You are on page 1of 5

Learning and Individual Differences 23 (2013) 267–271

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Learning and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/lindif

Thinking styles in implicit and explicit learning


Qiuzhi Xie a, b,⁎, 1, Xiangping Gao a, Ronnel B. King b, c
a
Department of Psychology, Shanghai Normal University, No. 100 Guilin Road, Shanghai, PR China
b
Faculty of Education, The University of Hong Kong, Pokfulam Road, Hong Kong
c
Department of Special Education and Counselling, The Hong Kong Institute of Education, Tai Po, Hong Kong

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study investigated whether individual differences in thinking styles influence explicit and implicit learn-
Received 18 March 2012 ing. Eighty-seven university students in China participated in this study. Results indicated that performance
Received in revised form 23 September 2012 in the explicit learning condition was positively associated with Type I thinking styles (i.e. legislative and lib-
Accepted 18 October 2012
eral styles) and the internal style and negatively associated with a Type II thinking style (i.e. conservative
style) and the external style. There was no significant relationship between thinking styles and performance
Keywords:
Implicit learning
in the implicit learning condition. Taken together, these findings suggest that implicit and explicit learning
Explicit learning are distinct, each influenced by different individual difference variables. It also provides support to the
Thinking styles value-laden nature of styles, giving further evidence to the adaptiveness of Type I over Type II styles.
© 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction & Cleeremans, 2001; Gebauer & Mackintosh, 2007; Mathews et al.,
1989).
1.1. Implicit learning

Reber (1967) categorized learning mechanisms into explicit and 1.2. Individual differences in implicit learning and explicit learning
implicit learning. Explicit learning refers to learning that involves
consciousness and effort. Implicit learning, on the other hand, is Reber and Allen (2000) reviewed a large body of work on implicit
largely independent of conscious awareness of either the learning learning and concluded that individual differences in implicit learning
process or the learning products. Experimental tasks have been do exist. They also raised the question as to whether inter-individual
designed to study implicit learning, the three most popular ones variation in implicit learning is distinct from that found in explicit
being artificial grammar learning (Reber, 1976), sequence learning learning. From an evolutionary perspective, Reber and Allen (2000)
(Lewicki, Czyzewska, & Hoffman, 1987), and process control (Berry hypothesized that unlike explicit learning, implicit learning is inde-
& Broadbent, 1988). pendent of psychometric intelligence.
With regard to research on implicit learning, it is important to note Several studies have examined whether intelligence is related to im-
that implicit learning is distinct from explicit learning. The dissociation plicit and explicit learning. In general, it was found that intelligence was
between explicit and implicit learning has been supported by differ- positively related to performance on explicit learning tasks (e.g. Gebauer
ences in verbal reports: participants can report explicitly acquired & Mackintosh, 2007; Kaufman et al., 2010). However, the findings re-
knowledge, but fail to report implicitly acquired knowledge (e.g. Berry garding the association between intelligence and performance on im-
& Broadbent, 1984; Cleeremans & McClelland, 1991). Several scholars plicit learning tasks were rather inconsistent. Some studies reported a
have criticized such evidence as weak, arguing that verbal reports may non-significant correlation between the two variables (e.g. Gebauer &
not be sensitive enough to detect implicit learning (Dulany, Carlson, & Mackintosh, 2007; Reber, Walkenfeld, & Hernstadt, 1991), while other
Dewey, 1985; Shanks & St. John, 1994). The dissociation between studies reported a significant relationship (e.g. Danner, Hagemann,
these two learning modes, however, was also supported by studies Schankin, Hager, & Funke, 2011). Kaufman et al. (2010) found that per-
that documented differences in behavioral outcomes (e.g. Destrebecqz formance on the implicit learning task was associated with some intelli-
gence dimensions (i.e. verbal reasoning), while being independent of
some other dimensions (i.e. perceptual reasoning and mental rotation
ability). Nevertheless, Kaufman et al. (2010) concluded that the relation-
⁎ Corresponding author at: Faculty of Education, the University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong. ship between intelligence and performance was stronger for explicit
Tel.: +852 66876774; fax: +852 25471924.
E-mail address: qiuzhi-xie@hku.hk (Q. Xie).
learning tasks and weaker for implicit learning tasks. The relationship
1
Qiuzhi Xie conducted this study at Department of Psychology, Shanghai Normal between intelligence and implicit learning is still equivocal; however,
University. based on previous research outlined above, it is possible that even if

