You are on page 1of 9

The Self-Concept of Siblings of Handicapped Children:

A Comparison

LILYDYSON
University of Victoria
REBECCA R. FEWELL
Tulane University

Self-concept of siblings of handicapped children was compared with that of children without handi.
capped siblings. Results of a standardized children’s self-concept scale showed that siblings of handi-
capped children did not differ from other children in self-concept. Extreme scores, however, suggested
differential responses among the siblings. Some children in both groups had poor self-concept scores.
Though statistically insignificant, females in both groups showed higher self-concept scores than males.
Males with handicapped siblings had higher self-concepts than the males without handicapped siblings.
Results are discussed in terms of theoretical models and research evidence. Suggestions are made for in-
tervention and research.

The relationship between siblings is special altered. A handicapped child may not func-
and intimate. This is especially true during tion as a playmate, a caretaker, or a socializ.
childhood when siblings serve each other as ing agent for his siblings. In some families,
playmates and models of identification, as the handicapped child may generate care-
well as caretakers and socializing agents taking responsibilities for his siblings and
(Cicirelli, I982; Werner, 1979). In these roles provide a negative model of identification.
siblings can have profound influences on The sibling relationship may be negatively
each other’s social and emotional develop- affected by the additional resources required
ment (Dunn, 1983; Sutton.Smith & Rosen- by the handicapped child.
berg, 1970). Roles are affected by family size A child may also be negatively influenced
and birth order, for example, which are by a handicapped sibling indirectly, through
related to a child’s intelligence (Belmont & parental reaction. A handicapped child re-
Marolla, 1973; Zajonc, 1976). On the prac- quires parents to revise their hopes for their
tical level, siblings share family resources child. Research supports the notion that a
and may affect resources available to each handicapped child creates stress for the
other, and hence, each other’s development. parents (Bradshaw & Lawton, 1978; Cum-
Sibling relationships take’on special mings, 1976; Dyson & Fewell, 1986). Paren-
significance when one sibling is handi- tal stress may in turn negatively affect the
capped (Powell & Ogle, 1985). The roles a siblings. Ample reasons thus exist to sug-
sibling normally assumes are necessarily gest that a child may be adversely affected

230

Downloaded from jei.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 8, 2016


by a handicapped sibling, particularly in self- tioning of handicapped children (Gath,
concept, which is theorized to be a motiva- 1973) but not to self-concept (Lobato et al.,
tional force with pervasive consequences 1987).
(Coopersmith, 1967), and a determinant of a Disparate findings on the effect of a hand-
person’s behavior and sense of self-worth icapped child on his siblings’ self-concept
(Harvey & Greenway, 1984). Siblings influ- and the effect of sibling and familial vari-
ence a child’s self-concept (Tesser, 1980). ables may be partly due to methodological
Their effect is likely to be negative when the weaknesses. These include a lack of a con-
sibling is handicapped, due to character- trol group (e.g., Gayton, Friedman, & Tavor-
istics of the handicapped sibling and the mina, 1977), inadequate control for sex and
nature of the relationship with a handi- age, and unequal sample size (e.g., Harvey &
capped sibling. Greenway, 1984). More stringent experimen-
There are few empirical studies of the self- tal control was applied by Ferrari (1984;
concept of siblings of handicapped children, 1987) and Lobato et al. (1987), yet conflict.
and their results are conflicting. Some stud- ing results remain. Further investigation is
ies have found a lower than average self- needed to understand the impact of a hand-
concept in siblings of physically handi- icapped child on siblings.
capped children (Harvey & Greenway, 1984) The main purpose of this study was to in-
and of diabetic children (Ferrari, 1987). vestigate self-concept in siblings of handi-
Other studies, however, have found no dif- capped children and siblings of nonhandi-
ferences in the self-concept of siblings of capped children. The study also examined
chronically ill (Ferrari, 1984), developmen- the mediating effect of sibling and familial
tally handicapped (Lobato, Barbour, Hall, & characteristics such as sex of the sibling,
Miller, 1987), or nonhandicapped children. type of handicap, and socioeconomic status.
Still another study found a higher than
average self-concept in siblings of children
with cystic fibrosis (Gayton, Friedman, METHOD
Tavormina, & Tucker, 1977).
Equally unclear is the role of the sibling’s Subject
sex and birth order in a relationship with a The subjects were 74 children equally di-

