarily actual performance, could be contains three portions: inference, recognition of assump-
increased by giving an external incentive. tions, and deduction. To increase the similarity of the task
In addition, Beyer?’noted that self-evaluations of perfor- to normal testing conditions for college students, we ad-
mance on longer tasks are more accurate than self-evalua- ministered the test with a 40-minute time limit.
tions of performance on shorter tasks. Self-report estimates We used self-report scales to measure mood, off-task
of performance have also been shown to be altered by feed- cognitions, effort, concentration, and estimated perfor-
back on the accuracy of actual performance as the person mance. To assess current mood, we used the Profile of
completes the task.??These findings indicate that self-report Mood States (POMS; Educational and Industrial Testing
data on psychological variables can be manipulated by a va- Service, San Diego, CA). The POMS scale provides a list of
riety of experimental conditions. One experimental condi- 65 words describing current mood states (see Table 1). The

122 JACH
SLEEP DEPRIVATION

student participants rated each word based on their current ion to either a sleep-deprived ( n = 2 3 ) or a nondeprived
mood. group ( n = 21), were given the final set of instructions for
We assessed the number of off-task cognitions while the the experiment, and signed consent forms. In an effort to
participant completed the WG task, using the Cognitive In- create realistic sleep loss and nonsleep loss conditions lor
terference Questionnaire (CIQ).?' The ClQ provides a list of college students, we chose to limit the length of sleep de-
types of thoughts. The participants respond by stating how privation to 24 hours for the sleep-deprived group and to
often they experienced those thoughts while completing the allow the nodeprived group to sleep in their own beds
WG task. We developed a short psychological variables under normal sleeping conditions for approximately 8
questionnaire, using Likert-type scales ( I to 7), to measure hours.
self-reported estimates of effort, concentration, and estimat- After the meeting at the sleep laboratory on the Friday
ed performance. In the written instructions for the question- night of the experiment. the members of the nondeprived
naire, participants were told to respond to the questions in group were told to go home and sleep approximately 8
relation to the WG task. A complete copy of the psycholog- hours. They were instructed to go to bed between 1 I I'M and
ical variables questionnaire is available from the author on 1 A M and to get out of bed between 7 A M and 9 A M on Sat-
request. Higher numbers on each of the self-report variables urday morning. The nondeprived participants were called at
represent a greater frequency of that variable. For example, 9 A M on Saturday morning to ensure that they were awake,
higher numbers on the estimated performance scale indicate and they were encouraged to eat breakfast before reporting
a higher level of estimated performance. to the testing site at 10 A M .
Downloaded by [Clemson University] at 06:03 30 August 2011

The sleep-deprived group remained awake under the su-


Procedures pervision of t w o research assistants in the sleep laboratory.
The experiment began at 10 PM on a Friday night and Participants interacted with each other and with the re-
concluded at 1 I A M the next morning. Approximately 8 par- search assistants, watched movies, played video and board
ticipants were tested each Friday night. All participants games, or worked on personal projects during the night.
were requested in advance not to drink alcoholic beverages They were allowed to bring food to eat during the night,
or take nonprescription drugs from 10 PM on Thursday night but were asked to limit caffeinated beverages and sugary
until the conclusion of the experiment. In addition, we snacks to two of each. Sleep-deprived participants were es-
asked all participants to get out of bed between 7 AM and 9 corted to a restaurant for breakfast at about 8 AM on Satur-
A M on Friday morning and not to nap during the day. day morning. After breakfast, they were escorted to the
The experiment commenced with all participants report- testing area at 10:00 AM.
ing to the sleep laboratory at 10 PM on Friday night. At that Testing took place at the university library in an isolated
time, the students were randomly assigned in a block fash- room of study cubicles. with one person per cubicle. To as-

