You are on page 1of 7

Psychological Medicine, 2005, 35, 387393.

f 2004 Cambridge University Press


DOI : 10.1017/S0033291704003538 Printed in the United Kingdom

The stability of the Parental Bonding Instrument over


a 20-year period
K A Y W I L H E L M*, H E A T H E R N I V E N, G O R D O N P A R K E R
A N D D U S A N H A D Z I-P A V L O V I C

School of Psychiatry, University of New South Wales and Mood Disorders Unit, Black Dog Institute,
Sydney, Australia

ABSTRACT
Background. The Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) measures the perception of being parented to
the age of 16 years. Low scores on the care dimension and high scores on the overprotection
dimension are considered to be risk factors of depression. While the PBI has been shown to be a
reliable and valid instrument, the stability of the PBI over extended periods (taking into account
individual characteristics and life experience) needs to be demonstrated.
Method. The PBI was measured in a non-clinical cohort on four waves between 1978 and 1998,
along with a series of self-report measures including state depression and neuroticism. Dierences in
PBI change over time were examined by gender, lifetime major depression diagnosis, and life event
variables, as well as by scores on neuroticism and state depression.
Results. Acceptable retest coecients on PBI scores over the 20-year study were found for the
cohort. No dierences were found in PBI scores over time on the variables examined, including sex
and depression measures.
Conclusions. The results indicate long-term stability of the PBI over time. The inuences of mood
state and life experience appear to have little eect on the stability of the perception of parenting as
measured by the PBI. The present study increases condence in the PBI as a valid measure of
perceived parenting over extended time periods.

INTRODUCTION the role of parental style to be accurately


examined and quantied.
The Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) is a self-
The reliability of the PBI has been tested in a
report questionnaire developed to measure the
number of studies over brief intervals (ranging
subjective experience of being parented to the
from 134 weeks) (Richman & Flaherty, 1987 ;
age of 16 years (Parker et al. 1979). It is the most
Plantes et al. 1988 ; Mackinnon et al. 1989). One
consistently used measure of parenting style
study reported longer-term reliability (Gotlib
(Enns et al. 2002) in a range of clinical and
non-clinical subject groups (Parker, 1983 ; Silove et al. 1988) for the PBI in depressed and non-
depressed women, rated in the post-partum
et al. 1991 ; Mak, 1994). Scores on the measure
period and again 24 years later (mean maternal
have been shown to be associated with an
increased risk of several psychiatric disorders, care scores at time 1 and time 2 as 28.2 v. 26.7
for non-depressed and 17.3 v. 17.6 for depressed,
particularly certain expressions of depression
and mean maternal overprotection scores as 9.6
and anxiety (Parker, 1983), and have allowed
v. 8.7 for non-depressed and 22.0 v. 23.5 for
depressed). A study of 10-year testretest re-
* Address for correspondence: Associate Professor Kay Wilhelm, liability (Wilhelm & Parker, 1990) also found a
Consultation Liaison Psychiatry, Level 4, DeLacy Building, St
Vincents Hospital, Victoria Street, Sydney, NSW, 2010, Australia. consistency of care and overprotection scores
(Email : kwilhelm@stvincents.com.au) (maternal care, 26.3 v. 26.3, r=0.63 ; maternal
387
388 K. Wilhelm et al.

