You are on page 1of 13

Received: 22 November 2020 Accepted: 20 June 2022

DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12764

RESEARCH REPORT

Phonological processing skills in children with speech


sound disorder: A multiple case study approach

Sherine R. Tambyraja1 Kelly Farquharson2 Laura M. Justice1

1 CraneCenter for Early Childhood


Research and Policy, The Ohio State Abstract
University, Columbus, OH, USA Background: Children with speech sound disorder (SSD) are at increased risk
2 School of Communication Science and
of reading difficulties due to poor phonological processing skills. However, the
Disorders, Florida State University,
Tallahassee, FL, USA extent to which children with SSD demonstrate weaknesses on specific or all
phonological processing tasks is not well understood.
Correspondence
Aims: To examine the phonological processing abilities of a clinically identified
Sherine R. Tambyraja, Ohio Department
of Education, 25 S. Front Street, 6th floor, sample of children with SSD, with and without reading difficulties. To determine
Columbus, OH 43215, USA. the extent to which the proportion of children with concomitant SSD and reading
Email: stambyraja@yahoo.com
difficulties exhibited weaknesses in specific areas of phonological processing, or
Funding information a more general phonological deficit.
Institute of Education Sciences, Methods & Procedures: Data were obtained from 157 school-aged children
Grant/Award Number: R324A090012;
This research was supported by Grant,
(Mage = 77 months, SD = 7.01) in receipt of school-based speech therapy. Approx-
Grant/Award Number: R324A090012; U.S. imately 25% of the sample was identified as poor readers, based on a standardized
Department of Education, Institute of measure of word decoding. We compared the proportion of children who scored
Education Sciences, awarded to Laura M.
Justice, and an internal seed grant from at or below 1 SD below the mean, or the bottom 16th percentile, on measures
The Crane Center for Early Childhood of phonological awareness, rapid automatized naming and verbal short-term
Research and Policy, awarded to Sherine
memory among those identified as poor readers and good readers.
R. Tambyraja
Outcomes & Results: Children with SSD demonstrated a range of phonological
processing difficulties, particularly on the measure of verbal short-term memory.
No specific skill differentiated groups of children with SSD with and without
reading difficulties; however, those classified as poor readers on the word-
decoding measure exhibited more widespread difficulties, even after controlling
for language ability.
Conclusions & Implications: Results support a cumulative risk model such
that children with SSD and reading difficulties are likely to demonstrate
generally poor phonological processing abilities.

KEYWORDS
phonological processing, reading, speech sound disorder

This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and distribution in any medium,
provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non-commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made.
© 2022 The Authors. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of Royal College of Speech and Language
Therapists.

Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2023;58:15–27. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jlcd 15


14606984, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1460-6984.12764 by Universidade Do Algarve, Wiley Online Library on [18/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
16 PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING SKILLS IN CHILDREN

What this paper adds


What is already known on the subject
∙ Children with SSD are at heightened risk of reading difficulties, particularly if
their SSD persists into school age. However, not all children with SSD experi-
ence reading problems. Research aimed at determining which children are at
the highest risk is mixed as to how best to identify which children with SSD
are most likely to experience reading difficulties.

What this paper adds to existing knowledge


∙ The study used a multiple case study approach to determine if performance
on phonological processing skills might differentiate children with SSD who
were poor readers from those who were good readers. As a group, children
with SSD exhibited poor verbal short-term memory but relatively intact rapid
automatized naming skills. No one phonological processing skill differentiated
children who were poor readers from good readers. However, children with
reading difficulties appeared to experience more general difficulties across
phonological processing tasks, even after controlling for language abilities.

What are the potential or actual clinical implications of this work?


∙ A single deficit (i.e., speech sound production) is not sufficient data to make a
complete diagnosis or treatment decisions. Multiple sources of data, including
several aspects of phonological processing, should be obtained to understand
reading risk in children with SSD.

INTRODUCTION ficulties for children with SSD has been somewhat mixed.
For example, although some studies suggest that risks for
The diagnosis of speech sound disorder (SSD) is conferred reading difficulties are highest for children with SSD and
on children who exhibit a broad spectrum of difficulty with concomitant language impairment (LI) (Peterson et al.,
speech sound production, and constitutes a heterogeneous 2009; Sices et al., 2007), recent evidence indicates that
group with respect to the nature of their disorder, the types variation in reading abilities among school-age children
of speech errors they produce and the severity of their with SSD may be more closely related to phonological
unintelligibility (Dodd, 2014; Macrae & Tyler, 2014; Pre- skills (e.g., Burgoyne et al., 2019; Tambyraja et al., 2020),
ston & Edwards, 2010; Waring & Knight, 2013). Increasing regardless of language ability. This explanation aligns with
evidence suggests that regardless of the underlying cause, prevailing theories of reading development in that strong
children with SSD, including those with idiopathic SSD, phonological skills are a prerequisite for the word decod-
are at heightened risk of subsequent reading problems ing process of reading (e.g., Gough & Tunmer, 1986), which
(Cabbage et al., 2018; Lewis et al., 2011, 2015; Raitano et al., subsequently relates to reading comprehension. Linkages
2004), particularly if their speech difficulties are present between SSD and word decoding difficulties are also in
at school entry, when formal reading instruction begins line with speech processing theories in that speech diffi-
(Burgoyne et al., 2019). culties are just one manifestation of a generally impaired
This is an especially salient issue for speech and lan- phonological system in children with SSD (e.g., Stack-
guage pathologists (SLPs) serving school-age children, house & Wells, 1997), which extends to the word decoding
who must often simultaneously support children’s speech, process of reading. Considered altogether, it might be
language and reading skills within the context of therapy concluded that children with SSD, with and without LI,
(American Speech–Language–Hearing Association, 2001). are at risk of reading difficulties, but that children with
Research focused on determining the extent of reading dif- poor phonological processing skills are at highest risk.
14606984, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1460-6984.12764 by Universidade Do Algarve, Wiley Online Library on [18/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
TAMBYRAJA et al. 17

