You are on page 1of 6

Journal of Abnormal Psychology

1968, Vol. 73, No. 4, 317-322

RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SELF-ACCEPTANCE AND


ADJUSTMENT, REPRESSION-SENSITIZATION
AND SOCIAL COMPETENCE
CAROL Z. FEDER1
Adelphi University

Ss were 40 male patients hospitalized on a psychiatric ward and defined as


maladjusted and 40 male patients hospitalized on a medical-surgical ward and
defined as adjusted. The medical and psychiatric groups were each composed
of equal subgroups of low-competence repressers, low-competence sensitizers,
high-competence repressers, and high-competence sensitizers. All Ss completed a
social desirability minimized Q sort for "real self" and for "ideal self." Results
showed that self-acceptance as expressed in a self-ideal discrepancy score is
significantly related to the defensive-style continuum of repression-sensitization,
with repressers expressing significantly smaller self-ideal discrepancies than
sensitizers. No relationship was found between self-acceptance and the dimen-
sions of adjustment or social competence.

The self-concept and the concept of self- (1954) stated that the degree of self-accept-
acceptance have been the central focus of ance an individual experiences is positively
much research in the last IS years (Wylie, related to his level of psychological adjust-
1961). Though it was the individual's present ment. Although Rogers and his associates
self-concept which received major focus in related adjustment positively and linearly to
early theoretical work (Rogers, 19S1; Snygg the degree of self-acceptance an individual
& Combs, 1949), the interest of later in- experiences, research results have often been
vestigation has often focused on the relation- contradictory. A number of studies have
ship between the individual's present self- found the postulated positive relationship be-
concept and his ideal self-concept. Rogers and tween self-acceptance and adjustment (Bills,
Dymond (1954) think that the degree of con- 1954; Calvin & Holtzman, 1953; Chase,
gruence which an individual expresses be- 1957; Cowen, Heilizer, Axelrod, & Alexander,
tween his present self-concept and his ideal 1957; Hanlon, Hofstaetter, & O'Connor,
self-concept is indicative of the degree of 1954; Martire & Hornberger, 1957; Shlien,
self-acceptance he experiences. This theoreti- Mosak, & Dreikurs, 1962; Turner & Vander-
cal position generated a good deal of interest lippe, 1958; Williams, 1962). Other studies,
in the concept of self-acceptance, and many however, have found either a negative or zero
investigators have attempted to define its correlation between self-acceptance and ad-
relationship to other variables. justment (Borislow, 1962; Kamano, 1961;
The variable which has received the most Zuckerman, Baer, & Monashkin, 1956;
consistent attention in relation to self-accept- Zuckerman & Monashkin, 1957), while still
ance is adjustment. Rogers and Dymond other studies have found a curvilinear rela-
1
This paper is based on portions of a dissertation tionship between self-acceptance and adjust-
submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements ment (Block & Thomas, 1955; Chodorkoff,
for the PhD degree at Adelphi University, 1966. The 1954; Friedman, 1955; Hillson & Worchel,
author wishes to express her gratitude to the mem- 1957). Thus, the nature of the relationship
bers of her committee, Harold Levine and Norman between self-acceptance, as expressed in a
Berk, for their encouragement and suggestions during
all phases of this research. Particular thanks are conscious self-report instrument, and adjust-
extended to the chairman of the dissertation com- ment is still not clearly delineated.
mittee, George Strieker, for his unstinting guidance Recently, in an attempt to understand self-
and support, and to Bernard Locke, chief psycholo- acceptance in a different light, self-acceptance
gist, and the staff of the Veterans Administration
Hospital, Manhattan, for their fullest cooperation has been theoretically related to two other
and support. variables: repression-sensitization (Altrocchi,
317
318 CAROL Z. FEDER

