You are on page 1of 391

Autism and Child Psychopathology Series

Series Editor: Johnny L. Matson

Johnny L. Matson
Editor

Handbook of
Social Behavior
and Skills in
Children
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series

Series Editor
Johnny L. Matson
Department of Psychology
Louisiana State University
Baton Rouge, LA, USA

More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/8665


Johnny L. Matson
Editor

Handbook of Social
Behavior and Skills in
Children
Editor
Johnny L. Matson
Department of Psychology
Louisiana State University
Baton Rouge, LA, USA

ISSN 2192-922X     ISSN 2192-9238 (electronic)


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series
ISBN 978-3-319-64591-9    ISBN 978-3-319-64592-6 (eBook)
DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6

Library of Congress Control Number: 2017954353

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017


This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole or
part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of
illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way,
and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software,
or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this
publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are
exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in
this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor
the authors or the editors give a warranty, express or implied, with respect to the material
contained herein or for any errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains
neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Printed on acid-free paper

This Springer imprint is published by Springer Nature


The registered company is Springer International Publishing AG
The registered company address is: Gewerbestrasse 11, 6330 Cham, Switzerland
Contents

 istory of Social Skills��������������������������������������������������������������������������    1


H
Johnny L. Matson and Claire O. Burns
 efining Social Skills ����������������������������������������������������������������������������    9
D
Steven G. Little, John Swangler, and Angeleque Akin-Little
Challenging Behavior����������������������������������������������������������������������������   19
Catia Cividini-Motta, Sarah E. Bloom, and Claudia Campos
Developmental Issues����������������������������������������������������������������������������   47
Patricia Soto-Icaza and Pablo Billeke
 ocial Competence: Consideration of Behavioral,
S
Cognitive, and Emotional Factors��������������������������������������������������������   63
Karen Milligan, Annabel Sibalis, Ashley Morgan,
and Marjory Phillips
Observational Methods�������������������������������������������������������������������������   83
Susan M. Vener, Alison M. Wichnick-Gillis,
and Claire L. Poulson
 hecklists and Scaling Methods ����������������������������������������������������������  101
C
W. Jason Peters and Johnny L. Matson
Behavior Analytic Methods ������������������������������������������������������������������  115
Marc J. Lanovaz, Marie-Michèle Dufour,
and Malena Argumedes
 ocial Learning Instructional Models��������������������������������������������������  133
S
Renae Beaumont, Jo Hariton, Shannon Bennett,
Amy Miranda, and Elisabeth Sheridan Mitchell
 elf-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective������������  149
S
Yair Ziv, Moti Benita, and Inbar Sofri
 ocial and Emotional Learning: Recent Research
S
and Practical Strategies for Promoting Children’s Social
and Emotional Competence in Schools������������������������������������������������  175
Eva Oberle and Kimberly A. Schonert-Reichl

v
vi Contents

 sing Parent Training Programmes to Teach Social Skills����������������  199


U
John Sharry and Caoimhe Doyle
 ocial Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders����������������������������������������  217
S
Chieko Kanai, Gabor Toth, Miho Kuroda, Atsuko Miyake,
and Takashi Itahashi
I ntellectual Disability and Social Skills������������������������������������������������  249
Jeff Sigafoos, Giulio E. Lancioni, Nirbhay N. Singh,
and Mark F. O’Reilly
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder��������������������������������������������  273
Katy E. Tresco, Jessie L. Kessler, and Jennifer A. Mautone
Anxiety Disorders����������������������������������������������������������������������������������  293
Thompson E. Davis III, Peter Castagna, Georgia Shaheen,
and Erin Tarcza Reuther
 vidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties
E
in Conduct Disorder������������������������������������������������������������������������������  323
Kimberly Renk, J’Nelle Stephenson, Maria Khan,
and Annelise Cunningham
 epression and (Hypo)mania����������������������������������������������������������������  363
D
Patrick Pössel and Thomas D. Meyer

Index��������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������  383
About the Editor

Johnny L. Matson, Ph.D.,   is professor and distinguished research master


in the Department of Psychology at Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge,
LA, USA. He has also previously held a professorship in psychiatry and clini-
cal psychology at the University of Pittsburgh. He is the author of more than
800 publications including 41 books. He served as the founding editor in
chief for the journals Research in Developmental Disabilities (Elsevier) and
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders (Elsevier) and currently serves as the
editor in chief for the Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
(Springer).

vii
Contributors

Angeleque Akin-Little Akin-Little and Little Behavioral Psychology


Consultants, Malone, NY, USA
Malena Argumedes École de Psychoéducation, Université de Montréal,
Montreal, QC, Canada
Renae Beaumont  Weill Cornell Medicine, New York Presbyterian Hospital,
New York, NY, USA
Moti Benita  Faculty of Education, Department of Counseling and Human
Development, Multidisciplinary Program for Early Childhood Education and
Development, University of Haifa, Mount Carmel, Haifa, Israel
Shannon Bennett  Pediatric OCD, Anxiety, and Tic Disorders Program, Youth
Anxiety Center, Weill Cornell Medicine, New York Presbyterian Hospital,
New York, NY, USA
Pablo Billeke División de Neurociencias, Centro de Investigación en
Complejidad Social (neuroCICS), Universidad del Desarrollo, Santiago,
Chile
Sarah E. Bloom  University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
Claire O. Burns Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Claudia Campos  University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
Peter Castagna Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Catia Cividini-Motta  University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA
Annelise Cunningham Department of Psychology, University of Central
Florida, Orlando, FL, USA
Thompson E. Davis III Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Caoimhe Doyle  Parents Plus, Phibsborough, Dublin, Ireland
Marie-Michèle Dufour  École de Psychoéducation, Université de Montréal,
Montreal, QC, Canada

ix
x Contributors

Jo Hariton  Weill Cornell Medicine, New York Presbyterian Hospital, New


York, NY, USA
Takashi Itahashi  Medical Institute of Developmental Disabilities Research,
Showa University, Tokyo, Japan
Chieko Kanai Medical Institute of Developmental Disabilities Research,
Showa University, Tokyo, Japan
Jessie L. Kessler Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine,
Philadelphia, PA, USA
Maria Khan Department of Psychology, University of Central Florida,
Orlando, FL, USA
Miho Kuroda  School of Human Care Studies, Nagoya University of Arts
and Sciences, Aichi, Japan
Giulio E. Lancioni  University of Bari, Bari, Italy
Marc J. Lanovaz  École de Psychoéducation, Université de Montréal, Montreal,
QC, Canada
Steven G. Little  Walden University, Minneapolis, MN, USA
Johnny L. Matson  Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Jennifer A. Mautone  Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, Perelman School
of Medicine at the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
Thomas D. Meyer Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences,
McGovern Medical School, University of Texas Health Science Center,
Houston, TX, USA
Karen Milligan  Department of Psychology, Ryerson University, Toronto,
ON, Canada
Amy Miranda  Department of Child Psychiatry, Payne Whitney Clinic, New
York Presbyterian Hospital and Weill Cornell Medicine, New York, NY, USA
Elisabeth Sheridan Mitchell  Westchester Division, Department of Psychiatry,
Weill Cornell Medicine, Center for Autism and the Developing Brain, New
York Presbyterian Hospital, NY, USA
Atsuko Miyake Department of Child and Adolescent Mental Health,
National Institute of Mental Health, Tokyo, Japan
Ashley Morgan  Integra Program, Child Development Institute, Toronto, ON,
Canada
Eva Oberle  Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver,
BC, Canada
Mark F. O’Reilly  The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
Contributors xi

W. Jason Peters Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,


Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Marjory Phillips  Integra Program, Child Development Institute, Toronto,
ON, Canada
Patrick Pössel Department of Counseling and Human Development,
University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, USA
Claire L. Poulson Queens College, The City University of New York,
Flushing, NY, USA
The Graduate Center, The City University of New York, New York, NY, USA
Kimberly Renk  Department of Psychology, University of Central Florida,
Orlando, FL, USA
Erin Tarcza Reuther LSU Health Science Center and the Children’s
Hospital of New Orleans, New Orleans, LA, USA
Kimberly A. Schonert-Reichl  Faculty of Education, University of British
Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
Georgia Shaheen  Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
John Sharry  Parents Plus, Phibsborough, Dublin, Ireland
University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
Annabel Sibalis  Department of Psychology, Ryerson University, Toronto,
ON, Canada
Jeff Sigafoos School of Education, Victoria University of Wellington,
Wellington, New Zealand
Nirbhay N. Singh Medical College of Georgia, Augusta University,
Augusta, GA, USA
Inbar Sofri  Faculty of Education, Department of Counseling and Human
Development, Multidisciplinary Program for Early Childhood Education and
Development, University of Haifa, Mount Carmel, Haifa, Israel
Patricia Soto-Icaza Laboratorio de Neurociencias Cognitivas, Pontificia
Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile
J’Nelle Stephenson Department of Psychology, University of Central
Florida, Orlando, FL, USA
John Swangler  Phoenix, AZ, USA
Gabor Toth  Department of Education and Child Studies, Faculty of Arts
and Sciences, Sagami Women's University, Kanagawa, Japan
Katy E. Tresco  Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine, Philadelphia,
PA, USA
xii Contributors

Susan M. Vener New York Child Learning Institute, College Point, NY,


USA
Alison M. Wichnick-Gillis New York Child Learning Institute, College
Point, NY, USA
Yair Ziv Faculty of Education, Department of Counseling and Human
Development, Multidisciplinary Program for Early Childhood Education and
Development, University of Haifa, Mount Carmel, Haifa, Israel
History of Social Skills

Johnny L. Matson and Claire O. Burns

The development of modern approaches to comments, (2) making self-disclosure state-


social skills training dates to the 1970s (McFall ments, and (3) making high-interest statements
& Twentyman, 1973). Initially, research was with respect to movies, school, friends, social
carried out using discrete behaviors considered events, and TV shows. Training followed the
to be components of appropriate assertiveness template of assertive skills training for typically
training. These early papers largely focused on developing adults. Instructions, modeling, behav-
typically developing adults (McFall & ioral rehearsal, feedback, and reinforcement were
Lillesaud, 1971). These efforts helped to estab- included in the learning trials. Raters noted client
lish a methodology for training and to demon- responses on a 7-point scale from 1 (very poor) to
strate the efficacy of learning-based methods to 7 (very good). The authors reported marked
improve these skills. improvements that were maintained at a 5-month
follow-up. These methods and findings were sim-
ilar to Kelly, Furmna, Philips, Hathorn, and
Special Populations Wilson (1979) who taught two adolescents with
intellectual disabilities and to a study by Kelly,
Soon after these early efforts, the focus of social Wildman, Very, and Thurman (1979). The latter
skills shifted largely to special populations. For paper added an additional twist to the treatment
example, Bradlyn, Himadi, Crimmins, Graves, by using a group format.
and Kelly (1983) taught conversational skills to Social skills training packages have also been
five adolescents (14–18 years old) who were applied to emotionally disturbed children
functioning in the severe to profound range of (Matson et al., 1980). These authors worked with
intellectual disabilities. Training for this study four children 9–11 years of age who were fre-
occurred in a therapy room of a large develop- quently engaging in fights, provoking others,
mental center. Two trainers worked with each of being noncompliant, and verbalizing psychoso-
the five adolescents separately. Among the skills matic complaints. The learning-based training
trained were (1) reinforcing or acknowledging package proved to be highly effective in assisting
these children with acquiring relevant adaptive
behaviors.
Furman, Geller, Simon, and Kelly (1979) used
J.L. Matson (*) • C.O. Burns
the socials skills training package to enhance these
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA behaviors for three adults. One person was diag-
e-mail: johnmatson@aol.com nosed with chronic schizophrenia, another person

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 1


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_1
2 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

had been diagnosed with hysterical personality dis- study on this topic was published by Azrin and
order, and the third had evinced depression. The Hayes (1984). They focused on teaching social
focus of training was on teaching job interviewing sensitivity to 89 males between 17 and 29 years
skills. Particular focus was placed on volunteering of age. The focus was on identifying nonverbal
relevant information and showing initiative. Other cues to assist with respect to identifying interest a
skills trained included describing work experience female had in a male. Half of the individuals
and asking the interviewer questions. The interven- were then provided feedback based on the actual
tion, which emphasized brief behavioral rehearsal, level of interest the female felt she was display-
proved useful in teaching these relevant skills. ing. This feedback proved helpful in determining
Schizophrenia is another handicapping condi- interest level and generalized to real-life social
tion that has marked profound deficits (Bellack, situations.
Brown, & Thomas-Lohtman, 2006). Hayes, Another social skills study that focused on shy
Halford, and Varghese (1995) studied 63 people males was reported by Christoff et al. (1985).
with chronic schizophrenia. Participants were Their participants were four girls and two boys
assigned randomly to either a social skills group or 12–14 years of age. Target behaviors included
a placebo group (i.e., discussion group but with no academic performance, social adjustment, con-
specific focus on techniques or treatment meth- versational ability, number of friends, ease in
ods). Social skills were taught using manuals that interacting with others, and range of activities
focused on interpersonal skills, social problem- and interests. A two-phase treatment approach
solving, and how to manage one’s time more was used. First, a problem-solving program was
effectively. The social skills group proved to be implemented over four sessions. Training was in
superior for teaching social skills. These data rep- a group format (most of these early studies
licate and extend data obtained in other studies involved training one person at a time). Using
with similar populations (Beidel, Bellack, Turner, worksheets and a social problem of their choos-
Hersen, & Luber, 1981; Curran, Graves, & Cirelli, ing, each child defined their problem, generated
1982). However, this limited form of training may multiple solutions, discussed possible positive
not be sufficient to produce large global changes. and negative outcomes, chose what they consid-
Liberman (1992) suggested a more broad-based ered to be the best solutions, and made a plan to
approach for persons with schizophrenia. implement it. The next four sessions involved
Recreational and leisure skills were mentioned. At improving conversational skills. Behaviors that
the time these behaviors were not conceptualized were targeted included listening, talking about
as social skills. This approach has changed over oneself, making requests, and starting conversa-
time so that these behaviors and related social con- tions. This program proved effective. Also, this
tacts, such as play skills, are now subsumed under study was one of the first to use an educational
the social skills moniker. model versus more traditional behavior therapy
Another of these behavior sets that adults for- strategies to train skills. Given that children are
mally hospitalized in psychiatric inpatient units familiar with these school-focused strategies, an
received training on was job interviewing skills educational approach has proven to be very pop-
(Furman et al., 1979). Specific target behaviors ular. With time, entire curriculums and learning-­
addressed were positive comments about previ- based didactic programs using this educational
ous educational experience, hand and arm ges- approach have emerged.
tures used along with verbalizations, showing
enthusiasm and/or interest, asking the interviewer
questions, and providing facts about one’s family Populations
or job experience. Staff successfully used coach-
ing, praise, and feedback to enhance this skill set. A broad range of different populations have con-
Shy, unassertive adults were among the first tinued to be treated with social skills treatments.
social skills groups to be targeted and effectively Foxx, Faw, and Weber (1991) treated 9–15-year-­
trained in the research literature. One interesting olds with borderline intellectual functioning and
History of Social Skills 3

a host of mental health problems such as conduct or inappropriate. Correct responses were praised
disorder, aggression, and adjustment disorder. by the therapist. Tangible reinforcers were given
Group training was used along with modeling by at the end of the game based on “game money.” A
the therapist. Clients were trained to state rules second strategy was labeled as a “social life pack-
and provide responses to social vignettes and age.” This treatment used the game method
received feedback and praise from the therapist. described, and add-on generalization strategies
The treatment was effective and promoted gener- were also included. A group discussion of game
alization to novel individuals in naturalistic set- cards to teach problem-solving skills was also
tings. Similarly, Franco, Christoff, Crimmins, part of the training. Direct care staff were directed
and Kelly (1983) treated an adolescent. In this to provide opportunities to implement these social
case, the focus was on conversational skills of an skills in naturalistic settings. The social life condi-
extremely shy male. Eye contact, affect, speech tion was more effective and remained so at a
acknowledgments, and conversational questions 3-month follow-up.
were all improved. Psychiatric inpatient children have also been
Sacks and Gaylord-Ross (1989) worked with effectively trained on social skills (Kazdin,
15 visually handicapped children who were Esveldt-Dawson, & Matson, 1983). These
7–12 years of age. Training occurred in school authors treated 34 children, 7–13 years of age,
and involved a four-week social skills training who evinced conduct disorders, depression,
program. Five nonhandicapped peers were also ADHD, adjustment disorder, or anxiety disor-
enlisted to provide peer training. This group of ders. These children were asked to demonstrate
children received weekly training from a psy- good or poor social skills in response to predeter-
chologist and teacher. Treatment was successful mined role-play scenes. Using this strategy,
in increasing initiations, joining group activities, marked improvements were made on eye contact,
joining a group, sharing, gaze, and posture. number of words spoken, facial expression,
Another handicapped group that has received motor movements, verbal content, and intona-
attention with respect to social skills are people tion. Also, broader social behaviors were evalu-
with intellectual disabilities. Matson and Senatore ated including overall positive and negative
(1981) compared traditional psychotherapy to a situation ratings as well as overall total skills.
social skills package of role-play, modeling, per- Heiby (1986) treated four adult women with
formance feedback, and reinforcement. Adult depression for social skills deficits. Treatment
outpatients with mild and moderate intellectual consisted of 12 one-hour individual sessions. The
disabilities received training on social appropri- focus of treatment was on self-monitoring, self-­
ateness and socially inappropriate statements evaluation, and self-reinforcement. Homework
using the social skills package. Similarly, in a assignments were also used, and these assign-
related study with adults who display intellectual ments were received weekly during therapy ses-
disabilities, a behavioral package was used that sions. Marked improvements were noted in
included active practice, which was found to be general assertiveness and self-reinforcement.
more effective than when role-play, modeling, Hersen, Bellack, Himmelhoch, and Thase (1984)
instructions, and performance feedback alone also treated women with depression for social
were used (Senatore, Matson, & Kazdin, 1982). skills deficits. They added the interesting twist of
Griffiths, Feldman, and Tough (1997) also using role-play, reinforcement, and performance
described a treatment study with adults who feedback with amitriptyline. They found that this
evinced intellectual disabilities. Treatment condi- treatment was no more effective than social skills
tions consisted of a game condition where partici- treatment alone.
pants moved a board. A die was tossed to Another form of psychopathology treated for
determine which of 72 cards should be selected. social skills deficits was social phobia (Hoffart,
These cards were structured to provide social Borge, & Clark, 2009). These authors compared
skills instruction. Social situations were described, two forms of psychotherapy with 80 adults to treat
which the client had to determine as appropriate social phobias. The authors used two standardized
4 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

scales that measured interaction styles. Residential Social skills training can also serve as an add-
cognitive therapy (RCT) consisted of specific o­ n therapy. Herbert et al. (2005) used cognitive
exercises, procedures, and homework to teach new behavioral therapy (CBT) to treat social anxiety.
skills in the areas of beliefs, images, and safe Participants were 38 males and 37 females who
behaviors using instruction, videotapes, and per- had been assessed using the social phobia anxiety
formance feedback. The second treatment was inventory. CBT was provided to one group, while
residential interpersonal psychotherapy (RIPT) . a second group’s treatment involved CBT plus
This latter intervention targeted patterns of social social skills training. The latter strategy included
role insecurity. The focus of treatment was on education modeling of target behaviors such as
social behaviors such as showing empathy, appre- speech content, voice volume, tone of speech, tim-
ciation, and acceptance. Both interventions pro- ing of speech, eye contact, and facial expressions.
duced positive, similar effects. Treatment involved practicing the skills in session,
Another problem addressed through social provision of feedback, and practicing in the natu-
skills training has been social anxiety (Ledley ral environment. Those receiving the add-on social
et al., 2009). Participants were 38 adults. Their skills intervention developed better coping skills
immediate treatment was 16 sessions of than individuals in the CBT-only group.
1–1½ h. Components included psychoeduca- Another area focused on in social skills
tion using a workbook, gradual exposure to research is children with ADHD in a sports set-
social situations, cognitive restructuring, evalu- ting. In a study by O’Callaghan, Reitman,
ating, and, where appropriate, modifying of Northup, Hupp, and Murphy (2003), two boys
core beliefs. Finally, relapse prevention and ter- and two girls were taught good sportsmanship
mination of intervention were included. The such as praising other players and being attentive.
treatment resulted in improvement in a variety A token economy system was used to reinforce
of social behaviors. positive target behaviors. Feedback and praise for
Cervantes et al. (2013) evaluated the effects of the children was also provided by the trainers.
ADHD on social skills in 6–16-year-olds. Another approach to teaching prosocial behav-
Children with ASD and comorbid ADHD had iors in children is described by Sukhodolsky,
more socialization impairments than children Golub, Stone, and Orban (2005). Two manual-
with ASD only. Additionally, the former group ized treatments were compared over ten therapy
became more socially impaired over time as sessions. Behaviors targeted included social cog-
assessed using the MESSY-II. nitive deficits and social skills deficits. One inter-
Another study designed to identify social anx- vention focused on cognitive restructuring such
iety in female college students was described by as attention retraining, consequential thinking,
Greenwald (1977). Participants role-played three and helping the participants generate solutions.
1-min social situations. Behaviors targeted Conversely, the social skills manual focused on
included talk time, number of seconds the person the more traditional methods in the research lit-
spoke, and initiating speech. Specific differences erature such as rehearsal, performance feedback,
between students who were successful at dating modeling, and social reinforcement. Both treat-
and those who were not were described. ments resulted in improvements for over half of
Greco and Morris (2005) addressed social the children with treatment gains maintained at a
anxiety as well. Peer acceptance was addressed 3-month follow-up.
among middle school children, and participants
were 333, 8–12-year-olds. Teachers rated the
children on the social skills rating system. The Autism Spectrum Disorder
focus was on determining the relationship
between social anxiety and peer acceptance. Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has been a
Social skills proficiency served as a mediator major area for research on social skills assess-
between these two factors. ment and training in recent years. Flynn and
History of Social Skills 5