Thus, examining children’s phonological processing abil- Sutherland & Gillon, 2007), even among older children
ities, regardless of diagnostic status, may be particularly whose speech difficulties have resolved (e.g., Peterson
informative for identifying children with poor reading et al., 2009). Thus, although a large proportion of chil-
skills. dren with SSD have poor PA which will subsequently
To date, data concerning reading difficulties among impact their reading ability (Burgoyne et al., 2019; Tam-
school-age children with SSD who receive speech– byraja et al., 2020), the extent to which difficulties in PA
language therapy are relatively sparse, thus limiting the specifically differentiates those with and without reading
ability to understand the connections between phono- difficulties remains unclear.
logical processing and word decoding (hereafter used
interchangeably with reading), which has both theoretical
and clinical implications. Indeed, some studies have Rapid automatized naming (RAN)
supported the idea of the ‘critical age hypothesis’, and that
children whose SSD remains present at school entry are at Some studies suggest that RAN requires accessing and pro-
greatest risk of reading difficulties (e.g., Burgoyne et al., cessing stored phonological information from long-term
2019; Nathan et al., 2004), yet there is limited research memory (Wagner & Torgesen, 1987). RAN tasks measure
to guide clinical practice in identifying who will require the speed at which children retrieve and name the lexi-
reading support. As such, the present study determines cal codes associated with known items, such as colours,
the extent to which performance on three unique phono- objects or letters; thus, slow RAN times are thought to
logical processing skills may help to ascertain which reflect an inefficient timing process when children are
clinically identified children with SSD are at increased asked to quickly associate specific visual stimuli and
risk of reading difficulties. phonological codes (Wolf & Bowers, 1999). RAN is associ-
ated with word decoding skills in TD children (e.g., Clarke
et al., 2005; Manis et al., 1999), yet some research sug-
Word decoding: Links to phonological gests these connections are not evident in children with
processing LI and/or SSD (e.g., Burgoyne et al., 2019; Tambyraja
et al., 2015). Theoretical explanations for the relationship
Theories of reading acquisition converge on the notion between RAN and decoding differ, yet some empirical
that learning to decode words is largely contingent on evidence indicates that RAN accounts for significant and
children’s phonological processing abilities (e.g., Gough & unique variance in decoding ability above and beyond PA
Tunmer, 1986; Wagner & Torgesen, 1987; Wagner et al., (Clarke et al., 2005; Schatschneider et al., 2004). In con-
1994). According to the phonological processing theory of trast, Burgoyne et al. (2019) found that although children
reading (Wagner & Torgesen, 1987), for example, decoding with SSD had poorer PA and decoding skills compared
printed words requires three primary phonologically ori- with TD children, there were no group differences on a
ented skills, namely: phonological awareness (PA), rapid measure of RAN. However, given the relations between
automatized naming (RAN) and verbal short-term mem- RAN and decoding in TD children, and the available
ory (vSTM). The present study focuses on these three evidence suggesting it is a relatively intact phonological
phonological processing skills and examines the extent to processing skill for children with SSD, it is possible that
which performance on any one, or combination, differen- RAN tasks may help identify which children with SSD are
tiates children with SSD who have reading difficulties from likely to experience reading difficulties.
those whose reading skills are within normal limits.

Verbal short-term memory (vSTM)


Phonological awareness (PA)
vSTM refers to the short-term memory storage of auditorily
PA refers to an awareness of the sound structures of presented information. It is the mechanism readers use to
language (Whitehurst & Lonigan, 1998). It, particularly maintain the memory of the sounds of decoded graphemes,
phoneme awareness, is positively associated with word while continuing to decode later graphemes in a word (e.g.,
decoding ability (e.g., Lonigan et al., 2009; Schatschneider Gathercole et al., 1991). Thus, it is a phonological process-
et al., 2004; Strattman & Hodson, 2005) and the relation ing skill that is independent from PA and RAN. However,
appears to be causal (Castles & Coltheart, 2004; Ehri et al., some studies indicate that measures of vSTM and PA tap
2001, Wagner et al., 1994). Children with SSD perform the same underlying phonological skill (e.g., Chow et al.,
more poorly on measures of PA compared with typically 2005) and do not individually account for unique vari-
developing (TD) children (Rvachew & Grawburg, 2006; ance in word decoding (Muter & Snowling, 1998). Previous
14606984, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1460-6984.12764 by Universidade Do Algarve, Wiley Online Library on [18/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
18 PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING SKILLS IN CHILDREN

work has found that children with SSD have difficulties dyslexia (n = 16) to determine whether their primary
with vSTM tasks (e.g., Couture & McCauley, 2000; Lewis weaknesses supported a phonological processing theory of
et al., 2011). An fMRI study similarly found that the neu- dyslexia, an auditory/visual theory or a cerebellar theory.
ral regions activated during phonological memory tasks All adults with dyslexia exhibited phonological difficulties,
are different in children with SSD compared with children whereas only some exhibited auditory, visual and/or motor
with typical speech production abilities (Tkach et al., 2011). deficits. Carroll et al. (2016) used a multiple case study
Overall, research generally indicates that children with approach to determine how many children with dyslexia
SSD exhibit poorer phonological processing skills com- might be classified as demonstrating deficits, based on
pared with TD children. However, there remain relatively measure cut-points, on a battery of print, motor and
limited data to determine whether children with SSD who phonological tasks. There was no specific task on which
experience reading difficulties demonstrate difficulties all children classified as poor readers met the criteria for
across all phonological processing skills, or if difficulties a deficit, rather they demonstrated a large constellation of
with one or more specific phonological processing skills associated difficulties. The present study adopts the mul-
may be particularly associated with poor reading skills. tiple case study approach to further our understanding of
Even fewer studies have examined clinically representa- how to differentiate children with SSD who exhibit read-
tive samples to understand the rate of reading difficulties ing difficulties from those who do not, based on their
among school-age children who meet criteria for SSD, and phonological processing abilities.
are thus a heterogenous group, as they may have additional
diagnoses as well. This is an important clinical considera- Purpose of the current study
tion, as SLPs often have time and resource constraints to
meet the multiple needs of children on their caseloads. To sum up, the present study contributes to a small but
To that end, this study seeks to determine the extent to growing body of literature focused on clarifying the asso-
which specific phonological processing skills may be clin- ciations among SSD, phonological processing and reading.
ically useful for identifying children with SSD at risk of Increasing evidence suggests that school-age children with
concomitant reading difficulties. SSD are at heightened risk of reading difficulties, par-
ticularly decoding, but the extent to which that risk is
due to a generally impaired phonological system or can
A multiple case study approach be attributed to specific phonological processing skills
remains unclear. Further, the extent to which impairments
Historically, researchers have used variable-centred in one or multiple phonological processing skills may
approaches (e.g., linear regression) to understand the relate to a child’s cumulative risk status is also of interest.
skills and characteristics significantly associated with We address the following questions:
reading ability in children with SSD. These methods are
useful for identifying significant predictors of reading ∙ What proportion of children with SSD experience diffi-
ability, but do not account for individual variation within culties in PA, RAN and/or vSTM, and do those propor-
heterogeneous groups (Murphy & Farquharson, 2016; tions vary for children who are identified as poor or good
Tambyraja et al., 2015, 2018). In the present study, we take readers?
a person-centred approach to expand our understanding ∙ What proportion of children in each reading subgroup
of the most prevalent types of phonological processing (poor readers, good readers) exhibit difficulties in mul-
difficulties in children with SSD, and the extent to which tiple areas, and are there difficulties that cluster together
difficulties in specific aspects of phonological processing more frequently in either or both subgroups?
may differentiate children with SSD who have reading ∙ To what extent is the number of phonological pro-
difficulties from those who do not. cessing difficulties associated with reading difficulties
One method that has been used to understand which in children with SSD when controlling for language
skills areas underlie reading difficulties is a multiple case abilities?
study (e.g., Carroll et al., 2016; Ramus et al., 2003). Multiple
case study approaches apply a threshold (i.e., cut-point) on
measures to reflect a deficit (e.g., more than 1 SD (standard METHODS
deviation) below the mean) and determine the propor-
tion of individuals who exhibit difficulties on the selected Participants
measures, or to identify patterns of difficulties within a
specified group of individuals. For example, Ramus et al. Participants were 157 children with SSD who were
(2003) administered an assessment battery to adults with receiving school-based speech–language therapy in public
14606984, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1460-6984.12764 by Universidade Do Algarve, Wiley Online Library on [18/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
TAMBYRAJA et al. 19