Parsons, & Dickoff, 1960) and social com- cantly larger self-ideal discrepancy scores,
petence (Achenbach & Zigler, 1963). indicating less self-acceptance, than did low-
The repression-sensitization dimension is a competence Ss. Achenbach and Zigler (1963)
bipolar categorization of defensive behaviors. feel that a large self-ideal discrepancy score,
Both repression and sensitization refer to
. . . is concomitant with the demonstrated capacity
groups of defensive behaviors which are un- to achieve in areas most valued in our society.
consciously determined and used by the indi- Rather than being ominous in nature, high self-
vidual in an attempt to cope with anxiety image disparity would invariably appear to accom-
and threat-laden situations. At the repression pany the attainment of higher levels of development,
end of the continuum, since the greater cognitive differentiation found at
such levels must invariably lead to a greater capacity
. . . are those responses which involve avoidance for self-derogation, guilt, and anxiety . . . . [Within
of the anxiety-arousing stimulus and its consequents. a developmental framework] the degree of self-image
Included here are repression, denial, and many types disparity would be expected to be low at low levels
of rationalization. At the sensitizing extreme of the of maturity [low-competence individuals] and high
continuum are behaviors which involve an attempt at high levels of maturity [high-competence indi-
to reduce anxiety by approaching or controlling viduals] [p. 204].
the stimulus and its consequents. The latter mecha-
nisms include intellectualization, obsessive-compulsive Achenbach and Zigler (1963) cited Al-
behaviors, and ruminative worrying [Byrne, 1964, trocchi et al.'s (1960) results relating self-
p. 169]. acceptance to a repression-sensitization con-
tinuum as confirmatory of their results by
Altrocchi et al. (1960) initially related self- assuming that repressors are at a lower de-
acceptance to the repression-sensitization di- velopmental level than sensitizers and would
mension. They found that repressers had be classified as low-competence individuals,
much smaller self-ideal discrepancies than while sensitizers would be classified as high-
sensitizers. Repressers thus appeared more competence individuals. Thus, these research-
self-accepting than sensitizers. Byrne (1961) ers have given a third interpretation to self-
also related the repression-sensitization di- ideal discrepancy scores.
mension to a self-report measure of self- The purpose of this present study was to
acceptance. In two independent investiga- investigate the joint relationships of these
tions, repression-sensitization correlated .62 three variables—adjustment - maladjustment,
(p<,.Ql) and .55 (p < .01) with self-ideal repression-sensitization, and social competence
discrepancy scores. The results with both —to self-acceptance as expressed through
samples indicated that sensitizers have signifi- self-ideal discrepancy scores. It was hoped
cantly larger self-ideal discrepancy scores that this would help clarify the meaning of
than repressors. an individual's self-acceptance, as expressed
The results of both the Altrocchi et al. in a conscious self-report measure.
study and the Byrne studies suggest that
congruence between self-concept and ideal METHOD
self-concept may be related to a defensive- Subjects
style continuum as well as to, or instead of,
The Ss consisted of 80 hospitalized male patients
an adjustment-maladjustment dimension. at the Veterans Administration Hospital, Manhattan,
Achenbach and Zigler (1963) related self- New York. Of this number, 40 were hospitalized on
acceptance to a social competence dimension. either the medical or surgical wards. Only those medi-
In theory, an individual's level of social com- cal and surgical patients were used who had no known
petence is a broad approximation of the previous history of psychiatric hospitalization, no
terminal illness, no illness which was deemed by the
developmental level of personal and social attending physician to have a major psychiatric
maturity he has attained. Achenbach and component, and no signs of organicity. All medical
Zigler used psychiatric and nonpsychiatric and surgical patients were also ambulatory to the
patients with each group containing equal extent that they could leave their bed and ward
for a few hours, even though they might be in a
numbers of high- and low-competence Ss. Re- wheelchair.
sults showed that high-comptence 5s (both Only those psychiatric patients were used who
psychiatric and nonpsychiatric) had signifi- had no signs of organicity, were judged to be
SELF-ACCEPTANCE AND REPEESSION-SENSITIZATION 319
not actively hallucinating, and had not undergone Social competence. Each S was classified as to the
electric-shock treatment in the 6-wk. prior to being level of his social competence on the basis of mean
tested. score attained on an adaptation of the Achenbach
The medical and psychiatric groups were each and Zigler (1963) Social Competence Index.
composed of equal subgroups of low-competence re- Each individual's social competence was rated on