1041-6080/$ – see front matter © 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.10.014
268 Q. Xie et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 23 (2013) 267–271

there is a relationship between the two variables, the relationship would monarchic, local, and conservative styles) which are related to a pref-
be rather weak. erence for norms and cognitive simplicity are categorized as Type II.
Several studies also examined the relationship between performance Type I thinking styles are regarded as more adaptive and are related
on implicit learning tasks and basic cognitive functions. For instance, it to some positive attributes such as higher cognitive-developmental
was found that implicit learning was related to processing speed, but level and the use of a deep learning approach. Type II styles are
was independent of working memory capacity (e.g. Kaufman et al., considered to be less adaptive and are related to lower cognitive-
2010). Rathus, Reber, Manza, and Kushner (1994) investigated the rela- developmental level and the use of a surface learning approach
tionship of affective factors to both implicit and explicit cognitive pro- (e.g. Zhang & Sternberg, 2005). The other four styles (i.e. oligarchic,
cesses and found that: (1) anxiety interfered with explicit memory but anarchic, internal, and external styles) are largely reckoned as value-
not implicitly acquired knowledge and (2) depressive symptoms were differentiated and, thus far, no clear relationship has been found be-
related to neither explicit memory nor implicit learning. Several studies tween them and important educational outcomes. These styles are
also investigated the impact of personality on implicit learning. It has labeled as Type III styles. They may express the characteristics of
been argued that implicit learning and intuition are closely associated either Type I or II styles, depending on situational contingencies.
(e.g. Lieberman, 2000). Woolhouse and Bayne's (2000) study showed The relationship between thinking styles and academic achieve-
that when people were unaware of the underlying rules, those who pre- ment has been widely investigated in Western and Eastern countries
ferred intuition (seeking possibility and initiation) outperformed those (e.g. Cano-Garcia & Hughes, 2000; Zhang, 2004). It was consistently
who preferred sensing (seeking reality and convention) in an implicit found that thinking styles contribute to learning achievement beyond
learning task. Kaufman et al. (2010) reported the associations of two intelligence. Furthermore, Zhang (2004, 2007) found that the contri-
lower facets of Openness to Experience – Intellect and Openness – bution of thinking styles to academic achievement varied as a func-
with performance on an implicit learning task. Intellect pertains to tion of subject matter. Although the predictive power of thinking
quickness, ingenuity, and ideas, whereas Openness pertains to aes- styles for learning performance has been widely investigated, in all
thetics, imagination, and fantasy. It was found that participants' perfor- these studies, learning performance was tested through students' ex-
mance on the implicit learning task was related to Openness, but not amination scores in schools rather than through learning tasks under
to Intellect. Kaufman et al. (2010) also claimed that implicit learning rigorous experimental controls. Therefore, the contribution of think-
and impulsivity share a common characteristic, in the sense that both ing styles to the specific underlying learning processes has not yet
entail automatic processes. They found that the lack of premeditation been investigated.
(one of the dimensions of impulsivity) was positively related to perfor-
mance on the implicit learning task. Pretz, Totz, and Kaufman (2010)
1.4. Research purpose and hypotheses
found that the rational cognitive style, rather than the experiential
one, predicted better performance on implicit learning tasks. In addition,
The current study aims to investigate whether differences in think-
Kassin and Reber's (1979) study showed that performance in the implic-
ing styles influence implicit and explicit learning. First, it was hypothe-