schools in the Midwest and Southeastern regions of the Procedures


United States. Participants were drawn from a subset of
children participating in a larger study examining cur- Informed consent and all study procedures were approved
rent practices in school-based speech–language therapy by the Institutional Review Board at The Ohio State Uni-
sessions. Eligibility for the larger study required that versity. Consented children participated in a battery of
children: (1) were in kindergarten, first or second grade standardized language and literacy assessments at the
(i.e., between the ages of 5 and 8); (2) were receiv- beginning (timepoint 1) and end (timepoint 2) of the aca-
ing school-based speech–language therapy; (3) did not demic year. For the present investigation, only timepoint
have hearing loss or a severe cognitive impairment that 1 measures are included. All assessments were adminis-
would impact their ability to complete study tasks; and tered individually in a quiet room by trained field assessors.
(4) primarily communicated in English. For information Training for field assessors included: (1) a review of assess-
about the complete sample in the larger study, includ- ment manual and procedures, (2) completion of an online
ing recruitment processes, see Tambyraja et al. (2015) module about the measure and a score of 100% on an
(year). administration quiz, (3) completion of a mock assess-
As part of the larger study, we obtained the individ- ment and (4) live observation of the assessor’s initial test
ualized education plan (IEP) of each participating child. administration by the project manager.
The IEPs were coded according to the therapy targets
within goals (e.g., vocabulary, grammar, fluency, articu-
lation), which were then used to select children for the Measures
current study. Therefore, we operationalized SSD to reflect
current clinical practice such that the children selected Several measures were used to address the study’s main
for the current investigation had at least one IEP goal tar- research questions. In addition to the key outcome mea-
geting speech sound production, as determined by their sures detailed below, children were also administered
school-based SLP (e.g., Student will produce /s/ blends and the Core Language subtests of the Clinical Evaluation
/l/ blends at the beginning of words when naming pictures and Language Fundamentals—Fourth Edition (CELF-4;
with 90% accuracy for two consecutive sessions) and had Semel et al., 2003) as an index of spoken language, and the
completed the assessment of decoding in the beginning Matrices subtest of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test—
of the academic year. Of note, the larger study primar- Second Edition (KBIT-2; Kaufman & Kaufman, 2004) as
ily focused on children with LI. Thus, the present sample an index of non-verbal intelligence. These measures were
of 157 included a large proportion of children who had used to characterize children’s overall language and non-
both language and speech/articulation therapy goals (n verbal intelligence abilities and used as control variables
= 106), and a smaller proportion of children whose IEP in some analyses. Reliability coefficients for the Core Lan-
had only speech sound goals (n = 51). Despite this distinc- guage subtests of the CELF-4 range from 0.71 to 0.86 and
tion related to therapy goals, analyses were conducted with from 0.78 to 0.87 for the KBIT-2.
the entire sample as a whole in order to provide informa-
tion that directly reflects a representative clinical sample.
Furthermore, because of the larger study’s focus on exam- Word decoding
ining services for children with LI, the testing protocol did
not include a standardized assessment of children’s speech Children’s reading ability was measured with the Word
sound skills. Thus, further information about the specific Attack (WA) subtest of the Woodcock–Johnson III Tests
nature of children’s speech sound difficulties was also not of Achievement (Woodcock et al., 2001). The Word Attack
available. However, a narrative language sample for each subtest includes lists of non-words that increase in com-
child was transcribed and the total per cent consonants cor- plexity; children were required to decode the non-words.
rect (PCC) was calculated for all children selected for this The number of non-words accurately decoded represented
study. See Tambyraja et al. (2020) for a full description of the raw score; this was converted into a standard score,
this process. which was used in analyses. Using the multiple case study
Table 1 includes additional descriptive data for predic- approach, children whose standard score was less than
tor variables, including information on children’s age, sex, 85 (i.e., 1 SD or more below the mean) were classified as
socio-economic status (SES), race and ethnicity. Descrip- poor readers. In the current sample, 44 children (28%) were
tive data regarding children’s language and PCC are shown considered to be poor readers. Test–retest reliability for
in Table 2. children aged 4–7 years for the Word Attack subtest is 0.79.
14606984, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1460-6984.12764 by Universidade Do Algarve, Wiley Online Library on [18/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
20 PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING SKILLS IN CHILDREN

TA B L E 1 Participant characteristics and demographic information (n = 157)


Variable n %
Child age (months)
Mean 77.01
SD 7.77
Range 60–96
Grade
Kindergarten 49 31.2%
First grade 103 65.6%
Second grade 5 3.2%
Gender
Male 105 66.9%
Female 52 33.1%
Highest level of maternal education
Less than high school 13 8.3%
High school graduate 17 10.8
Some college 36 22.9%
Associates degree 19 12.1%
College graduate or higher 43 27.4%
Not reported 29 18.5%
Race/ethnicity
White/non-Hispanic Caucasian 93 59.2%
Hispanic 8 5.1%
Black/African-American 10 6.4%
Asian 3 1.8%
Other 8 5.1%
Not reported 35 22.3%
Note: SD, standard deviation.

