You are on page 1of 15

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2016, 57, 368–382 DOI: 10.1111/sjop.

12301

Personality and Social Psychology


Superstitious behavior in sport: A literature review
€ OT
ZSUZSANNA DOM € OR,
€ 1,2 ROBERTO RUIZ-BARQUIN3 and ATTILA SZABO2
1
Doctoral School of Psychology, Faculty of Education and Psychology, E€ otv€os Lor
and University, Budapest, Hungary
2
Institute of Health Promotion and Sport Sciences, E€otv€os Lorand University, Budapest, Hungary
3
Department of Developmental and Educational Psychology, Autonomous University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain

D€om€ot€or, Z., Ruız-Barquın, R. & Szabo, A. (2016). Superstitious behavior in sport: A literature review. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 57, 368–382.

The objective of this first literature review, in this area, is to unveil the current status of knowledge on superstition in sport. Its outcome reveals that
superstitious behaviors vary with the type of sport, athletic level, as well as athletic role. In agreement with past theories, they increase with the level of
challenge, as reflected by the importance of the competition, as well as with the level of uncertainty. Cultural factors, in conjunction with the education
level, as well as gender, have a strong influence on superstitious behaviors in sports. Based on current thoughts, religiosity and superstition are different
psychological constructs used as psychological aids by several athletes. Personality factors appear to mediate the manifestation of the behavior. Elite
athletes are clearly more superstitious than non-elite athletes, An interaction between athletic skill and task-difficulty emerges to be another strong predictor
of superstition in sport. It is evident that a set of complex personal and situational factors interact in the manifestation of superstitious behavior in sport that
is used for the regulation of emotions in a quest for optimal performance. It is concluded that the objective benefits (i.e., success) of superstition in sport
may be ascribed to the placebo effect that yields an increased sense of control and mental reassurance in unpredictable contest situations.
Key words: Athlete, magic, paranormal, placebo, ritual.
Attila Szabo, Institute of Health Promotion and Sports Sciences, Faculty of Education and Psychology, E€
otv€
os Lor
and University, H-1117 Budapest,
€ on u. 10, Hungary. Tel: +36 70-243-7123; e-mail: szabo.attila@ppk.elte.hu
Bogdanfy Od€

INTRODUCTION METHODS

Michael Jordan, the world-class basketball legend used to wear his Search strategy
lucky blue North Carolina shorts under his Chicago Bulls team We conducted the literature search using two databases, Pubmed and
shorts (as cited in Damisch, Stoberock & Mussweiler, 2010, Google Scholar for studies investigating superstitious behaviors and
p. 1014). In order to cover his lucky pair, Jordan started wearing rituals in sports or athletic performance. We used the main keywords
longer shorts, which inspired a trend in the NBA. However, he is ‘superstition* and sport*’ (also magic, mystical, supernatural in
conjunction with sport*) in the two databases; We searched the words
not the only world class athlete who exhibited superstitious
in the titles and the abstract. The search was not limited by language or
behavior(s). Bj€orn Borg, the tennis legend, in preparing for the to peer-reviewed publications. Subsequently, the reference lists of
Wimbledon tournaments always grew a beard and wore the same relevant articles were examined for further citations on the topic
brand of shirt. Did he believe that the shirt helped him to win five (Fig. 1).
straight Wimbledon titles from 1976 through 1980 (Newkey-
Burden, 2014)? Further, the Montreal Canadiens ice hockey team’s
famous goaltender, Patrick Roy – who won three Conn Smythe Selection criteria
trophies in his career – showed numerous superstitious behaviors. One author screened the identified titles and abstracts. Abstracts were
Among them, he regularly caressed and talked to the posts and also eliminated in the initial screen if they were not concerned with the topic of
thanked them when a puck was deflected (The Concordian, 2012). our review. Abstracts meeting the inclusion criteria (i.e., concerned with
superstition in the realm of sport or athletic performance) were eligible for
Such behaviors are performed in context of a positive outcome or full-text review.
expectation associated with sporting performance.
Several investigations have studied superstitious beliefs – as
based on objectively manifested behaviors – among athletes Summarization of data
(Becker, 1975; Buhrmann & Zaugg, 1981, 1983; Burger & Lynn,
The findings of the observational and experimental studies were
2005; Burke et al., 2006; Neil, 1982; Zaugg, 1980). The large summarized by two authors independently first, then discussed together.
extent of superstitions in sport was revealed by Schippers and The first author organized the located research by relevance and topics for
Van Lange (2006), who found that four out of five professional the subsequent qualitative evaluation.
athletes mentioned at least one superstitious behavior that they
perform before a contest. These figures strongly support the need
for systematic research on superstitious behaviors in sports. COMMON DEFINITIONS OF SUPERSTITIOUS BEHAVIOR
Further, the fact that interest in superstition in sports dates back Superstitious behaviors occur in certain situations in which an
over half a century (i.e., Samuelsen, 1957), begs for a review of erroneous assumption of a cause and effect relationship generates
the literature that summarizes the current knowledge. Given that a repeated course of action in the person’s behavior (Foster &
there is no literature review in this area, the present work was Kokko, 2009). Indeed, most superstitious behaviors are a result of
performed with the aim to fill this void in the literature. learning from an authoritative (trusted) source, like parents,

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol 57 (2016) Superstition in sport 369

Fig. 1. Literature search and inclusion method.

friends, or the media (Berger, 2012; Emme, 1940; Lindstr€ om & gap) of the unknown in a chance-situation to reduce anxiety
Olsson, 2015) and coincidental conditioning when people make a (Campbell, 1996; Malinowski, 1979). The resulting perceived
false linkage between two co-occurring, but unrelated events control, as well as the lowered anxiety, maybe an adaptive switch
(Hamerman & Johar, 2013; Skinner, 1948). between negative (i.e., learned helplessness) and positive (sense
There are several definitions for superstitious behaviors, all of control) thoughts in challenging situations (Dudley, 1999).
pointing toward a subjectively logical, but objectively irrational, A positive mindset is a psychological prerequisite in one’s
course of action. An earlier definition from Jahoda (1969, p. 139) successful athletic career (Sheard, 2012).
viewed superstitious behavior as a result of the “belief that one’s The beneficial effects of superstitious acts, whether anxiolytic
fate is in the hands of unknown external powers governed by or confidence boosting, may emerge via placebo effects.
forces over which one has no control.” Womack’s (1992a, p. 192) Programming the mind for enhanced performance, via strong
definition of superstition is: “an unusual, repetitive, rigid behavior subjective beliefs, could result in objective benefits. Indeed, Berdi,
that is perceived to have positive effect by the actor, whereas in K€oteles, Szabo and Bardos (2011), in their meta-analysis, have
reality there is no causal link between the behavior and the disclosed an overall medium effect size (0.40, the 95% confidence
outcome of an event.” Womack (1992b, p. 2) provides a sport- interval (CI) ranged from 0.24 to 0.56) for placebo-enhanced
specific definition for superstitious behaviors: “actions which sport performance. However, in a more recent meta-analysis a
are repetitive, formal, sequential, and distinct from technical smaller effect size of 0.20 (95% CI ranged from 0.02 to 0.41)
performance and which the athlete believes to be powerful in was disclosed (Lindheimer, O’Connor & Dishman, 2015). The
controlling luck or other external factors.” These definitions placebo mechanism in performance situations may trigger greater
assign a role for the magic, or an omnipotent superpower, that persistence on a given task that in turn may enhance self-efficacy,
cannot be justified in scientific views. Therefore, superstitious which can lead to better performance, as revealed in a series of
beliefs are seen as irrational or paranormal, yet they affect experimental studies (Damisch et al., 2010). Recent research
peoples’ thoughts, emotions, actions, and reactions (Zaugg, 1980). shows that the placebo effect and acts of superstition share 25%
Superstitions occur in different forms, such as, food, clothing, of common variance (Rekhviashvili & Gupta, 2015).
color, fetishes, taboos, etc. (Zaugg, 1980). In sports, they often It appears, then, that superstitious acts in sports are performed
surface in situations where the outcome is unpredictable, such as to regulate one’s emotions and bring the uncertain or seemingly
important competitions (Campbell, 1996; Neil, 1982). uncontrollable events under control by switching from a negative
to a positive mindset that aids the athlete in performance. In line
with this view, superstitions may have a double impact on most
REASONS BEHIND SUPERSTITIOUS BEHAVIOR IN SPORT people. One is the elimination of uncertainty, and the other is the
Sport competitions are characterized by stable (i.e., skill) and generation of control, a sort of reassurance, which jointly could
unstable (i.e., refereeing or opponents’ quality of play) factors. lead to enhanced performance. Perhaps it should be noted that the
Superstitious acts stem from the wish to control the elements of desired effect only occurs in the strong “believers” (the right
chance, or the unstable factors, and to reduce psychological scholastic term would be “responders”; Benedetti & Frisaldi,
tension (Brevers, Nils, Dan & No€el, 2011). This proposition 2014) who need external reassurances in seemingly uncertain
matches to Malinowski’s “gap theory” on superstition, which situations. What determines one’s susceptibility to superstitious
claims that the purpose of such behavior is to fill the void (the beliefs is influenced by numerous factors, ranging from culture

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
370 Z. D€om€ot€or et al. Scand J Psychol 57 (2016)

