You are on page 1of 9

The Robert W.

Kleemeier Memorial Lecture


Four sectors comprising "the good life" are described: behavioral competence, psychological
well-being, perceived quality of life, and objective environment. Each may be divided into
conceptually meaningful domains capable of being measured. Assessment approaches for each
sector are described and empirical evidence is presented regarding interrelationships among
them. Nonetheless, each sector implies individual and social goals that are defensible without
regard to how much the effect of change in one is reflected in change in another sector. All
sectors influence the self, which in turn re-energizes the sectors.

Environment and Other Determinants


of Weil-Being in Older People1

Downloaded from http://gerontologist.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Michigan on October 17, 2014


M. Powell Lawton, PhD:

The opportunity to deliver the Kleemeier Lecture would be the development of dimensional scales
gives me particular pleasure because it was Bob who which would define more precisely the setting char-
founded the thriving area that we know today as the acteristics" (Kleemeier, 1961, p. 304).
environmental psychology of later life. His landmark Despite advances in measurement since then, Bob
chapter (Kleemeier, 1959) in the original Birren Hand- would probably be disappointed with what the past
book (1959) was called "Behavior and the Organiza- two decades have netted in this respect. However, I
tion of the Bodily and the External Environment." I expect that he would be pleased with the extent to
have probably read it a dozen times. In reading it which the academic advancement committee of the
again for this lecture, I continue to be amazed at how behavioral sciences has affirmatively recruited the
far ahead of other work it was. In this chapter environment into full academic rank.
Kleemeier discussed the aging process as a modifier My task is to review my own work in a way that
of the relations between the person and the outside shows the environment as only one component of a
world. He raised issues regarding the visual environ- total behavioral system. I shall discuss a quadripartite
ment, the auditory environment, and the thermal concept that I have called "the good life," the mem-
environmentthat could be published today. His crea- bers of which are sectors called behavioral compe-
tive thinking about prosthetic environments pre- tence, psychological well-being, perceived quality of
ceded by 5 years that term as coined by Ogden Lind- life, and objective environment. The good life is a
sley (1964). His transactional view of person and res- grandiose construct, presuming to account for all of
idential environment appeared 7 years before the life. Indeed, the implication is that the good life (and
collection of papers that formed the first statement of its polar opposite, the bad life) subsumes all that we
environmental psychology (Kates & Wohlwill, 1966). define as legitimate personal and social goals. Its
Students of institutions still use actively the three sectors together include every aspect of behavior,
contextual dimensions posited by Kleemeier, which environment, and experience.
he called the segregate, the congregate, and the con- By using the term "good" I obviously mean to con-
trol aspects of the milieu (e.g., Kahana, 1982). I vey the idea that whatever falls into these four sectors
should like to end this attribution with a quotation is capable of being evaluated in terms of its positive
that my colleagues and I must have paraphrased to negative quality. The evaluation of such constructs
dozens of times: "One of the first tasks of research has been the focus of much of my research. Thus, I
conceived within this [environmental] framework shall describe a model of the good life and define it in
terms of the dimensions by which I and others have
tried to operationalize it. The good life is explicitly
intended as a metaconstruct to which the four sec-
1
Kleemeier Lecture, presented at the 35th Annual Scientific Meeting of tors contribute, both independently and in mutual
the Gerontological Society, Boston, November 20,1982. Special thanks are
due Frances M. Carp and Stanley J. Brody for their comments on earlier fashion.
versions of the paper. The assistance of Arthur Waldman, Bernard Liebo- Although the four sectors of the good life are re-
witz, Lucille Nahemow, Morton Kleban, Elaine Brody, Miriam Moss, and
Sandra Howell in various phases of the research supported here was major.
lated, I shall begin by defining and describing each
Some of the thoughts first were presented in the Kesten Award Lecture, separately, using this structure as a base for review-
Andrus Cerontology Center, University of Southern California, Los ing some relevant findings from my research. As you
Angeles, 1978.
2
Philadelphia Geriatric Center, 5301 Old York Road, Philadelphia, PA
will see, my favored approach has been to work to
19141. develop a taxonomy within each sector, with the

Vol. 23, No. 4, 1983 349


rationale that a necessary beginning for theory is No existing instrument or battery is capable of dis-
good description together with some understanding criminating quality of performance from very low to
of how the elements of description fit together. A very high competence for every domain and level of
major goal of my work has been to search for ways to complexity. However, Figure 1 schematizes the
assess how good or how bad life is in each sector. range of behaviors that the ideal assessment instru-
ment should include.
Assessment evaluates the quality of the person's
Behavioral Competence
behavior in each of the domains. To represent this
"Competence" has been used by many investiga- evaluative element in the model, a third dimension is
tors to define desirable personal qualities, presum- necessary, where the height of the solid represents
ably lying within the individual and occasioning the the assessed adaptive quality of the behavior, as
exhibition of adaptive behavior. My definition (Law- judged by social standards ("normative compe-
ton, 1982a) was, "the theoretical upper limit of ca- tence," Lawton, 1972).
pacity of the individual to function in the areas of bio- The MAI contains interview- or informant-based
logical health, sensation and perception, motor measures of each of these domains divided into sub-
behavior, and cognition" (p.38). These constructs, scales that may be used alone or summed as overall
however, are usually not directly measurable. There- domain scores: self-rated health, health behavior,
fore one way to measure competence is to define