RAN PA

RAN ability was tested using the Shapes and Colours sub- The Catts Deletion Task was used to evaluate children’s
test of CELF-4 (Semel et al., 2003). This subtest required PA abilities (Catts et al., 2001). In this task, children are
that children name visually presented shapes, colours presented with a target word and are asked to delete
and shape–colour combinations as quickly as possible. phonological segments that vary in size (word, syllable,
Although other types of RAN tasks (e.g., letters, digits) phoneme; ‘say baseball without base’). This task was cho-
are sometimes used, research suggests that all RAN tasks sen because it has been shown to be strongly predictive of
require overall similar processing skills (Cummine et al., reading ability (e.g., Torgesen et al., 1994). A binary scor-
2014). Children were timed as they named the colours and ing approach (i.e., correct/incorrect) was applied to all 21
shapes (e.g., blue circle), presented on a 6 × 6 grid. The items. Raw scores were used in the analyses. Children who
total number of errors paired with time (s) is used to deter- scored in the bottom 16% were classified as having a PA
mine a score. Criterion scores from the publisher’s manual deficit (Catts et al., 2005). Cronbach’s alpha for this sample
(Semel et al., 2003) were used to rate performance as nor- was 0.69.
mal, non-normal and slower than normal. Children who
were classified with a performance level that was non- vSTM
normal or slower than normal were considered to have a
RAN deficit. Test–retest reliability for RAN naming time is vSTM was assessed using the Number Repetition sub-
0.87. test from CELF-4 (Semel et al., 2003). This subtest was
14606984, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1460-6984.12764 by Universidade Do Algarve, Wiley Online Library on [18/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
TAMBYRAJA et al. 21

administered as part of the full battery of assessments in

SS = standard score; Language = Core Language subtests of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—4th Edition (CELF-4); Non-verbal IQ = Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test—Second Edition (KBIT-2).
the larger study; previous research suggests this task is
associated is often used as a measure of vSTM and is signif-
Effect size icantly correlated with other memory tasks, such as those
involving non-word recall (Cain et al., 2004; Rispins &

0.43
1.05
0.69

0.27
0.73

0.75
0.21
Baker, 2012). In this subtest, children are auditorily pre-
sented with a list of digits, which increase by one as the
trials progress. Children are asked to repeat the list of digits
in forwarding order. Raw scores were converted to stan-
dard scores (M = 10, SD = 3), which were used in the
Descriptive statistics for phonological processing variables and effect sizes for differences between the poor reader and good reader subgroups

analyses. Consistent with using –1 SD from the mean as


Good readers (n = 113)

a cut score, children with standard scores below 7 were


determined to have a deficit in vSTM. Internal consistency
reliability for this subtest was 0.79. Descriptive statistics
78.59 (15.67)

92.71 (11.84)
108.11 (41.55)
75.56 (7.60)

8.39 (7.63)
Mean (SD)

5.37 (1.75)

for all variables, as well as their PCC, including the effect


86.78 (7.5)

size from mean score differences between good and poor


readers on all variables, are presented in Table 2.

RESULTS

Phonological processing skills in poor and


good readers
Poor readers (n = 44)

The first aim of this study was to determine the propor-


133.41 (72.66)
62.23 (15.41)

tion of children with SSD who experience difficulties in


80.72 (7.00)

4.30 (1.90)

84.34 (9.41)
Mean (SD)

3.23 (5.13)
85.17 (7.5)

PA, RAN and/or vSTM, and the extent to which those pro-
portions differed for children classified as good or poor
readers. As described above, cut scores were created for
each measure to identify children who exhibited difficulty
on each task, as determined by performing below to cut
score criterion. Descriptive data in Table 2 indicate large
differences in measures of interest between the groups of
poor and good readers. The proportion of children who
Total sample (n = 157)

performed poorly on each phonological processing vari-


able for the entire sample, as well as for the good and poor
reader subgroups, are presented in Table 3. Chi-square
115.20 (53.15)

90.36 (11.79)
74.01 (17.21)
Mean (SD)

6.94 (7.38)
77.01 (7.78)

5.07 (1.85)
86.33 (7.5)

analyses confirmed that the proportion of children who


exhibited deficits across all three phonological process-
ing measures was significantly greater in the poor readers
Notes: SD, standard deviation; Effect size, Hedges’ g.

subgroup. Also, as a group, children with SSD exhib-


ited notable difficulty with the vSTM task, according to
Verbal short-term memory (vSTM) (raw

age-standardized performance.
Rapid automatized naming (RAN)
Phonological awareness (PA) (raw

*p = 0.01, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.


Per cent consonants correct

Children with multiple difficulties


Non-verbal IQ (ss)***

The second research question determined whether chil-


Language (ss)***

dren with SSD demonstrated difficulties in any one par-


Age (months)

(seconds)*
score)***

ticular area, or any specific combinations of, phonological


score)**
TA B L E 2

Variable

processing. As seen in Table 4, the total sample of clinically


identified children with SSD did not appear to demonstrate
difficulties on the RAN task; however, overall performance
14606984, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1460-6984.12764 by Universidade Do Algarve, Wiley Online Library on [18/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
22 PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING SKILLS IN CHILDREN

T A B L E 3 Chi-square analyses of the number (percentage) of children scoring below 1 SD from the mean on the PA and short-term
memory measures and below normal criterion on the RAN measure
Variable Total sample (n = 157) Poor readers (n = 44) Good readers (n = 113) χ2 p-value
Phonological awareness (PA) 79 (50%) 32 (73%) 47 (42%) 12.28 0.001
Rapid automatized naming (RAN) 33 (21%) 15 (34%) 18 (16%) 10.39 0.012
Verbal short-term memory (vSTM) 97 (62%) 36 (82%) 61 (54%) 6.29 0.001

T A B L E 4 Number (percentage) of children exhibiting deficits in each individual phonological processing area (and combinations
thereof) in the total sample, and among those who are (a) good readers and (b) poor readers
Phonological processing skill Total sample (n = 157) Poor readers (n = 44) Good readers (n = 113)
Phonological awareness (PA) 14 (9%) 3 (7%) 11 (10%)
Rapid automatized naming (RAN) 5 (3%) 1 (2.3%) 4 (3.5%)
Verbal short-term memory (vSTM) 31 (20%) 8 (18%) 23 (20.5%)
PA + RAN 4 (2.5%) 1 (2.3%) 3 (3%)
PA + vSTM 43 (27%) 16 (36%) 27 (24%)
RAN + vSTM 6 (4%) 1 (2.3%) 5 (4%)
All three 20 (13%) 14 (32%) 6 (5%)
No risk areas 34 (23.5%) 0 34 (30%)