TABLE 1
Examples of Self-Report Scale Used in Study of Sleep Deprivation

Test/question Responseslscale

Profile of Mood Status


I . Friendly Not ut till (0) to crtemely (4)
2. Tense Not at till ( 0 )to extemely (4)
3. Angry Not (it till (0) to estemely (4)
Cognitive Interference Questionnaire
I , I thought about how poorly I was doing Never ( I ) to very ofien ( 5 )
2. I thought about what the experiementer
would think of me. Never ( 1 ) to very ($en ( 5 )
3. I thought about other activities (eg,
assignments, work). Never ( 1 ) to very ofien ( 5 )
Psychological Variables Questionnaire
I . How well were you able to concentrate
o n the task'? Not at till (I) to exteniely well (7)
2. How well do you think you performed
on this task? Poorly ( I ) to extenwly well (7)
3. How much effort did this task take'? Very little ( 1 ) to v e r y much (7)
~~ ~ ~~~~~~

Note. These are examples of the types of questions to which participants wcre asked to respond

VOL 46, NOVEMBER 1997 123


COLLEGE HEALTH

sess their compliance with instructions, we asked the par- multiple analysis of variance (MANOVA), by sleep condi-
ticipants to complete a short questionnaire that included tion, on all variables.
questions on sleep times and items consumed since Thurs-
day night. All participants then completed the POMS scale, RESULTS
followed by the WG. After finishing the WG, all of the par- All of the student participants reported that they slept ap-
ticipants completed the questionnaire assessing self-report- proximately 8 hours on Thursday night. The sleep-deprived
ed effort, concentration, and estimated performance in rela- participants reported sleeping an average of 7.9 I hours ( S D
tion to the WG. The last 18 participants in each of the = 1.26), whereas nondeprived participants reported sleeping
groups also filled out the ClQ. The entire testing period an average of 7.79 hours ( S D = 0.69). The wake-up times on
took less than 1 hour. Friday morning were very similar for both groups. The de-
prived group reported a mean time of getting out of bed of
Data Analyses 8:55 AM ( S D = I .22 hours), and the nondeprived group re-
The data from the POMS, WG, and CIQ were initially ported a mean time of getting out of bed time of 8:30 AM.
scored according to the directions given for each measure. ( S D = I . 10 hours).
We calculated six POMS scores (tension-anxiety, depres- On Friday night, nondeprived participants reported sleep-
sion-dejection, anger-hostility, vigor, fatigue, and confu- ing an average of 7.92 hours ( S D = 0.5 1 hours) and a mean
sion-bewilderment), one WG score representing the perfor- time of getting out of bed on Saturday morning of 8:40 AM
mance percentile of the individual in relation to other ( S D = 0.73 hours). Two participants, one in each sleep con-
Downloaded by [Clemson University] at 06:03 30 August 2011

college students, and three CIQ scores (off-task cognitions dition, reported taking a nap of less than 30 minutes on Fri-
relevant to task, off-task cognitions irrelevant to task, and day. We analyzed the data both with and without the two
general mind wandering). We derived self-reported effort, napping participants included. Because the results from the
concentration, and estimated performance from the ques- two analyses were very similar, we report the results from
tions on the psychological variables questionnaire. We av- all participants. None of the participants reported using al-
eraged self-reported sleep data for the sleep-deprived and cohol or nonprescription drugs (except for acetaminophen)
the nondeprived groups separately, by group, for Thursday between 10 PM on Thursday and 10 AM on Saturday.
and Friday nights. For means and standard deviations on the WG and the
All statistical analyses were completed on SAS (SAS In- self-report tasks, see Table 2. As expected, the sleep-de-
stitute, Cary, NC). To assess whether sleep deprivation had prived participants performed significantly worse on the
an effect on actual performance and self-reported estimates WG than the nondeprived participants did, F( I , 42) = 4.02,
of psychological variables and mood states, we performed p < .05.

