You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Biological Education

ISSN: 0021-9266 (Print) 2157-6009 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rjbe20

Students’ arguments on the science and religion


issue: the example of evolutionary theory and
Genesis

Nicolai Basel, Ute Harms, Helmut Prechtl, Thomas Weiß & Martin
Rothgangel

To cite this article: Nicolai Basel, Ute Harms, Helmut Prechtl, Thomas Weiß & Martin Rothgangel
(2014) Students’ arguments on the science and religion issue: the example of evolutionary theory
and Genesis, Journal of Biological Education, 48:4, 179-187, DOI: 10.1080/00219266.2013.849286

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00219266.2013.849286

Published online: 04 Nov 2013.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 783

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 2 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=rjbe20

Download by: [179.210.72.254] Date: 18 February 2017, At: 11:44


Journal of Biological Education, 2014 179
Vol. 48, No. 4, 179–187, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00219266.2013.849286

Research paper
Students’ arguments on the science
and religion issue: the example of
evolutionary theory and Genesis
Nicolai Basel a, Ute Harmsa, Helmut Prechtl b,
Thomas Weißc and Martin Rothgangelc
a
Department of Biology Education, Leibniz-Institute (IPN) for Science and Mathematics Education at
the University of Kiel, Kiel, Germany; bDepartment of Biology Education, Institute for Biological
Chemistry and Biology at the University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany; cInstitute for Religious
Education, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria

Treating creationism as a controversial topic within the science and religion issue in the science classroom has
been widely discussed in the recent literature. Some researchers have proposed that this topic is best addressed
by focusing on sociocognitive conflict. To prepare new learning opportunities for this approach, it is necessary
to know the concrete arguments that students use in their discussions on this issue. Therefore, this study aimed
to provide a systematic description of these arguments. For this purpose, upper secondary students (N = 43)
argued for either the acceptance of evolutionary theory or faith in Genesis in a written speech. The study was
conducted during their regular biology and religious education classes. Generated arguments were analysed by
qualitative content analysis. Three dimensions of the arguments were described: the content (science or reli-
gion), the valuation of the argument (positive or negative), and whether the argument consisted of a descriptive
or normative argumentation. The results indicate that students found it easier to generate arguments about the
scientific side of the issue; however, these arguments were negatively constructed. The results are discussed with
regard to implications for educational approaches for teaching controversial issues at the high-school level.

Keywords: evolutionary theory; argumentation; science/religion issue; controversial issues

Introduction
The results of recent empirical studies have implied start college. Therefore, the authors rated its
that more attention should be paid to creationism in inclusion in high-school biology courses as
Europe both in and out of the classroom (eg Miller, ‘educational malpractice’ (Moore 2009, 95). In
Scott, and Okamoto 2006). Therefore, several contrast, Foster (2012, 2176) emphasised that it is
authors have advised that students be confronted necessary for students to ‘know what is not true as
with their misconceptions about evolutionary theory well as what is’ and concluded that creationism
(eg Evans 2008). Discussion has ensued regarding should be treated as any other misconception in the
whether and how teachers in the science classroom classroom. Although there is much literature regard-
should deal with myth-based misconceptions (Alters ing students’ views on science and religion, as well as
and Nelson 2002) about the origin and development on the reasons for the non-acceptance of scientific
of life. With regard to evolutionary theory in the concepts such as the theory of evolution, only a few
USA, Moore and Cotner (2009) found that if studies (eg Dagher and BouJaoude 1996) have
students are explicitly confronted with creationism in focused on the arguments that students incorporate
the science classroom, the chance that they accept into discussions dealing with this issue. To make
creationism and reject evolution increases when they discussions more ‘meaningful’ (Foster 2012) in the

Nicolai Basel, Department of Biology Education, Leibniz-Institute (IPN) for Science and Mathematics Education at the
University of Kiel, Kiel, Germany. Email: basel@ipn.uni-kiel.de
Ó 2013 Society of Biology
180 N. BASEL ET AL. _________________________________________________________________________________________________

