You are on page 1of 15

Inuit and Scientific Philosophies about

Planning, Prediction, and Uncertainty


Peter Bates

Abstract This paper explores Inuit philosophies ahout the future, long-term planning, and pre-
diction and investigates the ways that these contrast with Western ideas. By descrihing how
Inuit deal with the inherent uncertainty of the Arctic environment, and how this leads to dis-
tinct attitudes towards managing the future and its potential risks, the argument is made that
these philosophies are a highly effective method of knowing the Arctic environment. However,
the strengths of this approach may at times be ohscured by a current desire to show that Inuit
predict and plan in the same way as do Western scientists. In fact, such attempts can delegiti-
mize Inuit ways of knowing the future, by effectively setting Western science as a benchmark
by which Inuit knowledge is judged, thus reducing the likelihood of effective collaboration be-
tween Inuit and Western researchers in environmental management.

prediction, to the benefit of both Inuit and scien-


Introduction tists (e.g., Stevenson 1996:283).
Efforts to increase recognition and respect for In- In contrast to this view, early anthropolo-
uit ways of knowing have sometimes resulted in gists have written about the inabihty of Inuit to con-
claims that Inuit predict and plan in a similar way template the future. Inuit were reckoned not to
to that deemed appropriate by Western science place themselves within a chain of events, or per-
(e.g., Bielawski 1992; Ferguson et al. 1998; Free- ceive a chronology to their Uves, or perceive causal
man 1992:9; Northern Perspectives 1992:15-16 links between the past, present, and future (Car-
quoted in Vitebsky 1995:199). These claims are penter 1956:39). This charge has indeed been lev-
also made under the guise of sustainable harvest- eled at all hunter-gatherer groups across the world.
ing practices, in which Inuit are argued to be ac- Western scholars have suggested that pre-contact
tively mindful of their future needs in the ways hunter-gatherer societies did not conceive time to be
that they harvest and gain knowledge of animals linear, and that the past and future were seen to cir-
in the present (Freeman 1985:274-277; Nuttall cle around the present rather than define it (Adam
1998:71, 88-89; Riewe and Gamble 1988). Inuit are [1994] 2002:504; Brody 2000:139; Ingold 2000:336).
also increasingly represented as fearful of the fu- In this paper I aim to explore these contrasting
ture impacts of phenomena such as climate change views of Inuit planning and prediction, focusing
(Armstrong 2001; Clover 2005; Ehrlich 2006). All in turn on Inuit and Western scientific approaches
this suggests that Inuit knowledge and ideas may to negotiating the future in the Arctic. In so doing I
be directly applicable to scientifically founded will outline a distinct Inuit philosophy towards the
environmental management regimes based on future. This may not be based around prediction

Peter Bates, Anthropology Department, University of Aberdeen, Edward Wright Building


Aberdeen, Scotland AB24 3QY

ARCTIC ANTHROPOLOGY, Vol. 44, No. 2, pp. 87-100, 2007 ISSN 0066-6939
© 2007 by the Board of Regents of the University of Wisconsin System
88 Arctic Anthropology 44:2

and planning, but is nonetheless founded on a par- hunter-gatherer life (Bhabha 1993 in Childs and
ticularly astute awareness of the future and its rela- Williams 1997:127-129; Bravo 2000:470; Bravo
tionship to the present. I will also demonstrate the and Sörlin 2002:18; Pálsson 2002:277; Stuart
strengths of this approach in comparison to West- 2002:85-86; Wachowich 1998:86). It is also com-
ern scientific preoccupations with environmental monly argued that research aiming to engage with
management based on prediction and planning. the knowledge of indigenous people tends to ac-
My argument is based on observations made cumulate decontextualized, scientific facts rather
during 15 months of ethnographic fieldwork with than engaging with the philosophies that underpin
ecological scientists and Inuit elders and hunters in indigenous ways of knowing (Agrawal 1995; Bates
and around Cambridge Bay, an Inuit community of 2006; Cruikshank 1998:45-70; Kothari 2001:149;
around 1,300 individuals on the south coast of Vic- Morrow and Hensel 1992; Mosse 2001:32; Nadasdy
toria Island in Nunavut, Canada. Most Inuit in Cam- 2003:60-61, 94-98; Wenzel 2004:239). Accord-
bridge Bay are eastern Copper Inuit, the original in- ingly, Adam ([1994] 2002) warns that most stud-
habitants of the surrounding area. Sub-groups from ies of hunter-gatherers are fraught with Western as-
the south coast of Victoria Island, adjacent main- sumptions about time, which influence the ways
land coast and Bathurst Inlet came together to form hunter-gatherer philosophies are perceived and
the community, predominantly during the 1960s described by Western scholars (Adam 1994:506).
(Collignon 1993:71-75; Damas 2002:40-49, 72-73, These Western assumptions, and the impacts they
112-122,160-163; Freeman 1976:46-48). The gla- may have on representations of hunter-gatherers,
ciated landscape beyond the town consists of flat are therefore important to explore.
open tundra, broken occasionally by low ridges
and eskers. A multitude of lakes, ponds, streams, Assumptions about time as a linear flow
rivers, and swamps punctuate the tundra in sum- of constant rate, with neat chronologies linking
mer. The mainland south of the island is higher, events in the past, present, and future are con-
and rocky hills and cliffs surround Bathurst In- victions that are deeply embedded in Western
let. The ocean around Cambridge Bay consists of thought (Adam [1994] 2002:506). This percep-
narrow straits that separate Victoria Island from tion of linear causality has recently been further
the Canadian mainland. These waters have lit- reinforced by modern technologies. The hazards
tle tidal range and are bordered by exposed rocky that these technologies generate, such as nuclear
coast, sheltered bays, stretches of sandy beach, waste, biodiversity loss, or global warming, oper-
and small islands. The straits are frozen for much ate on timescales previously unimaginable, hav-
of the year, allowing access by skidoo to the main- ing potential repercussions over hundreds or thou-
land, and during this period there is no open wa- sands of years. This has created a preoccupation
ter within range of the community. Animal spe- with a long-term, open future; a future that is full
cies hunted by Inuit from Cambridge Bay include of potential hazards that must be understood and
muskox, caribou, Arctic fox, Arctic char, ringed mitigated to ensure our survival. Industrial so-
and bearded seal, ptarmigan, snow goose, Canada ciety's greatest tool in this endeavor is the long-
goose, and swan. All of these species are commonly term planning apparently afforded by the predic-
found around the community, although there is tive powers of Western science (Adam 1998:57;
seasonal variation in their numbers and accessi- Beck [1986] 1992; Harries-Jones 2004:295; Ingold
bility. Caribou perhaps receive the most attention 1993:39). Similarly, the storage of provisions such
from Inuit hunters, particularly the Dolphin-Union as food or money for periods of possible future
herd, which migrates between the interior of Victo- scarcity has come to be seen as an indication of a
ria Island and the Canadian mainland, passing by rational mind. There are also assumptions about
Cambridge Bay twice a year. Wolf, wolverine, and how time should be used in industrial society.
grizzly bear can also be found on Victoria Island, al- Regulating time by "the clock" and engaging in
though their numbers are far greater on the main- regular "work" and "free" time increasingly came
land across the straits. Polar bears are rare visitors to be seen as both practically and morally correct
and large sea mammals such as whales and walrus, during the industrial revolution (Thompson 1967:
which are abundant in other regions of the Arctic, 56-96).
are absent from the waters around Cambridge Bay. Against these assumptions, the apparent lack
of urgency in their activities and the unwillingness
to prepare for the future displayed by many hunter-
gatherer groups have been judged by observers as
Western Perception of indolence or a lack of foresight (Ingold 2000:336;
Time and Inuit Sahlins 1972:30-32; Stern 2003:148). Similarly,
recognition by Western observers that Inuit do not
Western representations of hunter-gatherer societ- appear to concern themselves with long-term plan-
ies have always been more influenced by the pre- ning and prediction may become an assumption
occupations of researchers than the realities of that Inuit are incapable of conceptualizing the fu-
Bates: Inuit and Scientific Philosophies 89