(Calin-Jageman & Caldwell, 2014) to neurological functioning that aids the athlete in coping with adversity. This view is
(Rao, Zheng, Zhou & Li, 2014). supported by the finding that superstition correlates positively
with the psychological tension in athletes (Brevers et al., 2011).
Such a correlation also supports Neil’s (1982) hypothesis,
COGNITIVE-BEHAVIORAL DISCORD IN SPORT according to which the superstitious behavior has an anxiety
SUPERSTITION reducing effect in sport. In this view, superstition may assume the
Athletes often do not admit or see themselves as superstitious role of a psychological placebo that helps athletes to cope with
even though they may report practicing a superstitious behavior. adversity and increase self-confidence via perceived control.
Such a defensive denial stems from the fact that superstitious Schippers and Van Lange (2006) have shown that superstitious
behaviors are pejorative (Jahoda, 1969; Samuelsen, 1957). behaviors are the most evident at times of uncertainty, before
Further, paradoxically, many athletes may not believe in the important contests when the stakes are high, and when the
efficacy of their own superstitious acts. According to Bleak and athletes think that success is affected by external factors over
Frederick’s (1998) investigation, the effectiveness of superstitious which they have no control. The support for the uncertainty
practices does not determine their popularity (Burger & Lynn, hypothesis comes from an inquiry in which the more players
2005). Supporting these results, it was reported that the most believed that chance alone affects the outcome, the more likely
popular superstitious behaviors among footballers were linked to they were to engage in superstitious behaviors (Burger & Lynn,
appearance and clothing in spite of the fact that the players 2005). Regardless of the outcome, however, superstitious
thought that prayers to God may be the most effective in behaviors could be maintained as a “habit” over time in spite of
influencing the outcome of the contest (Ofori, Biddle & Lavallee, the discord between the action and its results (Ciborowski, 1997).
2012). This habit is then explained by mental conditioning in addition to
There are several explanations for a cognitive-behavioral the superstition-linked expectancy.
disharmony in sport superstition. According to earlier works by
Skinner (1948), a perceived causal relationship may surface
between two elements simply by chance. In his classical SUPERSTITIOUS BEHAVIORS VERSUS
experiments, pigeons that had random access to food attempted to PRE-PERFORMANCE ROUTINES
recreate the circumstances in which the food was obtained and Pre-performance routines in sports are often confounded with
behaved accordingly. In sports, wearing particular clothing or superstition. It has been shown that routines performed before the
carrying out certain acts (like the tying of the shoe laces before a motor tasks (e.g., basketball free throw) contribute to the accuracy
free shoot) may be followed by a successful outcome (reward), of the shot, so they improve the subsequent performance
which is then irrationally linked to the behavior. This association (Cotterill, 2010; Lobmeyer & Wasserman, 1986; Singer 2002).
later acts as a prompt in anticipating the re-occurrence of the It is important to distinguish superstitious behaviors from
reward. However, if the reward (or the expected outcome) does pre-performance/pre-game routines (PPR). Unfortunately, the
not occur, the mental link weakens and may even disappear. distinction between PPR and superstitious behaviors is not always
Therefore, Skinner’s observations stemming from work with clear (see Table 1). Nevertheless, several authors noted the
pigeons may provide a plausible explanation for the cognitive differences between superstitious behaviors and PPR. The former
discord, while it cannot fully account for the superstitious acts is a learned, behavioral and/or cognitive strategy, which is used
in sport. by athletes to facilitate performance (Cohn, 1990; Wright &
Another explanation may be linked to the illusion of control. Erdal, 2008). The latter is devised with the help of a sports
This illusion is created with a purpose through the practicing of a professional and lacks magical meaning, while superstitious
superstitious behavior. The purpose is the regulation or control of behaviors are initiated by the athletes’ needs in a seemingly
the emotions, which also influences the practice of superstitions random manner and have excessive rigid timing and fixed order
(Rudski, 2001, 2004; Wright & Erdal, 2008). Furthermore, in (Bleak & Frederick, 1998; Brevers et al., 2011). The efficacy of
accord with Lerner’s (1965) Just World Hypothesis, everybody superstitious behaviors emerges in context of actual or perceived
needs the faith in the equity of the world. In other words, if an lower anxiety level and possibly higher self-efficacy. Conversely,
athlete does everything to win (e.g., hard training, right food PPR directly affects the technical or mental aspect of the sporting
intake, etc.) then the athlete may believe that she or he deserves performance. It may exert an impact on performance outcomes
the reward. The illusion of the “merited reward” strengthens the through focusing on self-control – focused attention – or be an
anticipation of the positive outcome and, again, serves to regulate integral part in the warm-up exercises during the preparation for a
emotions in contest situations. A discord may surface when the critical motor sequence (Bleak & Frederick, 1998; Burke et al.,
mental expectation does not materialize in the situational 2006). In addition, athletes control the PPRs, while they are
outcome. almost always controlled by superstitions (Burke et al., 2006).
Overall, athletes need to be in control, or at least have the Foster, Weigand and Baines (2006) examined the effect of
perception of control (emotion regulation), which can be removing superstitious behavior and replacing it with a PPR on
generated by certain superstitious behaviors (Brevers et al., 2011). basketball free-throw performance. These authors hypothesized
This contention mirrors Malinowski’s (1979) philosophy about that performance will be the worst when no pre-performance
the need to fill the gap of uncertainty in challenging situations in strategy is adopted, better when athletes use superstitious
an adaptive manner. Therefore, if one disregards the “irrational” behaviors (SB), and the best when PPRs are adopted by the
nature of superstitious behaviors, a “rational” function emerges athletes. In accord with the researchers’ hypothesis, the results

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol 57 (2016) Superstition in sport 371

Table 1. The differences between superstitious behavior and pre- In an early comparison of athletes with nonathletes, Gregory and
performance routines in sport Petrie (1972) found that athletes were not more superstitious than
nonathletes in general, but they listed more than twice as many
Superstitious rituals Pre-performance routines (PPR)
superstitions associated with sports than nonathletes. In another
Formal, repetitive, sequential behavior inquiry, Ciborowski (1997) examined baseball players who saw
An attempt to seek control over Behavior that directly affects the themselves as more superstitious than nonathletes and also
highly stressful situations (Bleak technical performance (Foster thought that the intensity of their superstitious beliefs is higher
& Frederick, 1998). et al., 2006).
than that of nonathletes. Further, more baseball players had a
Maintains emotional stability Involve cognitive and behavioral
(Womack, 1992) elements that intentionally help lucky charm or object than nonathletes. However, there was no
regulate arousal, enhance significant difference in other type of superstitious beliefs (such as
concentration (Crews & Boutcher, astrology, Tarots, and the like) between the two samples. The
1986; Induce optimal existence of superstitions in athletes could be linked to the
physiological and psychological
uncertainty levels involved in various sports. The phenomenon
states (Cohn, 1990).
Lowers anxiety level and yields Results in: (1) attentional control; is observable in gamblers too (Joukhador, Blaszczynski &
higher self-efficacy or self (2) warm-up decrement; and Maccallum, 2004; Rogers, 1998) and college students, where the
confidence. (3) automatic skill execution beliefs relating to exams (increased stakes) are most prominent
(Boutcher & Crews, 1987). (Rudski & Edwards, 2010; Saenko, 2014; Vyse, 2013). These
Initiated by the athlete. Usually developed by a
findings agree with the results of Hamerman and Morewedge’s
professional outsider (coach or
sport psychologist). (2015) studies, where performance goals were more likely to elicit
Has a magical meaning (have only Lacks mystical or magical meaning. superstitious behavior, than learning goals. Also, if participants
impact while the athlete believes pursued performance goals (for instance defeating the opponent in
in it); It is characterized by rigid a competition in the realm of sport), but not learning goals,
timing and fixed order
superstitious behavior increased in line with the uncertainty of
The athletes feel controlled by it. Athletes have control over it
goal achievement (Hamerman & Morewedge, 2015).
Sports-related superstitions are also seen in nonathletes, if they
have strong affiliation with sports (McClearn, 2004). Therefore,
revealed that performance was indeed the worst when neither PPR superstitious beliefs in sports are not restricted to participation,
nor SB was used, but they have found very little difference but they are also linked to personal orientations. Most athletic
between the effects of PPR and SB. In fact the use of PPRs only contests comprise elements of chance that increase the practice of
returned performance to the baseline (defined as the performance superstitious behaviors, probably through conditioning (Skinner,
when players used SB). 1948). Many sports fans associate themselves with their favorite
team or athlete and resort to superstitions as a means of projection
of control, or increased influence over the (their) desired outcome,
REVIEW OF RESEARCH ON SUPERSTITION IN SPORT
which is the success of their team or an athlete (Pronin, Wegner,
Despite the apparent high prevalence of superstitious behavior in McCarthy & Rodriguez, 2006). In line with Malinowski (1979),
sport, to date only a very limited number of investigations have the fans engage in superstitions in order to fill the gap of the
looked at the issue. Most of the reported studies used surveys, uncertainty, involved in chance situations, to increase and/or
and only a few of them have implemented an intervention. strengthen their expectation of the desired outcome. If the
Moreover, at the time of writing, we could locate only one expectation is fulfilled, the superstitious behavior is rewarded – as
literature review on superstitious behavior in sports, which was predicted by the conditioning theory – without any rational link
published in Greek and had a delimited focus on the locus of between the action and the outcome.
control, anxiety and several demographic factors (Perkos,
Barkoukis & Christopoulos, 2013). The conclusion of this review, Players’ role and type of sport. Superstition in sport may vary
was that superstitious behavior in sport is influenced by the with the role of the player as well as with the type of sport
athletes’ locus of control and anxiety. The review also revealed (Menapace, 2012; Neil, 1982; Perkos et al., 2013). The greater is
gender differences in superstitious behavior in sport that were the element of chance and uncertainty in a sport or a player’s
related to the nature of the behaviors used. Finally, Perkos et al. task, the more prevalent is the practice of superstition (Neil,
(2013) concluded that there are differences in superstitious 1982). The player’s role in baseball supports this surmise, because
behavior that are related to the type of sport and the player superstitious behaviors are more often linked to batting or
position in a team. In addition to re-evaluating the findings, we pitching, than to fielding (Ciborowski, 1997; Gmelch, 1972; Neil,
expand the examination of superstition in sport to other variables 1980). However, Burger and Lynn (2005) could not demonstrate
that may mediate the behaviour (Table 2). this contrast in their investigation of American and Japanese
baseball players. Studies with goaltenders in ice hockey have also
shown that these players practice a high number of superstitions
Survey research (Gregory & Petrie, 1975; Neil, 1982; Womack, 1979). In football,
Athletic level. It appears that elite athletes are more superstitious clothing and appearance were linked to superstitious behaviors,
than non-elite athletes and those in team sports are more but no differences were disclosed between goalkeepers and
superstitious than athletes in individual sports (Flanagan, 2013). attackers (Ofori et al., 2012). The authors thought that the level