classroom, teachers and researchers should know issue – by detachment, reconciliation and confrontation.
about the arguments that students use. This will Detachment means showing the explanatory power of
enable teachers to better foster classroom discussion evolutionary theory and detaching it from the reli-
and to increase students’ understanding of the nature gious explanation, reconciliation means searching for
of science (NOS) and scientific concepts. For this consistencies between the two approaches (eg theistic
reason, this study focused on students’ arguments on evolution), and confrontation means confronting the
the issue of science and religion, exemplified by the scientific knowledge with the religious views. What
relation between evolutionary theory and Genesis. all of these approaches have in common is that
evolutionary theory is seen as a more or less
controversial issue (eg Skehan and Nelson 2000), as
Literature review
From an epistemological point of view, the differences (a) two opposing groups exist, (b) the controversy
between science and religion are quite clear: science has led to heated debates, (c) the answer is not
with its methodological materialism is concerned with luminously clear to all reasonable people and (d) the
the natural world and how it works. Religion is knowledge of evolution is controversial knowledge
another approach to reality, dealing with its ritual, because there is acknowledged uncertainty
emotional and mythic dimensions (eg Reiss 2009). [especially for students, author’s note] and disagree-
Therefore, as one possibility for science education, it ment surrounding the evolution/creation debate.
has been proposed that science and religion be treated (Hermann 2008, 1027)
as two separate non-overlapping magisteria (NOMA)
(Gould 1997). This means that both domains have dif- Following Reiss (1992), Hermann (2008) proposes
ferent and not overlapping legitimate teaching author- different possibilities for how science teachers can
ities. From an educational standpoint, this separation deal with the science and religion issue in classroom
should help students to avoid contradictions in their discussion in practice: advocacy, when teachers argue
minds concerning the knowledge and epistemologies for their own position; affirmative neutrality, when
of the different disciplines (Smith and Scharmann they offer multiple perspectives on the issue without
2008; Smith 2010). It is obvious that this apparently uncovering their own position; and procedural
clear distinction between science and religion does not neutrality, when teachers let students define their own
reflect all aspects of the two disciplines and does not opinion with the help of external resources.
exist in students’ minds. A review of the research on Some of these approaches have been realised on
the issue shows that, along with other cognitive different educational levels and for different student
(eg understanding) and affective (eg attitudes towards ages, with a focus either on the scientific concepts
science) factors, students’ religiosity is a prevalent fac- (eg Andrews, Kalinowski, and Leonard 2011) or on
tor influencing their acceptance or non-acceptance of concepts and the students’ understanding of the
evolutionary theory (for a review, see Allmon 2011; NOS (eg differences between theories and laws, the
Wiles and Alters 2011). Apart from a non-acceptance creativity behind science, the sociocultural embedd-
of the scientific concept, the problems that occur are edness of science, the empirical basis of science)
what Alters and Nelson (2002, 1895) summarised as (Lederman et al. 2002). In high school, Duveen and
‘religious and myth-based’ misconceptions; for exam- Solomon (1994) practised a role play (a trial) for
ple, a belief that ‘the earth is too young for evolution teaching different aspects of the NOS in the context
(and most geological processes) to have occurred’. Stu- of evolutionary theory (eg social influence on science,
dents seem to prefer either one or the other domain, as the nature of scientific theories) with 15–16-year-old
Taber and colleagues (2010) were able to show in their students. Using this kind of role-playing game, in
qualitative approach when they examined contradic- their opinion, ‘students not only learn science, but
tion and conflict between science and religion in 13– about science’ (Duveen and Solomon 1994, 581),
14-year-old students. In particular, there are conse- which Foster (2012) summarised as ‘show, not tell’
quences for the learning of scientific content, as only students about how science is done and what the
those students who take a position that accepts the nature of scientific debate is. To overcome religious-
compatibility of the two domains seem to be able to based misconceptions, Jensen and Finley (1997) used
learn without conflict (Yasri and Mancy 2012). pair discussions of different evolutionary problems
When summarising students’ views on the science and, after discussion, showed an increase in
and religion issue, it becomes obvious that this issue ‘Darwinian ideas’ and a decrease in ‘non-Darwinian
creates a lot of controversy. Different educational ideas’ (eg ‘natural theology’, consisting of an under-
approaches have been proposed for dealing with this standing that ‘God had created a delicate balance in
conflict in the science classroom. Thagard and nature’: Jensen and Finley 1997, 209).
Findlay (2010) summarised three ways in which the As an example of procedural neutrality, in the
conflict between evolutionary theory and creation Science and Religion in Schools Project (2006, cited
stories is addressed – in contrast to avoiding the in Reiss 2008), comparable to the study by Duveen
____________________________________________________________________________ RESEARCH PAPER 181