on the PA and vSTM tasks were much poorer. However, tinuous variables, rather than further dichotomizing the
as Table 4 also shows, the subgroup of children classified sample.
as poor readers were more likely to experience difficulties The initial model was significant, χ2 (3) = 36.75, p <
in all three areas compared with those classified as good 0.001, Nagelkerke R2 = 0.30; oral language ability (B =
readers. Furthermore, children classified as poor readers –0.055, p < 0.001) and non-verbal IQ (B = –0.050, p
demonstrated difficulties in at least one phonological pro- = 0.019) were significantly associated with poor reader
cessing area, whereas 30% of children classified as good status. These results suggest that children with stronger
readers did not exhibit difficulties in any of the three language skills and higher non-verbal IQ were less likely to
phonological processing skills. be categorized as poor readers. The number of phonologi-
cal processing difficulties that each child experienced was
added in the next step as a categorical variable (Table 5).
Predicting reading status from the number The final hierarchical logistic regression model, includ-
of difficulties ing all variables was statistically significant, (χ2 = 42.23,
p < 0.001, –2 LL = 144.04). The final model explained
Because data from the previous research question deter- 33.9% (Nagelkerke R2 ) of the variance in reading status
mined that this group of children with SSD did not appear and correctly classified 80% of cases. The final model indi-
to have significant RAN deficits, but did demonstrate dif- cated that children demonstrating difficulties in all three
ficulties with PA and vSTM, it was of interest to know phonological processing areas, based on cut-points, were
whether the combination of deficits in two or three areas significantly more likely to be classified as poor readers
would account for significant variance in poor reader sta- compared with children demonstrating difficulties in one
tus. A hierarchical logistic regression, using SPSS software or two areas of phonological processing. Notably, these
(version 27.0), ascertained the extent to which the total results show that the number of phonological processing
number of factors for which children exhibited deficits abilities accounted for significant variance in poor reader
(i.e., one, two or three) would be significantly associated status above and beyond language skills.
with classification as a poor reader, while controlling for
children’s language ability and non-verbal intelligence, as
this sample of children with SSD demonstrated a range of DISCUSSION
language skills and non-verbal IQ. Although these vari-
ables were not included as control variables in earlier This study contributes to our understanding of the types
analyses, the logistic regression approach allowed us to of phonological processing difficulties that school-age chil-
do so and include language and non-verbal IQ as con- dren with SSD, with and without reading problems, are
14606984, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1460-6984.12764 by Universidade Do Algarve, Wiley Online Library on [18/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
TAMBYRAJA et al. 23

TA B L E 5 Hierarchical logistic regression predicting poor readers from the number of phonological processing risk
Variable B Wald Exp(β) p-value R2 change
Step 1 0.300
Language –0.055 16.049 0.947 0.000
Non-verbal IQ –0.050 5.486 0.951 0.091
Step 2 0.039
Language –0.048 8.786 0.953 0.003
Non-verbal IQ –0.046 4.552 0.955 0.033
1 PP difficulty –1.649 3.411 0.192 0.065
2 PP difficulties –1.185 3.015 0.306 0.083
3 PP difficulties –1.339 4.396 0.262 0.036
Constant 7.633 14.101 2066.08 0.000
Note: Language = Core Language subtests of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—4th Edition (CELF-4); Non-verbal IQ = Matrices subtest of the
Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test—Second Edition (KBIT-2). PP = phonological processing.

likely to demonstrate. An important and novel aspect of quharson, 2019) and that greater consideration should be
this work is that our sample was clinically identified and given to assessing a wider array of phonological processing
included children who were currently in receipt of school- abilities.
based services for their SSD. Thus, although our results Similar heterogeneity in underlying phonological skills
largely align with previous literature, we offer a contempo- is found in other children with communication disorders
rary and clinically impactful perspective on this population as well. For example, Gray et al. (2019) administered a
of children. Specifically, (1) children with SSD are hetero- battery of working memory tasks to four distinct groups
geneous with respect to reading ability, (2) performance of second-graders: TD, dyslexia, developmental language
on phonological processing measures may differentiate disorder, or both dyslexia and developmental language dis-
good and poor readers, and (3) children with SSD and order. These researchers identified four unique working
reading difficulties experience more widespread difficul- memory profiles ranging from low to high abilities. Chil-
ties across phonological processing tasks compared with dren from all four diagnostic groups were present across
children with SSD and age-appropriate reading abilities, each of the four profiles. Thus, even children with dyslexia,
even after accounting for language skills. who are often identified on the basis of their weak phono-
First, our results support that children with SSDs are logical processing skills, had variable working memory
heterogeneous with respect to their reading status, lan- abilities. The results from Gray et al. (2019) and from
guage abilities and phonological processing skills (e.g., the current investigation, when taken together, are rather
Dodd, 2014; Macrae & Tyler, 2014). Approximately one- surprising: children with disorders that are largely classi-
third of the participants in this study were identified as fied by their ability to use or understand the phonological
being poor readers. Descriptive data suggested that chil- system (i.e., dyslexia and SSD, respectively) do not have
dren with SSD who were poor readers were older, on consistently low performance on phonological processing
average, and had significantly lower scores on all mea- tasks. That is, their underlying phonological processing
sures of phonological processing compared with children abilities may not translate into the expected performance
with SSD who were good readers. Further, although this for their diagnosis. In the present study, it is possible
particular study included a large number of children with that the observed heterogeneity in phonological process-
therapy goals related to both language and speech, even ing skills was in part due to the intervention they received.
those who only had speech-related goals still demonstrated As described above, all participants in the current study
relatively poor language skills. Thus, this sample is rep- were in current receipt of speech–language therapy, with
resentative of children receiving services in school-based potentially variable therapy targets. However, these data,
settings, making this investigation ecologically valid. Of in conjunction with previous work (e.g., Gray et al., 2019),
particular interest, however, children with SSD who were suggest that it can be difficult to ascertain which chil-
poor readers did not exhibit significantly poorer speech dren with SSD, with and without concomitant language
sound production abilities, as measured by PCC, com- difficulties, may be at risk of reading difficulties based on
pared with those who were good readers. Given that this performance on one task.
data is taken from a clinically identified sample, this Second, study results showed that PA and vSTM, but
indicates that many school-age children may not exhibit not RAN, were particularly challenging tasks for children
‘simple’ articulation issues or ‘just’ articulation issues (Far- with SSDs. Nearly 62% of children in our sample performed
14606984, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1460-6984.12764 by Universidade Do Algarve, Wiley Online Library on [18/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
24 PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING SKILLS IN CHILDREN