it learning condition was positively related to locus of control: those
sized that Type I styles and the internal (Type III) style would be
having an internal locus of control outperformed those with an external
positively related to performance on the explicit learning task, whereas
locus of control on an implicit artificial grammar learning task.
Type II styles and the external style would be negatively related to it.
To date, there is still a dearth of research about individual differ-
This prediction was based on the cognitive-complexity/simplicity char-
ences in implicit learning. In addition, extant studies are plagued by
acteristic of Type I and II styles. In addition, those preferring the internal
a methodological problem. Most studies, except for two (Gebauer &
style tend to be more introverted, whereas those preferring the external
Mackintosh, 2007; Maybery, Taylor, & O'Brien-Malone, 1995), used
style tend to be more extraverted (Sternberg, 1997). The introverts usu-
different tasks to measure performance under the implicit learning
ally have a higher resting level of arousal (Eysenck & Eysenck, 1985)
condition and the explicit learning condition. Therefore, it is possible
and thus may outperform the extraverts in tasks requiring reflection
that their different relationships to individual differences factors
(Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2005; Matthews, 1992). The explicit
might have been due to task differences (Gebauer & Mackintosh,
learning task, therefore, would facilitate those preferring the internal
2007; Reber & Allen, 2000). This study addresses this weakness by
style to the external style.
using the same task for both learning conditions.
Moreover, we did not expect any relationship between thinking
styles and performance on the implicit learning task. Because thinking
1.3. Thinking styles
styles arise from both cognition and personality (Sternberg, 1997), we
reckoned that styles on the dimensions of function, level, and leaning
Sternberg's (1997) theory of thinking styles, also known as the
should be pertinent to the subscale of Intellect under the broader trait
theory of mental self-government, is one of the most recent and influ-
of Openness to Experience. Additionally, thinking styles on the dimen-
ential theories on styles (Zhang & Sternberg, 2005). According to
sion of form should be relevant to the subscale of Orderliness under
Sternberg (1997), thinking styles refer to mental tendencies to ap-
the broader trait of Conscientiousness (DeYoung, Quilty, & Peterson,
proach tasks in a certain manner. Sternberg (1997) used societal or-
2007). However, Intellect and Orderliness were found to be unrelated
ganization as a metaphor for understanding thinking styles. He
to implicit learning (Kaufman et al., 2010; Norman, Price, & Duff,
proposed five dimensions of thinking styles: function (legislative, ex-
2006). Thus, we did not posit any relationship between thinking styles
ecutive, and judicial styles), form (monarchic, hierarchic, oligarchic,
and performance on the implicit learning task.
and anarchic styles), level (global and local styles), scope (internal
and external styles), and leaning (liberal and conservative styles). De-
scriptions for each of these styles can be found in Appendix A. 2. Method
Insomuch as the 13 thinking styles are subsumed into five dimen-
sions, the theory can furnish a general profile of styles, instead of 2.1. Participants and research design
just relying on one or two style categories to describe an individual
(Sternberg, 1997). Eighty-seven students (6 males and 81 females) between ages
Zhang and Sternberg (2005) categorized the 13 thinking styles 20 years and 24 years (M = 21.3 years, SD = 1.03) in a university in
into three types. Thinking styles (i.e. legislative, judicial, hierarchical, Shanghai, China participated in this research. Thirty-four students
global, and liberal styles) pertinent to creativity and cognitive com- were sophomores, 22 were juniors, and 31 were seniors. These stu-
plexity are classified as Type I. Thinking styles (i.e. executive, dents majored in Humanities, Science, and Management.
Q. Xie et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 23 (2013) 267–271 269