pressers, low-competence sensitizers, high-competence the following variables:
sensitizers, and high-competence repressers. In order
to obtain 40 medical and 40 psychiatric patients with 1. Intelligence—A total score on the Shipley-
this combination of characteristics, it was necessary Hartford scale equivalent to a Wechsler-Bellevue IQ
to test 104 medical patients and 103 psychiatric of 89 or below (0), 90-109 (1), 110 or above (2).
patients. 2. Education—None or some grades including un-
graded or special classes (0), finished grade school,
Test Instruments some high school, or finished high school (1), some
Self-acceptance. A Q sort developed by Cowen, college or more (2).
Budin, Wolitzky, and Stiller (1960) was used. It 3. Occupation—The Dictionary of Occupational
consists of 44 adjectives chosen from a relatively neu- Titles (United States Government, Department of
tral range of the social desirability continuum. The Labor, 1949) was used to place each occupation into
adjectives were arranged in alphabetical order below the categories of unskilled or semiskilled (0), skilled
an 11-point rectangular Q sort, following the sug- and service (1), clerical sales, professional, or mana-
gestion of Livson and Nichols (1956). The adjectives gerial (2). If an individual had many occupations,
were presented to each 5 twice, first to be sorted he was rated either on the occupation in which he
according to the way S saw himself at present (self- was presently employed or on the one in which he
concept), and then to be sorted according to the worked most frequently.
way he would ideally like to be (ideal self). The 4. Employment history—Usually unemployed (0),
discrepancy between the placements of each adjec- seasonal, fluctuating, frequent shifts, or parttime em-
tive on the two sorts was calculated. An S's total ployment (1), regularly employed (2).
discrepancy score was the sum of each of the 44 5. Marital status—Single (0), separated, divorced,
discrepancy scores without regard to the direction remarried, or widowed (1), single continuous mar-
of change, with a high score indicating a low degree riage (2).
of self-acceptance.
Adjustment-maladjustment. For the purposes of Information pertaining to these variables was ob-
this study, the patients who resided on the medical tained from each S on a history sheet distributed at
or surgical wards were classified as "adjusted," and the beginning of the testing session.
the patients who resided on the psychiatric wards The Ss were designated as low competence if their
were classified as "maladjusted." After the elimina- mean competence score was between .4 and 1.0. The
tion of surgical or medical patients with possible 5s were designated as high competence if their mean
psychiatric involvement as potential Ss, it was felt competence score fell between 1.4 and 2.0.
that mere presence on the two different types of
wards—medical-surgical versus psychiatric—provided Procedure
prima facie evidence of the patients' adjustment
status.2 Medical and psychiatric patients were tested sepa-
Repression-sensitization. The Ss were classified as rately. The patients were tested in groups, usually
repressers or sensitizers on the basis of the rating comprising not less than three nor more than six
they obtained on the MMPI after it had been scored patients. All of the patients took the following tests,
according to Byrne's (1961) scoring system. In order some for purposes of another study:
to obtain a cutoff point for repressers and sensitizers,
the repression-sensitization scores of all 161 psychi- 1. A questionnaire designed to elicit information
atric and medical patients who had been rated as regarding social competence.
either high or low in social competence were placed 2. The Shipley-Hartford scale.
in a frequency distribution. Cutoff points were estab- 3. The Cornell Index.
lished which classified 40% of the Ss as repressers 4. A Q sort of 44 trait-descriptive adjectives to be
and 40% as sensitizers. In order to have 10 Ss in sorted first for real self and then for ideal self.
each group for the statistical analysis, 1 high-com- 5. The 15-item Couch and Keniston Acquiescent
petence medical S and 6 low-competence psychiatric Response Set Scale, which was labeled Personal
Ss were taken from the middle range of the Reaction Inventory I.
repression-sensitization continuum. 6. The Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability scale,
2
which was labeled Personal Reaction Inventory II.
Scores on the Cornell Index, Form N«, clearly 7. The MMPI.
differentiated the psychiatric and medical-surgical
groups. The mean score obtained by psychiatric pa- The questionnaire was always given first, and the
tients on the Cornell Index was 25.5, while the mean MMPI was always given last. The order of the
score obtained by medical-surgical patients was 14.9. other tests was randomized. Testing was usually
This difference is statistically significant (F — 25,50, split into two sessions, with the MMPI being given
d/=l/72). in the second session.
320 CAROL Z. FEDKE