overprotection, 14.8 v. 13.8, r=0.68; paternal perceptions over time could be ascertained. The
care, 21.9 v. 21.4, r=0.72; paternal over- life event of whether or not cohort members
protection, 13.0 v. 11.9, r=0.56). had become a parent was considered separately.
Consistent PBI scores over extended periods The extended period includes assessment of
argue against attitudinal change due to life the PBI at times when cohort members not
experience or uctuations in mood inuencing only became parents themselves, but had also
recollections of earlier parental environment. parented teenage children. As the PBI assesses
We sought to examine the long-term reliability perceptions of being parented to the age of 16
of the PBI with a cohort who had rst com- years, the time span allowed an opportunity
pleted the measure more than two decades for the cohort to revisit the experience of being
previously and to consider factors which may parented after they had viewed the experience
have aected PBI scores over time, including from the other side . Lastly, gender dierences
gender, number of life events, depression were considered as previous studies had found
history, current mood and neuroticism level. that women were more consistent reporters
While the importance of early parenting ex- of both their depressive episodes over time
periences in the development of adult depression (Angst & Mikola, 1984; Wilhelm & Parker,
has been well established (Perris, 1966; Raskin 1994) and their perception of the quality of their
et al. 1971), the eect of depression on percep- parents marital relationships (Wilhelm et al.
tions of parenting has been less researched. 2000).
Most studies investigating the relation between In summary, we hypothesized that the pass-
depression and perceptions of parenting have age of time would have no eect on perception
involved a single assessment, which does not of parental care ; second, that as cohort mem-
allow the direction of causality to be deter- bers had children of their own and more life
mined. One longitudinal study which sought to experience, any changes in perception of their
disentangle the eect of mood on perceptions of own parenting would be reected in relation to
parenting (Gotlib et al. 1988) hypothesized that control and overprotection ; third, that those
depressed individuals negatively distorted per- who had experienced multiple life events may
ceptions of their environment (Beck et al. 1979) show a greater amount of change in parental
that facilitated the perception and recall of perceptions over the study period than those
negative information (Bower, 1981). However, with less life events experienced ; fourth, that
they found that stable perceptions of early depression and neuroticism would not aect
parenting did not appear to be aected by de- parental perceptions over time, and lastly, that
pressed mood (Gotlib et al. 1988). Neuroticism women were likely to be more consistent
has been found to be a more stable construct reporters of their early experience of being
than state depression (Wilhelm & Parker, 1990) parented than men.
but changes in neuroticism levels have been
linked to current symptoms of depression
(Farmer et al. 2002) and repeated episodes METHOD
(Wilhelm et al. 1999). We have detailed the study extensively in pre-
In the present study, comparisons of PBI vious publications (Wilhelm & Parker, 1989,
scores over time between those with and without 1993, 1994), and here only summary details are
a lifetime history of major depression, and uc- provided. In 1978, students completing a 1-year
tuations over time in mood and neuroticism postgraduate teacher-training programme were
levels, have been assessed. Intra-individual var- invited to participate in a longitudinal study.
iations were reported by the use of change The 170 (114 women and 56 men, mean age
measures (assessing individual variations in 23 years) who gave informed consent and com-
scores at each study wave) applied to mood and pleted baseline data formed the study cohort,
neuroticism scores. As life events were recorded and have been followed up at 5-yearly intervals.
for the period between initial assessment and At each follow-up assessment (1983, 1988,
nal assessment of the PBI, the relationship 1993, 1998), participants completed a series of
between the total number of life events in this self-report questionnaires as part of a semi-
designated period and changes in parental structured interview, which covered physical
The stability of the Parental Bonding Instrument 389

and mental health including depression, and assessments of depressive episodes are made
aspects of lifestyle, work and social support. closer to the time of their occurrence (Wilhelm
The PBI was administered on three of the & Parker, 1994).
four follow-up assessment waves (1983, 1988, As part of the comprehensive semi-structured
1998) in addition to baseline assessment. PBI interview at 15-year and 20-year follow-up as-
data was obtained from 164 in 1983, 163 in sessments, the occurrence of a range of signi-
1988, and 144 (93 females and 51 males) in 1998, cant life events between the years 1978 and 1998
consisting of 85 % of the original cohort. was recorded on life charts. The 20-year follow-
Specically, complete maternal PBI data at all up served as a verication of previously identied
four waves were obtained for 144 participants, life events and the years of their occurrence, in
and for paternal PBI data, 138 participants. The addition to reporting on new life events that had
six remaining participants with incomplete occurred in the recent 19931998 interval. A wide
paternal PBI data, due to such reasons as death range of events with diering qualities, severities
of the father or lack of contact with the father, and types were recorded, including positive life
were not included in the paternal PBI analysis of events such as having a child or being promoted,
the present study. Those who were followed up and negative life events such as marital break-
in 1998 signicantly diered from the baseline down and death of parent. This method of eli-
sample only on the maternal care score of the citing life events, similar to that used by Brown
PBI, with the 1998 sample having a signicantly & Harris (1978) is both a comprehensive and
higher maternal care score (mean=26.6, S.D.= highly specic account of each participants ex-
6.9) than the sample who were no longer part of perience of life events. Lastly, information was
the cohort [mean=23.3, S.D.=7.4 ; t(163)= obtained at each assessment on whether the
x2.0, p=0.04]. The magnitude of the dier- cohort had become a parent, as well as the ages
ences in the means, using guidelines proposed by of their respective children. This variable was
Cohen (1992), was small to moderate (Cohens d considered separately in the analyses.
eect size=0.46).
Two other administered self-report ques- Statistical analyses
tionnaires relevant to the present study measure Stability and change in mean levels of the PBI
neuroticism and state depression . Neuroticism were tested using repeated-measures ANOVA
was measured using the Eysenck Personality over the four assessments. Results for both the
Inventory (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1964) at base- linear and quadratic trends in scores over time
line and all four follow-up assessments. State have been reported. Interactions between gender
depression was measured using the Wilson and time were also tested in a between-subjects
Lovibond state depression measure (Wilson, repeated-measures ANOVA design.
1979) at baseline and the rst two follow-up In order to observe diering patterns in scores
assessments (1978, 1983, 1988). between subgroups of the sample (i.e. lifetime
The presence of DSM major depressive epi- history of major depression; becoming a parent ;
sodes was assessed on all four follow-up assess- and total number of reported life events between
ments, using the Diagnostic Interval Schedule 1978 and 1998), separate repeated-measures
(DIS ; Robins et al. 1981) in 1983 and 1988, and ANOVAs for each of these variables were con-
later the Composite International Diagnostic ducted, and interaction eects with time were
Interview (CIDI ; Robins & Helzer, 1988) in examined. For these variables, mean PBI scores
1993 and 1998. A lifetime major depression at baseline were compared to mean PBI scores
diagnosis was made using an add-on strategy. at the 20-year follow-up in 1998 (i.e. only
Those who met criteria for major depression in scores on these two occasions were compared),
the initial 1983 assessment remained cases , as the variables relate to events or diagnoses
and new cases were added as they were newly that may have occurred at any time during
identied in subsequent assessments (i.e. those the 20-year period. The variable for the total
who had their rst depressive episode in the number of reported life events experienced over
preceding interval since their last assessment). the 20-year period was found to closely resemble
This strategy was employed as we have a normal distribution, and remained as a con-
previously demonstrated that more stable tinuous variable for the analyses.
390 K. Wilhelm et al.