Healy (2012), for example, found 22 studies that high school students (n = 3) served as participants.
looked at problems with social skills and self-­ The focus was on social conversations in the school
help skills. Similarly, Cappadocia and Weiss cafeteria. Participants were provided with two cue
(2011) found multiple studies that studied social cards which focused on asking a question, telling
skills training groups for children and adoles- something, or starting a conversation. Training ses-
cents who evinced either Asperger syndrome or sions occurred for 30 min before lunch. These three
high-functioning autism. Wang, Cui, and Parilla participants then began conversations with some of
(2011) in another review of social skills and the approximately 200 students who were at lunch.
autism found 64 papers. Thirteen of these met As with the other studies reviewed in this chapter,
criteria for peer-mediated and video-modeling the intervention was effective.
treatments. Finally, Matson and Wilkins (2007) Hill, Varela, Kamps, and Niditch (2014) spec-
reported on a substantial number of studies that ulated on the role of anxiety and cognitive func-
laid out specific excesses and deficits that are tioning on social skills of 102 children with
common among persons with autism. Language ASD. They found that lower intellectual func-
and recognition of emotions had particular idio- tioning and greater anxiety were directly related
syncrasies specific to ASD. to poorer social skills. Other factors associated
Within the context of autism, a number of inter- with social skills excesses and deficits have also
vention strategies have been used successfully. been evaluated with children who have been
One notable intervention of this type is described diagnosed with ASD.
by Celiberti and Harris (1993). The participants Another interesting assessment of children
were three dyads, an older typically developing with ASD involved cultural difference in social
sister and a young brother or sister aged 4, 4, and skills. Comparing children from South Korea and
2 years. A curriculum was used to teach the older the USA on the MESSY-II, no major social dif-
sisters to be peer trainers. Peer-­related commands ferences were noted (Matson et al., 2012). The
involved modeling and specific neutral prompts. MESSY was considered to be an appropriate
Target behaviors included eye contact, attention to measure for these analyses, since it had been
tasks, and appropriate play. The children with normed on children with ASD in earlier research
ASD improved on these important social skills. (Matson, Horovitz, Mahan, & Fodstad, 2013).
These children were able to generalize social skills Following the theme of using various inter-
in a play situation with a novel boy and were main- ventions to treat social deficits and excesses
tained at a 16-week follow-up. among children with ASD, a few examples will
Brim, Buffington Townsend, DeQuinzio, and be mentioned. Koning, Magill-Evans, Voldem,
Poulson (2009) focused on social referencing and Dick (2013) described a 15-week program to
among children with ASD. Four children were teach social skills to boys with ASD who were
treated for impaired eye contact and not respond- 10–12 years of age. A curriculum was used fol-
ing to their names being called. Training occurred lowing a CBT model. General skill areas targeted
in a therapy room with a table, chairs, a TV, and a involved social aspects of motivation, initiation,
VCR. A token board was placed on the table. perception, responding, and problem-solving. A
Graduated guidance, manual prompts, and rein- manual and worksheets were used to monitor
forcement were among the training procedures weekly goals. Training strategies involved a
that were employed. Children were able to learn to focus on cooperation during group activities.
evince socially appropriate behaviors to standard Therapists focused on verbal praise, prompts,
materials. They were also able to use these new and problem-solving skills. Marked improve-
social skills in the context of ambiguous stimuli ments in the target skills were noted during the
which the authors called social referencing. course of the intervention.
Another group of authors who used peer-­ Using group teaching methods has been
mediated intervention was Bambara, Cole, Kunsch, described by Leaf, Dotson, Oppenheim, Sheldon,
Tsai, and Ayad (2016). For this study, however, and Sherman (2010). They taught five children,
6 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

ages 4–6 years, who had been diagnosed with modeling and pivotal response training. Video
ASD. The intervention consisted of two 1½ h modeling involved a child engaging in social
group meetings per week for 7 months. Structured behaviors and practicing social skills in two ses-
and unstructured games were used to work on sions daily. They continued to practice the social
specific social tasks such as saying “thank you,” skills until they reached criteria. For pivotal
“that’s cool,” and stating their name. Other tar- response training, each child had three preferred
geted skills included appropriate tone of voice toys that they could play with. Symbolic toy play
and facing the person that you are talking to. In was the target skill. Specific behaviors such as
addition to the above, the teacher used praise, eye gaze, touching, and verbal requests were pri-
tokens, performance feedback, and re-practicing orities. The trainer played with the toy and also
situations where social skills errors had been modeled appropriate responses. Marked improve-
made. All of the children showed marked ment was noted for both treatments, although the
improvement in the skills trained. Leaf et al. pivotal response training was more effective than
(2009) also describe a teaching package for social the video modeling approach.
skills for three children 5–7 years of age who had Another classroom-based intervention is
been diagnosed with ASD. Skills taught included described by Banda, Hart, and Liu-Gitz (2010).
play skills, language, and emotional skills. Their study included two children with ASD and
Finally, there was a focus on choosing a friend. three typically developing peers who were
First, the teacher stated the goal for the session. 6 years of age. The focus of training was on ask-
This goal was followed by a provision on why the ing and answering questions. Prompts, modeling,
behavior would help the child. The teacher then and reinforcement were used to teach these skills.
asked the child the steps needed to meet the social Increased responding and more social initiations
skills goals. Incorrect answers were followed resulted from the intervention. In a broader con-
with performance feedback from the teacher. A text, underscoring the technology studies first
second focus on generalization involved “friend- reviewed, DiGennaro Reed, Hyman, and Hirst
ship tickets” for using the appropriate social (2011) used a social skills intervention that used
behaviors with peers. This approach also proved DVDs and videos. They noted that conversational
successful. skills were most frequently taught, followed by
Radley et al. (2014) also described a pro- play skills.
gram that focused on generalization of social
skills to three children 10–14 years of age who
had ASD. Skills focused on were perspective- Conclusions
taking, participation, conversations, and prob-
lem-solving. These authors provided ten The focus of this chapter has been on tracing the
1½-h-long treatment sessions over 5 weeks. development of social skills trainings, with par-
The superheroes DVD served as a major focus ticular emphasis on children and adolescents.
of training. Participants viewed the DVD and Initial efforts were geared toward college-aged
then the therapist modeled correct as well as people with failures in assertiveness. However,
incorrect skill usage. Next, participants were the field has shifted dramatically since that time
taught to self-monitor performance during the with respect to the populations served. A good
role-play scenes. Finally, therapists provided deal of focus on social skills training has
feedback on how the children performed. This occurred. However, the greatest focus at this
methodology proved to be successful. writing is on special needs populations, particu-
Lydon, Healy, and Leader (2011) also used larly individuals with neurodevelopmental disor-
technology to promote social skills via video ders such as ASD and intellectual disabilities.
modeling. Five children with ASD who were The types of behaviors targeted for interven-
3–6 years of age participated. Each child was tion have remained fairly stable. However, the
presented with two separate interventions: video focus of intervention has gone from one-to-one
History of Social Skills 7

therapist to client interventions to methods that Brim, D., Buffington Townsend, D., DeQuinzio, J. A., &
Poulson, C. L. (2009). Analysis of social referencing
are less trainer intensive. Among these trends are
skills among children with autism. Research in Autism
group teaching, the use of teaching methods, and Spectrum Disorders, 3, 942–958.
curriculums and technology such as DVDs and Cappadocia, M. C., & Weiss, J. A. (2011). Review of
videotape feedback and training. social skill training groups for youth with Asperger
syndrome and high functioning autism. Research in
This volume is geared toward providing an in-­
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 70–78.
depth discussion of social skills for children. The Celiberti, D. A., & Harris, S. L. (1993). Behavioral inter-
topic is now a well-established focus of assessment vention for siblings of children with autism: A focus on
and training, since these behaviors have important skills to enhance play. Behavior Therapy, 24, 573–599.
Cervantes, P. E., Matson, J. L., Adams, H. L., Williams,
implications for a range of problems. How social
L. W., Goldin, R. L., & Jang, J. (2013). Comparing
skills are defined and assessed are topics that are social skill profiles of children with autism spectrum
covered as well as theoretical and developmental disorders versus children with attention deficit hyper-
issues. A range of treatment strategies are covered activity disorder: Where the deficits lie. Research in
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 1104–1110.
such as behavior analytic methods, social learning,
Christoff, K. A., Scott, W. O. N., Kelley, M. L., Schlundt,
and parent training. As noted in this chapter, special D., Baer, G., & Kelly, J. A. (1985). Social skills and
populations are also discussed. Different neurode- social problem-solving training for shy young adoles-
velopmental and mental health issues require some cents. Behavior Therapy, 16, 468–477.
Curran, J. P., Graves, D. J., & Cirelli, J. (1982). Social
modifications to assessment and treatment. Various
skills training manual. Unpublished manuscript.
topics are covered and the available research on DiGennaro Reed, F. D., Hyman, S. R., & Hirst, J. M.
these topics are discussed in-depth. The hope is that (2011). Application of technology to teach social
this volume will aid the reader in better understand- skills to children with autism. Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1003–1010.
ing the breadth of available methods available to
Flynn, L., & Healy, O. (2012). A review of treatments for
aid in the improvement of social behavior and deficits in social skills and self-help skills in autism
skills in children. spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders, 6, 431–441.
Foxx, R. M., Faw, G. D., & Weber, G. (1991). Producing
generalization of inpatient adolescents’ social skills
References with significant adults in a natural environment.
Behavior Therapy, 22, 85–99.
Azrin, R. D., & Hayes, S. C. (1984). The discrimination Franco, D. P., Christoff, K. A., Crimmins, D. B., & Kelly,
of interest within a heterosexual interaction: Training, J. A. (1983). Social skills training for an extremely shy
generalization, and effects on social skills. Behavior young adolescent: An empirical case study. Behavior
Therapy, 15, 173–184. Therapy, 12, 568–575.
Bambara, L. M., Cole, C. L., Kunsch, C., Tsai, S.-C., Furman, W., Geller, M., Simon, S. J., & Kelly, J. A.
& Ayad, E. (2016). A peer-mediated intervention to (1979). The use of a behavior rehearsal procedure
improve the conversational skills of high school stu- for teaching job-interviewing skills to psychiatric
dents with autism spectrum disorder. Research in patients. Behavior Therapy, 10, 157–167.
Autism Spectrum Disorder, 27, 29–43. Greco, L. A., & Morris, T. L. (2005). Factors influencing
Banda, D. R., Hart, S. L., & Liu-Gitz, L. (2010). Impact of the link between social anxiety and peer acceptance:
training peers and children with autism on social skills Contributions of social skills and close friendships
during center time activities in inclusive classrooms. during middle childhood. Behavior Therapy, 36,
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorder, 4, 619–625. 197–205.
Beidel, D. C., Bellack, A. S., Turner, S. M., Hersen, M., & Greenwald, D. P. (1977). The behavioral assessment of
Luber, R. F. (1981). Social skills training for chronic differences in social skills and social anxiety in female
psychiatric patients: A treatment manual. JSAS college students. Behavior Therapy, 8, 925–937.
Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 11, 36. Griffiths, D., Feldman, M. A., & Tough, S. (1997).
Bellack, A. S., Brown, C. H., & Thomas-Lohtman, S. Programming generalization of social skills in adults
(2006). Psychometric characteristics of role-play with developmental disabilities: Effects on gener-
assessment of social skills in schizophrenia. Behavior alization and social validity. Behavior Therapy, 28,
Therapy, 37, 339–352. 253–269.
Bradlyn, A. S., Himadi, W. G., Crimmins, D. B., Graves, Hayes, R. L., Halford, W. K., & Varghese, F. T. (1995).
K. G., & Kelly, L. A. (1983). Conversational skills Social skills training with chronic schizophrenic
training for retarded adolescents. Behavior Therapy, patients: Effects on negative symptoms and commu-
14, 314–325. nity functioning. Behavior Therapy, 26, 433–449.
8 J.L. Matson and C.O. Burns

Heiby, E. M. (1986). Social versus self-control skills defi- Matson, J. L., Esveldt-Dawson, K., Andrasik, F.,
cits in four cases of depression. Behavior Therapy, 17, Ollendick, F., Petti, T., & Hersen, M. (1980). Direct,
158–169. observational, and generalization effects of social
Herbert, J. D., Gaudino, B. A., Rheingold, A. A., Myers, skills training with emotionally disturbed children.
V. H., Dalyrymple, K., & Nolan, E. M. (2005). Social Behavior Therapy, 11, 522–531.
skills training augments the effectiveness of cognitive Matson, J. L., Horovitz, M., Mahan, S., & Fodstad,
behavioral group therapy for social anxiety disorder. J. (2013). Reliability of the Matson Evaluation of
Behavior Therapy, 36, 125–138. Social Skills with Youngsters (MESSY) for children
Hersen, M., Bellack, A. S., Himmelhoch, J. M., & Thase, with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism
M. E. (1984). Effects of social skill training, amitripty- Spectrum Disorders, 7, 405–410.
line, and psychotherapy in unipolar depressed women. Matson, J. L., & Senatore, V. (1981). A comparison of
Behavior Therapy, 15, 21–40. traditional psychotherapy and social skills training
Hill, T. L., Varela, R. E., Kamps, J. L., & Niditch, L. A. for improving interpersonal functioning of mentally
(2014). Local processing and social skills in children retarded adults. Behavior Therapy, 12, 369–382.
with autism spectrum disorders: The role of anxi- Matson, J. L., & Wilkins, J. (2007). A critical review
ety and cognitive functioning. Research in Autism of assessment targets and methods for social skills
Spectrum Disorders, 8, 1243–1251. excesses and deficits for children with autism spectrum
Hoffart, A., Borge, F.-M., & Clark, D. M. (2009). Change disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1,
processes in residential cognitive and interpersonal 28–37.
psychotherapy for social phobia: A process-outcome Matson, J. L., Worley, J. A., Kozlowski, A. M., Chung,
study. Behavior Therapy, 40, 10–22. K.-M., Jung, W., & Yang, J. W. (2012). Cross-cultural
Kazdin, A. E., Esveldt-Dawson, K., & Matson, J. L. differences of parent reported social skills in children
(1983). The effects of instructional set on social skills with autistic disorder: An examination between South
performance among psychiatric inpatients. Behavior Korea and the United States of America. Research in
Therapy, 14, 413–423. Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 971–977.
Kelly, J. A., Furmna, W., Philips, J., Hathorn, S., & Wilson, McFall, R. M., & Lillesaud, D. B. (1971). Behavioral
T. (1979). Teaching conversational skills to retarded rehearsal with modeling and coaching in asser-
adolescent. Child Behavior Therapy, 1, 85–97. tion training. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 77,
Kelly, J. A., Wildman, B. G., Very, J. R., & Thurman, 313–323.
C. (1979). Group social skills training to improve McFall, R. M., & Twentyman, C. T. (1973). Four experi-
the conversational repertoire of retarded adolescents. ments on the relative contributions of rehearsal, mod-
Child Behavior Therapy, 1, 323–336. eling, and coaching to assertion training. Journal of
Koning, C., Magill-Evans, J., Voldem, J., & Dick, B. Abnormal Psychology, 81, 199–218.
(2013). Efficacy of cognitive behavior therapy-­ O’Callaghan, P. M., Reitman, D., Northup, J., Hupp,
based social skills intervention for school-aged boys S. D. A., & Murphy, M. A. (2003). Promoting social
with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism skills generalization with ADHD-diagnosed chil-
Spectrum Disorders, 7, 1282–1290. dren with a sports setting. Behavior Therapy, 4,
Leaf, J. B., Dotson, W. H., Oppenheim, M. L., Sheldon, 313–330.
J. B., & Sherman, J. A. (2010). The effectiveness Radley, K. C., O’Handley, R. D., Ness, E. J., Ford, B.,
of a group teaching interaction procedure for teach- Battaglia, A. A., McHugh, M. B., & McLemore, C. E.
ing social skills for young children with pervasive (2014). Promoting social skill use and generalization
developmental disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum in children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in
Disorders, 4, 186–198. Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8, 669–680.
Leaf, J. B., Taubman, M., Bloomfield, S., Palos-Rafuse, Sacks, S., & Gaylord-Ross, R. (1989). Peer-mediated
L., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., & Oppenheim, M. L. and teacher-directed social skills training for visually
(2009). Increasing social skills and pro-social behav- impaired students. Behavior Therapy, 20, 619–638.
ior for three children diagnosed with autism through Senatore, V., Matson, J. L., & Kazdin, A. E. (1982). A
the use of a teaching package. Research in Autism comparison of behavioral methods to train social
Spectrum Disorders, 3, 275–289. skills to mentally retarded adults. Behavior Therapy,
Ledley, D. R., Heimberg, R. G., Hope, D. A., Hayes, S. A., 13, 313–324.
Zaider, T. I., Dyke, M. V., … Fresco, D. M. (2009). Sukhodolsky, D. G., Golub, A., Stone, E. C., & Orban,
Efficacy of a manualized and workbook-driven indi- L. (2005). Dismantling anger control training for
vidual treatment for social anxiety disorder. Behavior children: A randomized pilot study of social problem-­
Therapy, 40, 414–424. solving versus social skills training components.
Liberman, R. P. (1992). Handbook of psychiatric rehabili- Behavior Therapy, 36, 15–23.
tation. New York: Macmillian. Wang, S.-Y., Cui, Y., & Parilla, R. (2011). Examining the
Lydon, H., Healy, O., & Leader, G. (2011). A compari- effectiveness of peer-mediated and video-modeling
son of video modeling and pivotal response training social skills interventions for children with autism
to teach pretend play skill to children with autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis in single-case
spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum research using HLM. Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders, 5, 872–884. Disorders, 5, 562–569.
Defining Social Skills

Steven G. Little, John Swangler,
and Angeleque Akin-Little

As a psychologist who works primarily with To illustrate the wide variety of definitions of
­children and youth, it is clear that parents and social skills, an internet search yielded these defi­
teachers frequently identify “social skills” as a nitions. “These are the skills that allow a person
primary concern. The variability in what they to interact and to act appropriately in given social
mean by “social skills” is tremendous however. It contexts” (http://psychologydictionary.org/
could be a simple as not making consistent eye social-skills/). “Social skills are ways of dealing
contact when conversing with someone, to inter­ with others that create healthy and positive inter­
rupting others, to aggression, and other mala­ actions” (http://study.com/academy/lesson/what-
daptive behaviors. While there may be no one are-social-skills-in-children-development-defini­
accepted definition of social skills, our under­ tion-teaching-techniques.html). “The personal
standing of social skills has come a long way skills needed for successful social communica­
since Libet and Lewinsohn (1973, p. 311) defined tion and interaction” (http://www.dictionary.
social skills as the “complex ability to maximize com/browse/social-skills). “Social skills are the
the rate of positive reinforcement and to mini­ skills we use to communicate and interact with
mize the strength of punishment elicited from each other, both verbally and non-verbally,
others.” This chapter will present a number of through gestures, body language and our per­
definitions and methods of assessing social defi­ sonal appearance” (http://www.skillsyouneed.
cits and excesses with a focus on translating these com/ips/social-skills.html). “The skills that are
definitions into an understanding of common dis­ necessary in order to communicate and interact
orders involving social skills and methods of with others” (http://www.collinsdictionary.com/
operationalizing social skill/competence. dictionary/english/social-skills). “A level of
interpersonal savvy, which often determines
future social adjustment and success” (http://
medical-dictionary.thefreedictionary.com/
Social+skills). “Social skill is any skill facilitat­
S.G. Little (*) ing interaction and communication with others”
Walden University, Minneapolis, MN, USA
e-mail: stevenlittlephd@yahoo.com (https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Social_skills).
The two most common threads throughout
J. Swangler
Phoenix, AZ, USA these definitions involve communication and
interaction with others, and while these defini­
A. Akin-Little
Akin-Little and Little Behavioral Psychology tions are outside the confines of traditional schol­
Consultants, Malone, NY, USA arly literature, they reflect the consensus within

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 9


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_2
10 S.G. Little et al.

scholarly writing. For example, Gresham and putting) and how these contributed to the final
Elliott (1984) defined social skills as socially score. These are analogous to social skills. The
acceptable learned behaviors that enable a person remainder of this chapter will focus on social
to interact with others in ways that elicit positive skills.
responses and assist in avoiding negative Gresham (1986, 1997) categorized social
responses. Phillips (1978) defined social skills as skills into three definitional areas: peer accep­
the interaction between a person and his/her envi­ tance, behavioral, and social validity. The peer
ronment and the ability to begin and sustain inter­ acceptance definition uses peer acceptance or
personal relationships. Cook, Gresham, Barreras, popularity to define social skills (Oden & Asher,
Thornton, and Crews (2008) described social 1977). In other words, children accepted and
skills as involving learned behaviors that involve thought of as popular by their peers are consid­
interactions with others which enable individuals ered socially skilled via peer-referenced assess­
to function competently at social tasks. Ladd ment procedures such as sociometric assessment
(2005) identified culturally associated learned (Frick, Barry, & Kamphaus, 2010; Gresham &
behaviors exhibited during interactions between Little, 1992). However, this definition defines
a child and peers and adults as comprising social social skills by their outcome and fails to identify
skills. While social skills definitions may vary the specific behaviors which lead to peer accep­
somewhat, there is some basic agreement as to tance. The behavioral definition, however,
how social skills are developed. Specifically, focuses on situation-specific behaviors that maxi­
social skills involve specific learned behaviors, mize the probability of reinforcement while
are comprised of both initiation and response decreasing the probability of punishment (Foster
behaviors, and entail interactions with others. & Ritchey, 1979; Libet & Lewinsohn, 1973).
These skills are also socially reinforced and This definition allows social behavior to be speci­
denote skills that are context specific. Simply put, fied and operationalized for measurement and
social skills are those skills that enable individu­ intervention. However, it does not ensure that the
als to function competently at social tasks (Cook identified social behaviors are socially significant
et al., 2008). or socially relevant. The social validity definition
Related to social skills is the term social com­ defines social skills as exhibiting behaviors that
petence. Social competence, however, is believed predict important social outcomes in particular
to reflect social judgment about the quality of an situations (Gresham, 1983; Kazdin, 1977; Wolf,
individual’s performance, while social skills con­ 1978). Gresham (1986) sees this definition as a
sist of specific skills that form the basis for hybrid of the peer acceptance and behavioral
socially competent behavior (Hops, 1983). Social definitions.
skills are considered to be the “most malleable of Gresham (1986) goes on to discuss social
the components of social competence” (Elliott & skills difficulty as categorized into four types:
Busse, 1991, p. 64). Semrud-Clikeman (2007) skill deficits, performance deficits, self-control
described social competence as consisting of skill deficits, and self-control performance defi­
social, emotional, cognitive, and behavioral com­ cits. Skill deficits are present when the individual
ponents needed for successful social adaptation. lacks the skills needed for appropriate social
Social skills can be thought of as the behavioral interaction. It is this definition of social skills
component of Semrud-Clikeman’s definition. deficit that some have used when advocating for
Greenspan (1981) provided an interesting anal­ social skills deficits as a specific learning disabil­
ogy that helps clarify the distinction between ity (Gresham & Elliott, 1989). Performance defi­
social skills and social competence. Competence cits categorize children who possess the capability
as a golfer is usually defined by the outcome, the of performing the behavior but do not perform
final score. This is analogous to social compe­ these behaviors at an acceptable level due to fac­
tence. The golf score, however, tells us nothing tors such as motivation or lack of opportunity. If
about the skills (e.g., driving, approach shots, you observe situation-specific performance of the
Defining Social Skills 11

behavior (e.g., at home but not at school), it is Children, Third Edition; BASC-3; Reynolds &
most likely a performance deficit. Self-control Kampaus, 2015; Achenbach system of empiri­
skill deficits are identified when an individual has cally based assessment; Achenbach et al., 2004),
not learned a social skill due to some sort of emo­ and autism rating scales (Childhood Autism
tional arousal. For example, anxiety interferes Rating Scale, Second Edition; CARS-2; Schopler,
with learning the social skill. Finally, self-controlVan Bourgondien, Wellman, & Love, 2010:
performance deficits also involve interference Gilliam Autism Rating Scale; GARS-3, Gilliam,
from emotional arousal, but in this case it has not 2014). In addition, sociometric ratings such as
interfered with the acquisition of the behavior but peer nominations are also frequently used. The
rather with its performance. following section will review various measures
Constantino, Przybeck, Friesen, and Todd and describe the dimensions of the social skills
(2000) defined social skills in part through five defined and assessed in each.
components: social awareness, social cognition, Social Skills Improvement System Rating
social communication, social motivation, and Scales (SSIS). The SSIS (Gresham & Elliott,
autistic mannerisms. Social awareness involves 2008) is probably the most comprehensive norm-­
the ability to recognize social cues and represents referenced measure to screen and classify chil­
the sensory aspects of reciprocal behavior. Social dren and youth suspected of having significant
cognition is the ability to interpret social cues social skills deficits. With an age range of 3–18,
once they are identified and represents the the SSIS consists of four components. The main
cognitive-­interpretive aspect of reciprocal behav­ measure, the social skills scale, measures positive
ior. Social communication includes behaviors social behaviors while interacting with others.
such as expressive social communication and The behavior problems scale focuses on negative
represents the motoric aspect of reciprocal behav­ behaviors that compete with social competence
ior. Social motivation incorporates factors such and may be needed to be addressed in interven­
as social anxiety, inhibition, and empathic orien­ tion planning. The autism spectrum subscale pro­
tation that influence the individual’s motivation vides a screen of ASD symptomology, and the
to respond in a socially responsive manner. academic competence scale consists of a teach­
Finally, autistic mannerisms include aspects of er’s rating of academic performance relative to
ASD such as stereotyped behaviors and highly classmates. Social skills, as defined by the social
restricted interests that, while idiosyncratic, need skills scale, consist of seven domains of social
to be addressed in any intervention designed to behavior (Table 1) that the test authors identify as
improve social functioning. important: communication, cooperation, asser­
tion, responsibility, empathy, engagement, and
self-control. The communication subscale
Operationalizing Social Skills includes behaviors such as making eye contact
when talking and saying “please” and “thank
In practice social skills are most likely going to you.” The cooperation subscale includes behav­
be defined based on the measure used to assess iors such as following rules and completing tasks
the construct. There are a number of norm-­ without bothering others. The assertion subscale
referenced measures that either focus primarily includes behaviors such as asks for help when
on social skills or have a social skills dimension. needed and says when there’s a problem. The
These include comprehensive measures of social responsibility subscale includes behaviors such
skills (e.g., social skills improvement system rat­ as respects the property of others and is well
ing scales, SSIS; Gresham & Elliott, 2008), adap­ behaved when unsupervised. The empathy sub­
tive behavior measures (e.g., Vineland Adaptive scale includes behaviors such as tries to comfort
Behavior Scales, Third Edition; Sparrow, others and feels bad when others are sad. The
Cicchetti, & Saulnier, 2016), behavior rating engagement subscale includes behaviors such as
scales (e.g., Behavior Assessment System for invites others to join in activities, makes friends
12 S.G. Little et al.