Downloaded from http://gerontologist.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Michigan on October 17, 2014


health conditions, self-maintaining activities of daily
behaviors that imply the presence of some element living; instrumental activities of daily living; mental
of competence. Most such behaviors are, in fact, status, cognitive symptoms; time use (enriching ac-
actual performance rather than ability. Thus, breath- tivities); family interaction, and friends' interaction.
ing, walking, or caring for one's physical needs may This array represents only a limited number of all the
be indicators of good health; performance on an possible behaviors that could be evaluated, but it is a
intelligence test or time taken to solve a problem are basic list that meets many of the assessment needs
behavioral indicators of intelligence; behaviors ex- encountered in both research and service-delivery
hibited by nonpatients and not by patients become settings. The hierarchy itself serves as a guide to the
indicators of ego strength. location of other behaviors for which special-
A technology for the assessment of competence purpose instruments may be required. For example,
has developed over the years. The most elaborate the assessment of range of motion, so critical to re-
assessment systems such as the Duke OARS (Duke habilitation efforts, constitutes a more differentiated
University, 1978), the Comprehensive Assessment subset of activities of daily living and could be added
and Referral Evaluation of Gurland et al. (1977-1978), to the basic MAI. Another example is "social integra-
or the Aging Needs Survey in Manitoba (Havens & tion," an aspect of behavioral competence very high
Thompson, 1976) attempt to evaluate a number of in the complexity dimension of social behavior, for
domains of behavioral competence. which appropriate measures could be devised (Liang
The Philadelphia Geriatric Center Multilevel et al., 1980).
Assessment Instrument (MAI) is another such The MAI includes but does not require scales for
measurement device (Lawton, et al., 1982) which trained professionals to perform overall integrative
attempts to assess the most important behavioral do- ratings for each domain. Therefore, it can be admin-
mains with the most efficient resources. The first task istered and scored by a variety of people, from re-
in designing the MAI was to define the most impor- search assistants to case-management aides. Further,
tant behavioral-competence categories that should it was designed with a short, medium, and full-length
be included in any comprehensive assessment sys- form for each scale and subscale. Each scale may be
tem. The body of literature in assessment led to a used separately, so that, if time is short, the most
good consensus that health, functional health, efficient evaluation may be done.
cognition, time use, and social behavior formed a For the present purpose, however, the specific
usable minimal set of such categories. In an attempt characteristics of the MAI are less important than the
to impose some order on these behavioral- principle which states that all behavior is capable of
competence sectors and their relationships to one being located along a hierarchy of complexity and
another I proposed a hierarchy of categories (as re- evaluated in terms of its normative competence.
vised in Figure 1), based on the complexity of the
behaviors, within which specific behaviors relevant
to living in the real world could be categorized (Law- Psychological Well-being
ton, 1972). The domains of behavior progress in their Psychological well-being, the next sector of the
minimal levels of complexity from left to right, this
good life, is one's subjective evaluation of the overall
relative complexity being indicated by the elevation
quality of one's inner experience. Other things being
of the bottom of each of the columns. Within each
equal, the common wisdom is that we would rather
domain, sublevels of complexity may also be dif-
feel good or neutral than feel distressed. Such an
ferentiated; examples of behaviors falling at different
levels of complexity within each domain are shown. evaluation is thus private, but we sometimes attempt
Although behaviors in different domains may be re- to assess psychological well-being by proxy when we
lated to one another, their relative vertical height is ask an observer to estimate another person's subjec-
relevant only within a domain, not across domains. tive state. The long history of attempts to name and
measure this construct cannot be reviewed here. The

350 The Gerontologist


COMPLEX
Creative Creotive Creative
Innovation Innovation Leadership,
Love,Parent-
hood, Altru-
Problem istic Behavior
Solving

Exploration
Symbolic Nurturonce
Thinking

Operant
Conditioning Intimacy
Recreotion

Clossicol
Conditioning Cosual
Paid Contact
Curiosity
Employment
Memory

Fmonciol Sensory
Management Stimulus Contoct
Perception
Variation
Body
Instrumental
System ADL Sensory
Reception

Downloaded from http://gerontologist.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Michigan on October 17, 2014


Orgon Physical
ADL

Cell

SIMPLE
HEALTH FUNCTIONAL COGNITION TIME SOCIAL
HEALTH USE BEHAVIOR

Figure 1. Hierarchy of behavioral competence.

