You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions

Teaching Daily Living Skills to Seven 13(3) 144­–153


© 2011 Hammill Institute on Disabilities
Reprints and permission: http://www.
Individuals With Severe Intellectual sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1098300710366593

Disabilities: A Comparison of Video http://jpbi.sagepub.com

Prompting to Video Modeling

Helen I. Cannella-Malone1, Courtney Fleming1,


Yi-Cheih Chung1, Geoffrey M. Wheeler1,
Abby R. Basbagill1, and Angella H. Singh1

Abstract
We conducted a systematic replication of Cannella-Malone et al. by comparing the effects of video prompting to video
modeling for teaching seven students with severe disabilities to do laundry and wash dishes. The video prompting and
video modeling procedures were counterbalanced across tasks and participants and compared in an alternating treatments
design within a multiple probe across participants design. For six participants, video prompting was more effective than
video modeling, which was generally ineffective. For one participant, neither video modeling nor video prompting was
effective, but in vivo instruction led to skill acquisition. One participant who was deaf was also able to learn both skills
using video prompting, even though he could not hear the voice-over instructions. These data suggest that the duration
of the video may influence its effectiveness as a teaching tool and that the voice-over instructions may not be necessary.

Keywords
video prompting, video modeling, developmental disability, comparison

Many individuals with severe intellectual disabilities expe- Being able to independently complete daily living tasks would
rience deficits in daily living skills (e.g., Jacobson & assist individuals with severe intellectual disabilities in attain-
Ackerman, 1990; Kraijer, 2000), which may have negative ing that goal. Using video modeling and video prompting to
affects on their overall quality of life (Parmenter, 1994) teach daily living skills may lead to greater acquisition and
and may limit independent functioning in their natural maintenance of these skills and, therefore, enhance overall
environments. In fact, individuals with severe intellectual quality of life and community membership (Parmenter, 1994).
disabilities may be unable to acquire desired living and As described by Cannella-Malone and her colleagues (2006),
working opportunities until they have the ability to indepen- two general procedures that can be applied when using video
dently complete various daily living skills (Heller, Bigge, & technology include video modeling and video prompting.
Allgood, 2005). For example, when individuals with severe Video modeling involves making a video of someone perform-
intellectual disabilities are unable to care for their living ing the entire target behavior. The video is then shown in its
environments (e.g., do laundry, maintain a clean living entirety to the individual at the beginning of each training ses-
environment), it limits their self-determination and may sion. After viewing the entire video, the individual is given the
also limit their choices for living environments as adults opportunity to perform the behavior in its entirety. In contrast,
(e.g., individual supported living vs. group home). This video prompting involves showing an individual a video clip
reliance on caregivers to assist (and often complete) daily
living activities will likely reduce the overall level of par-
ticipation of individuals with severe intellectual disabilities 1
The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA
in these basic skills, which may also increase passivity
and lead to learned helplessness (Cannella, O’Reilly, & Corresponding Author:
Lancioni, 2005). Helen I. Cannella-Malone, A348 PAES Building, 305 W 17th Avenue,
Rainforth and York-Barr (1997) stated that “a desirable Columbus, OH 43235, USA
future is one in which a person with disabilities is a participat- Email: malone.175@osu.edu
ing member of a family and integrated community” (p. 97). Action Editor:  William Frea

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at HOWARD UNIV UNDERGRAD LIBRARY on February 27, 2015
Cannella-Malone et al. 145

