You are on page 1of 8

J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:1060–1067

DOI 10.1007/s10803-006-0249-0

ORIGINAL PAPER

The Adjustment of Non-Disabled Siblings of Children


with Autism
Ryan J. Macks Æ Ronald E. Reeve

Published online: 27 October 2006


Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2006

Abstract This study compared the psychosocial and family unit (Gray & Holden, 1992; Kaminsky &
emotional adjustment of siblings of children with Dewey, 2002; Wolf, Noh, Fisman, & Speechley, 1989).
autism and siblings of non-disabled children. In Having a family member with any illness, handicap, or
addition, differences between self and parent reports, disability may be challenging, but the unique
as well as various demographic characteristics were combination of impairments associated with autism
examined. Fifty-one siblings of children with autism and related disorders may place family members at an
and 35 siblings of non-disabled children, between the especially high risk for psychological difficulties
ages of 7 and 17, along with one parent of each sibling, (Glasberg, 2000; Holmes & Carr, 1991; Morgan,
participated. Results indicated that the presence of a 1988). Autism presents extreme variation in ability
child with autism appears to enhance the psychosocial and behavior, which magnifies the effects on the family.
and emotional development of non-disabled siblings Specifically, the greater complexity, unpredictability,
when demographic risk factors are limited. However, and inexplicability of autism places the family at greater
the presence of a child with autism appears to have an risk for poor psychological adjustment (Rodrigue,
increasingly unfavorable impact on the non-disabled Geffken, & Morgan, 1993). Additionally, Bebko,
sibling as demographic risk factors increase. Konstantareas, and Springer (1987) reported that
there is more stress in families of children with autism
Keywords Families  Children with autism  Autistic than in families with children who have other
children  Psychosocial adjustment and development disabilities. There is a greater disruption of family
functioning, more upset and disappointment about the
child with a handicap, and fewer recreational activities
and vocational possibilities.
Introduction Given the ways in which children with autism have
been shown to affect their family, it seems only logical
Autism is a severe developmental disorder that to assume that these effects pertain to their siblings.
presents a particularly difficult challenge for the Studies have shown that the characteristic of a child
with a disability that is most clearly related to poorer
sibling adjustment is the severity of the disability. It has
R. J. Macks (&) also been shown that ambiguous disabilities are
Division of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, MLC
associated with poorer sibling outcome. These points
4002, Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center, 3333
Burnet Avenue, Cincinnati, OH 45229-3039, USA are particularly relevant to siblings of children with
e-mail: Ryan.Macks@cchmc.org autism, given the severity and ambiguity of their
disorder (Bristol, 1984; Gold, 1993; McHale,
R. E. Reeve
Simeonsson, & Sloan, 1984).
Curry Programs in Clinical and School Psychology,
University of Virginia, 405 Emmet Street, Charlottesville, Surprisingly, the impact that a child with autism may
VA 22904-4261, USA have on the psychosocial and emotional adjustment of