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
372 Z. D€om€ot€or et al. Scand J Psychol 57 (2016)

Table 2. Observational studies about the relationship of superstition and performance

Author(s) Assessment method Participants Statistics Key findings

Gregory Own questionnaire on personal Athletes Frequencies Athletes had fewer general superstitions than
and Petrie superstitions (6 different nonathletes.
(1972) types of sports) Females reported more general superstitions
N = 137 and than males.
nonathletes
N = 115
Zaugg Own questionnaire about Athletes Gamma and Female basketball players were more
(1980) superstitious beliefs (basketball) Z-scores superstitious than males.
N = 310 Younger basketball players were more
superstitious than older players and those who
regularly attended church were more
superstitious than the others; Basketball players
in a winning team had more superstitious beliefs
than the losing team.
Neil et al. 23 items, open–ended questionnaire Intramural and Greater involvement in hockey was linked to a
(1981) intercollegiate greater prevalence of superstitions.
hockey players
N = 58
Buhrmann Likert–type instrument of 40 items related to School basketball Gamma and There was little difference in the number of
et al. superstitious beliefs and behaviors teams N = 529 Spearman superstitions endorsed by male and female
(1982) rank order athletes; Gender differences were seen in the
type of superstitions.
Ciborowski Superstitious Belief Questionnaire (SBQ), Athletes Chi-square Baseball players reported higher intensity belief
(1997) specifically constructed for the study (baseball) than non–athletes; No significant difference was
N = 83 and observed in other (non–sport related) beliefs.
nonathletes
N = 348
Bleak and Superstitious Beliefs Questionnaire; a standard Athletes Correlation, None of the predictor variables in the study
Frederick measure of religiosity (church attendance), (football, T-test contributed significantly to total superstitious
(1998) Fitness Locus of Control Scale; Sport Anxiety gymnastics, ritual use; Religiosity was related to greater use
Scale. track games) of prayers as a sport superstition, lower belief in
N = 107 chance was related to higher usage of
appearance monitoring ritual; One aspect of
sport anxiety, concentration disruption, was
significantly related to use of eating rituals.
Todd and Superstitious Behavior Scale; Jenkins Activity Athletes (track Multiple Athletic identity and locus of control (LoC) were
Brown Survey; Athletic Identity Measurement Scale; and field) regression, predictors of the practice of superstitious
(2003) Rotter’s Locus of Control (LoC) Scale N = 177 MANOVA behavior – but only among Division III athletes;
LoC was a significant predictor of sports
practice, but only for the Div. III track and field
athletes. Significant group differences were
found: Div III athletes reported greater type A
behavior pattern than Div I athletes.
McClearn Belief in Sports Superstitions Scale; Interest in Nonathletes, Correlation Positive correlation was found between interest in
(2004) Sports Scale; Tobacyk and Milford’s Paranormal students N = 51 sports and belief in sport superstitions; Belief in
Belief Scale sports superstitions and scores on the
paranormal belief scale were also correlated.
Burger and Questions about superstitions: behaviors, Athletes Correlation Large amount of superstitious behavior (74.3%);
Lynn frequency, outcome, impact, confidence in (baseball) More believe luck affected outcomes was
(2005) superstitious behavior N = 77 associated with more engagement in
superstitious behavior; American more
superstitious than Japanese players; American
players expected enhancement of individual
performance from superstitions Japanese players
expected enhancement of the team’s
performance.
Burke et al. Superstitious Ritual Questionnaire; Athletes Correlation Positive correlation was found between the
(2006) Life Orientation Test–Revised; Belief in Personal (10 different internal locus of control and optimism; Negative
Control Scale types of sports) correlation emerged between internal locus of
N = 208 control and the beliefs in clothing and
appearance– effectiveness; Correlation was
disclosed between exaggerated sense of internal
control and fetish effectiveness; An exaggerated

(continued)

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol 57 (2016) Superstition in sport 373

Table 2 (continued)

Author(s) Assessment method Participants Statistics Key findings

sense of internal control was also linked to pre-


game/meet effectiveness. A negative correlation
emerged between a God-mediated locus of
control and prayer frequency as well as prayer
effectiveness. Further negative correlation with a
God-mediated locus of control and overall
frequency for all rituals and overall effectiveness
for all rituals.
Ofori et al. Superstitious Ritual Questionnaire; Athletes Correlation, A negative correlation was found between the
(2012) Sport Attributional Style Scale (football) regression, number of rituals and scores for positive –
N = 120 t-test internality and between superstitious behavior
and total scores for positive internality; Positive
correlation was found between superstitious
behavior and positive-controllability; Attribution
styles were also related to superstitious
behavior.
Flanagan The Superstitious Ritual Questionnaire; Athletes, Correlation, Elite athletes were more superstitious than non-
(2013) The Competitive State Inventory–2 elite (N = 63) regression, elites. Athletes in the team sports were more
and non–elite t-test superstitious than athletes in individual sports.
(N = 96) No gender differences were disclosed.
Superstitious and competitive anxiety, were
positively (weakly) correlated (r = 0.19).
Torma Belief in Sport Superstition Scale; Somatosensory Physically active Correlations, Belief in sport superstition was correlated with
et al. Amplification Scale; Life Orientation Test, university regression somatosensory amplification (r = 0.38; p < 0.01)
(2013) Revised; Short version of Positive Negative students and spirituality (r = 0.23; p < 0.01) and was
Affect Schedule (PANAS) (N = 242) independent of dispositional optimism
(r = –0.06; p > 0.05).

of risk may be a mediating factor in superstitious behavior linked an adaptive value in emotional regulation. Calming the spirits and
to the player position. In football the level of risk could be more gaining self-confidence may trigger better performance, by also
homogeneous among the players than in baseball or ice hockey. altering expectations and, therefore, possibly also involving the
These observations suggest that there may be a personal, as well placebo effect (Szabo, 2013).
as social, superstition in team sports. The former helps individual
performance while the latter is an aid in the group or team Athletic skill and level of competition. The practicing of
performance. superstitious behaviors is linked to athletic skills as well. Neil,
Bleak and Frederick (1998) studied superstitious behaviors in Anderson and Sheppard (1981) compared competitive hockey
three different sports (football, gymnastics, track and field) and players with recreational players and found that taking part in
found several differences. Gymnasts, for example, reported the competitions is linked to far more superstitious behaviors than
most superstitious behaviors, presumably due to the focus placed playing in recreational hockey games. In contrast, Todd and
on individual performance. This result is in contrast with Brown (2003) revealed no differences between Division I and
Flanagan’s (2013) investigation where participants who play in Division III track and field athletes in the use of superstitious
team sports had more superstitious behavior than athletes in behaviors. The results of another research supported the
individual sports. According to the author, the reason for the uncertainty hypothesis in that highly skilled golfers showed more
increased superstitious ritual use in team sports is that athletes superstitious behaviors in difficult situations, while less skilled
could believe that performance is shared by the entire team and it players showed more superstitious behavior in easy situations.
is outside of their control (Flanagan, 2013). Each sport had its There were no differences in the overall superstitious behavior
specific behavior. In football – similar to Ofori et al. (2012) – the between the high and low skill level golfers (Wright & Erdal,
players’ behavior was associated with clothing and prayer. Both, 2008). The level/ importance of competition may interact with the
athletic roles as well as the type of sports may have an inherited athletic skill in the attempt to regulate anxiety and flow (Swann,
superstition that is passed on to the next generation, which could Keegan, Piggott & Crust, 2012). In this context, the intensity of
explain – at least in part – the observed differences in their the superstitious behavior could be determined by the perceived
practicing. Therefore, superstitious behaviors may be learned or ratio of one’s skill to the challenge. In support of this view,
conditioned with the aim to increase the chance of the desired Brevers et al. (2011) found that at higher levels of competition
outcome in uncertain situations and to regulate the subjective the practicing of superstitious behavior was more pronounced,
levels of anxiety, confidence, control, etc., all contributing to the probably due to an attempt to regulate anxiety and, therefore,
generation of a psychological comfort zone in a challenging sport flow. If the regulation is successful, the mechanism may be
contest. Again, in accord with Malinowski’s gap theory (1979), ascribed – again – primarily to a placebo effect. For example, the
superstitious behaviors performed in difficult sport contests have elite athlete “expects” to be in control as a result of the