of one step of a task and then giving the person the opportunity moderate mental retardation attending day programming.
to complete that step before the next step is shown. The effects of video modeling and video prompting should
In the past decade, numerous studies have examined the be examined with school-age participants who have more
effectiveness of using video modeling and video prompting severe disabilities. Finally, the previous study included par-
to teach new daily living skills to individuals with develop- ticipants whose hearing acuity was within normal ranges,
mental disabilities, including using an ATM (e.g., Cihak, so they were able to hear the voice-over instructions. The
Alberto, Taber-Doughty, & Gama, 2006), house cleaning effects of video modeling and video prompting should be
(e.g., Van Laarhoven & Van Laarhoven-Meyers, 2006), examined with participants who are deaf or hard of hearing
and food preparation (e.g., Graves, Collins, Schuster, & to determine if this technology is effective in teaching this
Kleinert, 2005). For example, Bidwell and Rehfeldt (2004) population of individuals. The current study sought to
successfully used video modeling to teach three adult explore these limitations. First, seven school-age students
women with severe mental retardation to make coffee, with with severe intellectual disabilities participated. Second, all
an embedded social skill of serving coffee to—and sitting videos (i.e., both video modeling and video prompting)
down beside—a peer. In this study, a multiple baseline were presented from the perspective of the participant.
across participants design was used to demonstrate that Finally, one of the participants was deaf.
video modeling led to the acquisition of the new skills,
which were maintained at a 1-month follow-up. In another
example, Sigafoos et al. (2007) used video prompting with Method
error correction to teach four individuals with autism and Participants
moderate mental retardation to set the table. With video
prompting alone, one participant immediately mastered and Seven individuals (five boys, two girls) with severe intel-
maintained the skill. The other three participants success- lectual disabilities participated. All seven participants
fully mastered the task when video prompting was paired attended the same classroom in an urban school designated
with an error correction procedure. specifically for individuals with severe to profound disabili-
Although the research on video modeling and video ties. The participants were selected for this study because of
prompting indicates that using it to teach new skills can their deficits in daily living skills, as confirmed by informal
lead to positive outcomes, one area that needs to be explored classroom observations and scores on the Vineland Adap-
further is whether video modeling or video prompting is tive Behavior Scales (Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984).
more effective and/or efficient at teaching new skills to They were also considered good candidates for learning
individuals with severe intellectual disabilities. To date, from video modeling or video prompting because their
only one study was identified that compared video model- vision was within normal range. None of the participants
ing to video prompting (Cannella-Malone et al., 2006). In had received any previous video-based instruction. Although
this study, the researchers taught six individuals with autism no formal imitation assessment was conducted, informal
and moderate mental retardation to set the table and put classroom observations confirmed that each participant
away groceries. Using a combined alternating treatment was able to imitate. Table 1 provides demographic infor-
and multiple probe across pairs of participants design, video mation for each participant, including name, race, age,
modeling and video prompting were counterbalanced diagnoses, and Vineland age equivalents for the social, com-
across tasks and participants. The results showed that video munication, and daily living skills domains, as well as their
prompting was more effective (and efficient) than video composite age equivalence.
modeling for all six participants whereas video modeling
had little to no effect.
Although the aforementioned study provides an initial Setting
comparison of video modeling to video prompting, there This study was conducted at a school for individuals with
were several limitations that should be addressed. First, this severe to profound intellectual and physical disabilities
study only provided six data sets, so the results should be between the ages of 5 and 21. Sessions for Laundry were
replicated with additional participants and tasks. Second, conducted in one of the school laundry rooms. The laundry
video modeling was shown from the perspective of a spec- room was equipped with one front-opening washer and one
tator (i.e., the video showed a person completing the task), dryer, shelving units that held folded laundry, and baskets.
but video prompting was shown from the perspective of the Sessions for Washing Dishes were either conducted at the
participant (i.e., the video showed only the arms and hands sink in the participants’ classroom or in the home economics
completing the task). It is possible that video modeling room. The sink areas had one sink and an adjacent counter.
might be more effective if the videos were presented from Training was conducted individually with each participant
the perspective of the participant rather than that of a spec- to avoid incidental modeling effects, though other students
tator. Third, the participants were all adults with autism and were in the classroom during sessions for Washing Dishes.

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at HOWARD UNIV UNDERGRAD LIBRARY on February 27, 2015
146 Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions 13(3)

Table 1. Participant Demographics

Vineland Age Equivalents (Years:Months) Task or Procedure

Name Ethnicity Age Diagnosis Social Communication Daily Composite Prompt Model

Keith African 12 Autism moderate to severe MR 1:5   1:11 2:6   1:11 Laundry Dishes
American seizure disorder
Tina Caucasian 13 Autism 1:0 1:0 1:9 1:3 Dishes Laundry
   moderate to severe MR
   seizure disorder
   fragile X syndrome
Jon African 12 Autism 1:5 1:6 2:4 1:9 Dishes Laundry
American    moderate to severe MR
Carrie African 11 Autism 1:0 1:0 2:4 1:5 Laundry Dishes
American    moderate to severe MR
Andrew Somalian 12 Autism 2:4   2:10 3:1 2:9 Laundry Dishes
   moderate to severe MR
Adam Biracial 13 Moderate to severe MR 3:7 2:7 4:2 3:5 Dishes Laundry
   seizure disorder
Michael Caucasian 13 Autism 1:3 1:0   2:10 1:5 Laundry Dishes
   moderate to severe MR
   severe bilateral hearing loss

Note: MR = mental retardation.