TABLE 2
Means and Standard Deviations of Sleep and
Nondeprived Participant Groups

Sleep deprived Nondeprived


Variables M SII M sn

Watson-Glaser 24.52 2 I .29 38.71 25.63"


Cognitive Interference Questionnnaire
Distracting task-relevant thoughts 2.36 0.62 2.22 0.53
Distracting task-irrelevant thoughts I .59 0.70 I .5x 0.58
General mind wandering 4.17 I .92 3.72 I .60
Estimated effort 4.03 I .00 3.41 0.70*
Estimated concentration 4.30 1.66 3.28 1.31"
Estimated performance 4.54 I .36 3.36 0.84***
Profile of Mood States
Tensionhnxiety 14.22 7.30 11.19 8.05
Depression/dejection 11.96 12.08 9.86 10.22
Anger/hostility 11.65 9.00 8.00 7.46
Vigor 16.87 6.90 17.86 6.06
Fatigue 12.35 6.80 7.95 5.88*
Confusionlbewilderment 10.65 5.22 5.95 4.10**

Nore. Significant differences between groups: * p < .OS; * * p < .01; ***/I < ,001.

124 JACH
SLEEP DEPRIVATION

Although we expected that sleep-deprived participants recognized internal effort. In turn, this could have led the
would have more difficulty concentrating on the task and, sleep-deprived participants to believe that they were ex-
thus. would show an increase in off-task cognitions, none of pending more effort than they actually were, which may
the CIQ scales was significantly increased in the sleep-de- also have led to increases in estimated performance and
prived group. Furthermore, instead of the expected decrease self-rated concentration. Regardless of the mechanism be-
in self-reported concentration, as measured by the psycho- hind the self-report data, the results indicated that our sleep-
logical variables questionnaire, the sleep-deprived partici- deprived participants did not realize the extent to which
pants reported higher subjective levels of concentration their own performances were affected by sleep loss, and
while completing the task than the nondeprived participants they appeared to be making incorrect assumptions about
did, F( I , 42) = 5.03, p < .OS. their ability to concentrate and to provide the necessary ef-
The sleep-deprived participants also estimated that they fort to complete the task.
expended signifiantly more effort to complete the task than Interestingly, sleep deprivation did not result in the ex-
did the nondeprived participants. F( I , 42) = 5.49, p < .OS. pected change in reporting off-task cognitions. Although a
Interestingly, although sleep-deprived participants actually previous study"' found that participants who habitually re-
performed worse on the WG than the nondeprived partici- ported distracting thoughts were more likely to do so when
pants, the students deprived of sleep reported significantly deprived of sleep, it appears that the effect of sleep depriva-
higher levels of estimated performance than the nonde- tion on off-task cognitions depends on whether the sleep-
prived participants did, F( I , 42) = 1 1.79, p < .001. deprived person regularly experiences high levels of off-
Downloaded by [Clemson University] at 06:03 30 August 2011