Neugarten, Havighurst, and Tobin (1961) Life Satis- The problem with concluding that such a consen-
faction Indices (LSI), Bradburn's (1969) Affect Balance sus exists on these four dimensions is that no single
Scale, and my (Lawton, 1975) Philadelphia Geriatric research has worked with content representing all of
Center Morale Scale are three of the most-frequently these and other constructs. Kleban and I attempted
used instruments to measure psychological well- this task in a study of a purposive sample of about 300
being in the aged at present. older people of widely varying presumed levels of
A major research issue has been whether psycho- psychological well-being (Lawton & Kleban, 1982).
logical well-being represents a single construct that We found very strong multi-item factors represent-
can be measured by many indicators or whether it is ing negative affect and happiness and less strong
multidimensional. If it is the latter, we do an injustice ones that were clearly identifiable as positive affect
to the person in attempting to represent psychologi- and congruence. Other factors were denial, social
cal well-being as a single score composed of highly ease, and two related to self-esteem in that some of
varied indicators. In an attempt to pursue this ques- the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (1965) items were
tion, I concluded from a recent review of the litera- split between them. Other theoretically promising
ture on psychological well-being (Lawton, 1982b) that factors were sought but failed to appear as empirical-
four domains have appeared frequently enough in ly clustered items: age-related morale, cognitive
diverse research to warrant being thought of as sepa- symptoms, futurity, or psychophysiological symp-
rate consensual aspects of psychological well-being: toms.
Neuroticism or negative affect includes anxiety, Psychological well-being thus seems to be capable
depression, agitation, worry, pessimism, and other of further differentiation in terms of negative affect,
clearly distressing psychological symptoms (Brad- happiness, positive affect, and congruence. Where
burn, 1969; Lawton, 1975). does this leave our favorite global constructs like
Happiness represents a cognitive judgment of the satisfaction and morale? I do not have relevant find-
pervasiveness of positive affects over a relatively long ings, but Liang and Bollen (1983) have produced evi-
but indefinite time interval (Bradburn, 1969; Gurin et dence to justify both the unidimensional and the
al., 1960; Kozma & Stones, 1980). multidimensional view of psychological well-being;
Positive affect is a contemporary and time-limited that is, negative affect and age-related morale appear
feeling of active pleasure, a description of an emo- to be stable and separate dimensions of the PGC
tional state more than a cognitive judgment (Brad- Morale Scale, but the hypothesis that they are sepa-
burn, 1969; Kozma & Stones, 1980). rate aspects of an overall second-order factor,
Congruence between desired and attained goals morale, was also upheld. Liang & Bollen's work and
was an essential element of the LSI (Neugarten et al., ours imply that people may be ranged on a dimen-
1961) and has been the only recurrent factor obtained sion of general psychological well-being, but for
by different investigators in a series of factor-analytic some purposes it may be worth differentiating
studies of that instrument (reviewed in Lawton, among its components. I shall return to this problem
1982b). later with some empirical data.

Vol. 23, No. 4, 1983 351


Perceived Quality of Life ing to characterize the environment in terms of its
The third sector of the good life is perceived quality presumed positive or negative effect on the average
of life, the set of evaluations that a person makes person. A variety of economic indicators such as the
about each major domain of his or her life. As in the stock average, the gross national product, new build-
case of general psychological well-being, one may ing starts, or the unemployment rate all presumably
confidently assert that most of us would rather not be reflect external conditions that have a high, although
grossly dissatisfied in any domain, although we might not absolute, probability of affecting the economic
not all agree that a state of satisfaction is a universal well-being of people in favorable or unfavorable
ways. Similarly, social indicators such as infant mor-
goal. The best-known research in this area was done
tality, disease morbidity rates, housing dilapidation,
by Campbell etal. (1976) and by Andrews and Withey
the crime rate, and so on (U.S. Bureau of the Census,
(1976). Both research teams investigated people's
1977; U.S. Department of Health, Education and Wel-
satisfaction with self, family, friends, activities, work,
fare, 1969) have been sought to reflect the quality of
income, neighborhood, housing, and so on. They
other aspects of life.
also explored the relationship between satisfaction
in these areas and generalized psychological well- Pointing to the "out-there" quality of the environ-
being in addition to how these satisfactions varied ment constitutes the first defining feature of this con-
with important statuses such as age, marital status, ception of environment. Whatever else it is, it lies
race, and so on. outside the individual and is capable of being

Downloaded from http://gerontologist.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Michigan on October 17, 2014


