You are on page 1of 4

Spr

SLanguage
pecial as an features
emergent system
Scott Thornbury
How did we acquire our mother tongue? that emergence is more effective than more
Was it like switching on an electronic circuit, interventionist processes, such as formal
hardwired into the brain? Or was it like instruction?
Topsy, who “just growed?” That is to say,
did it emerge as a result of general learning The study of emergence – the idea that
processes responding to massive exposure certain systems are more than the sum of
and driven by the need to belong? The their parts, and that “a small number of
question divides linguists into two opposed rules or laws can generate systems of
camps: the Chomsky-ites, on the one hand, surprising complexity 2 ” – is a relatively new
and the proponents of what are now called branch of science. It is closely associated
usage-based theories of language with complexity theory. A system is said to
acquisition, on the other. And the debate have emergent properties when it displays
has a long history. complexity at a global level that is not
specified at a local level. For example, the
“Die Grammatik kommt aus der Sprache, capacity of an ant colony to react in unison
nicht die Sprache aus der Grammatik” to a threat, or a flock of starlings to swoop
(Grammar comes from speaking, not and dive as if it were a single organism, is
speaking from grammar). In the late the aggregate effect of relatively simple
nineteenth century Gustav Langenscheidt interactions between individual members.
(he of the dictionaries) developed a
teaching method based on that notion. It is These interactions are not co-ordinated in
an early statement of the principle of any centralised way, e.g. by a “leader ant”
language emergence, although it was not or “chief starling”. As John Holland puts it,
known as such as the time. A century later, “Somehow the simple laws of the agents
the idea that language could be learned as generate an emergent behaviour far
part of an ongoing conversation was beyond their individual capacities” 3 .
rehabilitated in a number of forms, Because there is no “central executive”
including task-based learning, community determining the emergent organisation of
language learning, whole-language the system, the patterns and regularities
learning, and the notion of the process that result have been characterised as
syllabus. All had in common the belief that, order for free. In Roger Lewin’s words, “A
in Michael Breen’s words, “The language I fundamental property of complex adaptive
learn in the classroom is a communal systems is the counterintuitive
product derived through a jointly crystallisation of order – order for free 4 .”
constructed process. 1 ” Emergent systems, then, have the following
properties:
Basic to such approaches is the belief that
– given the right conditions – the learner’s
language system will emerge. But, in what
sense is language an emergent system?
And what grounds are there for believing 2
Holland, J. (1998, 2000) Emergence: From
Chaos to Order. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
1 3
Breen, M. (1985). The social context for ibid.
4
language learning – a neglected situation? Lewin, R. (1993). Complexity: Life on the
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 7. edge of chaos. London: Phoenix.

Spring/ Summer 2008


Spr
• they are dynamic and do not • it has different levels of different
always reach a fixed, final state scale sizes: texts, sentences,
• they evolve over time clauses, words, sounds, and a
• they are non-linear, i.e. change change in one of these levels can
doesn’t happen in a one-step-at-a- have an effect across levels. For
time, incremental kind of way, but example, formulaic chunks, like I
often in bursts (or “phase shifts”) wanna, gimme, can be “unpacked”
• they have many interacting parts, into their individual elements, which
often acting in parallel can then provide a grammatical
• they have different levels of template for new combinations;
different scale sizes, and • it displays periods of random
emergence happens across these behaviour, or what is called “free
levels variation”, when the learner
• their elements exhibit a degree of randomly alternates between
autonomy and randomness in their standard and non-standard ways of
behaviour at times saying the same thing;
• they are de-centralised – the order • it is de-centralised: there is no
emerges from the system itself single determining factor, such as
• they are unpredictable and instruction, that “causes” language
surprising! development;
• although there are certain common
Some scholars – notably Diane Larsen- patterns to development across
Freeman, Lynn Cameron, and Nick Ellis – learners, the path, and the pace, of
have been studying language through the learning is often idiosyncratic,
lens of complexity theory, both in the way unpredictable, and surprising.
languages develop in society, and in the
way that a learner’s first language (or the In other words, language development
second learner’s interlanguage) develops displays many of the characteristics of an
over time. They have found some emergent system. As Diane Larsen-
interesting parallels. For example, Freeman puts it: “Language is not fixed, but
is rather a dynamic system. Language
• language development is dynamic, evolves and changes... [it] grows and
and does not always reach a fixed, organises itself from the bottom up in an
final state – it is often highly organic way, as do other complex
variable, and it can also stabilise for systems. 5 ”
long periods of time;
• it evolves over time – often many Tallying, chunking, and priming
years;
• it is non-linear: both L1 and L2 The processes by which language “grows
learners sometimes seem to “get and organises itself” are thought to be
worse” before they get better, as processes that, rather than being language-
when they “regress” from he went specific, are basic to human cognition and
to *he goed, for example; hence learning. As Nick Ellis puts it:
improvement may occur in sudden
bursts;
• it has many interacting parts: there
are different sub-systems at work,
such a phonology, lexis, grammar,
each working in parallel, and each
5
comprising many – sometimes Larsen-Freeman, D. (2006). The emergence
thousands – of different items; of complexity, fluency, and accuracy in the
oral and written production of five Chinese
learners of English. Applied Linguistics, 27/4.