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION individuals who use avoidance, denial, and


Differences in self-ideal discrepancy scores other repressive modes in the face of threat
among the eight experimental subgroups were or anxiety-evoking stimuli. Thus, the small
tested by means of a three-dimensional analy- self-ideal discrepancy scores obtained by re-
sis of variance, the results of which are pre- pressers can best be viewed as a reflection of
sented in Table 1. The hypothesis that there their defensive style, of another instance
would be a relationship between an adjustment- where any feelings of self-discontent which
maladjustment dimension and self-acceptance might be present are not available to con-
was not supported. The hypothesis that there scious awareness. On the other hand, sensi-
would be a relationship between the social tizers, who express large self-ideal discrepan-
competence dimension and self-acceptance was cies, are those individuals who deal with
also not supported. However, the hypothesis anxiety-evoking situations by intellectualiza-
that there would be a relationship between tion, obsessive-compulsive behaviors, and
the repression-sensitization dimension and ruminative worrying. The larger self-ideal
self-acceptance was supported (p < .05). Re- discrepancy scores obtained by sensitizers can
sults showed that repressers have significantly thus also be viewed as a reflection of their
smaller self-ideal discrepancy scores than defensive style, as they attempt to master
sensitizers and can therefore be considered anxiety aroused by discontent with their pres-
more self-accepting. There were no significant ent self-concept with an open admission of
interactions. this discontent.
These findings strongly support the theo- The hypothesis that self-acceptance as ex-
retical position of Altrocchi et al. (1960) and pressed in a conscious self-report instrument
Byrne (1961, 1964) who posit that self- is related to an adjustment-maladjustment di-
acceptance as expressed in a conscious self- mension was not supported. In view of the
report instrument and operationally defined relationship obtained in this study between
by a self-ideal discrepancy score is related to self-acceptance and the defensive-style con-
a repression-sensitization defensive-style con- tinuum of repression-sensitization, it is possi-
tinuum. The results clearly support the posi- ble that some of the positive relationships
tion that repressers express significantly previously obtained between self-acceptance
smaller self-ideal discrepancies on a conscious and adjustment were really reflections of a
self-report instrument than sensitizers. Under- defensive dimension. This hypothesis is sup-
ported by Zuckerman and Monashkin (19S7)
lying this difference in consciously expressed
and Block and Thomas (19SS) who found
self-acceptance is an important difference in
the mode of defensive adaptation. Repressers, that Ss who scored high on measures of self-
who express small self-ideal discrepancies, and acceptance also obtained high scores on the K
scale of the MMPI, often considered a mea-
are thereby felt to be more self-accepting, at
least at the conscious level, are also those sure of defensiveness. It is also possible that
some of the high correlations obtained be-
tween self-acceptance and adjustment at the
TABLE 1 termination of a course of psychotherapy are
ANALYSIS or VARIANCE as SELF-IDEAL not reflections of an adjustive dimension, but
DISCREPANCY MEAN SCORES
rather of an increased use of repressive de-
Source df ' 55 MS F fenses at this time, as the need is felt to deny
any feelings of dissatisfaction and anxiety
Competence (A) 1 .88 .88 1.39 which may exist so as to "look good," both for
Repression-Sensitization (B) 1 3.70 3.70 5.87*
Psychiatric-Medical (C) 1 .01 .01 .01 oneself and for one's therapist. However,
AXB 1 .95 .95 1.50 though no relationship was found in this study
AXC 1 .11 .11 .17
BXC 1 .61 .61 .96 between self-acceptance and adjustment, it is
AXBX C 1 .04 .04 .06 possible that this was due to the forced divi-
Within cells 72 45.82 .63 sion of the psychiatric and nonpsychiatric
Total 79 52.12
groups into equal numbers of repressors and
* P < .05. sensitizers for purposes of the statistical
SELF-ACCEPTANCE AND REPEESSION-SENSITIZATION 321