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:1060–1067 1061

siblings has been largely unexplored (Gray, 1998; Mates, 1990; McHale, Sloan, & Simeonsson, 1986,
Kaminsky & Dewey, 2002). Among the studies that Rodrigue et al., 1993). Kaminsky and Dewey (2002)
have been completed, results have been inconsistent. found that siblings of children with autism were not at
Some studies have reported poor adjustment, higher inflated risk for adjustment difficulties and did
rates of depression, and poor social competence for not display deficits in social competence. Similarly,
siblings of children with autism (Bagenholm & Rodrigue, Morgan, and Geffken (1992) found that
Gillberg, 1991; Gold, 1993). However, other studies siblings of children with autism did not differ
have reported siblings of children with autism are significantly from siblings of non-disabled children
typically well-adjusted, with positive self-concepts and with regard to perceived competence and social/
good social competence (Ferrari, 1984; Mates, 1990). behavioral adjustment. Furthermore, although the
The common assumption made by most researchers is siblings of children with autism had more internalizing
that the siblings of children with autism will be and externalizing behavior problems when compared to
more likely to experience problems of psychological siblings of non-disabled children, their average scores on
adjustment than will siblings of non-disabled children. both dimensions fell within the normal range. A few
Researchers in this area have listed several stressors that studies have even reported that siblings of children with
the siblings of children with autism may experience, autism fare better than siblings of non-disabled children
including changes in family roles, restructuring of the with respect to self-concept and the overall quality of
family’s functioning and activities, loss or absence of the sibling relationship (Berger, 1980; McHale et al.,
parental attention, feelings of guilt, a sensation of shame 1986).
often brought on by the negative evaluation of their peer Several studies have reported that psychosocial and
group, and perplexity at the bizarre behavior that emotional adjustment of siblings of children with
children with autism may display (Randall & Parker, disabilities is largely dependent upon the number of
1999). siblings in the family, the socioeconomic status of the
Some research has found that siblings are often family, and the gender and birth order of the non-
resentful of the child with autism because of the disabled sibling (Farber, 1968; Ferrari, 1984; Gold,
resulting restrictions on family activities and the time 1993; Gath, 1974; Mates, 1990; Schwirian, 1976). Mates
and resources that have to be expended on them. (1990) reported that research on siblings of children
These siblings are also frequently embarrassed by the with disabilities indicates that females and siblings from
child with autism for their inappropriate public two-child families are at a greater risk for poor
behavior. Additionally, the siblings often resent not adjustment. Other studies have supported the finding
being able to invite friends to their home, and if they that siblings who live in families with more than two
do, must deal with the possibility of being embarrassed children are more likely to be well-adjusted than are
by the misbehavior of the child with autism (Gray, siblings living in two-child families (Ferrari, 1984; Gath,
1998). 1974; Kaminsky & Dewey, 2002). The explanation for
Perhaps the most obvious reason why siblings may this effect states that, in larger families, parents are less
be negatively affected is because of the great deal of likely to overburden individual siblings with childcare
parental time, energy, attention, and resources that the responsibilities and high expectations. Furthermore,
child with autism requires. Howlin (1988) found that these siblings have a ‘‘normal’’ brother or sister to
many of these siblings felt that they did not receive the whom they can relate and compare themselves (Mates,
same amount of parental attention as the child with 1990).
autism. These siblings often report feeling as though Other family variables are important to consider as
they are treated unfairly by their parents because the well. Older siblings appear to have greater difficulty
child with autism is held to a lower standard. with feelings, behaviors, and coping abilities.
Furthermore, due to the greater amount of parental Additionally, older siblings showed higher rates of
time and attention that children with autism require, internalizing and externalizing behavior problems
siblings are often expected to do more household (Rodrigue et al., 1992).
chores and physically care for the child with autism However, like most of the research concerning
(Randall & Parker, 1999). siblings of children with autism, there are some studies
In contrast to the aforementioned findings, other that contradict the finds of those just mentioned. Some
studies have reported that siblings of children with researchers have reported that family size, birth order,
autism are well-rounded with positive self-concepts, and the gender of the non-disabled sibling were not
high levels of social competence, and healthy academic related to adjustment (Gold, 1993; Mates, 1990; McHale
and behavioral adjustment (Ferrari, 1984; Gold, 1993; et al., 1986; Rodrigue et al., 1993).

