You are on page 1of 11

DEPRESSION AND ANXIETY 20:59–69 (2004)

Research Article
ACTUAL AND PERCEIVED MEMORY DEFICITS IN
INDIVIDUALS WITH COMPULSIVE HOARDING
Tamara L. Hartl, Ph.D.,1 Randy O. Frost, Ph.D.,2 George J. Allen, Ph.D.,1 Thilo Deckersbach, Ph.D.,2
Gail Steketee, Ph.D.,3 Shannon R. Duffany,1 and Cary R. Savage, Ph.D.4

Memory problems have been hypothesized to underlie compulsive hoarding


behavior [Frost and Hartl, 1996: Behav Res Ther 34:341–350]. This study
examined memory performance, memory confidence, and memory beliefs in 22
individuals with severe hoarding symptoms and 24 matched normal control
subjects. Participants were administered two measures of learning and memory
that required strategic planning and organization for successful performance:
the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (RCFT) and California Verbal
Learning Test (CVLT). Self-reports of memory confidence, perceived con-
sequences of forgetting, importance of remembering, and need to keep
possessions in sight also were assessed. In comparison to controls, participants
with compulsive hoarding recalled less information on delayed recall of the
RCFT and CVLT and used less effective organizational strategies on the RCFT
but not the CVLT. Hoarders also reported significantly less confidence in
their memory, more catastrophic assessments of the consequences of forgetting,
and a stronger desire to keep possessions in sight. Results provide initial
evidence of learning and memory impairment and poor memory confidence in
subjects with compulsive hoarding. Depression and Anxiety 20:59–69, 2004.
& 2004 Wiley-Liss, Inc.

Key words: hoarding; obsessive-compulsive disorder; memory; neuropsychology;


organizational strategies

1
Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs,
Connecticut
INTRODUCTION 2
Department of Psychology, Smith College, Northampton,
Clinical investigators have become increasingly inter- Massachusetts
3
Boston University School of Social Work, Boston University,
ested in the phenomenon of compulsive hoarding.
Boston, Massachusetts
Although not clearly delineated in the Diagnostic and 4
Hoglund Brain Imaging Center, Kansas University Medical
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV [American
Center, Kansas City, Kansas
Psychiatric Association, 1994], Frost and Hartl [1996]
proposed the following definition for compulsive This research was presented at the Association for the Advance-
hoarding: 1) the acquisition of, and failure to discard, ment of Behavior Therapy, November 1999, Toronto, Canada.
a large number of possessions that seem to be useless or Contract grant sponsor: Obsessive–Compulsive Foundation;
of limited value, 2) living spaces sufficiently cluttered Contract grant sponsor: David Judah Research Fund.
so as to preclude activities for which those spaces were
designed, and 3) significant distress or impairment in *Correspondence to: Cary R. Savage, Ph.D., Hoglund Brain
Imaging Center, Mail Stop 1052, Kansas University Medical
functioning caused by the hoarding. Hoarding beha-
Center, 3901 Rainbow Blvd., Kansas City, KS 66160.
vior has been described in a number of clinical E-mail: csavage@kumc.edu
disorders including anorexia nervosa [Frankenburg,
1984], schizophrenia [Greenberg et al., 1990], and Received for publication 15 August 2003; Accepted 25 January
dementia [Hwang et al., 1998]; however, it is most 2004
often associated with obsessive-compulsive disorder DOI: 10.1002/da.20010
(OCD). Hoarding symptoms are present in approxi- Published online 9 June 2004 in Wiley InterScience (www.
mately 18–30% of cases with OCD [Frost et al., 1996; interscience.wiley.com).

& 2004 WILEY-LISS, INC.