Table 1  Operationalization of social skills on common norm-referenced assessments


SSIS Vineland-3 GARS-3 CARS BASC-3 ASEBA/CBCL
Communication Interpersonal Social interaction, Relating to Social Social
relationships people skills competence
Cooperation Play and leisure Social Imitation
time communication
Assertion Coping skills Emotional
response
Responsibility Communication Social-emotional
understanding
Empathy
Engagement
Engagement
Self-control

easily, and introduces himself to others. Finally, awareness of manners, social sensitivity, and fol­
the self-control subscale includes behaviors such lowing social rules.
as stays calm when teased and uses appropriate Behavior Assessment System for Children,
behavior when upset. Third Edition (BASC-3). The BASC-3 (Reynolds
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third & Kampaus, 2015) is a comprehensive set of rat­
Edition. The Vineland (Sparrow et al., 2016) is an ing scales which include the teacher rating scales
individually administered measure of adaptive (TRS), parent rating scales (PRS), self-report of
behavior. The publisher describes the purpose of personality (SRP), student observation system
the Vineland as aiding in diagnosing and classi­ (SOS), and structured developmental history
fying intellectual and developmental disabilities (SDH), the purpose of which is to provide a com­
and other disorders, such as autism. It is orga­ plete picture of a child’s behavior. Both the TRS
nized using the three domains of adaptive func­ and PRS include a social skills scale which is
tioning specified by the American Association on defined as “the skills necessary for interacting
Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities and successfully with peers and adults in home,
by DSM-5: communication, daily living skills, school, and community settings” (Pearson
and socialization. Social skills are assessed in Education, 2016, p. 4). In addition, both the TRS
both the communication and socialization and PRS include a functional communication
domains (Table 1). Communication is broken scale which is also related to social skills. It is
down into expressive, receptive, and written sub­ defined as “the ability to express ideas and com­
domains with the expressive and receptive subdo­ municate in a way others can easily understand”
mains most related to social skills. Receptive (Pearson Education, 2016, p. 4) (Table 1).
communication is the process of receiving and Achenbach System of Empirically Based
understanding a message, while expressive com­ Assessment (ASEBS). The ASEBA (Achenbach
munication involves the way one conveys the et al., 2004) provides a comprehensive approach
message to others. The socialization domain is to assessing adaptive and maladaptive function­
broken down into three subdomains: interper­ ing in children and youth. The child behavior
sonal relationships, play and leisure, and coping checklist (CBCL) is the primary behavior rating
skills. Interpersonal relationships cover social scale component of the ASEBA and consists of
interaction, dating, and friendship skills. Play the CBCL/1½–5 and C-TRF for preschoolers and
and leisure consists of behaviors such as going to the CBCL, teacher report form (TRF), and youth
clubs, playing games, hobbies, and leisure activi­ self-report (YSR) for school age children. All
ties. Lastly coping skills includes factors such as versions of the CBCL are completed by parents
or surrogates, while the TRF is completed by
Defining Social Skills 13

teachers or other school staff. In addition, the sion is “appropriate imitation. The child can imi­
preschool CBCL contains a language develop­ tate sounds, words, and movements that are
ment survey (LDS) which assesses a child’s appropriate for his or her skill set” (Perry,
vocabulary and word combinations relative to Condillac, Freeman, Dunn-Geier, & Belair, 2005,
norms for ages 18–35 months. Social skills is p. 629). A second example is “mildly abnormal
defined primarily by the social competence scale imitation. The child imitates simple behaviors”
and social problems syndrome scale of the CBCL (Perry et al., 2005, p. 629).
(Table 1). Gilliam Autism Rating Scale (GARS-3). The
The social competence scale on the CBCL, GARS-3 (Gilliam, 2014) is a norm-referenced
TRF, and YSR assesses participation in group screening instrument used to “identify persons
activities and social relationships. Informants are [ages 3–22 years] who have autism spectrum dis­
asked to list the number of clubs/teams organiza­ orders [ASD]” (manual, p. 1). Based on the diag­
tions in which the child participates, involvement nostic criteria of the DSM-5, the GARS-3 is
in jobs/chores, the number of friends the child composed of six subscales, two descriptive of the
has, and number of times he/she interacts with DSM-5 domain of deficits in social communica­
friends. In addition, informants are asked to rate tion and interaction and four descriptive of the
how well or how frequently the child performs repetitive behavior domain, yielding six standard
these actions compared to same age peers. This scores and an autism index. Social skills is
scale is believed to be a positive indicator of defined based on the social interaction and social
social functioning. The social problems scale on communication subscales (Table 1). Social inter­
the CBCL, TRF, and YSR assesses immature action is defined as a child’s ability to relate
social behaviors and difficulties in peer relation­ appropriately to people, events, and objects.
ships. Examples of items from this scale include Examples of behaviors assessed on this subscale
“clings to adults or too dependent,” “gets teased,” include behaviors such as making eye contact,
“not liked,” “too dependent,” “prefers being with recognizing the presence of others, and laughs,
younger children,” and “lonely.” This scale is giggles, and cries appropriately. Social commu­
believed to be a negative indicator of social nication involves behaviors of social reciprocity
functioning. (social interaction, social skills) and the behav­
Childhood Autism Rating Scale, Second iors of communication and language that result in
Edition (CARS-2). The CARS-2 (Schopler et al., one’s ability to communicate socially. Examples
2010) is designed to identify behavioral symp­ of behaviors assessed on this subscale include
toms of autism. The standard version (CARS-­ initiating conversations with peers or adults,
2-­ST) is used with children below ages 6 and using “yes” and “no” appropriately, using pro­
those with communication deficits or an esti­ nouns appropriately, and using the word “I”
mated IQ of 79 or below. The high-functioning appropriately.
version (CARS-2-HF) is designed for children Sociometric Ratings. Sociometric assessment
age 6 and up with estimated IQ above 80. The involves the measurement of interpersonal rela­
standard version is designed to be completed by tionships in the context of a social group such as
parents or caregivers and provides scores on 16 a classroom with the goal of gaining information
dimensions. Included in these dimensions are about an individual’s social competence and
relating to people, imitation, emotional response, standing within a peer group. This includes
and social-emotional understanding all of which dimensions such as social popularity, peer accep­
relate to social skills and aid in assessing the tance, peer rejection, and reputation (Merrell,
DSM-5 ASD diagnostic criteria for “persistent 1999). The most widely used sociometric rating
deficits in social communication and social inter­ technique is peer nomination which involves
action across multiple contexts” (American individuals in a group (e.g., classroom) are asked
Psychiatric Association, 2013, p. 50) (Table 1). to nominate peers he or she likes most or likes
An example of an item from the imitation dimen­ least (Gresham & Little, 1992; Poulin & Dishion,
14 S.G. Little et al.

2008). The nominations received are tallied and tions (e.g., outside to inside), and making and
used to create sociometric categories (rejected, keeping friends. According to the DSM-5
popular, controversial, neglected, and average) or (American Psychiatric Association, 2013, p. 48),
a continuous index of peer status (acceptance, “the deficits result in functional limitations in
rejection, social preference) (Coie, Dodge, & effective communication, social participation,
Coppotelli, 1982). Peer ratings are involved pro­ social relationships, academic achievement, or
viding a list of children’s names in the social occupational performance, individually or in
group along with a rating for social acceptance combination.”
items such as “The most fun to play with” on a Autism spectrum disorder. Social skills defi­
three- or five-point Likert scale. Sociometric rat­ cits comprise one of the essential features of
ings can also be completed by adults (e.g., a autism spectrum disorder (ASD). This is defined
teacher) who have had opportunities to observe as “persistent deficits in social communication
children in the group in multiple contexts. The and social interaction across multiple contexts”
ratings can be on any social dimension but tend to (American Psychiatric Association, 2013, p. 50)
be similar to those used with children in peer rat­ and is composed of three specific areas of deficit:
ings. While these techniques have been used (a) social emotional reciprocity (e.g., failure of
more in research than in practice (McClelland & normal back and forth conversation), (b) nonver­
Scalzo, 2006), they do provide an interesting bal communication used in social interactions
manner in which to operationalize social skills. (e.g., abnormalities in eye contact), and (c)
“developing, maintaining, and understanding
relationships” (p. 50) (e.g., difficulty in making
Social Skills and Disability friends). The DSM-5 also specifies that these
deficits are “pervasive and sustained” and are
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental present from early development.
Disorders (DSM 5, American Psychiatric Social Anxiety Disorder (Social Phobia).
Association, 2013) contains only one disorder in Unlike SCD or ASD which tends to be diagnosed
which social skill-related behavior is included in relatively early in child development, social anxi­
the title, social (pragmatic) communication dis­ ety disorder (SAD) typically develops in later
order. Social skills, however, contribute greatly to childhood and early adolescence with a median
the diagnostic criteria of a number of other disor­ age of onset of 13 years (American Psychiatric
ders including autism spectrum disorder, intellec­ Association, 2013). SAD also differs from the
tual disability, attention-deficit/hyperactivity social skills deficits essential to SCD and ASD as
disorder, and social anxiety disorder. the etiology is thought to be due more to behav­
Social (Pragmatic) Communication Disorder. ioral inhibition, as opposed to an actual deficit in
Social (pragmatic) communication disorder social skills (Angélico, Crippa, & Loureiro,
(SCD) encompasses problems with social inter­ 2013). SAD is defined by timidity, distress, and
action, social understanding, and pragmatics avoidance in/of social settings (Beidel, Rao,
(using language in proper context). SCD is dif­ Scharfstein, Wong, & Alfano, 2010), and it may
ferentiated from autism spectrum disorder (ASD) be best described as an impairment in social
in that ASD includes the presence of restrictive/ skills rather than a deficit. DSM-5 diagnostic cri­
repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, and teria describe these impairments as marked by
activities which are not present in SCD. Both ver­ fear and anxiety of social situations in which the
bal and nonverbal social communication skills individual is exposed to possible scrutiny from
define the social skills deficits associated with others which in turn impairs social interactions
SCD, including using gestures, reciprocal talking (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
or playing, maintaining focus on the topic being Intellectual Disability. The DSM-5 (American
discussed, adjusting speech to conform to Psychiatric Association, 2013) describes intellec­
changes in people (e.g., child to adult) or situa­ tual disability (ID) as impairment of general
Defining Social Skills 15

mental abilities that impact adaptive functioning it using a more social competence definition, but
in three domains. The conceptual domain con­ by using this definition, you may inadvertently
sists of functioning in areas such as language, ignore the requisite component behaviors that
reading, writing, math, reasoning, knowledge, contribute to an individual being considered
and memory. The social domain refers to empa­ socially skilled/competent. There is also a social
thy, social judgment, interpersonal communica­ validity component to social skills which defines
tion skills, the ability to make and retain social skills as exhibiting behaviors that predict
friendships, and similar capacities. The practical important social outcomes in particular situations
domain centers on self-management in areas (Gresham, 1983; Kazdin, 1977; Wolf, 1978)
such as personal care, job responsibilities, money which cannot be ignored.
management, recreation, and organizing school In practice social skills are most likely going
and work tasks. to be defined based on the measure used to assess
Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. The the construct. There are a number of norm-­
DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, referenced measures that either focus primarily
2013) defines attention-deficit hyperactivity dis­ on social skills or have a social skills dimension.
order (ADHD) as being characterized by symp­ These include comprehensive measures of social
toms of hyperactivity, inattention, and impulsivity skills, adaptive behavior measures, behavior rat­
that interfere with daily and occupational func­ ing scales, and autism rating scales as well as
tioning. While social skills deficits are not spe­ sociometric ratings (e.g., peer nominations) that
cifically identified in the ADHD diagnostic are used to assess an individual’s social standing
criteria, behaviors common to ADHD such as with a social group (e.g., a classroom). The most
inattention and impulsivity may interfere with comprehensive of these measures is the social
the development or expression of positive social skills improvement system rating scale (SSIS;
skills (Bunford, Evans, Becker, & Langberg, Gresham & Elliott, 2008) which assesses the
2015). For example, children with ADHD may areas of communication, cooperation, assertion,
not recognize how their behavior is affecting oth­ responsibility, empathy, engagement, and
ers. Behaviors such as interrupting or making self-control.
inappropriate comments may interfere with their Finally, it is important to understand social
making or maintaining friends. skills not only in the context of the normal range
of behavior. Social skills deficits contribute a
large component to many childhood disorders.
Summary While social skills deficits are a core component
of an autism spectrum diagnosis, it also plays an
Social skills are a common concern for both par­ important role in many other disorders including
ents and teachers. There are, however, many social (pragmatic) communication disorder,
ways to define and operationalize the construct. intellectual disability, attention-deficit/hyperac­
While definitions may vary on the specific behav­ tivity disorder, and social anxiety disorder.
iors, almost all involve aspects of social commu­
nication and interaction with others in a social
context. While published more than 30 years ago, References
Gresham and Elliott’s (1984) definition of social
skills as socially acceptable learned behaviors Achenbach, T. M., Rescorla, L. A., McConaughey, S. H.,
Pecora, P. J., Wetherbee, K. M., Ruffle, T. M., &
that enable a person to interact with others in Newhouse, P. A. (2004). Achenbach system of empiri-
ways that elicit positive responses and assist in cally based assessment. Burlington, VT: ASEBA
avoiding negative responses probably best sums Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families.
up the thinking of those involved in social skills American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
research over the past few decades. Social skills, Washington, DC: Author.
however, is not a unitary construct. Some define
16 S.G. Little et al.

Angélico, A. P., Crippa, J. A. S., & Loureiro, S. R. Gresham, F. M. (1997). Social competence and students
(2013). Social anxiety disorder and social skills: A with behavior disorders: Where we’ve been, where we
critical review of the literature. International Journal are, and where we should go. Education and Treatment
of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 7, 16–23. of Children, 20, 233–249.
doi:10.1037/h0100961 Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1984). Assessment and
Beidel, D. C., Rao, P. A., Scharfstein, L., Wong, N., & classification of children’s social skills: A review of
Alfano, C. A. (2010). Social skills and social phobia: methods and issues. School Psychology Review, 13,
An investigation of DSM-IV subtypes. Behaviour 292–301.
Research and Therapy, 48, 992–1001. doi:10.1016/j. Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1989). Social
brat.2010.06.005 skills deficits as a primary learning disability.
Bunford, N., Evans, S. W., Becker, S. P., & Langberg, Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 120–124.
J. M. (2015). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disor­ doi:10.1177/002221948902200207
der and social skills in youth: A moderated media­ Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (2008). Social skills
tion model of emotion dysregulation and depression. improvement system rating scales. San Antonio, TX:
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 283–296. Pearson Assessment.
doi:10.1007/s10802-014-9909-2 Gresham, F. M., & Little, S. G. (1992). Peer-referenced
Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982). assessment strategies. In M. Hersen & T. Ollendick
Dimensions and types of social status: A cross-age (Eds.), Handbook of child and adolescent assessment
perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18, 557–570. (pp. 165–179). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.18.4.557 Hops, H. (1983). Children's social competence and
Constantino, J. N., Przybeck, T., Friesen, D., & Todd, skill: Current research practices and future direc­
R. D. (2000). Reciprocal social behavior in children tions. Behavior Therapy, 14, 3–18. doi:10.1016/
with and without pervasive developmental disorders. s0005-7894(83)80084-7
Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, Kazdin, A. E. (1977). Assessing the clinical or applied
21, 2–11. doi:10.1097/00004703-200002000-00002 importance of behavior change through social
Cook, C. R., Gresham, L. K., Barreras, R. B., Thornton, validation. Behavior Modification, 1, 427–451.
S., & Crews, S. D. (2008). Socials skills train­ doi:10.1177/014544557714001
ing for secondary students with emotional and/ Ladd, G. (2005). Children’s peer relations and social
or behavioral disorders: A review and analy­ competence: A century of progress. New Haven, CT:
sis of the meta-analytic literature. Journal of Yale University Press.
Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 16, 131–144. Libet, J., & Lewinsohn, M. (1973). Concept of social
doi:10.1177/1063426608314541 skills with special reference to the behavior of
Elliott, S. N., & Busse, R. T. (1991). Social skills depressed patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
assessment and intervention with children and ado­ Psychology, 401, 304–312.
lescents: Guidelines for assessment and training pro­ McClelland, M. M., & Scalzo, C. (2006). Social skills
cedures. School Psychology International, 12, 63–83. deficits. In M. Hersen (Ed.), Clinician’s hand-
doi:10.1177/0143034391121006 book of child behavioral assessment (pp. 313–335).
Foster, S. L., & Ritchey, W. L. (1979). Issues in the assess­ New York: Elsevier.
ment of social competence in children. Journal of Merrell, K. W. (1999). Behavioral, social, and emotional
Applied Behavior Analysis, 12, 625–638. doi:10.1901/ assessment of children and adolescents. Mahwah, NJ:
jaba.1979.12-625 Erlbaum.
Frick, P. J., Barry, P. T., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2010). Oden, S., & Asher, S. R. (1977). Coaching children in
Clinical assessment of child and adolescent personal- social skills for friendship making. Child Development,
ity and behavior. New York: Springer. 48, 495–506. doi:10.2307/1128645
Gilliam, J. E. (2014). Gilliam autism rating scale—Third Pearson Education. (2016). BASC-3 scales, composites
edition. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. and indexes for the TRS, PRS, and SRP. Retrieved from
Greenspan, S. (1981). Social competence and handi­ http://images.pearsonclinical.com/images/assets/
capped individuals: Practical implications and pro­ basc-3/CLINA15775-29093-BASC3-WhitePaper-
posed model. Advances in Special Education, 3, Hr-f_FINAL.pdf
41–82. Perry, A., Condillac, R. A., Freeman, N. L., Dunn-­
Gresham, F. M. (1983). Social skills assessment as Geier, J., & Belair, J. (2005). Multi-site study of the
a component of mainstreaming placement deci­ Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) in five clini­
sions. Exceptional Children, 49, 331–336. cal groups of young children. Journal of Autism and
doi:10.1177/001440298304900406 Developmental Disorders, 35, 625–634. doi:10.1007/
Gresham, F. M. (1986). Conceptual and definitional s10803-005-0006-9
issues in the assessment of children's social skills: Phillips, E. L. (1978). The social skills basis of psychopa-
Implications for classification and training. Journal thology. New York: Grune & Stratton.
of Clinical Child Psychology, 15, 3–15. doi:10.1207/ Poulin, F., & Dishion, T. J. (2008). Methodological
s15374424jccp1501_1 issues in the use of peer sociometric nominations with
Defining Social Skills 17

­ iddle school youth. Social Development, 13, 908–


m Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2007). Social competence in chil-
921. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00473.x dren. New York: Springer.
Reynolds, C. R., & Kampaus, R. W. (2015). Behavior Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Saulnier, C. A. (2016).
assessment system for children (3rd ed.). San Antonio, Vineland adaptive behavior scales (3rd ed.). San
TX: Pearson Assessment. Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessment.
Schopler, E., Van Bourgondien, M. E., Wellman, G. J., & Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: The case for subjec­
Love, S. R. (2010). Childhood autism rating scale (2nd tive measurement or how behavior analysis is finding
ed.). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services. its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11,
203–214. doi:10.1901/jaba.1978.11-203
Challenging Behavior