ture and its inherent dangers. Meanwhile, for those The Arctic environment is extremely change-
researchers who aim to increase recognition and re- able and one of the first things one learns is that
spect for Inuit ways of knowing, Western assump- nothing can be taken for granted. While animals,
tions about the future, its risks, and how these the weather, and snow and ice cover follow yearly
should he managed may lead to assertions that In- cycles, variations in these patterns will always oc-
uit do plan and predict in the same ways as West- cur. Ice cover or open water can remain for months
ern scientists (e.g., Bielawski 1992; Ferguson et longer in one year than in the next. This makes
al. 1998; Freeman 1992:9; Northern Perspectives crossing the straits that separate Cambridge Bay
1992:15-16 quoted in Vitehsky 1995:199). This may from the Canadian mainland hazardous, as' drift-
however again speak more of Western assumptions ing ice can disrupt and damage boats even in mid-
than the preoccupations of the Inuit themselves. summer, while on years of late freeze-up patches
To suggest that Inuit frequently engage in long- of open water or thin ice can swallow skidoos well
term planning and prediction may therefore he to into December. Also, animals such as carihou and
set Western knowledge claims as the henchmark hy Arctic char, which are arguably the two species
which other knowledge forms should he judged, most often hunted by Inuit from Cambridge Bay,
rather then engaging fully with Inuit philosophies. move with a striking alacrity during their migra-
tions. An Inuit hunter may know roughly what to
expect from these animals for a given time of year,
Do Inuit Plan and Predict? for example that the caribou should he nearing the
On the one hand, therefore, it is claimed that In- community on their trek north, but the exact tim-
uit have no concept of the future and its links to ings and routes chosen by the caribou across such
the present. This claim can he dismissed immedi- a vast expanse of land and ice can never be fully
ately. It seems highly unlikely that Inuit in the pre- predicted. Caribou migrations can also fail en-
contact era could not conceptualize the future, for tirely with little warning (Burch 1994:166; Fosset
the future is knowahle on a hroad scale in the Arc- 2001:154). Meanwhile, wolves, bears, and wolver-
tic environment. The tundra and seas change dras- ines are highly dispersed in the area around Cam-
tically from one season to the next, from snow and bridge Bay and their locations are almost com-
ice to rock and water. Animal populations also pletely unpredictable. Furthermore, the weather
fluctuate from ahundaiice to almost complete ah- in particular can never he relied upon at any time
sence on a roughly regular cycle. Negotiating these of year. It can change completely in a matter of
changes would always have required an astute hours, catching the unwary and unprepared in
awareness of what was coming next, coupled with thick fog, torrential rain, gathering waves, blow-
appropriate preparation for these events, for ex- ing snow, or plummeting temperatures. In the days
ample the sewing of new winter clothes prior to a when Inuit were entirely reliant on the hunt for
move onto the sea-ice (Damas 2002:13-14) or stor- food, to presume that something was going to hap-
age of ample food in readiness for times of scar- pen could therefore prove fatal (Briggs 1968:53 in
city that might he of unknown duration (Fosset Omura 2002:106; Briggs 1991:262).
2001:97). Moreover, the predictable nature of fixed It might seem that present-day Inuit are af-
astronomical occurrences, such as the winter sol- fected to a lesser extent by these potential hazards.
stice or the return of the sun in spring, allowed The Inuit are no longer wholly reliant on success-
some Inuit groups to develop complex calendars fully acquiring game, greatly reducing the risk of
which allowed the accurate forecasting of these starvation and the need to hunt and travel in poor
events (Stern 2003:150). It is therefore improbable conditions. Modern technologies such as skidoos,
that pre-contact Inuit did not perceive any link be- GPS, and radios might also seem to have made the
tween the past, present, and future, and a certain Arctic safer. However, the Arctic remains unpre-
amount of general preparation would have been dictable and dangerous (Briggs 1991:260). Hunting
essential to negotiating Arctic seasonal variations and traveling remain fundamental aspects of In-
(Tyrrell 2005:55-60). uit culture (Hicks and White 2000:38) and are still
Having established that Inuit can conceptual- subject to the vagaries of the environment, which
ize the future in this way, it might be assumed that occasionally prove fatal to those who make mis-
they do therefore readily engage in prediction and takes. Reliance on modern technologies can also
planning. However in my experience Inuit gener- introduce new^, unpredictable dangers, such as me-
ally prefer not to predict and plan in a way that chanical failure (Aporta and Higgs 2005:735-736).
conforms to Western assumptions about the fu- For Inuit, it is acceptance of this continuing
ture. I will now explore the reasons that Inuit may uncertainty and danger that marks the truly expe-
have for not wanting to predict and plan, demon- rienced, a point also noted about the Sami by In-
strating that preferring not to engage in these activ- gold and Kurttila (2000:189). Accepting that the
ities is based on a particularly astute awareness of future cannot he known allows appropriate prep-
the future. aration for uncertainty, rather than condemning
90 Arctic Anthropology 44:2

a traveler to futile struggle against it. Inuit there- pable. Hunters will set out over the sea-ice within
fore focus on "keeping the show on the road," that the next few days, often with particular loca-
is, opening up a path to the future in the present, tions already in mind, determined from what oth-
rather than becoming overly fixated on the future ers have said, combined with personal knowledge
before it has arrived. They respond to each situa- of animal behavior. Hunters know well where the
tion as and when it presents itself, and reaction to caribou are likely to go next once in a certain area,
an opportunity or obstacle is often spontaneous. and how best to reach locations specified by other
Having adequate knowledge of the present hunters. Similarly with the Arctic char run, as the
therefore becomes far more important than pre- ice is breaking up in the spring and the fish begin
dicting what might happen next. A hunter re- to move across the lakes, through the rivers and to-
lies on acquiring a wealth of information on cur- wards the ocean, daily fishing reports will filter
rent conditions and opportunities. This is done around town, determining where people focus
in part by maintaining frequent personal contact their attentions on the next day. To be even a lit-
with the land, and Inuit will continually note cur- tle behind of what is going on is to stand for hours
rent snow and ice conditions and animal locations on the rapidly crumbling lake ice, without a single
as they travel. This personal experience is com- bite, wondering at the amount of fish someone re-
plemented by constant communication with other ported catching here just days ago.
hunters. On first arriving in Camhridge Bay I was Such information does not however provide
struck by the frequency with which animal move- fixed instructions governing how a hunt will pro-
ments or environmental conditions came up in ceed. It instead provides vague guidelines that
conversation among the Inuit. Hunters would re- may be reassessed or abandoned entirely at any
late the numbers of animals that they had seen point during the process, and Inuit with backup
whilst out on the land and their location, each and plans are the most likely to be successful (Berkes
every time they crossed paths with another hunter, and Jolly 2001). The vitality of this knowledge
and anything out of the ordinary also received par- therefore lies in the freedom it allows for improvi-
ticular attention. In this way, as a hunter moved sation and flexibility, and it should be thought of
through the town and the land, meeting other as providing general "rules of thumb" rather than
hunters, he or she would quickly build up a de- a fixed plan (Suchman 1987:52).
tailed impression of where the animals had been
within the last couple of days. It was noticeable This Inuit preference for rapid response
that it was not the past that was discussed, but and flexibility is complemented by skills of im-
contemporary events. While Inuit would occasion- provisation and adaptability. I will focus on
ally reminisce about past hunting trips, informa- these skills here in order to further explain the
tion pertaining to current hunting conditions oc- strength and utility behind this approach to un-
cupied the bulk of their attention. certainty. New technologies have increasingly be-
come a part of Inuit life and it might be expected
The citizens' band (CB) radio also provides that they would, to an extent, have reduced the
a source of information on present hunting con- need for these rapid response systems, conse-
ditions. These machines are set up as an integral quently diminishing Inuit skills in improvisation
part of pitching a camp or moving into a cabin, no (Aporta and Higgs 2005:740-742). However, as Si-
matter how temporary. The cumbersome recep- gaut (1993:109-110) notes, although the fear that
tor wires will be stretched out and pitched up us- mechanization will lead to the deskilling of hu-
ing anything that comes to hand, be it a rock, oar, manity has long accompanied the rise of technol-
or a skidoo. The radio is then tuned to a partic- ogy, this fear has proved groundless, as skills have
ular frequency used by the community. It is left survived, thrived, and evolved in the new era of
running almost constantly and its disjointed con- machines. This is certainly true with Inuit and
versations, hissing static, and eerily melodic in- the tendency for adaptation and improvisation
terference are an ever-present background noise to has evolved alongside changes to Inuit lifestyles.
life on the land. It is used either to communicate This yields a distinct approach to new technol-
with other hunters, or simply to listen in on other ogy, an approach of which Inuit are proud (Briggs
conversations that are taking place. The informa- 1991:263; Omura 2002:107-108).
tion passed is a nearly constant stream of updates This quick and creative improvisation, and
of who is out on the land, what location they are the confidence to rely on the products of their
at, how the weather is and what animals they have work, was demonstrated to me when a hunter's fi-
seen or caught. berglass boat had a chunk torn out of the hull, ei-
Inuit often act on this new information im- ther from drifting ice or from him dragging it along
mediately, since to hesitate can mean missing an rocks. In preparation for an upcoming eight-hour
opportunity. In the spring, when the first hunt- boat journey, traversing the open waters of the
ers begin to report that the caribou have reached straits that separate Victoria Island from the main-
the mainland coast, the excitement in town is pal- land, he repaired the boat using a collection of
Bates: Inuit and Scientific Philosophies 91