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
374 Z. D€om€ot€or et al. Scand J Psychol 57 (2016)

superstitious act; the performance of the act, then triggers the research in the area, our view is consistent with the conclusions of
subjective sense control that in turn influences the performance of Perkos et al. (2013) in that gender differences in sport may be
the athlete. primarily qualitative rather than quantitative. However, the impact
of the situation(s) should not be underestimated, since women
Cultural differences. Few studies have addressed the role of may show more superstition in different situations than men.
cultural heritage in superstitious behavior in sports. Burger and Therefore, the shortage of research prevents us from drawing a
Lynn (2005) compared superstitious behavior between Japanese definite conclusion at this time.
and American baseball players. In line with our conceptualization
of personal and social superstition (see above), they found Connection between religion and superstitious behavior. Several
American baseball players to be more superstitious than Japanese studies investigated religiosity and superstitious behavior jointly.
players. The Americans also believed that superstitions improve Zaugg (1980) found that those basketball players who regularly
their individual performance, so they exhibited a personal attended church reported more superstitious behavior than non-
superstition. In contrast, Japanese players used superstitious acts attendees, especially in the context of the prayers. In another
to aid the performance of their team, which points toward a social investigation, a positive relationship was disclosed between
superstition. This finding may reflect a cultural difference between church attendance and superstitious behavior. Mormons were
the two nations: Japan is a collectivist culture while the American more superstitious than Catholics/Protestants (Buhrmann &
culture is individualist. Also, Japanese players are taught that Zaugg, 1983). In contrast, Bleak and Frederick (1998) could not
failure or success is a result of their effort, so they did not reveal a link between the level of superstitious behavior and
attribute the outcome to superstitious behaviors, but rather they religiosity, but higher religiosity was associated with more prayers
assumed responsibility for their performances. It is likely that as a means of sport superstition. In Burke et al.’s (2006)
cultural differences may affect superstitious behaviors through the investigation a lesser belief in God-mediated control was
nations’ mentality that is a heritage of past traditions and ways of associated with lesser belief in the effectiveness of superstitions
thinking. However, superstitious beliefs and acts interact – and and, therefore, the lesser involvement in superstitious behaviors.
demonstrate a negative correlation – with peoples’ level of The authors suggested that the use of superstitious behaviors in
education, which is independent of national origin (Mocan & sport and religion could be complementary, or similar, practices
Pogorelova, 2014). Therefore, the cultural values interact with (Burke et al., 2006). In contrast, Maranise (2013) believes that
people’s level of education in determining the openness toward superstitious and religious behaviors cannot be considered as the
resorting to superstitious behaviors in various sport situations. same entities, because of major differences between the two.
Religious behaviors may provide a wide range of psychological
Gender differences. Gender differences in superstitious behavior benefits, as well as holistic meaning, that is experienced as a
appear to exist, in that women tend to be more superstitious than feeling of well-being by the athlete, and thus provide a deeper
men (Wiseman & Watt, 2004). In sports, a study that compared and more universal scope to life itself. Belief in God was shown
superstitious behavior of the two genders revealed that women to be a constantly available means of coping with challenge in
held more superstitious beliefs than men. Further, gender itself (Pargament, Ensing, Falgout et al., 1990). Another
differences have also emerged on the nature of the superstitious difference between superstitious- and religious-beliefs is that
beliefs (Gregory & Petrie, 1972; Neil, 1982). For example, while both of them could be methods of coping with uncertainty,
women’s superstitions were related to team cheers, uniforms, hair in case of failure the former can play the role of the scapegoat in
accessories, and pre-game social functions, while men’s the failed outcome (i.e., failed superstition) (Maranise, 2013). It
superstitions were more closely related to the specifics of the should be emphasized that superstitious beliefs and acts could
practiced sport (Gregory & Petrie, 1972). However, these findings have both positive (placebo) and negative (nocebo) outcomes that
may not be universal. Indeed, Neil et al. (1981) found that male are underpinned by neurophysiological responses to various
hockey players were more superstitious than females, but the level situational factors as well as an interaction between the two
of involvement in sport was a stronger determinant than gender. (Frisaldi, Piedimonte & Benedetti, 2015).
Hockey players with greater involvement in sport exhibited more
superstitious beliefs than those who were less involved (Neil Personality and situational factors. There are certain personality
et al., 1981). In another inquiry, Buhrmann, Brown, and Zaugg characteristics that appear to be linked to superstitious behaviors,
(1982) compared the superstitious beliefs of 529 male and female for instance the perception of control in people with high anxiety
basketball players. The authors did not find gender differences in (Zebb & Moore, 2003). Among these, in a sport context, attention
the number of superstitions. However, gender differences emerged was focused on locus of control (LoC – Bleak & Frederick, 1998;
in the form of the superstitions (e.g., personal appearance was Burke et al., 2006; Schippers & Van Lange, 2006; Todd &
more important for women), but no clear pattern of them could be Brown, 2003), type A personality pattern (Todd & Brown, 2003),
identified. The outcome of a study with psychology students, who athletic identity (Brevers et al., 2011; Todd & Brown, 2003),
were examined during an intervention study using a golf task, personal control (Irwin, 1994; Ofori et al., 2012), optimism and
also failed to disclose gender differences in superstition (Van pessimism (Rudski, 2004; Torma, Berdi, K€ oteles & Bardos,
Raalte, Brewer, Nemeroff & Linder, 1991). These findings were 2013). It is possible that some of these personality factors may
echoed by Flanagan (2013), who also did not reveal a gender predispose a person to superstitious behavior in some situations.
difference in sport superstition in a sample of 159 Irish athletes. Therefore, an interactional view on the link between personality
While to date, there is only a limited number of published and superstitious behaviors should keep in perspective the role of

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol 57 (2016) Superstition in sport 375

the situation in which the superstitious behavior is observed. behaviors than athletes with internal LoC. This reasoning,
At this time, there is no causal link between personality however, remains to be tested in future research.
characteristics and superstitious behaviors in specific sport
situations. Controllability. A positive correlation may exist between the
perceived need for control and superstitious behavior (Ofori et al.,
Locus of control. In a study with Division III track and field 2012). Therefore, players may adopt superstitious acts to control
(that is a lower National Collegiate Athletic Association the stressful aspects of competition in sports. However, in Burke
(NCAA) level) athletes with an external LoC exhibited more et al.’s (2006) study, personal control could not be linked to the
superstitious behaviors than athletes with internal LoC (Todd & use of superstitions, but it seemed to play a role in subjective
Brown, 2003). This observation is in accord with the results of superstitious beliefs in athletes. Controllability may be negatively
another study by Schippers and Van Lange (2006), which also associated with superstitious behavior. In contrast, a positive
reported that players with an external LoC were more likely to connection may be expected between perceived lack of control
engage in superstitious behaviors than players with an internal and superstitious behaviors. More studies are needed concerning
LoC. Similarly, Ofori et al. (2012), in an in situ examination of the association between the perceived control in a particular
Ghanaian footballers, found that players who believed that situation and superstitious behaviors.
events occur due to their own skills and abilities have exhibited
less superstitious behaviors than players with an external LoC Type A behavior pattern and athletic identity. Type A behavioral
(i.e., negative relationship between superstitious behavior and pattern is a personality trait that is characterized by, impatience,
internality). Therefore, athletes who perceive a contest as irritability, hostility or aggressiveness, that are linked to an over-
uncontrollable, tend to ascribe more control to the external ambitious personal lifestyle, which may be perceived by others as
factors, which prompts them to engage in superstitious hard-driving and competitive (Friedman & Rosenman, 1974). In a
behaviors. Contrary to these findings, Van Raalte et al. (1991) study looking at the effects of this personality characteristic on
found a negative correlation between chance orientation (the superstitious behavior, only negative findings have emerged
degree of someone’s belief in control over chance events) and (Todd & Brown, 2003). Knowing the nature of this personality,
number of superstitious behaviors (choosing the “lucky ball”). one would expect a negative relationship between Type A
That is, the more the person believes to have influence over behavior and superstitious behavior. At this time there is one
chance events, the more likely she or he may choose the “lucky study to contradict this conjecture.
ball” after a successful putt. Clearly, Van Raalte et al. (1991) A positive link was found between athletic identity and
found that those with internal LoC relied more on superstitious superstitious behaviors (Brevers et al., 2011). Similar findings
behavior than subjects with external LoC. have emerged in an inquiry with track and field athletes (Todd &
Bleak and Frederick (1998) failed to disclose a link between Brown, 2003). It was suggested that athletes with a strong athletic
the total level of superstitious behaviors used and LoC, as well as identity may practice more superstitious behaviors. These athletes
between the importance of success and anxiety. However, these identify themselves with the stereotypical athlete role and have a
measures were related to a higher reliance on specific superstitious tendency to be highly ego-involved, so they could experience
behaviors. It appears that athletes may develop behaviors that increased anxiety (Neil et al., 1981). Indeed, in an earlier study
correspond with their personal belief systems. Perhaps to them is higher level of ego-involvement was associated with more
more important to select the right form of behavior (quality) than superstitious behaviors (Neil et al., 1981). Presumably, these
the total use (quantity) of behaviors (Bleak & Frederick, 1998). athletes use superstitious behaviors as a coping mechanism in
This conjecture is supported by the results of a recent study, anxiety provoking or stressful situations.
which showed that peoples’ expectations, regarding the perceived Optimism and pessimism. In a study by Rudski (2004) optimism
efficacy of a hypothetical performance enhancing agent, changes was positively related to religiosity, while Torma et al. (2013)
not only as a function of the shape and/or color of the agent, but reported that sport superstition was only moderately linked to
also as a function of the purpose for which that hypothetical agent spirituality, while it was independent of dispositional optimism.
is to be used (Szabo, Berdi, K€oteles & Bardos, 2013). Echoing Pessimism was negatively linked to religiosity, but it was
Bleak and Frederick (1998), other investigators also failed to positively associated with superstitious beliefs in the sport context
disclose a link between superstitious behavior and LoC in sports (Burke et al., 2006). However, these authors could not disclose a
(Burke et al., 2006). The incongruent findings, concerning the connection between optimism, pessimism and the use of
link between superstitious behavior and LoC, may be ascribed to superstitious behaviors in Division I athletes. Based on the
the combination of the different dynamics between and within surmise that pessimism is linked to a perceived lack of control,
sports, the perceived importance of the competitions, and the uncertainty, and helplessness (i.e., Martin, Marsh & Debus,
inherent uncertainty factors, which interact with personality 2003), the link between optimism, pessimism and superstitious
characteristics, as well as conditioned or learned experiences. behavior in sport settings deserves systematic research attention in
Future research in this area is needed, because theoretically an the future. Given that the personality factors exert different effects
internal LoC should be negatively linked to superstitious in different situations (Abele & Brack, 2015), examining them in
behavior. Earlier it was thought that paranormal belief in those an interactional framework, is warranted.
with external LoC is an attempt to deal with perceived
uncontrollability in life (Irwin, 1994). Therefore, athletes with Interest in sport and paranormal beliefs. McClearn (2004)
external LoC may be more susceptible to resort to superstitious questioned 51 nonathletes, university students, about their:

