You are on page 1of 3

Factor Structure and Convergent Validity of

the Creative Achievement Questionnaire (CAQ)


Maja Balta (maja.balta@outlook.com)
MSc Psychology programme, Faculty of Philosophy, University of Banja Luka

Siniša Subotić (sinisa.subotic@pmf.unibl.org)


Faculty of Natural Sciences and Mathematics, University of Banja Luka

Milica Drobac Pavićević (milica.drobac@ff.unibl.org)


Faculty of Philosophy, University of Banja Luka

Abstract Silvia, Kaufman, & Pretz, 2009; White & Shah, 2011). The
CAQ measures creative accomplishments in 10 domains:
The Creative Achievement Questionnaire (CAQ) is a measure
of creative achievement across 10 domains of creativity. The visual arts, music, dance, architectural design, creative
goal of this study was to examine the factor and convergent writing, humor, invention, scientific discovery, theater/film,
validity of the CAQ’s Serbian translation on a general sample and culinary arts. Each domain is measured with 7 true/false
from the Republic of Srpska. A total of 300 participants items.
(M = 24.48, SD = 3.07 years; 66.3% female; 75.3% college Regarding the CAQ’s latent structure, Carson and
students or graduates) competed the CAQ, a measure of self- colleagues (2005) originally proposed the three-factor
assessed creative behaviour, and a creative personality
checklist. Both two-factor (Arts and Science) and three-factor
solution, comprised of Expressive, Performance, and
(Expressive, Performance, and Scientific) CAQ models Scientific achievement factors, but they also considered the
achieved good fit. However, even though the data supports two-factor solution, which assumes Arts and Science factors.
both two- (slightly better) and three-factor structures of the Note that some other findings, based on the Rasch analysis,
CAQ, there is an issue with the dimensions’ low reliabilities supported the two-factor structure (Wang et al., 2014).
(slightly worse in the three-factor model) and pronounced The CAQ has been shown to have low to moderate
correlations of residuals. The mean CAQ score is also much correlations with IQ (i.e., r = .14, Carson et al., 2005;
lower than the referent value. Because of this, even though the
convergent measures, i.e., correlations with self-assessed r = .34, Mar et al., 2006) and divergent thinking tests
creativity and creative personality are in the expected range (i.e., r = .21; Silvia & Kimbrel, 2010; r = .47, Carson et al.,
(albeit slightly lower for the creative personality than the 2005), and moderate correlations with Openness to
referent value) and the CAQ is often used for the non-artist Experience (i.e., r = .33, Carson et al., 2005), and broader
general (namely: student) samples, we are reluctant to fully measures of creative personality, such as the scores on the
recommend its usage on the general samples in our culture, as Creative Personality Scale (Gough, 1979) (i.e., r = .33,
the CAQ’s content might be “too hard” for our participants.
Carson et al., 2005). The CAQ also strongly correlates with
Keywords: creativity, creative achievement, creative other self-report creativity measures, e.g., with the Creative
personality, questionnaire validation Behavior Inventory, which is a measure of everyday
creativity (r = .59; Dollinger, 2003).
Introduction Given the fact that the CAQ is the most popular creativity
Creativity can be studied and measured on different ‘levels’, measure (Wang et al., 2014), which has not been formally
most commonly conceptualized as the ‘4P’s’ (Gruszka & validated to date in the Republic of Srpska’s territory, we
Tang, 2017; Rhodes, 1961), which stands for: person, wanted to test its factor structure and to establish some
product, process, and press (of the environment). In this aspects of its convergent validity, namely in relation to the
paper, we were interested in psychometric validation of the Creative Personality Scale (Gough, 1979) and self-reported
Creative Achievement Questionnaire – CAQ (Carson, creative behaviours (Batey, 2007; Batey & Furnham, 2008).
Peterson, & Higgins, 2005), which measures the Since the CAQ can be used on both general samples and
product/achievement aspect of creativity. The CAQ assumes artists, we focused our analysis on the general sample
that creativity is domain specific, i.e., creative achievement participants (Carson et al., 2005).
in one creativity domain does not necessarily imply creative
excellence in all areas (Carson et al., 2005). Method
The CAQ is the most frequently used self-reported
creativity measure (Wang, Ho, Cheng, & Cheng, 2014), Sample, procedure, and measures
utilized in various creativity-related research (e.g., Jauk, A total of 300 general sample participants from the Republic
Benedek, & Neubauer, 2014; Kaufman, 2013; Kaufman et of Srpska (M = 24.48, SD = 3.07 years; 66.3% female; 75.3%
al., 2016; Mar, DeYoung, Higgins, & Peterson, 2006; Silvia, college students or graduates) competed the Serbian
Kaufman, Reiter-Palmon, & Wigert, 2011; Kéri, 2011; translation of the CAQ (Carson et al., 2005), another measure