analysis. When this division is not forced, it TABLE 2


has been found that psychiatric or more RELATIONSHIP or SOCIAL COMPETENCE AND
poorly adjusted populations tend to contain a REPRESSION-SENSITIZATION STATUS
preponderance of sensitizers, whereas non-
High Low
psychiatric or better-adjusted populations competence competence
tend to contain a preponderance of repressors Repressors 42 (41)" 22 (23) 64
(Byrne, Golightly, & Sheffield, 196S; Feder, Sensitizers 40 (41) 24 (23) 64
1966; Ullmann, 1962). In view of this fact, it Total 82 46 128
seems wisest to view an individual's conscious
Note.—x» = .134, df = 1, p > .05.
level of self-acceptance as a reflection of both ' Expected frequencies appear in parentheses.
his defensive style and of his adjustment
status. tives minimized for social desirability in a
The hypothesis which evolved from Achen- (?-sort format, may be getting at quite a
bach and Zigler's (1963) study, that self-
acceptance would be related to a social compe- different aspect of a self-acceptance dimen-
tence dimension, was also not supported. The sion than other instruments. However, to the
question can be raised as to whether the fail- degree that the perceivability of item social
ure to find a relationship between the social desirability has been reduced by this instru-
competence dimension and self-acceptance ment, the results of this study were less con-
may be due to the unreliability of the method founded by the social desirability variable. In
of classification along the social competence light of this, it is particularly impressive that
dimension. However, since classification along the repression-sensitization dimension, which
this dimension was based primarily on ob- is significantly correlated with social desira-
jective behavioral indexes (e.g., education, bility (Byrne, 1964; Siber & Grebstein, 1964),
employment history), it is unlikely that this still provided the only significant source of
explanation would account for the lack of variance among the scores.
relationship found between these two varia-
bles. The results of this study also argue REFERENCES
against the assumption made by Achenbach ACHENBACH, T., & ZIGLER, E. Social competence and
and Zigler (1963) that the repressors and self-image disparity in psychiatric and nonpsychi-
sensitizers described by Altrocchi etal. (1960) atric patients. Journal of Abnormal and Social
correspond, respectively, to low- and high- Psychology, 1963, 67, 197-205.
competence individuals, with the sensitizers AITROCCHI, J., PARSONS, 0. A., & DICKOFF, H.
Changes in self and ideal discrepancy in repressors
being at a higher, and repressors at a lower, and sensitizers. Journal of Abnormal and Social
developmental level and, therefore, that the Psychology, 1960, 61, 67-72.
results of the Altrocchi et al. (1960) study BILLS, R. E. Self-concept and Rorschach signs of de-
somehow confirm their own. A chi-square test pression. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1954,
was performed to investigate the relationship 18, 135-137.
between social competence and the repression- BLOCK, J., & THOMAS, H. Is satisfaction with self a
measure of adjustment? Journal of Abnormal and
sensitization dimension. As can be seen in Social Psychology, 1955, 51, 254-259.
Table 2, no consistent relationship was found BORISIOW, B. Self-evaluation and academic achieve-
(x»=1.34,#>.05). ment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1962, 9,
The foregoing, then, are the results ob- 246-253.
tained with this particular self-acceptance in- BYRNE, D. The Repression-Sensitization scale: Ra-
tionale, reliability, and validity. Journal of Person-
strument. A main difficulty in comparing ality, 1961, 29, 334-349.
these results with those obtained in other BYRNE, D. Repression-sensitization as a dimension of
studies is the fact that different self-accept- personality. In B. A. Maher (Ed.), Progress in
ance instruments have been used and may not experimental personality research. New York: Aca-
be comparable (Crowne & Stephans, 1961; demic Press, 1964.
BYRNE, D., GOLIOHTLY, C., & SHEFFIELD, J. The Re-
Wylie, 1961). The self-acceptance measure pression-Sensitization scale as a measure of ad-
employed in this study, which used the justment: Relationship with the CPI. Journal of
Cowen et al. (1960) trait-descriptive adjec- Consulting Psychology, 1965, 29, 586-589.
322 CAROL Z. FEDEE