items mostly found at the dump: a sheet of scrap other hand, to presume that something will hap-
metal, rubber from an old inner tube, rivets, and pen can be dangerous. As a consequence, many In-
some glue that might not have been the right kind, uit today remain unwilling to plan ahead or be-
he was not siu:e. come locked into a course of action. Indeed, Inuit
In relation to skidoos, some Inuit will set out philosophies may dictate that prediction, plan-
alone with no tent, radio, or back-up of any kind, ning, and forecasting are impractical, foolhardy,
on a disheveled skidoo that has broken down fre- and rather arrogant (Briggs 1968:53 in Omura
quently over the last three months, but is today 2002:106; Briggs 1991:262).
held together by some old wire, or a splint made of
duct-tape and a chisel. I, on the other hand, fussed Planning in the Community
over my machine and fretted over every clank or
groan it made, ensuring as best I could that it was While Inuit philosophies about time reveal them-
at its best for every trip out. The results of the two selves during hunting and traveling trips on the
approaches to skidoo maintenance soon revealed land, most Inuit now spend much of their time
which was superior. Inuit skidoos broke down fre- within permanent settlements. Settlement life ad-
• quently, while mine only had problems on a few heres to certain timetables and rules, which to an
occasions. However, Inuit skidoos generally got extent structure the lives of contemporary Inuit
moving again somehow and the owner was able (Goehring and Stager 1991; Stern 2003:152-154). It
to make his way home. When my skidoo broke, is necessary therefore to briefly consider the ways
I was rather helpless. The Arctic environment is that Inuit philosophies about planning and pre-
tough on these machines, as they are designed for diction, so well suited to life on the Arctic tundra,
temperatures approximately 20-30° C higher than have been affected by the move to a largely seden-
what can be standard for the Cambridge Bay win- tary lifestyle.
ter. Consequently, even the newest, most cared for Necessities of settlement life often dictate
skidoo could break down at a moment's notice, in that Inuit commit themselves to rigid plans long
varied and unpredictable ways. It became appar- in advance of their commencement, in particular
ent that it is best to go out on the land armed with around work contracts. Cambridge Bay is the ad-
first-rate knowledge and a dubious machine, rather ministrative center for the Kitikmeot region of Nu-
than dubious knowledge and a first-rate machine. navut. Many Inuit in the town have found employ-
Planning ahead was all very well, but being able to ment in the government offices, as well as in the
deal with the unexpected was the only sure way to schools, health center, bank, garages, stores, a large
get home safely. meat plant, and other facilities. Many Inuit work-
Good hunters therefore rely on being in con- ers are also involved in the basic infrastructure
stant contact with both those around them and the that a settlement of Cambridge Bay's size requires,
land itself to gain an impression of opportunities such as water delivery and sewage removal, or
and obstacles presented at any given point in time. huilding work on the town's ever expanding in-
This is complimented by their own knowledge of frastructure. Trips to the south of Canada by plane
the land and animals and their skills at improvi- are another common feature of settlement life, and
sation gained and honed through personal experi- also require long term planning. Such plans might
ence, to generate a wealth of flexibly utilized op- not come to ftuition for many months, and of-
portunities at any given point in time. Having ten require extensive preparation in the interim.
knowledge of the present is therefore an essential That the strict timetables brought to the Arctic
component of Inuit hunting practices, as is main- by colonialism can disrupt Inuit hunting prac-
taining the flexibility which allows response to tices was illustrated by Stefansson (1922:89-97),
opportunities and obstacles as they arise. Mean- who recounts with some alarm the ramifications
while, the ability to work with the unpredictable of the prohibition against working on a Sunday
Arctic environment, rather than fighting against it, that was taken up by Inuit following their con-
and acceptance of current conditions rather than version to Christianity. He writes of crucial whale
trying to second guess what is coming next reveal hunting opportunities missed and individuals left
themselves as characteristics of contemporary In- stranded on the land without a rescue mission be-
uit culture. The ingenuity and spontaneity of Inuit ing launched. Today, constraints of employment
hunters are revealed whether Inuit are fixing a bro- may cast even greater limits on the ability of some
ken skidoo, repairing an aluminum boat, building Inuit hunters to respond to hunting opportunities
a shelter, reacting to a change in the weather, or as they arise (Condon et al. 1995).
taking advantage of a shifting animal population. Goehring and Stager (1991) have conse-
Their skills also prove to be admirable, enviable quently suggested that Inuit had to develop new
and rarely misplaced, amounting to a highly effec- concepts of time in order to take part in industrial
tive method of approaching the uncertainty inher- society. They write that the immediacy of action
ent in traveling in the Arctic environment. On the and result that occurred in the previous hunting
92 Arctic Anthropology 44:2