Tasks and Materials Table 2. Task Analyses

Intervention focused on teaching the participants to com- Washing Dishes Laundry


plete two daily living tasks: (a) starting a load of laundry Pick up the sponge Pick up basket
(i.e., Laundry) and (b) hand-washing a plate, spoon, and cup Turn on the water Walk to laundry room
(i.e., Washing Dishes). These tasks were identified by school Wet the sponge Put basket down on table
staff as important for each participant to learn, because being Turn off the water Open washing machine door
able to complete these skills could lead to greater indepen- Pick up the soap bottle Step onto platform
Put a dime size amount Put all clothes into washing machine
dence in the future. The participants’ teacher reported that of soap on the sponge
they had not received any instruction on either task. Put the soap bottle down Step off of the platform
The materials for Laundry included a laundry basket, Pick up the plate, wash it, Close washing machine door
soiled towels, liquid detergent, and the washing machine. and put it in the sink
The materials for Washing Dishes consisted of a plate, a Pick up the cup, wash it, Open soap drawer
spoon, a cup, one sponge, a bottle of dish soap, a dish drain, and put it in the sink
and a dish towel. The task analyses (see Table 2) were devel- Squeeze out the sponge Take cap off of soap bottle
oped on site with the classroom teacher, using the materials Place sponge in holder Fill cap half full with soap
Turn on water Pour soap into soap drawer
needed to complete each task. The two tasks were compa-
Rinse the plate Put cap back onto soap bottle
rable in terms of difficulty and number of steps. Place the plate in drainer Close soap drawer
Each task was filmed from the performer’s perspective. Rinse the cup Press start on the washing machine
That is, when participants viewed the videos, they saw two Place the cup in drainer Pick up basket
arms and hands completing the task, not the perspective of Turn off the water Walk to classroom
a spectator watching someone else complete the task. All Dry hands Put down basket
videos were filmed with a Cannon ZE 850 and shown on a
Mac 13″ Notebook using commercially available software
(i.e., Quicktime). Washing Dishes, each video clip lasted from 2 to 15 s, with
Video prompting. For the video prompting version of each an average duration of 5.1 s. In addition to demonstrating
task, 18 separate video clips were filmed. Each clip showed the actions required for completing the step, each video clip
only one step of the task. For Laundry, each video clip included a one-sentence voice-over instruction. For exam-
lasted from 3 to 16 s, with an average duration of 7.1 s. For ple, the video clip for Step 1 of Laundry consisted of an

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at HOWARD UNIV UNDERGRAD LIBRARY on February 27, 2015
Cannella-Malone et al. 147

