You are on page 1of 8

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2009, 50, 645–651 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00779.

Health and Disability


Psychoneuroimmunology: A bio-psycho-social approach to health
and disease
ROBERT ZACHARIAE
Psychooncology Research Unit, Aarhus University Hospital and Department of Psychology, University of Aarhus, Denmark

Zachariae, R. (2009). Psychoneuroimmunology: A bio-psycho-social approach to health and disease. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50, 645–651.
Since the early 1980s, the interdisciplinary field of psychoneuroimmunology has explored the complex bi-directional interactions between brain, behav-
ior, and the immune system. Taken together, this research has expanded the limits of the questions we can ask about the organism by challenging the
biomedical paradigm of the immune system as predominantly ‘‘autonomous’’. Psychoneuroimmunology has played a key role in establishing a biolog-
ical basis for the ancient idea that the mind can play a role in health and disease. This article describes the development of psychoneuroimmunology
and reviews a number of key findings concerning psychological phenomena of potential relevance to understanding brain-behavior-immune interac-
tions, including learning, emotions, stress, and the role of sensory processes.
Key words: Psychoneuroimmunology, immunology, systems theory, health, stress, learning, emotions, sensory processes.
Professor Robert Zachariae, MDSci, Psychooncology Research Unit, Jens Chr. Skous Vej 4, DK-8000 Århus C, Århus, Denmark. E-mail: bzach@
aarhus.rm.dk

INTRODUCTION historically, the predominant focus of immunological research


has been on interactions between the immune system and for-
Over the past decades there has been increased scientific as well eign antigen and between the various components of the
as public interest in the possible influence of psychological and immune system itself, a small number of ‘‘deviant’’ studies have
social factors on human physiology, and a growing body appeared over the years. The first study, which reasonably can
of research has provided evidence for psychosocial influences be termed as ‘‘psycho-immunologic’’, was published in 1919
on both the development and the course of a number of medical and suggested that negative emotions could influence the
illnesses. Psychoneuroimmunology, the interdisciplinary area of immune system of patients with tuberculosis (Ishigami, 1919).
research examining interactions between brain, behavior, and In the 1920s, a series of animal studies of interactions between
the immune system, has played a key role in the exploration of the brain and the immune system were conducted in the Soviet
behavioral and biological mechanisms linking psychosocial fac- Union by pupils of Pavlov (1849–1936), indicating that immune
tors, health, and disease (Zachariae, 1996). By challenging the responses could be influenced by classical conditioning.
biomedical concept of the immune system as an ‘‘autonomous’’ Repeated pairings of a neutral stimulus (e.g. heat) with an
defense system, psychoneuroimmunology represents a shift immune-stimulating antigen (e.g. inactivated bacteria) were
from a predominantly biomedical paradigm of health and dis- found to result in activation of the immune response when the
ease towards an interdisciplinary bio-psycho-social approach neutral stimulus was presented alone (Metal’nikov & Chorine,
(Engel, 1977). In the following, the development of psychoneu- 1926). In the 1950s and 1960s, results from a small number
roimmunology and some key findings within this field will be of studies suggested that psychological stress could increase
described. susceptibility to infection (Rasmussen, Marsh & Brill, 1957)
and exacerbate autoimmune diseases such as lupus (Fessel &
Solomon, 1960) and rheumatoid arthritis (Solomon, Amkraut &
THE EARLY HISTORY OF Kasper, 1964). The term ‘‘psychoneuroimmunology’’ itself,
PSYCHONEUROIMMUNOLOGY however, was not coined until 1981, when it appeared as the
The behavior of the immune system, e.g. interactions between title of an anthology reviewing the evidence available at the
antibodies and antigens, can be investigated intelligibly through time for associations between behavioral, neural, endocrine, and
cloned cells in vitro, i.e. in test tubes outside the living organ- immune system processes (Ader, 1981). The first official psy-
ism, and this has no doubt contributed to the fact that for many choneuroimmunology meeting held in 1986 proposed a number
decades the dominant ‘‘normal’’ scientific view of the immune of key questions: (1) Are there interactions between the brain
system has been one of an ‘‘autonomous’’ defense against exter- and the immune system? (2) If so, how are they mediated? (3)
nal and internal threats to the organism, which is essentially Are they bi-directional? and (4) are they trivial or important to
unrelated to other systems and processes of the organism. While understanding health and disease? (Cohen, 1987).