Although no attempt has been made to develop a counted or rated consensually by observers other
than the subjects, or measured in centimeters,
taxonomy or a theoretical rationale for the choice of
grams, and seconds. It seems essential to separate
domains, there is reasonably good consensus that 12
this "objective" environment from the manner in
to 16 domains cover the aspects of life that are most
which the environment is experienced by the indi-
salient to most people. There are, of course, prob-
vidual. The environment as perceived by the person
lems in assessing perceived quality of life. Campbell
is of great interest but is a domain of perceived quali-
et al. (1976), Carp (1975), Herzog and Rodgers (1981)
ty of life, not of the objective environment. For exam-
and others have shown that people's general tenden- ple, the variable "number of people per room" is an
cy to proclaim themselves satisfied may be accentu- objective environmental characteristic, whereas
ated in the elderly. A variety of mechanisms such as "crowding" is an experienced, perceived quality of
denial, dissonance reduction, or ego defense may life characteristic. Most existing attempts to develop
moderate older people's critical evaluations of their a useful taxonomy of the environment are very primi-
daily lives. Lowered aspiration and poverty of com- tive indeed. Some time ago I suggested a set of basic
parison standards have been shown to increase ex- distinctions that should be made when we speak of
pressed satisfaction (Campbell et al., 1976). Despite "the environment" (Lawton, 1970):
the limitations of satisfaction ratings in estimating
absolute levels of evaluation, the work of Campbell — The physical environment, whether natural or man-
et al. (1976) and many others shows that comparing made;
perceived satisfaction responses across domains or — The personal environment, composed of the indi-
across subpopulations (Lawton, 1977) can partially viduals who play some significant social role in relation to
overcome the methodological limitations of this the subject;
head-on approach to defining the good life. — The small-group environment, an aggregate of two or
My own research has not explored the perceived more people with whom the person interacts (usually face-
to-face);
quality of life sector in systematic fashion, but our — The suprapersonal environment, which refers to the
factor-analytic study of psychological well-being did dominant characteristics of the aggregate of other indi-
include indicators of satisfaction with four such do- viduals in physical proximity to the subject (i.e., their aver-
mains: housing and neighborhood, the use of time, age age, socioeconomic status, most-prevalent race, and
family, and friends. It was comforting to see that so on).
none of these indicators fell into factors that we had — The social environment, composed of the major so-
hypothesized to represent psychological well-being. cial-institutional, normative, and cultural forces to which
This separateness helps confirm that perceived quali- the individual is exposed.
ty of life and psychological well-being are separate
sectors. It was also comforting to see residential satis- Standardized measures for many aspects of en-
faction and time-use satisfaction emerge as identifi- vironment have not been developed. Some exam-
able factors but discomforting to observe that satis- ples of objective measures include the Census
faction with family and satisfaction with friends did Bureau's list of housing deficiencies (U.S. Bureau of
not form statistically coherent clusters. Of the three the Census, 1977), the square footage of a dwelling
sectors discussed thus far, perceived quality of life is unit, the crime rate of a neighborhood, the number
most in need of further development of measure- of close relatives a person possesses, or the age mix-
ment methods. ture of an apartment building. In our own research
on older Jewish people in an urban slum, some basic
observer-rated characteristics of the neighborhood
Objective Environment
were factored: clusters of environmental attributes
The last sector of the good life is the objective named Jewish concentration, distance from facilities,
environment. There has been a tradition of attempt- busy location, neighborhood quality and block quali-

352 The Gerontologist


ty were obtained. They accounted for a significant perceived quality of life and objective environment.
proportion of variance in the motility of people in A phenomenological view would suggest that the
their neighborhood (Lawton & Cohen, 1974a). Re- only true measure of the goodness of existence lies in
search of this type has more recently been pursued the individual's personal assessment of his or her life,
vigorously by Carp and Carp (in press), Moos and as in psychological well-being, rather than in the
Lemke (1980), and Windley and Scheidt (1982), each behavior that he or she presents in public. Yet, we
of whom has striven for an environmental taxonomy have every reason to be dissatisfied with both the
based on higher-order dimensions that can be mea- ability of the person to articulate how he or she "real-
sured independently of people's perceptions. This ly" feels and the ability of the instruments themselves
work is still in its infancy, however. Kleemeier's ver- to represent these nuances. Psychological well-being
dict would probably be, "Good try, so far." is thus both a flawed and an incomplete indicator of
the good life, but one whose centrality has been
affirmed repeatedly in the research literature.
The Relationships Among the Good Lives
How can we also avoid considering whether a per-
I have thus far deliberately avoided dealing with son feels that his or her life is good in the different
the interrelationships among the sectors of the good environmental and interpersonal contexts where it is
life. Of course, they are related. In fact, one of the lived? The gerontological literature is full of demon-
main occupations of researchers is to look for the

Downloaded from http://gerontologist.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Michigan on October 17, 2014


strations suggesting that amount of satisfaction with
effects of variation in one sector on well-being in a domain of life is a stronger determinant of psycho-
another sector. Where the research deals with poli- logical well-being than is an objective indicator of
cies, programs, or services, a very usual approach is quality in that same domain (e.g., Liang et al., 1980).
to evaluate the program in terms of whether it can In our analyses of all of our own and others' data sets
affect an increase in some sector of the good life we have found substantial correlations between per-
other than its own. A typical question is, "Does X ceived quality of life in a domain and the indicator of
program, in addition to serving people with X need, behavioral competence in the relevant domain.
also make a person more active, more satisfied with Psychological well-being has also been shown to be
several domains of life quality, or more satisfied with related to perceived quality of life in virtually every
life in general?" I shall discuss further how adequate domain (Lawton, 1978).
each sector is in representing the good life and men- Some aspects of the objective environment can be
tion in passing some of the empirically-confirmed clearly characterized as desirable or undesirable.
relationships between sectors that have come from Lack of complete plumbing facilities, the total ab-
my research. sence of any relatives, residence in a high-crime
The competent performance of complex behaviors neighborhood, or daily exposure to institutional rac-
has been assumed to represent one aspect of the ism are examples of negative attributes in the objec-
good life. Is this a tenable assumption? For the lower tive environmental domains. Although the source of
levels of physical and functional health, cognition, their characterization as negative is ultimately the
and perhaps instrumental skills, this generalization judgment of individual persons, consensus on these
would be acceptable to all, but in the more complex judgments is so high that one becomes willing to
domains of time use and social behavior the social- accept the aggregate negative evaluation as "objec-
normative element intrudes increasingly. What tive." We should probably agree not only that they
about individual needs and tastes, or the capacity of are negative but that their presence could be taken as
the environment to support or inhibit competent an indicator of "the bad life," regardless of whether
behavior? For example, some of us do not wish to they were associated with behavioral competence or
interact with people while others may be unable to psychological well-being. However, it is often more
do so because of environmental barriers. For them, a difficult to assert that particular environmental attri-
low level of social interaction could not legitimately butes are favorable or unfavorable than to make such
be construed as indicating low competence in this assertions regarding indicators of behavioral compe-
area. Although socially-defined "normative compe- tence or psychological well-being. Rather, the favor-
tence" (Lawton, 1972) is one element of a positive able or unfavorable quality of some aspects of the
lifestyle, even in the time use and social areas, wide environment frequently remain to be discovered in
variations that exist in the degree to which normative terms of their association with desirable behavioral
competence describes individual goals leaves the or psychological outcomes. Because so much of my
good life incompletely defined by behavioral compe- research has been concentrated in the environmen-
tence alone. A test of the varying centrality of the tal arena, I shall cite briefly and in highly selective
domains of behavioral competence may be seen in fashion a few findings from this research that illus-
their correlations with psychological well-being in trate these cross-sector relationships. Similar docu-
the research that produced the MAI (Lawton et al., mentation would be easy to provide for every pair of
1982). Health correlated highest (.57) and social be- sectors; space limitations do not permit such ex-
havior the lowest (.23), while functional health, tended discussion.
cognition, and time use were intermediate in their
covariation with psychological well-being. Much Objective Environment and Behavioral Competence
more modest, but significant, correlations were The press-competence model of adaptation and
observed between behavioral competence and both aging developed by Nahemow and myself suggests