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
376 Z. D€om€ot€or et al. Scand J Psychol 57 (2016)

(1) interest in sports; (2) belief in sports superstition; and Jageman and Caldwell (2014) have disclosed notable
(3) various paranormal beliefs. A positive correlation was found heterogeneity in the effects of superstition on performance. The
between ratings of sports-related superstitions and other inconsistent results may be ascribed to different research designs,
paranormal beliefs. Higher interest or involvement in sports was unknown moderators, type of task, test situations, the studied
associated only with specific sports-related superstitions, but not participants, etc., which warrant the systematic investigation of
with general paranormal or other irrational beliefs. While no other the various mediators in this field.
studies were carried out in this area, the specificity of sport The level of competition may be one of the mediators of the
superstition appears to exist even among nonathletes. effect of superstition on performance. Brevers et al. (2011)
manipulated both the level of competition and uncertainty with
the aim to measure the relative contribution of personal (athletic
EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES identity) and situational (level of competition, psychological
A few experimental studies were conducted in the field of tension, uncertainty, and the importance of the outcome) factors
superstition in sport. An increased interest in this subject emerged on nonprofessional athletes’ sport superstition. The findings
only relatively recently (Brevers et al., 2011; Damisch et al., revealed a positive link between athletic identity and superstitious
2010; Foster et al., 2006; Rudski, 2001; Schippers & Van Lange, behaviors. However, the degree of superstitious behavior was
2006; Van Raalte et al., 1991; Wright & Erdal, 2008). Based on also dependent on situational factors, in that higher level of
the earlier research of Buhrmann and Zaugg (1981), in which competitions were linked to greater behavioral commitment.
superstitious beliefs of basketball players were positively Uncertainty exerted a lesser influence on athletes’ behavioral
associated with performance, a set of studies led by the same commitment than the importance of the game. Supporting the
researcher investigated superstitions in 10-trial golf putting, lucky- results, uncertainty, and more so the importance of the contest,
charm, and motor-dexterity tasks under different laboratory increased the level of behavioral commitment in an earlier
settings (Damisch et al., 2010). Supporting the past correlational inquiry (Schippers & Van Lange, 2006). These experimental
results, performance benefits from superstitious beliefs have results agree with the findings from correlational studies and
emerged. Later, a brief meta analysis of these studies yielded a suggest that the external circumstances concerning the outcome
large effect size (d = 0.83) for the interventions, as calculated in a (importance) and the opponent (uncertainty) affect psychological
replication inquiry carried out by Calin-Jageman and Caldwell tension, which then prompts the predisposed individual to engage
(2014). One’s perceived self-efficacy was thought to play a in superstitious behaviors, possibly to reach a source of comfort
mediating role between the observed superstition-performance and reassurance.
link (Damisch et al., 2010). The authors suggested that higher Belief in control, could be a strong mediator of superstition
levels of self-efficacy may lead to the performance benefits of on performance. Earlier, Van Raalte et al. (1991) tested the
superstitions, partly through an increased task persistence. Indeed, hypothesis that individuals may be more prone to display
in two of their experiments the authors manipulated idiosyncratic superstitious acts when they believe that they can exert control
superstitious beliefs (using personal lucky charm) to demonstrate over a chance situation. The authors found that those who
that the performance enhancing effects were mediated by believed that they could influence the chance were more likely to
heightened feelings of perceived self-efficacy. Subsequently, they engage in superstitious behavior. Later, Wright and Erdal (2009)
looked at the possible mechanism that could mediate the used the same design in their experimental work while testing
improved performance observed in the highly self-efficacious the impact of task difficulty on superstitious behavior. The
individuals. It was posited that a tendency to set more challenging researchers created an easy and difficult condition of golf-putting
goals (Zimmerman, 1995) and/or longer perseverance in task task and hypothesized that participants will exhibit more
(Bandura, 1986) may possibly be responsible for the observed superstitious behaviors when they encounter the difficult task. The
beneficial effects. The findings revealed that persistence in task results, however, showed no difference between superstitious
has mediated the effects of superstition-enhanced self-efficacy on behaviors in the two tasks. Nevertheless, a statistically significant
performance. interaction was disclosed between task difficulty and skill level.
In support of the studies reported by Damisch et al. (2010) it Low skilled participants manifested more superstitious behaviors
was reported that golfers who were told that they were using a in the easy condition while highly skilled participants exhibited an
famous golfer’s putter performed better on a putting task than a opposite trend. This finding supports the “uncertainty hypothesis”
control group (Lee, Linkenauger, Bakdash, Joy-Gaba & Profitt, in superstitious behavior (Burger & Lynn, 2005), according to
2011). However, later contradicting findings have also emerged. which, when the outcome of a situation is increasingly uncertain,
Acts of superstitions related to prayers were found to be unrelated superstitious behavior will increase. The highly skilled
to performance on a reasoning test. Further, asking participants participants were confident in the easy condition, so they did not
to think about their religious values and to pray also failed to need luck (color of the ball). However, in the difficult condition
affect the results (Aruguete, Goodboy, Jenkins, Mansson & they struggled more, hence they needed to rely on some external
McCutcheon, 2012). A more recent replication of Damisch et al. help. The lack of superstitions in the difficult condition for the
(2010) research also failed to disclose the robust effects reported low skill participants could reflect the surfacing of a learned
by the original research team (Calin-Jageman & Caldwell, 2014). helplessness, which was claimed to be on the opposite end of the
The replication work was done with special attention to possible same continuum (Matute, 1994).
cultural differences and a research design encompassed strict In a laboratory setting, Rudski (2001) investigated superstitions
rigour that specifically controlled for the Type I error. Calin- in competitive situations (refer to Table 3). Their findings were

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol 57 (2016) Superstition in sport 377