Catia Cividini-Motta, Sarah E. Bloom,
and Claudia Campos

When children, whether or not they have a


Challenging Behavior d­ isability, do not have the social skill repertoire
and Social Skills required to get what they want in a particular situ-
ation, they may engage in other behavior in an
Although challenging behavior is seen in children attempt to get their needs met. This other behavior
without disabilities, its prevalence is greater in may be shaped into severe topographies like
children with disabilities (Didden, Korzilius, van aggression or self-injury, or it may remain in
Oorsouw, & Sturmey, 2006; Emerson, 2003; milder forms, like tantrums or flopping.
Whitaker & Read, 2006). Holden and Gitlesen Regardless, challenging behavior can result in
(2006) report that approximately 10–15% of indi- placement in restrictive settings or result in the
viduals with intellectual disabilities engage in use of restrictive or punishment-based procedures
challenging behavior. Other estimates place that (Borkwick-Duffy, Eyman, & White, 1987;
percentage much higher (e.g., Atkinson et al., Lerman & Vorndran, 2002) and can result in seri-
1994; Tonge & Einfeld, 2003). Children with ous harm (Kahng, Iwata, & Lewin, 2002; Symons,
intellectual and developmental disabilities are also Harper, McGrath, Breau, & Bodfish, 2009).
more likely to also experience social deficits Addressing challenging behavior often involves
(Odom, McConnell, & Brown, 2008). In fact, teaching a response related to social skills. Later in
some disorders are characterized by the presence this chapter, we will describe specific interven-
of social deficits. For example, autism spectrum tions based on functions of problem behavior, but
disorder’s (ASD) diagnostic criteria include both one common general approach is to determine
social and communication impairments (American what the child is attempting to change in his or her
Psychiatric Association, 2013; McPartland, environment (e.g., access adult attention, escape
Reichow, & Volkmar, 2012). Although age, sex, from a particular task) and to teach them other,
and level of intellectual disability are not predic- more socially acceptable, ways to make that
tive of challenging behavior (Murphy, Healy, & change happen. Thus, the challenging behavior is
Leader, 2009), a lack of adequate social skills is a replaced with a socially appropriate behavior.
risk factor for the development of challenging This approach is one of many approaches that
behavior (Didden et al., 2006). can be used after the challenging behavior has
already been established. However, challenging
behavior can presumably be prevented from
C. Cividini-Motta • S.E. Bloom (*) • C. Campos developing by targeting social deficits and pro­
University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USA viding interventions to strengthen social skills
e-mail: sarahbloom@usf.edu

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 19


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_3
20 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

(Hanley, Heal, Tiger, & Ingvarsson, 2007; pleted by caregivers, staff, or teachers with the
Luczynski & Hanley, 2013). This way, situations assistance of a behavior analyst, to gather infor-
in which a child does not have the social repertoire mation about the settings and environmental
to obtain what they need are avoided, and chal- contingencies that are associated with the prob-
lenging behavior does not fill the gap left by the lem behavior. There are several practical indi-
social skill deficit. Developing social skills, and rect assessment tools that are available for
the resulting social relationships, are suggestive of behavior analysts to use; some of these include
positive social and educational outcomes (National the Functional Analysis Screening Tool (FAST;
Research Council and Institute of Medicine, Iwata, DeLeon, & Roscoe, 2013), Functional
2000). Strategies to teach social and play skills can Assessment Interview (FAI; O’Neill, Albin,
be found in early childhood special education cur- Storey, Horner, & Sprague, 2014), and the
riculum (e.g., Sandall & Schwartz, 2008). Social Questions About Behavioral Function (QABF;
competence, or the degree to which a child can Matson, Bamburg, Cherry, & Paclawskyj, 1999;
achieve social goals in ways appropriate for their Matson & Vollmer, 1995). Moreover, some indi-
environments (Wright, 1980), can be measured rect assessments are also used to obtain infor-
using a variety of approaches (Odom et al., 2008), mation about the topography, intensity, and
and particular deficits can be addressed using a frequency of the problem behavior (e.g., the
variety of interventions (Terpstra & Tamura, FAST and the FAI). In general, indirect assess-
2008). This chapter examines assessment and ments are useful because they identify a hypo-
intervention for challenging behavior related to thetical function of the problem behavior and
social skill deficits. may provide more information about the topog-
We will first review strategies for assessing the raphy. This information could be used to modify
function of challenging behavior. This will or select the most appropriate functional analy-
include both indirect and direct forms of assess- sis (e.g., typical functional analysis, precursor
ment. Particular attention will be paid to func- functional analysis, trial-based functional anal-
tional analysis and variations on functional ysis) to be conducted. However, indirect assess-
analysis that may be useful in different situations. ments should only be used as an initial step in
Next, we will review a number of different social the functional assessment process. An appropri-
skills assessments and provide suggestions for ate functional analysis must be conducted before
how to select and use particular assessments. an accurate function of a problem behavior can
Then we will examine interventions, both those be identified.
that target social skill deficits directly and those
that are oriented to the function of challenging
behavior, but also may incorporate social skill Direct Assessment
support or may be used in conjunction with social
skills training. Finally, we will discuss a particular Direct assessments involve the observation of the
instance in which challenging behavior may not individuals, typically in their natural environ-
be directly addressed with social skills training. ment (Bijou, Peterson, & Ault, 1968). Direct
assessments are often conducted in the setting in
which the problem behavior is most likely to
 ssessment of Challenging
A occur. For example, for a child who engages in
Behavior tantrums at school, a direct assessment would be
conducted in his or her classroom during the
Indirect Assessment times in which the problem behavior is more
likely to occur (e.g., mathematics class). In cases
An indirect assessment is often the initial step in in which the problem behavior happens in all set-
identifying the function of a problem behavior. tings (e.g., school, home, grocery store), the
Indirect assessments typically involve a ques- behavior analyst may observe the behavior in two
tionnaire, rating scale, or interview that is com- or more settings.
Challenging Behavior 21

In the simplest version of this type of assess- Functional Analysis


ment, behavior analysts record all the anteced-
ents (stimuli that precede the target problem After conducting either an indirect assessment, a
behavior) and the consequences (stimuli that fol- direct assessment, or both, a functional analysis
low the target problem behavior) that occur dur- (Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, & Richman,
ing the observation. This type of direct assessment 1982/1994) should be completed to identify the
is called A-B-C recording (Cooper, Heron, & actual function(s) of the challenging behavior. A
Heward, 2007). For example, if a child engages functional analysis involves the experimental
in crying in the classroom after the teacher asks manipulation of antecedents and consequences
the child to complete an assignment and teacher during tests and control conditions. The comple-
provides the child with a break, the behavior ana- tion of a functional analysis is considered best
lyst will record that the teacher asked the child to practice in applied behavior analysis (ABA)
complete an assignment (antecedent), the child when individuals engage in challenging behavior
engaged in crying (problem behavior), and the (Beavers, Iwata, & Lerman, 2013) because the
teacher provided the child with a break from results can be used to design function-based
work (consequence). These recorded data could treatments (see below for examples of function-­
be used to identify a hypothetical function of the based treatments). The conditions included in the
problem behavior (e.g., escape from academic functional analysis typically depend on the infor-
tasks) as well as to identify the current strategies mation gathered from the indirect and direct
being used to address challenging behavior. assessments. For example, if the target problem
Another type of direct assessment is the struc- behavior is aggression, defined as biting, hitting,
tured descriptive assessment (Freeman, Anderson, or grabbing another individual, the functional
& Scotti, 2000). Structured descriptive assess- analysis is likely to exclude an alone condition
ments include the arrangement and manipulation (will be described in the following section) as the
of antecedent variables to assess how caregivers behavior requires the presence of another person
typically respond to problem behavior. In this by definition.
assessment, consequences are not manipulated. In general, the typical conditions included in a
Although this type of assessment relies heavily on standard functional analysis as outlined by Iwata
antecedent control, it can be a more efficient et al. (1982/1994) are the following: alone or
approach to more naturalistic observation because ignore, attention, demand (sometimes called
it necessitates the presentation of antecedents that escape), and play. Additional conditions (e.g.,
might otherwise not be observed or that might take tangible) or variations of the abovementioned
a great deal of observation to capture. conditions could be included as well. Sessions
Finally, scatterplots are another type of descrip- typically last 10 min (although variations include
tive assessment (Touchette, MacDonald, & Langer, 5-min sessions, Thomason-Sassi, Iwata, Neidert,
1985). They are commonly used to identify whether & Roscoe, 2011, and 15-min sessions, Wallace &
challenging behavior is more likely during specific Iwata, 1999) and conditions are best conducted in
periods of time. If the challenging behavior is cor- a fixed sequence (Hammond, Iwata, Rooker,
related to a specific period of time, the child’s Fritz, & Bloom, 2013).
schedule can be examined to identify whether cer-
tain activities are associated with those periods of Attention. The attention condition is used to
time and, hence, with problem behavior. This may identify if the challenging behavior is main-
suggest an environmental-behavior relationship, tained by social positive reinforcement in the
but because antecedents and consequences are not form of adult attention. During the attention
manipulated (and specific instances of potential condition, the behavior analyst and the partici-
antecedents and consequences are not recorded), pant are together in a room. The participant has
the scatterplot is not designed to identify function free access to a moderately preferred item, and
of problem behavior. the behavior analyst begins the session by
22 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

letting the participant know that he or she will sented. If the participant engages in the targeted
be busy (e.g., “I will be reading a magazine”). challenging behavior at any time during the ses-
Therefore, if the target problem behavior is sion, the behavior analyst removes all materials
maintained by attention, the establishing opera- (e.g., pen and paper used to write name) and pro-
tion to request for attention will be present. If vides the participant a 30-s break from tasks. If
the participant engages in the target problem the participant does not engage in problem
behavior during the session, the behavior ana- behavior or engages in nontargeted behavior, the
lyst provides appropriate attention to the partici- behavior analyst continues to present demands
pant. The type of attention provided depends on until the session is over.
the attention that is typically provided by the
individuals in the natural environment of the Tangible. The tangible condition is conducted to
participant. For example, if the participant identify if the challenging behavior is maintained
receives attention in the form of verbal repri- by positive reinforcement in the form of access to
mands (e.g., “do not hit me, hitting is not nice,” tangibles (e.g., toys, food). The tangible condi-
“stop biting your arm”), the behavior analyst tion is typically conducted if the indirect or
should provide similar verbal reprimands con- descriptive assessments (or both) have suggested
tingent on the target challenging behavior. If the that problem behavior frequently results in access
form of attention typically provided by the care- to items or food or if the behavior is most likely
givers in the natural environment is different, when the items or food is removed or not pro-
such as hugs or statements of concerns (e.g., vided to the participant. If indirect or descriptive
“please stop, you are hurting yourself”), the assessments do not suggest that problem behavior
behavior analyst would provide the same kind might be maintained by positive reinforcement in
of attention. Furthermore, the behavior analyst the form of tangibles, it might not be appropriate
should ignore all appropriate behavior or non- to conduct a tangible condition as this might
targeted challenging behavior. increase the likelihood of a false-positive func-
tion (Beavers et al., 2013). During a tangible con-
Demand/Escape. The escape, also called demand, dition, the behavior analyst and the participant
condition is conducted to identify if the challeng- are together in a room and the tangible(s) are
ing behavior is maintained by social negative available to the participant. Immediately after the
reinforcement in the form of escape from session begins, the behavior analyst removes the
demands. During the escape condition, the items from the participant and tells the participant
behavior analyst and participant are together in a that the items are no longer available (e.g., “no
room, and the behavior analyst presents demands more toys”). If the participant engages in the tar-
to the participant. The demands presented are get challenging behavior, the items are provided
selected based on previous indirect or direct to the participant for 30 s. The behavior analyst
assessments. The demands should be requests ignores all other behavior including appropriate
that are unlikely to be completed without prob- requests for the items and nontargeted challeng-
lem behavior by the participant. Some examples ing behavior during the session.
of typical demands used in this condition include
academic tasks (e.g., writing letters, adding num- Alone/Ignore. The alone and ignore conditions are
bers), gross motor activities (e.g., clapping two different test conditions that are conducted to
hands), and daily living chores (e.g., folding tow- identify if challenging behavior is maintained by
els). These demands are typically presented using automatic reinforcement. If an alone condition is
a three-step prompting procedure (Horner & conducted, an ignore condition is not necessary,
Keilitz, 1975) consisting of sequential verbal, and the inverse is also true. Whether to use the
model, and physical prompts. Contingent on the alone or ignore condition is based on the setting in
completion of a request, no verbal praise should which the functional analysis is conducted. For
be provided; instead a new request should be pre- example, if the functional analysis is conducted in
Challenging Behavior 23

a controlled environment in which a one-way mir- Brief Functional Analysis. The brief functional
ror might be available (e.g., clinic room), an alone analysis (Northup et al., 1991) was developed to
condition might be feasible. However, if the func- decrease the duration of the functional analysis.
tional analysis is conducted in a more natural Northup et al. (1991) conducted brief functional
setting (e.g., home) in which observing the analyses to assess problem behavior within
participant’s behavior might not be feasible unless restricted time (i.e., 90 min) in an outpatient set-
the behavior analyst is in the room with the par- ting. The brief functional analyses consisted of
ticipant, an ignore condition might be more appro- the limited replication (e.g., single presentation)
priate to conduct. An alone condition involves of each test condition and a brief contingency
having the participant be alone in a room, while reversal. The results from this study suggest that
the behavior analyst observes the participant’s brief functional analyses may reveal maintaining
behavior from a different room. No programed variables of challenging behavior when time is
consequences are provided for any behavior. An restricted (e.g., one 90-min visit). However, due
ignore condition involves having the participant to the limited repetition of the assessment, which
and the behavior analyst in the same room. The often results in one data point of problem behav-
behavior analyst does not provide any attention to ior per condition, additional assessment may be
the participant and does not engage in eye contact necessary to clarify results. Therefore, even
with the participant. The behavior analyst also though it might be beneficial to start with a brief
ignores all participant behavior including the tar- functional analysis if the time for assessment and
get problem behavior. treatment is very limited, a traditional functional
analysis (Iwata et al., 1982/1994) may be required
Play. The play condition is the control condition. to identify the maintaining variables of problem
Responding in all conditions is compared to the behavior in some cases.
play condition. During the play condition, the
participant has free access to preferred items, fre- Precursor Functional Analysis. Conducting
quent attention from the behavior analyst (e.g., functional analysis for individuals who engage in
attention approximately every 30 s), and no pre- very dangerous challenging behavior (e.g., high-­
sentation of demands. During this condition, the intensity head banging) could be problematic.
behavior analyst ignores all targeted or nontar- However, not having a clear function of the chal-
geted challenging behavior. If the participant lenging behavior could result in ineffective inter-
engages in appropriate behavior (e.g., requests vention, which also poses risk of harm. Therefore,
for an item), the behavior analyst provides the Smith and Churchill (2002) developed the pre-
item and interacts with the participant as much or cursor functional analysis, a variation of the
as little as the participant requests. functional analysis that can be used in these
cases. In a precursor functional analysis, contin-
gencies are placed on the precursors or responses
Variation of Functional Analysis that happen prior to the target behavior. For
example, if the target challenging behavior is
Functional analyses have been evaluated and rep- head banging, a precursor might be screaming
licated in hundreds of published studies. They and programed consequences would be provided
have been conducted in a variety of settings, on on the precursor so that the more dangerous
many different topographies of behavior, and response does not happen. By identifying the
with various populations. However, there are cer- function of a precursor to the target challenging
tain conditions in which conducting a traditional behavior, the risks associated with the more dan-
functional analysis could be difficult or, in some gerous challenging behavior are decreased.
cases, not feasible. Therefore, researchers have However, identifying precursor behaviors that
developed several variations of functional analy- are part of the same response class as the target
sis to target these conditions. behavior might not be simple for all individuals.
24 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

Furthermore, precursor functional analyses are Latency Functional Analysis. Traditional func-
not useful when the challenging behavior is tional analyses typically use repetition of a
maintained by automatic reinforcement because response as a measure of behavior (Thomason-
(a) precursors to automatically maintained prob- Sassi et al., 2011). When behavior happens in
lem behavior may not exist, and (b) if they do one test condition (e.g., attention) at higher rates
exist, the behavior analyst is faced with logistical than in the control condition (e.g., play), it sug-
challenges reinforcing the precursor if it is auto- gests that the target behavior is maintained by
matically maintained. Therefore, the establishing that contingency (e.g., social positive). However,
operation elimination that occurs in a precursor when the target behavior is very severe and it is
functional analysis that is responsible for the desirable to limit its occurrence, it might be
elimination of the targeted challenging behavior practical to use a latency functional analysis
is unlikely to occur. (Thomason-Sassi et al., 2011). Also, when the
behavior may not be repeatable in a session
Trial-Based Functional Analysis. Another varia- because of its topography (e.g., eloping or enure-
tion of the traditional functional analysis is the sis), it might be beneficial to use latency func-
trial-based functional analysis (Bloom, Iwata, tional analysis. Latency functional analyses
Fritz, Roscoe, & Carreau, 2011). This variation consist of the same test and control conditions
was developed to assess challenging behavior in used in the traditional functional analysis.
naturalistic environments (e.g., schools) when Sessions last up to 5 min and terminate when the
controlled settings (e.g., clinic) are not available. first target response takes place. The time from
Furthermore, trial-based functional analyses have the beginning of the session to the first instance
also been conducted by teachers (Bloom, of problem behavior is graphed and analyzed.
Lambert, Dayton, & Samaha, 2013) and group Short latencies in a condition, meaning,
home staff (Lambert, Bloom, Kunnavatana, responses that occur quickly after the sessions
Collins, & Clay, 2013), which reduces the begin, suggest that behavior is maintained by the
resources required to conduct a functional analy- contingency tested in that condition. For exam-
sis. Trial-based functional analyses consist of ple, if the target challenging behavior for a child
control and test segments used to identify the is elopement and a latency functional analysis is
variables maintaining the challenging behavior conducted, the faster in the session the child
(e.g., attention, tangibles, escape from demands). elopes (e.g., 4 s in attention), compared to the
Trials are embedded into ongoing activities, and control condition, the stronger the contingency
trial segments last 2 min or until challenging we can demonstrate. In this case, 4 s is a short
behavior takes place in each segment, which ter- latency, especially compared to a long latency
minates the segment. Even though the length of (e.g., 240 s in control) or no occurrence in the
the trials is shorter than traditional functional control condition, and if similar results continue
analysis sessions (i.e., 2–4 min vs. 10 min), the for several sessions, we can say that challenging
duration of the trial-based functional analysis is behavior is maintained by attention.
not necessarily shorter compared to the tradi-
tional functional analysis. However, research Extended Alone Condition Sessions or
suggests (Lambert, Bloom, & Irvin, 2012; LaRue Functional Analysis Screening. When
et al., 2010) that this variation can successfully ­challenging behavior is likely to happen in the
identify the maintaining consequences of prob- absence of social contingencies or when other
lem behavior, even though it may not be suitable individuals do not have to be present for the
when longer exposure to contingencies is needed. behavior to occur (e.g., rumination), it is some-
In those cases, a traditional functional analysis times possible and efficient to run extended
should be conducted. alone conditions or a functional analysis screen-
Challenging Behavior 25

ing (Querim et al., 2013) to confirm that the Hazel (1984) described four general approach for
problem behavior is maintained by automatic assessments: behavioral observation codes,
reinforcement prior to commencing a functional behavioral checklists, sociometric assessment,
analysis consisting of multiple test conditions and behavioral rating scales.
and a control condition. Extended alones consist
of several alone conditions ran consecutively Behavior Observation Codes. This approach
(lasting 5 or 10 min each). If challenging behav- requires that the target behaviors be categorized
ior consistently occurs during the alone condi- so that an observation code can be used to track
tion, the data suggest that challenging behavior the occurrence of the target responses. Once the
is automatically maintained, and the process is code is developed, clinicians then observe the
considered sufficient as a screening procedure individual and collect data on the occurrence of
for automatically maintained behavior. each behavior. An example of this method is the
Peer Social Behavior Code (Walker & Severson,
1992). This behavioral code includes five catego-
Social Skill Assessment ries: social engagement, participation, parallel
play, alone, and no codable response. In addition,
As noted earlier many individuals with disabili- the form is broken into 10-s intervals so that data
ties, including ASD, have deficits in social skills. may be collected on the occurrence of each of
Therefore, it is important to ensure that program- these responses per interval. Although this
ming addresses these deficits. However, in order method can yield some helpful information such
to select specific objectives for the child, one as the likelihood of the behavior occurring in the
must first evaluate the child’s repertoire. An natural environment, one issue is that the target
assessment should allow determination of the response may not actually occur during the
child’s current repertoire and perhaps help iden- scheduled observation. Therefore, this type of
tify variables that may impede learning such as assessment can be time consuming as multiple
lack of motivation and prompt dependency. It is observations may be necessary to capture a repre-
also necessary to determine whether lack of per- sentative sample of the individual’s repertoire. In
formance is due to a skill deficit or a performance addition, behavioral observational codes usually
deficit. In the case of a skill deficit, the child does are used to collect data only on the occurrence of
not emit the response because the child has never the target responses without considering the qual-
learned the response. Meaning, it is not in the ity of the response.
child’s repertoire. In the case of a performance
deficit, the child can emit the response but does Observational Checklists. This approach involves
not do it consistently likely because of a lack in developing a list of all behavior of interest. Once
motivation. Meaning, the skill is in the child’s the checklist is developed, the individual is
repertoire but the child does not perform the skill. observed during naturally occurring or contrived
Different interventions will be necessary depend- situations and record data on the individual’s per-
ing on whether the skills are not performed due to formance, occurrence, and/or quality, of the tar-
a skill or performance deficit. get responses. This method of evaluation is fairly
simple and can be used across observations and
potentially across individuals. However, as was
Types of Assessments the case with behavioral observation codes, target
behaviors may not occur during naturalistic
A variety of assessment tools are currently avail- observations. In addition, if a behavior occurs
able to assess social skills and related skills such that it is not included in the checklist, the observ-
as communication. These may include inter- ers will likely not record that behavior unless
views, observations, direct testing, or a combina- specific instructions are given to note responses
tion of these methods. In fact, Schumaker and that are not included in the checklist.
26 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

