You are on page 1of 2

PERSPECTIVES

A Air B A typical ocean island volcano. These cross sections of an ocean island volcano such as a Hawaiian seamount
illustrate the main stages of its evolution: preshield alkalic stage (A), main shield-building stage (B and C),
Water
postshield alkalic stage (D), and rejuvenated stage (E). The rocks most easily sampled or dredged on the
volcano’s surface can be considerably younger than the major phase of volcanism that built the volcano.
Preshield alkalic volcano
Submarine shield
Oceanic plate (Tholeiitic composition) connections among mantle dynamics, Pacific played by ocean drilling on this and many other
plate motion, and major reorganizations of seamount chains.
C
plate boundaries in the western Pacific
Ocean. The Aleutian, Izu-Bonin-Marianas, References
1. D. A. Clague, G. B. Dalrymple, USGS Prof. Pap. 1350, 5
and Tonga-Kermadec subduction zones, (1987).
which all started between 55 and 45 million 2. G. B. Dalrymple, D. A. Clague, Earth Planet. Sci. Lett. 31,
years ago, involved major changes in the 313 (1976).
Subaerial shield (Tholeiitic) volcano 3. T. Atwater, in The Geology of North America, The Eastern
geometry and forces at the boundaries of the Pacific Ocean and Hawaii, E. Winterer, D. M. Hussong, R.
D Postshield alkalic Pacific plate (14, 15). None of these geomet- W. Decker, Eds. (Geological Society of America, Boulder,
ric changes could have occurred unless there CO, 1989), vol. N, p. 21–72.
4. I. O. Norton, Tectonics 14, 1080 (1995).
had already been a change in the relative 5. W. D. Sharp, D. A. Clague, Science 313, 1281 (2006).
velocity of the Pacific plate with respect to 6. B. C. Kerr, D. W. Scholl, S. L. Klemperer, Geochem.

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on June 28, 2010


adjacent plates. The time lag between these Geophys. Geosyst. 6, 10.1029/2004GC000705 (2005).
7. T. A. Bianco, G. Ito, J. M. Becker, M. O. Garcia, Geochem.
changes and the occurrence of the Hawaiian- Geophys. Geosyst. 6, 10.1029/2005GC000945 (2005).
Emperor bend was a major conundrum; the 8. R. A. Duncan, R. A. Keller, Geochem. Geophys. Geosyst.
E Rejuvenated (posterosional) alkalic 5, 10.1029/2004GC000704 (2004).
problem has now been removed by Sharp and 9. J. A. Tarduno et al., Science 301, 1064 (2003); published
Clague. Their results will spur new efforts online 24 July 2003 (10.1126/science.1086442).
to model mantle dynamics and plate kine- 10. M. A. Richards, C. Lithgow-Bertelloni, Earth Planet. Sci.
Lett. 137, 19 (1996).
matics through times of major changes in 11. B. Steinberger, R. Sutherland, R. J. O’Connell, Nature
plate configuration, as well as additional 430, 167 (2004).
data collection efforts. 12. C. A. Raymond, J. M. Stock, S. C. Cande, AGU Geophys.
Monogr. 121, 359 (2000).
More details of the spatial variation in 13. J. M. Parés, T. C. Moore, Earth Planet. Sci. Lett. 237, 951
tion source clearly has not been stationary paleolatitude and in volcanic propagation rates (2005).
with respect to Earth’s spin axis, because for the Hawaiian-Emperor volcanic seamount 14. M. A. Cosca, R. J. Arculus, J. A. Pearce, J. G. Mitchell, E. J.
Essene, Island Arc 7, 579 (1998).
the latitude of the Emperor seamount erup- chain remain to be identified, and the published 15. M. G. Gurnis, C. Hall, L. Lavier, Geochem. Geophys.
tions decreased with time until roughly the age progressions of other volcanic seamount Geosyst. 5, 10.1029/2003GC000681 (2004).
time of the Hawaiian-Emperor bend (9). chains will need to be scrutinized. The results
Duncan and Keller (8) recently showed that of Sharp and Clague highlight the key role 10.1126/science.1131789
the rate of propagation of volcanism along
the Emperor section of the seamount chain
varied considerably, first speeding up and BEHAVIOR
then slowing down. Sharp and Clague now
show that the change in orientation of the
seamount chain was not abrupt: The change
started 50 million years ago and took more
The Power of Social Psychological
than 8 million years to complete.
The revision of the age of such a tectonic
Interventions
feature by 7 million years has far-reaching con- Timothy D. Wilson
sequences. Studies of plate motions and mantle
dynamics that assumed a younger age of the Brief social psychological interventions that focus on people’s perceptions of themselves and their
bend will have to be adjusted (10, 11). Plate environment have been shown to increase academic performance.
reconstructions based only on marine magnetic
anomalies from seafloor spreading centers are