The sleep-deprived participants reported higher levels on task cognitions. Therefore, reporting off-task cognitions
five of the six POMS scales, but only the increases in the fa- does not appear to be specifically affected by sleep depriva-
tigue and confusion scales were significant: fatigue, F( I , tion, independent of baseline levels.
42) = 5.21, p < .OS: confusion, F( I , 42) = 10.88,p < .01. A second major finding of this research is that sleep de-
privation differentially affected mood states in these college
DISCUSSION students. The current findings indicate that sleep depriva-
As we expected, the results from our current study indi- tion significantly affected only the fatigue and confusion
cated that participants who were deprived of sleep for 24 subscales on the POMS. The reported increase in fatigue
hours performed significantly worse on a complex cognitive and confusion could have contributed to the significant de-
task than nondeprived participants. Although they actually crease in actual performance that we observed in the sleep-
performed worse, the sleep-deprived participants reported deprived student participants. It is interesting to note that
significantly higher levels of estimated performance, as none of the remaining POMS subscales changed signifi-
well as more effort expended on the cognitive task, than the cantly i n the sleep-deprived participants, indicating that
nondeprived participants did. In addition, sleep-deprived some mood changes commonly ascribed to sleep depriva-
participants reported a significantly higher level of self- tion, such as anger, irritability, and anxiety, were not neces-
rated concentration than nondeprived participants did. We sarily products of 24 hours of sleep loss.
found no significant differences in levels of off-task cogni- The current tindings on mood states are very similar to
tions between the sleep-deprived and nondeprived groups. those reported by Dinges and colleagues.'3 Sleep-deprived
The apparent contradiction between the self-reported participants in both studies reported significantly more fa-
data on effort, concentration, and estimated performance tigue and confusion than nondeprived participants. Dinges
and the actual performance level of sleep-deprived partici- and colleagues reported significantly more tension and sig-
pants is somewhat surprising. I t is unlikely that the dis- nificantly less vigor in sleep-deprived participants.
agreement between the self-reported variables and actual Similarly, we noted a trend for more tension and less
performance was a result of the type of task used. The Wat- vigor in the sleep-deprived participants in our study. The
son-Glaser task should have provided a suitable scenario for most likely reason for the small differences between the two
accurately assessing psychological variables because more studies is that Dinges and colleagues collected mood data
difficult and longer tasks have been shown to result in more every 2 hours for a 64-hour sleep-deprivation period,
accurate self-estimates of both effort and whereas we collected mood data only once-immediately
Several explanations for the disagreement between the before the students' completion of the cognitive task. Fur-
self-report data and the actual performance levels are possi- thermore, neither study reported a significant increase in
ble. Sleep-deprived participants may have expended more angry or depressed feelings following sleep deprivation, in-
effort to complete the task, but the effort was not sufficient dicating that sleep deprivation does not necessarily increase
to overcome the performance decrements caused by being reports of anger and depression, as is commonly believed.
deprived of sleep. Furthermore, the increase in effort could In sum, our findings suggest that college students are not
have led the sleep-deprived participants to believe that they aware of the extent to which sleep deprivation impairs their
were performing better and concentrating more than they ability to complete cognitive tasks successfully because
actually were. they consistently overrate their concentration and effort, as
An alternative explanation is that sleep deprivation may well as their estimated performance. In addition. the current
have negatively affected the degree to which participants data suggest that 24 hours of sleep deprivation significantly

VOL 46, NOVEMBER 1997 125


COLLEGE HEALTH

affects only fatigue and confusion and does not have a more 16. Angus RG, Heslegrave RJ. Effects of sleep loss on sus-
general effect on positive or negative mood states. The prac- tained cognitive performance during a command and control sim-
ulation. Behuv Res Merhods Imtruments Computers. 1985; 17:
tical implication of these findings is that many college stu-
55-67.
dents are unknowingly sabotaging their own performance 17. Linde L, Bergstrom M. The effect of one night without
by choosing to deprive themselves of sleep before they sleep on problem-solving and immediate recall. P
complete complex cognitive tasks. 199234: 127- 136.
18. Brendel DH, Reynolds CF 111, Jennings JR, et al. Sleep
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS stage physiology, mood, and vigilance responses to total sleep de-
privation in healthy 80-year-olds and 20-year-olds. Psvchophysi-
The authors wish to thank Elizabeth S. Ott for her assistance in
ologv. 1990;27:677-686.
the initial data analyses. This research was supported by a Bradley
19. Leung L, Becker CE. Sleep deprivation and house staff per-
University Research Excellence Committee Award to June J.
formance: Update. J Occup Mad. 1992;34: 1 153-1 160.
Pilcher.
20. Johnson NE, Saccuzzo DP, Larson GE. Self-reported effort
For a copy of the psychological variables questionnaire. please
versus actual perforniance in information processing paradigms. J
write June J. Pilcher, PhD, Department of Psychology, Bradley
Gen Psychol. 1995;122(2): 195-210.
University. Peoria, IL 6 1625.
2 1. Beyer S. Gender differences in the accuracy of self-evalua-
tions of performance. J Pers SOL.Psycho/. 1990;59(5):960-970.
REFERENCES 22. Critchtield TS. Bias in self-evaluation: Signal probability
1. Hawkins J, Shaw P. Self-reported sleep quality in college effects. J Exp Anul Behuv. 1994;62:235-250.
students: A repeated measures approach. Sleep. 1992; 15(6): 23. Dinges DF, Gillen KA, Powell JW, et al. Mood reports dur-
545-549. ing total and partial sleep deprivation: Is anger inevitable‘? Sleep
Downloaded by [Clemson University] at 06:03 30 August 2011