and gathering regime would have had to be sup- on receipt, and does not seem to have any value
planted by a more abstract concept of time in or- in itself. On receiving a lump sum, many Inuit
der to recognize the delayed returns of settlement will spend all they need on food, give some away
society. However, I have already argued that dur- to family and friends, and gamble the rest in card
ing the pre-contact era Inuit would plan and pre- games. Shortly afterwards they may begin to re-
pare for the future when they deemed it appropri- quire money again and will search round the com-
ate. I would therefore suggest that the increased munity for a loan. This affects the way that many
need for planning enforced by settlement life is Inuit work, as once enough money has been se-
unlikely to have caused a dramatic change in cured for the immediate future they see little rea-
thought process. Inuit philosophies of time have son to continue earning. The Euro-Canadians in
therefore been rather more resistant than might Cambridge Bay, who may employ Inuit workers
be expected, despite undeniable disruptions to in their businesses, can find this highly frustrat-
hunting practices caused by settlement life. Con- ing. Meanwhile, Inuit can find white people to be
sequently, many Inuit continue to use long-term greedy and mean with their money, storing it away
planning and prediction selectively and sparingly and perhaps never even spending it.
where possible.
Such apparently carefree habits may give the
Certainly in terms of money and employ- initial impression that Inuit are giving no thought
ment, a focus on preparation for uncertainty rather to the future, a point occasionally made ruefully
than rigid planning for the future often seems to by their employers. However, these habits indicate
be in evidence within the settlement. It must be a continuation of Inuit philosophies about the fu-
noted that many Inuit have held down the same ture, in which uncertainty is embraced and pre-
steady job for many years. For others, however, pared for accordingly. Through the widespread
employment is more flexibly utilized. Opportu- sharing and circulation of food and money, Inuit
nities for full time employment are in somewhat are investing in family and friends for the future,
short supply in Cambridge Bay, forcing many In- as the person who borrows today may be relied on
uit to be flexible in their approach to working. to lend tomorrow (Bird-David 1992:31; Woodburn
Furthermore, I found that many acquaintances 1982:440^42), although sharing relationships are
who had gained stable full time positions would not necessarily directly reciprocal. Rather, wide-
feel constrained by the routine and would con- spread sharing cements social relationships and
sequently leave, especially in the spring, when ensures that Inuit have a throng of people who
the 24-hour sun, the warming air, and the linger- will aid them in the future (Bodenhorn 2000;
ing sea ice are ideal for traveling. Regardless of op- Hovelsrud-Broda 2000; Wenzel 1995a; 2000). Per-
portunity, therefore, a sizeable proportion of Inuit sonal hoarding of wealth is rather the antithesis of
do not engage in full time emplojnnent, and will this philosophy towards the future, as there is no
instead support themselves throughout the year guarantee that money or food saved up and stored
through a range of occasional employment oppor- away will ever serve any useful purpose, or that it
tunities. These include guiding for sports hunts, will be enough in the event of disaster.
commercial fishing, working out of town at one of Inuit resistance to scheduled work is not the
the nearby mines, and conducting military exer- only way that Western concepts of time come un-
cises with the Canadian Rangers. Sporadic income der siege in Arctic settlements. Blizzards sweep
can also be generated from the manufacture and in off the tundra, disrupting community sched-
sale of soapstone carvings, wall hangings, parkas, ules for days, as offices and schools are closed
mitts, and boots. High levels of unemployment and planes are grounded or prevented from land-
and income assistance in the settlement, com- ing. At any time of the year fog or low cloud sim-
bined with the fact that many families also sup- ilarly disrupt airplane timetables, and many trips
port themselves through various subsistence activ- to the airport with bags packed for a journey end
ities on the land that reduce family expenditure on only in disappointment. Illnesses similarly grind
store-bought food items, also decrease the need for the community to a halt, as flu epidemics close of-
commitment to full time work for many Inuit. The fices and schools. The 24-hour darkness of mid-
"replacement value" of land food in Nunavut has winter and the 24-hour daylight of midsummer
been estimated at between $30 and $35 million a also ill suit those trying to participate in regular
year and its continuing importance, both economi- work patterns (Stern 2003). Another threat to com-
cally and culturally, should not be underestimated munity schedules emerged while I was living in
(Hicks and White 2000:38). the Cambridge Bay, as the pipes that deliver water
The money gained from employment may into town froze and burst, delaying and disrupting
also be flexibly utilized. Again, some Inuit are fru- water services for the entire winter. It may appear
gal with their money and may save carefully. For that Western concepts of time are absolutely im-
others, however, despite the presence of a bank in posed onto Inuit settlements by office hours, fixed
the community, cash is often spent immediately weekends, opening times of stores and banks.
Bates: Inuit and Scientific Philosophies 93

curfews for children, and bans on driving all ter- reluctance from Inuit to make predictions about
rain vehicles (ATVs) at night, even during periods the Dolphin-Union caribou herd, or to be drawn
of 24-hour sun. However, the community rarely into discussion of the management of the caribou
runs so smoothly. Inuit philosophies of time often to ensure the future prosperity of the animals. In-
therefore prove to he well placed even within the uit would frequently decline to answer these ques-
context of settlement society, and are consequently tions, explaining that they did not know or that
reinforced. they could not say. Even where an answer was
provided, it was often offered with clear reluc-
tance, and the interviewee would reiterate many
Inuit Philosophies and times that they were uncertain. Overall, it became
Environmental Management apparent that questions about the future of the ani-
mals made many Inuit rather uncomfortable.
The ways that Inuit manage uncertainty while As already discussed, claiming knowledge
hunting and traveling might he thought to hear lit- of the future is perceived as rather futile and
tle relevance to the input they might wish to have even arrogant hy many Inuit, and it is little won-
in research projects revolving around environ- der therefore that they might he unwilling to have
mental management. However, my experiences of such comments documented within an interview.
hunting and traveling with Inuit became crucial to Such Inuit philosophies may however be partic-
my understanding of the ways that Inuit dealt with ularly relevant to discussion about animals. Mor-
certain lines of questioning within research inter- row (1990:150), in her work with Yupiit in Alaska,
views that I undertook towards the end of my time discusses a "social commitment" to the assump-
in Cambridge Bay. I argue therefore that Inuit phi- tion that one can never fully understand or know
losophies about the future, although founded in the motivations of anyone else, nor predict what
activities taking place around hunting and trav- they might do. Inuit also abhor efforts to control
eling, can become highly significant when Inuit others (Briggs 1968:53 in Omura 2002:106; Briggs
are called upon to provide knowledge that will 1970:42; Briggs 1991:267). These social commit-
aid in environmental management. In these situa- ments would be extended to include animals, as
tions the Inuit distaste for efforts to plan and pre- relationships between Inuit and animals rest on
dict may clash with the preoccupations of Western the same principals as social interactions (Wen-
science, which is increasingly focused on manage- zel 1991:140; also see Fienup-Riordan 1990:167;
ment founded on prediction (Adam 1998:57). Two 1999:58 for similar observations about Yup'ik in
examples may illustrate this point, focusing on Alaska). In the past, these ideas would have dis-
discussions around caribou management and cli- couraged Inuit from attempting to manage future
mate change respectively. These are taken from animal populations. Accordingly, there is little ev-
12 semi-structured interviews that I carried out idence that Inuit or Eskimo groups managed ani-
with Inuit elders and hunters towards the end mal populations via sustainable harvesting strate-
of my time in the community of Cambridge Bay. gies (Burch 1994; Fienup-Riordan 1990:167-189;
These interviews had the aim of documenting In- Cunn et al 1988:26), despite claims to the con-
uit knowledge about the Dolphin-Union caribou trary (Freeman 1985:274-277; Nuttall 1998:71,
herd, which migrates past Cambridge Bay twice a 88-89; Riewe and Cambie 1988). Instead, it was
year. A translator was used in all but two of these maintaining respectful relationships with animals
interviews, as interview participants were more that would facilitate future accessibility to game
comfortable speaking Inuinnaqtun than English. (Fienup-Riordan 1990:167-189; Wenzel 1991:4-5,
During these interviews, questions about the 138-140). Efforts within research situations to en-
present and past were answered with comparative courage Inuit to speculate on management solu-
willingness. Past journeys on the land, life histo- tions for sustainable caribou futures may therefore
ries, and old hunting techniques were clearly a go against Inuit philosophies of how one can know
pleasure to discuss for most people and the con- both the animals and the future, and may amount
versation would here flow relatively easily. Elders to being disrespectful to both. As such, they may
told of their childhood memories, how and where be particularly troubling to Inuit interviewees.
they had hunted in their youth, and how they I also noted a particular reluctance to dis-
had ended up coming to Cambridge Bay. Mean- cuss the future impacts of potentially hazardous
while, younger hunters related what they had seen phenomena such as climate change in negative
while out traveling. Respondents also appeared to terms. During my interviews, Inuit elders were
be happy enough to point out caribou migration keen to explain that climate change was happen-
routes and suggest timings for their movements, ing. I was told of previously permanent ice banks
to recount where they had seen calves, rutting or that had started disappearing in the summer time
wolf kills, or to recall the history of caribou pop- during the last decade, of lakes that used to keep
ulation fluctuations. However, there was marked their ice all year but now melt entirely, and of the
94 Arctic Anthropology 44:2