92
of self-assessed creativity, which targets the creative score from our sample was substantially lower than student
behaviours (i.e., Biographical Inventory of Creative sample mean scores of 14.4 reported by Carson and
Behaviours; Batey, 2007; Batey & Furnham, 2008), and the colleagues (2005): t(299) = -27.55, p < .001, d = 1.59.
Creative Personality Scale (Creative Personality Scale;
Gough, 1979).
The data was gathered anonymously, using an online
survey disseminated via the social networks.
The confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted in
the “lavaan” program (Rosseel, 2012) and other analyses in
JASP 0.9.2 (JASP Team, 2018).

Results
Both two-factor (CFI = .969, TLI = .957, RMSEA = .046
90% CI [.017, .070], SRMR = .063) and three-factor
(CFI = .969, TLI = .953, RMSEA = .047 [.019, .072], SRMR
= .061) CAQ models achieved good fit (Hooper, Coughlan,
& Mullen, 2008). Since Carson and colleagues (2005) also Figure 1: The CAQ total summary scores distributions. M =
use the total scale score, we tested a single-factor model, 5.09, SD = 5.85, Sk = 2.52, Ku = 10.16.
which also achieved the overall acceptable fit values (Hooper
et al., 2008): CFI = .921, TLI = .898, RMSEA = .065 [.046, The CAQ total score correlated with self-assessed creative
.084], SRMR = .089. Internal consistencies, however, were behavior in a moderate range: r = .40, p < .001, and with
generally low in both two- and three-factor models (two- creative personality in a lower range: r = .18, p = .001.
factor: Arts: α = .76, ω = .63, Science: α = .55, ω = .41; three-
factor: Expressive: α = .58, ω = .49; Performance: α = .71, ω
= .57 ; Scientific: α = .55, ω = .41) and only somewhat better Discussion
for a total scale score (α = .81, ω = .68).
Overall, even though the data supports both two- and three-
Table 1: The CFA factor loadings for two-factor model. factor structures of the CAQ on a general sample of
participants from the Republic of Srpska, there is an issue
Loadings (Λ) with the dimensions’ low reliabilities and pronounced
No. Domains correlations of residuals. Since two-factor model showed
Arts Science
9 Drama (theater/film) .66 marginally better fit and better (i.e., “less bad”) internal
5 Creative writing .69 consistencies, we are giving it a slight advantage over the
6 Humor .51 three-factor model. Although Carson and colleagues (2005)
2 Music .68 favored the three-factor solution, they also noted that “some
1 Visual arts .45 researchers may find it useful to employ the arts versus
3 Dance .60 science dichotomy of the two-factor solution” (p. 46). Other
7 Invention .56 researchers also provided support for the two-factor CAQ
8 Scientific discovery .50 model (Wang et al., 2014).
10 Culinary arts .62 Convergent measures, i.e., CAQ’s correlations with self-
Notes: Architectural design is not included in the two- assessed creative behaviours and creative personality are in
factor model, since it had weak loadings on Science factor the expected range, albeit slightly lower for the creative
in the original study by Carson and colleagues (2005). personality than the referent value reported by Carson and
Including Architecture in the model resulted in worsening colleagues (2005).
of the fit: CFI = .924, TLI = .899, RMSEA = .065 The CAQ is often used for the non-specialized (i.e., non-
[.046, .085], SRMR = .089. artist), namely student samples. However, the mean CAQ
scores on our sample (which is predominantly comprised of
There were between 6 and 9 substantially correlated students and college graduates) are much lower than referent
(i.e., ≥ .20) residual pairs, depending upon a model. Factors values from a similar sample used in the study by Carson and
were interrelated in a lower-to-moderate range (two-factor colleagues (2005). Because of this, and because of the
model: .31; three-factor model: .25, .21, and .27, pronounced ‘measurement error’ (i.e., low internal
respectively). Due to marginally better overall fit and consistencies) we are reluctant to fully recommend the
somewhat better internal consistencies of the two-factor CAQ’s usage on general samples in our culture, as the CAQ’s
model compared to the three-factor model, we are reporting content might simply be “too hard” for our participants.