over-the-shoulder shot of the performer’s two hands picking Tina, when neither the video prompting nor video modeling
up a full laundry basket. While doing this, the performer— were found to be effective, a final in vivo phase was imple-
who could be heard but not seen—said, “First, pick up the mented in which both tasks received live instruction.
basket.”
Video modeling. For the video modeling versions of each
task, a single video was made showing all 18 steps of the two Procedures
respective tasks being completed from beginning to end. The Baseline. During baseline, participants were individually
durations of the two videos were 2 min 18 s (Laundry) and brought to the laundry basket in the classroom for Laundry
1 min 55 s (Washing Dishes). The video models used the or to the sink for Washing Dishes. The participant was
same videos as the video prompts, but they were shown in placed in front of the laundry basket (or sink) and told to
sequence as one video. In other words, the participant saw “Do the laundry” (or “Wash the dishes”). During the ses-
the performer’s hands completing each step of the task and sion, the trainer and a reliability observer (when available)
heard the one sentence voice-over instruction as each step recorded the number of steps that the participant completed
was completed. correctly. If the participant did not initiate the first step of
the task within 30 s or complete subsequent steps with 30 s
of a previous step, the session was terminated, and the
Dependent Measure and Data Collection participant was given noncontingent access to a choice of
Using the task analyses shown in Table 2 as data sheets, we snacks.
recorded whether each step of the task was completed Video prompt versus video model. During this phase, par-
correctly or not on a session-by-session basis. We then cal- ticipants were brought to the laundry basket or sink in the
culated percentage correct by dividing the number of steps classroom and provided with training using either video
completed correctly by 18 (i.e., the number of steps in each prompting or video modeling (see Table 1). The computer
task). To be scored as correct during baseline, the first step was placed in a location where it could be easily operated
of the task had to be completed within 30 s of the initial by the trainer and viewed by the participant. For Laundry,
instruction (e.g., “Do the dishes” or “Do the laundry”). All the trainer held the computer for the first two steps, then
subsequent steps had to be completed within 30 s of the placed it on top of the dryer. For Washing Dishes, the com-
completion of the previous step. During intervention with puter was placed on the counter next to the sink.
video prompting, only steps completed within 30 s after During training sessions that involved video prompting,
viewing the video clip for that step were scored as correct. the participant was oriented toward the computer screen
During intervention with video modeling, the first step had and the trainer said, “Watch this.” The trainer then showed
to be initiated within 30 s of the end of the video, and all the video clip of the first step of the task. When this video
subsequent steps had to be completed within 2 min 30 s of clip ended, the trainer said, “Now you do it.” The partici-
the initial step to be scored as correct. pant was then given 30 s to complete the step. If the
Data were collected during one-to-one training sessions participant failed to correctly complete the step within 30 s,
that occurred two to three times per week. Sessions lasted the trainer completed—or corrected—the step as unobtru-
approximately 10 to 15 min, with one exception. When error sively as possible and proceeded to show the next clip in the
correction was implemented with Tina, she had one session task analysis using the same procedures as for the first step.
that lasted 45 min because the error correction procedure This was done because each video clip showed only one
had to be used with each step of the task analysis. After each step of the task and participants were only expected to
session, the participants were given a choice of one noncon- complete one step at a time. We therefore had to complete
tingent snack item for participating. steps that the participants failed to do to ensure that the
progression of the task would remain in sync with the order
of video prompts. No additional instructions, feedback, or
Experimental Design prompts were delivered.
The study combined a multiple probe across participants During training sessions that involved video modeling,
design (Horner & Baer, 1978) with an alternating treatments the participant was oriented toward the computer screen
design (Barlow & Hersen, 1984). As an added control, we and the trainer said, “Watch this.” The trainer then showed
counterbalanced the use of video prompting and video mod- the entire video of the task being completed. When the
eling across tasks and participants as indicated in Table 1. video ended, the trainer said, “Now you do it.” The partici-
When visual analysis of the trends from the alternating treat- pant was given 30 s to initiate the first step and 2 min 30 s
ments phase indicated that video prompting was superior to to complete the remaining steps in the task. If the partici-
video modeling, a final (prompt all) phase was implemented pant failed to complete the entire task within 2 min 30 s
in which both tasks were taught using video prompting. For of initiating the task, the session was terminated. Any steps

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at HOWARD UNIV UNDERGRAD LIBRARY on February 27, 2015
148 Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions 13(3)

that were not successfully completed by the participant basis using the formula: [Agreements/(Agreements + Dis-
were left uncompleted, because there was no need to ensure agreements)] × 100%. Average interobserver agreement was
the participant remained in sync with separate video clips calculated to be 99.5% (range 97%–100%).
and correct performance did not necessarily require task Data on treatment integrity were collected for an average
steps be completed in the order specified in the respective of 19.4% (range 13%–31%) of sessions. During these ses-
task analysis. No additional instructions, feedback, or sions, a second observer noted whether all procedural steps
prompts were delivered. were completed correctly. Treatment integrity was calcu-
Video prompting plus error correction. In video prompting lated by dividing the number of procedural steps completed
plus error correction, the procedures were identical to the correctly by the total number of procedural steps and multi-
initial video prompting condition, except that error correc- plying by 100%. Treatment integrity was calculated to be
tion was added if the participant did not complete a step 100% across all conditions.
correctly within 30 s of watching the video clip for that
step. The error correction procedure consisted of interrupt-
ing the participant if they were attempting to complete a Results
step incorrectly and saying, “Sorry, [name], that’s not quite Figures 1 and 2 show the percentage of steps completed
right. Here, watch this.” The video clip was then shown a correctly across tasks, participants, and sessions. Because
second time while the trainer pointed to the computer screen we counterbalanced the procedures and tasks across the
and said, “Watch this.” When the video clip ended, the participants, they were arranged into three pairs, with the
trainer said, “Now you do it.” The participant was then final participant receiving the intervention independent of
given another 30 s to complete the step. If the second view- another person. Introduction of the intervention was stag-
ing of the video clip failed to produce the correct response, gered across the four groups. The results are presented for
the trainer completed the step correctly while saying to the each pairing.
participant, “Watch me.”
Prompt all. When data from the previous phase indicated
that video prompting was more effective than video model- Keith and Tina
ing, the task that had been taught using video modeling was During baseline, Keith completed an average of 8.3%
moved to the video prompting procedure. In other words, (range 0%–11%) of Washing Dishes but did not complete
video prompting was used for both tasks. any steps correctly in Laundry. Tina did not complete any
In vivo training. When the data indicated that a specific steps correctly for either task. When the intervention was
skill was not being acquired with either video prompting introduced, Keith showed rapid acquisition of Laundry with
or video modeling, in vivo training was implemented. These video prompting and no correct responding in Washing
training procedures were identical to the video prompting Dishes with video modeling. When Washing Dishes was
procedures, except the model for each step was presented moved to video prompting, he again showed rapid acquisi-
live rather than on the video. For example, for the first step tion of the skill. He finished the study performing 100% of
of Laundry, the trainer said to the participant, “Watch this,” the steps correctly on both tasks. When intervention was
then picked up the basket while saying, “Pick up the basket.” implemented with Tina, she showed a slight increase in
Following the model of the step, the trainer placed the basket responding in Washing Dishes with video prompting, but
back on the floor and said, “Now you do it.” The participant her performance was low and variable (range 6%–39%)
was then given 30 s to complete the step. If they completed with a decreasing trend. In Session 36, the error correction
the step incorrectly, the trainer completed the step and then procedures were implemented with Washing Dishes, but
modeled the next step. they did not lead to a change in her behavior. Video model-
ing had no effect on her performance of Laundry, for which
she continued to perform zero steps correctly. In Session
Interobserver Agreement and Treatment Fidelity 43, the in vivo procedures were implemented, and she
One of two independent observers collected data on the showed an immediate increase in responding with both
number of steps performed correctly during at least 20% Washing Dishes and Laundry. At the end of the study, she
(range 20%–46%) of the sessions in all phases of the study. was performing 95% of Laundry and between 83% and
Observers were graduate students in special education who 89% of Washing Dishes correctly.
were trained to collect data by showing them the data sheet,
explaining what constituted a correct response, and show-
ing them how to record data on the data sheet. Agreement Jon and Carrie
between the trainer and the independent observer on the steps During baseline, Jon completed an average of 7.5% (range
performed correctly was calculated on a session-by-session 0%–11%) of steps correctly in Washing Dishes and no steps