handicapped child. These are variables that vided into two groups: children with handi-
are recognized to influence sibling relation- capped siblings (handicapped group) and
ships (Sutton-Smith & Rosenberg, 1970), children without handicapped siblings (non-
and sex and birth order have been found to handicapped group). The two groups were
influence the psychological adjustment of matched by sex, geographical region,
siblings of handicapped children (Breslau, socioeconomic status (SES), and age within
Weitzman, & Messenger, 1981; Lavigne & 18 months. There were 52 Americans and
Ryan, 1979). The influence of these variables 22 Canadians, with 26 Americans and 11
on a sibling’s self-concept is less clear. For Canadians in each group. There were 34
example, the effect of sex has been found to males and 40 females, with ages ranging
be negligible (Ferrari, 1987; Lobato et al., from 7 to 14 years (mean age, 10 years).
1987) and the impact of birth order also not Demographic information on subjects is
significant in studies of siblings of handi- presented in Table 1.
capped children (Ferrari, 1984; 1987). The handicapped children ranged in age
Likewise, family socioeconomic status (SES) from one to 16 years (mean age 7.5 years).
has been related to the psychological func- Fifteen of the American siblings of handi-

231

Downloaded from jei.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 8, 2016


TABLE 1
Demographic Characteristics o/Subjects (1‘I =
74)

capped children had attended a sibling pro- rolled. The Canadian siblings of nonhandi-
gram offered quarterly by a university-based capped children were recruited from private
family support program that included out- sources and a private school. The American

ings and workshops featuring discussion siblings of nonhandicapped children were


groups and opportunities for the siblings to selected from a public school in a middle-
raise and share concerns and questions per- income neighborhood. Upon provision of in-
taining to .their handicapped brother or formed consent by the parents or schools, a
sister (Meyer, Vadasy, ~ Fewell, 1985). children’s self-concept scale was admin-

istered to the subjects either at home, at a
Procedure workshop, or at school. With the American
Siblings of handicapped children were re- sample, the scale was administered to two
cruited from the special education programs classes of children in the public school. Sib-
in which the handicapped children were en- lings of nonhandicapped children who

232

Downloaded from jei.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 8, 2016


matched the siblings of handicapped chil- =
65.4) than the American sample (M =

dren in sex, age, SES, and the presence of a 61.3). In subsequent analyses the two sam-
sibling were included in the final sample. ples were treated as a homogeneous group.
Instrumentation
The study employed the Piers-Harris Chil- Comparison of the Handicapped and the
dren’s Self-Concept Scale-The Way I Feel Nonhandicapped Group
About Myself (CSCS) (Piers & Harris, 1969). Table 2 shows mean self-concept scores for
The CSCS consists of 80 yes-no items, and the handicapped siblings, the nonhandi-
was standardized on children grades 4 capped siblings, and for the combined group
through 12. The scale can be administered of siblings. The mean self-concept score of
individually or in a group. Sample items in- 62.5 for the total group falls in the top range
clude, &dquo;I am strong,&dquo; &dquo;I am shy,&dquo; and &dquo;It is of the normative data compiled by Piers and
hard for me to make friends:’ The internal Harris (1969). There were no significant dif-
consistency ranges from .88 to .93, and the ferences between the two groups.
test-retest reliability from .42 to .96 (Epstein, Differences between the two groups of
1985). The correlation between this scale siblings within each region were also ex-
and Coopersmith’s Self-Esteem Inventory amined to reaffirm the effects so far ob-
was .85 (Piers, 1976). The CSCS is psycho- served. As with the total group, no difference
metrically sound (Bentler, 1972; Epstein, was found between the handicapped and the
1985; Jeske, 1985) and a promising re- nonhandicapped group in either sample. A
search tool (Wylie, 1974). wide range of self-concept scores was found
The CSCS provides a total self-concept within both groups. For the siblings of hand-
score. A high score represents a positive and icapped children, the minimum score was
high self-concept, and a low score, a negative 38 and the maximum 78, with a range of 40
and low self-concept. A total of six cluster (median, 65). A similar distribution was ob-
scales are factorially determined and in- served in the nonhandicapped siblings’
clude : Factor I-Behavior, Factor 2-Intel- group with a minimum of 33, maximum of
lectual and School Status, Factor 3-Physical 80, a range of 47, and a median of 63.
Appearance and Attributes, Factor 4-Anxi- The two groups of siblings were further
ety, Factor 5-Popularity, and Factor 6-Hap- compared on the six-factor scales of the
piness and Satisfaction. Piers-Harris. No factor differentiated the two
groups. Comparisons of the two groups
along the subscales were also made within
RESULTS each geographical region. Again, no differ-
ences on any of the factors were found be-