factor loadings for the former (see Table 1).
The mean CAQ score was low, with pronounced lower
value frequencies (see Figure 1). For example, total scale
93
References Kéri, S. (2011). Solitary minds and social capital: Latent
inhibition, general intellectual functions and social
Batey, M. (2007). A psychometric investigation of everyday
network size predict creative achievements. Psychology of
creativity. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University
Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 5(3), 215-221.
College, London, UK.
Mar, R. A., DeYoung, C. G., Higgins, D. M., & Peterson, J.
Batey, M., & Furnham, A. (2008). The relationship between
B. (2006). Self‐liking and self‐competence separate self‐
measures of creativity and schizotypy. Personality and
evaluation from self‐deception: Associations with
Individual Differences, 45(8), 816-821.
personality, ability, and achievement. Journal of
Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005).
Personality, 74(4), 1047-1078.
Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the creative
Rhodes, M. (1961). An analysis of creativity. The Phi Delta
achievement questionnaire. Creativity Research Journal,
Kappan, 42(7), 305-310.
17(1), 37-50.
Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural
Dollinger, S. J. (2003). Need for uniqueness, need for
equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2),
cognition, and creativity. The Journal of Creative
1-36. Retrieved from http://goo.gl/l4kgYS
Behavior, 37(2), 99-116.
Silvia, P. J., & Kimbrel, N. A. (2010). A dimensional analysis
Gough, H. G. (1979). A creative personality scale for the
of creativity and mental illness: Do anxiety and depression
adjective check list. Journal of Personality and Social
symptoms predict creative cognition, creative
Psychology, 37(8), 1398-1405.
accomplishments, and creative self-concepts? Psychology
Gruszka, A., & Tang, M. (2017). The 4P’s creativity model
of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(1), 2-10.
and its application in different fields. In M. Tang & C. H.
Silvia, P. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Pretz, J. E. (2009). Is
Werner (Eds.), Handbook of the management of creativity
creativity domain-specific? Latent class models of creative
and innovation: Theory and practice (pp. 51-71). Toh Tuck
accomplishments and creative self-descriptions.
Link, SG: World Scientific Publishing.
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3(3),
Hooper, D., Coughlan, & J., Mullen, M. (2008). Structural
139-148.
equation modelling: Guidelines for determining model fit.
Silvia, P. J., Kaufman, J. C., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Wigert, B.
Electronic Journal of Business Research Methods, 6(1),
(2011). Cantankerous creativity: Honesty–Humility,
53-60. Retrieved from http://goo.gl/NfO8SD
Agreeableness, and the HEXACO structure of creative
JASP Team (2018). JASP (Version 0.9.2) [Computer
achievement. Personality and Individual Differences,
software]. Retrieved from https://jasp-stats.org/
51(5), 687-689.
Jauk, E., Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2014). The road
Wang, C. C., Ho, H. C., Cheng, C. L., & Cheng, Y. Y. (2014).
to creative achievement: A latent variable model of ability
Application of the Rasch Model to the measurement of
and personality predictors. European Journal of
creativity: The Creative Achievement Questionnaire.
Personality, 28(1), 95-105.
Creativity Research Journal, 26(1), 62-71.
Kaufman, S. B. (2013). Opening up openness to experience:
White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2011). Creative style and
A four‐factor model and relations to creative achievement
achievement in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity
in the arts and sciences. The Journal of Creative Behavior,
disorder. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(5),
47(4), 233-255.
673-677.
Kaufman, S. B., Quilty, L. C., Grazioplene, R. G., Hirsh, J.
B., Gray, J. R., Peterson, J. B., & DeYoung, C. G. (2016).
Openness to experience and intellect differentially predict
creative achievement in the arts and sciences. Journal of
personality, 84(2), 248-258.

94

You might also like