and Solomon (1994), certain positions are presented the argumentative process and therefore on the
to students. Here, the aim is to make students arguments that students incorporate in their
understand ‘why, for cultural reasons, the theory of discussions to resolve their cognitive conflict and to
evolution has been so controversial for some people’ determine whether they are able to present varied
(Edexcel 2005, 31, cited in Reiss 2008) to make opposing views and arguments about the problem.
them respect others’ opinions. It is obvious that this Furthermore, it is necessary to analyse which
aim is closely related to Reiss’ understanding of conceptions students include in their discussions on
creationism as a world view (Cobern 1996). Finally, the science and religion issue, especially when they
with a focus on teacher–student interaction in a start learning about evolutionary theory and have
project-based learning unit, Oliveira and colleagues little previous knowledge about either side of the
(2011) analysed a teacher taking a neutral position in issue. Therefore, our research questions were:
the discussion, framing student discussion with a
focus on respect, politeness and humour. With the • Which arguments do students use when discussing
exception of this last study, the cited studies have in the science and religion issue in the context of
common that they focused on the outcome of the evolutionary theory at the upper secondary level?
discussion. • What are the most frequent arguments that occur
The results of the outlined studies in a wider sense in the students’ statements for or against the
all show that discussion can foster conceptual change. acceptance of evolutionary theory and in relation
The main reasons for this could be the occurrence of to a positive or negative attitude towards science?
cognitive conflict (eg Limón 2001) – a conflict
between students’ individual prior knowledge and
the viewpoints presented during discussion – and Methods
students’ deep conceptual engagement during To record and systematise students’ arguments on the
argumentation (Nussbaum and Sinatra 2003). Here, science and religion issue, we chose a qualitative
formally spoken counterargument and rebuttal are approach. We decided to confront students directly
evidently essential: research indicates that classroom with the science and religion issue and randomly
discussion can be more effective in the case of the assigned them to two groups, taking the role of a
occurrence of dialectical argumentation, meaning theologian (task A) or an evolutionary biologist (task
that at least two opposing views are presented during B). In each case, the task was to argue in a written
discussion (Asterhan and Schwarz 2009). Bringing speech from the particular standpoint either for the
together research on argumentation and on the sci- acceptance of evolutionary theory (biologist) or for
ence and religion issue, it is obvious that students the belief in Genesis (theologian). With this kind of
should have the opportunity to argue and, through task we did not explicitly ask them to take a position
that practice, to choose (Yasri and Mancy 2012) and that would either be scientistic (ie including a rejection
make an informed decision on their own as to why of the religious idea of a Genesis) or creationist (ie
evolutionary theory, in contrast to its unscientific with an opposition to evolutionary theory). Thus, it
alternatives, is a scientifically valid theory. In the was possible to create positions where evolutionary
end, being able to make such decisions is an expres- theory and Genesis were compatible (Yasri and
sion of scientific literacy (eg Kolstø 2001). Therefore, Mancy 2012). Using this kind of task, the students
classroom discussion is a necessary condition with had the opportunity to decide for themselves how
regard to controversial issues in particular (Duschl controversial the issue was in their view. In addition,
and Osborne 2002). Foster (2012) argued for treating students received an excerpt from a textbook on
creationism in the classroom by inducing a sociocog- religious education (Rupp and Reinert 2004, 27)
nitive conflict. He concluded with open questions that included a speech from the physician Sir Arthur
about the areas in which further research is needed Eddington in which he tries, using a parable as an
to make these discussions meaningful. One of his example, to oppose science and religion by showing
questions was whether this approach could be differences in their epistemologies and arguing for a
effective in high school, before the university level clear separation of the two domains.
(Foster 2012, 2177). According to Grace (2009), We chose this kind of role-playing task to ensure
high-quality decision making takes place when a large variety of arguments. Before the task was used
different arguments and counterarguments are at the in our study, it was field tested with five students
disposition of the participants, and the final decision from upper secondary classes and evaluated by five
is based on the consideration of alternatives. biology, religious education and German teachers.
The teachers confirmed that students generally had
the necessary foreknowledge to work on the task,
Objective and research questions that the necessary language skills were available, and
Hence, to answer Foster’s question with regard to that they would be able to solve the problem in the
the high-school level, it is first necessary to focus on time provided (90 minutes).
182 N. BASEL ET AL. _________________________________________________________________________________________________

Forty-three students in grades 11 and 12 who at


most had undertaken a short-term course on evolu-
tionary theory in middle school (grades 7–10) were
presented with the task. They had also taken a course
on Genesis in religious education, which is part of
the general German school curriculum. The students
attended two secondary schools from suburban areas
in Berlin and Brandenburg in Germany. The
students were 16–19 years old (female: 62.7%; male:
37.3%). Two students were exempt from paying for
school books. Students were administered the task
during their regular biology lessons or religious
education. They were informed that participation
was voluntary and that their results would have no
influence on their final grades. Tasks A and B were
Figure 1. Simplified model for the
distributed equally across the two samples and classification of students’ arguments on
lessons, and randomly assigned to students within the science and religion issue
each group. A translation of the task and the text
that were given to the students is available from the
first author. (Science and evolutionary theory or Religion, faith and
Genesis) and the conceptions included; second,
according to the direction of the argument for or
Data analysis
against the addressed content; and third, according to
To identify students’ arguments on evolutionary whether the content consisted of a descriptive
theory and Genesis, we applied a qualitative content (rationalistic or knowledge-based reasoning) or
analysis (Mayring 2000) supported by the current normative (value-based or emotive reasoning) argu-
version of MaxQDA software. Students’ written mentation. This last distinction was adapted from
speeches on the science and religion issue were tran- other controversial topics such as socioscientific issues
scribed and reformulated, if the quality of the writing which distinguish between rationalistic and emotive
made it necessary. The argumentative parts of the informal reasoning patterns (eg Sadler and Zeidler
speeches were then identified according to Walton’s 2005).
(2006) definition of arguments. In this definition, In agreement with the controversy that is often
arguments are the reasons that are used to support or implied by the science and religion issue and in order
to criticise a certain claim or conclusion. The prere- to distinguish between positive and negative
quisite for an argumentation is a doubt attached to argumentations, we chose to arrange the arguments
the proposition held by the arguer (eg Basel, Harms, into five main categories according to their nature
and Prechtl 2013). In our example, this condition and content. In the first main category we
was met by the controversy implicated in the task. summarised positive arguments for the acceptance of
Afterwards, categories for the analysis of students’ scientific concepts and the expression of positive
arguments were developed inductively according to attitudes towards science in general. In the second
the research questions, and included the category’s category negative arguments for the rejection of
name, its definition and at least one typical example scientific concepts and criticisms of science (eg
(‘anchor example’). Categories were revised if they restriction of its methods) were summarised. By con-
included the same content and could therefore be trast, positive arguments for the acceptance of faith
combined into larger main categories, or they were in general and the belief in Genesis in particular and
separated if their content was not consistent. Finally, negative arguments aiming to criticise faith, religion
the interrater reliability of categorical systems was and the Church were summarised in the main
tested on more than 10% of the material and showed categories III and IV. The last group contained
an approximate 91% agreement for two raters (first arguments that were used to express neutrality or
and second authors). All problems were resolved were used affirmatively for both sides of the issue. A
through discussion by the raters. description of the most frequently used categories is
presented in Table 1.
Two-hundred and eighteen argumentations were
Results identified in the texts. In general, students (N = 43)
Main categories were able to express more arguments on the
Using the developed categories, arguments could be scientific content (main categories I and II; 113
described using three dimensions (Figure 1): first, arguments, 51.9%) than on the religious one (main
according to the content addressed by the argument categories III and IV; seventy-two arguments, 33%).
____________________________________________________________________________ RESEARCH PAPER 183