Table 1. Means and standard deviations on the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) subscales
over the 20-year period
Mean score (S.D.)
Linear or
PBI subscale 1978 1983 1988 1998 Quadraticb F p

Maternal care (n=144) 26.6 (6.9) 26.4 (8.6) 26.9 (8.2) 26.7 (7.8) L 0.12 0.73
Q 0.00 0.99
Maternal OPa (n=144) 14.5 (7.4) 13.7 (8.3) 13.9 (8.5) 13.6 (7.9) L 2.49 0.12
Q 0.55 0.46
Paternal care (n=138) 21.9 (8.9) 21.6 (9.5) 21.7 (8.9) 21.3 (8.9) L 1.09 0.30
Q 0.00 0.96
Paternal OP (n=138) 13.5 (7.5) 12.3 (7.3) 11.9 (7.6) 12.6 (8.2) L 2.14 0.15
Q 9.81 0.002

a
OP, Overprotection.
b
Linear (L) and quadratic (Q) trends are both reported (i.e. within-subjects polynomial contrasts).

The stability in PBI subscales over the 20-year study period. There were no signicant dier-
period of the study was tested by comparing ences in linear trends over time for the cohort.
data from the various time points, using In general, mean care scores appeared to be
Pearson product-moment correlations. The cor- more stable over time than overprotection
relations were repeated for each gender, and for scores. When quadratic trends in scores were
those with and without a lifetime history of considered, paternal overprotection scores re-
major depression. vealed a signicant trend over time [F(1, 137)=
To observe for the possible eects of intra- 9.81, p=0.002]. Mean overprotection scores for
individual variations, change scores were gen- the cohorts fathers lowered over the time in-
erated by subtracting individual PBI scores terval between baseline and 10-year follow-up
at each assessment time with those at other (i.e. between the mean ages of 23 and 33 years),
assessments. These change scores were then and then rose at 20-year follow-up. At this time
correlated with change scores generated for the mean age of the group was 43 years, and
state depression and neuroticism, at the same 80 % of the cohort had become a parent, with
time comparison. most having a teenage child (mean age of
An a-level of 0.05 was used for all statistical oldest child=14.1, S.D.=5.0 years). Separately,
analyses performed. Power restraints of the stat- maternal overprotection scores appeared to
istical tests due to the modest size of the sample gradually decline over time, although this trend
would increase the likelihood that a Type II was not signicant.
error would occur (i.e. failing to reject the null When examining for gender dierences in a
hypothesis when it is, in fact, false). To com- between-subjects repeated-measures ANOVA,
pensate for the size of the sample, the a-level has no signicant interaction eects between gender
been set to 0.05 for the present statistical tests, and time were found [maternal care, F(1, 142)=
without the use of Bonferroni adjustments. This 2.77; maternal overprotection, F(1, 142)=0.94;
decision also appears appropriate in light of the paternal care, F(1, 136)=0.01, paternal over-
nature of the present hypotheses, which propose protection, F(1, 136)=0.83].
that PBI scores do not change signicantly over Interaction eects between the following
time. variables and time were each investigated sep-
arately in a repeated-measures ANOVA design :
lifetime history of major depression, becoming a
RESULTS
parent, and total number of reported life events
Stability and change in mean levels over time : between 1978 and 1998. There were no sig-
repeated-measures analyses nicant interaction eects between lifetime his-
Table 1 summarizes the repeated-measures tory of major depression and time [maternal
ANOVA results on the four subscales of the care, F(1, 142)=0.38; maternal overprotection,
PBI, on four time intervals over the 20-year F(1, 142)=0.07; paternal care, F(1, 136)=0.33;
The stability of the Parental Bonding Instrument 391