 2009 The Author. Journal compilation  2009 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations. Published by Blackwell Publishing Ltd., 9600
Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. ISSN 0036-5564.
646 R. Zachariae Scand J Psychol 50 (2009)

THE IMMUNE SYSTEM IN THE LIVING ORGANISM: Guillaumin, Degenne, Bardos & Renoux, 1985). Second, a
A SYSTEMIC APPROACH large body of research has identified ‘‘hard-wired’’ neural con-
Posing the first question: is there evidence of interaction nections between the brain and immune system. For example,
between the brain and the immune system, is not possible there is evidence for both sympathetic and parasympathetic
within the framework of the conventional ‘‘autonomous’’ view innervation of organs and tissues associated with the immune
of immunology. This requires a broader framework, as provided system, including the lymph nodes, thymus, spleen, and bone
by a general systems perspective of the organism (von Berta- marrow, and other neural-immune cell associations are found in
lanffy, 1969). From this perspective, we should expect that the cutaneous, gastro-intestinal, and mucosal tissue (Bellinger,
various systems of the organism have developed the ability to Lorton, Lubahn & Felten, 2001). Finally, it is now recognized
interact in a coherent way with the self-regulatory purpose of that the nervous and immune systems communicate through a
maintaining a dynamic internal equilibrium while adapting to common biochemical language that involves shared neuroendo-
the challenges posed by changes in the external and internal crine hormones, neurotransmitters, cytokines, and their respec-
environment (Zachariae, 1996). While conventional immunol- tive receptors (Carr & Blalock, 1991). Taken together, the
ogy may be able to describe a number of important immunolog- available evidence has confirmed the existence of a complex
ical phenomena independently of other processes, the full bidirectional brain-immune network, providing a biological
understanding of the behavior of the immune system in the liv- basis for the ancient anecdotal notion that the mind can play a
ing organism requires investigation of both the cooperation role in health and disease by its ability to influence relevant
between the elements of the immune system itself as well as of biological processes. Figure 1 summarizes the verified commu-
their interdependencies with other elements of the system as a nication pathways between brain and immune system.
whole.
While regarded as rather heretical at the time, the immunolo-
ENVIRONMENT-BRAIN-BEHAVIOR-IMMUNE EFFECTS
gist Niels Jerne (1911–1994), in his controversial Network The-
ory of the immune system (Jerne, 1974), pointed to the apparent The existence of an extensive brain-immune network suggests
functional similarities between the immune system and the brain. that the immune system should be under at least partial influ-
Both the brain and the immune system are self-referential, i.e. ence by psychological processes. Examples of psychological
are involved in the recognition of ‘‘self’’ versus ‘‘non-self’’, and, phenomena of potential relevance to understanding the brain-
according to Jerne, the immune network constitutes the identity behavior-immune connection include learning, psychological
of the organism at the molecular level. Similarly, as suggested stress, emotions, and sensory processes.
by more recent developments in neuroscience, neural networks
of the brain can be viewed as constituting a neuromatrix, repre-
Learned immune responses
senting the physiological ‘‘self’’ of the organism (Melzack,
1993). Furthermore, both systems involve innate responses as Learning is a primary function of the brain by which the organ-
well as characteristics acquired through interactions with the ism adapts to the environment. One example of the role of the
environment, i.e. ‘‘learning’’ and ‘‘memory’’. Finally, both the brain in modulating the immune system is provided by studies
brain and the immune system can be described as ‘‘sensory showing that the immune response can be modified by classical
organs’’. While the five ‘‘classical’’ senses enable us to be cogni- conditioning. The early studies by Soviet researchers described
zant of what we can see, hear, touch, taste, and smell, the above went largely unnoticed at the time, and the first modern
immune system can be viewed as a ‘‘sixth sense’’ that enables psychoneuroimmunology conditioning experiment was a result
the organism to recognize bacteria, viruses, cancer cells, and of a serendipitous finding by the psychologist Robert Ader.
other entities too small to see, touch, taste or smell (Blalock, A taste-aversion experiment using a saccharine-flavored drink-
1984). While this challenge to the ‘‘autonomous’’ view of the ing solution as the conditioned stimulus (CS) paired with an
immune system is clearly ‘‘meta-theoretical’’, it has inspired unconditioned stimulus (UCS) in the form of a tasteless nausea-
researchers to pose a number of questions that cannot be asked inducing (and immunosuppressive) agent (cyclophosphamide)
within conventional immunology. These questions and their revealed that some of the animals died when subsequently pre-
answers form the basis of psychoneuroimmunology. sented with the CS alone. This led to the hypothesis that a con-
ditioned immunosuppressive effect of the nausea-inducing agent
had taken place, which was confirmed in a subsequent experi-
BRAIN-IMMUNE INTERACTIONS ment (Ader & Cohen, 1975). In the following years, animal as
Over the last three decades, psychoneuroimmunology research well as human studies confirmed that a large number of immune
has provided evidence for extensive bidirectional communica- parameters can be both suppressed and enhanced using various
tion between the brain and the immune system. The evidence types of pharmacological and environmental stimuli as UCS
stems from several sources. First, early animal studies indicated (Ader & Cohen, 2001). Cancer patients treated with cytotoxic
that left- and right-brain lesions produce different patterns of agents usually experience side-effects in the form of nausea,
suppression and activation of various immune measures (Biziere, vomiting, and fatigue as well as immunosuppressive effects

 2009 The Author. Journal compilation  2009 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 50 (2009) Psychoneuroimmunology 647

Symbolic
stimuli from the
environment

Neuropeptides

Autonomic
Sensory Nervous system
Nervous system
Endocrine Vascular system
system Cytokines

Immune Immuno-
system genic
stimuli

”Hormonal”
peptides

Fig. 1. The bi-directional brain-immune network.