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at HOWARD UNIV UNDERGRAD LIBRARY on February 27, 2015
Cannella-Malone et al. 149

Figure 1. Percentage of correct responding for Keith, Tina, Jon, and Carrie across baseline, video prompting versus video modeling,
and prompt all or in vivo (Carrie) phases

correctly with Laundry. Carrie performed none of the steps of Washing Dishes showed a slow but steady increase with
correctly for Washing Dishes and completed an average video prompting, and he continued to perform zero steps
of 1.2% (range 0%–6%) of steps correctly with Laundry. of Laundry correctly with video modeling. Because the
When intervention was introduced to Jon, his performance trend with Washing Dishes was low, the error correction

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at HOWARD UNIV UNDERGRAD LIBRARY on February 27, 2015
150 Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions 13(3)

Figure 2. Percentage of correct responding for Andrew, Adam, and Michael across baseline, video prompting versus video modeling,
and prompt all phases

procedures were introduced in Session 37. When both tasks which showed an initial increase in performance and then
were moved to the prompt all phase, his performance of decreased again before finally ending on an increasing trend.
Laundry increased to 72% of steps completed correctly When Washing Dishes was moved to video prompting,
after seven sessions. With video prompting and error cor- Carrie’s performance showed a gradual increasing trend. In
rection, his performance of Washing Dishes remained low Session 60, after increases in her responding of Washing
(range 11%–50%), so the in vivo procedures were intro- Dishes had slowed significantly, the in vivo procedures
duced with this task for the final two sessions, and his were implemented, which led to an immediate increase in
performance immediately improved to 77% of steps com- her behavior, with her final sessions showing 83% correct
pleted correctly. When the intervention was introduced to responding.
Carrie, her performance of Laundry also showed a slow
increase with video prompting that stopped at 38% of steps
completed correctly, and she continued to perform zero steps Andrew and Adam
of Washing Dishes with video modeling. In Session 35, the In baseline, Andrew performed an average of 3.4%
error correction procedure was implemented with Laundry, (range 0%–11%) of steps correctly with Washing Dishes