Table 2. Stability coecients (95 % CI) of the Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) over four
review intervals
Retest correlations (95% CI)

1978 with 1978 with 1978 with 1983 with 1983 with 1988 with
PBI subscale 1983 1988 1998 1988 1998 1998

Maternal care (n=144) 0.75 0.64 0.73 0.83 0.80 0.78


(0.630.86)* (0.510.77)* (0.620.84)* (0.740.92)* (0.700.90)* (0.680.88)*
Maternal OPa (n=144) 0.75 0.67 0.69 0.77 0.70 0.74
(0.640.86)* (0.550.80)* (0.570.81)* (0.670.88)* (0.580.82)* (0.630.85)*
Paternal care (n=138) 0.82 0.74 0.75 0.81 0.79 0.79
(0.720.91)* (0.630.85)* (0.640.86)* (0.710.91)* (0.690.90)* (0.680.89)*
Paternal OP (n=138) 0.74 0.62 0.59 0.67 0.68 0.78
(0.630.86)* (0.490.75)* (0.450.73)* (0.540.79)* (0.560.81)* (0.67.89)*

a
OP, Overprotection.
* p<0.01.

paternal overprotection, F(1, 136)=0.03], and Intra-individual variations over time :


becoming a parent and time [maternal care, relationships with self-report measures
F(1, 142)=0.95; maternal overprotection, To quantify the relationship between changes in
F(1, 142)=0.20; paternal care, F(1, 136)=0.20; PBI scores over time and changes in other factors
paternal overprotection, F(1, 136)=0.29]. such as mood state, intra-individual variations
Lastly, there were no signicant interaction on the PBI were correlated with intra-individual
eects between total reported life events over variations on two self-report measures ; state
the 20-year period and time [maternal care, depression (measured by the WilsonLovibond
F(1, 140)=1.73; maternal overprotection, measure) and neuroticism (measured by the
F(1, 140)=2.17; paternal care, F(1, 134)=1.61; Eysenck Personality Inventory). Correlations
paternal overprotection, F(1, 134)=0.91]. between intra-individual variations, measured
Stability in individual PBI ratings : retest by change scores (i.e. the subtracted dierence
correlations between two time measurement intervals), were
all small, ranging from r=x0.24 to 0.16 for the
Table 2 reports retest correlations on the PBI state depression comparisons, and from x0.22
over the four time intervals ; with all retest cor- to 0.22 for neuroticism.
relations being signicant at the p<0.01 level.
PBI scores were stable across time, with retest
coecients in the range of 0.640.83 for DISCUSSION
maternal care, and 0.740.82 for paternal care. The results of this study indicate that percep-
Maternal overprotection coecients were in the tions of parental care and overprotection, as
range of 0.670.77, and paternal overprotection measured by the PBI, remained relatively stable
scores 0.590.78. over two decades. The stability of the PBI over
There was no evidence of confounding eects an extended period of time further attests to its
of gender or lifetime history of major depression validity as a measure of perceived parental
on PBI subscales. For gender, retest correlations characteristics to the age of 16 years (Parker
for men were in the range of 0.630.85 for the et al. 1979). The stability of the PBI is well sup-
care dimension, and 0.430.80 for the over- ported in the literature (Plantes et al. 1988 ;
protection dimension, and for women 0.640.83 Mackinnon et al. 1989), and this is the rst known
for care and 0.620.78 for overprotection. study to show its stability over two decades.
For those with a lifetime history of major de- While both the care and overprotection scales
pression, retest correlations were in the range of are robust, the ndings support our hypothesis
0.520.83 for care, and 0.580.83 for over- that the care scale appears the more stable
protection, and for those with no history of dimension. The non-signicant decrease in
major depression, 0.690.86 for care and maternal overprotection scores over the two
0.540.80 for overprotection. decades may reect a mellowing in attitudes,
392 K. Wilhelm et al.