Table 1. Main category labels and number of occurrences

Main category Category labels d/n No. % Code

I. Science and evolutionary theory: Appeal to scientific evidence – explicit d 10 4.6 I.1
positive arguments Appeal to scientific evidence – implicit d 7 3.2 I.2
Appeal to high quality of scientific methods d 2 0.9 I.3
Conceptual quality of evolutionary theory n 2 0.9 I.4
Benefits of science n 2 0.9 I.5
Total 23 10.6%
II. Science and evolutionary theory: Limitations of evolutionary theory d 23 10.6 II.1
negative arguments Limitations of scientific inquiry d 20 9.2 II.2
Cognitive obstacles n 15 6.9 II.3
Effects of evolutionary theory on humans n 12 5.5 II.4
Others – 20 9.2 –
Total 90 41.3%
III. Religion, faith and Genesis: Benefits of faith and religion n 29 13.3 III.1
positive arguments Explanations in the Bible are more plausible d 5 2.3 III.2
Personal value n 3 1.4 III.3
Number of believers d 2 0.9 III.4
Others – 2 0.9 –
Total 41 18.8%
IV. Religion, faith and Genesis: No scientific evidence for creation d 11 5.0 IV.1
negative arguments Scientific evidence refutes creation d 7 3.2 IV.2
Bible is distorted by humans n 5 2.3 IV.3
Lack of open-mindedness of the Church n 5 2.3 IV.4
Conflicts as a result of faith n 3 1.4 IV.5
Total 31 14.2%
V. Neutral arguments Relation between humans and apes d/n 15 6.9 V.1
Freedom of belief n 14 6.4 V.2
‘Agnosticism’ d 4 1.8 V.3
Total 33 15.1%
218 100%
Note: d = descriptive; n = normative; in the category ‘Others’ are summarised arguments that occurred with limited frequency. They are described in the
Results section.

This difference was statistically significant (Wilcoxon Detailed findings


signed-rank test: n = 43, z = –2.041, p < 0.05). In In this section we want to exemplify the categories
the group of arguments on science, students and show the relations of the arguments to students’
produced more negative arguments concerning prior knowledge with regard to their understanding
evolutionary theory and science in general (ninety of the nature of science and theology, as this may
arguments, 41.3%) than positive ones (twenty-three explain the particular rationale of the different
arguments, 10.6%). This distinction was also signifi- arguments.
cant (Wilcoxon signed-rank test: n = 43, z = –3.145,
p < 0.01). By contrast, positive arguments concerning Positive arguments concerning science and
Genesis and faith or religion in general were more
evolutionary theory
frequent (forty-one arguments, 18.8%), whereas neg-
ative arguments about these contents did not occur In this category, students used scientific evidence by
very frequently (thirty-one arguments, 14.2%). mentioning this evidence explicitly through scientific
Although there was this observable difference, it was methods and results (Code I.1); for example, DNA
not significant (Wilcoxon signed-rank test: n = 43, analysis to explain the close relation between humans
z = –0.961, p = 0.34). The category of neutral and apes or the existence of transitional fossils to
arguments consisted of thirty-three arguments (main support evolutionary theory:
category V; 15.1%). A chi-squared test was used to
Nowadays, we’ve got the required auxiliary material
analyse the relationship between content (Science and
to compare the DNA of animated beings. The
Religion) and normative or descriptive argumentation.
similarity of the different DNA of all animals could
There was a significant association between Science be determined by further experiments. Therefore,
and descriptive argumentation and Religion and the evolutionary model, which was based on fossils,
normative argumentation [w2(1) = 60.314, p < 0.001]. could be confirmed. (Student b11)
184 N. BASEL ET AL. _________________________________________________________________________________________________