S
ome readers will undoubtedly be sur- unprecedented. Previous studies have found
not affected, but the exact rates of relative hot prised, or even incredulous, that a 15- results of similar magnitude in samples of
spot drift calculated from these reconstructions min intervention can reduce the racial United States college students (see the table)
(12) may have to be adjusted in light of the new achievement gap by 40%. Yet this is precisely (2–4). These studies share important features:
ages assigned to the Hawaiian-Emperor what Cohen et al. (1) report on page 1307 of Each drew on social psychological theories to
volcanism. There will be better agreement this issue. African American seventh graders change people’s self- and social perceptions
between two different methods for determining randomly assigned to write about their most (i.e., people’s explanations for their poor
the northward motion of the Pacific plate—one important values achieved significantly better performance, their views of the malleability
based on sedimentation patterns that show end-of-semester grades than students in a con- of their own intelligence, or their sense of
CREDIT: P. HUEY/SCIENCE

when locations on the Pacific plate crossed the trol condition. How can this be? social connectedness). Each did so with
equator (13), the other based on the age pro- As the authors note, these results are not brief, inexpensive interventions. In each
gression of the Hawaiian seamounts. study, people in the treatment conditions
The new ages for the Hawaiian-Emperor The author is in the Department of Psychology, University of achieved better grades than people in the
bend and seamount chain help to clarify the Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22904–4400, tdw@virginia.edu control conditions. These increases were

www.sciencemag.org SCIENCE VOL 313 1 SEPTEMBER 2006 1251


Published by AAAS
PERSPECTIVES

modest, averaging .29 on a grade-point opposing forces exist in a delicate state of The fact that small, theory-based inter-
average (GPA) scale (where A = 4, B = 3, equilibrium, such that small interventions can ventions can have large effects should not be
and so on; see the table). Nonetheless, these set in motion processes that change the entire taken as a criticism of large-scale attempts at
gains are impressive, given that grades were system. Altering people’s views of them- social change. As important as people’s con-
assessed from several weeks to several selves, or how they think others view them, struals of their environment are, often the
months after the interventions. can lead to cascading changes in motivation environment itself needs changing. The
The Cohen et al. study adds substantially and performance. For example, students can achievement gap is surely caused by multiple
to this earlier work. They used an intervention be caught in a self-perpetuating “exacerba- factors, including poverty, racism, and lack of
tailored to help African Americans, a group tion cycle,” whereby poor academic perform- parental involvement (6). Findings such as
that has underperformed in American educa- ance confirms their worst fears about them- Cohen et al.’s should not divert our attention
tional settings (5). Students in the treatment selves, which increases their anxiety, which from these critical problems. By the same
token, the fact that large-scale
Social psychological interventions societal factors need changing
Participants Intervention Increase in GPA should not prevent us from seek-
African American 7th graders (1) Students wrote about why a selected value was important to them 0.30 ing proximate solutions that are
First year college students (2) Information that grades improve after the first year 0.27 easy to implement.
There are, of course, limita-
College students (3) Information that intelligence is malleable 0.23