2. Dinges DF. The nature of sleepiness: Causes, contexts, and Res. 1995;24:44 I .
consequences. In: Enring. Sleeping, rind Sex Stunkard A, Baum A, 24. Sarason IG, Sarason B. Keefe D, Hayes B, Shearin EN.
eds. Hillsdale. NJ: Erlbaum; 1988. Cognitive interference: Situational determinants and traitlike char-
3. Pilcher JJ, Huffcutt AI. Effects of sleep deprivation on per- acteristics. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;51 : 2 15-226.
formance: A meta-analysis. Sleep. 1996;I9(4):3 18-326.
4. Johnson LC. Sleep deprivation and performance. In: Webb
WW. ed. Biological RhJrhms. Sleep. c i n d Petjbrmcince. New York:
Wiley; 1982.
5. Meddis R. Cognitive dysfunction following loss of sleep.
In: Burton E, ed. The Puthology and Psvchology of’ Cognition. The UMI Article Clearinghouse offers articles from
London: Methuen; 1982. more than 11,000 copyright-cleared periodicals in a
6. Williams HL. Lubin A. Speeded addition and sleep loss. J wide range of subjects. You can place your orders
hol. 1967;73:313-317. electronically, as well as by phone, mail, and tele-
7. Elkin AL, Murray DJ. The effects of sleep loss on short- facsimile. For more information, please complete
term recognition memory. Cun J Psychol. I974;28: 192-1923, and mail this coupon to UMI Article Clearinghouse,
8. Polzella DJ. Effects of sleep-deprivation on short-term 300 North Zeeb Road, Box 11, Ann Arbor, MI 48106
memory and recognition. J Exp Psycliol. 197.5;104:194-200. USA. Or call toll-free for an immediate response:
9. Hockey GRJ. Changes in attention allocation in a multi- 800-521-0600. From Alaska and Michigan call COI-
component task under loss of sleep. Br J Psychol. 1970:61(4): lect 313-761-4700. From Canada, call toll-free
473480. 800-343-5299.
10. Mikulincer M, Babkoff H, Caspy T, Weiss H. The impact of
cognitive interference on performance during prolonged sleep
loss. P.sjc.hol Rex 1990;52:80-86. YES! I’d like to know more about
1 1. Kjellberg A. Sleep deprivation and some aspects of perfor-
mance. Wrikirig Sleeping. 1977: 1 : 139-1 54. UMI Article Clearinghouse.
12. Horne JA. Why We Sleep. New York: Oxford University
Press; 1988.
13. Horne JA. Pettitt AN. High incentive effects on vigilance Name
performance during 72 hours of total sleep deprivation. Acru Psy- Title
chologicti. 1985;SX: 123-1 39. Company/lnstitution
14. Mikulincer M, Babkoff H, Caspy T, Sing H. The effects of
72 hours of sleep loss on psychological variables. Br J Psychol. Address
1989;XO:145-162. City/State/Zip
1.5. Dinges DF. Kribbs NB. Steinberg KN. Powell JW. Do we Telephone1-(
lose the willingness to perform during sleep deprivation? Sleep
Res. 1992;2 I :3 18.

126 JACH

View publication stats

You might also like