60 Hartl et al.

Rasmussen and Eisen, 1992] and share several of its capacity to remember is so poor that they cannot recall
defining clinical features, including indecisiveness information or personal events without retaining some
[Frost and Gross, 1993], perceptions of responsibility hard copy or visual reminder of the event. Despite
[Frost et al., 1995], and perfectionism [Frost and Gross, these perceived memory deficits among hoarders, there
1993]. have been no neuropsychological studies of memory
Despite these similarities with OCD, hoarding has performance in this group. The question remains
consistently emerged in factor analytic investigations as whether hoarders suffer from actual memory deficits.
a separate symptom factor, distinct from other ob-
sessive–compulsive characteristics such as checking,
symmetry, and cleanliness concerns [Baer, 1994; Leck- LEARNING AND MEMORY DEFICITS IN
man et al., 1997; Summerfeldt et al., 1999]. Similarly, a INDIVIDUALS WITH OCD
cluster analysis of OCD symptoms by Calamari et al. An extensive literature has examined memory
[1999] indicated that hoarding symptoms comprise an deficits in individuals with OCD. Several studies failed
independent subgroup. There also is evidence that to find evidence of memory deficits associated with
hoarding symptoms predict poorer prognosis in treat- OCD on measures of verbal memory [e.g., Abbruzzese
ment outcome studies of OCD, including treatments et al., 1993; Dirson et al., 1995], word recognition [Foa
with paroxetine or cognitive behavioral treatment et al., 1997], word recall [MacDonald et al., 1997;
[Black et al., 1998], and other selective serotonin Zielinski et al., 1991], working memory [Martin et al.,
reuptake inhibitors (SSRI) [Mataix-Cols et al., 1999; 1995], and recall of ‘‘safe’’ versus neutral objects [Tolin
Winsberg et al., 1999]. These findings have led et al., 1999]. OCD washers and checkers have also
researchers to suggest that the neurobiological features shown some indication of having a positive memory
underlying hoarding may be distinct from those of bias toward remembering threat-relevant information
other subgroups of individuals with OCD [Black et al., [Radomsky et al., 2001; Radomsky and Rachman,
1998; Winsberg et al., 1999]. 1999].
Other research does provide evidence for learning
and memory problems in clinical and subclinical OC
ORGANIZATION DEFICITS AND MEMORY
groups [Christensen et al., 1992], in particular deficits
BELIEFS AMONG HOARDERS related to spatial working memory and spatial recogni-
Frost and Hartl [1996] proposed a cognitive- tion [Purcell et al., 1998a,b], and memory for actions
behavioral model of hoarding, defining it as a multi- [Sher et al., 1984, 1989]. In response to these mixed
faceted behavior involving information processing outcomes, Savage [1998] suggested that memory
deficits and behavioral avoidance, as well as a tendency measures stressing organizational planning skills may
to form strong emotional attachments to objects and be most sensitive to memory dif ferences in individuals
have erroneous beliefs about the nature of possessions. with OCD. Such problems would be similar to findings
In this model, memory disturbance is believed to be an in neurologic groups with frontal-striatal neural system
essential deficit underlying hoarding behavior. Indivi- dysfunction [Savage, 1997] and, therefore, consistent
duals with hoarding report certain beliefs about their with neurobiological models of OCD [Rauch and
memory ability, including poor memory confidence, Baxter, 1998].
over-estimating the importance of remembering pos- Several studies have now shown learning and
sessions, exaggerating the consequences of forgetting, memory deficits among OCD patients when memory
and preferring to keep possessions in sight to serve as assessments include an organizational component
visual cues [Frost and Hartl, 1996]. Whether these [Deckersbach et al., 2000a; Martinot et al., 1990;
beliefs are actually more salient for hoarders compared Savage et al., 1999, 2000]. Evidence suggests that
to nonhoarders has not yet been investigated. memory impairment among OCD patients is statisti-
The relevance of hoarders’ poor memory confidence cally mediated by poor organizational strategies during
is reflected in four classes of difficulty observed in encoding [Savage et al., 1999, 2000]. This seems to be
association with their patterns of saving [Frost and true for both verbal and nonverbal tests, provided they
Hartl, 1996]. First, hoarders often have dif ficulty adequately stress organizational capacities. Secondary
selecting individual categories when organizing posses- to strategic impairments, OCD patients may have poor
sions, fearing that they will forget assigned file head- representations of episodes during encoding, yet
ings. Second, hoarders seem to over-emphasize the correctly recognize items that had been presented.
importance of remembering information and posses- Savage et al. [1996, 2000] have demonstrated that
sions, often reporting that seemingly insignificant OCD patients tend to perform as well as controls on
phone numbers or newspaper articles are ‘‘crucial to recognition measures of verbal and visual memory, but
remember.’’ Third, hoarders reportedly believe that fare less well on tasks of free recall.
serious negative consequences may result from forget- There is evidence that patients with OCD show
ting information. Fourth, hoarders often report a abnormal learning and memory, particularly in condi-
preference to keep possessions in sight so as not to tions that are ambiguous or complex and require
forget their existence. Many hoarders report that their organizational planning. These problems may also be
Research Article: Hoarding and Memory Deficits 61

present in individuals with compulsive hoarding. clutter was not a defining feature of their saving
Although there have been several recent investigations behavior or if they met criteria for substance abuse or
into the phenomenology of hoarding behavior, none dependence. Control participants were excluded if they
have examined memory functioning. To our knowl- met SCID criteria for an Axis I disorder or if they were
edge, this is the first neuropsychological investigation taking any psychoactive medication. The final sample
of a sample of individuals with hoarding symptoms as consisted of 22 people with compulsive hoarding
their primary concern. It was hypothesized that symptoms and 24 healthy control participants.
hoarders, relative to controls, would perform more Despite the fact that the hoarding sample on average
poorly on measures of verbal and visual memory that manifested clinically dysfunctional obsessive-compul-
include an organizational component and report sive features, only four of the participants met SCID
significantly poorer confidence in their memory. In criteria for OCD. Twelve participants in the hoarding
addition, based on Frost and Hartl’s [1996] model, sample had at least one current Axis I diagnosis and six
hoarders are predicted to 1) over-emphasize the had more than one Axis I condition, broken down as
importance of remembering possessions and informa- follows: major depressive episode (n ¼ 3), obsessive-
tion, 2) report keeping possessions in sight to serve as compulsive disorder (n ¼ 4), post-traumatic stress
visual cues significantly more, and 3) associate more disorder (n ¼ 4), dysthymia (n ¼ 2), panic disorder
severe negative consequences with forgetting informa- (n ¼ 3), agoraphobia (n ¼ 1), social phobia (n ¼ 1),
tion relative to controls. specific phobia (n ¼ 5), and generalized anxiety dis-
order (n ¼ 5). Six had more than one Axis I condition.

PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS SAMPLE CHARACTERISTICS


PARTICIPANTS The SCID was conducted by trained interviewers to
Hoarding participants were solicited from self-help determine the diagnoses of hoarding participants and
groups in Connecticut or referred by clinicians at the to verify the absence of Axis I disorders in the control
OCD Clinic and Research Unit at the Massachusetts sample. Participants were matched according to
General Hospital. Control participants were solicited gender, handedness (as verified with the Edinburgh
by advertisements in a state university and in the Handedness Inventory; Oldfield, 1971), age, education,
surrounding community. This study was approved by and estimated verbal IQ (VIQ; estimated using short
the Subcommittee for Human Studies at MGH and the forms of the Information, Vocabulary, and Similarities
University of Connecticut. All participants gave written subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-
consent after a full explanation of procedures. Three Revised; WAIS-R). Independent sample t-tests and w2
criteria were used to dif ferentiate hoarders from non- analyses showed no significant dif ferences between
hoarders. First, participants were screened with the groups on these variables (Table 1).
Saving Behavior Scale (SBS) [Frost et al., 1999] to Participants also completed the Yale-Brown Obses-
ensure that the two groups were statistically differ- sive-Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS) [Goodman et al.,
entiated on this index of hoarding behavior. SBS total 1989], the revised Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II)
scores ranged from 3–31 in the control group and from [Beck and Steer, 1987] and the Beck Anxiety Inventory
38–100 in the hoarding sample. Second, participants (BAI) [Beck et al., 1988]. Hoarders scored significantly
were included in the hoarding sample, and excluded higher on these measures, indicating greater psycho-
from the control sample, if they reported severe distress pathology than in the control sample. Demographic
or interference in quality of life as a function of saving and clinical characteristics for the sample are summar-
or cluttering behaviors. Third, all participants con- ized in Table 1.
sented to a home visit to verify self-reported saving and
cluttering behaviors among hoarding participants and PROCEDURE
normal clutter among control participants. Three Participants were first administered the SCID,
hoarding participants were retained whose SBS scores followed by the neuropsychological measures and the
were extremely high and who did not allow a home visit WAIS subscales in counterbalanced order across
because of reported embarrassment over the conditions participants. Self-reports were completed either before
of their homes. or immediately after the assessment session.
To reduce the possibility of influences on memory
performance other than group status, potential parti-
cipants were excluded if they 1) reported any psychotic NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL MEASURES
symptoms during the Structured Clinical Interview for Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (RCFT)
DSM-IV Disorders (SCID) [First et al., 1995], 2) [Osterrieth, 1944]. Participants were given the RCFT
reported ever having been diagnosed with bipolar figure and a blank piece of paper and asked to copy the
disorder, or 3) had a history of epilepsy or any other figure as accurately as possible (copy condition). After
seizure disorder. In addition, participants for the completing the copy condition, the figure was removed
hoarding sample were excluded if they indicated that and participants were instructed to redraw the figure
62 Hartl et al.

TABLE 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of hoarding and control samples

Hoarders (n ¼ 22)a Controls (n ¼ 24)a t


Female (%) 81.8 83.3 
Right-handed (%) 95.0 96.0 
Age (yr) 52.73 (8.76) 50.38 (6.11) 1.06
Education (yr) 16.73 (2.75) 17.21 (2.64) 0.61
Estimated VIQ 125.23 (14.90) 124.71 (17.12) 0.11
Y-BOCS 9.59 (9.52) 1.08 (1.79) 4.30*
Beck Depression Inventory II 13.06 (13.08) 2.08 (2.64) 4.03**
Beck Anxiety Inventory 9.05 (10.75) 1.33 (2.08) 3.45**
a
Values are expressed as meand (sd), unless otherwise indicated.
*Po.001.
**Po.01.

from memory, without intervening distraction (im- presented for 5 trials, with a recall score assessed after
mediate recall condition). After a 20-min delay, each trial, and a learning score assessed by adding trials
participants were again asked to redraw the figure 1–5. After a 20-min delay, participants are asked to
from memory (delayed recall condition). During each recall as many words as possible and then discriminate
of the three conditions, the examiner gave the between items on the original list and a set of
participant a predetermined sequence of differently distracters, for which recognition discriminability
colored pencils every 10–15 s to assess organization of scores were calculated.
the drawing. After completing the delayed recall Verbal strategy scores were based on a semantic
condition, participants were given a recognition task clustering index that measured the extent to which
[Meyers and Meyers, 1995] in which they were asked to participants grouped words into semantic categories
discern which smaller pieces on several sheets of paper during recall over the 5 learning trials. Participants
were part of the larger Rey figure. Scoring for received points toward the semantic clustering score
recognition was based on the total number of correctly when they recalled words from the same category in
identified elements. succession. Scoring followed standard protocols [Delis
The RCFT drawings were scored for both accuracy et al., 1987], and corrections were made to account for
and organization. To score copy accuracy, 24 individual an increase in semantic clustering points by chance
segments of the figure were identified, each having with an increase in the number of words recalled.
three criteria. It was possible to earn 0–3 points for We were interested in the following CVLT mea-
each segment to achieve a total possible accuracy score sures: Trial 1 and Trial 5 free recall, learning over Trials
of 72 [Denman, 1984]. Scoring for organization 1–5, short- and long-delay free recall, semantic
involved a quantitative analysis of the construction of clustering (averaged over Trials 1–5), and long-delay
five configural elements (base rectangle, two diagonals, recognition discriminability [1((False PositivesþFalse
vertical midline, horizontal midline, and the right-side Negatives)/32)]. In addition, two contrast measures
triangle vertex) [Deckersbach et al., 2000c; Savage et were calculated to evaluate retention rates: 1) percen-
al., 1999]. Participants received points for constructing tage recall, Trial 5 to short-delay ¼ short-delay/trial
each element as an unfragmented unit. The order of 5  100), and 2) percentage recall, short-delay to long-
construction did not af fect the organization score. The delay ¼ long-delay/short-delay  100.
largest configural element, the base rectangle, was
assigned two points, and each of the other components
was assigned one point. Participants could therefore SELF-REPORT CLINICAL MEASURES
score between 0–6 points on organization. Reliability Saving Behavior Scale. The Saving Behavior Scale
for half of the sample’s organizational scores was (SBS) [Frost et al., 1999] is a 28-item self-report
verified via three independent scorers. Correlations measure of various aspects of hoarding behavior,
between raters ranged from .93–.98. including clutter, distress, avoidance, and saving.
California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) [Delis et Responses are made on a scale of 0–4 with 4 indicating
al., 1987]. The CVLT is a well-established measure of maximum distress or severity. Earlier versions of the
verbal learning and memory consisting of a list of 16 SBS have been found to be both reliable and valid
shopping items that can be grouped into four [Frost and Gross, 1993; Frost et al., 1995]; the internal
categories (spices, tools, clothing, and fruits). The list consistency in this sample for the SBS was extremely
is presented so that no two words from the same high (Cronbach coefficient a ¼ .98). Four exploratory
category occur in sequence and participants are not measures were developed to assess beliefs in memory
informed of the semantic structure. The list is hypothesized by Frost and Hartl [1996].
Research Article: Hoarding and Memory Deficits 63

Memory Reliability Questionnaire. The Memory Importance of Remembering. The Importance of