as the cohort moved further away in time from (Angst & Mikola, 1984). The group has been
their adolescence and became more reective. shown to have rates of depression and anxiety
No signicant linear relationships were found, that are similar or greater to those in the general
but the paternal overprotection scores showed community (Wilhelm et al. 1997), but they may
a signicant quadratic trend. Paternal over- be more able to articulate and identify their own
protection scores decreased over the rst 10 experiences compared to those who lead more
years for the cohort (between the mean ages of chaotic lives (Morgan et al. 1993).
23 and 33 years), and then rose again at 20-year A second potential limitation of the present
follow-up (when the mean age was 43 years and study is the modest sample size of the cohort,
most parents had at least one teenage child). limiting power in statistical tests performed. The
Although the signicance of this nding is not possibility of an increase in likelihood of a Type
clear and may be due to Type I error, it may also II error occurring (i.e. failing to reject the null
reect some identication with fathers over- hypothesis when it is, in fact, false) was com-
protection during the years when their own pensated for by maintaining an a-level of 0.05
children were entering adolescence. These nd- for the analyses. However, the possibility that
ings may be worth exploring further in another the non-signicant results obtained in the pres-
population. ent analysis may be the result of a Type II
The present ndings suggest that recollections error cannot be dismissed, and further studies
of ones parental environment are not substan- supporting the ndings, with larger sample
tially inuenced by gender, depression history sizes, would increase condence.
and life experience and that subjects percep- The present study reports on the reliability of
tions do not shift with uctuations in depressed the perception of parenting over time, rather
mood or neuroticism level. The present study than necessarily actual parenting. We did not
allows for an extended analysis of perceptions independently verify actual parenting behaviour
on the PBI over two decades. The study has with the parents of this cohort. Moderate
maintained an excellent retention rate (85 %), agreement has been found between the parents
even two decades after the study. A further and children on the quality of the parenting
strength of the study to assess the stability of experience in previous validity studies (Parker,
perceptions in parenting as measured by the PBI 1983 ; Wilhelm et al. 2000). However, it can also
is the non-clinical basis of the initial recruitment be argued that it is the individuals perception of
into the study. As the PBI was administered at parenting, rather than necessarily the actual
the commencement of the study (in 1978), fol- parenting behaviour, that holds the greatest risk
lowed by subsequent administrations 5, 10 and for subsequent psychopathology. Given the
20 years later, an examination of the potential stability of scores on the PBI over time and the
inuence by such factors as mood state and limited inuence of factors that may abstract
depressive episodes can be more adequately perception such as mood state, the PBI can
explored. This leads to the consideration of the arguably proximate actual parenting behaviour.
generalizability of ndings to the wider popu- In conclusion, the PBI has gained wide
lation. acceptance as a robust measure of perceived
First, this study observes a well-educated co- parenting which renders it a useful instrument in
hort, in which subjects have been conscientious risk factor research. The current ndings of the
in completing a number of measures over 20 reliability of perceptions over two decades fur-
years, and which may produce greater stability ther increase condence in the instrument.
than could be expected in a non-volunteer or
clinical sample. Also, retention bias may exist,
with signicantly lower baseline maternal care ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
scores being found for those who were not suc- The authors thank Lucy Wedgwood and Adam
cessfully followed up 20 years later. The cohort Finch for editorial assistance, and Karen Baikie
has previously been found to be reliable reporters for helpful comments. The work is supported by
of their own experience in earlier reports from NHMRC Program Grant 222708 and by an
the study (Wilhelm & Parker, 1990, 1994), and Infrastructure Grant from the Centre for
to be similar to Angsts larger Swiss cohort Mental Health, NSW Department of Health.
The stability of the Parental Bonding Instrument 393