In contrast to explicit statements, students likewise development of cognitive abilities; for example,
referred to scientific evidence implicitly (Code I.2) ‘What is not said, is […] how this incredible intelli-
and used science as a kind of authority, ‘we all know gence of humans, the ability to think and feel
that there is enough scientific evidence to support emerged’ (Student a10) and does not respect the
evolutionary theory. This can’t be challenged’ special position of the human being. From an argu-
(Student A5) or ‘this has been proven by science and mentative point of view, many of these arguments
can’t be questioned’ (Student A3). criticising science and rejecting evolutionary theory
Although other arguments that students used in could be interpreted as problematic ‘arguments from
support of evolutionary theory were rare, the follow- ignorance’ (eg Walton 1996), as these are common
ing ones (I.3–I.5) underline the relevance of an fallacies of intelligent design approaches (eg Wilkens
understanding of the NOS and a positive attitude and Elsberry 2001).
towards science, which are both important for an In contrast to these descriptive arguments, students
acceptance of evolutionary theory (eg Lombrozo, expressed doubts about the intelligibility of
Thanukos, and Weisberg 2008; Akyol et al. 2012). evolutionary theory, which was valued as ‘hard to
In our sample, for instance, students referred to the understand’ or ‘not plausible’ (II.3), such as certain
‘improvement of scientific methods’ and the ‘nature concepts as the ‘role of chance’, ‘complexity’ and
of scientific knowledge’ (Student B27) as support for ‘diversity originating from simplicity’. In accordance
evolutionary theory. It has to be pointed out that with the systematisation of Thagard and Findlay
these arguments, which show a deeper understanding (2010), we summarised them as ‘cognitive obstacles’
of the NOS, were rare. In contrast to these descrip- in the understanding and the acceptance of
tive lines of argumentation, students also expressed evolutionary theory. Roughly, these can be seen as
normative arguments indicating that evolutionary ‘arguments from incredulity’ (Plutynski 2010)
theory should be viewed positively. This was done because evolutionary theory proceeds in dimensions
either by the consideration of a certain ‘logic’ or that students cannot imagine or understand.
‘consistency’ (Student b06) in the theory, leading to Another common theme that appeared in students’
its acceptance and a rejection of a literal understand- speeches consisted of ‘appeals to the bad conse-
ing of Genesis (see ‘Negative arguments concerning quences’ and the influence of evolutionary theory on
religion, faith and Genesis’, below), or by consider- everyday life. These particularly normative arguments
ing the positive effects of science as ‘science provides mentioned the effects of evolutionary theory on
progress’ (Student a10). These can be seen as expres- human beings and their cohabitation (II.4), such as
sions of a positive attitude towards science and its social Darwinism and the negative effects of science.
benefits (eg Astley and Francis 2010). Overall, taking Brem, Ramney, and Schindel (2003) described these
the number of justifications as an indicator for a as negative attitudes towards evolution that lead to a
higher quality of argumentation (eg Nussbaum and rejection of the theory in the observation of small
Edwards 2011), the small number of arguments in group discussion. Furthermore, these students added
this category could be interpreted as evidence that the ‘senselessness’ of research (in relation to a valua-
students seem to have problems arguing in support tion of believing as ‘sense-giving’) and the negative
of evolutionary theory. consequences of the sciences (eg loss of values) to the
list of negative items they attributed to evolutionary
theory. In these cases, these could be interpreted as
Negative arguments concerning science
arguments from a certain anti-intellectual standpoint
and evolutionary theory (Thagard and Findlay 2010). Furthermore, the argu-
In contrast to the first main category, students ments hidden in the Others category are arguments in
expressed far more arguments against evolutionary that students referred to hasty or wrong generalisa-
theory than for it. As expected, when they took the tions that may be made in science. These are
position of the theologian they scrutinised evolution- arguments that are also reported for creationist stand-
ary theory by using diverse lines of argumentation. points. Other typical creationist arguments, such as
To this end, students frequently referred either to the ‘missing links’ in the fossil record or the ‘just a
the limitations of evolutionary theory as a way to theory’ (eg Blancke et al. 2010) argument, rarely
explain the world and the diversity in it (II.1) or occurred.
more generally to the limitations of scientific inquiry
(II.2).
In the case of evolutionary theory, they argued Positive arguments concerning religion,
either that it cannot explain the beginning – ‘life had faith and Genesis
to be generated to start up evolution’ (Student A4) When the students in the sample argued for the
or ‘… but even the first particles have to come from religion side of the issue with regard to believing in
somewhere’ (Student A24) – or that evolutionary Genesis, they especially emphasised the benefits of
theory cannot explain the human mind and the faith and religion (III.1) in general. These normative
____________________________________________________________________________ RESEARCH PAPER 185