poorly on the measure of vSTM, including some who had have. For example, although our measure of vSTM (i.e.,
age-appropriate decoding abilities. This result is somewhat number recall) was static, other types of working mem-
of a paradox, as poor vSTM is often a strong predictor of ory tasks are dynamic which may provide a more accurate
poor reading abilities (e.g., Carroll et al., 2016). As such, it assessment of the ‘working’ part of working memory (Far-
was surprising to see that some children with SSDs exhib- quharson et al., 2018). Research certainly suggests that
ited poor vSTM but age-appropriate word reading abilities. children with SSD, particularly those with concomitant
Although our work is the first to make this explicit observa- language difficulties similar to those in the current study,
tion, it is possible that this result is due to the type of vSTM often demonstrate poor vSTM abilities (Farquharson et al.,
task used (i.e., digit span recall) and that performance on 2018; Waring et al., 2017), it is critical to continue exam-
a non-word recall task, for example, would have been in ining which types of memory tasks are most sensitive and
stronger alignment with word reading. Overall, however, informative for children with SSD.
previous research has found that vSTM skills are markedly The overall stronger performance on RAN was not unex-
weak in children with SSDs compared with their TD peers pected (Raitano et al., 2004). Previous investigations of
(Adams & Gathercole, 1995; Couture & McCauley, 2000; children with SSDs similarly found that RAN was not
Farquharson et al., 2018), although these studies did not predictive of reading risk (Tambyraja et al., 2020), and Bur-
include a parallel examination into reading abilities, or goyne et al. (2019) also found that children with speech
control for language abilities, as in the present work. Our difficulties performed comparably with TD children on
results question the extent to which an SSD may magnify RAN. Our RAN results could also be explained by the
an impairment in other aspects of the phonological system. specific measure. Compton et al. (2002), for instance,
Identifying children with SSD who are likely to have read- administered two different RAN tasks to a group of chil-
ing difficulties is not straightforward. For example, there dren. The traditional RAN (RAN-T) task scored children’s
is the possibility that children who are subthreshold at ability to name all alphanumeric items on a matrix; the
one time point may begin to experience more difficulties alternative RAN (RAN-A) scored children’s ability to name
as reading instruction becomes more complex and relies as many alphanumeric items as possible within 15 s. The
more heavily on PA and/or vSTM skills. Therefore, it is RAN-A version accounted for more of the variance in word
important to continue to assess the phonological process- recognition than did RAN-T. Previous studies indicating
ing and reading skills of children with SSD to ensure their RAN difficulties in children with SSD have used alphanu-
reading skills are supported in later school years. This is meric items (e.g., Anthony et al., 2011; Leitao et al., 1997).
particularly true with respect to reading comprehension, As such, future work should explore alternative applica-
which becomes a more heavily tested and relied-upon skill tions of RAN tasks to clarify the relation of this skill to
set as children progress through schooling. If phonologi- reading in children with SSDs.
cal processing skills are weak, this may lead to difficulties Finally, although our results did not indicate that one
with decoding more complex words, and eventually lead to specific type of phonological processing skill differenti-
difficulties with reading comprehension. ated children who were poor or good readers, these data
The overall difficulties with the PA task were not unex- do support that children clinically identified as having
pected, as research indicates that many children with SSD SSD who are also poor readers experienced more global
experience difficulties specific to PA (e.g., Pi & Ha, 2020; difficulties compared with children with SSD who were
Raitano et al., 2004). The task selected for measuring good readers, across phonological processing skills (e.g.,
PA in this study was a deletion task that required chil- Peterson et al., 2009). Further, even after controlling for
dren to verbally respond to prompts requiring them to language and non-verbal intelligence, children with SSD
state new words after deleting either a word, syllable or who were identified as demonstrating difficulties in all
phoneme. This is a highly sensitive task with respect to three areas of phonological processing were more likely to
manipulating different phonological units, but it does not have a comorbid reading problem, indicating cumulative
assess varying PA skills such as rhyming, blending, seg- risk as the number of impaired phonological process-
mentation, and initial/final sound awareness. Thus, it is ing skills increases. This is certainly in line with the
possible that other measure of PA might have revealed double deficit hypothesis (Wolf & Bowers, 1999), which
more pronounced differences in the proportion of chil- suggests that children with dyslexia may have difficulty
dren exhibiting PA difficulties in each subgroup (i.e., good with PA, RAN or both. These measures are considered to
versus poor readers). be independent sources of reading impairment (but see
Although the generally poor performance on the vSTM Schatschneider et al., 2002), thus, difficulty with both will
was widespread, we concede that only one measure was yield a more severe case of dyslexia. Badian (1997) extended
included and thus may not have captured nuances in mem- this theory to the triple deficit hypothesis, which included
ory performance that other, or additional, measures might an orthographic knowledge task. Similar to the current
14606984, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1460-6984.12764 by Universidade Do Algarve, Wiley Online Library on [18/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
TAMBYRAJA et al. 25

results, Badian (1997) reported that when two or three skill classroom performance to bolster the ecological validity of
areas were impaired, children were classified as poor read- the findings. In addition, although this work offers impor-
ers. Notably, children in our clinically identified sample tant information about clinically identified children with
exhibited an additional area of impairment: speech sound SSD, further subgrouping according to SSD subtype and
production. As such, these data support a more complex language ability was outside the scope of this study. Future
hypothesis that considers multiple deficits across multiple studies should more carefully investigate the extent to
domains. which phonological profiles may differ for children meet-
Pennington (2006) conceptualized such a hypothesis ing varying SSD subclassifications. Finally, future research
when examining the relations between children with should incorporate a variety of phonological processing
dyslexia, attention deficit hyperactive disorder (ADHD), measures, particularly RAN tasks that require retrieval
and SSD in a review of decades of behavioural and genetic of both alpha and numeric objects (Schatschneider et al.,
work. Evidence suggested that a multiple cognitive deficit 2002), vSTM tasks involving word span, and PA tasks that
model is consistent with the profiles seen in these groups measure abilities other than phonological deletion.
of children. Although our results can be interpreted within
the double-, triple- or multiple-deficit hypotheses to some
extent—an important conclusion is that a single deficit CONCLUSIONS
(i.e., speech sound production) is not sufficient data to
make a complete diagnosis or treatment decisions. Mul- Children with SSD are heterogeneous and demonstrate
tiple sources of data must be obtained to examine word variable phonological processing abilities. The extent
decoding and several aspects of phonological processing. to which these variations interact with their speech
Our results support that multiple areas of the expressive sound production difficulties requires further investiga-
(speech sound) and receptive (decoding and phonological tion. However, the results of the present investigation
processing) phonological system are likely to be impaired suggest that phonological processing skills underpin read-
in children with SSDs. It is important that next steps ing abilities, and that vSTM, in particular is a sensitive
build on this and develop profiles that could be applied measure for children with SSDs. Clinically, it is crucial
to assessment and treatment. Specifically, from a clini- to also note that these skills may very likely change over
cal perspective, our results underscore the critical need to time; children should be screened at least annually and
evaluate multiple phonological processing skills for chil- be comprehensive enough to capture potential multiple
dren with SSD to understand which children might benefit phonological difficulties.
from additional reading supports. In the United States, for
instance, it is not commonplace to assess these or literacy CONFLICT OF INTEREST
skills when determining eligibility for services for children The authors have no relevant conflicts of interest.
with SSD (Farquharson & Tambyraja, 2019).
D A T A AVA I L A B I L I T Y S T A T E M E N T
Research data are not shared.
Limitations and future directions