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at HOWARD UNIV UNDERGRAD LIBRARY on February 27, 2015
Cannella-Malone et al. 151

and an average of 2.2% (range 0%–11%) of steps cor- The results of this study provide further support to the use
rectly with Laundry. Adam performed an average of 18.3% of video prompting for teaching new skills to individuals
(range 0%–39%) of steps correctly with Washing Dishes with developmental disabilities (e.g., Le Grice & Blampied,
and 15% (range 0%–39%) of steps correctly with Laundry 1994; Sigafoos et al., 2005) and extend the research to
in baseline. When the intervention was introduced to Andrew, include adolescents with more severe disabilities as well as
his performance immediately increased with Laundry using provide a systematic replication across seven participants.
video prompting and his performance of Washing Dishes As in Cannella-Malone et al. (2006), the findings contradict
showed 0% correct responding with video modeling. When those of studies indicating that video modeling can be used
both tasks were moved to the prompt all phase, his perfor- to teach new skills to individuals with developmental dis-
mance of Laundry remained high (M = 88.4%, range abilities (e.g., D’Ateno, Mangiapanello, & Taylor, 2003;
72%–100%), and his performance of Washing Dishes Nikopoulos & Keenan, 2003).
immediately increased, but stabilized at about 72% of steps One possible explanation for video prompting being
completed correctly. In Session 59, the error correction more effective than video modeling is that the participants
procedures were utilized with Washing Dishes, and his per- only had to attend to short video clips with video prompting,
formance increased to 83% of steps completed correctly versus longer clips with video modeling. For modeling to be
for the final three sessions. When intervention was introduced effective, the participant has to attend to the entire model
to Adam, his performance on Washing Dishes quickly before he or she can imitate the behavior (Miltenberg, 1997).
increased to 100% correct responding using video prompt- It may be that the participants in this study were not able to
ing, where it remained (with the exception of one session) attend to the entire task during video modeling; thus, imita-
for the duration of the study. Adam’s performance of Laundry tion of the model did not occur. With video prompting, on
increased using video modeling but never exceeded 50% the other hand, they appear to have been capable of attend-
correct responding (range 33%–50%). Once Laundry was ing to a model of each individual step, thus allowing them to
moved into the prompt all phase, his performance quickly imitate short pieces of the task. Knowing that individuals
increased to 100% correct, where it remained for his final with developmental disabilities have deficits with atten-
three sessions. tion and memory (Matson & Smiroldo, 1999), it might be
expected that they would learn new tasks more efficiently
when there are fewer demands placed on their attention and
Michael memory. Video prompting appears to limit these demands,
In baseline, Michael performed an average of 6% (range whereas video modeling may have been too taxing on the
0%–15%) of steps correctly in Washing Dishes and no steps attention and memory capabilities of our participants.
correctly with Laundry. When intervention was introduced, A second possible explanation for these findings may be
his performance of Laundry quickly increased from 11% to the level of disability of our participants combined with time
67% of steps completed correctly using video prompting. limitations experienced. All of the participants had severe
With video modeling, his performance of Washing Dishes and multiple disabilities, rather than moderate disabilities.
showed a slight improvement (M = 10.7%, range 0%–21%). Research indicates that individuals with severe developmen-
When Washing Dishes was moved to the prompt all phase, tal disabilities generally take longer to learn new skills
his performance immediately increased (M = 63%, range (Sigafoos, O’Reilly, & Lancioni, 2010). It is possible that all
56%–67%). In Michael’s final session, he performed 67% of the participants may have mastered both skills had we
of Washing Dishes correctly and 76% of Laundry correctly. been able to continue working with them beyond the termi-
Michael did not attend any summer programming, so his nation of this study.
participation in this study was terminated at the end of the A third possible explanation may be the perspective of
school year. the videos, which were shown from the perspective of the
participant watching themselves complete the task. Three
of our participants required in vivo instruction for one or
Discussion both tasks, which represented a spectator perspective (i.e.,
The results of this study replicate those of Cannella-Malone the participants watched the experimenter complete each
et al. (2006) in that video prompting was more effective step). It may be that when the participants observed the
than video modeling in teaching two daily living skills to experimenter completing the step, they were able to observe
seven participants with significant intellectual disabilities— those aspects of the task that were most salient to them,
including one who was deaf—although not all participants which may have been different from what they could
achieved mastery with video prompting. Video modeling observe on the video of the arms and hands. Future research
had no effect with five of the seven participants, even when should compare the effects of using different perspectives
the perspective of video presentation was controlled for. in the video with the same instructional strategy (i.e., video