Comparison of the American and tween the handicapped and the nonhandi-
Canadian Sample _ capped group in either the Canadian or the
Differences in the level of self-concept be- American subsample.
tween the American and the Canadian sam- To ensure that significant differences be-
ple were first examined to determine if the tween the groups were not overlooked, the
two samples represented two distinct popu- two groups were compared on all items
lations. The difference between the two using chi square. The results showed that
groups was not statistically significant (p =
the two groups were distinguished only on 5
.13), though the Canadian sample obtained of the 80 items, a proportion no greater than
slightly higher mean self-concept scores (M that expected by chance.

233

Downloaded from jei.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 8, 2016


TABLE 2
Means and Standard Deviations of Self- Concept Scores

Comparison of Males and Females 3. Severe behavior disorders (autism)


Table 3 shows comparisons by sex on self- 4. Mental retardation
concept scores across samples. Although
sex differences failed to reach statistical sig- These categories were submitted to a one-
nificance (p =
.09), a trend was found with way analysis of variance. The results showed
females scoring significantly higher than no differences among the siblings of dif-
males when the two groups of siblings were ferent kinds of handicapped children (F [3,
pooled. When males were compared across 32] 2.24, p
=
.10). Statistical analyses
=

groups, the mean self-concept score of were further performed to examine the ef-
males with handicapped siblings was 61.2, fects of birth order and SES in the handi-
compared to 59.5 for males with nonhandi- capped group. As with the type of handicap
capped siblings. The mean scores for the no significant differences were detected on
females in the handicapped and nonhandi- birth order (t = -.65,p .52)orSES(F[l,
=

capped group were nearly the same, 64.7 35] .14, p = .71).
=

and 64.3, respectively. Neither of these dif- Finally, a comparison was made between
ferences was significant. the siblings who were enrolled in the sibling
support program and other siblings of hand-
Comparisons of Other Conditions icapped children from the same geographi-
In comparing the effects of different types of cal region. No significant difference was
handicaps, we collapsed the five categories found between the two groups (t = -.98,
of handicaps into four due to the low fre- p = .34).
quencies of speech disorders and develop-
mental delays. This produced four categor-
ies for analyses: DISC(ISSION

1. Speech disorders and developmental This study included siblings from two coun-
delays tries, the U.S. and Canada. The two groups of
2. Physical and sensory handicaps siblings were mostly combined for analyses

TABLE 3
Comparisons o jSexes on Mean Self. Concept Scores Across Samples

234

Downloaded from jei.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 8, 2016


since they showed a similar level of self-con- pie size and uneven distribution in birth
cept. All analyses showed no differences in order and SES which may have obscured the
the level of self-concept between siblings of effects of these variables.
children with handicaps and siblings of non- Siblings enrolled in a support program
handicapped children. The two groups also did not differ from other siblings in self-
did not differ along various dimensions of concept. This may be attributed to the short
self-concept. It appears that, as a group, sib- duration and infrequent meetings of the
lings of handicapped children are more sim- group. Perhaps participation in widely
ilar to than different from other children in spaced quarterly workshops and discussion
self-concept. Although conflicting with re- groups was insufficient to cause changes,
ports on siblings of diabetics (Ferrari, 1987) especially in a relatively enduring personali-
and physically handicapped children (Har- ty dimension such as self-concept.
vey & Greenway, 1984), the present observa- We found no evidence to support that the
tion is consistent with findings on siblings of type of handicap affected self-concept. The
children with pervasive developmental dis- significance level (p .10), however, sug-
=