Reliability Questionnaire (MRQ) [Frost and Longo, Remembering (IOR) is a 20-item scale assessing the
1996] is a 22-item scale designed to assess self-reported importance placed on remembering possessions and
confidence in memory and the reliance on memory aids information. For example, participants were asked to
to compensate for perceived deficits. The MRQ has a indicate how important it is to remember ‘‘what you
Likert-type response format with responses ranging read in the newspaper,’’ ‘‘what books you own,’’ ‘‘where
from 1–5. Items included ‘‘I am confident that I will you filed something,’’ and ‘‘what you did yesterday.’’
remember really important things’’ and ‘‘I get fru- Items were scored from 1, ‘‘not at all important,’’ to 5,
strated with my memory.’’ The scale demonstrated ‘‘extremely important.’’ Internal consistency for this
good internal consistency in this sample (a ¼ .86). measure also was good (a ¼ .89). Three subsets of
Possessions in View Scale. The Possessions in items from the IOR were considered separately,
View Scale (PIVS) is an 11-item scale assessing the including six items pertaining to the importance of
extent to which individuals keep their belongings in remembering information (IOR-info., a ¼ .75), four
sight to serve as visual retrieval cues. Items included ‘‘I items that specifically pertained to remembering what
cannot remember something unless I keep a possession possessions are kept (IOR-poss., a ¼ .74), and two
in sight to remind me of it’’ and ‘‘If I put something in items that pertained to the importance of remembering
storage I will forget I own it (1 ¼ strongly disagree, where possessions are kept (IOR-location, a ¼ .60).
5 ¼ strongly agree).’’ The scale had good internal
consistency in this sample (a ¼ .88).
Consequences of Forgetting Scale. The Conse- RESULTS
quences of Forgetting Scale (COFS) contains 10-items
ASSESSMENT OF VISUAL AND VERBAL
assessing the perceived consequences and severity of
forgetting information. Items included ‘‘Other people MEMORY
will think I am a failure if I forget something’’ and ‘‘If I Scores for mean RCFT accuracy and organization
forget some information, I will never have access to it are presented in Table 2. RCFT accuracy scores were
again.’’ Participants were asked to indicate their level of evaluated via a mixed-model 2 (Group)  3 (Condition)
agreement with each item, from 1, ‘‘strongly disagree,’’ ANOVA with Condition (copy, immediate recall,
to 5, ‘‘strongly agree.’’ In this sample, the COFS also delayed recall) as the within-subjects factor. Although
had good internal consistency (a ¼ .89). there was no main effect for Group, there was a

TABLE 2. Descriptive statistics and significance tests comparing hoarders and control respondents on indices of visual
and verbal memory functioningw

Hoarders mean (sd) Controls mean (sd) t


RCFT measures
Accuracy in copying 67.00 (4.29) 67.29 (3.95) 0.24
Accuracy in immediate recall 33.59 (12.46) 38.79 (14.24) 1.31
Accuracy in delayed recall 32.05 (12.52) 40.58 (12.64) 2.30*
% Recall –immediate to delayed 98.18 (23.31) 110.64 (31.55) 1.51
Copy organization 3.14 (1.58) 4.58 (1.64) 3.04**
CVLT measures
General learning and memory
Trial 1 7.91 (2.29) 8.70 (2.38) 1.13
Trial 5 12.59 (2.15) 14.04 (1.69) 2.52*
Trials 1–5 55.00 (9.25) 59.39 (8.85) 1.63
SD free recall 10.86 (3.59) 12.74 (2.22) 2.12*
LD free recall 11.64 (2.70) 13.17 (2.21) 2.10*
Retention over a delay
% Recall: Trial 5 to SD 84.97 (21.88) 90.75 (11.59) 1.12
% Recall: SD to LD 117.61 (51.27) 104.44 (14.91) 1.18
Encoding strategies
Semantic clustering 2.36 (0.98) 2.67 (0.81) 1.13
Serial clustering 2.32 (1.84) 1.95 (1.45) 0.76
Response to retrieval aids
LD recognition discriminability 95.55 (4.46) 96.91 (3.42) 1.16
w
LD, long duration; SD, short duration.
n
Po.05.
nn
Po.01.
64 Hartl et al.