DECLARATION OF INTEREST Perris, C. (1966). A study of bipolar and unipolar recurrent


depressive psychoses. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 194 (Suppl.),
None. 189.
Plantes, M., Pruso, B., Brennan, J. & Parker, G. (1988). Parental
representations of depressed outpatients from a U.S.A. sample.
REFERENCES Journal of Aective Disorders 15, 149155.
Raskin, A., Boothe, H., Reatig, N., Schulterbrandt, J. & Odle, D.
Angst, J. & Dobler-Mikola, D. (1984). The Zurich study. II. The (1971). Factor analyses of normal and depressed patients
continuum from normal to pathological depressive mood swings. memories of parental behavior. Psychological Reports 29, 871879.
European Archives of Psychiatry and Neuroscience 234, 2129. Richman, J. & Flaherty, J. (1987). Adult psychosocial assets
Beck, A., Rush, A., Shaw, B. & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive Therapy and depressive mood over time : eects of internalized
of Depression. Guilford Press : New York. childhood attachments. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease
Bower, G. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist 36, 175, 703712.
129148. Robins, L. & Helzer, J. (1988). New diagnostic Instruments. In
Brown, G. & Harris, T. (1978). The Social Origins of Depression. Handbook of Social Psychiatry (ed. S. Henderson and G.
Tavistock : London. Burrows), pp. 312. Elsevier : Sydney.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin 112, Robins, L., Helzer, J., Croughan, J. & Ratcli, K. (1981). National
155159. Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule: its his-
Enns, M. W., Cox, B. J. & Clara, I. (2002). Parental bonding and tory, characteristics and validity. Archives of General Psychiatry
adult psychopathology : results from the US national comorbidity 38, 381389.
survey. Psychological Medicine 32, 9971008. Silove, D., Parker, G., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., Manicavasagar, V. &
Eysenck, H. & Eysenck, S. (1964). Manual of the Eysenck Personality Blaszczynski, A. (1991). Parental representations of patients with
Inventory. Hodder and Stoughton : London. panic disorder and generalised anxiety disorder. British Journal of
Farmer, A., Redman, K., Harris, T., Mahmood, A., Sadler, S., Psychiatry 159, 835841.
Pickering, A. & McGun, P. (2002). Neuroticicm, extraversion, Wilhelm, K., Brownhill, S. & Boyce, P. (2000). Marital and family
life events and depression. The Cardi Depression Study. British functioning : dierent measures and viewpoints. Social Psychiatry
Journal of Psychiatry 181, 118122. and Psychiatric Epidemiology 35, 358365.
Gotlib, I., Mount, J., Cordy, N. & Whien, V. (1988). Depression and Wilhelm, K. & Parker, G. (1989). Is sex necessarily a risk factor to
perceptions of early parenting : a longitudinal investigation. British depression ? Psychological Medicine 19, 401413.
Journal of Psychiatry 152, 2427. Wilhelm, K. & Parker, G. (1990). Reliability of the Parental Bonding
Mackinnon, A., Henderson, A., Scott, R. & Duncan-Jones, P. (1989). Instrument and Intimate Bond Measure scales. Australian & New
The Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI) : an epidemiological study Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 24, 199202.
in a general population sample. Psychological Medicine 19, Wilhelm, K. & Parker, G. (1993). Sex dierences in depressiogenic
10231034. risk factors and coping strategies in a socially homogeneous group.
Mak, A. S. (1994). Parental neglect and overprotection as risk factors Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 88, 205211.
in delinquency. Australian Journal of Psychology 46, 107111. Wilhelm, K. & Parker, G. (1994). Sex dierences in depression rates:
Morgan, A., Harris, M., Boyce, P. & Wilhelm, K. (1993). Has social fact or artefact ? Psychological Medicine 24, 97111.
psychiatry meets its Waterloo ? Methodological and ethical issues Wilhelm, K., Parker, G., Dewhurst-Savellis, J. & Asghari, A. (1999).
in a community study. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychological predictors of single and recurrent Major Depressive
Psychiatry 27, 411421. Episodes. Journal of Aective Disorders 54, 139147.
Parker, G. (1983). Development of the parental bonding instrument Wilhelm, K., Parker, G. & Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. (1997). Fifteen years
and the parent opinion form. In Parental Overprotection : A Risk on: evolving ideas in researching sex dierences in depression.
factor in Psychosocial Development. Grune and Stratton : New Psychological Medicine 27, 875883.
York. Wilson, P. (1979). Behavioural and pharmacological treatment of
Parker, G., Tupling, H. & Brown, L. (1979). A parental bonding depression. The University of New South Wales : Unpublished
instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology 52, 110. PhD thesis.

You might also like