Vol. 23, No. 4, 1983 353


that for some people an increment in environmental physical aspects of the housing that appeared to lead
quality should lead to an increment in behavioral to increased general housing satisfaction: total
competence (Lawton & Nahemow, 1973). Planned amount of kitchen storage, amount of counter space,
housing for the elderly and institutions are environ- amount of closet space, presence of a private yard or
mental types that, being purpose-built, lend them- balcony, an electric (versus gas) stove, and others
selves naturally to the incorporation of social and (Lawton & Nahemow, 1979b). Finally an analysis of
physical design features that could affect compe- the HUD — Bureau of the Census Annual Housing
tence. In a study of people before and after moving to Survey data tapes revealed that an index composed
five new housing projects, their participation in activ- of 23 physical indicators of housing quality showed a
ities increased compared to that of people who did correlation of .38 with the occupant's satisfaction
not make such a move (Lawton & Cohen, 1974b). In a with his housing (Lawton, 1980).
cross-sectional study of 3,000 tenants in 150 such
projects, living in a project with a larger proportion of
age peers (Teaff et al., 1978) or a more age-dense local Objective Environmental Quality and Psychological
neighborhood (Lawton & Nahemow, 1979a) was Well-being
associated with greater activity. Although the size It is expecting a great deal of an objective environ-
(number of units) of one's project was unrelated to mental attribute to have an across-the-board direct
well-being, living in a taller building was associated effect on psychological well-being. However, just

Downloaded from http://gerontologist.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Michigan on October 17, 2014


with less travel outside the housing environment, as such an effect was found to be associated with mov-
if the vertical distance were a barrier to the full use of ing into planned housing (Lawton & Cohen, 1974b),
the local neighborhood (Lawton et al., 1975). with living in a project with a high age concentration
in both housing and neighborhood (Lawton & Nake-
mow, 1979a; Teaff et al., 1978), and with neighbor-
hood crime rate (Lawton et al., 1980).
Objective Environment and Perceived Quality of Life A reasonable conclusion is that well-being in one
We are continually surprised at how relatively low sector has a good chance of being reflected in one or
the relationships between objective and subjective more of the other sectors. But there is still more to be
environmental evaluations are, but such rela- learned about such relationships — that is, how they
tionships do usually exist. In our MAI study, for ex- relate to one another in concert. Thus, Kleban and I
ample, dollar income and perceived adequacy of in- set up a hypothetical model of well-being, using mea-
come were correlated at the .26 level while observer- sures of as many of the constructs as we had in our
and self-rated housing quality showed a .34 correla- MAI data set. The most basic elements of behavioral
tion (Lawton et al., 1982). In the large cross-sectional competence (health, cognition, and functional
study of housing we were able to identify a number of health) were predicted to influence friends' interac-

44

SEX

AGE

INCOME

EDUCA-
TION

Figure 2. Structural model of well-being (LISREL estimates).

354 The Gerontologist


tion and time use, which in turn should predict tween domains of two sectors of the good life; these
psychological well-being as measured by the PCC bivariate correlations were obtained from our analy-
Morale Scale. Family interaction, not an element of ses of five large data sets. The relatively low predicta-
basic competence, is determined by factors outside bility between sectors is obviously in part a function
the model. Objective environment was suggested to of measurement error. Those of us who work with
influence friends interaction, activities, and psycho- large data sets from heterogeneous populations have
logical well-being, both directly and indirectly learned to be grateful for one percent of explained
through environmental satisfaction. All of these fac- variance!
tors were examined while controlling for the basic The kernel of my message, however, is that lack of
background characteristics shown at the far left of predictability from one sector of well-being to
Figure 2. It shows an overall confirmation of this another, even if we reduce measurement error to its
model, with a few misses. The three misses are a lack bare minimum, is the normal state of affairs to be
of environmental influence on time use and the un- expected for the human condition. Specifically I sug-
expected significant paths from family contact to gest that the relative autonomy among sectors is what
friends contact and from cognition to psychological makes normal human existence possible. If a change
well-being. This model represents only the behavior- in one sector were to be immediately reflected in
al competence sector in multidimensional fashion. every other sector, chaotic instability would be the
We need to represent the real complexity of the result. The idea of relative autonomy among systems