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at HOWARD UNIV UNDERGRAD LIBRARY on February 27, 2015
152 Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions 13(3)

prompting using spectator vs. participant perspectives) on the fact that it is awkward to hold. It might be worthwhile
skill acquisition. If both are equally effective, it might be to examine the effects of video prompting using a smaller
worthwhile to use the spectator perspective so that partici- device, such as a portable data assistant (PDA). Second,
pants can view those aspects of the video that are the most there are very few studies that have examined the effects of
salient to them. video prompting when the participant has been responsible
One possible explanation for decreased efficacy of video for starting the video clips themselves (e.g., Van Laarhoven,
prompting could be the complexity of some of the steps. Johnson, Van Laarhoven-Myers, Grider, & Grider, 2009). If
Several steps contained multiple instructions, and failure to the participants are able to control the video themselves, this
complete any part of these instructions resulted in that step would increase their self-determination even further. Future
being scored incorrectly. For example, participants would studies should examine the ease of teaching the participants
often pick up the plate, wash it, but not return it to the sink, to control the technology themselves and then examine the
resulting in being scored as incorrect for the entire step. effects of the video prompting intervention.
Future researchers should develop task analyses so that Providing individuals with significant intellectual disabili-
each step represents one discrete behavior (e.g., wash the ties appropriate instruction in the area of daily living skills has
plate), which might enhance the participants’ likelihood of the potential to enhance the individual’s self-determination
success, potentially increase the efficiency of instruction, as well as potentially provide further opportunities to be
and provide slightly more flexibility in the topography of more independent. The findings also provide support to the
the behavior. idea that students with severe disabilities can be taught new
One difference in this study from Cannella-Malone et al. daily living skills using video prompting. Future research
(2006) was the inclusion of a deaf participant, who demon- should continue to examine the factors that might make
strated significant gains in both skills even though he was video prompting more effective than video modeling so that
not able to hear the voice-over instructions. His results indi- enhancements can continue to be made with the instructional
cate that the video alone (without the auditory instruction) technology.
may be effective for teaching daily living skills. Relative to
other participants, video modeling was slightly more effec- Declaration of Conflicting Interests
tive for Michael, who typically completed the beginning and The authors declared no conflicts of interests with respect to the
ending steps of the task. Given his reliance on visual cues in authorship and/or publication of this article.
the everyday environment, he may have had a stronger his-
tory of reinforcement with observational learning (Bandura, Funding
1986). This history may have led to the increased effective- The authors disclosed receipt of the following Financial support
ness of video modeling. Based on these findings, future for the research and/or authorship of this article:
researchers should compare the use of video prompting with This research was supported by the Franklin County Board of
and without the verbal instruction. If it is found that the Developmental Disabilities (Contract GRT00012835).
voice-over instructions are not necessary, it might make the
use of video instruction in busy classrooms more effective. References
Aside from the points already discussed, one limitation Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A
of this study is that we were unable to withdraw the inter- social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
vention to measure maintenance. This study was conducted Barlow, D. H., & Hersen, M. (1984). Single case experimental
for the last 4 months of the participants’ school year, then for designs: Strategies for studying behavior change. New York,
2 weeks of their summer session for all participants except NY: Pergamon.
Michael. Because of these time constraints, not all partici- Bidwell, M. A., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2004). Using video modeling
pants met mastery with both skills. Given more time, we to teach a domestic skill with an embedded social skill to adults
believe that all participants would have achieved mastery with severe mental retardation. Behavioral Interventions, 19,
based on the trends in their data. Only at that point would we 263–274.
have withdrawn the intervention to test for maintenance of Cannella, H. I., O’Reilly, M. F., & Lancioni, G. E. (2005). Choice
the skills. Future research should examine the durability of and preference assessment research with people with severe to
skills learned via video prompting to ensure that they are profound developmental disabilities: A review of the literature.
maintained. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 1–15.
The results of this study suggest several additional pos- Cannella-Malone, H. I., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., de la Cruz, B.,
sible directions for future research. First, although the use of Edrisinha, C., & Lancioni, G. E. (2006). Comparing video
a laptop computer was appropriate for this study, it would prompting to video modeling for teaching daily living skills
have been difficult to use one in a community setting to to six adults with developmental disabilities. Education and
teach a skill such as grocery shopping because of its size and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 41, 344–356.