Sociometric Assessment. This method usually educational setting, so it is only appropriate for
consists of using a scale (Likert-type) to evaluate individuals attending school. Approximately
whether an individual is liked and/or socially 20–95 min is necessary to complete the assess-
accepted. The scale is usually completed by indi- ment, depending on which form is selected. This
viduals who know the person such as classmates assessment evaluates adaptive behavior in the
and colleagues. In some cases, a group of indi- domains of communication, daily living, and
viduals may complete the scale for all members socialization. A scoring manual is used to sum-
of the group, whereas in others the scale is marize the results although a scoring software,
completed by all members of the group but only ASSIST™, is also available. The final report
for a subset of individuals. This technique may be includes a lot of information such as domains
most helpful in identifying individuals who may and adaptive behavior composite standard
benefit from social skills training although a fur- scores, adaptive levels, and age equivalence.
ther assessment would be required to identify Finally, the results of the assessment can be used
skill deficits. This approach is quick and easy; to determine eligibility for services, guide inter-
however, reactivity may be an issue if the rating vention, and track progress.
scale is completed frequently (Gresham, 1981).
The Assessment of Basic Language and Learning
Behavioral Rating Scales. This method involves Skills – Revised (ABLLS-R; Partington, 2006).
the use of a rating scale (usually Likert-­type) that This assessment consists of direct observation of
is completed by persons who are familiar with the the child although caregiver interview may be
individual. The scale usually includes a list of tar- necessary to evaluate skills that were not seen
get responses or descriptive items, and informa- during the observation. This is a criterion-refer-
tion is collected on whether the individual emits enced assessment. It assesses a variety of skills
certain behaviors and, if so, how well and/or how such as language, academic, self-help, and motor
often. This approach can also be quick and simple skills. In addition, the language assessment is
but rating scales may be unreliable. based on Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior
(1957). This assessment is usually implemented
with children with disabilities. A curriculum
 pecific Assessment Tools and How
S guide and skills tracking system with visual dis-
to Select Among Them play accompany the assessment, and these can be
used to facilitate decision making about program-
In this section, we provide a brief description of ming and to evaluate progress. The amount of
assessments tools commonly employed in clini- time needed to complete the assessment varies
cal settings, research, or both. We will also pro- and is related to the repertoire of the individual
vide a brief overview of variables to be considered being evaluated, but it typically requires a few
when selecting an assessment tool. hours. ABBLS-R has been translated to Spanish,
French, and Norwegian and an electronic version
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second (WebABLLS) is also available.
Edition (Vineland ™-II; Sparrow, Cicchetti, &
Balla, 2005). The Vineland™-II includes a care- The Verbal Behavior Milestone Assessment and
giver interview form, a caregiver rating form, an Placement Program, Second Edition (VB-Mapp;
expanded interview form, and a teacher rating Sundberg, 2014). This assessment consists of
form. These forms evaluate the same skills. direct testing and/or observation (some of which
Thus, selection is usually based on the amount are timed) of the child. It is developmentally
of time available to complete the assessment and sequenced and a ­criterion-­referenced assessment
the primary context of interest (i.e., school vs. that compares the child to typically developing
home). For instance, the teacher rating form peers. The assessment evaluates learning, lan-
evaluates the individual’s performance in an guage, and social skills, and it includes transition
Challenging Behavior 27

as well as barriers assessments. The results of the another for negative skills, and these are further
transition assessment are helpful in guiding divided into six dimensions: positive verbal, pos-
placement of the child, whereas the barriers itive nonverbal, general positive, negative verbal,
assessment identifies factors that may be imped- negative nonverbal, and general negative. This
ing learning (e.g., prompt dependency). The rating scale was developed to assess social skills
Early Echoic Skills Assessment (EESA) devel- of adults with disabilities. Finally, the assessment
oped by Esch is also included. In addition, the is quick and it is usually completed in a semi-­
guide includes placement and program objec- structured format.
tives. The assessment is appropriate for children
of any age, diagnosis, and language ability and it FISH: Functional Independence Skills Handbook
has three developmental levels (0–18, 18–30, (Killian, 2008; http://www.proedinc.com/cus-
30–48 months). The amount of time required to tomer/productView.aspx?ID=1392). This hand-
complete the assessment varies based on the book includes both assessment booklets and
child, but it typically takes several hours. The curriculum for seven domains, adaptive behav-
VB-MAPP App is also available which offers ior, affective, cognitive, sensorimotor, social,
among other feature electronic scoring and auto- speech and language, and vocational skills. The
matic charts. handbook was developed to be used with indi-
viduals with developmental disabilities of vari-
MESSY: Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with ous ages although it may also be appropriate for
Youngsters (Matson, 1988; Matson et al., 2010; individuals with cognitive deficits and typically
Matson, Rotatori, & Helsel, 1983). This assess- developing children. The assessment is criterion-
ment includes self-report (62 items) and teacher referenced. The assessment is completed by
report (64 items) scales. The scales are usually interviewing a caregiver although direct interac-
completed in an interview format, with the indi- tion with the individual, either before or after the
vidual or caregiver, and items are answered using assessment is recommended to verify some of
a Likert-type of scale ranging from 1 (“not at the information obtained from the caregiver. It is
all”) to 5 (“very much). This assessment was unclear how much time is required to complete
originally developed for children ages 4–18 years the assessment although it will likely vary based
although the newer version, MESSY-II, is on the individual’s repertoire. Once the initial
intended for use with individuals between the assessment is completed, that information can be
ages of 2 and16 years. Administration of the used to select appropriate lessons for that indi-
scale is fairly simple and quick. The MESSY has vidual. The assessment can be repeated to evalu-
been used with a variety of populations such as ate progress.
typically developing children and individuals
with ASD, and it also has been adapted for use Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler
with visually impaired and deaf individuals (see Development – Third Edition (Bayley-III; Bayley,
Matson & Wilkins, 2007). It has also been trans- 2006a, 2006b; http://images.pearsonclinical.
lated to several languages including Spanish, com/images/PDF/Bayley-III_Webinar.pdf;
Japanese, Slovakian, etc. http://www.pearsonclinical.com/childhood/
products/100000123/bayley-scales-of-infant-
MESSIER: The Matson Evaluation of Social and-toddler-development-third-edition-bayley-
Skills for Individuals with Severe Retardation iii.html#tab-details). This assessment consists of
(Matson, 1995; Matson, LeBlanc, & Weinheimer, play interaction with the child and caregiver
1999). This assessment consists of an 85-item questionnaires that are used to evaluate social-
rating scale completed by caregivers. The items emotional and adaptive behavior. It is a norm-
are scored using a 3-point scale to indicate the referenced assessment comparing the child to
frequency at which each item occurs. The scale typically developing children of the same age,
includes two subscales, one for positive and and it assesses five developmental domains, cog-
28 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

nition, language, motor, social-emotional, and Do-Watch-Listen-Say Assessment and


adaptive behavior. The adaptive behavior scale, Curriculum (Quill, 2000; http://products.
ABAS-II, was developed by Patti L. Harrison and brookespublishing.com/DO-WATCH-LISTEN-
Thomas Oakland. It is intended to evaluate devel- SAY-P18.aspx). This is an assessment and inter-
opment delays and facilitate intervention, and it vention guide that combines a developmental and
is appropriate for children between ages 1 and behavioral approach. The assessment, which
42 months. It can be used to identify develop- includes the Social Skills Checklist, consists of
mental delays, and it also guides intervention and questionnaires and checklists and it evaluates
can be used to track progress. Administration core, social, and communication skills. It com-
time varies depending on the age of the child, but bines developmental and behavioral approach to
it usually requires 30–90 min. assessment. It is intended for children with an
ASD who communicate vocally or with an aug-
Social Skills Improvement System (SSIS; Crosby, mentative alternative communication (AAC). It
2011; Gresham & Elliott, 2008; http://www.pear- includes, among other things, datasheets, guide
sonclinical.com/education/products/100000322/ for designing intervention, as well as curriculum
social-skills-improvement-system-ssis-rating- for social skills, core skills, and communication
scales.html). This assessment includes caregiver skills. The time required to administer the assess-
questionnaires (parent and teacher) as well as a ment is not provided, but given that it consists of
questionnaire for the child. It can be used to eval- questionnaires and checklists, it is likely no more
uate the child’s skills across home and educa- than 2 h.
tional settings. It assesses social skills, academic
competence, and problem behavior and it was Profile of Social Difficulty (POSD; Coucouvanis,
developed for individuals between the ages of 3 2005). This assessment consists a questionnaire
and 18 years. The test includes subcales such as scored using a Likert scale ranging from “very
bullying and autism spectrum. The assessment is difficult” to “very easy.” Anyone familiar with
quick, requiring about 10–25 min per form, and it the individual, parent, teacher, sibling, or the
is linked to interventions tools. The parent and individual himself (self-report version) can com-
student questionnaires are available in Spanish, plete the questionnaire. It evaluates a variety of
and software to facilitate scoring and reporting is skills and these are categorized into subscales:
available from the publisher. fundamental skills, social initiation skills, social
response skills, and getting along with others. It
The Social Responsiveness Scale II (SRS; is quick (15–20 min) and it is intended for chil-
Constantino & Gruber, 2012; Bölte, Poustka, & dren ages 6–11 years.
Constantino, 2008) http://www.wpspublish.com/
store/p/2994/social-responsiveness-scale-sec- The Autism Social Skills Profile (ASSP; Bellini,
ond-edition-srs-2). This assessment consists of a 2006). It consists of a rating scale and items are
65-item questionnaire that is completed by a par- separated into three scales, social reciprocity, social
ent or teacher. It evaluates the child’s social participation/avoidance, and detrimental social
impairments and it can be used as a screening behaviors. Results of the subscales can be summa-
tool for autism or to aid in an autism diagnosis. rized as a total score of the individual’s overall
Items are scored using a 3-point Likert scale cor- social functioning. This assessment is appropriate
responding to severity level, and the items are for individuals between 6 and 17 years old and it
summarized as a total score with higher scores only requires about 15–20 min to complete.
indicating more deficits in social skills. Although
the SRS was developed for individuals ages The Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC;
4–18 years, different forms are available for spe- Bishop, 1998; 2006 http://www.pearsonclinical.
cific ages (e.g., adult, school aged, preschool). In com/language/products/100000193/childrens-
addition, the Social Responsiveness Scale II was communication-checklist2-us-edition-ccc-2.
published in 2012. html#tab-details). It consists of a rating scale that
Challenging Behavior 29

can be completed by a teacher or caregiver. It the other hand, would suggest that the inter-
assesses language impairments in the areas of vention is not appropriate and thus needs to
pragmatics, syntax, morphology, semantics, and be modified. In addition, some of the tools
speech. The second edition of the checklist was described below include program recom-
published in 2006. It is intended for children mendations thus allowing clinicians to make
between 4 and 16 years old who communicate in data-based decisions about programming for
full sentences and primarily in English. It is quick their clients.
and can be completed in 5–10 min. (c) Experience conducting assessments. In gen-
eral, assessment tools include instructions
Triad Social Skills Assessment, Second Edition for the implementation and scoring of the
(TSSA©; Stone et al., 2010). This assessment assessment. In addition, trainings may be
consists of teacher and parent reports as well as available online or in workshops. Before pur-
observation and direct interaction with the child. chasing an assessment tool, it is important to
Additional information is obtained through consider whether individuals are qualified to
activity-­based evaluation and a questionnaire that implement and interpret the assessment and
are completed with the child. This is a criterion-­ whether additional training is necessary.
based assessment of a child’s ability to take anoth- Indirect assessments such as interviews and
er’s perspective, initiate and maintain interactions, rating scales are likely easier to complete
respond to others, and understand emotions, thus may be an alternative until training is
respectively. The test was developed for children received on the implementation of the other
ages 6–12 years, and it requires that the child can assessments methods.
read at or above a first-grade level. The test is (d) Available sources of information. It is also
fairly easy to administer and the manual includes important to consider which sources of infor-
a list of potential objectives for each area. mation are available. In most cases the person
whose repertoire is being evaluated will be the
primary source of information. Information
Guide for Selecting an Assessment may also be obtained from caregivers, includ-
Tool ing siblings, and teachers. At times the type of
assessment selected will be related to accessi-
When selecting the most appropriate assessment bility. For instance, if information is needed
tool, here are some variables to be considered: on the child’s performance at school yet direct
evaluation of the child in the school setting is
(a) Time available to conduct the assessment. not possible, then an interview of the teacher
Some assessments can be completed very may be a good alternative.
quickly whereas others require multiple (e) Ease of implementation. As noted above,
hours. The brief assessments may provide some assessments may be easier than others.
enough information to identify general defi- Therefore, if differing assessment tools yield
cit areas, but the more lengthy assessments similar information, one might select the one
will allow clinicians to gain information that is easier to complete. In addition to
about very specific skills that the child can or implementation, it is also important to con-
cannot perform. sider the steps involved summarizing and
(b) Purpose of the assessment. Some assessment interpreting the results. Some assessment
tools have multiple purposes and can be used tools have scoring software or applications
to evaluate the child’s current repertoire as available that can decrease the time required
well as assess progress. If the child is making to summarize the results.
appropriate progress, it is presumed that the (f) Cost. Another variable to consider is the cost
intervention is effective; limited progress, on of the assessment tool. Although this should
30 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

not be the main variable affecting all deci- social skills coaching dates back to 1977 when
sions, choosing a cheaper (or free) option Oden and Asher (1977) completed a study com-
that provides similar information on the paring the effects of coaching, peer pairing, and a
child’s repertoire is likely a good option. control condition on the children’s sociometric
ratings. Children were instructed to complete
three questionnaires in regard to other children in
Social Skills Interventions the classroom, and these questionnaires yielded
ratings for each of the children. Following inter-
Deficits in social and/or communication skills are vention, coaching was associated with higher rat-
defining characteristics of autism spectrum disor- ing following the intervention. Additional studies
ders (ASD; DSM 5). Elliot and Gresham (1993) have also evaluated coaching, alone (e.g.,
defined social skills as “socially acceptable Gresham & Nagle, 1980) or in combination with
learned behaviors that enable a person to interact other interventions (e.g., Cooke & Appoloni,
with others in ways that elicit positive responses 1976), and these studies have been successful at
and assist in avoiding negative responses” improving social skills.
(p. 287). Thus, social skill refers to a class of
responses that when emitted in a specific context
is likely to result in reinforcers. As noted by Modeling
Bellini and Peters (2008), programs designed to
teach social skills usually attempt to establish Modeling of a response may be considered a type
new skills or improve skills already in the child’s of instruction or perhaps a type of prompt, and
repertoire. Social skills training (SST) programs this type of instruction seems to have its origins
may therefore focus on increasing the frequency back to Bandura (1977) who demonstrated that
of certain responses, ensure that responses occur children might learn new skills simply by observ-
in the appropriate context, or that responses are ing other individuals engaging in them. This is
performed with fluency. Several reviews of the referred to as observational learning. Bandura
literature on social skills interventions are cur- also showed that individuals engage in the same
rently available (e.g., Bellini, 2008; Matson, response as demonstrated by the model even in
Matson, & Rivet, 2007; McConnell, 2002; Rao, cases when reinforcers are not delivered for imi-
Beidel, & Murray, 2008; Rogers, 2000; Weiss & tation. Thus, one potential procedure for teaching
Harris, 2001; White, Keonig, & Scahill, 2007). novel skills to individuals with disabilities is
Therefore, we will focus here on a general modeling. Research on the use of modeling has
description of some of these interventions and included in vivo modeling, when another person
related research. is emitting the responses in the presence of the
target individual and video modeling. In the case
of video modeling, the model (individual, peer,
Coaching or adult) is recorded emitting the target responses.
In addition, one adaptation of modeling includes
Coaching, according to Elliott and Gresham demonstration of the skills from a first person
(1993), involves a review of the rules or expecta- perspective (e.g., Nishizawa, Kimura, & Goh,
tion, practice of the skills, and feedback that it 2015). Previous research has found that modeling
delivered during rehearsal. Thus, coaching may be effective in teaching a variety of skills to
includes verbal instruction provided by a “coach.” individuals with disabilities such as social initia-
Although the term “coach” seems to imply some- tions (e.g., Nikopoulos & Keenan, 2004), com-
one with expertise in the topic, the coach can be a munication skills (e.g., Charlop-Christy, Le, &
parent, teacher, or even a peer. Freeman, 2000), play skills (e.g., Dupere,
In addition, coaching may include modeling; MacDonald, & Ahearn, 2013), and daily living
it is not a necessary component. Research on skills (e.g., Shipley-Benamou, Lutzker, &
Challenging Behavior 31

Taubman, 2002), and it may result in a decrease Technology-Based Instruction


in problem behavior (e.g., Buggey, 2005).
Research has highlighted some advantages of As technology has progressed, a number of
video modeling (VM) including faster acquisi- technology-­ based instructions have emerged
tion than in vivo modeling (e.g., Charlop-Christy including video modeling (see above section),
et al., 2000), consistent modeling of the target virtual reality (VR), instructional software, and
skills across instructional sessions, and being less robotics. Virtual reality is an interactive space
resource intensive because it does not require that allows the user to learn about and practice a
continuous presence of a model. Video modeling variety of skills (see review by Muscott &
programs currently available include Model Me Gifford, 1994). It includes 3D learning environ-
Kids and Watch Me Learn. ments (e.g., Kandalaft, Didehbani, Krawczyk,
In a research review of the literature, Bellini Allen, & Chapman, 2013) and immersive virtual
and Akullian (2007) identified 23 articles investi- environment (e.g., Lorenzo, Pomares, & Lledo,
gating the effects of either VM or video self-­ 2013), and it can be set up for individual use or
modeling (VSM) with individuals with an ASD collaborative learning. VR systems require both
ages 3–21 years. The authors used percentage of hardware (computer) and software to create the
nonoverlapping data points (PND) scores to eval- simulated environment. Usually, the user is
uate the effectiveness of each intervention and exposed to the virtual world via interfaces that
utilized the criteria described by Mastropieri and may include head-mounted sticks, joysticks,
Scruggs (2001) to categorize interventions. By headphones, and even gesture-recognizing
aggregating the PND scores across studies on gloves. VR systems can simulate a variety of
VM and VSM, moderate effects were identified environments such as classrooms, grocery stores,
for intervention and maintenance of VM and etc. According to Parsons and Mitchell (2002),
VSM as well as generalization of VM. These virtual reality should allow for repetition, allow
data led the authors to conclude that VM and for fading, and promote generalization. Muscott
VSM meet the criteria for evidence-based prac- and Gifford note that potential advantages of
tice. Additional things to be considered include VRS include the fact that the virtual world can be
the fact that individuals may acquire skills faster easily adapted to meet the needs of the learner
with VM and in vivo modeling (e.g., Charlop-­ (easier or more challenging levels), and it can
Christy et al., 2000) and that VM may be easier also be set up so that the learner has to display
and more cost-effective because the materials can specific responses. Software for virtual reality
be used for multiple sessions as a second person, may also be developed in a manner that it pres-
the model, is not necessary other than when ents a range of potential responses to the learner
developing the videos. and allows the learner to experience a “natural”
It is also important to consider prerequisite consequence by being exposed to differing con-
skills. Bandura (1977) suggested that one poten- texts based on their answers. Finally, virtual real-
tial prerequisite for learning through observation ity has been found to be effective in increasing a
is attending to a model. In a study completed by variety of skills such as conversations skills and
MacDonald, Dickson, Martineau, and Ahearn theory of mind (e.g., Kandalaft et al., 2013).
(2015), several potential prerequisites for learn- Instructional software may also allow for indi-
ing through VM were evaluated, and the results vidual or collaborative learning, and a number of
of this study suggested that attending to model software, targeting different skills, are currently
was not sufficient. Acquisition was correlated available. For instance, Hopkins et al. (2011)
with the participants’ performance in delayed addressed joint attention and emotional and facial
imitation of actions with objects and in delayed recognition; Bernard-Opitz, Sriram, and
match to sample tasks. Nakhoda-Sapuan (2001) addressed social prob-
32 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

lem solving. Finally, robotics has been found to Prompting Procedures


be effective in increasing joint attention (e.g.,
Robins, Dickerson, Stribling, & Dautenhahn, Prompts are added stimuli that assist the child
2004) and body awareness (Costa, Lehmann, engage in a target response. These are typically
Dautenhahn, Robins, & Soares, 2015). used when a new skill is introduced to allow the
child to practice the skills but then should be
faded so that the child eventually performs the
Pivotal Response Treatment skills without assistance. Several types of
prompts and prompt fading procedures are cur-
Pivotal response treatment (PRT) (also described as rently available such as physical, verbal, model,
pivotal response training) was described in detail by gestural, textual prompts (e.g., scripts) and tactile
Koegel, Koegel, Harrower, and Carter in 1999. The prompts. The type of prompt needed will depend
emphasis of this manualized program is to teach on the skill being targeted as well as the child. In
children “pivotal” responses, meaning responses regard to prompt fading procedures, these may
that, once acquired, will impact a variety of other include progressive increase of the delay of a
responses. Thus, this intervention model may be prompt delivery or fading the prompt itself, such
more cost and time efficient. Pivotal responses iden- as in most-to-least prompt. Prompts may be faded
tified include responding to multiple cues, motiva- within or across sessions. Here are some exam-
tion, self-management, and child initiations. In the ples of prompts:
pivotal response model, teaching occurs in the natu-
ral environment and usually includes the use of Script. In this case usually a written script of the
stimuli that are present in the child’s environment target skill is provided and then systematically
such as toys. During intervention, maintenance tri- faded. For instance, Ganz and Flores (2008) used
als are interspersed with acquisition trials, natural scripts in the context of group play sessions.
reinforcers are delivered contingently on the Participants included typically developing chil-
response and for attempts, the child is given several dren and children with ASD. The script presented
opportunities to choose reinforcers, and the child is to the typically developing children provided
taught to respond to multiple cues (e.g., conditional instructions for interacting with the children with
discrimination). Another important aspect of the ASD. The scripts presented to the children with
pivotal response model is that intervention occurs in ASD consisted of phrases the child could state
the most inclusive setting and with a lot of support during an interaction with the other children.
and intervention is delivered by multiple agents This study found that the intervention resulted in
(family members, school personnel, consultants). an increase in verbal comments that included
Therefore, the intervention should facilitate gener- scripted and unscripted statements. Similarly,
alization. Various studies have found that pivotal Krantz and McClannahan (1998) used scripts to
response training is an effective procedure for teach children with autism to gain an adult’s
increasing social and communicative responses attention, and performance remained high even
(e.g., Ingersoll & Schreibman, 2006; Whalen & after the scripts were completely faded.
Schreibman, 2003). For instance, Whalen and
Schreibman (2003) evaluated the effects of PRT Tactile Prompts. Tactile prompts usually consist
combined with component of discrete trial teaching of vibrating pagers that are programed to deliver
(DTT) on the acquisition of joint attention responses a prompt at specified intervals. This type of
by young children with and without ASD. In this prompt may be less intrusive than other prompts
study, all participants acquired responses to joint because it can be hidden under the clothing of
attention initiated by the therapist (e.g., following a the participant (e.g., in a pocket). A few studies
gaze or point cue), and most of the participants also have evaluated the effects of tactile prompts.
learned to initiate a joint attention interaction (e.g., For instance, Shabani et al. (2002) used a vibrat-
shifting a gaze from a toy to the experimenter, ing pager to prompt social initiations with three
pointing to an item). children with ASD. The prompt was effective in
Challenging Behavior 33