Downloaded from http://gerontologist.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Michigan on October 17, 2014


other sectors in similar fashion. is well-known in the biological sciences. For the be-
A partial approach to such an exploration of havioral system each sector of the good life has its
psychological well-being was taken by testing similar own structure and its own legitimacy as a goal for
causal models using positive and negative affect as individuals and for society as a whole. Our usual
the ultimate dependent variables. The two models practice is to look for, expect, and feel disappointed
(explicated further in Lawton, in press) showed, as if we do not obtain huge correlations between our
predicted, different relationships among their ele- measures in different sectors. We must learn to be
ments. Specifically, positive affect, as hypothesized, more discriminating in our expectations, which in
was strongly determined by the outwardly-focused turn will lead to better science.
behaviors (time use and friends interaction) but very It would be possible to cite illustrations of disjunc-
little by health. Conversely, negative affect was tions between every combination of sectors. First let
strongly determined by health but not by time use or us consider a few examples of people whose good
friends interaction, as we had hypothesized. Thus an life in one sector is unmatched in the others:
excellent reason is found for distinguishing between
the positive and negative varieties of psychological 1) High behavioral competence is clearly man-
well-being. ifested in the activist behavior of a Grey Panther. Yet
It is clear that the total amount of predictability of her environment may be deprived, she may be dis-
psychological well-being is relatively low. Even in this satisfied with her milieu, and she may be unable to
truncated model that represents only a few compo- achieve a sense of internal happiness as long as the
nents of the good life, however, relationships among unjust gap between ideal and actuality remains.
the sectors of the good life are evident. Figure 3 is a , 2) Some people can maintain psychological well-
rough attempt to illustrate the relative amount of being even in the face of physical illness and its be-
overlap among the sectors. The relative sizes of the havioral concomitants, an objectively unrewarding
grey areas represent the median correlations be- situation, and a judgment that the world outside one-
self has little to offer one—this is "integrity," in the
Eriksonian (1963) sense.
3) Even if one's competence is eroded, one lives
PSYCHOLOGICAL PERCEIVED where most people would prefer not to live, and
QUALITY one's subjective mental state is negative, there may
WELLBEING nevertheless be some adaptive value in looking on
OF LIFE
the bright side of the domains of perceived quality of
life. Some people in nursing homes give this impres-
sion; when asked how things are, the answer is "It's
fine—I have to like it." I suggest that the liking is not
necessarily false but may be a psychologically posi-
tive way of dealing with what would otherwise be a
worse situation.
4) Perhaps the most familiar disjunctive type is the
behaviorally and psychologically impaired person
BEHAVIORAL OBJECTIVE whose functioning and subjective state even the best
COMPETENCE ENVIRONMENT of objective environments cannot improve.

The same point can be illustrated by thinking of


impairments in one sector that are not reflected in
Figure 3. Four sectors of the good life. others:

Vol.23, No. 4,1983 355


1) Some senile dementia patients whose behavior Now to end this lecture where Kleemeier began, it
is grossly impaired nonetheless appear to be satisfied is worth calling to everyone's attention the fact that
and in good psychological health, perhaps enhanced he probably got as close as any gerontologist has to
by a favorable environment. the elusive self. A substantial portion of his Hand-
2) Depressed people may retain a high level of book chapter is devoted to what he called "the body
behavioral competence, live in the best of environ- as an environmental object" (1959, p. 401). The "body
ments, and evaluate positively every domain of their image" construct was popular at that time and has
daily lives except themselves. been used very effectively as an aid to self-
3) On the other hand, some individuals are chroni- exploration by Butler (1963) in his technique of inter-
cally dissatisfied. Regardless of their own compe- viewing older people before a full-length mirror. Un-
tence, their internally positive psychological well- like the physical environmental p o r t i o n of
being, or the objective quality of their environment, Kleemeier's contribution, which has been taken up
they are hypercritical of some or all domains of their by many of us over the past two decades, the internal
external life. environment has been relatively neglected. It is very
4) Finally, the world is full of older people who live possible that there is as much potential for new be-
in very stressful or very deprived environments, yet havioral knowledge in the exploration of the body as
manage to remain "up" in every other way. a stimulus object to oneself and to others as there has
been in the exploration of the external environment.