abilities and diabetes (Ferrari, 1984), cystic gests the small numbers in several of the
fibrosis (Gayton et al., 1977), autism (Mates, handicap groups may have masked the ef-
1982), and various developmental handi- fects of type of handicap.
caps (Lobato et al., 1987). Two hypotheses may account for the find-
Differential levels of self-concept, however, ings that siblings of handicapped children
did emerge, with a wide range of scores for did not differ from other children in self-
siblings of handicapped children. Whereas concept. One hypothesis is that the actual
most of these siblings displayed a high self- effect of a child’s handicap on sibling self-
concept, a few showed an extremely low self- concept is slight because of the permanent
concept. This phenomenon, however, was nature of self-concept. A second hypothesis
not unique to the siblings of handicapped suggests that siblings may have developed a
children and thus may reflect a general de- positive concept through certain transac-
velopmental pattern in children. tional processes. A model of stress and cop-
When all subjects, regardless of group ing proposes that a family’s response to the
assignment, were compared, a trend ap- stress of a handicapped child could be in-
peared in which the self-concept of females fluenced by the family’s resources (Crnic,
was higher than that of males. When sex dif- Friedrich, & Greenberg, 1983). The siblings
ferences within groups were examined, no under study may have developed adaptive
differences were found in either group, or in coping due to the family’s social supports,
the combined group. The study also failed to individual, and group resources. A handi-
find significant effects for birth order or SES, capped sibling may also provide oppor-
although siblings who were older than their tunities for a child’s growth (Pless & Pinker-
handicapped brother or sister tended to ton, 1975) and may hence contribute
have a lower self-concept score (X =
62.3) through transactional experiences to a pos-
than the siblings who were younger (X =
itive self-concept. These opportunities may
65.0) than their handicapped brother or come with the increased responsibilities for
sister. These findings suggest that personal the care of the handicapped sibling, thus
and familial attributes such as sex, birth developing competence. Performance com-
order, and SES may not have significant ef- petence has been found to correlate with a
fects. These results, however, need to be sense of self-worth (Harter, 1986). A handi-
viewed cautiously because of the small sam- capped sibling may further provide a frame

235

Downloaded from jei.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 8, 2016


of social comparison that would elevate a handicapping conditions. Diabetes requires
child’s self-concept. Siblings provide a ready attention to a daily medical regimen and,
comparison for one’s own performance more than other handicaps, may create in-

(Tesser, 1980), and a higher self-concept convenience and stress for the entire family,
could result when a child compared himself possibly affecting the sibling’s self-concept.
with a handicapped sibling. Handicapped The effect of the type of handicap has also
children with higher self-esteem were found been found in other studies (Holroyd &
to compare themselves with those whose McArthur, 1976; Lavigne & Ryan, 1979). It is
achievement was like their own, rather than quite clear that the influence of the type of
with normally achieving peers (Harter, handicap on self-concept and personal ad-
1986). It is perhaps no coincidence that non- justment needs further investigation in well-
gifted siblings of gifted children, who repre- designed studies.
sent the opposite situation, were reported to The self-concept of siblings of handi-
be less well adjusted than comparison capped children may also be influenced by
children (Cornell, 1984). Cornell hypothe- psychological factors. Parents’ attitudes and
sized that low self-concept may result from a perceptions have been related to siblings’
child’s unfavorable comparison with a more acceptance of the handicapped child (Gra-
able sibling. liker, Fishler, ~ Koch, 1962; Grossman,
The present findings suggest that the ef- 1972) and self-concept (Dyson & Edgar,
fect of handicapped children on their sib. 1986). Studies need to take into account
~