Downloaded from www.sciencemag.org on June 28, 2010


tions to the findings from these
African American college students (4) Information that worries about social belonging lessen over time 0.34 studies, as well as many unan-
Better grades. Brief theory-based interventions improved students’ grades [increases shown on a four-point grade point swered questions. It is not clear
average (GPA) scale, relative to randomly assigned control groups]. why students in the Cohen et al.
sample failed to self-affirm on
condition spent 15 min writing about why hampers their subsequent performance, their own. Why did it take an in-class essay
certain values, such as relationships with which further confirms their worst fears, and to focus students’ attention on values that
other people, were important to them. Stu- so on (9, 10). Giving people nonpejorative were important to them? Issues of generaliz-
dents in the control condition wrote about explanations for poor performance (e.g., ability also arise, such as whether the
why specific values were important to other everyone has to “learn the ropes” at college) self-affirmation exercise would work with
people. African American students in the can interrupt this cycle and prevent its spiral- younger age groups. These questions can be
treatment condition achieved better end-of- ing consequences (2, 11). answered only with additional experiments,
semester grades than did African American The success of such interventions— ideally ones as well conducted as the Cohen
students in the control condition. These indeed, of any intervention—is best tested et al. studies.
results are especially encouraging given how with experimental designs in which people
intractable a problem the racial achievement are randomly assigned to treatment and con- References
1. G. L. Cohen, J. Garcia, N. Apfel, A. Master, Science 313,
gap has appeared to be (6). trol conditions. As obvious as this point is in
1307 (2006).
The Cohen et al. study and the others like some settings (e.g., tests of medical treat- 2. T. D. Wilson, P. W. Linville. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 49, 287
it illustrate key social psychological points. It ments), it is not fully appreciated in others (1985).
can be as important to change people’s “con- (e.g., educational settings, in which new pro- 3. J. Aronson, C. B. Fried, C. Good. J. Exper. Soc. Psychol.
38, 113 (2002).
struals”—their interpretations of the social grams are frequently implemented without 4. G. M. Walton, G. L. Cohen J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., in press.
world and their place in it—as it is to change being evaluated experimentally) (12). There 5. C. M. Steele, Amer. Psych. 52, 613 (1997).
the objective environment. In none of the are a number of reasons for this, such as peo- 6. R. Rothstein, Class and Schools (Teachers College Press,
studies, for example, was there an effort to ple’s reliance on common sense to judge the New York, 2004).
7. K. Lewin, Field Theory in Social Science (Harper, New
change the quality of the instruction students effectiveness of interventions in familiar York, 1951).
received, the clarity of the texts they read, or domains (e.g., the classroom). A lesson of the 8. L. Ross, R. E. Nisbett, The Person and the Situation
any other objective feature of the educational studies reviewed here is that common sense is (McGraw Hill, New York, 1991).
environment. Instead, the researchers at- sometimes wrong. Without the experimental 9. S. Valins, R. E. Nisbett, in Attribution: Perceiving the
Causes of Behavior, E. E. Jones et al., Eds. (General
tempted to change students’ construals of results presented by Cohen et al., who would Learning Press, Morristown, NJ, 1972), pp. 137–150.
themselves or how other people viewed them, have thought that a 15-min exercise would 10. M. D. Storms, K. D. McCaul, in New Directions in
with promising results. The objective envi- have had such long-lasting effects? Attribution Research, J. H. Harvey et al., Eds. (Erlbaum,
Hillsdale, NJ, 1976), vol. 1, pp. 143–164.
ronment is obviously important as well; as Social psychological theories, based on
11. T. D. Wilson, M. Damiani, N. Shelton, in Improving
Cohen et al. note, the success of their inter- carefully conducted laboratory experiments, Academic Achievement, J. Aronson, Ed. (Academic Press,
vention depended on students being in a sup- generate (often nonobvious) predictions San Diego, CA, 2002), pp. 89–108.
portive environment. How people perceive about human behavior. Dozens of experi- 12. T. D. Wilson. Psychol. Inq. 16, 185 (2005).
that environment, however, can be key to ments, conducted primarily with college 13. Psychology, C. M. Steele, in Advances in Experimental Social
L. Berkowitz, Ed. (Academic Press, San
bringing about lasting change. students in laboratory settings, have docu- Diego, CA, 1988), pp. 261–302.
PHOTO CREDIT: PHOTOS.COM

Brief theory-based interventions that mented the damaging effects that stereo- 14. C. M. Steele, S. J. Spencer, J. Aronson, in Advances in
focus on people’s construals can reap large types about one’s group can have on individ- Experimental Social Psychology, M. P. Zanna, Ed.
benefits. Kurt Lewin, the founder of modern uals’ performance (known as stereotype 15. (Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 2002), pp. 379–440.
D. K. Sherman, G. L. Cohen, in Advances in Experimental
social psychology, noted that many behav- threat), as well as relatively simple ways of Social Psychology, M. P. Zanna, Ed. (Academic Press, San
ioral patterns are the result of a complex inter- avoiding this damage (13–15). The Cohen et Diego, CA, 2006), pp. 183–242.
play of opposing social forces, resulting in a al. intervention follows directly from this
“tension system” (7, 8). Sometimes these basic research. 10.1126/science.1133017

1252 1 SEPTEMBER 2006 VOL 313 SCIENCE www.sciencemag.org


Published by AAAS

You might also like