(Zachariae, Paulsen, Mehlsen, Jensen, Johansson & von der and later exposed to a challenge with the same allergen under
Maase 2007b). Some patients display anticipatory or condi- similar psychophysiological conditions of either relaxation or
tioned side-effects, with the hospital environment and tastes and no-relaxation (Zachariae et al., 1997).
odors associated with the treatment serving as CS (Zachariae,
Paulsen, Mehlsen, Jensen, Johansson & von der Maase 2007a),
and studies using cancer treatment as a natural experiment have Stress and immunity
shown results suggestive of conditioned immunity (e.g. The available evidence shows that acute stressors generally are
Bovbjerg, Redd, Maier et al., 1990). associated with enhanced immunity and long-term or chronic
The strength of learned associations depends on several fac- stressors with suppressed immune function (Dhabhar & Mc-
tors. Certain personality traits related to sensory perception and Ewen, 1997). While it makes sense from an evolutionary per-
imaginative involvement such as hypnotizability and absorption spective that stress is associated with enhanced immunity
(Zachariae, Jorgensen & Christensen, 2000b) have been associ- thereby preparing the organism to deal more effectively with
ated with both increased conditionability (Zachariae et al., potential infections, it seems more puzzling that under long-term
2007a) and increased reactivity of the autonomic nervous and stress, the same brain-immune mechanisms are associated with
immune systems (Ehrnrooth, Zachariae, Svensen et al., 2002; suppression of some immune parameters, e.g. antibody produc-
Zachariae, Jorgensen, Bjerring & Svendsen, 2000a; Zachariae, tion, and enhancement of others, e.g. allergic and inflammatory
Jorgensen, Christensen & Bjerring, 1997). The term state- responses (Dhabhar & McEwen, 2001). The currently best
dependent learning (SDL) describes the phenomenon that explanation seems to be that the immune system has developed
behavior learned in one physiological state is better remembered the ability to selectively up- and down-regulate its responses
when retention is tested in the same state (Overton, 1991). under different conditions, e.g. up-regulate during infection and
Although the concept of SDL generally applies to human mem- down-regulate to avoid autoimmune reactions when the upregu-
ory and behavior, the hypothesis that SDL also applies to the lated immune response is no longer needed. The concept of
memory and behavior of the immune system has been con- ‘‘allostasis’’ represents the processes involved in the adaptive
firmed in an experiment showing that the so-called delayed-type responses to stressful situations, e.g. increased number of circu-
hypersensitivity (allergic) response (DTH) was stronger when lating immune cells and enhanced cell-mediated immunity,
the individual had been sensitized to an experimental allergen while ‘‘allostatic load’’ represents the physiological costs to the

 2009 The Author. Journal compilation  2009 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
648 R. Zachariae Scand J Psychol 50 (2009)