123
1062 J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:1060–1067

One potential explanation for the lack of consistency In two-parent families, the parents decided between
within this area of research is the different types of themselves which of the two would participate. When
measures that have been used. Most researchers have families had more than one non-disabled child, the
used parent reports while others have used self-reports. participating child was the one who was closest in age
Parent reports appear to yield different results than do to the child with autism. Thirty-six families with
sibling self-reports (Ferrari, 1984; Lobato et al., 1987). children who do not have autism or any other
It appears that parents, particularly mothers, rate non- developmental disabilities were also included as a
disabled siblings as being more aggressive, depressed, comparison group. These families had to have a
and having a higher overall level of pathology than the minimum of two children, with at least one child
siblings rate themselves (Gold, 1990; Lobato et al., being between the ages of 7–17. If there were multiple
1987). Consequently, the value of collecting data from children in a family who were between the ages of 7–
multiple informants, for research purposes, has been 17, one child was randomly selected by the researcher
stressed, but has not been widely practiced (Compas, for participation. As well, one parent of each child was
Howell, Phares, Williams, & Giunta, 1989; Offord, also included in the study. All of the participating
Boyle, Flemming, Blum, & Grant, 1989). parents from both the experimental and comparison
Given the lack of consistency among previous groups were mothers.
research studies related to siblings of children with Families with children with autism were recruited
autism, the researcher hypothesized that there would from special day schools in suburban areas in Maryland
be no significant differences between the experimental (The Ivymount School) and Virginia (The Virginia
and comparison groups on the Piers–Harris, CDI-S Institute of Autism, Mathew’s Center for Visual
(Children’s Depression Inventory-Short Form ), or Learning, and New Horizons). Families with children
BASC–PRS (Behavior Assessment System for with autism in residential schools were not recruited.
Children–Parent Rating Scales). In accordance with For the comparison group, families of children who do
previous research studies in which some consistent not have autism or any other developmental
findings have occurred, the researcher also predicted disabilities were recruited from Baltimore County
that non-disabled siblings from both the experimental Public Schools in Maryland and Fluvanna County
and comparison groups would score better on the Public Schools in Virginia. Demographic breakdowns
Piers–Harris Children’s Self-Concept Scale, CDI-S, and of the experimental and comparison groups, for SES
BASC–PRS if they: were male, had more than one and the number of siblings in the family can be found
sibling, were younger than the child with autism, and in Table 1. Information about birth order in relation to
came from a family with a high SES level. Additionally, the sibling with autism and gender can be found in
the researcher hypothesized that a significant Table 2.
interaction effect would occur between the All participating families were informed that they
demographic characteristics and the child’s scores on would automatically be entered in to a random
the various report measures. It was predicted that the drawing for cash prizes. Three families were selected
interaction effect would be significantly larger for to receive a cash prize.
siblings of children with autism than for siblings of
non-autistic children.

Methods Table 1 SES and number of siblings for experimental and


comparison groups
Participants Demographic variable n M SD

Fifty-one families with an autistic child were recruited SES (Experimental) 51 2.76a 1.12
SES (Comparison) 36 2.67b .72
for this study. The participants from each family
Number of sibs (Experimental) 51 1.35 .77
included one parent and one non-disabled sibling who Number of sibs (Comparison) 36 1.36 .59
was between the ages of 7–17. The age of the child with a
This score means that the average SES level for the
autism was not considered during the recruitment
experimental group fell in-between the annual income ranges
process. However, all of these children had a diagnosis of $26,000–50,000 and $51,000–75,000
of autism as opposed to other autism spectrum disorders b
This score means that the average SES level for the
such as Asperger’s syndrome, Pervasive Developmental comparison group fell in-between the annual income ranges of
Disorder, or high functioning autism. $26,000–50,000 and $51,000–75,000