significant Group  Condition interaction (F[1,44] ¼ findings in OCD [Deckersbach et al., 2000a; Savage
4.24, P ¼ .02). Post hoc t-tests indicated that hoarders et al., 1999, 2000].
did not differ from controls in copy performance Based on Savage et al. [1999, 2000] findings in OCD
(t[44] ¼ .24, P ¼ .81) or in performance during the that memory deficits were mediated by poor organiza-
immediate recall condition (t[44] ¼ 1.31, P ¼ .20, effect tion during encoding, the long-delay free recall
size ¼ .39). Hoarders recalled significantly less infor- measure on the RCFT was selected as the dependent
mation from the figure during the delayed recall variable in a test of the same mediation hypothesis.
condition 20 min later (t[44] ¼ 2.30, P ¼ .03). Figure Structural model analyses for the RCFT follow the
1 depicts mean RCFT copy and recall scores for both procedure outlined in Baron and Kenny [1986] and by
groups. Savage et al. [1999, 2000] with OCD patients. Group
Mean percent recall from immediate to delayed (controls ¼ 1, hoarders ¼ 2) had a direct effect on long-
recall, defined as: [(delayed recall/immediate recall) * delay recall (b ¼ .33, P ¼ .03; control M ¼ 40.58,
100] (Table 2) showed no significant difference hoarder M ¼ 32.05) and on the hypothesized mediator,
between groups regarding how much information was copy organization (b ¼ .42, P ¼.00; control M ¼
retained between immediate and delayed recall 4.58, hoarder M ¼ 3.14). In the multiple regression
(t[44] ¼ 1.51, P ¼ .14, effect size ¼ .45). This suggests analysis, both copy organization (b ¼ .23, P ¼.15) and
that the significant dif ference in accuracy at delayed the direct effect of group (b ¼ .23, P ¼ .14) reduced in
recall may be due to improvement shown by controls, size and were no longer predictive for long-delay recall.
rather than substantial forgetting in the hoarding Thus, group and encoding strategy both predicted
group. There was no group dif ference on the recogni- delayed recall independent of one another, but their
tion test (t[44] ¼ .48, P ¼ .64). During the copy effects weakened when considered together. Change in
condition, hoarders’ drawings of the figure were R2 from the direct to the mediated model increased
significantly less organized than nonhoarders only .04, indicating that the mediated model did not
(t[44] ¼ 3.04, P ¼ .001), which is similar to previous better explain group dif ferences in recall. This out-
come is different from previous findings in OCD
patients [Savage et al., 1999, 2000] in which organiza-
tional strategies during encoding were found to
statistically mediate recall.
Figure 2 depicts the CVLT recall means for Trials
1–5, short and long-delay free recall, and recognition.
For the general learning and memory measures (Table
2), a mixed-model 2 (Group)  4 (Condition; Trial 1,
Trial 5, short-delay free recall, long-delay free recall)
ANOVA, with Condition as the within-subjects factor,
showed a significant main effect for Group
(F[1,43] ¼ 4.98, P ¼ .03) with no Group  Condition
interaction (F[1,129] ¼ 1.16, P ¼ .33). Hoarding parti-
cipants recalled less information overall. Results are
summarized in Table 2.
There were no group differences for percent recall
from Trial 5 to short-delay (t[43] ¼ 1.12, P ¼ .27) or
from short-delay to long-delay recall (t[43] ¼ .74,
P ¼ .46), suggesting that although hoarders did not
recall as much verbal information as controls, this was
not the result of abnormal forgetting rates. In addition,
there were no significant group differences in semantic
clustering during encoding. Therefore, hoarders were
as likely as controls to use semantic clustering to
organize items on the shopping list into categories
(t[43] ¼ 1.13, P ¼ .27, effect size ¼ .35). This finding is
also different from previous studies in participants with
OCD. Finally, there were no group dif ferences
on recognition discriminability, t(43) ¼ 1.16, P ¼
.25. Hoarders and controls performed similarly when
Fig. 1. Summary of the primary Rey Complex Figure Test
(RCFT) [Osterrieth, 1944] scores. Error bars ¼ SEM. Hoarders
asked to discriminate previously learned words from
recalled significantly less than control participants on delayed distracters.
recall, but the difference does not reflect an abnormal rate of In summary, results indicate that hoarders are
forgetting in the hoarding group. Delayed recognition accuracy impaired relative to their matched controls on organi-
was also normal. zation and delayed recall of visual information (RCFT),
Research Article: Hoarding and Memory Deficits 65

Fig. 2. Summary of the primary California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) [Delis et al., 1987] scores. Error bars ¼ SEM. Hoarders recalled
significantly less than controls on free recall measures, including Trial 5, short-delay, and long-delay free recall. Long delay recognition
accuracy was normal in the hoarding group.

and on short and long-delayed recall measures of verbal might be an accurate appraisal of their actual memory
information (CVLT). Hoarders did not show evidence functioning. Three ANCOVA were run with the MRQ
of difficulty with semantic organization during encod- as the dependent variable, and measures of actual
ing of verbal information, as evidenced by their normal memory on which hoarders showed deficits as the
semantic clustering scores on the CVLT. covariates (long-delay recall on the RCFT, and short
and long-delay free recall on the CVLT). After
covarying out actual memory deficits, all three analyses
SUBJECTIVE SELF-ASSESSMENTS OF
remained statistically significant (F[1,43] ¼ 14.55, and
HOARDING BEHAVIOR AND MEMORY F[1,42] ¼ 12.87, and 13.71, respectively, Po.001) in
BELIEFS each case. Poor memory confidence endorsed by
Table 3 presents descriptive statistics pertaining to hoarders was not explained by actual memory deficits.
hoarding severity scores and memory beliefs for Conversely, when the MRQ was used as the covariate
hoarders and control participants. Relative to non- and each recall variable that was significantly lower for
hoarders, the hoarding sample reported significantly hoarders (long-delay recall on the RCFT, and short-
(Po.01) poorer confidence in their memories, stronger and long-delay recall on the CVLT) was evaluated as
inclinations to keep possessions in sight, and more the dependent variable, the F-test remained significant
concerns about serious and detrimental consequences only for delayed recall on the RCFT (F[1,43] ¼ 4.45,
for forgetting information. Hoarders also placed great- P ¼ .04). For the two CVLT analyses, results were no
er importance on remembering where possessions are longer statistically significant (short delay: F[1,43] ¼
kept (IOR-location) (t[44] ¼ 2.15, P ¼ .04). The IOR 1.30, P ¼ .26; long delay: F[1,43] ¼ 1.82, P ¼ .18).
total score and two of its subscales (remembering These results suggest that nonverbal recall deficits
information and possessions) were the only self-report are not explicable by group dif ferences in memory
measures that did not significantly differentiate hoar- confidence, but that poor memory confidence may
ders from controls (Table 1). These three measures indeed contribute to verbal recall deficits.
were excluded from subsequent analyses. To explore whether the phenomenon of keeping
Given that hoarders reported significantly poorer things in sight could be largely accounted for by having
confidence in their memory than controls and also poor memory confidence, an ANCOVA was run with
displayed visual and verbal memory deficits, we sought PIVS as the dependent variable and the MRQ as
to determine whether their poor memory confidence the covariate (F[1,43] ¼ 6.88, P ¼ .01). With memory
66 Hartl et al.