Our results provide unique and robust information regard- REFERENCES


ing the complexities of phonological processing as they Adams, A.M. & Gathercole, S.E. (1995) Phonological working mem-
ory and speech production in preschool children. Journal of
related to word decoding for children with SSDs. However,
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 38, 403–414.
there are several limitations, which relate to opportunities
American Speech–Language–Hearing Association. (2001) Roles and
for future research. First, these data are cross-sectional. responsibilities of speech–language pathologists with respect to
Longitudinal investigations are necessary to better under- reading and writing in children and adolescents [Position State-
stand how phonological processing relates to long-term ment]. Available from: www.asha.org/policy.
reading outcomes for children with SSD. Related, we do Anthony, J.L., Aghara, R.G., Dunkelberger, M.J., Anthony, T.I.,
not have information regarding classroom performance, Williams, J.M. & Zhang, Z. (2011) What factors place children
classroom reading instruction, and/or receipt of supple- with speech sound disorders at risk for reading problems?
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 20, 146–160.
mental reading instruction. Children in this study were in
https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2011/10-0053).
early elementary school, and thus would have had read- Badian, N.A. (1997) Dyslexia and the double deficit hypothesis.
ing instruction by the time of the study; however, details Annals of Dyslexia, 47, 69–87.
regarding classroom reading practices were not obtained. Burgoyne, K., Lervag, A., Malone, S. & Hulme, C. (2019) Speech diffi-
Although our measures classified children as good and culties at school entry are a significant risk factor for later reading
poor readers, future studies should collect data regarding difficulties. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 49, 40–48.
14606984, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1460-6984.12764 by Universidade Do Algarve, Wiley Online Library on [18/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
26 PHONOLOGICAL PROCESSING SKILLS IN CHILDREN

Cabbage, K.L., Farquharson, K., Iuzzini-Seigel, J., Zuk, J. & Hogan, vocabulary development in children. British Journal of Psychology,
T.P. (2018) Exploring the overlap between dyslexia and speech 82, 387–406.
sound production deficits. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services Gough, P.B. And Tunmer, W.E. (1986) Decoding, reading, and reading
in Schools, 49, 774–786. disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.
Cain, K., Oakhill, J. & Bryant, P. (2004) Children’s reading compre- Gray, S., Fox, A.B., Green, S., Alt, M., Hogan, T.P., Petscher, Y., et al.
hension ability: concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal (2019) Working memory profiles of children with dyslexia, devel-
ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, opmental language disorder, or both. Journal of Speech, Language,
96, 31–42. and Hearing Research, 62, 1839–1858.
Carroll, J.M., Solity, J. & Shapiro, L.R. (2016) Predicting dyslexia using Kaufman, A.S. & Kaufman, N.L. (2004) Kaufman brief intelligence
prereading skills: the role of sensorimotor and cognitive abilities. test–Second Edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 750–758. Leitao, S., Hogben, J. & Fletcher, J. (1997) Phonological processing
Castles, A. & Coltheart, M. (2004) Is there a causal link from phono- skills in speech and language impaired children. International
logical awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition, 91, Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 32, 91–111.
77–111. Lewis, B.A., Avrich, A.A., Freebairn, L.A., Hansen, A.J., Sucheston,
Catts, H.W., Adlof, S.M., Hogan, T.P. & Weismer, S.E. (2005) Are L.E., Kuo, I., et al. (2011) Literacy outcomes of children with
specific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? early childhood speech sound disorders: Impact of endopheno-
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1378– types. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54,
1392. 1628–1643.
Catts, H.W., Adlof, S.M., Hogan, T.P. & Weismer, S.E. (2005) Are spe- Lewis, B.A., Freebairn, L., Tag, J., Ciesla, A.A., Iyengar, S.K., Stein,
cific language impairment and dyslexia distinct disorders? Journal C.M., et al. (2015) Adolescent outcomes of children with early
of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1378–1392. speech sound disorders with and without language impairment.
Catts, H.W., Fey, M.E., Zhang, X. & Tomblin, J.B. (2001) Estimat- American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 24, 150–163.
ing the risk of future reading difficulties in kindergarten children. Lonigan, C.J., Anthony, J.L., Phillips, B.M., Purpura, D.J., Wilson,
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 32, 38–50. S.B. & Mcqueen, J.D. (2009) The nature of preschool phonologi-
Chow, B.W.Y., McBride-Chang, C. & Burgess, S. (2005) Phonological cal processing abilities and their relations to vocabulary, general
processing skills and early reading abilities in Hong Kong Chinese cognitive abilities, and print knowledge. Journal of Educational
kindergarteners learning to read English as a second language. Psychology, 101, 345–358.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 81–87. Macrae, T. & Tyler, A.A. (2014) Speech abilities in preschool chil-
Clarke, P., Hulme, C. & Snowling, M. (2005) Individual differences in dren with speech sound disorder with and without co-occurring
RAN and reading: a response timing analysis. Journal of Research language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
in Reading, 28, 73–86. Schools, 45, 302–313.
Compton, D.L., Olson, R.K., Defries, J.C. & Pennington, B.F. (2002) Manis, F.R., Seidenberg, M.S. & Doi, L.M. (1999) See Dick RAN: rapid
Comparing the relationships among two different versions of naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first
alphanumeric rapid automatized naming and word level reading and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 129–157.
skills. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 343–368. Murphy, K.A. & Farquharson, K. (2016) Investigating profiles of
Couture, A.E. & McCauley, R.J. (2000) Phonological working mem- lexical quality in preschool and their contribution to first grade
ory in children with phonological impairment. Clinical Linguistics reading. Reading and Writing, 29, 1745–1770.
& Phonetics, 14, 499–517. Muter, V. & Snowling, M. (1998) Concurrent and longitudinal predic-
Cummine, J., Szepesvari, E., Chouinard, B., Hanif, W. & Georgiou, tors of reading: the role of metalinguistic and short-term memory
G.K. (2014) A functional investigation of RAN letters, digits, and skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 320–337.
objects: how similar are they? Behavioural Brain Research, 275, Nathan, L., Stackhouse, J., Goulandris, N. & Snowling, M.J. (2004)
157–165. The development of early literacy skills among children with
Dodd, B. (2014) Differential diagnosis of pediatric speech sound speech difficulties. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
disorder. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 3, 189–196. Research, 47, 377–391.
Ehri, L.C., Nunes, S.R., Willows, D.M., Schuster, B.V., Yaghoub- Pennington, B.F. (2006) From single to multiple deficit models of
Zadeh, Z. & Shanahan, T. (2001) Phonemic awareness instruction developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 385–413.
helps children learn to read: evidence from the National Reading Peterson, R.L., Pennington, B.F., Shriberg, L.D. & Boada, R. (2009)
Panel’s meta-analysis. Reading Research Quarterly, 36, 250–287. What influences literacy outcome in children with speech sound
Farquharson, K. (2019) It might not be “just artic”: the case for the sin- disorder? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52,
gle sound error. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups, 1175–1188.
4, 76–84. Pi, M. & Ha, S. (2020) Classification of subgroups of children with
Farquharson, K., & Tambyraja, S.R. (2019) Describing how school- speech sound disorders: a preliminary study. Communication
based SLPs determine eligibility for children with speech sound Sciences & Disorders, 25, 113–125.
disorders. Seminars in Speech and Language, 40, 105–112. Preston, J. & Edwards, M.L. (2010) Phonological awareness and types
Farquharson, K., Hogan, T.P. & Bernthal, J.E. (2018) Working mem- of sound errors in preschoolers with speech sound disorders.
ory in school-age children with and without a persistent speech Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 44–60.
sound disorder. International Journal of Speech–Language Pathol- Raitano, N.A., Pennington, B.F., Tunick, R.A., Boada, R. & Shriberg,
ogy, 20, 422–433. L.D. (2004) Pre-literacy skills of subgroups of children with speech
Gathercole, S.E., Willis, C. & Baddeley, A.D. (1991) Differentiat- sound disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45,
ing phonological memory and awareness of rhyme: reading and 821–835.
14606984, 2023, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/1460-6984.12764 by Universidade Do Algarve, Wiley Online Library on [18/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
TAMBYRAJA et al. 27

Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S.C., Day, B.L., Castellote, J.M., White, Tambyraja, S.R., Farquharson, K. & Justice, L.M. (2020) Reading risk
S., et al. (2003) Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from in children with speech sound disorder: prevalence, persistence,
a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841–865. and predictors. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research,
Rispens, J. & Baker, A. (2012) Nonword repetition: the relative con- 63, 3714–3726.
tributions of phonological short-term memory and phonological Tkach, J.A., Chen, X., Freebairn, L.A., Schmithorst, V.J., Holland,
representations in children with language and reading impair- S.K. & Lewis, B.A. (2011) Neural correlates of phonological pro-
ment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, cessing in speech sound disorder: a functional magnetic resonance
683–694. imaging study. Brain and Language, 119, 42–49.
Rvachew, S. & Grawburg, M. (2006) Correlates of phonological Torgesen, J.K., Wagner, R.K. & Rashote, C.A. (1994) Longitudi-
awareness in preschoolers with speech sound disorders. Journal nal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of
of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 74–87. Learning Disabilities, 27, 276–286.
Schatschneider, C., Carlson, C.D., Francis, D.J., Foorman, B.R. & Wagner, R.K. & Torgesen, J.K. (1987) The nature of phonological
Fletcher, J.M. (2002) Relationship of rapid automatized nam- processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills.
ing and phonological awareness in early reading development: Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.
implications for the double-deficit hypothesis. Journal of Learning Wagner, R.K., Torgesen, J.K. & Rashotte, C.A. (1994) Development
Disabilities, 35, 245–256. of reading-related phonological processing abilities: new evidence
Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J.M., Francis, D.J., Carlson, C.D. & of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study.
Foorman, B.R. (2004) Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: a Developmental Psychology, 30, 73.
longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychol- Waring, R. & Knight, R. (2013) How should children with speech
ogy, 96, 265–282. sound disorders be classified? A review and critical evaluation of
Semel, E., Wiig, E.H. & Secord, W.A. (2003) Clinical evaluation of current classification systems. International Journal of Language
language fundamentals–Fourth Edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson & Communication Disorders, 48, 25–40.
Assessments. Waring, R., Eadie, P., Rickard, L.S. & Dodd, B. (2017) Do children
Sices, L., Taylor, H.G., Freebairn, L., Hansen, A. & Lewis, B. (2007) with phonological delay have phonological short-term and phono-
Relationship between speech-sound disorders and early literacy logical working memory deficits? Child Language Teaching and
skills in preschool-age children: impact of comorbid language Therapy, 33, 33–46.
impairment. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics: Whitehurst, G.J. & Lonigan, C.J. (1998) Child development and
JDBP, 28, 438. emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848–872.
Stackhouse, J. & Wells, B. (1997) Children’s speech and literacy diffi- Woodcock, R.W., Mcgrew, K.S & Mather, N. (2001) Woodcock–
culties, Book1: a psycholinguistic framework (Vol., 9). London: John Johnson III tests of achievement. Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside
Wiley & Sons Incorporated. Publishing.
Strattman, K. & Hodson, B.W. (2005) Variables that influence decod- Wolf, M. & Bowers, P.G. (1999) The double-deficit hypothesis for the
ing and spelling in beginning readers. Child Language Teaching developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91,
and Therapy, 21, 165–190. 415–438.
Sutherland, D. & Gillon, G.T. (2007) Development of phonologi-
cal representations and phonological awareness in children with
speech impairment. International Journal of Language & Commu-
nication Disorders, 42, 229–250.
Tambyraja, S.R., Farquharson, K., Logan, J.A. & Justice, L.M. (2015) How to cite this article: Tambyraja, S.R.,
Decoding skills in children with language impairment: Contri- Farquharson, K. & Justice, L.M. (2023)
butions of phonological processing and classroom experiences. Phonological processing skills in children with
American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 24, 177–188. speech sound disorder: A multiple case study
Tambyraja, S.R., Schmitt, M.B., Farquharson, K. & Justice, L.M.
approach. International Journal of Language &
(2018) Home literacy environment profiles of children with lan-
guage impairment: associations with caregiver- and child-specific
Communication Disorders, 58, 15–27.
factors. International Journal of Language & Communication https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12764
Disorders, 52, 238–249.

You might also like