value-based arguments consisted of a summary of the is. The author of the Bible is a person. […] People
diverse positive effects of faith and religion for may have written everything down backwards’
human cohabitation. These were used, for example, (Student a8).
by mentioning that faith gives ‘values and order’ Finally, comparable to the negative arguments
(Students A1, a2), ‘hope’ (a4), ‘strength’ (b10) or concerning evolutionary theory (eg II.4), students
‘sense’ (b10), although this last one was mostly expressed arguments criticising the Church directly
connected to negative conclusions about science. for a certain ‘lack of cosmopolitanism’ (IV.4). These
Furthermore, when arguing for Genesis, students arguments were connected with the association
rated the explanations in the Bible as more plausible between faith and a certain ‘unworldliness’ of the
(III.2). These pragmatic arguments indicate that Church or they exposed conflicts as a result of faith
biblical explanations are easier to understand, as in (IV.5). Faith could give rise to conflicts that
this example, which begins with an argument against culminate in religious wars, martyrdom, etc: ‘We kill
the mass of scientific information (II.3): people, animals and plants […] often this is done in
the name of God’ (Student a10).
Someone who wants to form an opinion on this
topic is overwhelmed by the number of scientific
facts [about something] which is unimaginably big, Neutral arguments
happened an unimaginably long time ago and is The descent of the human race from modern apes as
beyond all imagination of everyday life. Religion is our ancestors was used as an argument both for and
more familiar. (Student a7)
against evolutionary theory (V.1). It occurred in
many texts as a descriptive statement in support of
Other arguments highlight the personal value (III.3)
evolutionary theory, but in single texts it was even
of faith and belief as a subjective perspective that can
used to criticise evolution, although these arguments
be interpreted personally by everyone and should
were based on a misconception: ‘Evolutionary theory
therefore not be criticised, as ‘each person decides says that people are descendants of apes […] but both
for him- or herself which conception of creation he
exist. This is not logical and contradicts itself for that
or she has’ (B3). Finally, students referred to the reason’ (Student A4).
large number of believers (III.4), which was in their
In the second category of arguments for both sides
opinion a justifiable reason to believe and represents of the issue, students normatively referred to
an argument from popular opinion (Walton 1996):
democratic values such as freedom of belief (V.2)
‘Millions of believers share my opinion and they trust and freedom of opinion: ‘The right to freedom of
in the Holy Scripture, which was handed down by
belief is irrevocable’, or,
other believers’ (Student 28A).
however, we live in a democracy. Every person can
choose to believe in what he or she wants. We are
Negative arguments concerning religion, free to design our life to our own ideas and to live
faith and Genesis out our faith and rituals the way we want to.
As students already used scientific evidence in (Student B10)
support of evolutionary theory, this kind of evidence
was used in these descriptive arguments in two The last category of arguments that was used by
different ways, either concluding with the rejection students in support of both sides of the issue was
of Genesis as a scientific report by mentioning that ‘agnosticism’ (V.3). It was a frequent creationist
there is no scientific evidence for creation, and that argument that was used with the question of: ‘Were
is why it cannot be true (IV.1 ‘This is why I have to you there?’ It illustrates the opinion that people can-
say that the biblical creation is lacking in my not be certain about the origin of the world. ‘In the
opinion, as there is no evidence for it’: Student b2), end, we come from the point that the truth does not
or by emphasising that scientific evidence refutes exist. At least it is not attainable’ (Student B27).
creation by a metaphysical power (IV.2 ‘Creation by
God can be rejected completely because scientific
evidence refutes it’: Student a4). Discussion
Concerning normative arguments, other kinds of In this exploratory study we wanted to analyse
arguments against a literal understanding of Genesis students’ arguments on the science and religion issue
consisted of arguments that the Bible is distorted by by considering the number of occurrences of certain
people (IV.3) as it was handed down from person to arguments in the sample, the directions of these
person for thousands of years. It was written down, arguments (positive or negative) and the conceptions
translated and thereby significantly altered. These that students incorporated into them. The results can
arguments concluded that the Bible did not contain be summarised as follows. Students in our sample
true knowledge. ‘This is exactly where the mistake made more arguments involving evolutionary theory
186 N. BASEL ET AL. _________________________________________________________________________________________________