increasing target response, but they were unsuc- Milford, & Lang, 2013) or even be made into a
cessful in fading the prompt with two of the storybook with audible dialogue (e.g., Murdock,
children. Taylor and Levin (1998) compared Ganz, & Crittendon, 2013). In addition, social
verbal prompt, delivered by an adult, and tactile stories are easy to construct and require few
prompt, the gentle reminder, to assess their resources to implement (e.g., Kokina & Kern, 2010);
effects on initiation toward an adult. They found therefore, they may be welcomed by caregivers
that the tactile prompt was more effective than and other service providers.
the verbal prompt. In a recent review of social stories as an inter-
vention for social skills in children with ASD,
Echoic Prompts. Echoic prompts consist of a Sani Bozkurt and Vuran (2014) reviewed 32
vocal model of the target response. It is therefore research articles. Their review included a descrip-
appropriate when teaching vocal responses to tive analysis of all of the articles identified as
individuals that have a vocal verbal repertoire. well as a meta-analysis of 22 of these articles
Taylor and Hoch (2008) use echoic prompts as (those that include single-subject design and fig-
well as gesture and physical prompts to teach ures showing the baseline and intervention data).
children to respond and initiate bids for joint In the meta-analysis, the authors calculated per-
attention. In a more recent study, Vedora, centage of nonoverlapping data (PND), and they
Meunier, and Mackay (2009) compared textual used the recommendations made by Mastropieri
and echoic prompts on the acquisition of intra- and Scruggs (2001) to analyze the data: if scores
verbal behavior and found that textual prompts above 90, very effective; 70–90, effective; 50–70,
were more effective. questionable; and below 50, ineffective.
Participants in these studies ranged in age from 0
to 15 years, and intervention was implemented in
Social Stories a variety of environments including school set-
tings, health center, institutions, and home. In
Social stories consist of a brief story describing addition, the intervention was implemented by a
social skills that should be displayed given a spe- variety of people including researchers, teachers,
cific social context (e.g., social cues; Gray, 2002). parents, and paraprofessionals. In the majority of
According to Gray, social stories may include the studies (56.25%), social stories were used to
descriptive, perspective, affirmative, directive, teach the participants to initiate communication
control, and cooperative sentences, and for every and social interaction skills, but in general the
directive sentence, 2–5 descriptive, perspective, rationale for selection social stories was vague.
and affirmative sentences should be included For the studies that included information, 68.75%
(2002). Although social stories are usually writ- consisted of the fact that either the participant
ten (e.g., Delona & Snell, 2006), they may and/or the teacher noted that the current reper-
include or be paired with pictures that represent toire of the individual was not appropriate. Thus,
different items (e.g., Crozier & Tincani, 2007) or social stories were selected due to the need to
even self-pictures of the individuals engaging in establish specific responses in the participant’s
the target responses (e.g., Ozdemir, 2008), or repertoire and not necessary due to the type of
accompanied by comprehension questions as intervention. In regard to the PND analysis, when
well as other visual cues such as drawings and implemented alone, social stories were effective
diagrams (e.g., Klett & Turan, 2012). Previous in 13.63% of the studies but were never very
research has also added a modeling component effective. If looking at social stories alone or
(e.g., Chan & O’Reilly, 2008), and social stories combined with another intervention, these were
may be presented using computers or other elec- effective in 31.81% of the studies. The authors
tronics such as Ipads (e.g., Vandermeer, Beamish, concluded that social stories are a promising
34 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

intervention but not yet evidence-based practice. modeling was implemented to teach children
Thus, in considering the use of social stories, play and social skills, and children received edi-
based on this review, it seems that in clinical ble reinforcers during video modeling.
application social stories should be combined Everything else was the same across both
with other interventions. groups. Groups met for 5 weeks. Prosocial
behaviors increased in both groups; however, the
direct teaching group had a greater increase in
Social Skills Groups social skills. Owens, Granader, Humphrey, and
Baron-Cohen evaluated two types of social skills
These usually consist of a group of children (with groups. LEGO™ therapy (LeGoff, 2004) is a
and/or without disabilities) that meet for multiple social skill intervention in which children work
weeks at least once a week. The session itself together to construct a LEGO™ structure.
usually includes a structured lesson on a specific Usually the group consists of three children and
topic/skill, demonstration of the skill (e.g., each person is assigned role, the engineer, the
in vivo or video modeling), practice opportuni- supplies, and the builder. Thus, the children
ties, and feedback. In fact, in a review completed must work together to accomplish the activity.
by Reichow, Steiner, and Volkmar (2013), social The Social Use of Language Program (SULP;
skills groups lasted between 5 and 20 weeks and Rinaldi, 2004) is a structured curriculum for
sessions were 60–90 min in length. Potential ben- teaching social and communication skills. It
efits of social skills groups include the fact you includes social stories, adult modeling, child
can deliver the intervention to multiple children practice, and group games. Results of the study
at once thus decreasing the cost of the interven- showed the social interaction scores of the chil-
tion, opportunities for incidental teaching may dren in the LEGO™ therapy group improved
arise, and it should facilitate generalization since more than that of the children in the SULP
instruction includes multiple individuals. A few group, but problem behavior decreased in both
authors have reviewed the literature on social groups in comparison to the control group.
skills groups. Reichow and colleagues reviewed
five randomized controlled trials and concluded
that social skills groups improved social compe- Priming Procedures
tence and friendship quality; however, additional
research on the efficacy of social skills groups is In general terms, in a priming procedure (Zanolli
warranted. White et al. (2007) and Reichow and & Daggett, 1998), a stimulus (e.g., a model of
Volkmar (2010), in reviews of interventions for the target response) is provided and removed
establishing social skills, concluded that the out- prior to evaluating the individual’s performance.
come of the available research supports the It is presumed that the delivery of this prompt
implementation of social skills groups, but given will affect the individual’s performance when,
that usually social skills groups are combined at another time, it is assessed. Thus, priming is
with other interventions, additional research on different than prompt procedures which usually
the use of social skills groups alone is needed. delivers a prompt whenever the target behavior
Reichow and Volkmar (2010) identified two should be occurring. The effects of priming on
studies that evaluated social skills groups alone social initiations were evaluated by Zanolli and
(Kroeger, Schultz, & Newsome, 2007; Owens, Daggett (1998). In this study, during the prim-
Granader, Humphrey, & Baron-Cohen, 2008). ing session, echoic prompts were provided to
Kroeger, Schultz, and Newsom compared two occasion the target responses and correct
social skills groups; one included direct teaching responding resulted in preferred activities.
and the other consisted of unstructured play Then, during subsequent activity sessions, the
activities. In the direct teaching group, video participants had access to activities, but no
Challenging Behavior 35

prompts were delivered, and initiations resulted through interaction or modeling, teaching a peer
in highly preferred items which were delivered to initiate interactions with the learner to promote
either continuous or a VI schedule. The results learning by encouraging the learner to engage in
of this study showed that priming led to an social interaction, and teaching a peer to prompt
increase in social initiations and that responding specific responses and provide consequences. In
was higher when preferred items were accord- a recent study evaluating peer-mediated proce-
ing to a denser schedule of reinforcement. dures, Schmidt and Stichter (2012) evaluated the
effects of a peer-mediated proximity and peer-­
mediated initiation interventions on generaliza-
 ehavioral Rehearsal and Behavior
B tion of social skills. The learners first received
Skills Training instruction on social competence through the
SCI-A curriculum developed by Stichter et al.
In behavioral rehearsal (Elliott & Gresham, (2010). This curriculum addresses facial expres-
1993), the child is given an opportunity to prac- sions, sharing ideas, turn taking, recognition of
tice the skills in a structured environment and feelings and emotions, and problem solving.
skills can be practiced covertly, overtly, or ver- Then during the generalization phase, either the
bally (Elliott & Gresham, 1993). Behavior skills two peer procedures were implemented. During
training (BST; Miltenberger et al., 2004) includes the peer-mediated proximity intervention, the
rehearsal as well as instruction, modeling by, and peers were instructed to seat near or across the
feedback from an expert. In a recent research learner and to only respond (no initiate) interac-
project evaluating the effects of BST, Peters and tions with the learner. During the peer-mediated
Thompson (2015) taught children with ASD to initiation intervention, the peers also sat near the
respond appropriately to nonvocal behavior of learner, but in addition to responding to the
others. In this study children were first taught to learner, peers were instructed to gain the learner’s
tact the nonvocal behavior of their listener as attention and initiate social interaction using
either interested or uninterested. Then they were topic starters or by commenting on activities or
taught to respond appropriately to uninterested the conversation. This study found that the peer-­
behavior by trying to engage the listener either by mediated initiation intervention led to greater
asking a question, changing the topic of the con- increases in the target social skills than did the
versation, or shifting to another topic again if the peer-mediated proximity intervention. Despite a
first change was not effective. Another example lot of research supporting the implementation of
of a package intervention that incorporates a BST peer-mediated procedures, some of the draw-
approach is the preschool life skills (PLS). PLS backs include the need to have peers available
incorporates 13 skills for preschoolers designed and willing to participate and the time required to
to reduce the likelihood of problem behavior in train the peers. However, peer-mediated interven-
the preschool classroom (Hanley, Fahmie, & tions may perhaps benefit the peers by teaching
Heal, 2014). them valuable skills.

Peer-Mediated Interventions General Interventions


for Performance Deficits
In peer-mediated interventions (Rogers, 2000;
Schmidt & Stichter, 2012), usually a typically Bellini, Benner, and Peters-Myszak (2009) pro-
developing child is taught to prompt and/or vide a guide for teaching socials skills to indi-
respond to (e.g., reinforce) response from the viduals with ASD. They highlight the importance
learner. Odom and Strain (1984) noted, however, of differentiating between skill deficits and per-
that peer-mediated procedures may consist of formance deficits, and they propose one method
placing a peer near a learner to facilitate learner for identification of performance deficits which
36 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

consists of answering the questions below Differential Reinforcement. Differential rein-


(pp 30). If you answer “yes” to any of these ques- forcement consists of providing the functional
tions, then you have identified a performance reinforcer contingent on an alternative appropri-
deficit: ate behavior (e.g., differential reinforcement of
alternative behavior, DRA) or, in some cases,
(a) “Can the child perform the skill across mul- the absence of the problem behavior (i.e., dif-
tiple settings or peers?” ferential reinforcement of other behaviors,
(b) “Can the child perform the skill without sup- DRO) while providing no consequences for the
port or assistance?” problem behavior. The most relevant type of dif-
(c) “Does the child perform the skill if reinforce- ferential reinforcement to social skills is func-
ment is provided?” tional communication training (FCT; Carr &
(d) “Does the child perform the skill if environ- Durand, 1985). Functional communication
mental modifications are made?” training consists of teaching the individual to
request for the functional reinforcer using a
If you have identified a performance deficit, functional communication response (FCR). For
you then need to select interventions that are example, if the problem behavior is maintained
appropriate. As suggested by Bellini and col- by negative reinforcement in the form of task
leagues, these may include the use of reinforce- removal, FCT is used to teach the individual to
ment (intrinsic and extrinsic), environmental request for a break from work. Functional com-
modifications, additional practice opportunities, munication responses may consist of vocal
priming and prompting strategies, and peer train- requests, picture or word cards, or functional
ing, among others. communication devices that assist individuals to
communicate appropriately. During FCT, the
individual receives the functional reinforcer
Functioned-Based Interventions (e.g., break, attention, toy) contingent on the
FCR, while all problem behavior is placed on
After conducting a functional assessment (e.g., extinction (see Extinction section below).
functional analysis) in which the antecedents
and consequences that maintain problem Extinction. Extinction is one of the most used
behavior have been identified, a function-based interventions to decrease problem behavior (e.g.,
intervention can be implemented to target the Iwata, Pace, Cowdery, & Miltenberger, 1994).
specific contingencies that maintain behavior. Typically, extinction is used in combination with
Function-­based interventions address the ante- another intervention (e.g., reinforcement) for the
cedents and consequences identified by the individual to engage in another (hopefully, appro-
functional assessments. For example, if a func- priate) response to receive the functional rein-
tional analysis determines that a child engages forcer. If the individual is not taught how to access
in problem behavior to receive adult attention, the reinforcer appropriately, another inappropri-
a function-­based intervention will target the ate response could be shaped. To implement
antecedent events (e.g., establishing operation extinction, the functional reinforcer is withheld
and discriminate stimulus) that increase the for the problem behavior. This way, the contin-
likelihood of the problem behavior and will gency between the occurrence of the problem
address the consequences that currently main- behavior and the delivery of the reinforcer is bro-
tain the problem behavior (e.g., receiving ken. For example, if a child consistently engages
attention). Function-­based interventions in crying for chocolate and the parent reinforces
include differential reinforcement, extinction, the child’s crying with the chocolate, there is a
and antecedent manipulations (Cooper, Heron, functional relationship between crying and receiv-
& Heward, Chapter 21). ing chocolate. To break this relationship, extinc-
Challenging Behavior 37

tion is implemented. Meaning, when the child Durand, 1985; Horner, Day, Sprague, O’Brien,
cries for chocolate, the parent does not provide & Heathfield, 1991; Lalli, Casey, & Kates,
the chocolate to the child. If another intervention 1995). However, one of the biggest limitations
is implemented with extinction (e.g., FCT), the of FCT, and other treatments of problem behav-
child learns how to request for chocolate using a ior, is the schedule in which the new alternative
communication response, and the parent is taught response is reinforced and the possible imprac-
to reinforce that response with the functional rein- ticality of providing reinforcement in the natural
forcer, in this case, the chocolate. environment. It is not always feasible for staff,
parents, and caregivers to reinforce all FCRs a
Antecedent Manipulations. Antecedent manipu- child emits (Hagopian, Boelter, & Jarmolowicz,
lations involve the altering of the establishing 2011). For example, if the responses are close
operations (EOs; Keller & Schoenfeld, 1950) and together in proximity (e.g., child requests for
discriminative stimuli (SDs) to decrease the like- attention every 10 s), the reinforcer is not avail-
lihood of problem behavior. When an EO is pres- able (e.g., iPad without battery), or the rein-
ent, a behavior is more likely to occur and a forcer cannot be provided (e.g., break from
reinforcer is more potent. Similarly, when an SD medical care), it may become very difficult for
is present, it signals to the individual that rein- the individuals to receive immediate reinforce-
forcement is available for a behavior that, in the ment after emitting the FCRs. Therefore, prob-
past, has been reinforced in the presence of that lem behavior may reemerge and the FCRs and
SD. For example, if a child has not eaten cookies reinforcement contingencies may be weaken
for 1 month (EO) and his grandma who typically (Fisher, Thompson, Hagopian, Bowman, &
reinforcers the child’s behavior of asking for Krug, 2000). Meaning, after attempting to
cookies is home and has brought cookies, the request for the desired item (e.g., toy) without
child might engage in the behavior of asking for receiving reinforcement, a child may engage in
cookies because this has being reinforced by his the previous problem behavior for which delay
grandmother under similar conditions in the past. to reinforcement was shorter. Therefore, to
In this example, the time since his last cookie increase the practicality and effectiveness over
would be the EO and the presence of grand- time of FCT and other treatments, it is very
mother and the cookies would be the SD. Putting important to thin the schedule of reinforcement
them together increases the likelihood of the (Hagopian, Fisher, Sullivan, Acquisto, &
child to request for cookies. Now, if we change LeBlanc, 1998) or increase the delay to rein-
the example, if the child engages in self-biting to forcement (Fisher et al., 2000).
receive cookies when he has not had cookies for
some time and grandma and the cookies are pres- Schedule Thinning and Delay to Reinforcement.
ent, altering the EO would result in providing the Hagopian et al. (2011) identified the four most
child with cookies prior to engaging in the behav- used schedule arrangements in the literature after
ior, and altering the SD would result in having implementing FCT: (a) delay schedules, (b) chain
grandma come visit without cookies. When con- schedules or demand fading, (c) multiple sched-
sequent-based treatments are not feasible or safe ules, and (d) response restriction.
to implement, antecedent manipulations could be
very effective in decreasing problem behavior. Delay Schedules. Delay schedules consist of
delaying the functional reinforcer (e.g., toy) con-
tingent on the functional communication response
 racticality of Function-Based
P (e.g., “toy please”) after the response has been
Interventions reinforced on a continuous schedule (e.g., imme-
diately after every response). During the imple-
Implementing treatments such as FCT success- mentation of delays, a verbal prompt (e.g., “wait”)
fully reduces problem behavior (e.g., Carr & is provided immediately after the functional
38 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

c­ ommunication response, and the individual has activity, or wait for their names to be called to
to “wait” until the delay duration is complete. The receive teachers’ attention. Moreover, waiting is
duration of delays typically increases across ses- a prerequisite skill for more difficult tasks
sions (e.g., Fisher et al., 1993). (Newquist, Dozier, & Neidert, 2012). For exam-
ple, if a child is working on increasing social
Chain Schedules or Demand Fading. Chain sched- skills, the child must know how to wait for his or
ules, also called demand fading, are used when the her turn to talk in a conversation.
functional analysis suggests that the ­target behav- Children who engage in challenging behavior
ior is maintained by negative reinforcement in the typically lack waiting skills. Therefore, placing
form of escape from task demands. Chain sched- these children in situations (e.g., first time at an
ules consist of the presentation of consecutive amusement park) in which they must wait to
demands that increase systematically before the receive reinforcement (e.g., get on rides) may
functional reinforcer (i.e., break) is provided for evoke challenging behavior. Previous research
the FCR (e.g., Lalli et al., 1995). has identified different procedures that can be
used to teach waiting in children. Some of these
Multiple Schedules. Multiple schedules consist procedures include engaging in another activity
of two or more signaled components that alter- while waiting (e.g., repeating rules; Hanley et al.,
nate. Each signal corresponds to a reinforcement 2007; having access to preferred items; Newquist
or extinction component. The duration of the et al., 2012) or using delay fading (e.g., Vollmer,
reinforcement component is systematically Borrero, Lalli, & Daniel, 1999). These interven-
decreased, while the duration of the extinction tions are important because teaching children
component systematically increases. During the with challenging behavior to wait may result in
reinforcement component, all FCRs are rein- higher access to reinforcement and social interac-
forced on a continuous schedule, and during the tions (Newquist et al., 2012).
extinction component, all FCRs are placed on
extinction, meaning no reinforcement is pro- Taking Turns. Closely related to waiting is taking
vided. Some of the signals that have been used turns. Turning-taking skills are a necessary foun-
for multiple schedules in the literature include dation for cooperative play and other successful
colored cards (e.g., Hanley, Iwata, & Thompson, peer social interactions (Schneider & Goldstein,
2001), colored lights (e.g., Campos, Leon, 2008). Like waiting, taking turns involves a delay
Sleiman, & Urcuyo, 2016), and colored bracelets between when a child may wish to respond and
(e.g., Betz, Fisher, Roane, Mintz, & Owen, 2013). when a response is socially desirable. In the case
of taking turns, a child must learn to respond only
Response Restriction. Response restriction con- after their partner has completed their response.
sists of the restriction of the functional commu- This may involve visual cues for conversational
nication response by removing the card or turn taking (Spohn, Timko, & Sainato, 1999;
device used to engage in the verbal response. Terpstra & Tamura, 2008) or in the context of
During this schedule thinning procedure, the play (Brok & Barakova, 2010; Stanton-Chapman
restrictions are progressively increased over & Snell, 2011). One approach to teaching turn
time (e.g., Roane, Fisher, Sgro, Falcomata, & taking is a “plan-do-­review” sequence used in a
Pabico, 2004). preschool curriculum in which conversational
turn taking skills are presented in the context of a
Waiting. Waiting is the time between the presen- dramatic play theme using social stories, children
tation of a stimulus and the opportunity to access have the opportunity to interact with one another
a reinforcer. Children must learn to wait to have in the dramatic play activity, and then their per-
access to a variety of reinforcers. In a school set- formance during the play activity using the skills
ting, children must wait in line to go to the play- is reviewed with them (Stanton-Chapman,
ground, wait for their turn to participate in Denning, & Roorbach Jamison, 2012).
Challenging Behavior 39