organism, e.g. in terms of suppressed immunity to antigens or suppression, but is in concordance with the clinical implications,
chronic inflammation, when these mechanisms are activated fre- i.e. that both suppressed chemotaxis and enhanced inflammatory
quently over longer periods of time. The different immune reaction are associated with negative clinical outcomes. Under
response patterns depend on the timing and duration of the natural conditions, the associations between immunity and emo-
stressor and are modulated by stress-hormones such as gluco- tions might be mediated by other behavioral factors. Negative
corticoids and catecholamines. affect is thus associated with impaired sleep quality (Thomsen,
While the research demonstrating effects of various types of Mehlsen, Christensen & Zachariae, 2003), which in turn is
stressors on immune and inflammatory processes (Herbert & Co- known to affect immune function (Born, 1999), and other stud-
hen, 1993b) has provided evidence for potential mechanisms link- ies have confirmed that the associations found between depres-
ing stress with increased risk of a number of diseases, including sion and immune function found in clinical populations, e.g.
infections, allergies, autoimmune diseases, and even cardiovascu- HIV-infected, are partly mediated by impaired sleep quality
lar diseases, the current evidence for a link between stress and (Cruess, Antoni, Gonzalez et al., 2003).
cancer is less convincing. One reason could be the limited focus Theories that various types of emotional inhibition, e.g. alexi-
of the research so far. The prognosis of many cancer patients is thymia (Jorgensen, Zachariae, Skytthe & Kyvik, 2007) and
not only related to the cancer itself, but also to complications emotional repression (Jorgensen & Zachariae, 2006) are associ-
related to the cancer and its treatment, such as infections and post- ated with increased risk of somatic symptoms, have led to the
ponement of treatment due to immunosuppression (Bovbjerg, hypothesis that emotional inhibition may be a risk factor for
Valdimarsdottir & Zachariae, 1999). Stress has been shown to development of cancer (LeShan, 1959). Later findings, however,
increase susceptibility to infection (Pedersen, Bovbjerg & Zacha- suggest that the increased level of repression often found in can-
riae, 2009b), and to influence immuno-competence, e.g. mea- cer patients may be a coping response to the cancer diagnosis,
sured as the strength of the immune response to vaccination rather than a premorbid personality trait (Zachariae, Jensen,
(Pedersen, Zachariae & Bovbjerg, 2009c). The role of stress for Pedersen et al., 2004). This does not exclude the possibility that
the risk of infection in cancer patients, however, has so far only repressive coping could be associated with poorer cancer prog-
been explored in one study, which showed that stress measured nosis. The plausibility of this theory would however be
prior to treatment was associated with increased risk of being hos- strengthened, if clear associations between emotional inhibition
pitalized with infection during treatment (Pedersen, Zachariae, and cancer-relevant immune-measures could be identified. Only
Jensen, Bovbjerg, Andersen & von der Maase, 2009d). few studies have so far addressed this issue with mixed results
(e.g. Esterling, Antoni, Kumar & Schneiderman, 1993; Jamner,
Schwartz & Leigh, 1988).
Immune modulation by emotions
Systematic reviews of the literature generally conclude that neg-
ative emotions such as depression are associated with increased Voluntary modulation of the immune system through sensory
mortality (Cuijpers & Smit, 2002), and – conversely – that posi- imagery
tive well-being may be associated with reduced mortality (Chida While the psychoneuroimmunologic phenomena described
& Steptoe, 2008). While there is good evidence of a link above can still be understood as ‘‘autonomous’’ – i.e. non-vol-
between depression and immunity (Herbert & Cohen, 1993a), it untary – responses to various types of external stimuli (e.g.
is not clear to what extent this link can explain the association stressors) and psychological states (e.g. emotions), the idea that
between depression and mortality. However, as positive and the immune system can be influenced by voluntary, conscious
negative emotions are essential psychological mechanisms, processes is more controversial from a conventional biomedical
which serve adaptive purposes in the regulation of behavior, we view. Nevertheless, there is growing evidence that this may
should expect that changes in emotional state be reflected in indeed be possible by altering sensory perception through vari-
changes in the immune system. This hypothesis is supported by ous psychological techniques such as guided imagery or hypno-
results from studies showing that experimentally induced emo- sis (Zachariae, 2001). The response of the organism to
tional states can affect immunological and inflammatory param- potentially damaging external stimuli is regulated by a number
eters. In one study, experimentally induced negative mood was of highly integrated mechanisms, involving both the central and
found associated with suppression and positive mood with peripheral nervous systems. The two systems interact with the
enhancement of chemotaxis, a measure of immune function aim of producing those responses of the cardiovascular, endo-
(Zachariae, Bjerring, Zachariae et al., 1991). In another, the crine, immune, and sensory nervous (SNS) systems that pro-
inflammatory skin response to histamine, a model of relevance mote the most adaptive physiological and behavioral reactions
to immediate-type allergic reactions, was found increased after under different conditions.
negative mood and suppressed after positive mood (Zachariae, For instance, inflammatory reactions of the skin have been
Jorgensen, Egekvist & Bjerring, 2001). It is worth noting that shown to be partly regulated by ascending and descending path-
the association between mood valence and the direction of the ways between the brain and the SNS (Zachariae, 1996).
immune or inflammatory response is not a simple question of For example, animal studies have shown that lesioning of the
positive moods being associated with increase and negative with sensory nerves suppresses the inflammatory skin reaction to

 2009 The Author. Journal compilation  2009 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 50 (2009) Psychoneuroimmunology 649