Downloaded from http://gerontologist.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Michigan on October 17, 2014


An important corollary of this conclusion regard-
ing intersector autonomy is that one should not insist
on demonstrating that an improvement in one sector
References
of well-being should lead to improvement in another
sector (Carp, 1977; Rosow, 1977; Taylor, 1977). Trans- Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S B. Social indicators of well-being. Plenum Press,
New York, 1976.
lating the good life into social goals, well-being in the Birren, J. E. (Ed.). Handbook of aging and the individual. University of
different sectors might be looked upon as the right to Chicago Press, Chicago, 1959.
health, happiness, satisfaction with daily life, and a Bradburn, N. M. The structure of psychological well-being. Aldine, Chicago,
1969.
good environment. Does it make sense to suggest Butler, R. N. Psychiatric evaluation of the aged. Geriatrics, 1963,18,220-232.
that in order to justify our attempts to improve one of Campbell, A., Converse, P. E., & Rodgers, W. L. The quality of American life:
these sectors we must show that its effects are re- Perceptions, evaluations, and satisfactions. Russell Sage Foundation,
New York, 1976.
flected in another sector? Sometimes we can, but we Carp, F. M. Ego defense or cognitive consistency effects of environmental
should not be surprised or consider our program a evaluation. Journal of Gerontology, 1975, 30, 707-716.
Carp, F. M. Morale: What questions are we asking of whom? In C. Nydegger
failure if we cannot. Above all we must insist on the (Ed.). Measuring morale: A guide to effective assessment. Cerontologi-
right of each person to define his idiosyncratic life cal Society, Washington, DC, 1977.
goals and to eschew the neatness of total congruence Carp, F. & Carp, A. A complementary/congruence model of well-being on
mental health for the community elderly. In I. Alt man, J. F. Wohlwill, &
among elements of the good life if he wishes. M. P. Lawton (Eds.). Human behavior and the environment: Current
As this presentation nears its end, it may seem theory and research (Vol. 7) Plenum Press, New York, in press.
somewhat surprising that this psychologist has never Duke University Center for the Study of Aging. Multidimensional functional
assessment: The OARS methodology (2nd ed.). Duke University,
mentioned self. That is because I saved it for last, not Durham, NC, 1978.
because it is last in importance. The self is where the Erikson, E. H. Childhood and society. Norton, New York, 1963.
Curin, C , Veroff, J., & Feld, S. Americans view their mental health. Basic
ultimate meaning of the four sectors is stored. The Books, New York, 1960.
exercise of behavioral competence builds perceived Curland, B. J., Kuriansky, J., Sharpe, L , Simon, R., Stiller, P., & Berkett, P.
competence. The varieties of psychological well- The comprehensive assessment and referral evaluation (CARE):
Rationale, development, and reliability. International Journal of Aging
being are reflections of the self. Perceived quality of and Human Development, 1977-78, 8, 9-41.
life may be seen as the net of one's evaluation of the Havens, B., & Thompson, E. Aging needs. Assessment schedule. Depart-
congruence between self and the external world. The ment of Health and Community Services, Province of Manitoba, Win-
nipeg, 1976.
objective environment is most distant from the self, Herzog, A. R., & Rodgers, W. L. Age and satisfaction: Data from several large
but where it is salient to the person it may be the surveys. Research on Aging, 1981, 3,142-165.
Kahana, E. A congruence model of person-environment interaction. In M. P.
symbol of her competence and generativity, as in a Lawton, P. C. Windley, &T. O. Byerts (Eds.),Agingandtheenvironment:
long-occupied home. Theoretical approaches. Springer, New York, 1982.
The self, however, is indicated only indirectly and Kates, R. W., & Wohlwill, J. F. (Eds.). Man's response to the physical environ-
ment. Journal of Social Issues, 1966, 22 (Whole No. 4).
in a glancing fashion by the many measurable attri- Kleemeier, R. W. Behavior and the organization of the bodily and the
butes from the sectors of the good life. The self is external environment. In J. E. Birren (Ed.). Handbook of aging and the
inaccessible to measurement directly (misnamed individual. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1959.
Kleemeier, R. W. (Ed.). Aging and leisure. Oxford University Press, New
measures such as self-esteem notwithstanding) but it York, 1961.
may be thought of as the repository of the most Kozma, A., & Stones, M. J. The measurement of happiness. Journal of
meaningful aspects of the good life. If all four sectors Gerontology, 1980, 35, 906-912.
Lawton, M. P. Ecology and aging. In L. A. Pastalan & D. H. Carson (Eds.).
channel into the self, the self in turn thrusts outward, Spatial behavior of older people. Institute of Gerontology, University of
providing the motive force that constantly conditions Michigan, Ann Arbor, 1970.
behavioral competence, psychological well-being, Lawton, M. P. Assessing the competence of older people. In D. Kent, R.
Kastenbaum, & S. Sherwood (Eds.). Research, planning, and action for
perceived quality of life, and the portion of the objec- the elderly, Behavioral Publications, New York, 1972.
tive environment that is actively shaped by the per- Lawton, M. P. The Philadelphia Geriatric Center Morale Scale: A revision.
son. Perhaps the small dark area of Fig. 3 where all Journal of Gerontology, 1975, 30, 85-89.
Lawton, M. P. Methodologies for evaluation in environment and aging. In P.
four sectors overlap is one way to represent the self. Suedfeld,'J. A. Russell, L. M. Ward, F. Szigeti, & G. Davis (Eds.). The