We adopted a between-subject design, because the same task was were new. They were asked to judge whether each item was old or
employed for both implicit and explicit learning tasks. Forty-six new.
(Mage = 20.8 years, SD = 1.04) students were randomly assigned the Participants in the explicit learning condition were informed at
implicit learning task and forty-one (Mage = 21.9 years, SD = .65) stu- the beginning that the letter strings were generated from a rule sys-
dents were assigned the explicit learning task. tem determining which letter could follow another and which letters
could appear at the beginning or the end of a letter string. Participants
2.2. Materials and procedure were encouraged to discover as many rules as possible. In the follow-
ing testing phase, they were asked to judge whether the new letter
2.2.1. Thinking Styles Inventory-Revised II (TSI-R2, Sternberg, Wagner, & strings followed the previous grammatical rules.
Zhang, 2007)
The TSI-R2 contains 65 items with 13 scales corresponding to the 2.2.3. Task order
13 thinking styles. Each scale has 5 items. The questionnaire was All the participants commenced with the AGL task, followed by
scored on a 7 point Likert scale with higher scores indicating a greater the paper-and-pencil test measuring thinking styles. Participants
endorsement of the item (1 = does not fit you at all; 7 = fits you ex- given the implicit instruction were tested prior to those given the ex-
tremely well). plicit instruction.
The present study employed the Chinese version of this inventory
that was developed through translation and back translation. The 3. Results
Chinese version has been used in a number of studies and has been
found to have good psychometric properties (e.g. Zhang, 2002, 2004, 3.1. Preliminary investigation
2007). The Cronbach alpha coefficient for each scale typically ranged
from .60 to .80 in previous research, indicating acceptable internal con- The mean score of percentage correct (Pc) of performance under
sistency reliability of this inventory. the implicit instruction was .57 (95% CI = .54 to .59) and that of per-
formance under the explicit instruction was .61 (95% CI = .58 to .64). 2
2.2.2. Artificial Grammar Learning (AGL, Gebauer & Mackintosh, 2007) One sample t-tests showed that the proportions of correct answers in
The material and testing procedure of AGL was adopted from both groups were significantly higher than what would have been
Gebauer and Mackintosh's (2007) study. This computer-based task obtained by chance alone, thus suggesting that the performances
consisted of two phases: the learning phase and the testing phase. were above chance (timplicit = 5.30, p b .001; texplicit = 7.76, p b .001).
In the learning phase, each of the 20 letter strings following artifi- The standard deviation for learning under the implicit instruction
cial grammar (see Fig. 1) was presented for 4 s on a computer screen (2.76) was comparable to that for learning under the explicit instruc-
one by one. Participants were required to type each item (letter tion (2.90). Such result did not support Reber and Allen's (2000)
string) right after its presentation was finished. If typed incorrectly, argument that the inter-individual variation of implicit learning
the previous item would appear again until it could be correctly should be smaller than that of explicit learning. Moreover, in this
typed. After the presentation of every 10 items, all the previous 10 study, performance under the explicit instruction and implicit in-
items were presented in order altogether for 65 s for the participants struction was not correlated with age or year of study (see Table 1).
to review. All the participants were informed of such procedures be-
fore the task began. 3.2. The relationships of thinking styles to performance under implicit
In the following testing phase, 32 new items were presented one and explicit instructions
after another. Half of these items followed the artificial grammar in
the learning phase and the other half violated the grammar. Partici- Bivariate correlations were used to examine the relationships of
pants were required to judge whether each of the new items followed thinking styles to performance on the learning task (see Table 1). As
or violated the grammar. Each right answer deserved one point and a expected, performance under the explicit instruction was correlated
total score of right answers was calculated. Different instructional positively with Type I styles (i.e. the legislative and the liberal styles)
sets were used to dissociate implicit learning from explicit learning. and the internal style, whereas it correlated negatively with a Type II
Participants in the implicit learning condition were informed at style (i.e. the conservative style) and the external style. No significant
the beginning that it was a memory task and that they were required correlation was found between performance under the implicit in-
to memorize these letter strings, which were described as nonsense struction and thinking styles.
and randomized. In the testing phase, they were informed that half We also conducted stepwise regression to examine how individu-
of the items had appeared in the learning phase and the other half al differences in thinking styles predict performance. Performance
under the explicit instruction was designated as the dependent vari-
able and thinking styles were designated as the independent variable
(see Table 2). The external style and the conservative style contribut-
ed negatively to and accounted for approximately 33% of the variance
in performance under the explicit instruction.