mosquitoes around Cambridge Bay getting more Sach's Harbour are successfully negotiating cli-
numerous. It was only when I asked about the fu- mate change, through flexibly adjusting hunting
ture that there would be a pause. Mostly respon- techniques, locations, timings and species sought.
dents said they did not know, or they suggested Flexibility is almost a defining feature of Inuit so-
that it would end up being fine, and that the cari- ciety (Adams 1971:9), and over the centuries the
bou and the Inuit could cope. Wenzel (1995:175) Inuit have proved themselves to be adept at em-
notes a similarly optimistic view of climate change bracing change, be it migration to a new geographi-
expressed by Inuit in Clyde village on Baffin cal area, environmental fluctuations, crashes in an-
Island. imal populations or colonialism (Dahl 2000:7-10;
The philosophies about the future already Damas 2002:6,23-24; Dorais 1997:88-101; Fossett
described may be compounded in this case by an 2001:67,113; Nuttall 1992:29; Turner 1994:
Inuit belief that thoughts and words have an in- 150-156; Wenzel 1995:172-174). It is unlikely
herent agency, as Morrow describes for Alaskan therefore that climate change will be their undo-
Yupiit (1990:150; 1996:417). This can be distin- ing, and they are right to be confident in their ap-
guished from the Western view that only actions proach to future uncertainty.
deliver intended effects. Thus careless words, and
in particular definitive statements that fail to rec-
ognize the inherent uncertainty of the world, are Planning and "The West"
dangerous. Speaking of the future in negative The examination of Inuit philosophies about pre-
terms may thus actualize that scenario, creating diction and uncertainty in this paper has illumi-
the bleak situation described. Briggs (1991:285) nated a distinctive attitude to knowing the en-
and G'Neil et al. (1997:35-36) also report an In- vironment that contrasts somewhat with the
uit belief that worrying, brooding, or being pessi- preoccupations of Western science. Inuit appear
mistic about the future shortens one's life. Indeed, to accept uncertainty, and find great utility in an
O'Neil et al. (1997:36) suggest that Inuit in Nuna- in-depth knowledge of the present, coupled with
vik perceive the immediate worry caused by sci- skills of improvisation that allow flexibility to re-
entific discourses about risks to be more damaging spond to situations as they arise. They may there-
than the potential future dangers that such dis- fore find attempts to predict the future unnec-
courses aim to mitigate. essary or foolhardy. It might seem therefore that
These Inuit philosophies described above, Inuit and Western ideas about the future are rather
as they emerge around climate change, are a con- at odds with one another. It is however possible
trast to the frequently reported Inuit fear and de- to navigate this apparent impasse, if we focus on
spair over such phenomena (Armstrong 2001; Clo- the ways that uncertainty and planning play out
ver 2005; Ehrlich 2006). This suggests that Inuit in practice during scientific research and manage-
philosophies about the future and the management ment. It is towards these processes that I now turn.
of its potential risks may frequently be overlooked. During my fieldwork in Cambridge Bay,
Industrial society may be committed to deal- the problems that can occur in the Arctic due
ing with potential future hazards such as climate to commitment to a "plan," and the compara-
change through rigorous environmentally manip- tive strengths of the Inuit methodology of deal-
ulative management strategies based on prediction ing with the future, were perhaps most forcefully
(for example the setting of carbon emission quotas illustrated to me while I worked as both partici-
worldwide or the creation of fiood defenses based pant and observer on an ecological science proj-
on estimates of likely sea level rise). This may lead ect (Bates 2006). This project was based at the Uni-
to assumptions that if Inuit recognize global warm- versity of Aberdeen and the Centre of Ecology and
ing, they must also desire predictive management Hydrology in Banchory, Scotland. Its aim was the
strategies. Such assumptions are often fundamen- investigation of aspects of the behavior of the Dol-
tal to the agendas of research projects with indige- phin-Union caribou herd. I accompanied the main
nous people, and come to structure and dictate the investigator and her assistant to their field site
information generated (Agrawal 1995; Bates 2006; west of Cambridge Bay on numerous occasions
Cruikshank 1998:45-70; Kothari 2001:149; Mor- and helped with vegetation sampling and collec-
row and Hensel 1992; Mosse 2001:32; Nadasdy tion of muskox feces. I also accompanied the ecol-
2003:60-61, 94-98; Wenzel 2004:239). ogists on trips on the land with Inuit hunters.
However, Inuit may be more concerned During these expeditions we collected jaws and
with maintaining the flexibility of their knowl- sections of stomach from caribou in order to de-
edge through continuous interaction with the land termine the ages and parasite loads of the animals
and animals, so that they are able to respond ef- taken by hunters.
fectively to whatever the future may bring. True In dealing with the unpredictability inherent
to this, Berkes and Jolly (2001) and Jolly et al. in ecological fieldwork on the Arctic tundra, the
(2002:112-115) describe ways in which Inuit in study's best-laid plans rapidly proved to be more
Bates: Inuit and Scientific Philosophies 95