histamine, and, in humans, inflammatory skin reactions can be unidirectional, and there should be at least some form of ‘‘feed-
suppressed by local anesthetics. This suggests that the intensity back-loop’’ from the immune system to the brain (Zachariae,
of a local inflammatory reaction, e.g. to a burn, partly depends 1996). This has been demonstrated most clearly by studies of
on sensory feedback to the brain from the affected area. It is so-called cytokine-induced sickness behavior during infection
well documented that hypnotic suggestions can alter both the (Dantzer, 2001). At the behavioral level, infected individuals
self-reported perception of sensory stimuli such as pain as well show depressed activity, loss of interest in the environment, and
as sensory-related brain-correlates (Zachariae & Bjerring, 1994; reduced food intake, and, subjectively, infection is associated
Zachariae, Andersen, Bjerring, Jorgensen & Arendt-Nielsen, with fatigue, depressed mood, and increased pain sensation.
1998; Zachariae, Bjerring, Arendt-Nielsen, Nielsen & Gotlieb- These changes can be experimentally induced in healthy ani-
sen, 1991a), and in an integrated bio-psycho-social system we mals and humans by injections of proinflammatory cytokines
should therefore expect that psychologically induced altered (e.g. interleukin-1) that are normally released by activated
perception can reduce local inflammatory reactions. This immune cells during the response to bacterial or viral infections.
hypothesis was confirmed in a study showing reduced inflam- It could be argued that the sickness behavior pattern induced by
matory reactions to histamine under a hypnotic analgesia condi- the immune system is the expression of a motivational state,
tion, compared to reactions in a control condition, with rather than the consequence of physiological weakness, and that
reductions in pain-evoked brain potentials confirming reduced it represents a response that promotes behaviors that support the
sensation during hypnotic analgesia (Zachariae & Bjerring, adaptive physiological changes that occur during infection. For
1990). instance, the increase in body temperature that is needed to pro-
Other studies have confirmed that hypnotic suggestions to duce fever leaves little room for costly activities that are of no
suppress or enhance reactions of the skin can alter both inflam- use in fighting the infectious pathogens, and the reduced level
matory skin reactions associated with immediate-type allergic of activity characteristic of sickness behavior can thus be seen
reactions and delayed-type immune reactions of relevance to as a highly organized strategy that is critical to the survival of
eczemas, psoriasis and other skin diseases (Zachariae, Bjerring the organism (Dantzer, 2001).
& Arendt-Nielsen, 1989). Further results indicate that the This perspective on cytokine-induced sickness behavior could
immune system is susceptible to psychological influences both potentially lead to new psychoneuroimmunologic perspectives
during the sensitization or ‘‘learning’’ phase of the immune on cancer. Cancers are often associated with increased inflam-
response and during the later challenge or ‘‘retention’’ (Zacha- mation, and the high prevalence of depression found among
riae & Bjerring, 1993; Zachariae et al., 1997). The available cancer patients (Christensen, Zachariae, Jensen et al., 2009)
results suggest that voluntary changes of local immune reactions could, at least partly, represent a cytokine-induced sickness syn-
require sufficiently strong sensory pathways (Locke, Ransil, drome (Raison & Miller, 2003). As increased inflammation has
Zachariae et al., 1994b), and that effects on specific immune been associated with poorer prognosis, this could theoretically
components of skin and mucosa are likely to be mediated explain the links found between depression and increased cancer
through neural influences on local vascular processes, rather mortality. Another area of potential interest is the common com-
than through direct differential effects on specific types of plaint of impaired cognitive function following cancer treat-
immune cells (Zachariae, Oster & Bjerring, 1994). This was ment, often referred to by patients as ‘‘chemo brain’’. As such
supported by results of a study comparing effects of non-spe- cognitive complaints are less likely to be due to the chemother-
cific instructions to relax and guided imagery instructions to apy itself (Mehlsen, Pedersen, Jensen & Zachariae, 2009; Peder-
imagine enhancement of specific immune cells (Zachariae, Han- sen, Rossen, Mehlsen, Pedersen, Zachariae & von der Maase,
sen, Andersen et al., 1994). While both interventions may influ- 2009a), they could perhaps be due to cancer-induced immune-
ence the function of various immune cells in the blood stream influences on the brain.
when compared to no intervention, the results of this study
showed no difference between the effects of relaxation and
guided imagery, suggesting that effects on cells in the peripheral CONCLUSION
blood stream are probably due to more general non-specific As reviewed above, psychoneuroimmunologic research over the
effects of relaxation and stress-reduction through altered auto- last three decades has confirmed the existence of bi-directional
nomic nervous system activity. Few studies have explored the interactions between the brain and the immune system, and
clinical applicability of these findings, but preliminary results results from a growing number of studies indicate that negative
indicate that patients with immune-related skin diseases such as psychosocial factors such as depression and stress are associated
psoriasis may benefit from psychological intervention (Zacha- with increased morbidity and mortality. Still lacking, however,
riae, Oster, Bjerring & Kragballe, 1996). is sufficiently convincing evidence that the links between psy-
chosocial factors and health and disease are mediated by the
identified brain-immune pathways, and further research is
IMMUNE-BRAIN-BEHAVIOR EFFECTS needed. Psychoneuroimmunologic research has revealed the
As expected from theories of living systems, the pathways high degree of complexity of the brain-immune interactions,
between the brain and the immune system are unlikely to be and the adaptive function of many of these interactions is as yet