Spring/ Summer 2008


Spr
“Language is cut of the same cloth as other
cognitive processes.” 6 These processes For a start, the intricate associative network
include the following: that has been created for the first language
tends to slow down the forming of new
• tallying: the process whereby associations in a second one, or even to
learners become automatically block them entirely. It is like trying to install
sensitised to the fact that certain a software program on a computer’s hard
elements and combinations of disc without first un-installing a rival
elements occur frequently in the program. Also, the ability to discriminate
language that they are exposed to; sounds in a second language is much less
• pattern extraction: the capacity to sensitive than in the first, where the system
identify and extract regularly was primed at infancy, when the neural
occurring sequences from input; “stuff” was at its most plastic. Because so
• priming: the gradual strengthening, many grammatical distinctions such as:
through repeated use, of verb endings; auxiliaries; articles and
associations between elements, prepositions are phonologically reduced in
such as words and other words, or naturally occurring talk, it is often difficult to
utterances and their contexts; pick them out.
• chunking: the welding together of
sets of already formed associations The good news, though, is that language
into larger units. emergence can be facilitated by some kind
of direct intervention. What form should this
According to this view, the patterns and intervention take? Nick Ellis suggests that
chunks that are extracted from the input are “acquisition can … be speeded by making
rehearsed in short-term memory, and the underlying patterns more salient as a
eventually establish themselves in long- result of explicit instruction or
term memory, where they serve as a consciousness-raising” 8 . That is to say, if
template for the acquisition of grammar. As learners are having trouble identifying and
Nick Ellis puts it: “Learning grammar abstracting patterns, their attention can be
involves abstracting regularities from the purposefully directed at them. This is what
stock of known lexical sequences” 7 . some writers, including Michael Long, have
termed a focus on form. A focus on form
Focus on form “overtly draws students’ attention to
Given enough exposure and use, then, it linguistic elements as they arise incidentally
would seem that language emergence – at in lessons whose overriding focus is on
least first language emergence – is the meaning or communication” 9 .
inevitable result. So, why is this not the
case in second language learning? Sadly, A focus on form, then, aims to redress the
the processes that make first language weaknesses in the second language
acquisition so easy, such as tallying and learner’s innate capacity to notice, tally,
pattern extraction, function far less and abstract patterns from the input, and to
successfully for second language re-use these as output. There are many
acquisition. Why is this? ways that this focus can be engineered.
Traditional error correction is one, although
for some learners this may have negative
6
Ellis, N. (2001) Memory for language. In side-effects, inhibiting the free flow of talk.
Robinson, P. (Ed.) Cognition and Second Re-casting (or reformulating) the learner’s
Language Instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.
7 8
Ellis, N. 1997. Vocabulary acquisition: word Ellis, N. (1997), op. cit.
9
structure, collocation, word-class. In Schmitt, Long. M. (1991) quoted in Doughty, C. and
N., and McCarthy, M. (Eds.) Vocabulary: Williams, J. (eds) (1998) Focus on form in
Description, Acquisition, and Pedagogy. classroom second language acquisition.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Spring/ Summer 2008


Spr
utterances into a more target-like form, as
part of the process of “scaffolding”
conversations, is another. Identifying and
labelling frequently occurring items in texts,
or in transcripts of talk, is yet another.
Writing the record of a conversation, after
the event, can help focus attention on the
non-salient grammatical items that might
have been overlooked in the real-time
interaction. A cycle of task performance
followed by some kind of awareness-raising
activity, followed by a repeat performance
of the task (with different partners, for
example) has been shown to have positive
effects. Even the explicit teaching of
grammatical rules, when the need arises,
can help prime learners to notice
occurrences of the targeted item in
authentic use.

Second language learning, then, probably


shares many of the emergent properties of
first language acquisition, and, if left to its
own devices, will “just grow”. But for most
learners, direct intervention on the part of
the teacher is necessary, if the full potential
of this complex and dynamic system is to
be realised.

Scott Thornbury teaches an on-line MA


TESOL course for the New School
University, New York. He recently wrote An
A-Z of ELT (Macmillan) and is the series
editor of the Cambridge Handbooks for
Language Teachers (CUP).

Spring/ Summer 2008

You might also like