4. Discussion

The results of this study supported the hypothesis concerning the


relationships between thinking styles and performance upon the ex-
plicit learning task. The internal style and Type I styles – the ones
concerning the preferences for creating rules (i.e. the legislative
style) and going beyond existent rules (i.e. the liberal style) – were
positively related to performance under the explicit instruction. The
external style and the Type II conservative style, which pertain to
Fig. 1. Artificial Grammar from “Psychometric Intelligence Dissociates Implicit and Ex-
2
plicit Learning”, by G. F. Gebauer and N. J. Mackintosh, 2007, Journal of Experimental The mean scores of Pc in this study were not substantially higher than .60, meaning
Psychology: Learning Memory, and Cognition, 33(1), p. 38. APA as publisher. that learning rates observed in this study were lower than typical.
270 Q. Xie et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 23 (2013) 267–271

Table 1 2010). This study showed that people's thinking styles are associated
Pearson correlations of learning performances with age, year of study, and thinking with explicit learning but not with implicit learning.
styles.
Further, the findings have significant implications to the theory of
Implicit Explicit thinking styles, proposed to explain individual differences in learning
Age .07 −.73 performance. This is the first study that investigates the contributions
Year of study .13 .25 of thinking styles to performance on an experimental learning task
legislative .10 .46⁎⁎ and, thus, the findings can suggest the contributions of thinking styles
Executive .01 −.07
to specific learning processes. This study implies that thinking styles
Judicial .04 −.01
Global −.01 .01 are more strongly associated with the conscious learning process (in
Local −.03 −.24 explicit learning condition) rather than the unconscious learning pro-
Liberal −.12 .31⁎ cess (in implicit learning condition).
conservative −.15 −.43⁎⁎ Also, the results imply that those having Type I thinking styles
hierarchic .03 .13
seem to be at a considerable advantage in the acquisition of knowl-
monarchic −.06 −.06
oligarchic −.05 −.30 edge entailing reasoning and reflection. The results support Zhang
Anarchic −.04 −.09 and Sternberg's (2005) argument that Type 1 styles are more adap-
Internal .24 .37⁎ tive compared to Type II styles.
External −.08 −.52⁎⁎⁎
To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that investigat-
Note. Implicit = performance under implicit instruction and Explicit = performance ed whether thinking styles are related to implicit and explicit learn-
under explicit instruction. ing. Future studies that use other types of tasks to assess implicit
⁎ p b .05.
⁎⁎ p b .01. and explicit learning and those involving larger sample sizes are
⁎⁎⁎ p b .001. needed to replicate our findings.

Appendix A. Descriptions of thinking styles


the preference for adhering to existing rules, were negatively associ-
ated with performance under the explicit instruction. Results of the
regression equation indicated that the external thinking style was
the strongest predictor of performance on the explicit learning task. Dimension Thinking style Description
The negative contributions of the external and the conservative
Function Legislative Preference for creating rules and autonomy
thinking styles exceeded the positive contributions of the internal
Executive Preference for following rules
and Type I thinking styles. Also as expected, thinking styles were Judicial Preference for evaluating rules
not found to be related to performance in the implicit learning Form Monarchic Tendency to be single-minded and driven
condition. Hierarchic Tendency to set hierarchy for goals and tasks
Because the same task was employed for both implicit and explicit Oligarchic Tendency to be motivated by several things with
equally perceived importance
learning conditions, the findings rule out the possibility that the dif- Anarchic Tendency to be driven by an assortment of needs
ferent relationships of thinking styles to performance under the two and goals with disorder
learning modes were due to task differences. It presents a consider- Level Global Preference for something abstract and large
able improvement over previous research which has failed to disen- Local Preference for something concrete and with details
Scope Internal Inclination to work independently
tangle effects due to task differences from that due to different
External Inclination to work with others
learning processes. Leaning Liberal Liking for going beyond rules and maximizing
This study has implications for both the literature on implicit and changes
explicit learning and the theory of thinking styles. First, this study Conservative Liking for adhering to existing rules and minimizing
supported the dissociation between explicit and implicit learning. In changes

line with extant research, this study showed that there was a perti- Adapted from "Thinking styles" by Sternberg, 1997.
nent consequential behavioral difference between explicit and im-
plicit learning: the performance under explicit instruction was
related to thinking styles, whereas the performance under implicit in-
struction was not. This finding is also congruent with Evans and
Frankish's (2009) dual-process theories about two distinguishable References
processing systems. Berry, D. C., & Broadbent, D. E. (1984). On the relationship between task performance and
Second, this study showed that individual differences variables associated verbalizable knowledge. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.
that influence implicit learning are distinguishable from those that A: Human Experimental Psychology, 36(2), 209–231.
Berry, D. C., & Broadbent, D. E. (1988). Interactive tasks and implicit–explicit distinc-
impact explicit learning. Previous studies have indicated that implicit tion. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 252–272.
learning and explicit learning are associated with different individual Cano-Garcia, F., & Hughes, E. H. (2000). Learning and thinking styles: An analysis of
differences factors (e.g. Gebauer & Mackintosh, 2007; Kaufman et al., their interrelationship and influence on academic achievement. Educational Psy-
chology, 20, 413–430.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2005). Personality and intellectual competence.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Cleeremans, A., & McClelland, J. L. (1991). Learning the structure of event sequences.
Table 2
Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 120(3), 235–253.
Stepwise regression for predicting explicit learning task score from thinking styles.
Danner, D., Hagemann, D., Schankin, A., Hager, M., & Funke, J. (2011). Beyond IQ: A
Adj. R2 F Beta t latent state-trait analysis of general intelligence, dynamic decision making, and
implicit learning. Intelligence, 39(1), 323–334.
1 External .255 92.082⁎⁎⁎ −1.124 −3.835⁎⁎⁎ Destrebecqz, A., & Cleeremans, A. (2001). Can sequence learning be implicit? New ev-
2 External .328 6.805⁎⁎⁎ −.949 −3.286⁎⁎ idence with the process dissociation procedure. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,
Conservative −.992 −2.286⁎ 8(2), 343–350.
DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between facets and domains: 10
Note. aspects of the Big Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 880–896.
⁎ p b .05. Dulany, D. E., Carlson, R. A., & Dewey, G. I. (1985). On consciousness in syntactic learning
⁎⁎ p b .01. and judgement: A reply to Reber, Allen, and Regan. Journal of Experimental Psychology.
⁎⁎⁎ p b .001. General, 114(1), 25–32.
Q. Xie et al. / Learning and Individual Differences 23 (2013) 267–271 271