CALVIN, A. D., & HOLTZMAN, W. H. Adjustment and MARTIRE, J. G., & HORNBERGER, R. H. Self-congru-
the discrepancy between self-concept and inferred ence, by sex and between the sexes, in a "normal"
self. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1953, 17, population. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1957,
39-43. 13, 188-291.
CHASE, P. H. Self concepts in adjusted and malad- ROGERS, C. R. Client-centered therapy. Boston:
justed hospital patients. Journal of Consulting Houghton Mifflin, 1951.
Psychology, 1957, 21, 495-497. ROGERS, C. R., & DYMOND, R. Psychotherapy and
CHODORKOIT, B. Adjustment and the discrepancy be- Personality change. Chicago: University of Chi-
tween the perceived and ideal self. Journal of cago Press, 1954.
Clinical Psychology, 1954, 10, 266-268. SHLIEN, J. M., MOSAK, H. H., & DREIKURS, R. Effect
COWEN, E., BUDIN, W., WOLITZKY, D. L., & STILLER, of tune limits in a comparison of two psycho-
A. The social desirability of trait descriptive terms: therapies. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1962,
A factor in the prediction of Q sort. Journal of 9, 31-34.
Personality, 1960, 28, 530-544. SILBER, L. D., & GREBSTEIN, L. C. Repression-sensi-
COWEN, E. L., HEIMZER, F., AXELROD, H. S., & ALEX- tization and social desirability responding. Journal
ANDER, S. The correlates of manifest anxiety in of Consulting Psychology, 1964, 28, 559.
perceptual reactivity, rigidity, and self-concept. SNYGG, D., & COMBS, A. Individual behavior. New
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1957, 21, 405- York: Harper, 1949.
411. TURNER, R. H., & VANDERLIPPE, R. H. Self-ideal
CROWNE, D. P., & STEPHANS, M. W. Self-acceptance congruence as an index of adjustment. Journal of
and self-evaluating behavior: A critique of meth- Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1958, 57, 202-
odology. Psychological Bulletin, 1961, 58, 104-121. 206.
FEDER, C. Z. The relationship between self-acceptance ULLMANN, L. P. An empirically derived MMPI scale
and adjustment, repression-sensitization, and so- which measures facilitation-inhibition of recogni-
cial competence in hospitalized male veterans. tion of threatening stimuli. Journal of Clinical
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Adelphi Uni- Psychology, 1962, 18, 127-132.
versity, 1966. UNITED STATES GOVERNMENT, DEPARTMENT OF LABOR.
FRIEDMAN, I. Phenomenal, ideal and projected con- The dictionary of occupational titles. Vol. 2. (2nd
ceptions of self. Journal of Abnormal and Social ed.) Washington, D. C.: United States Govern-
Psychology, 1955, 51, 611-615. ment Printing Office, 1949.
HANLON, T. E., HOESTAETTER, P. R., & O'CONNER, J. WILLIAMS, J. E. Changes in self and other percep-
P. Congruence of self and ideal self in relation to tions following brief educational-vocational coun-
personality adjustment. Journal of Consulting Psy- seling. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 1962, 9,
chology, 1954, 18, 215-217. 18-30.
HmsoN, J. S., & WORCHEL, P. Self-concept and de- WYLIE, R. C. The self-concept. Lincoln: University
fensive behavior in the maladjusted. Journal of of Nebraska Press, 1961.
ZUCKERMAN, M., BAER, M., & MONASHKIN, I. Ac-
Consulting Psychology, 1957, 21, 83-88.
ceptance of self, parents, and people in patients
KAMANO, D. K. Self-satisfaction and psychological and normals. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 1956,
adjustment in schizophrenics. Journal of Consult- 12, 327-332.
ing Psychology, 1961, 25, 492-496.
ZUCKERMAN, M., & MONASHKDJ, I. Self-acceptance
LTVSON, N. H., & NICHOLS, T. F. Discrimination and and psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psy-
reliability in Q-sort personality descriptions. Jour- chology, 1957, 21, 145-148.
nal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1956, 52,
159-165. (Received November 18, 1966)

You might also like