 ituations in Which Social Skills


S lectual disabilities in Ontario. Toronto, ON: Ontario
Mental Health Foundation.
May Not Be Relevant to Challenging Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. New York:
Behavior General Learning Press.
Bayley, N. (2006a). Bayley scales of infant and toddler
One potential function for challenging behavior development—Third edition: Administration manual.
San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
is what is called “automatic reinforcement”
Bayley, N. (2006b). Bayley scales of infant and toddler
(Vaughan & Michael, 1982). Because automati- development—Third edition: Technical manual. San
cally reinforced behavior produces its own Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
­reinforcer (i.e., pleasurable sensation or escape Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development®,
Third Edition (Bayley-III®). (n.d.). Retrieved from
from an aversive stimulus), social skills are not as
http://www.pearsonclinical.com/childhood/prod-
relevant as they are with socially mediated behav- ucts/100000123/bayley-scales-of-infant-and-toddler-
ior. See Shore and Iwata (1999) and Vollmer development-third-edition-bayley-iii.html#tab-details
(1994) for discussion of assessment and treat- Beavers, G. A., Iwata, B. A., & Lerman, D. C. (2013).
Thirty years of research on the functional analysis
ment challenges posed by automatically rein-
of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
forced behavior. For the most part, approaches to Analysis, 46, 1–21. doi:10.1002/jaba.30
treating challenging behavior maintained by Bellini, S. (2006). Autism social skills profile within
automatic reinforcement will not focus on train- building social relationships; A systematic approach
to teaching social interaction skills to children and
ing social skills. However, there are potential
adolescents with autism spectrum disorders and other
exceptions. Some automatically reinforced social difficulties.
behavior can be replaced with the development of Bellini, S. (2008). Building social relationships: A sys-
socially appropriate independent play skills. This tematic approach to teaching social interaction skills
to children and adolescents with autism spectrum dis-
is relevant to automatically reinforced behavior
orders and other social difficulties. Shawnee Mission,
that is thought to produce pleasurable sensations KS: Autism Asperger Publishing.
(automatic-­ positive reinforcement). There is Bellini, S., & Akullian, J. (2007). A meta-analysis of
another category of automatically reinforced video modeling and video self-modeling interven-
tions for children and adolescents with autism spec-
behavior that is thought to alleviate pain or aver-
trum disorder. Exceptional Children, 73, 264–287.
sive stimulation (automatic-negative reinforce- doi:10.1177/001440290707300301
ment; Shore & Iwata, 1999). Treating this Bellini, S., Benner, L., & Peters-Myszak, J. (2009). A sys-
category of challenging behavior may involve tematic approach to teaching social skills to children
with autism spectrum disorders: A guide for practitio-
teaching individuals to signal the presence of
ners. Beyond Behavior, 19, 26–39.
pain, asking for medical intervention to reduce Bellini, S., & Peters, J. (2008). Social skills training
aversive stimulation (e.g., asking for an over-the- for youth with autism spectrum disorder. Child and
counter pain reliever in the context of a head- Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17,
857–873. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2008.06.08.
ache), or both (Iwata, Vollmer, & Zarcone, 1990).
Bernard-Opitz, V., Sriram, N., & Nakhoda-Sapuan, S.
This type of approach involves a social skill, in (2001). Enhancing social problem, solving in chil-
the form of asking for assistance. However, in dren with autism and normal children through com-
most cases, if the functional behavioral assess- puter assisted-instruction. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 31, 377–384. doi:10.102
ment indicates automatically reinforced behav-
3/A:1010660502130
ior, approaches to intervention will not default to Betz, A. M., Fisher, W. W., Roane, H. S., Mintz, J. C.,
treatments related to social skills. & Owen, T. M. (2013). A component analysis of
schedule thinning during functional communication
training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46,
219–241. doi:10.1002/jaba.23.
References Bijou, S. W., Peterson, R. F., & Ault, M. H. (1968). A
method to integrate descriptive and experimental field
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic studies at the level of data and empirical concepts.
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 175–191.
Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. doi:10.1901/jaba.1968.1-175.
Atkinson, L., Feldman, M. A., McNamara, A., Uhlin, L., Bishop, D. V. (1998). Development of the Children’s
Niccols, A., & Leiserson, V. (1994). Survey of aber- Communication Checklist (CCC): A method for
rant behavior and its treatment in persons with intel- assessing qualitative aspects of communicative
40 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology Cooke, T. P., & Appoloni, T. (1976). Developing positive
and Psychiatry and Allies Disciplines, 39, 879–981. social-emotional behaviors: A study of training and
Bishop, D. V. M. (2006). CCC-2; Children’s generalization effects. Journal of Applied Behavior
Communication Checklist-2, United States edition, Analysis, 9, 65–78. doi:10.1901/jaba.1976.9-65
manual. San Antonio, TX: Pearson. Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007).
Bloom, S. E., Iwata, B. A., Fritz, J. N., Roscoe, E., & Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle
Carreau, A. (2011). Classroom-application of a River, NJ: Pearson.
trial-­based functional analysis. Journal of Applied Costa, S., Lehmann, H., Dautenhahn, K., Robins, B., &
Behavior Analysis, 44, 19–31. doi:10.1901/ Soares, F. (2015). Using a humanoid robot to elicit
jaba.2011.44-19. body awareness and appropriate physical interac-
Bloom, S. E., Lambert, J. M., Dayton, E., & Samaha, A. L. tion in children with autism. International Journal
(2013). Teacher-conducted trial-based functional anal- of Social Robotics, 7, 265–278. doi:10.1007/
yses as the basis for intervention. Journal of Applied s12369-014-0250-2
Behavior Analysis, 46, 208–218. doi:10.1002/jaba.21. Coucouvanis, J. (2005). Super skills: A social skills group
Bölte, S., Poustka, F., & Constantino, J. N. (2008). program for children with Asperger syndrome, high-­
Assessing autistic traits: Cross-cultural validation functioning autism, and related challenges. Shawnee
of the social responsiveness scale (SRS). Autism Mission, KS: Autism Asperger Publishing.
Research, 1, 354–363. doi:10.1002/aur.49. Crosby, J. W. (2011). Test review: F. M. Gresham & S. N.
Borkwick-Duffy, S. A., Eyman, R. K., & White, J. F. Elliott “Social Skills Improvement System Rating
(1987). Client characteristics and residential placement Scales.” Minneapolis, Minnesota—NCS Pearson,
patterns. American Journal of Mental Deficiencies, 92, 2008. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 29,
24–30. 292–296. doi:10.1177/0734282910385806
Brok, J. C. J., & Barakova, E. I. (2010). Engaging autis- Crozier, S., & Tincani, M. J. (2007). Using a modi-
tic children in imitation and turn-taking games with fied social story to decrease disruptive behavior of
multiagent system of interactive lighting blocks. In a child with autism. Focus on Autism and Other
H. S. Yang, R. Malaka, J. Hoshino, & J. H. Han (Eds.), Developmental Disabilities, 20, 150–157. doi:10.117
Entertainment computing – ICEC 2010. ICEC 2010 7/10883576050200030301
lecture notes in computer science (Vol. 6243). Berlin, Daniel B. LeGoff, (2004) Use of LEGO� as
Heidelberg: Springer. a Therapeutic Medium for Improving Social
Buggey, T. (2005). Video self-modeling applications with Competence. Journal of Autism and Developmental
students with autism spectrum disorder in a small Disorders 34 (5):557–571
private school setting. Focus On Autism And Other Delona, M., & Snell, M. E. (2006). The effects of social
Developmental Disabilities, 20, 52–63. doi:10.1177/1 stories on the social engagement of children with
0883576050200010501 autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 8,
Campos, C., Leon, Y., Sleiman, A., & Urcuyo, B. 29–42. doi:10.1177/10983007060080010501
(2016). Further evaluation of the use of multiple Didden, R., Korzilius, H., van Oorsouw, W., & Sturmey,
schedules for behavior maintained by negative rein- P. (2006). Behavioral treatment of challenging behav-
forcement. Behavior Modification, 41, 269–285. iors in individuals with mild mental retardation: Meta-­
doi:10.1177/0145445516670838 analysis of single-subject research. American Journal
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior on Mental Retardation, 111, 290–298.
problems through functional communication training. Do-Watch-Listen-Say. (n.d.). Social and communication
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126. intervention for children with autism. Retrieved from
doi:10.1901/jaba.1985.18-111 http://products.brookespublishing.com/DO-WATCH-­
Chan, J. M., & O’Reilly, M. F. (2008). A social stories LISTEN-SAY-P18.aspx
intervention package for students with autism in inclu- Dupere, S., MacDonald, R. P. F., & Ahearn, W. H. (2013).
sive classroom settings. Journal of Applied Behavior Using video modeling with substitutable loops to
Analysis, 41, 405–409. doi:10.1901/jaba.2008.41-405 teach varied play to children with autism. Journal of
Charlop-Christy, M. H., Le, L., & Freeman, K. A. (2000). Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 662–668. doi:10.1002/
A comparison of video modeling with in vivo mod- jaba.68.
eling for teaching children with autism. Journal of Elliott, S. N., & Gresham, F. M. (1993). Social skills
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 537–552. interventions for children. Behavior Modification, 17,
doi:10.1023/A:1005635326276 287–313. doi:10.1177/01454455930173004
Children’s Communication Checklist-2 U.S. Edition Emerson, E. (2003). Prevalence of psychiatric disorders in
(CCC—2). (n.d.). Retrieved from http://www. children and adolescents with and without intellectual
pearsonclinical.com/language/products/100000193/ disability. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research,
childrens-­c ommunication-checklist2-us-edition-­ 47, 51–58. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2788.2003.00464.x.
ccc-2.html#tab-details FISH. (n.d.). Functional independence skills hand-
Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2012). The Social book: Assessment and curriculum for individuals
Responsiveness Scale manual, second edition (SRS-­2). with developmental disabilities. Retrieved from
Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. http://www.proedinc.com/customer/productView.
aspx?ID=1392
Challenging Behavior 41

Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C., Cataldo, M., Harrell, R., Hanley, G. P., Heal, N. A., Tiger, J. H., & Ingvarsson, E. T.
Jefferson, G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional com- (2007). Evaluation of a classwide teaching pro-
munication training with and without extinction and gram for developing preschool life skills. Journal of
punishment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 277–300. doi:10.1901/
26, 23–36. doi:10.1901/jaba.1993.26-23. jaba.2007.57–06
Fisher, W. W., Thompson, R. H., Hagopian, L. P., Bowman, Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & Thompson, R. H. (2001).
L. G., & Krug, A. (2000). Facilitating ­tolerance of Reinforcement schedule thinning following treatment
delayed reinforcement during functional communica- with functional communication training. Journal of
tion training. Behavior Modification, 24, 3–29. Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 17–38. doi:10.1901/
Fisher, W. W., Thompson, R. H., Hagopian, L. P., jaba.2001.34-17
Bowman, L. G., & Krug, A. (2016). Facilitating tol- Holden, B., & Gitlesen, J. P. (2006). A total popula-
erance of delayed reinforcement during functional tion study of challenging behavior in the county of
­communication training. Behavior Modification, 24(1), Hedmark, Norway: Prevalence and risk markers.
3–29. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 27, 456–465.
Freeman, K., Anderson, C., & Scotti, J. (2000). A struc- doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2005.06.001
tured descriptive methodology: Increasing a­ greement Henry S Roane, Wayne W Fisher, Gina M Sgro, Terry S
between descriptive and experimental analysis. Falcomata, Robert R Pabico, (2004) An alternative
Education and Training in Mental Retardation and method of thinning reinforcer delivery during dif-
Developmental Disabilities, 35, 55–66. ferential reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior
Ganz, J. B., & Flores, M. M. (2008). Effects of the use Analysis 37 (2):213-218
of visual strategies in play groups for children with Hopkins, I. M., Gower, M. W., Perez, T. A., Smith, D. S.,
autism spectrum disorders and their peers. Journal of Amthor, F. R., Wimsatt, C., & Biasini, F. J. (2011).
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 926–940. Avatar assistant: Improving social skills in students
doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0463-4 with an ASD through a computer-based intervention.
Gray, C. A. (2002). The new social story book. UK: Future Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 41,
Horizons. 1543–1555. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1179-z
Gresham, F. M. (1981). Social skills training with handicapped Horner, R. H., Day, H. M., Sprague, J. R., O’Brien, M., &
children: A review. Review of Educational Research, Heathfield, L. T. (1991). Interspersed requests: A non-
51, 139–176. doi:10.3102/00346543051001139 aversive procedure for reducing aggression and self-­
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1993). Social skills injury during instruction. Journal of Applied Behavior
intervention for children. Behavior Modification, 17, Analysis, 24, 265–278. doi:10.1901/jaba.1991.24-265
287–313. doi:10.1177/01454455930173004 Horner, R. D., & Keilitz, I. (1975). Training mentally
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (2008). Social retarded adolescents to brush their teeth. Journal of
skills improvement system rating scales manual. Applied Behavior Analysis, 8, 301–309. doi:10.1901/
Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson. jaba.1975.8-301
Gresham, F. M., & Nagle, R. J. (1980). Social skills train- Ingersoll, B., & Schreibman, L. (2006). Teaching recip-
ing with children: Responsiveness to modeling and rocal imitation skills to young children with autism
coaching as a function of peer orientation. Journal using a naturalistic behavioral approach: Effects on
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 718–729. language, pretend play, and joint attention. Journal of
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.48.6.718 Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 487–505.
Hagopian, L. P., Boelter, E. W., & Jarmolowicz, D. P. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0089-y
(2011). Reinforcement schedule thinning following Iwata, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., & Roscoe, E. M. (2013).
functional communication training: Review and rec- Reliability and validity of the functional analysis
ommendations. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 4, 4–16. screening tool. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Sullivan, M. T., Acquisto, 46, 271–284. doi:10.1002/jaba.31
J., & LeBlanc, L. A. (1998). Effectiveness of func- Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E.,
tional communication training with and without & Richman, G. S. (1982/1994). Toward a functional
extinction and punishment: A summary of 21 inpatient analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior
cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 211– Analysis, 27, 197–209. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1994.27-
235. doi:10.1901/jaba.1998.31-211 ­197. (Original work published 1982)
Hammond, J. L., Iwata, B. A., Rooker, G. W., Fritz, J. N., Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Cowdery, G. E., & Miltenberger,
& Bloom, S. E. (2013). Effects of fixed versus random R. G. (1994). What makes extinction work: An
condition sequencing during multielement functional analysis of procedural form and function. Journal of
analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 131–144. doi:10.1901/
22–30. doi:10.1002/jaba.7 jaba.1994.27–131
Hanley, G. P., Fahmie, T. A., & Heal, N. A. (2014). Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., & Zarcone, J. R. (1990). The
Evaluation of the preschool life skills program in Head experimental (functional) analysis of behavior disor-
Start classrooms: A systematic replication. Journal of ders: Methodology, applications, and limitations. In A.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 47, 443–448. doi:10.1002/ C. Repp & N. N. Singh (Eds.), Perspectives on the use
jaba.132 of nonaversive and aversive interventions for ­persons
42 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

with developmental disabilities (pp. 301–330). ing functional analyses. Research in Developmental
Sycamore, IL: Sycamore. Disabilities, 31, 480–487.
Kahng, S., Iwata, B. A., & Lewin, A. B. (2002). LeGoff, D. B. (2004). Use of LEGO© as a therapeutic
Behavioral treatment of self-injury, 1964-2000. medium for improving social competence. Journal of
American Jour­nal of Mental Retardation, 107, 212– Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 557–571.
221. doi:10.1352/0895-8017(2002)107<0212:BTOSI Lerman, D. C., & Vorndran, C. M. (2002). On the sta-
T>2.0.CO;2 tus of knowledge for using punishment: Implications
Kandalaft, M. R., Didehbani, N., Krawczyk, D. C., for treating behavior disorders. Journal of Applied
Allen, T. T., & Chapman, S. B. (2013). Virtual real- Behavior Analysis, 35, 431–464.
ity social cognition training for young adults with Lorenzo, G., Pomares, J., & Lledo, A. (2013). Inclusion
high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and of immersive virtual learning environments and visual
Developmental Disorders, 43, 34–44. doi:10.1007/ control systems to support learning of students with
s10803-012-1544-6 Asperger syndrome. Computer Education, 62, 88–101.
Keller, F. S., & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). Principles of Luczynski, K. C., & Hanley, G. P. (2013). Prevention of
psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. problem behavior by teaching functional communica-
Killian, W. (2008). FISH: Functional independence skills tion and self-control skills to preschoolers. Journal of
handbook assessment and curriculum for individuals Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 355–368. doi:10.1002/
with developmental disabilities. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed. jaba.44
Klett, L. S., & Turan, Y. (2012). Generalized effects Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (2001). Promoting
of social stories with task analysis for teaching inclusion in secondary classrooms. Learning Disability
menstrual care to three young girls with autism. Quarterly, 24, 265–274. doi:10.2307/1511115
Sexuality and Disability, 30, 319–336. doi:10.1007/ Matson, J. L. (1988). The Matson Evaluation of Social
s11195-011-9244-2 Skills with Youngsters (MESSY). Worthington, OH:
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Harrower, J. K., & Carter, International Diagnostic Systems.
C. M. (1999). Pivotal response intervention I: Matson, J. L. (1995). The Matson Evaluation of Social
Overview of approach. The Journal of the Association Skills for Individuals with Severe Retardation
for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24, 174–185. (MESSIER). Baton Rouge, LA: Disability Consultants,
doi:10.2511/rpsd.24.3.174 LLC.
Kokina, A., & Kern, L. (2010). Social story interventions for Matson, J. L., Bamburg, J. W., Cherry, K. E., & Paclawskyj,
students with autism spectrum disorders:A meta-­analysis. T. R. (1999). A validity study on the Questions About
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, Behavioral Function (QABF) scale: Predicting treat-
1803–1814. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0931-0 ment success for self-injury, aggression, and stereo-
Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1998). Social typies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 20,
interaction skills for children with autism: A script-­ 163–176. doi:10.1016/S0891-4222(98)00039-0
fading procedure for beginning readers. Journal of Matson, J. L., LeBlanc, L. A., & Weinheimer, B.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 191–202. doi:10.1901/ (1999). Reliability of the Matson Evaluation of
jaba.1998.31-191 Social Skills in Individuals with Severe Retardation
Kroeger, K. A., Schultz, J. R., & Newsome, C. (2007). (MESSIER). Behavior Modification, 23, 647–661.
A comparison of two group-delivered social skills doi:10.1177/0145445599234008
programs for young children with autism. Journal of Matson, J. L., Matson, M. L., & Rivet, T. T. (2007). Social
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 808–817. skills treatment for children with autism spectrum
doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0207-x disorders: An overview. Behavior Modification, 31,
Lalli, J. S., Casey, S., & Kates, K. (1995). Reducing 682–707. doi:10.1177/0145445507301650
escape behavior and increasing task completion Matson, J. L., Neal, D., Fodstad, J. C., Hess, J. A.,
with functional communication training, extinction, Mahan, S., & Rivet, T. T. (2010). Reliability and
and response chaining. Journal of Applied Behavior validity of the matson evaluation of social skills with
Analysis, 28, 261–268. doi:10.1901/jaba.1995.28-261 youngsters. Behavior Modification, 34(6), 539–558.
Lambert, J. M., Bloom, S. E., & Irvin, J. (2012). Trial-­ doi:10.1177/0145445510384844.
based functional analysis and functional communica- Matson, J. L., Rotatori, A. F., & Helsel, W. J. (1983).
tion training in an early childhood setting. Journal of Development of a rating scale to measure
Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 579–584. social skills in children: The Matson Evaluation
Lambert, J. M., Bloom, S. E., Kunnavatana, S. S., Collins, of Social Skills with Youngsters (MESSY).
S. D., & Clay, C. J. (2013). Training residential staff Behavior Research and Therapy, 21, 335–340.
to conduct trial-based functional analyses. Journal of doi:10.1016/0005-7967(83)90001-3
Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 296–300. doi:10.1002/ Matson, J. L., & Vollmer, T. (1995). Questions about
jaba.17 behavioral function (QABF). Baton Rouge, LA:
LaRue, R. H., Lenard, K., Weiss, M. J., Bamond, M., Disability Consultants, LLC.
Palmieri, M., & Kelley, M. E. (2010). Comparison of Matson, J. L., & Wilkins, J. (2007). A critical review
traditional and trial-based methodologies for conduct- of assessment targets and methods for social skills
Challenging Behavior 43

excesses and deficits for children with autism spectrum Northup, J., Wacker, D., Sasso, G., Steege, M., Cigrand,
disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1, K., Cook, J., & DeRaad, A. (1991). A brief func-
28–37. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2006.07.003 tional analysis of aggressive and alternative behavior
McConnell, S. R. (2002). Interventions to facilitate social in an outclinic setting. Journal of Applied Behavior
interactions for young children with autism: Review Analysis, 24, 509–522. doi:10.1901/jaba.1991.24-509
of available research and recommendations for edu- Oden, S., & Asher, S. R. (1977). Coaching children in
cational intervention and future research. Journal of social skills for friendship making. Child Development,
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 351–372. 48, 495–506. doi:10.2307/1128645
doi:10.1023/A:102053780515 Odom, S. L., McConnell, S. R., & Brown, W. H.
MacDonald, R. P., Dickson, C. A., Martineau, M., & (2008). Social competence of young children. In
Ahearn, W. H. (2015). Prerequisite skills that sup- W. Brown & S. L. Odom (Eds.), Social competence
port learning through video modeling. Education and of young children: Risk, disability, and interven-
Treatment of Children, 38, 33–47. tion (pp. 3–30). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes
McPartland, J. C., Reichow, B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2012). Publishing Co.
Sensitivity and specificity of proposed DSM-5 Odom, S. L., & Strain, P. S. (1984). Peer-mediated
diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder. approaches to promoting children’s social interaction:
Journal of the American Academy of Child and A review. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 54,
Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 368–383. doi:10.1016/j. 544–557.
jaac.2012.01.007 O’Neill, R. E., Albin, R. W., Storey, K., Horner, R. H., &
Miltenberger, R. G., Flessner, C., Gatheridge, B., Johnson, Sprague, J. R. (2014). Functional assessment and pro-
B., Satterlund, M., & Egemo, K. (2004). Evaluation gram development. Ontario, Canada: Nelson Education
of behavioral skills training to prevent gun play in Owens, G., Granader, Y., Humphrey, A., & Baron-Cohen,
children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, S. (2008). LEGO therapy and the social use of lan-
513–516. doi:10.1901/jaba.2004.37-513 guage programme: An evaluation of two social skills
Murdock, L. C., Ganz, J., & Crittendon, J. (2013). Use interventions for children with high functioning
of an iPad play story to increase play dialogue of pre- autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism
schoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1944–1957.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 2174–2189. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0590-6
doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1770-6 Ozdemir, S. (2008). The effectiveness of social sto-
Murphy, O., Healy, O., & Leader, G. (2009). Risk fac- ries on decreasing disruptive behaviors of children
tors for challenging behaviors among 157 chil- with autism: Three case studies. Journal of Autism
dren with autism spectrum disorder in Ireland. and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1689–1696.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 474–482. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0551-0
doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2008.09.008 Parsons, S., & Mitchell, P. (2002). The potential of virtual
Muscott, H. S., & Gifford, T. (1994). Virtual reality and reality in social skills training for people with autistic
social skills training for students with behavioral spectrum disorders. Journal of Intellectual Disability
disorders: Applications, challenges and promising Research, 46, 430–443.
practices. Education and Treatment of Children, 17, Partington, J. W. (2006). The Assessment of Basic
417–434. Language and Learning Skills – Revised (ABLLS-R).
National Research Council and Institute of Medicine. Walnut Hill, CA: Behavior Analysts, Inc.
(2000). Making friends and getting along with peers. Peters, L. C., & Thompson, R. H. (2015). Teaching chil-
In J. P. Shonkoff & D. A. Phillips (Eds.), From neu- dren with autism to respond to conversation partners’
rons to neighborhoods: The science of early child- interest. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48,
hood development (pp. 163–181). Washington, DC: 544–562. doi:10.1002/jaba.235
National Academy Press. Querim, A. C., Iwata, B. A., Roscoe, E. M.,
Newquist, M. H., Dozier, C. L., & Neidert, P. L. (2012). Schlichenmeyer, K. J., Ortega, J. V., & Hurl, K. E.
A comparison of the effects of brief rules, a timer, (2013). Functional analysis screening for problem
and preferred toys on self-control. Journal of Applied behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement.
Behavior Analysis, 45, 497–509. doi:10.1901/ Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 47–60.
jaba.2012.45–497 doi:10.1002/jaba.26
Nikopoulos, C. K., & Keenan, M. (2004). Effects of video Quill, K. (2000). Do-Watch-Listen-Say: Social and com-
modeling on social initiations by children with autism. munication intervention for children with autism.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 93–96. Baltimore, MD: Brooks Publishing Co.
doi:10.1901/jaba.2004.37-93 Rao, P. A., Beidel, D., & Murray, M. (2008). Social
Nishizawa, H., Kimura, T., & Goh, A. C. (2015). The skills interventions for children with asperger’s
effect of different imitation models on the accuracy and syndrome or high-functioning autism: A review
spend of imitation of movement. Journal of Physical and recommendations. Journal of Autism and
Therapy Science, 27, 3417–3420. doi:10.1589/ Developmental Disorders, 38, 353–361. doi:10.1007/
jpts.27.3417 s10803-007-0402-4
44 C. Cividini-Motta et al.