TABLE 3. Self-reported memory beliefs of hoarders and control respondents

Hoarders Controls
mean (sd) mean (sd) t
Saving behavior (SBS) 67.52 (16.06) 14.54 (8.40) 14.19***
Memory reliability (MRQ) 69.03 (11.02) 56.84 (9.80) 3.97**
Possessions in view (PIVS) 27.31 (8.83) 16.75 (5.18) 5.00***
Importance of remembering (IOR) 53.41 (10.10) 49.52 (11.43) 1.22
IOR-Information 16.64 (3.92) 14.67 (3.55) 1.79
IOR-Possessions 9.00 (3.24) 8.63 (2.75) 0.43
IOR-Location 7.64 (1.43) 6.58 (1.84) 2.15*
Consequences of forgetting (COFS) 26.95 (8.97) 18.29 (6.40) 4.30**
n
Po.05.
nn
Po.01.
nnn
Po.001.

confidence controlled for, there was still a group subjective memory measures. As noted, four hoarding
dif ference on the PIVS. Therefore, hoarders’ tenden- participants met full DSM-IV criteria for OCD. To
cies to keep possessions in sight cannot be fully verify that findings did not primarily reflect effects
explained by lack of confidence in memory. from these four subjects, we repeated the analyses
excluding them. All significant dif ferences remained
RELATIONSHIPS AMONG SELF-REPORT significant and all nonsignificant differences remained
nonsignificant. Similarly, five hoarders on psychotropic
MEASURES AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL medications were removed from the analysis to assess
PERFORMANCE whether medication ef fects influenced the results. This
A matrix of 32 correlations was computed between also caused no changes in significance on the neurop-
the four exploratory self-report measures (MRQ, PIVS, sychological measures that discriminated between
COFS, and IOR), four RCFT performance indices, hoarders and controls.
and four CVLT performance criteria. Only two of
these correlations were significant, suggesting that
memory performance could not be consistently pre-
DISCUSSION
dicted by various indices of memory confidence. We Participants with compulsive hoarding were im-
carried out additional analyses examining the relation- paired on specific verbal and nonverbal learning and
ships between strategy measures and memory scores. free recall measures compared to matched control
For the entire sample, better organization predicted participants. Hoarders also showed significant plan-
better RCFT immediate recall (r ¼ .36, Po.05) and ning and organization problems when copying a
delayed recall (r ¼ .33, Po.05) but not copy perfor- complex figure, drawing disjointed elements of the
mance (r ¼ .26). When broken down by group, stimulus rather than its major organizational features.
organization was not significantly correlated with any The ability to use a verbal learning strategy, measured
of these three performance measures in the non- in semantic clustering, was not impaired in hoarding
hoarding sample (r ¼ .11, .03, and .18), but was so in participants. Hoarders, therefore, had dif ficulties
the hoarding sample (r ¼ .57, .46, and .43 for short- utilizing spatial, but not verbal, encoding strategies.
delay, long-delay, and copy conditions, respectively). Both verbal and nonverbal strategy measures were
Interestingly, semantic clustering was also relatively correlated with subsequent recall in the hoarding
more predictive of better performance on the CVLT by group.
hoarders, but not control participants, for recall at trial Results of this study also indicate that hoarders
5 (r ¼ .68 vs. .28), at short-delay (r ¼ .39 vs. .06), and at believe that they have general and specific memory
long-delay (r ¼ .65 vs. .19). problems. In validation of Frost and Hartl’s [1996]
cognitive-behavioral model of hoarding, hoarders in
this study reported significantly less confidence in their
ANALYSES BASED ON DEMOGRAPHIC
memory, stronger needs to keep possessions in sight
CHARACTERISTICS, DIAGNOSIS, AND
(even when poor memory confidence was controlled
MEDICATIONS for), and more concerns about catastrophic conse-
With the exception of verbal IQ, none of the quences of forgetting where objects were placed. In
demographic variables in Table 1, including age, the addition, covariance analyses indicated that memory
YBOCS, the BDI-II, and the BAI were correlated confidence measures statistically explained verbal
significantly with any of the neuropsychological or memory problems in the hoarding group. Thus,
Research Article: Hoarding and Memory Deficits 67