lings’ self-concept are individual. A child’s psychological resources that may mediate
self-concept, therefore, may not be affected the self-concept of children with a handi-
by the mere presence or absence of a hand- capped sibling.
icapped sibling. Other factors may exist that This study is limited by a small sample
would affect a child’s self-concept. size, a limited age range, and a lack of con-
Research has not provided decisive infor- trol of birth order and family structure (only
mation on the possible contribution of hand- one single-parent family was represented in
icap type (i.e., autism, mental retardation, the handicapped group). Inconsistent find.
chronic illness, etc.) to siblings’ self-concept. ings on the effect of birth order suggest that
Though this study failed to find statistically the effect of this variable may be negligible.
significant differences at preset alpha levels Nonetheless, an expanded study is sug-
in self-concept among siblings whose gested with improved control on such at-
brother or sister had specific types of hand- tributes as birth order.
icaps, the results point to a trend (p .10) =
A second concern that might be addressed
that might be further investigated in a larger in future studies is instrumentation. The
study. In the two studies by Ferrari (1984; findings of this study are based on a single
1987), conflicting results were found from self-report measure. The validity of findings
the same measurement scale, the Piers- might be increased by employing multiple
Harris Children’s Self-Concept Scale. Our measures of self-concept. Particularly help-
results are consistent with Ferrari’s earlier ful would be the addition of an observational
study (1984). The major factor distinguish- measure of sibling interactions, using proce-

ing Ferrari’s recent study (1987) from his dures similar to those employed by Stone-
earlier study (1984) is the type of handicap man, Brody, Davis, and Crapps (1987). Other
studied. In his 1987 study, Ferrari included possible measurement strategies include
only diabetic children, whereas his earlier parent, teacher, or peer ratings of sibling
study, like the present study, included other self-esteem.

236

Downloaded from jei.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 8, 2016


Despite these limitations this study pro- Dyson, L., & Edgar, E. (1986). The self-concept of
vides further evidence that living with a siblings of handicapped children-An explor-
brother or sister with special needs does not atory study. (Report No. EC182-371). Seattle,
WA: University of Washington. (ERIC Docu-
necessarily have a negative impact on how - ment Reproduction Service No. 268 767).
siblings come to view themselves. The study Dyson, L., & Fewell, R.R. (1986). Stress and adap-
indicates that siblings have very normal per- tation in parents of young handicapped and
ceptions of themselves despite the presence nonhandicapped children: A comparative
of a sibling with a disability. study. Journal of the Division for Early Child-
hood, 10, 25-35.
Epstein, J.H. (1985). Review of Piers-Harris
REFERENCES Children’s Self-Concept Scale (The Way I Feel
About Myself). In J.V. Mitchel, Jr. (Ed.), The
ninth mental measurements yearbook (pp.
Belmont, LZ., & Marolla (1973). Birth order, fam.
1167-1169). Lincoln, NE: The Buros Institute
ily size, and intelligence. Science, 182,
1096- of Mental Measurements, University of
1101. Nebraska.
Bentler, P.M. (1972). A review of the Piers-Harris Ferrari, M. (1984). Chronic illness: Psychological
Self-Concept Scale for Children. In O. Buros effects on siblings—I. Chronically ill boys.
(Ed.), Mental measurements yearbook (7th Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
ed.) (pp. 124-126). New Jersey: Gryphon 25,459.476.
Press.
Ferrari, M. (1987). The diabetic child and well sib-
Bradshaw, J., & Lawton, D. (1978). Tracing the Risks to the well child’s self-concept.
causes of stress in families with handicapped
lings:
Children’s Health Care, 15
(3), 141-147.
children. British Journal of Social Work, 8(2),
181.192. Gath, A. (1973). The school-age siblings of mon-
Breslau, N., Weitzman, M., & Messenger, K. (1981).
gol children. British Journal of Psychiatry,
123, 161-167.
Psychologic functioning of siblings of dis-
abled children. Pediatrics, 67(3),344.353. Gayton, W.F, Friedman, S.B., Tavormina, J.F, &
Tucker, F. (1977). Children with cystic fibrosis:
Cicirelli, VG. (1982). Sibling influence throughout I. Psychological test findings of patients, sib-
the lifespan. In M.E. Lamb & B. Sutton-Smith
lings and parents. Pediatrics,
59(6),888-894.
(Eds.), Sibling relationships (pp. 267.284).
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Graliker, B.V., Fishler, K., & Koch, R. (1962). Teen-
S. The antecedents
age reaction to a mentally retarded sibling.
Coopersmith, (1967). of self- American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 66,
esteem. San Francisco: N.H. Freeman.
838-843.
Cornell, D.G. (1984). Families of gifted children. Grossman, F.K. (1972). Brothers and sisters of
Ann Arbor: UMI Research Press. retarded children: An exploratory study. Syra-
Crnic, K.A., Friedrich, W.N., & Greenberg, M.T. cuse, NY: Syracuse University Press.
(1983). Adaptation of families with mentally Harter, S. (1986). Processes underlying the con.
retarded children: A model of stress, coping,
struction, maintenance, and enhancement of
and family ecology. American Journal of the self-concept in children. In J. Suls & A.G.
Mental Deficiency, 88,
125-138. Greenwald (Eds.), Psychological perspectives
Cummings, S.T. (1976). The impact of the child’s on the (Vol
self 3, pp. 137-181). Hillsdale, NJ:
.
deficiency on the father: A study of fathers of Lawrence Erlbaum.
mentally retarded and of chronically ill chil- Harvey, D.H.P, & Greenway, A.P. (1984). The self-
dren. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
concept of physically handicapped children
46, 246-255. and their nonhandicapped siblings: An em-
Dunn, L (1983). Sibling relationships in early pirical investigation. Joumal of Child Psycho-
childhood. Child Development, 54
, 787-811. logy and Psychiatry, 25(2),273.284.