of a hindrance than an advantage. The snows left process of ad hoc response and improvization, as
late in 2003, meaning that there wras no access to circumstances can never fully be anticipated and
the tundra vegetation, which was crucial to the many obstacles and opportunities will only reveal
ecological project's commencement. This set the themselves once a practitioner is fully engaged in
project schedule hack hy almost a month. Flights an activity. Plans are thus only really useful as ori-
out of the community had already heen predeter- entation prior to launching into an activity, so that
mined, and the Arctic summer can he frustratingly the best starting point for forthcoming improvisa-
short. This limited the period availahle for carry- tion can be obtained.
ing out the detailed methodology required, forcing Suchman (1987) also notes that in indus-
a restructuring of the timetahle of work. This was trial society fixed plans are most often invoked af-
not the only divergence from the initial plan. De- ter an event, to lend an aura of rationality, cred-
spite weeks of work having been devoted to it, a ibility, and tidiness to what actually took place.
whole section of the project investigating the dis- True to this, scientific fieldwork is generally rep-
persal potential of parasites in musk oxen feces resented as having followed a simple, algorithmic
was dropped entirely when no larval stages of the series of orderly, impersonal, and pre-determined
parasites were found. Furthermore, the carihou procedures. This conceals much of the messiness
themselves proved to he absent from areas that of such studies, lending the impression of meth-
were crucial for the study's predetermined meth- odologies so meticulously thought out in advance
odology, creating a brief panic and forcing a quick that their implementation requires minimal per-
realignment of the project's goals. sonal involvement and skill. Such representations
Commitment to a plan also proved diffi- often receive more attention from the academic
cult in other ways, as it dictated that activities west than the processes behind them (Turnhull
were carried out on days that were rather unsuit- 2000:91). The very idea of pre-determined plans
able. On one memorable occasion the exposed that can be flawlessly followed through may there-
hillsides on which we worked were blasted and fore be something of a myth, developed through
drenched by heavy winds and sporadic down- Western society's preoccupation with rationality,
pours. This weather rendered the study's meth- order, and certainty (Golinski 1998:2-6; Medawar
odology, which at this point involved the delicate 1969:8; Milton 2001:21, 129-132, 150; Polanyi
clipping and storage of the tundra vegetation, al- 1978:168-170; Turnbull 2000:82, 210).
most impossible. From the low hills on which we It is not only in terms of scientific fieldwork
worked we could watch these storms approaching practice that avenues for collaboration can be
out over the ocean, as menacing slate-grey cloud- found between Western and Inuit ways of know-
banks trailed by tendrils of torrential rain, yet we ing. The ways that scientific predictive models
hunkered down and worked regardless. While the function in practice has also led to questions over
assistant and I quickly grew fed up and started to some of the assumptions that underlie Western
moan at our predicament, the project leader was science. Science's history of environmental man-
admirably determined to get the work done, pres- agement hased on prediction is notable mostly
sured ever by the predetermined schedule of work. for its inaccuracy, as large time spans and multi-
Faced with the conflicting pressures of a cha- ple pathways are often unfathomable to scientific
otic Arctic environment and the desire for an orga- disciplines, which tend to focus on linear cause
nized scientific project, the ecologists showed con- and effect (Adam 1998:50; Anderson 1999; Feit
siderable skill in shifting between two approaches, 1988:83; Freeman 1985:267-277, 1992:9-10; Lud-
that of highly structured planning and that of wig et al. 1993; Riewe and Gamble 1988:31; Roep-
rapid reaction and adaptation. During this pro- storff 2001:85). Some branches of science, such as
cess it became increasingly clear which was the chaos theory, have made efforts to understand and
more appropriate mode of operation on the tundra. account for uncertainty. However, their influence
Overall, the ecologists' work emerged not as the has so far remained limited (Adam 1998:44-45).
orderly implementation of a pre-determined meth- Generally speaking, by assuming that it will even-
odology, but as a process of rapid response that tually be able to eliminate uncertainty from its
bore a marked similarity to that preferred by In- predictions, much of science presently limits its
uit. "The plan" seemed to trail along in the wake capacity for effective environmental management.
of this, shifting in response to actions taken rather Moreover, scientific knowledge about com-
than dictating them. Having to react in this way plex environmental phenomena is itself gener-
would seem to he an unavoidable consequence of ally uncertain and contradictory. Indeed, this con-
working in such an unpredictable environment. flict and uncertainty is essential to the dynamism
However, while the Arctic may provide an ex- and progression of science (Kuhn 1962:11-12; Me-
treme example, Suchman (1987:viii-ix, 52) sug- dawar 1969:49; Popper 1979:36-37). However, this
gests that no matter how planned any action is, process must be somewhat obscured from view
once underway it will always hecome a situated in order to maintain the public or policy maker's
96 Arctic Anthropology 44:2

confidence in tbe current authority of scientific References Cited


knowledge (Beck [1986] 1992:164). This drive to-
wards certainty and suppression of dissent may Adam, Barbara
cause crucial amhiguities to he overlooked, lead- 2002 Perceptions of Time. In The Companion Ency-
ing to policy and action for environmental man- [1994] clopaedia of Anthropology. Tim Ingold, ed. Pp.
agement heing hased on incomplete data (Walters 503-526. London and New York: Routledge.
1997). 1998 Timescapes of Modernity: The Environment and
Recognition of these limitations to scientific Invisible Hazards. London: Routledge.
knowing has led to calls for science in general to
confront, accept, and attempt to understand uncer- Agrawal, Arun
tainty (Adam 1998:59; Dove 1996:581; Ludwig et 1995 Dismantling the Divide between Indigenous
al. 1993). Adaptive management strategies, which and Scientific Knowledge. Development and
are constantly reviewed and updated, rather than Change. 26:413-429.
based on rigid strategies and long term predic- Anderson, David
tions, are increasingly seen as a way around this 1999 Rangifer and Human Interests. Rangifer 20
current impasse in relation to glohal environmen- (2-3):153-174.
tal change (Peterson et al. 1997). This may finally
Aporta, Claudio and Eric Higgs
allow appropriate action towards sustainable fu-
2005 Satellite Culture: Clobal Positioning
tures. In the case of research into Inuit knowledge Systems, Inuit Wayfinding, and the Need
of the environment, it may also allow for a more for a New Account of Technology. Arctic 46
meaningful collaboration with Inuit, which suits (5):729-753.
Inuit philosophies about the future.
Armstrong, Sue
2001 Ask the Experts: For Canada's Hard-pressed
Conclusion Inuit, Climate Change is More Than a Distant
Threat. Pp. 37-39. New Scientist, November.
Through an ethnographic study of the ways that
Inuit and scientific philosophies about the future Bates, Peter
and its uncertainties play out in practice, I have 2006 Knowing Caribou: Inuit, Ecological Science, and
argued that Inuit philosophies fair admirably in Traditional Ecological Knowledge in the Cana-
comparison to Western preoccupations. Rather dian Arctic. Ph.D. dissertation. Department of
than rigid planning and prediction, many Inuit Anthropology, University of Aberdeen.
instead focus on efficient response and improvi- Beck, Ulrich
sation to whatever reveals itself in the present, 1992 Risk Society: Towards a New Modernity.
alongside a certain amount of general preparation [1986] London: Sage Publications.
to cater for any eventuality. This Inuit approach to
knowing, in which future uncertainty is positively Berkes, Fikret and Dyanna lolly
embraced, does not limit itself in the ways that 2001 Adapting to Climate Change: Social-Ecological
science does. Appreciation and recognition of Resilience in a Canadian Western Arctic Com-
these Inuit philosophies may therefore allow for munity. Conservation Ecology 5(2):18.
more effective management of the Arctic environ- Bielawski, Ellen
ment, via adaptive management strategies. It may 1996 Inuit Knowledge and Science in the Arctic.
also generate a more meaningful collaboration be- In Naked Science: Anthropological Inquiry
tween researchers and Inuit. In this way Inuit may into Boundaries, Power, and iCnowledge.
be able to inform thinking on environmental man- Laura Nader, ed. Pp. 216-227. New York:
agement, rather than being limited to the submis- Routledge.
sion of information that can be slotted into prede- Bird-David, Nurit
termined scientific concepts about the future. 1992 Beyond "The Original Affluent Society": A
Culturalist Reformation. Current Anthropology
Acknowledgments. This work would not have ()
been possible without the hospitality, kindness,
Bodenhorn, B.
and tolerance of the people I worked with in
Cambridge Bay, for which they have my sincere 2000 It's Cood to Know Who Your Relatives Are
thanks. I am also grateful to Tim Ingold, Steve Al- But We Were Taught to Share with Everybody:
Shares and Sharing among Inupiaq Households.
bon, Nancy Wachowich, Joelene Hughes, Martina In The Social Economy of Sharing: Resource
T)n:rell, and Rachel Olson for reading early drafts Allocation and Modern Hunter-Catherers.
of this paper, or the thesis on which it is based. Ceorge Wenzel, Crete Hovelsrud, and Nobuhiro
Funding for my PhD research was generously pro- Kishigami, eds. Pp. 27-56. Senri Ethnologi-
vided through an interdisciplinary NERC/ESRC cal Studies, 53. Osaka: National Museum of
grant. Ethnology.
Bates: Inuit and Scientific Philosophies 97