confidence in memory seemed to be disproportionately functioning in individuals with OCD. Hoarders in this
af fected in comparison to objective verbal memory sample, like OCD patients in previous studies [Deck-
performance on the CVLT in this hoarding sample. ersbach et al., 2000a; Savage et al., 1999, 2000] recalled
This was not the case for nonverbal memory. Delayed less verbal information at short and long-delays, as well
recall deficits on the RCFT remained statistically as less visual information after a long-delay. Hoarders
significant in the covariance analyses. It is possible used semantic clustering strategies to a similar degree
that individuals with compulsive hoarding primarily as controls. Unlike previous studies with OCD patients
have neuropsychological problems affecting nonverbal [Savage et al., 1999, 2000], regression analyses in this
or spatial memory. These real problems might then study did not support mediation models in which
lead to more generalized concerns about the adequacy encoding strategies mediate group differences in
of memory that negatively impact other (e.g., verbal) subsequent recall. It is notable that these mediation
modalities. models were recently extended to a group with body
These findings provide some empirical validation for dysmorphic disorder [Deckersbach et al., 2000b]. Our
treatment approaches that combine organization train- findings indicate that there may be important qualita-
ing with cognitive modification interventions [Frost et tive differences in the nature of memory impairment in
al., 1999; Hartl and Frost, 1999; Steketee et al., 2000]. these various ‘‘OC-spectrum’’ groups.
Although these treatments have included help with Some caution is needed when interpreting results
devising organizational techniques such as filing from this initial study. One limitation is the relatively
systems, their ef ficacy might well be improved by small size of our patient and control samples. Other
providing rigorous organizational training at the outset limitations of this study include the fact that many of
of treatment and ensuring that such training remains a our clinical hoarding measures were designed specifi-
central focus. This focus may help prevent newly cally for this study and, therefore, have not been
acquired organizational systems from quickly becom- validated. The initial development of these instruments
ing nonfunctional. It may also help improve confidence was guided by a coherent theoretical model of hoarding
in memory, which could have beneficial effects in other [Frost and Hartl, 1996] and our findings are generally
areas. In addition, embedding specific exposure and in accord with predictions from that model and
response prevention exercises within a larger frame- expected relationships to more established clinical
work of teaching organizational strategies may help measures. The most significant limitation may pertain
hoarders better habituate to having possessions out of to the diagnostic quandaries surrounding compulsive
sight. hoarding. There is little consensus regarding the
Anxiety about not being able to remember where diagnosis of compulsive hoarding, or even whether it
possessions are kept might also contribute to a vicious is best characterized in Axis I or Axis II of the DSM-IV.
self-perpetuating cycle that fosters reluctance by However, we verified saving and cluttering behaviors
hoarders to relegate possessions to out-of-view storage with a home visit in all but three participants diagnosed
areas (Frost and Hartl, 1996). Cognitive interventions with compulsive hoarding. Participants in the hoarding
for poor memory confidence and over-estimations of group showed a clear pattern of extreme saving and
negative consequences of forgetting may, therefore, be cluttering.
a second crucial treatment element. In particular, In summary, results from this study suggest that
cognitive restructuring techniques should be aimed at hoarders have selected visual and verbal learning and
challenging hoarders’ pervasive beliefs about the memory problems and impaired use of organizational
importance of leaving every possession in sight and strategies during nonverbal encoding. They also have
the catastrophic consequences of not being able to reduced confidence in their memory, along with
remember everything. increased catastrophic assessments of the consequences
Our findings also speak to the questions regarding of forgetting and a stronger desire to keep possessions
the relationship between OCD and compulsive hoard- in sight. There are both similarities and differences
ing. Similar to Winsberg et al. [1999], we found that between these results and findings from previous work
the majority of individuals in our hoarding sample did in OCD. Both groups show reduced use of nonverbal
not meet SCID criteria for OCD. In both studies, organizational strategies and impaired free recall for
o20% of hoarders met SCID diagnostic criteria for verbal and nonverbal information. Participants in this
OCD. This may be a function of the SCID and the compulsive hoarding sample used semantic clustering
DSM-IV, as the appropriate location for compulsive strategies during verbal learning as effectively as
hoarding within the current classification system is as control subjects. Furthermore, statistical models docu-
yet unclear. Alternatively, as several studies have mented in OCD studies were not supported in this
suggested that hoarders are less responsive to tradi- group of participants with hoarding. We view this
tional medications and CBT for OCD [e.g., Mataix- investigation as a first step toward better understanding
Cols et al., 1999], it is possible that hoarding represents the complex relationships between subjective confi-
a fundamentally distinct disorder [Black et al., 1998]. dence and objective memory functioning in compulsive
Results from this study share similarities and discre- hoarders. Future studies should further explore these
pancies with previous findings regarding memory relationships in OCD and hoarding patient groups.
68 Hartl et al.