237

Downloaded from jei.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 8, 2016


Holroyd, J., & McArthur, D. (1976). Mental retarda. Piers, E.V. (1976). The Piers-Harris Children’s Self-
tion and stress on the parents: A contrast be- Concept Scale: Research Monograph No. 1.
tween Down’s syndrome and childhood Nashville: Counselor Recordings and Tests.
autism. American Journal of Mental Deficien. Pless, I.B., & Pinkerton, P. (1975). Chronic child-
cy, 80
, 431-436. hood disorder. London: Henry Kimpton
Jeske, P.J. (1985). Review of Piers-Harris Chil- Publishers.
dren’s Self-Concept Scale (The WayI Feel Powell, T.H., & P.A.
About Myself). In J.V. Mitchel, Jr. (Ed.), The
Ogle, (1985). Brothers and
sisters: A specialpart of exceptional families.
ninth mental measurements yearbook (pp. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
1169-1170). Lincoln, NE: The Buros Institute
of Mental Measurements, University of Stoneman, Z., Brody, G.H., Davis, C.H., & Crapps,
Nebraska. J.M. (1987). Mentally retarded children and
their same-sex siblings: Naturalistic in-home
Lavigne, J., & Ryan, M. (1979). Psychologic adjust- observations. American Journal of Mental Re
-
ment of siblings of children with chronic il- tardation, 92, 290-298.
lness. Pediatrics, 63, 616-626.
Sutton-Smith, B., & Rosenberg, B.G. (1970). The
Lobato, D., Barbour, L., Hall, L.J., & Miller, C.T. sibling. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
(1987). Psychosocial characteristics of pre-
school siblings of handicapped and non- Tesser, A. (1980). Self-esteem maintenance in
handicapped children. Journal of Abnormal family dynamics. Journal of Personality and
Child Psychology, 15(3),329-338. Social Psychology, 39, 77-91.

Mates, T.E. (1982, July). Which siblings of autistic Werner, E.E. (1979). How children influence
children are at greater risk for the development children: The role of peers in the socializa-
tion process. Children Today, 8(2),11-15.
of school andlor personality diffculties? Paper
presented at the National Society for Autistic Wylie, R. (1974). The self.concept (Vol. 1) (rev. ed.).
Children, Omaha, NE. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.
Meyer, D.J., Vadasy, P.F., & Fewell, R.R. (1985). Sib- Zajonc, R.B. (1976). Family configuration and in-
shops: A handbook for implementing work. telligence. Science, 192, 227-236.
shops for siblings of children with special
needs. Seattle: University of Washington
Press.
Piers, E.V., & Harris, D.E. (1969). The Piers-Harris
Children’s Self-Concept Scale (The Way I Feel
About Myself). Nashville: Counselor Record- Address corresportdence to Rebecca Fewell, Fortier Hall,
ings and Tests. Tulane University, New Orleans, LA 70118.

238

Downloaded from jei.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on May 8, 2016

You might also like