Evans, J. S. B. T., & Frankish, K. (2009). In two minds: Dual processes and beyond. New York: Reber, A. S. (1967). Implicit learning of artificial grammars. Journal of Verbal Learning
Oxford University Press. and Verbal Behavior, 6, 855–863.
Eysenck, H. J., & Eysenck, M. W. (1985). Personality and individual differences: A natural Reber, A. S. (1976). Implicit learning of synthetic languages: The role of instructional
science approach. New York: Plenum. set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2(1), 88–94.
Gebauer, G. F., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2007). Psychometric intelligence dissociates implicit Reber, A. S., & Allen, R. (2000). Individual differences in implicit learning: Implications for
and explicit learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and the evolution of consciousness. In R. G. Kunzendorf, & B. Wallace (Eds.), Individual dif-
Cognition, 33(1), 34–54. ferences in conscious experience. Advances in consciousness research (pp. 227–247).
Kassin, S. M., & Reber, A. S. (1979). Locus of control and the learning of an artificial lan- Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
guage. Journal of Research in Personality, 13(1), 112–118. Reber, A. S., Walkenfeld, F. F., & Hernstadt, R. (1991). Implicit and explicit learning:
Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Jimenez, L., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. J. Individual differences and IQ. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
(2010). Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition, 116, 321–340. and Cognition, 17(5), 888–896.
Lewicki, P., Czyzewska, M., & Hoffman, H. (1987). Unconscious acquisition of complex Shanks, D. R., & St. John, M. F. (1994). Characteristics of dissociable human learning sys-
procedural knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and tems. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 367–395.
Cognition, 13(4), 523–530. Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Thinking styles. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Intuition: A social cognitive neuroscience approach. Psycho- Sternberg, R. J., Wagner, R. K., & Zhang, L. F. (2007). Thinking style inventory-Revised II:
logical Bulletin, 126, 109–137. Unpublished test, Tufts University.
Mathews, R. C., Buss, R. R., Stanley, W. B., Blanchard-Fields, F., Cho, J. R., & Druhan, B. Woolhouse, L. S., & Bayne, R. (2000). Personality and the use of intuition: Individual
(1989). Role of implicit and explicit processes in learning from examples: A syner- differences in strategy and performance on an implicit learning task. European
gistic effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, Journal of Personality, 14, 157–169.
15(6), 1083–1100. Zhang, L. F. (2002). Measuring thinking styles in addition to measuring personality
Matthews, G. (1992). Extraversion. In A. P. Smith, & D. M. Jones (Eds.), Handbook of traits? Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 445–458.
human performance, Vol. 3. (pp. 95–126)London: Academic. Zhang, L. F. (2004). Revisiting the predictive power of thinking styles for academic perfor-
Maybery, M., Taylor, M., & O'Brien-Malone, A. (1995). Implicit learning: Sensitive to mance. Journal of Psychology, 138(4), 351–370.
age but not IQ. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 8–17. Zhang, L. F. (2007). Intellectual styles and academic achievement among senior secondary
Norman, E., Price, M. C., & Duff, S. C. (2006). Fringe consciousness in sequence learning: school students in rural China. Educational Psychology, 27(5), 675–692.
The influence of individual differences. Consciouness and Cognition, 15, 723–760. Zhang, L. F., & Sternberg, R. J. (2005). A threefold model of intellectual styles. Educational
Pretz, J. E., Totz, K. S., & Kaufman, S. B. (2010). The effects of mood, cognitive style, and Psychology Review, 17(1), 1–53.
cognitive ability on implicit learning. Learning and Individual Differences, 20,
215–219.
Rathus, J. H., Reber, A. S., Manza, L., & Kushner, M. (1994). Implicit and explicit learning:
Differential effects of affective states. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 79, 163–184.

You might also like