Acknowledgments. We thank D. Kenny for statis- Frost RO, Longo C. 1996. The Memory Reliability Questionnaire.
tical consultation. Unpublished scale.
Frost RO, Steketee G, Kyrios M. 1999. The Saving Behavior Scale.
Unpublished scale.
Goodman WK, Price LH, Rasmussen SA, Mazure C, Fleischmann
REFERENCES RL, Hill CL, Heninger GR, Charney DS. 1989. The Yale-Brown
Obsessive Compulsive Scale: I: Development, use, and reliability.
Abbruzzese M, Bellodi L, Ferri S, Scarone S. 1993. Memory Arch Gen Psychiatry 46:1006–1011.
functioning in obsessive compulsive disorder. Behav Neurol Greenberg D, Witzum E, Levy A. 1990. Hoarding as a psychiatric
6:119–122. symptom. J Clin Psychiatry 51:417–421.
American Psychiatric Association. 1994. Diagnostic and statistical Hwang J, Tsai S, Yang C, Liu K, Lirng J. 1998. Hoarding behavior in
manual of mental disorders. 4th Ed. Washington, DC: American
dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 6:285–289.
Psychiatric Association.
Leckman JF, Grice DE, Boardman J, Zhang H, Vitale A, Bondi C,
Baer L. 1994. Factor analysis of symptom subtypes of obsessive
Alsobrook J, Peterson BS, Cohen DJ, Rasmussen SA, Goodman
compulsive disorder and their relation to personality and tic
WK, McDougle CJ, Pauls DL. 1997. Symptoms of obsessive-
disorders. J Clin Psychiatry 55:18–23.
compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 154:911–917.
Baron RM, Kenny DA. 1986. The moderator-mediator variable
MacDonald PA, Antony MM, MacLeod CM, Richter MA. 1997.
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic,
and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51:1173–1182. Memory and confidence in memory judgments among individuals
Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. 1988. An inventory for with obsessive compulsive disorder and non-clinical controls.
measuring anxiety: Psychometric properties. J Consult Clin Behav Res Ther 35:497–505.
Psychol 56:893–897. Martin A, Wiggs CL, Altemus M, Rubenstein C, Murphy DL. 1995.
Black DW, Monahan P, Gable J, Blum N, Clancy G, Baker P. 1998. Working memory as assessed by subject-ordered tasks in patients
Hoarding and treatment response in 38 nondepressed subjects with with obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Clin Psychiatry 59:420–425. 17:786–792.
Calamari JE, Wiegartz PS, Janeck, AS. 1999. Obsessive-compulsive Martinot JL, Allilaire JF, Mazoyer BM, Hantouche E, Huret JD,
disorder subgroups: A symptom-based clustering approach. Behav Legaut-Demare F, Deslauriers AG, Hardy P, Pappata S, Baron JC.
Res Ther 37:113–125. 1990. Obsessive-compulsive disorder: A clinical, neuropsychologi-
Christensen KJ, Kim SW, Dysken MW, Hoover KM. 1992. cal and positron emission tomography study. Acta Psychiatr Scand
Neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disor- 82:233–242.
der. Biol Psychiatry 31:4–18. Mataix-Cols D, Rauch SL, Manzo PA, Jenike MA, Baer L. 1999.
Deckersbach T, Otto MW, Savage CR, Baer L, Jenike MA. 2000a. Use of factor-analyzed symptom dimensions to predict
The relationship between semantic organization and memory in outcome with serotonin reuptake inhibitors and placebo in the
obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychother Psychosom 69:101–107. treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry
Deckersbach T, Savage CR, Henin A, Mataix-Cols D, Otto MW, 156:1409–1416.
Wilhelm S, Rauch SL, Baer L, Jenike MA. 2000b. Reliability and McNally RJ, Kohlbeck PA. 1993. Reality monitoring in obsessive-
validity of a scoring system for measuring organizational approach compulsive disorder. Behav Res Ther 31:249–253.
in the complex figure test. J Clin Exp Psychol 22:640–648. Meyers JE, Meyers KR. 1995. Rey complex figure test and
Deckersbach T, Savage CR, Wilhelm S, Buhlman U, Rauch SL, Baer recognition trial: Professional manual. Odessa: Psychological
L, Jenike MA, Phillips KA. 2000c. Characteristics of memory Assessment Resources.
dysfunction in body dysmorphic disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc Oldfield RC. 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: The
6:673–681. Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia 9:97–113.
Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. 1987. California verbal Osterrieth PA. 1944. Le test de copie d’une figure complex:
learning test: manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corpora- Contribution à létude de la perception et de la memoire
tion.
[The test of copying a complex figure: A contribution to the
Denman SB. 1984. Denman Neuropsychology Memory Scale.
study of perception and memory]. Archives de Psychologie 30:
Charlestown, SC: S.B. Denman.
286–350.
Dirson S, Bouvard M, Cottraux J, Martin R. 1995. Visual memory
Purcell R, Maruff P, Kyrios M, Pantelis C. 1998a. Cognitive deficits
impairment in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder: a
in obsessive-compulsive disorder on tests of frontal-striatal
controlled study. Psychother Psychosom 63:22–31.
First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. 1995. Structured function. Biol Psychiatry 43:348–357.
Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders-Patient Edition Purcell R, Maruff P, Kyrios M, Pantelis C. 1998b. Neuropsycholo-
(SCID-I/D, Version 2.0). New York: Biometrics Research Depart- gical deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A comparison with
ment, New York Psychiatric Institute. unipolar depression, panic disorder, and normal controls. Arch
Frankenburg F. 1984. Hoarding in anorexia nervosa. Br J Med Gen Psychiatry 55:415–423.
Psychol 57:57–60. Radomsky AS, Rachman S. 1999. Memory bias in obsessive-
Frost RO, Gross RC. 1993. The hoarding of possessions. Behav Res compulsive disorder. Behav Res Ther 37:605–618.
Ther 31:367–381. Radomsky AS, Rachman S, Hammond D. 2001. Memory bias,
Frost RO, Hartl TL. 1996. A cognitive behavioral model of confidence and responsibility in compulsive checking. Behav Res
compulsive hoarding. Behav Res Ther 34:341–350. Ther 39:813–822.
Frost RO, Hartl TL, Christian R, Williams N. 1995. The value of Rasmussen S, Eisen L. 1992. The epidemiology and clinical features
possessions in compulsive hoarding: Patterns of use and attach- of obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 15:
ment. Behav Res Ther 33:897–902. 743–758.
Frost RO, Krause M, Steketee G. 1996. Hoarding and obsessive Rauch SL, Baxter LR. 1998. Neuroimaging in obsessive-compulsive
compulsive symptoms. Behav Modif 20:116–132. disorder and related disorders. In: Jenike MA, Baer L, Minichiello
Research Article: Hoarding and Memory Deficits 69

WE, editors. Obsessive-compulsive disorders: Practical manage- Sher KJ, Mann B, Frost RO. 1984. Cognitive dysfunction in
ment. 3rd Ed. St. Louis: Mosby. p 289–317. compulsive checkers: Further explorations. Behav Res Ther
Savage CR. 1997. Neuropsychology of subcortical dementias. 22:493–502.
Psychiatr Clin North Am 20:911–931. Sher KJ, Frost RO, Kushner M, Crews TM, Alexander JE. 1989.
Savage CR. 1998. Neuropsychology of OCD: Research findings and Memory deficits in compulsive checkers: Replication and extension
treatment implications. In: Jenike MA, Baer L, Minichiello WE, in a clinical sample. Behav Res Ther 27:65–69.
editors. Obsessive-compulsive disorders: Practical management. Summerfeldt LJ, Richter MA, Antony MM, Swinson RP. 1999.
3rd Ed. St. Louis: Mosby. p 254–275. Symptom structure in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A confirma-
Savage CR, Baer L, Keuthen NJ, Brown HD, Rauch SL, Jenike MA. tory factor-analytic study. Behav Res Ther 37:297–311.
1999. Organizational strategies mediate nonverbal memory Tolin DF, Brigidi BD, Abramowitz JS, Amir N, Street GP,
impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry Foa EB. 1999. Memory and confidence biases in obsessive-
45:905–916. compulsive disorder. Paper presented at the meeting of the
Savage CR, Deckersbach T, Wilhelm S, Rauch SL, Baer L, Reid T, Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy, Toronto,
Jenike MA. 2000. Strategic processing and episodic memory Canada.
impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology Winsberg ME, Cassic KS, Koran LM. 1999. Hoarding in obsessive
14:141–151. compulsive disorder: A report of 20 cases. J Clin Psychiatry 60:
Savage CR, Keuthen NJ, Jenike MA, Brown HD, Baer L, Kendrick 591–597.
AD, Miguel EC, Rauch SL, Albert MS. 1996. Recall and Zielinski CM, Taylor MA, Juzwin KR. 1991. Neuropsychological
recognition memory in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Neurop- deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychiatr Neu-
sychiatr 8:99–103. ropsychol Behav Neurol 4:110–126.

You might also like