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at HOWARD UNIV UNDERGRAD LIBRARY on February 27, 2015
Cannella-Malone et al. 153

Cihak, D., Alberto, P. A., Taber-Doughty, T., & Gama, R. I. (2006). video prompting for teaching microwave oven use to three
A comparison of static picture prompting and video prompting adults with developmental disabilities. Journal of Behavioral
simulations strategies using group instructional procedures. Education, 14, 189–201.
Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 21, Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M. F., & Lancioni, G. E. (2010). Mental
89–99. retardation. In J. Thomas & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of
D’Ateno, P., Mangiapanello, K., & Taylor, B. A. (2003). Using clinical psychology competencies (HCPC): Vol. III. Interven-
video modeling to teach complex play sequences to a pre- tion and treatment for children and adolescents. (pp. 1245-
schooler with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interven- 1274). New York, NY: Springer.
tions, 5, 5–11. Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland
Graves, T. B., Collins, B. C., Schuster, J. W., & Kleinert, H. (2005). Adaptive Behavior Scales: Interview edition survey form. Cir-
Using video prompting to teach cooking skills to secondary cle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
students with moderate disabilities. Education and Training in Van Laarhoven, T., Johnson, J. W., Van Laarhoven-Myers, T.,
Developmental Disabilities, 40, 34–46. Grider, K. L., & Grider, K. M. (2009). The effectiveness of
Heller, K. W., Bigge, J. L., & Allgood, P. (2005). Adaptations for using a video iPod as a prompting device in employment set-
personal independence. In S. J. Best, K. W. Heller, & J. L. Bigge tings. Journal of Behavioral Education, 18, 119–141.
(Eds.), Teaching individuals with physical or multiple disabili- Van Laarhoven, T., & Van Laarhoven-Myers, T. (2006). A compar-
ties (pp. 309–338). Columbus, OH: Prentice Hall. ison of three video-based instructional procedures for teaching
Horner, R. D., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Multiple-probe technique: A daily living skills to persons with developmental disabilities.
variation of the multiple baseline. Journal of Applied Behavior Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 41,
Analysis, 11, 189–196. 365–381.
Jacobson, J. W., & Ackerman, L. J. (1990). Differences in adaptive
functioning among people with autism or mental retardation. About the Authors
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 205–219. Helen I. Cannella-Malone, PhD, is an assistant professor of
Kraijer, D. (2000). Review of adaptive behavior studies in mentally special education and applied behavior analysis at The Ohio State
retarded persons with autism/pervasive developmental disorder. University. Her current research interests include working with
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 39–47. individuals with severe to profound intellectual disabilities to teach
Le Grice, B., & Blampied, N. M. (1994). Training pupils with new skills using video technology, change automatically main-
intellectual disability to operate educational technology using tained challenging behavior by manipulating motivating operations,
video prompting. Education and Training in Mental Retarda- and examine the effects of choice and preference on behavior.
tion and Developmental Disabilities, 29, 321–330.
Matson, J. L., & Smiroldo, B. B. (1999). Intellectual disorders. Courtney Fleming, MA, is a graduate student conducting
In W. K. Silverman & T. H. Ollendick (Eds.), Developmen- research in special education and applied behavior analysis at The
tal issues in the clinical treatment of children (pp. 295–306). Ohio State University.
Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Miltenberger, R. G. (1997). Behavior modification: Principles Yi-Cheih Chung, PhD, recently graduated from The Ohio State
and procedures. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. University, and her current research interests include the manipu-
Nikopoulos, C. K., & Keenan, M. (2003). Promoting social initia- lation of motivating operations to effect automatically maintained
tion in children with autism using video modeling. Behavioral challenging behavior in individuals with severe to profound intel-
Interventions, 18, 87–108. lectual disabilities.
Parmenter, T. R. (1994). Quality of life as a concept and measur-
able entity. Social Indicators Research, 33, 9–46. Geoffrey M. Wheeler, MA, recently graduated from The Ohio
Rainforth, B., & York-Barr, J. (1997). Collaborative teams for State University and now works as a behavior consultant for
students with severe disabilities: Integrating therapy and edu- Behavior Intervention Services, LLC, in St. Louis, Missouri.
cational services (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., Cannella, H., Edrisinha, C., de la Cruz, B., Abby R. Basbagill is a graduate student conducting research in
Upadhyaya, M., . . . , Young, D. (2007). Evaluation of a video special education and applied behavior analysis at The Ohio State
prompting and fading procedure for teaching dish washing University.
skills to adults with adults with developmental disabilities.
Journal of Behavioral Education, 16, 93–109. Angella H. Singh, PhD, is adjunct faculty at The Ohio State Uni-
Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., Cannella, H., Upadhyaya, M., Edrisinha, C., versity and also a teacher of intermediate students at Franklin
Lancioni, G. E., . . . , Young, D. (2005). Computer-presented County Board of Developmental Disabilities.

Downloaded from pbi.sagepub.com at HOWARD UNIV UNDERGRAD LIBRARY on February 27, 2015

You might also like