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:1060–1067 1063

Table 2 Birth order and gender data for experimental and When a completed index card was received, the
comparison groups examiner called the family to explain their role in the
Demographic variable Experimental Comparison study and verify the family’s desire and willingness to
participate. Once a family agreed to participate, a
Birth order
Younger 24 18
packet was sent to their home. The packet contained a
Older 27 18 cover letter explaining the proper procedure for filling
Gender out each questionnaire and returning it in the self-
Male 21 16 addressed stamped envelope. The packet also included
Female 30 20
one demographic questionnaire, one BASC–PRS, one
CDI-S, and one Piers–Harris Children’s Self-Concept
Scale.
Measures
Results
All participating children were asked to complete the
Children’s Depression Inventory-Short Form (CDI-S)
Several Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) procedures
(Kovacs, 1992) and the Piers–Harris Children’s Self-
were performed to compare the scores of siblings of
Concept Scale (Piers, 1984). All participating parents
children with autism and siblings of non-disabled
were asked to complete the Behavior Assessment
children on the CDI-S, Piers–Harris, and BASC–
System for Children–Parent Rating Scales (BASC–
PRS. To control for Type I error rate for multiple
PRS) with respect to the participating non-disabled
comparisons, the significance level was set at .01 for all
child. Additionally, parents were asked to complete a
analyses. Prior to running the ANOVA’s, a
demographic questionnaire, asking them to indicate
Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) was
their gender; the gender, age, and birth order of
performed to justify performing the ANOVA’s. A
the participating child; their approximate annual
MANOVA for the total scores of each questionnaire
household income (by indicating an income range in
was significant, Wilkes Multivariate F(4,82) = 6.50,
increments of $25,000); the total number of children in
P < .01. MANOVA’s were then performed for the
the family; and a space for comments by the non-
factor scores of each questionnaire. A MANOVA for
disabled child.
the Piers–Harris factor scores was significant, Wilkes
Multivariate F (7,79) = 20.29, P < .01. A MANOVA
Procedures for the BASC–PRS factor scores was also significant,
Wilkes Multivariate F (4,82) = 4.39, P < .01.
To recruit participants through a school, a recruitment An ANOVA did not reveal any significant
letter was sent home to the parents of each child with differences between the experimental and comparison
autism by a school administrator. Each letter was groups on the CDI-S, F (1,85) = 1.60, P > .05. There
accompanied by a self-addressed, stamped index card. also were no significant differences between these
The parents were instructed to write their name, phone groups on the BASC–PRS (see Table 3). However, an
number, and address on the card and mail it back to ANOVA revealed that the experimental group scored
the examiner if they wished to participate. significantly higher than the comparison group on the

Table 3 ANOVA for BASC


Measure df SS MS F P
total and factor scores
Total
Between groups 1 1684.64 1684.64 6.70 .011
Within groups 85 21364.97 251.35
Externalizing
Between groups 1 731.85 731.85 3.99 .049
Within groups 85 15579.56 183.29
Internalizing
Between groups 1 136.94 136.94 .81 .370
Within groups 85 14335.29 168.65
Adaptive skills
Between groups 1 54.89 54.89 .60 .440
Within groups 85 7765.46 91.36

123
1064 J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:1060–1067

Table 4 ANOVA for Piers–


Measure df SS MS F p
Harris total and factor scores*
P < .01 Total
Between groups 1 1046.66 1046.66 9.06* .003
Within groups 85 9816.40 115.49
Behavior
Between groups 1 703.69 703.69 9.65* .003
Within groups 85 6195.82 72.89
Intellectual & School status
Between groups 1 820.91 820.91 10.06* .002
Within groups 85 6936.15 81.60
Physical appearance
Between groups 1 309.05 309.05 3.43 .068
Within groups 85 7663.18 90.16
Anxiety
Between groups 1 775.95 775.95 8.15* .005
Within groups 85 8096.40 95.25
Popularity
Between groups 1 1.34 1.34 .01 .908
Within groups 85 8407.93 98.92
Happiness & Satisfaction
Between groups 1 183.76 183.76 2.90 .092
Within groups 85 5386.29 63.37