Table 3. Experimental studies about superstition and performance

Author(s) Research objectives Participants Methods Key findings

Van Tested the hypothesis that people may Nonathletes Levenson’s chance orientation scale; Subjects who believed that they could
Raalte be more likely to display superstition N = 37 The authors generated high ego- have control over chance events were
et al. when they believe in control over involvement in subjects, after the more likely to execute superstitious
(1991) chance situations; More ego–involved task, ego–involvement QTR; Golf– behavior (i.e., use a “lucky ball” after
and anxious athletes may report more putting task with four colored balls; successful putt).
superstitions. Superstitious behavior manifested in
the selection of the same color ball
after having made a putt with that
ball.
Rudski Studied the influence of reinforcement Athletes, Created six conditions and groups, Reinforcement schedule and
(2001) schedules and competition on students differing according to reinforcement competitive situation did not affect
triggering superstitious behaviors and N = 72 schedule and competitive situation; the degree of superstitious belief,
beliefs At specified intervals, lights were although people in “winning”
illuminated, but the contingency was competitive condition were less likely
not altered; Superstitious beliefs were to adopt a superstitious explanation
examined. for the reinforcement; Superstition
was associated with participants’
belief in improved future performance
and with participants’ perceived skill
relative to their opponents’ skills.
Schippers To test the hypotheses that people Athletes Used a 3 by 2 by 2 factorial design; Ritual commitment is greater when
and Van carry out rituals in (football, The within-participant variables were: uncertainty is high and importance of
Lange uncertain situations, where the volleyball, relative standing and importance of the game is also high; Both effects of
(2006) outcome is important to them; hockey) the outcome; Between-participants importance and uncertainty appeared
Psychological tension may mediate N = 197 variable was: locus of control; to be mediated by psychological
the predicted effects; Differences in Manipulated six vignettes: relative tension; External control was linked
locus of control influence standing (inferior, equal or superior to greater levels of commitment to
superstitious behavior. opponent) and importance (final rituals.
versus training match).
Foster Hypothesized that basketball Athletes Used a 2 by 3 (group by trials) Very little difference was found
et al. performance is worst when no pre- (basketball) repeated measures design (groups: between PPR and SB, worst
(2006) performance routine (PPR) is used, N = 20 experimental and control; performance when neither was used.
better when athletes use superstitious experimental trials: SB, removal of Use of PPRs only returned
behaviors (SB) and the best when SB, PPR); Task: 20 free-throw performance to baseline (as compared
PPR is used. attempts; The PPR consisted of a with SB).
concentration cue, a relaxation cue,
an imagery cue and a cue word.
Wright Tested the hypothesis that participants Nonathletes Golf–putting task – four different No difference was seen between
and show more superstitious behavior N = 40 colored balls (using the win-stay, superstitious behaviors in the easy
Erdal when they are faced with a more lose-shift strategy). versus the difficult condition;
(2008) difficult task. Significant interaction was found
between skill level and task difficulty
on superstitious behaviors; Low
skilled participants demonstrated
more superstitious behavior in the
easy condition, whereas highly skill
participants showed the same trend in
the difficult condition.
Damisch Activating superstitious acts leads to Students (i) 10–trial putting task; link the (i) Found better performance with an
et al. improved performance, this effect is N = 28 concept of good luck to the ball. ostensibly lucky ball.
(2010) mediated by increased level of self– N = 51 (ii) Motor–dexterity task; Superstition (ii) Participants in the superstitious
efficacy. activated group (“I keep my fingers condition solved the task faster than
crossed”); First control (“I press the those in the two control conditions.
watch for you”); Second control
(“on ‘go’ you go”).
N = 41 (iii) Memory game; presence versus (iii) Participants with lucky charm
absence of their personal lucky performed better and reported higher
charm; assessing participants’ self– levels of self-fficacy; No difference
efficacy beliefs. was found in anxiety or in the mood;
Bootstrapping analysis revealed that
self–efficacy mediated the effect of
the lucky charm.

(continued)

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
378 Z. D€om€ot€or et al. Scand J Psychol 57 (2016)

Table 3 (continued)

Author(s) Research objectives Participants Methods Key findings

N = 31 (iv) Anagram task; Presence or (iv) Participants with lucky charm


absence of a personal lucky charm; performed better and reported higher
Assessing participants’ self-efficacy, self-efficacy; Bootstrapping analysis
performance goals, task persistence. revealed that self-efficacy mediated
this effect; Higher goals and a longer
persistence in the presence of lucky
charm were mediated by perceived
self-efficacy; A final bootstrapping
showed that persistence mediated the
effect of self-efficacy.
Brevers Studied athletic identity in relation to Athletes Within-group design: vignette–method Three thirds (165 = 75.8%) of the
et al. sport superstition; Level of (soccer, (as Schippers & Van Lange, 2006): participant reported SR(s); No
(2011) competition affects superstitious volleyball, Manipulated competition importance difference between competition level
rituals (SR); Higher ritual table tennis, and uncertainty (relative standing); and score for superstition; The effect
commitment (RC) occurs in uncertain judo, fencing) The outcome measures were: of the competition level emerged only
and important situations; Tension is N = 219 psychological tension, number and in the case of elite sportsmen who
positively linked to a level of form of SR, degree of superstitious showed higher RC against superior
superstition and mediates the effects feelings, RC; The Athletic Identity opponents; Positive correlation
of uncertainty and importance on RC. Measurement Scale (AIMS; Brewer, emerged between athletic identity and
Van Raalte & Linder, 1993). SR; Intensity of superstition depended
on situational factors: uncertainty and
importance increased the level of RC;
Psychological tension mediated the
effect of both uncertainty and
importance on RC; Woman exhibited
more superstitious behaviors that was
attributed to subjective feelings of
pre-competitive psychological
tension.

generally supportive of a link between superstition and the mental state in sport contest and during high levels of
illusion of control, as well as with self-efficacy. The results of this competitions.
study also suggest that experimentally induced superstitious
beliefs may be qualitatively different from the already adopted –
learned and/or conditioned – superstitions, since previous studies STATUS OF KNOWLEDGE
identified athletes as especially superstitious individuals. Pioneering research contribution to the understanding of
However, in this experiment the degree of athletic involvement superstitious behaviors in sport should be credited to Neil (1982).
was not associated with any superstition generated by the He attempted to reveal how sports superstitions originate, and
experimental procedure, but only with traditional superstitions under what condition could they be observed. In doing so, he
such as having a lucky charm or number. studied the variations in superstition from one sport to another
Overall, there is an inconsistency about the beneficial effect and looked at sex differences as well. Neil (1982) observed that
of superstitious behaviors in the literature (Calin-Jageman & not only athletes have their own superstitions but coaches,
Caldwell, 2010) and several environmental and personality managers, and even spectators often exhibit sport-related
factors (e.g., the skill level of the athlete, the importance of the superstitions. He believed that most of the superstitions are kept
task, locus of control, etc.) can influence the ultimate effect. secret, because holding a superstition carries a pejorative taint.
However, several experiments can be found where superstitious Further, athletes often do not recognize that their irrational,
beliefs benefit the sport performer (Damisch et al., 2010; Foster unfounded beliefs are in fact superstitions. These factors present a
et al., 2006; Lee 2011). These benefits most likely result from a difficulty in the examining of superstitious behavior in sport. The
placebo effect yielding reassurance, perceived control, and major contribution of Neil’s (1982) work was the categorization
reduced anxiety. These acts could help the athlete to reach the of the superstitions into three categories: (1) origin of the
optimal performance state (Garfield & Bennett, 1984) or a superstition (a. Culture, b. Sport, c. Personal; Coffin, 1971);
mental comfort zone. With the examination of the factors that (2) type or form of superstition (clothing, manner of dressing,
influence the athletes’ beliefs, scholars can help coaches, sport numbers, etc.; Becker, 1975); and (3) anthropology of the
psychologists and other sports professionals to work more superstitions (a. Rituals, b. Taboos, c. Fetishes; Gmelch, 1972).
effectively with their athletes and help them to cope with Based on our review of the field, and the new knowledge that
uncertainty (Burke et al., 2006). In other words, personal beliefs has emerged in the past three decades, we suggest the expansions
could be used as a psychological means to achieving an optimal of Neil’s categories – with delimitation to a context in sports

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol 57 (2016) Superstition in sport 379

Personality
Ratio of (perceived) Current needs,
characteristics
challenge to skill ambitions, and
• Self confidence
key motivations
• Self efficacy
• Optimism
• Need to avoid failure,
etc.

Predictability of
Superstitious
Learning and
the outcome, or Behaviour in Conditioning
uncertainty Sport

Social approval Earlier or


and disapproval Perceived importance of past
(of the superstitious act) the event to the self experience

Fig. 2. The many factors that may affect superstitious behavior in sport. Due to their dynamic and interactional nature, the contribution of each will
depend on the context/situation.