Bravo, Michael T. Damas, David


2000 Cultural Geographies in Practice: The Rhetoric 2002 Arctic Migrants, Arctic Villagers: The lYansfor-
of Scientific Practice in Nunavut. Ecumene 7(4): mation of Inuit Settlement in the Arctic. Mon-
468^74. treal and Kingston: McGill-Queen's University
Press.
Bravo, Michael T. and Sverker Sörlin (eds.)
2002 Narrative Practice — An Introduction. In Narrat- Dorais, Louis-Jacques
ing the Arctic: A Cultural History of Nordic Sci- 1997 Quaqtaq: Modernity and Identity in an Inuit
entific Practices. Michael T. Bravo and Sverker Gommunity. Toronto: University of Toronto
Sörlin, eds. Pp. 3-32. Canton: Science History Press.
Publications.
Dove, Michael R.
Briggs, Jean L. 1996 Process Versus Product in Bornean Augury: A
1970 Never in Anger: Portrait of an Eskimo Family. Traditional Knowledge System's Solution to
Cambridge: Harvard University Press. the problem of iCnowing. In Redefining Natiure:
Ecology, Gulture, and Domestication. Roy Ellen
1991 Expecting the Unexpected: Canadian Inuit and Katsuyoshi Fukui, eds. Pp. 557-596. Ox-
Training in an Experiential Lifestyle. Ethos ford: Berg.
19(3):259-287.
Ehrlich, Gretel
Brody, Hugh 2006 Last Days of the Ice Hunters? National Geo-
2000 The Other Side of Eden: Hunter Gatherers, graphic, January:78-101.
Farmers and the Shaping of the World. London:
Faber and Faber. Feit, Harvey A.
1998 Reflections on Local Knowledge and Institu-
Burch, Ernest S., Jr. tionalised Resource Management: Differences,
1994 Rationality and Resource Use among Hunters. Dominances and Decentralisation. In: Aborigi-
In Circumpolar Religion and Ecology: An An- nal Environmental Knowledge in the North.
thropology of the North. Takishi Irimoto and Louis-Jacques Dorais, Murielle Nagy, and Judger
Takado Yamada, eds. Pp. 163-185. Tokyo: Uni- Müller-White, eds. Pp. 123-148. Québec: Gétic,
versity of Tokyo Press. Université Laval.
Carpenter, Edmund S. Ferguson, Micbael A. D., Robert Williamson, and Fran-
1956 The Timeless Present in the Mythology of the çois Messier
Aivilik Eskimos. Anthropologica 3:1—4. 1998 Inuit Knowledge of Long-term Ghanges in a
Population of Arctic Tundra Gciribou. Arctic
Childs, Peter and Patrick Williams
1997 An Introduction to Post Colonial Theory. Lon- 51(3):201-219.
don: Longman. Fienup-Riordan, Ann
Clover, Charles 1990 Eskimo Essays: Yup'ik Lives and How We See
Arctic Dwellers Sue U.S. Over Warming. Elec- Them. New Brunswick: Rutgers University
tronic document, http://wvvw.telegraph.co.uk/ Press.
news/main.jhtml?xml=/news/2005/12/09/ Fosset, Rennée
nsci209.xml 2001 In Order to Live Untroubled: Inuit of the Gen-
Collignon, Béatrice tral Arctic 1550-1940. Winnipeg: University of
1993 The Variations of a Land Use Pattern: Seasonal Manitoba Press.
Movements and Cultural Change among Freeman, Milton M. R.
the Copper Inuit. Études/Inuit/Studies 1976 Inuit Land Use and Occupancy Project. Ottawa:
Minister of Supply and Services Ganada.
Condon, Richard G., Peter Collings, and 1985 Appeal to Tradition: Different Perspectives
George Wenzel on Arctic Wildlife Management. In: Native
1995 The Best Part of Life: Subsistence Hunting, Eth- Power: The Quest for Autonomy and Nation-
nicity and Economic Adaptation among Young hood of Indigenous Peoples. Jens Br0sted, Jens
Inuit Males. Arctic 48:31-46. Dabi, Andrew Gray, Hans Ghristian Gull0v,
Georg Henriksen, J0rgen Br0cbner j0rgensen,
Gruikshank, Julie and Inge Kleivan, eds. Pp. 265-281. Oslo:
1998 The Social Life of Stories: Narrative and Knowl- Universitetsforlaget.
edge in the Yukon Territory. Lincoln: University
of Nebraska Press. 1992 The Nature and Utility of Traditional Ecological
Knowledge. Northern Perspectives 20(l):9-12.
Dahl, Jens
2000 Saqqaq: An Inuit Hunting Community in a Mod- Goebring, Brian and Jobn K, Stager
ern World. Toronto, Buffalo, London: University 1991 Tbe Intrusion of Industrial Time and Space
of Toronto Press. into tbe Inuit Lifeworld: Gbanging Perceptions
98 Arctic Anthropology 44:2

and Behaviour. Environment and Behaviour Kothari, Uma


23(6):669-679. 2001 Power, Knowledge, and Social Control in Par-
Golinski, Jan ticipatory Development. In: Participation: The
1998 Making Natural Knowledge: Constructivism and New Tyranny. Bill Cooke and Uma Kothari, eds.
the History of Science. Cambridge: Cambridge Pp. 139-152. London and New York: Zed Books.
University Press. Kuhn, Thomas S.
Gunn, Anne, Goo Arlooktoo, and David Kaomayok 1962 The Structure of Scientific Revolutions.
1988 The Contribution of Ecological Knowledge of [1963] Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Inuit to Wildlife Management in the Northwest Ludwig, Donald, Ray Hilborn, and Carl Waters
Territories. In: Traditional Knowledge and Re- 1993 Uncertainty, Resource Exploitation, and
newable Resource Management. Milton M. R. Conservation: Lessons from History. Science
Freeman and Ludwig N. Caibyn, eds. Pp. 22-30. 260:17-36.
Edmonton: Boreal Institute for Northern Studies
Occasional Paper, 23. Medawar, P. B.
1969 Induction and Intuition in Scientific Thought.
Harries-Jones, Peter London: Methuen and Co., Ltd.
2004 "The Risk Society": Tradition, Ecological Order
and Time-Space Acceleration. In: In The Way of Milton, Kay
Development: Indigenous Peoples, Life Projects 2001 Loving Nature: Towards an Ecology of Emotion.
and Globalisation. Mario Blaser, Harvey Feit, London: Routledge.
and Glen McRae, eds. Pp. 279-298. London:
Zed Books. Morrow, Phyllis
1990 Symbolic Actions, Indirect Expressions: Lim-
Hicks, Jack and Graham White its to Interpretations of Yupik Society. Études/
2000 Nunavut: Inuit Self Determination through Inuit/Studies 14(1-2}:141-158.
a Land Claim and Public Government? In: 1996 Yup'ik Eskimo Agents and American Legal
Nunavut: Inuit Regain Control of their Agencies: Perspectives on Compliance and Re-
Lands and their Lives. Jens Dahl, Jack Hicks,
sistance, The Journal of the Royal Anthropologi-
and Peter Jull, eds. Pp. 30-115. Copenhagen:
cal Institute 2(3):405^23.
IWGIA.
Morrow, Phyllis and Chase Hensel,
Hovelsrud-Broda, Grete 1992 Hidden Dissension: Minority-Majority Relation-
2000 "Sharing", TYansfers, Transactions, and the ships and the Use of Contested Terminology,
Concept of Generalized Reciprocity. In: The Arctic Anthropology 29(l):38-53.
Social Economy of Sharing: Resource Allocation
and Modern Hunter-Gatherers. George Wenzel, Mosse, David
Grete Hovelsrud, and Nobuhiro Kishigami, eds. 2001 "People's Knowledge," Participation, and Pa-
Pp.193-214. Senri Ethnological Studies, 53. tronage: Operations and Representations in
Osaka: National Museum of Ethnology. Rural Development. In: Participation: The New
Tyranny. Bill Cooke and Uma Kothari, eds.
Ingold, Tim Pp. 16-35. London and New York: Zed Books.
1993 Globes and Spheres: The Topology of Environ-
mentalism. in. Environmentalism: The View Nadasdy, Paul
from Anthropology. Kay Milton, ed. Pp. 31-42. 2003 Hunters and Bureaucrats: Power, Knowledge,
London: Routledge. and Aboriginal - State Relations in the South-
west Yukon. Vancouver and Toronto: UBC
2000 The Perception of the Environment: Essays Press.
on Livelihood, Dwelling, and Skill. London:
Routledge. Nuttall, Mark
Ingold, Tim and Terhi Kurttila 1992 Arctic Homeland: Kinship, Community, and
2000 Perceiving the Environment in Finnish Lapland. Development in Northwest Greenland. London:
Body and Society 6(3-4):183-196. Belhaven Press.
Jolly, Dyanna, Fikret Berkes, Jennifer Castleden, Theresa 1998 Protecting the Arctic: Indigenous Peoples
Nichols, and the community of Sach's Haibour and Cultural Survival. Amsterdam: Harwood
2002 We Can't Predict the Weather Like We Used Academic.
To: Inuvialuit Observations of Climate Change, Omura, Keiichi
Sachs Harbour, Western Canadian Arctic. In: 2002 Construction oilnuinaqtun (Real Inuit Way):
The Earth is Faster Now: Indigenous Observa- Self Image and Everyday Practices in Inuit So-
tions of Arctic Environmental Change. Igor ciety. In: Self and Other Images of Hunter Gath-
Krupnik and Dyanna Jolly, eds. Pp. 92-125. erers. Henry Stuart, Alan Bernard, and Keiichi
Fairbanks: Arctic Research Consortium of the Omura, eds. Pp. 101-111. Senri Ethnological
United States. Studies, 60.
Bates: Inuit and Scientific Philosophies 99