Piers–Harris Total score as well as the Piers–Harris the BASC–PRS Adaptive Skills factor. For the
Behavior, Intellectual and School Status, and Anxiety comparison group, no significant correlations were
factor scores (see Table 4). found (see Table 5).
A risk scale composite was created to compare the Following the Pearson correlations, Fisher Z-values
cumulative effect of various demographic characteris- were calculated to compare the risk scale correlations
tics on children from both the experimental and between the experimental and comparison groups.
comparison groups. Based on the findings from the Significant Z-values were found for the CDI-S, the
previously mentioned research studies that examined Piers–Harris Total score, the Piers–Harris Physical
demographic variables with respect to siblings of Appearance, Popularity, and Happiness and
children with disabilities, the demographics that were Satisfaction factors, and the BASC–PRS Adaptive
considered to place a child more at risk for social, Skills factor (see Table 6).
emotional, and scholastic difficulties included being a
male, coming from a family of low SES, only having Table 5 Correlations between the risk scale and the
one sibling, and being older than the child with autism. questionnaires for the experimental and comparison groups
Demographic characteristics that were found to Measure Risk scale Risk scale
decrease a child’s risk included being female, coming (experimental) (comparison)
from a family of high SES, having more than one CDI-S .64** .22
sibling, and being younger than the child with autism. Piers–Harris total –.71** –.31
A child received one point for each demographic risk PH behavior –.55** –.31
factor that applied to them, meaning that total scores PH intellectual & –.57** –.23
school status
ranged from 0–4. PH physical –.74** –.26
Once the risk scale was completed for each child, appearance
Pearson Correlations were performed to examine the PH anxiety –.56** –.28
relationship between the experimental and comparison PH popularity –.72** –.28
PH happiness & –.70** –.29
group’s risk scale and the CDI-S, Piers–Harris, and satisfaction
BASC–PRS. BASC total .26 –.12
For the experimental group, a significant positive BASC externalizing .22 .12
correlation was found between the risk scale and the BASC internalizing .22 .17
BASC adaptive skills –.63** –.12
CDI-S. Significant negative correlations were found
between the risk scale and the Piers–Harris Total * P < .01
score, all of the Piers–Harris factor scores, as well as ** P < .001

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:1060–1067 1065

Table 6 Fisher Z–values comparing the risk scale correlations adjustment somewhat more negatively than did
between the experimental and comparison groups parents who did not have any children with autism.
Measure Z P This discrepancy suggests that, in families of children
with autism, parents have markedly different views
CDI-S 2.37* .009
Piers–Harris total 2.51 .006
from the non-disabled sibling, regarding the ways in
PH behavior 1.32 .093 which siblings’ psychosocial and emotional adjustment
PH intellectual & school status 1.83 .034 are affected by the children with autism.
PH physical appearance 3.03* .001 Perhaps parents spend so much time attending to
PH anxiety 1.53 .063
PH popularity 2.74* .003
the needs of the child with autism that they really do
PH happiness & satisfaction 2.52* .005 not have an accurate view of the sibling’s social and
BASC total 1.71 .044 emotional functioning. Another possible explanation
BASC externalizing .46 .323 for the discrepancy is that parents may project any
BASC internalizing .23 .409
BASC adaptive Skills 2.75* .003
frustrations that they may have because of their child
with autism on to their non-disabled child. It is also
* P < .01 possible that if a child with autism causes a parent a
good deal of stress, the parent may then assume that
Discussion the sibling is also experiencing the same amount of
stress.
Siblings of children with autism appeared to have The discrepancy between the self-report and parent
a more positive self-concept than did siblings of report measures is very important when considering
non-disabled children, as indicated by the scores on that most studies done on siblings of children with
the Piers–Harris. They were much more likely to have autism have used only a single type of respondent.
a positive view of their behavior, intelligence, These results support an assertion made earlier: when
scholastic performance, and anxiety than were studying siblings of children with autism, it is
siblings of non-disabled children. These children also advisable for the examiner to use multiple types of
had a more positive view of their overall personal respondents.
characteristics in comparison to siblings of non- When multiple demographic risk factors were
disabled children. examined with the risk scale composite, they
There are various possible explanations for the significantly predicted psychosocial and emotional
responses by these groups. One possibility is that on adjustment for siblings of children with autism but
self-report measures, siblings of children with autism not for siblings of non-disabled children. This finding
compare themselves to their autistic sibling. It would suggests that demographic characteristics are more
only be natural to assume that these children would likely to impact a sibling of a child with autism than a
view themselves favorably in such a comparison. sibling of a non-disabled child. In fact, results from
Another possible explanation for these results could the Piers–Harris suggest that having a sibling with
be that there is a difference in the level of maturity. autism may not be a risk factor in and of itself, and
Gray (1998) noted that siblings of children with autism that children with autism may even have a positive
are often more mature than their peers. Perhaps this influence on the life of the non-disabled sibling.
maturity leads to improved behavior, better social However, when multiple demographic risk factors are
skills, and improved academic performance, thereby present, it becomes more difficult for the non-disabled
increasing their overall self-concept. sibling to deal with the child with autism, both
It should be noted that, while siblings of children emotionally and psychologically. Hence, the
with autism scored significantly better than the norms presence of a child with autism appears to have an
on the self-report measures of self-concept and increasingly negative effect on the non-disabled
depression, siblings of non-disabled children still sibling as the number of demographic risk factors
scored in the average range on these measures. increase.
Therefore, this discrepancy between the groups was This finding may help to explain why there has been
not a result of a misrepresented comparison group but a lack of consistency among past studies. Perhaps those
by a seemingly well-adjusted experimental group. studies that were reporting positive influences of
Interestingly, although significant differences were children with autism on their siblings were using a
not found between the experimental and comparison sample of children who were not exposed to multiple
groups on the BASC–PRS, parents of children with demographic risk factors. Alternatively, those studies
autism viewed the siblings’ social and emotional that were reporting negative influences of children with