 2009 The Author. Journal compilation  2009 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
650 R. Zachariae Scand J Psychol 50 (2009)

far from fully understood. This makes it difficult to interpret HIV-positive men and women on combination antiretroviral ther-
various immune outcomes in studies of psychosocial factors and apy. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 54, 185–189.
Cuijpers, P. & Smit, F. (2002). Excess mortality in depression: A
immunity and also underscores that the promises of a ‘‘strength-
meta-analysis of community studies. Journal of Affective Disorders,
ened immune system’’ frequently promoted by proponents of 72, 227–236.
complementary and alternative treatments are only metaphors Dantzer, R. (2001). Cytokine-induced sickness behavior: Mechanisms
that make little sense from a scientific perspective (Zachariae, and implications. Annals of the New York Academy of Science,
1996). Although the field of psychoneuroimmunology has pro- 933, 222–234.
Dhabhar, F. S. & McEwen, B. (2001). Bidirectional effects of stress
vided promising results, the critical question of whether behav-
and glucocorticoid hormones on immune function: possible expla-
ioral manipulation, e.g. stressors or intervention, can affect nations for paradoxical observations. In R. Ader, D. L. Felten & N.
immunity so as to influence health and survival, still remains to Cohen (Eds.), Psychoneuroimmunology (3rd edn, pp. 301–338).
be answered. New York: Academic Press.
Dhabhar, F. S. & McEwen, B. S. (1997). Acute stress enhances while
chronic stress suppresses cell-mediated immunity in vivo: A poten-
REFERENCES tial role for leukocyte trafficking. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity,
11, 286–306.
Ader, R. (1981). Psychoneuroimmunology. New York: Academic Press. Ehrnrooth, E., Zachariae, R., Svendsen, G., Jorgensen, M. M., Yishay,
Ader, R. & Cohen, N. (1975). Behaviorally conditioned immunosu- M., Sorensen, B. S., et al. (2002). Increased Thymidylate Synthase
pression. Psychosomatic Medicine, 37, 333–340. mRNA concentration in blood leukocytes following an experimen-
Ader, R. & Cohen, N. (2001). Conditioning and immunity. in R. Ader, tal stressor. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 71, 97–103.
D. L. Felten & N. Cohen (Eds.), Psychoneuroimmunology (3rd Engel, G. L. (1977). The need for a new medical model: A challenge
edn, pp. 3–34). New York: Academic Press. for biomedicine. Science, 196, 129–136.
Bellinger, D. L., Lorton, D., Lubahn, C. & Felten, D. L. (2001). Inner- Esterling, B. A., Antoni, M. H., Kumar, M. & Schneiderman, N.
vation of lymphoid organs - Association of nerves with cells of the (1993). Defensiveness, trait anxiety, and Epstein-Barr viral capsid
immune system and their implications in disease. in R. Ader, D. L. antigen antibody titers in healthy college students. Health Psychol-
Felten & N. Cohen (Eds.), Psychoneuroimmunology (3rd edn, pp. ogy, 12, 132–139.
55–111). New York: Academic Press. Fessel, W. J. & Solomon, G. F. (1960). Psychosis and systemic lupus
Biziere, K., Guillaumin, J. M., Degenne, D., Bardos, P. & Renoux, M. erythematosus. California Medicine, 92, 266–270.
(1985). Lateralized neocortical modulation of the T-cell lineage. In Herbert, T. B. & Cohen, S. (1993a). Depression and immunity: A
R. Guillemin et al. (Eds.), Neural modulation of immunity (pp. meta-analytical review. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 472–486.
81–94). New York: Raven Press. Herbert, T. B. & Cohen, S. (1993b). Stress and immunity in humans: a
Blalock, J. E. (1984). The immune system as a sensory organ. Journal meta-analytic review. Psychosomatic Medicine, 55, 364–379.