356 The Gerontologist


behavioral basis of design (Vol. 2). Dowden, Hutchinson, and Ross, well-being of elderly tenants in federally-assisted housing. Journal of
Stroudsburg, PA, 1977. Gerontology, 1975, 30, 601-607.
Lawton, M. P. What is the good life for the aging? Address delivered for the Lawton, M. P., Nahemow, L., & Yeh, T.-M. Neighborhood environment and
Kesten Award, University of Southern California Ethel Percy Andrus the well-being of older tenants in planned housing. International Journal
Center, Los Angeles, 1978. of Aging and Human Development, 1980, 11, 211-227.
Lawton, M. P. Residential quality and residential satisfaction among the Liang, j . , & Bollen, K. A. The structure of the Philadelphia Ceriatric Center
elderly. Research on Aging, 1980, 2, 309-328. Morale Scale: A reinterpretation. Journal of Gerontology, 1983,38,181-
Lawton, M. P. Competence, environmental press, and the adaptation of 189.
older people. In M. P. Lawton, P. C. Windley, &T. O. Byerts (Eds.). Aging Liang, J., Dvorkin, L., Kahana, E., & Mazian, F. Social integration and morale:
and the environment: Theoretical approaches. Springer, New York, A re-examination. Journal of Gerontology, 1980, 35, 746-757.
1982. (a) Lindsley, O. R. Geriatric behavioral prosthetics. In R. Kastenbaum (Ed.).
Lawton, M. P. The well-being and mental health of the aged. In T. Field, A. New thoughts on old age. Springer, New York, 1964.
Stein, H. Quay, L. Troll, & G. E. Finley (Eds.). Review of Human Develop- Moos, R. H., & Lemke, S. Assessing the physical and architectural features of
ment. Wiley-lnterscience, New York, 1982. (b) sheltered care settings. Journal of Gerontology, 1980, 35, 571-583.
Lawton, M. P. The dimensions of well-being. Experimental Aging Research, Neugarten, B. L., Havighurst, R. J., & Tobin, S. S. The measurement of life
in press. satisfaction. Journal of Gerontology, 1961, 76,134-143.
Lawton, M. P., & Cohen, J. Environment and the well-being of elderly inner Rosenberg, M. Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton University
city residents. Environment and Behavior, 1974, 6,194-211. (a)
Press, Princeton, NJ, 1965.
Lawton, M. P., & Cohen, J. The generality of housing impact on the well-
Rosow, I. Morale: Concept and measurement. In C. Nydegger (Ed.).
being of older people. Journal of Gerontology, 1974, 29,194-204. (b)
Measuring morale: A guide to effective assessment. Gerontological Soci-
Lawton, M. P., & Kleban, M. H. Psychological well-being in the aged: Facto-
ety, Washington, DC, 1977.
rial and conceptual dimensions. Philadelphia Ceriatric Center, 1982.
Taylor, C. Why measure morale? In C. Nydegger (Ed.). Measuring morale: A
Lawton, M. P., Moss, M., Fulcomer, M., & Kleban, M. H. A research and
guide to effective assessment. Gerontological Society, Washington, DC,

Downloaded from http://gerontologist.oxfordjournals.org/ at University of Michigan on October 17, 2014


service-oriented Multilevel Assessment Instrument. Journal of Geron-
tology, 1982, 37, 91-99. 1977.
Lawton, M. P., & Nahemow, L. Ecology and the aging process. In C. Eisdorfer Teaff, J. D., Lawton, M. P., Nahemow, L., & Carlson, D. Impact of age
& M. P. Lawton (Eds.), The psychology of adult development and aging. integration on the well-being of elderly tenants in public housing. Jour-
American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, 1973. nal of Gerontology, 1978, 33,126-133.
Lawton, M. P., & Nahemow, L. Social areas and the well-being of tenants in U.S. Bureau of the Census. Annual Housing Survey: 1975. Indicators of
planned housing for the elderly. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 1979, housing and neighborhood quality. U.S. Government Printing Office,
14, 463-484. (a) Washington, DC, 1977.
Lawton, M. P., & Nahemow, L. Social science methods for evaluating the U.S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. Toward a social report.
quality of housing for the elderly. Journal of Architectural Research, U.S. Government Printing Office, Washington, DC, 1969.
1979,7,5-11. (b) Windley, P. G., & Scheidt, R. J. An ecological model of mental health among
Lawton, M. P., Nahemow, L., & Teaff, J. Housing characteristics and the small-town elderly. Journal of Gerontology, 1982, 37, 235-242.

I IT Chicago-Kent College of Law


is pleased to announce the opening of

The Law and Aging Library


The Law and Aging Library the collection is available nation-
provides a unique resource that wide through the national inter-
has been developed and will be library loan program. The sev-
maintained for the purposes of: eral-hundred-page catalogue of
• Enhancing the legal commu- the collection, to be periodically
nity's awareness and under- updated, may be purchased at
standing of gerontological cost.
issues and of an aging For further information, write to
clientele or call:
Howard Eglit, Director
• Enhancing the social sciences 312/567-5037
community's understanding of
how the Constitution, statutes, Georgia Strohm, Chief Librarian
regulations, and case law 312/567-3147
apply to the lives and problems
of the elderly and of those
who interact with them.
Initially funded by a grant from
the Retirement Research Foun- I IT Chicago-Kent College of Law
77 South Wacker Drive
dation, The Law and Aging
Chicago, Illinois 60606
Library—the first of its kind in the
nation—includes more than
1,500 books and journals, and it
will continue to grow. Access to

Vol.23, No. 4,1983 357

You might also like