superstitions – for the more systematic investigation of the issue. by a set of complex interactions. Therefore, this form of
We recommend a differentiation between personal and social behavior can be best studied from a dynamic and interactional
superstitions, that fits well into Neil’s (1982) first category. The perspective, by taking into consideration the many factors that
qualitative differences in the superstitious acts between men and may affect it (Fig. 2). It is imperative to appreciate the
women fit into Neil’s second category. As for Neil’s third personality characteristics that interact with several situational
category, very little research was conducted in the area of sports, factors at any given time to determine the use and possibly
and most of them were restricted to rituals. We recommend the even the efficacy of superstitious behavior in sports. Whether
use of four further subcategories within sports: (i) in athletic level, superstitions lead to a desired result via a placebo effect is not
elite – non-elite (Flanagan, 2013); (ii) team sport versus clear, but recent research evidence revealed that superstition and
individual sport (Flanagan, 2013); (iii) player position in the team the placebo effect are strongly correlated and share almost 25%
(Ciborowski, 1997; Gmelch, 1972; Neil, 1980;); (iv) interaction of the common variance (Rekhviashvili & Gupta, 2015). There
of personal and situational factors: (a) skill and challenge (Wright are few differences in superstitious behaviors between athletes
& Erdal, 2008), and (b) level of uncertainty and importance and non-athletes, which could be observed in the quality and
of the competition (Schippers & Van Lange, 2006). These spectrum rather than the quantity of superstitions. Even inactive
“interactional” sub-categories are not independent of the involvement in sports, like being a sports fan, may be linked to
personality characteristics of the athletes that predispose them to more superstition than non-involvement. Superstitious behaviors
engage in superstitious acts. While there is a relative shortage of may vary with the type of sport and role of the player in team
research in this area, our review of the academic literature sports, and increases with the level of competition or challenge.
suggests that some of the personality factors are more likely to be Cultural differences, in conjunction with the education level,
linked to involvement in superstitious behaviors than others. appear to also influence superstitious behavior is a sport, while
Indeed, a fierce athletic-identity (ego-threat), external locus of gender differences are more closely connected to the form or
control, pessimism, religiosity, and anxiety were moderate, or at type of the superstition rather than its magnitude, and they may
least mild, links to superstitious behavior in sports. Clearly, these disappear at a higher level of challenge. In spite of the
relationships are very preliminary, and in some cases may only be presumed similarity, religiosity and superstition appear to be
tentative. However, they may serve a starting new infrastructure different behavioral facilitators in sport. Personality factors, like
for encouraging more empirical research that could clarify the locus of control, perceived control, skill and self-efficacy,
contribution of various personality traits to superstitious behavior optimism, pessimism, and paranormal beliefs are consistently
in sports. linked to superstitious behavior in sport. Further, the importance
of an event, and the degree of uncertainty associated with that
event, appear to play a crucial role in acts of superstitious
CONCLUSIONS AND OUTLOOK behavior in sport. Similarly, an interaction between the level of
Summing up the current review, it is evident that the athletic skill and task difficulty is another predictor of
manifestation of superstitious behaviors in sports is determined superstitious behavior.

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
380 Z. D€om€ot€or et al. Scand J Psychol 57 (2016)

Future studies need to investigate the actual contribution of the Calin-Jageman, R. J. & Caldwell, T. L. (2014). Replication of the
various determinants to the emergence of superstitious beliefs superstition and performance study by Damisch, Stoberock, and
Mussweiler (2010). Social Psychology, 45, 239–245.
and behaviors in sport, by considering the personality factors
Campbell, C. (1996). Half–belief and the paradox of behaviour
associated with the behavior. Such studies need to be complex, instrumental activism: A theory of modern superstition. British Journal
because apart from personality factors, cultural heritage, and of Sociology, 47, 151–166.
gender, situational factors like type of sport, role in sports, degree Ciborowski, T. (1997). “Superstition” in the collegiate baseball player. The
of challenge, level of skill and opponents’ characteristics, all need Sport Psychologist, 11, 305–317.
Coffin, T. P. (1971). The old ball game: Baseball in folklore and fiction.
to be considered in a very thorough examination of superstitious
New York: Herder and Herder Publishers.
behavior in sport and exercise settings. Finally, the impact of Cohn, P. J. (1990). Preperformance routines in sport: Theoretical support
negative superstitions (acting as nocebos) should also be and practical applications. The Sport Psychologist, 4, 301–312.
investigated in future works. Cotterill, S. (2010). Pre-performance routines in sport: Current
understanding and future directions. International Review of Sport &
 Nemzeti Kival
This research was supported by the Hungarian Uj osag Exercise Psychology, 3, 132–153.
Program. Crews, D. J. & Boutcher, S. H. (1986). An exploratory observational
behavior analysis of professional golfers during competition. Journal
of Sport Behavior, 9, 51–58.
Damisch, L., Stoberock, B. & Mussweiler, T. (2010). Keep your fingers
REFERENCES crossed! How superstition improves performance. Psychological
Abele, A. E. & Brack, S. (2015). Preference for other persons’ traits is Science, 21, 1014–1020.
dependent on the kind of social relationship. Social Psychology, 44, Dudley, R. T. (1999). The effect of superstitious belief on performance
84–94. following an unsolvable problem. Personality and Individual
Aruguete, M. S., Goodboy, A. K., Mansson, D. H., Jenkins, W. J. & Differences, 26, 1057–1064.
McCutcheon, L. E. (2012). Does religious faith improve Emme, E. E. (1940). Modification and origin of certain beliefs in
test performance? North American Journal of Psychology, 14, 185–196. superstition among 96 college students. The Journal of Psychology,
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social 10, 279–291.
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Fischer, G. (1997). Abstention from sex and other pre-game rituals used
Becker, J. (1975). Superstition in sport. International Journal of Sport by college male varsity athletes. Journal of Sport Behavior, 20, 176–
Psychology, 6, 148–152. 184.
Benedetti, F. & Frisaldi, E. (2014). Creating placebo responders and Flanagan, E. (2013). Superstitious ritual in sport and the competitive
nonresponders in the laboratory: boons and banes. Pain Management, anxiety response in elite and non–elite athletes. Unpublished
4, 165–167. dissertation, DBS eSource, Dublin Business School. Retrieved 20 June
Berger, A. S. (2012). The evil eye – an ancient superstition. Journal of 2015 from http://esource.dbs.ie/handle/10788/1599.
Religion and Health, 51, 1098–1103. Foster, K. R. & Kokko, H. (2009). The evolution of superstitious and
Berdi, M., K€oteles, F., Szabo, A. & Bardos, G. (2011). Placebo effects in superstition-like behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
sport and exercise: A meta–analysis. European Journal of Mental London B: Biological Sciences, 276, 31–37.
Health, 6, 196–212. Foster, D. J., Weigand, D. A. & Baines, D. (2006). The effect of removing
Bleak, J. L. & Frederick, C. M. (1998). Superstitious behavior in sport: superstitious behavior and introducing a pre-performance routine on
Levels of effectiveness and determinants of use in three collegiate basketball free-throw performance. Journal of Applied Sport
sports. Journal of Sport Behavior, 21, 1–15. Psychology, 18, 167–171.
Boutcher, S. H. & Crews, D. J. (1987). The effect of a pre-shot routine on Friedman, M. & Rosenman, R. (1974). Type A behaviour and your heart.
a well–learned skill. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 18, New York: Knopf.
30–39. Frisaldi, E., Piedimonte, A. & Benedetti, F. (2015). Placebo and nocebo
Brevers, D., Nils, F., Dan, B. & No€el, X. (2011). Sport superstition: effects: A complex interplay between psychological factors and
Mediation of psychological tension on non-professional sportsmen’s neurochemical networks. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 57,
superstitious rituals. Journal of Sport Behavior, 34, 3–24. 267–284.
Brewer, B. W., Van Raalte, J. L. & Linder, D. E. (1993). Athletic identity: Garfield, C. A. & Bennett, H. Z. (1984). Peak performance: Mental
Hercules’ muscles or Achilles heel? International Journal of Sport training techniques of the world’s greatest athletes. Los Angeles, CA:
Psychology, 24, 237–254. Tarcher.
Buhrmann, H. G., Brown, B. & Zaugg, M. K. (1982). Superstitious beliefs Gmelch, G. (1972). Magic in professional baseball. In G. P. Dans
and behavior. A comparison of male and female basketball players. Stone (Ed.), Games, sport and power (pp. 128–137). New
Journal of Sport Behavior, 5, 75–185. Brunswick: Transaction Books.
Buhrmann, H. G. & Zaugg, M. K. (1981). Superstitions among basketball Gregory, C. J. & Petrie, B. M. (1972). Superstition in sport. Paper
players: An investigation of various forms of superstitious beliefs and presented at the Fourth Canadian Psychomotor Learning and Sports
behavior among competitive basketballers at the junior high school to Psychology Symposium, Waterloo, October. Retrieved 31 May 2016
university level. Journal of Sport Behavior, 4, 163–174. from http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED085340.pdf.
Buhrmann, H. G. & Zaugg, M. K. (1983). Religion and superstition in the Gregory, C. J. & Petrie, B. M. (1975). Superstitions of Canadian
sport of basketball. Journal of Sport Behavior, 6, 146–157. intercollegiate athletes: An intersport comparison. International Review
Burger, J. M. & Lynn, A. L. (2005). Superstitious behavior among of Sport Sociology, 10, 59–68.
American and Japanese professional baseball players. Basic and Hamerman, E. J. & Johar, G. V. (2013). Conditioned superstition: Desire
Applied Social Psychology, 27, 71–76. for control and consumer brand preferences. Journal of Consumer
Burke, K. L., Joyner, A. B., Czech, D. R., Knight, J. L., Scott, L. A., Research, 40, 428–443.
Benton, S. G. & Roughton, H. K. (2006). An exploratory investigation Hamerman, E. J. & Morewedge, C. K. (2015). Reliance on luck:
of superstition, personal control, optimism and pessimism in NCAA Identifying which achievement goals elicit superstitious behavior.
division I intercollegiate student-athletes. Athletic Insight, 8 (2). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41, 323–335.
Retrieved June 22, 2015 from: http://www.athleticinsight.com/ Irwin, H. J. (1994). Paranormal belief and proneness to dissociation.
Vol8Iss2/Superstition.htm Psychological Reports, 75, 1344–1346.