of Immunology, 132, 1067–1070. Ishigami, T. (1919). The influence of psychic acts on the progress of
Born, J. (1999). Sleep and immune function. In M. Schedlowski & U. pulmonary tuberculosis. American Review of Tuberculosis, 2, 470–
Tewes (Eds.), Psychoneuroimmunology – An interdisciplinary intro- 484.
duction (pp. 417–442). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Pub- Jamner, L. D., Schwartz, G. E. & Leigh, H. (1988). The relationship
lishers. between repressive and defensive coping styles and monocyte, eo-
Bovbjerg, D., Valdimarsdottir, H. B. & Zachariae, R. (1999). Psycho- sinophile, and serum glucose levels: Support for the opioid peptide
neuroimmunology in oncology. in M. Schedlowski & U. Tewes hypothesis of repression. Psychosomatic Medicine, 50, 567–575.
(Eds.), Psychoneuroimmunology: A textbook (pp. 473–486). New Jerne, N. K. (1974). Towards a network theory of the immune system.
York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. Annals of Immunology (Paris), 125C, 373–389.
Bovbjerg, D. H., Redd, W. H., Maier, L. A., Holland, J. C., Lesko, Jorgensen, M. M. & Zachariae, R. (2006). Repressive coping style and
L. M., Niedzwiecki, D., et al. (1990). Anticipatory immune sup- autonomic reactions to two experimental stressors in healthy men
pression and nausea in women receiving cyclic chemotherapy for and women. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 47, 137–148.
ovarian cancer. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, Jorgensen, M. M., Zachariae, R., Skytthe, A. & Kyvik, K. (2007).
153–157. Genetic and environmental factors in alexithymia: A population-
Carr, D. J. J. & Blalock, J. E. (1991). Neuropeptide hormones and based study of 8,785 Danish twin pairs. Psychotherapy and Psy-
receptors common to the immune and neuroendocrine systems: chosomatics, 76, 369–375.
Bidirectional pathway of intersystem communication. in R. Ader, LeShan, L. (1959). Psychological states as factors in the development
D. L. Felten & N. Cohen (Eds.), Psychoneuroimmunology (2nd of malignant disease: A critical review. Journal of the National
edn, pp. 573–588). New York: Academic Press. Cancer Institute, 22, 18.
Chida, Y. & Steptoe, A. (2008). Positive psychological well-being and Locke, S. E., Ransil, B. J., Zachariae, R., Molay, F., Tollins, K., Covi-
mortality: A quantitative review of prospective observational stud- no, N. A., et al. (1994). Effect of hypnotic suggestion on the
ies. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 741–756. delayed-type hypersensitivity response. JAMA: The Journal of the
Christensen, S., Zachariae, R., Jensen, A. B., Vaeth, M., Moller, S., American Medical Association, 272, 47–52.
Ravnsbaek, J., et al. (2009). Prevalence and risk of depressive Mehlsen, M., Pedersen, A. D., Jensen, A. B. & Zachariae, R. (2009).
symptoms 3–4 months post-surgery in a nationwide cohort study No indications of cognitive side-effects in a prospective study of
of Danish women treated for early stage breast-cancer. Breast Can- breast cancer patients receiving adjuvant chemotherapy. Psychoon-
cer Research and Treatment, 113, 339–355. cology, 18, 248–257.
Cohen, J. J. (1987). Methodological issues in behavioral immunology. Melzack, R. (1993). Pain: Past, present and future. Canadian Journal
Immunology Today, 8, 33–34. of Experimental Psychology, 47, 615–629.
Cruess, D. G., Antoni, M. H., Gonzalez, J., Fletcher, M. A., Klimas, Metal’nikov, S. & Chorine, V. (1926). Rôle des réflexes conditionnels
N., Duran, R., et al. (2003). Sleep disturbance mediates the associ- dans l¢immunité. Annales de l¢Institut Pasteur, 40, 900.
ation between psychological distress and immune status among