O'Neil, John D., Brenda Elias, and Annalee Yassi ages of Hunter Gatherers. H. Stuart, A. Bernard,
1997 Poisoned Eood: Cultural Resistance to the Con- and K. Omura, eds. Pp. 88-100. Senri Ethno-
taminants Discourse in Nunavik. Arctic Anthro- logical Studies, 60.
pology 34(l):29-40.
Suchman, Lucy
Pálsson, Gísli 1987 Plans and Situated Actions: The Problem of
2002 Gender, Race, and Geography in the Writing Human-Machine Gommunication. Gambridge:
of Vilhjalmur Stefansson. In: Narrating the Cambridge University Press.
Arctic: A Cultural History of Nordic Scientific
Practices. Michael T. Bravo and Sverker Sör- Thompson E. P
lin, eds. Pp. 275-309. Canton: Science History 1967 Time, Work, Discipline, and Industrial Gapital-
Publications. ism. Past and Present 38:56-97.
Peterson, Garry, Giulio Alessandro De Leo, Jessica J. TurnbuU, David
Hellmann, Marco A. Janssen, Ann Kinzig, Jay R. Mal- 2000 Masons, Tricksters, and Gartographers. Amster-
colm, Karen L. O'Brien, Shealagh E. Pope, Dale S. Roth- dam: Harwood Academic.
man, Elena Shevliakova, and Robert R. T. Tinch Turner, Edith
1997 Uncertainty, Climate Change, and Adap- 1994 The Effect of Gontact on the Religion of the
tive Management. Conservation Ecology Inupiat Eskimos. In: Circumpolar Religion and
[online]l(2):4. URL: http://www.consecol.org/ Ecology: An Anthropology of the North. Takashi
voll/iss2/art4/ Irimoto and Takako Yamada, eds. Pp. 143-161.
Polanyi, Michael Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press.
1978 Personal Knowledge: Towards a Post-Critical Tyrrell, Martina
[1958] Philosophy. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. 2005 Inuit Perception, Knowledge, and Use of the
Popper, K. R. Sea in Arviat, Nunavut. Ph.D. dissertation.
1979 The Logic of Scientific Discovery. Department of Anthropology, University of
[1959] London: Hutchinson. Aberdeen.
Riewe, Rick and Lloyd Gamble Vitebsky, Piers
1988 The Inuit and Wildlife Management Today. 1995 Erom Gosmology to Environmentalism: Sha-
in; Traditional Knowledge and Renewable Re- manism as Local Knowledge in a Global Set-
source Management. Milton M. R. Ereeman and ting. In: Gounterworks: Managing the Diversity
Ludwig N. Carbyn, eds. Pp. 31-37. Edmonton: of Knowledge. R. Eardon, ed. Pp. 182-203.
Boreal Institute for Northern Studies Occasional London: Routledge.
Paper, 23.
Wacbowich, Nancy
Roepstorff, Andreas 1998 Exhibiting Knowledge: Videoconferencing, tbe
2001 Eacts, Styles, and Traditions: Studies in the Arctic Odyssey, and tbe Ganadian Museum of
Ethnography of Knowledge. Ph.D. dissertation. Nature. In: Aboriginal Environmental Knowl-
Department of Social Anthropology, University edge in tbe Nortb. L. Dorais, M. Nagy, and J.
of Aarhus. Müller-Wbite, eds. Pp 81-94. Québec: Gétic,
Université Laval.
Sahlins, M. D.
1972 Stone Age Economics. London: Tavistock. Walters, Garl
1997 Gballenges in Adaptive Management of
Sigaut, R Riparian and Goastal Ecosystems. Gonservation
1993 Learning, Teaching and Apprenticeship. New Ecology [online]l(2):l. bttp://www.consecol.
Literary History 24:105-114. org/voll/iss2/artl/
Stefansson, Vilhjalmur Wenzel, George
1992 My Life with the Eskimo. New York: 1991 Animal Rigbts, Human Rights: Ecology, Econ-
Macmillan Go. omy, and Ideology in tbe Ganadian Arctic.
Stern, Pamela Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
2003 Upside-Down and Backwards: Time Discipline
1995a Ningiqtuq: Resource Sharing and Generalized
in a Canadian Inuit Town. Anthropologica
Reciprocity in Glyde River, Nunavut. Arctic
45:147-161.
Anthropology 32(2):43-60.
Stevenson, Marc G.
1996 Indigenous Knowledge in Environmental As- 1995b Warming tbe Arctic: Environmentalism and
Ganadian Inuit. In: Human Ecology and Gli-
sessment. Arctic 48(3):278-291.
mate Gbange — People and Resources in the
Stuart, Henry Ear North. D. L. Peterson and D. R. Jobnson,
2002 Ethnonyms and Images: Genesis of the "Inuit" eds. Pp. 169-84. Washington, D.G.: Taylor and
and Image Manipulation. In: Self and Other Im- Erancis.
100 Arctic An thwpology 44:2

2000 Sharing, Money, and Modern Inuit Subsistence: 2004 From TEK to IQ: Inuit Qaujimajatuqangit and
Obligation and Reciprocity at Clyde River, Inuit Cultural Ecology. Arctic Anthropology
Nunavut. In The Social Economy of Sharing: 41(2):238-250.
Resource Allocation and Modern Hunter- ,,
Gatherers. George Wenzel, Grete Hovelsrud, and Woodburn, James
Nobuhiro Kishigami, eds. Pp. 61-85. Senri Eth- 1982 Egalitarian Societies. Man 17:431-452.
nological Studies, 53. Osaka: National Museum
of Ethnology.

You might also like