and science than involving their religious counter- discourse could occur, such as in small group or pair
parts, even though these arguments mostly involved discussions on the issue (eg Shipman et al. 2002). As
negative conclusions about evolution. Concerning the aim here was to use a standardised method and
both content areas, the students constructed descrip- the same starting point for the whole group of stu-
tive and normative arguments. With regard to scien- dents to produce arguments, we abstained from the
tific content, descriptive argumentation prevailed; for natural discourse method. To validate and to be able
religious content, normative argumentation was pre- to generalise our results, we would have to control
dominant. In these argumentations, students argued for the occurrence of arguments in such settings.
using scientific facts (eg categories I.1, I.2, IV.1, Despite these restrictions, it is remarkable that the
IV.2) and using either the quality of the scientific students in our sample were able to discuss the prob-
method (I.3) or the limitations of its significance lem of evolutionary theory and Genesis. Although
(II.1, II.2), but also norms and values (eg II.4, III.3). they had only restricted educational experience with
The arguments revealed both the importance of the both concepts, they produced a great deal of variabil-
NOS and the importance of background knowledge, ity in the types of arguments. They generated a
ie an understanding of scientific information. variety of arguments that could provide starting
Furthermore, their arguments revealed the relevance points for new argumentative learning opportunities,
of a diverse set of factors that influence students’ argu- such as small group discussions about specific argu-
ments and that are related to the acceptance of evolu- ments (eg the negative arguments about evolutionary
tionary theory (eg Deniz, Donnelly, and Yilmaz 2008). theory). Our study showed which were the students’
We must acknowledge certain limitations of the preferred arguments and counterarguments on the
study methods concerning the influence of these fac- scientific and the religious sides of the issue. Previous
tors. First, the students did not discuss their own studies that examined meaningful discussions among
views of the problem. Although taking a position is preservice teachers showed effects on the views of
a method that has been used with other controversial the participants (Shipman et al. 2002; Hegelson et al.
issues (eg socioscientific argumentation) and in other 2002); therefore, one might assume that this effect
contexts (learning opportunities in other subjects), could be replicated with students as long as arguments
we have to renounce a deeper analysis of individua- are available to them. Our study showed that students
lised argumentation skills. By using plausibility (eg at the secondary level have the necessary knowledge
Walton 2006) as an indicator of argument quality, to discuss the science and religion issue. Accordingly,
we tried to resolve this problem by applying an addi- counterargumentation, which has been successfully
tional analysis. The results of this study will be applied in everyday contexts at lower levels of educa-
reported in a following article. Plausibility provides tion (eg Goldstein, Crowell, and Kuhn 2009), could be
information regarding the reasonability of the propo- one of the central features of teaching when addressing
sition. To assess its plausibility, ‘one needs to assess the science and religion issue.
the reliability of the source, be it evidence, other
propositions, an expert and so on’ (Nussbaum, 2011,
90). This aim can be met through the use of critical References
Akyol, G., C. Tekkaya, S. Sungur, and A. Traynor. 2012. “Modeling the
questions related to a particular kind of argument (eg
Interrelationships among Pre-Service Science Teachers’ Understanding
Walton 1996). In this analysis, we were able to show and Acceptance of Evolution, Their Views on Nature of Science and
that the degree of plausibility highly depended on Self-Efficacy Beliefs Regarding Teaching Evolution.” Journal of Science
the acceptability of the arguments (ie the conceptual Teacher Education 23 (8): 937–957.
Allmon, W. 2011. “Why Don’t People Think Evolution is True?
quality of students’ arguments) and relevance of the Implications for Teaching, in and out of the Classroom.” Evolution:
arguments for argumentation in the domain (ie reli- Education and Outreach 4 (4): 648–665.
gion or science). Alters, B. J., and C. E. Nelson. 2002. “Perspective: Teaching Evolution in
Higher Education.” Evolution 56 (10): 1891–1901.
Secondly, in our analysis, we did not consider Andrews, T., S. Kalinowski, and M. Leonard. 2011. “‘Are Humans
potential factors of influence (eg students’ religiosity) Evolving?’ a Classroom Discussion to Change Student Misconceptions
because we focused instead on the variance and regarding Natural Selection.” Evolution: Education and Outreach 4 (3):
456–466.
frequency of the arguments. In future studies it
Asterhan, C. S. C., and B. B. Schwarz. 2009. “Argumentation and
would be advisable to measure other potential factors Explanation in Conceptual Change: Indications from Protocol Analyses
of influence to analyse the relation between certain of Peer-to-Peer Dialog.” Cognitive Science 33 (3): 374–400.
arguments and whether the student accepts or Astley, J., and L. J. Francis. 2010. “Promoting Positive Attitudes toward
Science and Religion among Sixth-Form Pupils: Dealing with Scientism
understands evolutionary theory. Thirdly, we and Creationism.” British Journal of Religious Education 32 (3): 189–200.
provided students with a text on the issue. Although Basel, N., U. Harms, and H. Prechtl. 2013. “Analysis of Students’ Argu-
in our opinion it was a neutral text, we cannot ments on Evolutionary Theory.” Journal of Biological Education.
doi: 10.1080/00219266.2013.799078.
exclude the possibility that it influenced students’ Blancke, S., M. Boudry, J. Braeckman, J. De Smedt, and H. De Cruz.
arguments (eg the large number of negative 2010. “Dealing with Creationist Challenges: What European Biology
arguments concerning Science). Finally, we did not Teachers Might Expect in the Classroom.” Journal of Biological Education
45 (4): 176–182.
analyse the arguments in a situation in which natural
____________________________________________________________________________ RESEARCH PAPER 187