 2009 The Author. Journal compilation  2009 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.
Scand J Psychol 50 (2009) Psychoneuroimmunology 651

Overton, D. A. (1991). Historical context of state dependent learning Zachariae, R., Bjerring, P. & Arendt-Nielsen, L. (1989). Modulation of
and discriminative drug effects. Special issue: Behavioral aspects type I immediate and type IV delayed immunoreactivity using
of drug discrimination. Behavioral Pharmacology, 2, 253–264. direct suggestion and guided imagery during hypnosis. Allergy, 44,
Pedersen, A. D., Rossen, P., Mehlsen, M. Y., Pedersen, C. G., Zacha- 537–542.
riae, R. & von der Maase, H. (2009a). Long-term cognitive function Zachariae, R., Bjerring, P., Arendt-Nielsen, L., Nielsen, T. & Gotlieb-
following chemotherapy in patients with testicular cancer. Journal sen, K. (1991a). The effect of hypnotically induced emotional
of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 296–301. states on brain potentials evoked by painful argon laser stimulation.
Pedersen, A. F., Bovbjerg, D. & Zachariae, R. (2009b). Stress and sus- Clinical Journal of Pain, 7, 130–138.
ceptibility to infectious disease: A review of the literature. In R. J. Zachariae, R., Bjerring, P., Zachariae, C., Arendt-Nielsen, L., Nielsen,
Contrada & A. Baum (Eds.), Handbook of stress science: Psychol- T., Eldrup, E., et al. (1991b). Monocyte chemotactic activity in sera
ogy, biology, and health. New York: Springer (in press). after hypnotically induced emotional states. Scandinavian Journal
Pedersen, A. F., Zachariae, R. & Bovbjerg, D. (2009c). Psychological of Immunology, 34, 71–79.
stress and antibody response to influenza vaccination: A meta- Zachariae, R., Hansen, J. B., Andersen, M., Jinquan, T., Petersen,
analysis. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 23, 427–433. K. S., Simonsen, C., et al. (1994b). Changes in cellular immune
Pedersen, A. F., Zachariae, R., Jensen, A. B., Bovbjerg, D., Andersen, function after immune specific guided imagery and relaxation in
O. & von der Maase, H. (2009d). Psychological stress predicts the high and low hypnotizable healthy subjects. Psychotherapy and
risk of febrile episodes in cancer patients during chemotherapy. Psychosomatics, 61, 74–92.
Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 78, 258–260. Zachariae, R., Jensen, A. B., Pedersen, C., Jorgensen, M. M., Christen-
Raison, C. L. & Miller, A. H. (2003). Depression in cancer: New sen, S., Lassesen, B., et al. (2004). Repressive coping before and
developments regarding diagnosis and treatment. Biological Psychi- after diagnosis of breast cancer. Psychooncology, 13, 547–561.
atry, 54, 283–294. Zachariae, R., Jorgensen, M. M., Bjerring, P. & Svendsen, G. (2000a).
Rasmussen, A. F., Marsh, J. T. & Brill, N. O. (1957). Increased sus- Autonomic and psychological responses to an acute psychological
ceptibility to herpes simplex in mice subjected to avoidance-learn- stressor and relaxation: The influence of hypnotizability and
ing stress or restraint. Proceedings of the Society for Experimental absorption. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Biology and Medicine, 96, 183–189. Hypnosis, 48, 388–403.
Solomon, G. F., Amkraut, A. A. & Kasper, P. (1964). Immunity, emo- Zachariae, R., Jorgensen, M. M. & Christensen, S. (2000b). Hypnotiz-
tions and stress. Annals of Clinical Research, 6, 313–322. ability and absorption in a Danish sample: Testing the influence of
Thomsen, D. K., Mehlsen, M. Y., Christensen, S. & Zachariae, R. context. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypno-
(2003). Rumination – relationship with negative mood and sleep sis, 48, 306–314.
quality. Personality and Individual Differences, 34, 1293–1301. Zachariae, R., Jorgensen, M. M., Christensen, S. & Bjerring, P. (1997).
von Bertalanffy, L. (1969). General systems theory: Foundations, Effects of relaxation on the delayed-type hypersensitivity
development, applications. New York: Brazilier. (DTH) reaction to diphenylcyclopropenone (DCP). Allergy, 52,
Zachariae, R. (1996). Mind and immunity: Psychological modulation 760–764.
of immunological and inflammatory parameters. Copenhagen: Zachariae, R., Jorgensen, M. M., Egekvist, H. & Bjerring, P. (2001).
Munksgaard. Skin reactions to histamine of healthy subjects after hypnotically
Zachariae, R. (2001). Hypnosis and immunity. In R. Ader, D. L. Felten induced emotions of sadness, anger, and happiness. Allergy, 56,
& N. Cohen (Eds.), Psychoneuroimmunology (3rd edn, vol. 2, pp. 734–740.
133–160). New York: Academic Press. Zachariae, R., Oster, H. & Bjerring, P. (1994a). Effects of hypnotic
Zachariae, R., Andersen, O. K., Bjerring, P., Jorgensen, M. M. & suggestions on ultraviolet B radiation-induced erythema and skin
Arendt-Nielsen, L. (1998). Effects of an opioid antagonist on pain blood flow. Photodermatology, Photoimmunology, and Photomedi-
intensity and withdrawal reflexes during induction of hypnotic anal- cine, 10, 154–160.
gesia in high- and low- hypnotizable volunteers. European Journal Zachariae, R., Oster, H., Bjerring, P. & Kragballe, K. (1996). Effects
of Pain, 2, 25–34. of psychologic intervention on psoriasis: A preliminary report.
Zachariae, R. & Bjerring, P. (1990). The effect of hypnotically induced Journal of the American Academy of Dermatology, 34, 1008–1015.
analgesia on flare reaction of the cutaneous histamine prick test. Zachariae, R., Paulsen, K., Mehlsen, M., Jensen, A. B., Johansson, A.
Archives of Dermatological Research, 282, 539–543. & von der Maase, H. (2007a). Anticipatory nausea: The role of
Zachariae, R. & Bjerring, P. (1993). Increase and decrease of delayed individual differences related to sensory perception and autonomic
cutaneous reactions obtained by hypnotic suggestions during sensi- reactivity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33, 69–79.
tization. Studies on dinitrochlorobenzene and diphenylcycloprope- Zachariae, R., Paulsen, K., Mehlsen, M., Jensen, A. B., Johansson, A.
none. Allergy, 48, 6–11. & von der Maase, H. (2007b). Chemotherapy-induced nausea,
Zachariae, R. & Bjerring, P. (1994). Laser-induced pain-related brain vomiting, and fatigue: The role of individual differences related to
potentials and sensory pain ratings in high and low hypnotizable sensory perception and autonomic reactivity. Psychotherapy and
subjects during hypnotic suggestions of relaxation, dissociated Psychosomatics, 76, 376–384.
imagery, focused analgesia, and placebo. International Journal of
Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 42, 56–80. Received 27 July 2009, accepted 29 July 2009

 2009 The Author. Journal compilation  2009 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations.

You might also like