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol 57 (2016) Superstition in sport 381

Jackson, S. A. & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Flow in sports. Chicago, Pronin, E., Wegner, D. M., McCarthy, K. & Rodriguez, S. (2006).
IL: Human Kinetics. Everyday magical powers: The role of apparent mental causation in
Jahoda, G. (1969). The psychology of superstition. London: The Penguin the overestimation of personal influence. Journal of Personality and
Press. Social Psychology, 91, 218–231.
Joukhador, J., Blaszczynski, A. & Maccallum, F. (2004). Superstitious Rao, L. L., Zheng, Y., Zhou, Y. & Li, S. (2014). Probing the neural basis
beliefs in gambling among problem and non-problem gamblers: of superstition. Brain Topography, 27, 766–770.
Preliminary data. Journal of Gambling Studies, 20, 171–180. Rekhviashvili, N. & Gupta, S. (2015). Superstition predicts favorable
Lee, C., Linkenauger, S. A., Bakdash, J. Z., Joy-Gaba, J. A. & Profitt, weight change in an open-placebo trial: A prospective study. Eating
D. R. (2011). Putting like a pro: The role of positive contagion in golf and Weight Disorders – Studies on Anorexia, Bulimia and Obesity, 20,
performance and perception. PloS One, 6, e26016. doi:10.1371/ 389–395.
journal.pone.0026016. Rogers, P. (1998). The cognitive psychology of lottery gambling: A
Lerner, M. J. (1965). Evaluation of performance as a function of theoretical review. Journal of Gambling Studies, 14, 111–134.
performer’s reward and attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Rudski, J. (2001). Competition, superstition and the illusion of control.
Social Psychology, 1, 355–360. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, Personality, Social,
Lindheimer, J. B., O’Connor, P. J. & Dishman, R. K. (2015). Quantifying 20, 68–84.
the placebo effect in psychological outcomes of exercise training: A Rudski, J. (2004). The illusion of control, superstitious belief, and
meta-analysis of randomized trials. Sports Medicine, 45, 693–711. optimism. Current Psychology: Developmental, Learning, Personality,
Lindstr€om, B. & Olsson, A. (2015). Mechanisms of social avoidance Social, 22, 306–315.
learning can explain the emergence of adaptive and arbitrary Rudski, J. M. & Edwards, A. (2010). Malinowski goes to college: Factors
behavioral traditions in humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: influencing students’ use of ritual and superstition. The Journal of
General, 144, 688–703. General Psychology, 134, 389–403.
Lobmeyer, D. & Wasserman, E. A. (1986). Preliminaries to free throw Saenko, IU, V. (2014). The superstitions of today’s college students.
shooting: Superstitious behavior? Journal of Sport Behavior, 9, Russian Education & Society, 47, 76–89.
70–78. Samuelsen, R. (1957). Superstition in sport. Detroit, MI: The Sport
Malinowski, B. (1979). The role of magic and religion. In W. A. Lessa & Library of Studebaler-Packard Corp.
E. Z. Vogt (Eds.), Reader in comparative religion: An anthropological Schippers, M. C. & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2006). The psychological
approach (4th edn, pp. 37–46). New York: Harper Collins Publishers. benefits of superstitious rituals in top sport: A study among top
Maranise, A. M. J. (2013). Superstition and religious ritual: An examination sportspersons. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 2532–2553.
of their effects and utilization in sport. The Sport Psychologist, 27, Sheard, M. (2012). Mental toughness: The mindset behind sporting
83–91. achievement. London: Routledge.
Martin, A. J., Marsh, H. W. & Debus, R. L. (2003). Self-handicapping Skinner, B. (1948). Superstition in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental
and defensive pessimism: A model of self-protection from a Psychology, 38, 168–172.
longitudinal perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, Singer, R. N. (2002). Preperformance state, routines, and automaticity:
1–36. What does it take to realize expertise in self-paced events? Journal of
Matute, H. (1994). Learned helplessness and superstitious behavior as Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24, 359–375.
opposite effects of uncontrollable reinforcement in humans. Learning Swann, C., Keegan, R. J., Piggott, D. & Crust, L. (2012). A
and Motivation, 25, 216–232. systematic review of the experience, occurrence, and controllability
McClearn, D. G. (2004). Interest in sports and beliefs in sports of flow states in elite sport. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 13,
superstitions 1. Psychological Reports, 94, 1043–1047. 807–819.
Menapace, V. (2012). Rituale im leistungssport. Diplomarbeit, Universit€at Szabo, A. (2013). Acute psychological benefits of exercise:
Wien. Retrieved 9 June 2016 from http://othes.univie.ac.at/19032/1/ Reconsideration of the placebo effect. Journal of Mental Health, 22,
2012-03-11_0508785.pdf. 449–455.
Mocan, N. & Pogorelova, L. (2014). Compulsory schooling laws and Szabo, A., Berdi, M., K€ oteles, F. & Bardos, G. (2013). Perceptual
formation of beliefs: Education, religion and superstition. National characteristics of nutritional supplements determine the expected
Bureau of Economic Research. Retrieved 1 June 2015 from http:// effectiveness in boosting strength, endurance, and concentration
www.nber.org/papers/w20557. performances. International Journal of Sport Nutrition and Exercise
Neil, G. (1980). The place of superstition in sport: The self–fulfilling Metabolism, 23, 624–628.
prophecy. Coaching Review, 3, 40–42. The Concordian (2012). A look at pre-game rituals and superstitions from
Neil, G. (1982). Demistifying sport superstition. International Review of your stingers. Retrieved 15 June 2015 from http://theconcordian.com/
Sport Sociology, 17, 99–124. 2012/10/a–look–at–pre–game–rituals–and–superstitions–from–your–
Neil, G., Anderson, B. & Sheppard, W. (1981). Superstitions among male stingers/.
and female athletes of various levels of involvement. Journal of Sport Tobacyk, J. J., Nagot, E. & Miller, M. (1988). Paranormal beliefs and
Behavior, 4, 137–148. locus of control: A multidimensional examination. Journal of
Newkey-Burden, C. (2014). 10 sportsmen and their strange superstitions. Personality Assessment, 52, 241–246.
The Telegraph, November 5. Retrieved 15 June 2015 from http:// Todd, M. & Brown, C. (2003). Characteristics associated with
www.telegraph.co.uk/men/the-filter/11208062/10-sportsmen-and-their- superstitious behavior in track and field athletes: Are there NCAA
strange-superstitions.html. divisional level differences? Journal of Sport Behavior, 26, 168–
Ofori, P. K., Biddle, S. & Lavallee, D., (2012). The role of superstition 187.
among professional footballers in Ghana. Athletic Insight: The Online Torma, N., Berdi, M., K€oteles, F. & Bardos, G. (2013). First experiences
Journal of Sport Psychology, 14. Retrieved 22 June 2015 from http:// with the Hungarian version of the Sport Superstition Scale. Magyar
www.athleticinsight.com/Vol14Iss2/Feature.htm. Sporttudom anyi Szemle, 14, 52–58 (in Hungarian).
Pargament, K. I., Ensing, D. S., Falgout, K., Olsen, H., Reilly, B., Van Van Raalte, J. L., Brewer, B. W., Nemeroff, C. J. & Linder, D. E. (1991).
Haitsma, K. & Warren, R. (1990). God help me: (I): Religious coping Chance orientation and superstitious behaviour on the putting green.
efforts as predictors of the outcomes to significant negative life events. Journal of Sport Behavior, 14, 41–50.
American Journal of Community Psychology, 18, 793–824. Vyse, S. A. (2013). Believing in magic: The psychology of superstition
Perkos, S., Barkoukis, V. & Christopoulos, I. (2013). Superstitious beliefs (updated edn). New York: Oxford University Press.
and behaviours in sport: Association with locus of control, anxiety and Wiseman, R. & Watt, C. (2004). Measuring superstitious belief: Why
demographic characteristics. Inquiries in Sport & Physical Education, lucky charms matter. Personality and Individual Differences, 37,
11, 20–31 (in Greek). 1533–1541.

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
382 Z. D€om€ot€or et al. Scand J Psychol 57 (2016)

Womack, M. (1979). Why athletes need ritual: A study of magic among Zaugg, M. K. (1980). Superstitious beliefs of basketball players. Theses,
professional athletes. In W. J. Morgan (Ed.), Sport and the Dissertations, Professional Paper 3784. Retrieved 31 May 2016 from
humanities: A collection of original essays (pp. 27–38). Knoxville, http://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/3784.
TN: Bureau of Educational Research and Service, The University of Zebb, B., J., Moore & M., C. (2003). Superstitiousness and perceived
Tennessee. anxiety control as predictors of psychological distress. Journal of
Womack, M. (1992a). Green cars, black cats, lady luck. In S. J. Hoffman Anxiety Disorders, 17, 115–130.
(Ed.), Sports and Religion (pp. 203–212). Champaign, IL: Human Zimmerman, B. J. (1995). Self–efficacy and educational development. In
Kinetics Publishers. A. Bandura (Ed.), Self–efficacy in changing societies (pp. 202–231).
Womack, M. (1992b). Why athletes need ritual: A study of magic among New York: Cambridge University Press.
professional athletes. In S. J. Hoffman (Ed.), Sport and religion
(pp. 191–202). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics Publishers. Received 12 December 2015, accepted 2 May 2016
Wright, P. B. & Erdal, K. J. (2008). Sport superstition as a function
of skill level and task difficulty. Journal of Sport Behavior, 31,
187–199.

© 2016 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd

You might also like