Brem, S. K., M. Ranney, and J. Schindel. 2003. “Perceived Consequences Nussbaum, E. M. 2011. “Argumentation, Dialogue Theory, and Probability
of Evolution: College Students Perceive Negative Personal and Social Modeling: Alternative Frameworks for Argumentation Research in Edu-
Impact in Evolutionary Theory.” Science Education 87 (2): 181–206. cation.” Educational Psychologist. 46 (2): 84–106.
Cobern, W. 1996. “Worldview Theory and Conceptual Change in Science Nussbaum, E. M., and O. V. Edwards. 2011. “Critical Questions and
Education.” Science Education 80: 579–610. Argument Stratagems: A Framework for Enhancing and Analyzing
Dagher, Z., and S. BouJaoude. 1997. “Scientific Views and Religious Students’ Reasoning Practices.” Journal of the Learning Sciences 20 (3):
Beliefs of College Students: The Case of Biological Evolution.” Journal 443–488.
of Research in Science Teaching 34 (5): 429–445. Nussbaum, E. M., and G. M. Sinatra. 2003. “Argument and Conceptual
Deniz, H., L. A. Donnelly, and I. Yilmaz. 2008. “Exploring the Factors Engagement.” Contemporary Educational Psychology 28 (3): 384–395.
Related to Acceptance of Evolutionary Theory among Turkish Oliveira, A. W., K. Cook, and G. A. Buck. 2011. “Framing Evolution
Preservice Biology Teachers: Toward a More Informative Conceptual Discussion Intellectually.” Journal of Research in Science Teaching 48 (3):
Ecology for Biological Evolution.” Journal of Research in Science Teaching 257–280.
45 (4): 420–443. Plutynski, A. 2010. “Should Intelligent Design Be Taught in Public School
Duschl, R. A., and J. Osborne. 2002. “Supporting and Promoting Science Classrooms?” Science & Education 19 (6–8): 779–795.
Argumentation Discourse in Science Education.” Studies in Science Reiss, M. 1992. “How Should Science Teachers Teach the Relationship
Education 38 (1): 39–72. between Science and Religion?” School Science Review 74: 127–129.
Duveen, J., and J. Solomon. 1994. “The Great Evolution Trial: Use of Reiss, M. J. 2008. “Should Science Educators Deal with the Science/
Role-Play in the Classroom.” Journal of Research in Science Teaching 31: Religion Issue?” Studies in Science Education 44 (2): 157–186.
575–582. Reiss, M. J. 2009. “The Relationship between Evolutionary Biology and
Evans, E. M. 2008. “Conceptual Change and Evolutionary Biology: A Religion.” Evolution 63 (7): 1934–1941.
Developmental Analysis.” In International Handbook of Research on Rupp, H., and A. Reinert. 2004. Kursbuch Religion Oberstufe: Schülerbuch.
Conceptual Change, edited by S. Vosniadou, 263–294. New York: [Course book for Religious Education in Upper Secondary School. Stu-
Routledge. dents’ Book]. Stuttgart: Calwer.
Foster, C. 2012. “Creationism as a Misconception: Socio-Cognitive Sadler, T. D., and D. L. Zeidler. 2005. “The Significance of Content
Conflict in the Teaching of Evolution.” International Journal of Science Knowledge for Informal Reasoning regarding Socioscientific Issues:
Education 34 (14): 2171–2180. Applying Genetics Knowledge to Genetic Engineering Issues.” Science
Goldstein, M., A. Crowell, and D. Kuhn. 2009. “What Constitutes Skilled Education 89 (1): 71–93.
Argumentation and How Does It Develop?” Informal Logic 29 (4): Shipman, H.L., N.W. Brickhouse, Z. Dagher, and W.J. Letts. 2002.
379–395. “Changes in student views of religion and science in a college astronomy
Gould, S. J. 1997. “Nonoverlapping Magisteria.” Natural History 106: course.” Science Education. 86 (4): 526–547.
60–62. Skehan, J. W., and C. E. Nelson. 2000. The Creation Controversy & the
Grace, M. 2009. “Developing High Quality Decision-Making Discussions Science Classroom. Arlington, VA: NSTA Press.
about Biological Conservation in a Normal Classroom Setting.” Smith, M. U. 2010. “Current Status of Research in Teaching and Learning
International Journal of Science Education 31 (4): 551–570. Evolution: I. Philosophical/Epistemological Issues.” Science & Education
Hegelson, L. J., J. Hoover, and J. Sheehan. 2002. “Introducing Preservice 19 (6): 523–538.
Teachers to Issues Surrounding Evolution and Creationism via a Mock Smith, M. U., and L. Scharmann. 2008. “A Multi-Year Program Develop-
Trial.” Journal of Elementary Science Education 14 (2): 11–24. ing an Explicit Reflective Pedagogy for Teaching Pre-Service Teachers
Hermann, R. S. 2008. “Evolution as a Controversial Issue: A Review of the Nature of Science by Ostention.” Science & Education 17: 219–248.
Instructional Approaches.” Science & Education 17: 1011–1032. Taber, K. S., B. Billingsley, F. Riga, and H. Newdick. 2011. “Secondary
Jensen, M. S., and F. N. Finley. 1997. “Teaching Evolution Using a Students’ Responses to Perceptions of the Relationship between Science
Historically Rich Curriculum & Paired Problem Solving Instructional and Religion: Stances Identified from an Interview Study.” Science
Strategy.” The American Biology Teacher 59 (4): 208–212. Education 95 (6): 1000–1025.
Kolstø, S. D. 2001. “‘To Trust or Not to Trust, …’ – Pupils’ Ways of Thagard, P., and S. Findlay. 2010. “Getting to Darwin: Obstacles to Accept-
Judging Information Encountered in a Socio-Scientific Issue.” ing Evolution by Natural Selection.” Science & Education 19 (6–8):
International Journal of Science Education 23 (9): 877–901. 625–636.
Lederman, N. G., F. Abd-El-Khalick, R. L. Bell, and R. Schwartz. 2002. Walton, D. 1996. Argumentation Schemes for Presumptive Reasoning. Mahwah,
“Views of Nature of Science Questionnaire: Toward Valid and NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Meaningful Assessment of Learners’ Conceptions of Nature of Science.” Walton, D. 2006. Fundamentals of Critical Argumentation. New York:
Journal of Research in Science Teaching 39: 497–521. Cambridge University Press.
Limón, M. 2001. “On the Cognitive Conflict as an Instructional Strategy Wiles, J. R., and B. Alters. 2011. “Effects of an Educational Experience
for Conceptual Change: A Critical Appraisal.” Learning and Instruction 11 Incorporating an Inventory of Factors Potentially Influencing Student
(4-5): 357–380. Acceptance of Biological Evolution.” International Journal of Science
Lombrozo, T., A. Thanukos, and M. Weisberg. 2008. “The Importance of Education 33 (18): 2559–2585.
Understanding the Nature of Science for Accepting Evolution.” Wilkens, J. S., and W. R. Elsberry. 2001. “The Advantages of Theft over
Evolution: Education and Outreach 1 (3): 290–298. Toil: The Design Inference and Arguing from Ignorance.” Biology and
Miller, J. D., E. C. Scott, and S. Okamoto. 2006. “Public Acceptance of Philosophy 16 (5): 711–724.
Evolution.” Science 313 (5788): 765–766. Yasri, P., and R. Mancy. 2012. “Understanding Student Approaches to
Moore, R., and S. Cotner. 2009. “Educational Malpractice: The Impact of Learning Evolution in the Context of their Perceptions of the
including Creationism in High School Biology Courses.” Evolution: Relationship between Science and Religion.” International Journal of
Education and Outreach 2 (1): 95–100. Science Education 1–22, iFirst Article.

You might also like