123
1066 J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:1060–1067

autism on their siblings may have used samples of Bristol, M. M. (1984). Family resources and successful adaptation
children who did experience multiple demographic risk to autistic children. In: E. Schopler, & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.).
The effects of autism on the family (pp. 289–310). New York:
factors. Plenum Press.
One limitation of this study was the small sample Compas, B. E., Howell, D. B., Phares, V., Williams, R. A., &
size for the both the experimental and comparison Giunta, C. T. (1989). Risk factors for emotional/behavioral
groups. Due to limited funding, the recruitment of problems in aging adolescents: A prospective analysis of
adolescent and parental stress and symptoms. Journal of
participants was limited. A second limitation was that Consulting Clinical Psychology, 57, 732–740.
families of children with autism in residential facilities Farber, B. (1968). Mental retardation: Its social context and social
were not included. Therefore, the results of this study consequences. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
should not be generalized to all siblings of children Ferrari, M. (1984). Chronic illness: Psychosocial effects of
siblings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25,
with autism, but rather, only to siblings of children with 459–476.
autism who live at home. A third limitation of this Gath, A. (1974). Sibling reactions to mental handicap: A
study is that the families for the experimental group comparison of brothers and sisters of mongol children.
were recruited from specialty day schools for children Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 15, 187–198.
Glasberg, B. A. (2000). The development of siblings’
with autism and other disabilities. Therefore, the understanding of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
families in this study all had community resources Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(2), 143–156.
available to them and were taking advantage of these Gold, N. (1993). Depression and social adjustment in siblings of
resources to some extent. Fourth, all of the boys with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 23, 147–163.
participating parents were mothers and consequently, Gray, D. E. (1998). Autism and the family: Problems, prospects,
these findings do not necessarily apply to fathers. and coping with the disorder. Springfield, Illinois: Charles C.
Finally, for future reference it would like be easier to Thomas, Publisher.
compare self and parent report measures that are Gray, D. E., & Holden, W. J. (1992). Psychosocial well-being
among the parents of children with autism. Australia and New
designed with the same intention. For example, it may Zealand Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 18, 83–93.
be better to compare self report from and Achenbach Holmes, N., & Carr, J. (1991). The pattern in care of families of
versus parent report of an Achenbach, rather than adults with a mental handicap: A comparison between
comparing a Piers–Harris to a BASC–PRS. families of autistic adults and Down Syndrome adults.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21, 159–
This study supports the assertion that future studies 176.
on this topic should include multiple respondents and Howlin, P. (1988). Living with impairment: The effects on
should examine various demographic characteristics. children of having and autistic sibling. Child: Care, Health
Some other demographics that could be examined are and Development, 14, 395–408.
Kaminsky, L., & Dewey, D. (2002). Psychosocial adjustment in
race, religion, marital status, and the gender of the siblings of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology
child with autism. Additionally, siblings are likely to and Psychiatry, 43(2), 225–232.
be the closest relatives of individuals with autism and Kovacs, M. (1992). The children’s depression inventory: Manual.
are expected to take over a supervisory/management North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems, Inc.
Lobato, D., Barbour, L., Hall, L. J., & Miller, C. T. (1987).
role of the death of their parents (Bigby, 1997). Psychosocial characteristics of handicapped and
Hence, research on adult sibling relationships would nonhandicapped children. Journal of Abnormal child
help clinicians be better able to prepare siblings for Psychology, 3, 329–338.
the tasks that lie ahead. Mates, T. E. (1990). Siblings of autistic children: Their
adjustment and performance at home and in school.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20(4),
545–553.
References McHale, S. M., Simeonnson, R. J., & Sloan, J. L. (1984). Children
with handicapped brothers and sisters. In: E. Schopler, & G.
Bagenholm, A., & Gillberg, C. (1991). Psychosocial effects on B. Mesibov (Eds.). The effects of autism on the family (pp.
siblings of children with autism and mental retardation: A 327–342). New York: Plenum Press.
population-based study. Journal of Mental Deficiency, 35, McHale, S. M., Sloan, J. L., & Simeonnson, R. J. (1986). Sibling
291–307. relationships of children with autistic, mentally retarded and
Bebko, J. M., Konstantareas, M. M., & Springer, J. (1987). nonhandicapped brothers and sisters. Journal of Autism and
Parent and professional evaluations of family stress Developmental Disorders, 16, 399–413.
associated with characteristics of autism. Journal of Morgan, S. B. (1988). The autistic child and family functioning:
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17(4), 565–576. A developmental family-systems perspective. Journal of
Berger, E. W. (1980). A study of self-concept of siblings of autistic Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 263–280.
children. Unpublished Dissertation, University of Cincinnati. Offord, D., Boyle, M. H., Flemming, M. D., Blum, H. M., &
Bigby, C. (1997). Parental substitutes? The role of siblings in the Grant, N. I. (1989). Ontario child health study: Summary
lives of older people with intellectual disability. Journal of of selected results. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 34,
Gerontological Social Work, 29(1), 2–22. 483–491.

123
J Autism Dev Disord (2007) 37:1060–1067 1067

Piers, E. V. (1984). Piers–Harris children’s self-concept scale: down syndrome, and normal development. Journal of
Revised manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Autism and Developmental Disorders, 22, 249–264.
Services. Schwirian, P. M. (1976). Effects of the presence of a hearing
Randall, P., & Parker, J. (1999). Supporting the families of impaired preschool child in the family on behavior patterns
children with autism. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. of older ‘‘normal’’ siblings. American Annals of the Deaf,
Rodrigue, J. R., Geffken, G. R., & Morgan, S. B. (1993). 121, 373–380.
Perceived competence and behavioral adjustment of siblings Wolf, L. C., Noh, S., Fisman, S. N., & Speechley, M. (1989). Brief
of children with autism. Journal of Autism and report: Psychological effects of parenting stress on parents
Developmental Disorders, 23(4), 665–674. of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Rodrigue, J. R., Morgan, S. B., & Geffken, G. R. (1992). Disorders, 19, 157–166.
Psychosocial adaptation of fathers of children with autism,

123

You might also like