Reichow, B., Steiner, A. M., & Volkmar, F. (2013). autism through instructional video modeling. Journal
Cochrane review: Social skills groups for people of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4, 165–175.
aged 6 to 21 with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). doi:10.1177/10983007020040030501
Evidence Based Child Health: A Cochrane Review Shore, B. A., & Iwata, B. A. (1999). Assessment and treat-
Journal, 8, 266–315. doi:10.1002/ebch.1903 ment of behavior disorders maintained by nonsocial
Reichow, B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2010). Social skills (automatic) reinforcement. In A. C. Repp & R. H.
interventions for individuals with autism: Evaluation Horner (Eds.), Functional analysis of problem behav-
for evidence-based practices within a best evi- ior: From effective assessment to effective support
dence synthesis framework. Journal of Autism and (pp. 117–145). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Developmental Disorders, 40, 149–166. doi:10.1007/ Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs,
s10803-009-0842-0 NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Rinaldi, W. (2004). Social Use of Language Programme: Smith, R. G., & Churchill, R. M. (2002). Identification
Infant and Primary School. Teaching book 1. Surrey: of environmental determinants of behavior disorders
Wendy. Rinaldi. through functional analysis of precursor behaviors.
Roane, H. S., Fisher, W. W., Sgro, G. M., Falcomata, T. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 125–136.
S., & Pabico, R. R. (2004). An alternative method of doi:10.1901/jaba.2002.35-125
thinning reinforcer delivery during differential reinfor­ Social Responsiveness Scale™, Second Edition
cement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37(2), (SRS™-2). (n.d.). Retrieved from http://www.
213–218. wpspublish.com/store/p/2994/social-responsiveness-
Robins, B., Dickerson, P., Stribling, P., & Dautenhahn, scale-second-edition-srs-2
K. (2004). Robot-mediated joint attention in children Social Skills Improvement System (SSIS) Rating Scales.
with autism: A case study in robot-human interac- (n.d.). Retrieved from http://www.pearsonclinical.
tion. Interaction Studies, 5, 161–198. doi:10.1075/ com/education/products/100000322/social-skills-­
is.5.2.02rob improvement-system-ssis-rating-scales.html
Rogers, S. J. (2000). Interventions that facilitate social- Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005).
ization in children with autism. Journal of Autism The Vineland adaptive behavior scales (2nd ed.).
and Developmental Disorders, 30, 399–409. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson Inc.
doi:10.1023/A: 1005543321840 Spohn, J. R., Timko, T. C., & Sainato, D. M. (1999).
Sandall, S. R., & Schwartz, I. S. (2008). Building blocks Increasing the social interactions of preschool children
for teaching preschoolers with special needs (2nd ed.). with disabilities during mealtimes: The effects of an
Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing Co. interactive placemat game. Education and Treatment
Sani Bozkurt, S., & Vuran, S. (2014). An analysis of the of Children, 22(1), 1–18.
use of social stories in teaching social skills to chil- Stanton-Chapman, T. L., Denning, C. B., & Roorbach
dren with autism spectrum disorders. Educational Jamison, K. (2012). Communication skill building in
Sciences: Theory and Practice, 14, 1875–1892. young children with and without disabilities in a pre-
Schmidt, C., & Stichter, J. P. (2012). The use of peer-­ school classroom. The Journal of Special Education,
mediated interventions to promote the generaliza- 46, 78–93. doi:10.1177/0022466910378044
tion of social competence for adolescents with Stanton-Chapman, T. L., & Snell, M. E. (2011). Promoting
high-functioning autism and Asperger’s syndrome. turn-taking skills in preschool children with disabili-
Exceptionality, 20, 94–113. doi:10.1080/09362835.2 ties: The effects of a peer-based social communication
012.669303 intervention. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
Schneider, N., & Goldstein, H. (2008). Social compe- 26, 303-319.
tence interventions for young children with com- Stichter, J. P., Herzog, M. J., Visovsky, K., Schmidt, C.,
munication and language disorders. In W. Brown Randolph, J., Schultz, T., & Gage, N. (2010). Social
& S. L. Odom (Eds.), Social competence of young competence intervention for youth with Asperger syn-
children: Risk, disability, and intervention (pp. 233– drome and high-functioning autism: An initial investiga-
252). Baltimore, Maryland: Paul H. Brookes tion. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Publishing Co. 40, 1067–1079. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-0959-1
Schumaker, J. B., & Hazel, J. S. (1984). Social skills Stone, W., Ruble, L., Coonrod, E., Hepburn, S.,
assessment and training for the learning disabled: Pennington, M., Burnette, C., & Brigham, N. B.
Who’s on first and what’s on second? Part I. Journal (2010). TRIAD social skills assessment (2nd ed.).
of Learning Disabilities, 17, 422–431. Nashville, TN: Vanderbilt TRIAD.
Shabani, D. B., Katz, R. C., Wilder, D. A., Beauchamp, Sundberg, M. L. (2014). VBMAPP: Verbal Behavior
K., Taylor, C. R., & Fischer, K. J. (2002). Increasing Milestones and Placement Program—Protocol: A lan-
social initiations in children with autism: Effects of a guage and social skills assessment program for chil-
tactile prompt. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, dren with autism or other developmental disabilities
35, 79–83. doi:10.1901/jaba.2002.35-79 (2nd ed.). Concord, CA: AVB Press.
Shipley-Benamou, R., Lutzker, J. R., & Taubman, M. Symons, F. J., Harper, V. N., McGrath, P. J., Breau, L. M.,
(2002). Teaching daily living skills to children with & Bodfish, J. W. (2009). Evidence of increased non-­
Challenging Behavior 45

verbal signs of pain in adults with neurodevelop- Vollmer, T. R., Borrero, J. C., Lalli, J. S., & Daniel, D.
mental disorders and chronic self-injury. Research in (1999). Evaluating self-control and impulsivity in
Developmental Disabilities, 30, 521–528. children with severe behavior disorders. Journal of
Taylor, B. A., & Hoch, H. (2008). Teaching children with Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 451–466. doi:10.1901/
autism to respond to and initiate bids for joint atten- jaba.1999.32–451
tion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 377– Walker, H. M., & Severson, H. H. (1992). Systematic
391. doi:10.1901/jaba.2008.41-377 screening for behavior disorders. Longmont, CO:
Taylor, B. A., & Levin, L. (1998). Teaching a student with Sopris West.
autism to make verbal initiations: Effects of a tactile Wallace, M. D., & Iwata, B. A. (1999). Effects of session
prompt. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, duration on functional analysis outcomes. Journal of
651–654. doi:10.1901/jaba.1998.31-651 Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 175–183. doi:10.1901/
Terpstra, J. E., & Tamura, R. (2008). Effective social jaba.1999.32-175
interaction strategies for inclusive settings. Early Wayne W. Fisher, Rachel H. Thompson, Louis P.
Childhood Education Journal, 35, 405–411. Hagopian, Lynn G. Bowman, Amy Krug, (2016)
doi:10.1007/s10643-007-0225-0 Facilitating Tolerance of Delayed Reinforcement
Thomason-Sassi, J. L., Iwata, B. A., Neidert, P. L., & During Functional Communication Training. Behavior
Roscoe, E. M. (2011). Response latency as an index of Modification 24 (1):3–29
response strength during functional analyses of prob- Weiss, M. J., & Harris, S. L. (2001). Teaching social skills
lem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, to people with autism. Behavior Modification, 25,
44, 51–67. doi:10.1901/jaba.2011.44-51 785–802.
Tonge, B. J., & Einfeld, S. L. (2003). Psychopathology Whalen, C., & Schreibman, L. (2003). Joint attention
and intellectual disability: The Australian child to adult training for children with autism using behavior
longitudinal study. International Review of Research ­modification procedures. Journal of Child Psycho­
in Mental Retardation, 26, 61–91. doi:10.1016/ logy and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 44,
S0074-7750(03)01002-4 456–468.
Touchette, P. E., MacDonald, R. F., & Langer, S. N. Whitaker, S., & Read, S. (2006). The prevalence of psy-
(1985). A scatter plot for identifying stimulus control chiatric disorders among people with intellectual
of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior disabilities: An analysis of the literature. Journal of
Analysis, 18, 343–351. doi:10.1901/jaba.1985.18-343 Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 19, 330–
Vandermeer, J., Beamish, W., Milford, T., & Lang, W. 345. doi:10.1111/j.1468-3148.2006.00293.x
(2013). iPad-presented social stories for young chil- White, S. W., Keonig, K., & Scahill, L. (2007). Social
dren with autism. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, skills development in children with autism spectrum
18, 75–81. doi:10.3109/17518423.2013.809811 disorders: A review of the intervention research.
Vaughan, M. E., & Michael, J. L. (1982). Automatic Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37,
reinforcement: An important but ignored concept. 1858–1868. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0320-x
Behaviorism, 10, 217–227. Wright, M. J. (1980). Measuring the social compe-
Vedora, J., Meunier, L., & Mackay, H. (2009). Teaching tence of preschool children. Canadian Journal of
intraverbal behavior to children with autism: A com- Behavioral Science, 69, 848–872. doi:10.1037/
parison of textual and echoic prompts. The Analysis of h0081042
Verbal Behavior, 25, 79–86. Zanolli, K., & Daggett, J. (1998). The effects of rein-
Vollmer, T. R. (1994). The concept of automatic reinforce- forcement rate on the spontaneous social initia-
ment: Implications for behavioral research in devel- tions of socially withdrawn preschoolers. Journal of
opmental disabilities. Research in Developmental Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 117–125. ­doi:10.1901/
Disabilities, 15, 187–207. jaba.1998.31-117
Developmental Issues

Patricia Soto-Icaza and Pablo Billeke

that allow us to interact with others and under-


Introduction stand other people’s intentions, feelings, emo-
tions, and behaviors (Billeke & Aboitiz, 2013).
Social development has been a subject of inter- Furthermore, social behavior refers to the ability
est for social sciences and neuroscience during to interact with others. Social functioning, in
the past decades. The aim of this chapter is to turn, refers to social behavior when it is inte-
integrate a vast range of scientific evidence in grated in different contexts and over time
order to comprehend how human beings are (Kennedy & Adolphs, 2012) (for a summary of
able to develop the ability to understand others key social concepts, see Table 1).
and properly interact in a complex and dynamic The analysis of social skill development
social world. should consider all the factors described above in
Social development involves an ever-­ a developmental perspective. The comprehension
increasing refinement of social behaviors, cogni- of such perspective should consider dynamic
tion, and brain during the entire life span. Even environmental changes across life span, cerebral
though social cognition is the most commonly functioning specialization, and skill learning. In
used concept in scientific literature, social analy- this context, an interesting approach that includes
sis includes several elements that interact with these aspects of analysis is Johnson’s perspective
each other, such as social brain, cognition, about interactive specialization (Johnson, 2011).
behavior, and functioning (Kennedy & Adolphs, This perspective includes two core elements:
2012). While social brain refers to several brain localization and specialization. While localiza-
areas that sustain social processing, social cog- tion refers to the association of an ability or func-
nition involves all kind of cognitive processing tion with a brain area or network, specialization
refers to the degree of refinement of this function.
Regarding the development of social skills, this
P. Soto-Icaza (*) approach might explain why certain behaviors
Laboratorio de Neurociencias Cognitivas, Pontificia that appear as rudimentary abilities during the
Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile first years of life acquire an undeniable social
e-mail: pasoto@uc.cl value years later. In this sense, social develop-
P. Billeke ment should not be understood as a mere linear
División de Neurociencias, Centro de Investigación trajectory of behaviors that are maturing. In fact,
en Complejidad Social (neuroCICS), Universidad del
Desarrollo, Santiago, Chile to achieve a better understanding of the develop-
e-mail: pablo.billeke@gmail.com ment of social functioning, it is crucial to attend

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 47


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_4
48 P. Soto-Icaza and P. Billeke

Table 1  Social concepts (based on Soto-Icaza et al., 2015)

Concept Definition Level involved


Neural Cognitive Behavioral

Social brain Brain network whose function is


associated with social processing. It
could be described as structures
operating in a network that could
enable an accurately social
performance.

Social All kind of cognitive processes that


cognition can allow us to interact with others
and to understand other people’s
intentions, feelings, emotions and
behaviors.

Social The ability to interact with others.


behavior

Social skills A wide group of abilities that


emerges from the appropriate
execution of social cognition
processing. This adequate
performance allows us to interact
and communicate with others, by
predicting and understanding other
people’s intentions, feelings,
emotions and behaviors.

Social Social behavior when it is


functioning integrated over time and context.

Social A group of inborn or early abilities


precursors readily observable in newborns or
early infancy such as eye-like
sensitivity, biological motion
preference and imitation.

The level that each social concept involves is represented by the shaded area in the three columns
on the right. From left to right, neural level is shown in the dark gray, cognitive level is shown in
the medium gray and behavioral level is shown in the light grey. Note that one concept can encom-
pass more than one level

the temporal dimension of the onset of social basic social signs coming from others. Thus,
behaviors together with the constraints imposed these adult skills come from simpler skills pres-
by neural and behavioral evidence (Soto-Icaza, ent in the early development, such as imitation,
Aboitiz, & Billeke, 2015). detection of biological motion, and sensitivity to
While it is true that development could be eyelike stimulus. In this context, abilities that
described in chronological terms, this approach arise during infancy can be understood as precur-
can reduce it to a mere number of actions disre- sors, not only because they appear first in human
garding the notion that all those behaviors are life but also because they are required for the
interconnected. Adult social skills are developed acquisition of further social abilities, such as the
from childhood abilities, which aim to handle ability to interpret other’s feelings and thoughts
Developmental Issues 49

(Charman et al., 2000; Happé & Frith, 2014; behaviors of adults are associated with a spe-
Soto-Icaza et al., 2015). Indeed, social capacities cialization process of visual attention and visual
that appear later in childhood allows us to deal orientation that begins early in life (Johnson &
with more complex social information. For de Haan, 2015). Indeed, the early development
example, in experimental paradigms of social of several visual abilities such as detection of
games, the ability to interpret other’s intentions biological motion, preference for eyes, face
enables children to predict their partner’s behav- perception, face discrimination, mutual gaze,
ior and modulate their own behavior in order to gaze following, directing one’s gaze, and joint
achieve a successful interaction (Axelrod & attention is crucial for the development of social
Hamilton, 1981; Billeke et al., 2014; Gonzalez-­ functioning. The early presence of these visual
Gadea et al., 2016; Steinbeis, Bernhardt, & abilities contributes to the detection of the social
Singer, 2012). agent and allows the child to communicate with
Following this development perspective, we others in order to obtain what is needed (Soto-­
will describe the main evidence related to typi- Icaza et al., 2015).
cally social functioning both at behavioral and The processing of biological motion has been
neural levels, highlighting both cerebral events a topic of interest to researchers for decades as
that have been associated with social behaviors it sheds light about the mechanisms through
and the role of specific skills in social function- which humans are able to interpret and imi-
ing. We will first focus on the infancy develop- tate complex sequences of action performed
ment of behaviors that are closely related to the by other humans (Johnson, 2006; Bertenthal,
visual sensory system. Here we will analyze Proffitt, & Cutting, 1984; Johansson, 1973;
how these behaviors can be understood as social Pavlova & Sokolov, 2000; Simion, Regolin, &
building blocks from which more refine behav- Bulf, 2008). A landmark study by Meltzoff and
iors are developed. Secondly, we will address Moore (1977) revealed that infants only 12 days
behavior and neural evidence of social develop- old have a mimicry behavior. They investigated
ment during preschool years focusing on men- imitation in both facial and manual gestures in
talizing skills. In both parts we will review how 12- to 21-day-­old infants. Results showed that
comprehension of conditions such as blindness, the imitation behavior of the infants occurred for
deafness, and autism can contribute for a better all four assessed gestures: lip protrusion, mouth
understanding of social development. Finally, opening, tongue protrusion, and sequential fin-
we will analyze the impact of these abilities in ger movement.
the development of more complex social skills The ability to imitate and discriminate motion
during school years such as egocentric and directions both in humans and animals seems to
altruistic actions trough the review of social reflect our social orientation. In fact, evidence
game paradigm. showed that human beings are able to discrimi-
nate between a biological and a non-biological
motion animation since birth (Bertenthal, Proffitt,
 ocial Skill Development
S & Cutting, 1984; Pavlova & Sokolov, 2000;
During Infancy: An Essentially Simion et al., 2008). An interesting study revealed
Sensory World? that newborns prefer biological motion over non-­
biological motion only if the stimulus is upright
Social development has a strong relationship (Simion et al., 2008). Moreover, this early ability
with sensory systems specialization. Several to discriminate between moving and static stimu-
studies have shown that social development is lus was also observed in infants of 3 and 5 months
closely related to maturation of visual system of age, regardless of whether the stimulus was
and especially with its capacity to recognize face up or face down (Bertenthal et al., 1984).
a particular feature of social agents (i.e., eyes, The ability of infants to identify upright and
face configuration, etc.). Thus, most of the social upside-down features is not restricted solely
50 P. Soto-Icaza and P. Billeke

to complete body images. In fact, infants are They observed that infants that were reared by a
capable of identifying upright and upside-down female primary caregiver and familiarized with
faces. Evidence demonstrated that 3-month-olds male photographs preferred the novel female
display a spontaneous visual preference for an stimulus. Meanwhile, those infants who were
upright image of a real face over an upside- familiarized with female photographs did not
down version of the same face (Turati, Valenza, prefer novel male stimulus. Also, and consis-
Leo, & Simion, 2005). This evidence shows that tently with the evidence mentioned above, they
eyes are not enough on their own to attract the found that this female preference occurred only
gaze of infants of this age, because their inter- when the face was upright. On the other hand,
est is modulated by the context in which the when infants are reared by a male primary care-
eyes are located, in this case, the upright face giver, this female preference was reverted. This
configuration. In addition, typically develop- evidence is in accordance with the skill learning
ing children 2–6 months old look more at eyes perspective on human functional brain develop-
than mouths and bodies (Jones & Klin, 2013). ment, in which expertise (training or frequency
Moreover, the trajectory for sensitivity to stimu- of exposure) seems to be the factor that leads
lus that resembles eyes displays an accentuated to specialization (Gauthier, Tarr, Anderson,
increase from 2 to 24 months of age. This evi- Skudlarski, & Gore, 1999).
dence concludes that human social engagement Cerebral evidence of social development in
may be related to visual capacity that is pres- infants derived mostly from electroencephalo-
ent early in development. Nevertheless, there is graphic (EEG) studies. The EEG technique is a
evidence that demonstrated that newborns and noninvasive and temporally accurate measure-
infants not only show special sensitivity to eyes ment of electrical brain activity (Billeci et al.,
or stimuli similar to eyes but also display spe- 2013; de Haan, Pascalis, & Johnson, 2002). As
cial sensitivity to direct eye contact since birth. this technique measures the electrical brain
Newborns 2–5 days old looked significantly lon- activity from scalp electrodes at any age, it
ger at a face that displayed a direct gaze rather becomes a particularly useful methodological
than an averted gaze (Farroni, Csibra, Simion, & tool to study infants and children (de Haan,
Johnson, 2002). More interestingly, the ability Johnson, & Halit, 2007).
to recognize human faces reveals a specializa- An important part of the EEG analysis comes
tion influence (Di Giorgio, Méary, Pascalis, & from the electrical brain activity phase locked to
Simion, 2013; Johnson, 2011; Macchi Cassia, stimulus presentation. This brain response is
Bulf, Quadrelli, & Proietti, 2014; Zieber et al., called event-related potentials (ERPs; Tallon-­
2013). Kelly et al. (2005) showed that newborns Baudry & Bertrand, 1999). Several ERPs have
did not exhibit a preference for neither ethnic been related to social development. For example,
group nor gender when they looked at faces. the N170 component, which is modulated spe-
This can be explained by the lack of exposure cifically by human faces in adults (Courchesne,
to faces in general, since 3-month-old infants Ganz, & Norcia, 1981; Csibra, Kushnerenko, &
demonstrate ethnic (but not gender) preference. Grossmann, 2008; Dawson, Webb, &
Caucasian infants attended more to their own McPartland, 2005; de Haan & Nelson, 1999; de
ethnic faces than any other, evidencing that envi- Haan et al., 2007; Elsabbagh et al., 2009;
ronmental experience during the first 3 months Hileman, Henderson, Mundy, Newell, & Jaime,
could prompt a visual preference. In addition, 2011; Itier, 2004; Johnson et al., 2005). The
Quinn, Yahr, Kuhn, Slater, and Pascalis (2002) N170 component is a negative deflection that
tested the representation of gender of human peaks between 140 and 170 ms after stimulus,
faces in 3- and 4-month-old infants using col- and it is most prominent over posterior temporal
ored photographs of the head and face of men sites (Courchesne et al., 1981; Csibra et al.,
and women. They proved that infants were able 2008; Dawson et al., 2005; de Haan & Nelson,
to discriminate between female and male faces. 1999; de Haan et al., 2002, 2007; Elsabbagh
Developmental Issues 51

et al., 2009; Hileman et al., 2011; Itier, 2004; These evidences are showing that face specificity is
Johnson et al., 2005). developed during childhood from a basic level of
In infants, neuroscience evidence has sug- face versus non-­face discrimination to a level in
gested that the P400 component could be a pre- which faces are grouped by categories like race and
cursor of the N170 component (Luyster, Powell, species and finally individual categories. These
Tager-Flusberg, & Nelson, 2014). The P400 changes might be revealing a trajectory of neural
component is a positive deflection observed and behavioral specialization (Johnson, 2011).
­predominantly on lateral electrodes of the right All the studies reviewed above indicate that
hemisphere. The infant P400 has been observed human beings display a group of sensorial sensi-
over occipitotemporal electrodes elicited by tivities early in life that facilitate the detection of
upright and inverted human and monkey faces social agents and ensure the construction of rela-
(de Haan et al., 2007). More interestingly, it has tionships with other human beings. Indeed,
been suggested that the human face specificity around 6–9 months of age, another visual ability
may experience a cortical specialization during is possible to be observed: the ability of joint
childhood. De Haan et al. (2002) examined this attention (JA). This ability has been described as
hypothesis in adults and 6-month-old infants. In the capacity to alternate the gaze in order to coor-
adults, they observed that upright and inverted dinate the interest in an object and in another per-
human and monkey faces evoked a N170 compo- son (Charman, 2003; Charman et al., 2000;
nent over occipitotemporal electrodes, but the Hopkins & Taglialatela, 2013; Lachat,
N170 elicited by upright human faces was smaller Hugueville, Lemaréchal, Conty, & George, 2012;
than that of the other faces. They also found that Morgan, Maybery, & Durkin, 2003; Mundy,
the amplitude and the latency of the N170 only Card, & Fox, 2000; Striano, Reid, & Hoehl,
increase for inverted human faces and not for 2006). At the age of 6 months old, children are
those of monkey. Moreover, they observed that able to follow the gaze of another person in order
the N170 latency was slower for human faces to perceive a common object, known as respond-
compared to monkeys, regardless of face orienta- ing JA (Mundy & Jarrold, 2010; Mundy, Sullivan,
tion. On the other hand, in 6-month-old infants, & Mastergeorge, 2009; Mundy et al., 2000).
although the amplitude of the N170 (P400) com- Around the age of 9 months old, children are able
ponent was larger for human faces than for mon- to initiate this kind of behavior in a spontaneous
key, orientation did not have significant effects manner (initiating JA; Mundy & Jarrold, 2010;
over both amplitude and latency. This orientation Mundy et al., 2009, Mundy et al., 2000). The
modulation of the N170 component shows that ability of JA has proved to be crucial for several
the specificity of the N170 depends on a cortical capacities such as social synchronization, devel-
specialization during the childhood. opment of language (Hopkins & Taglialatela,
Balas et al. (2010) examined a different level 2013; Lachat et al., 2012; Morgan et al., 2003;
regarding the ability to discriminate faces. They Mundy et al., 2000; Striano et al., 2006), and
assessed the brain activity in 6-month-old infants development of the mentalizing capacity, also
while they were watching face pictures of their known as theory of mind (ToM; Sodian &
mother or an unknown person. They observed that Kristen-Antonow, 2015; Mundy et al., 2000;
the amplitude of P400 was larger to the inverted Charman et al., 2000; Charman, 2003; Morgan
faces only in the case of their mother, revealing a et al., 2003; Striano et al., 2006; Lachat et al.,
specific selection process present as early in life as 2012; Hopkins & Taglialatela, 2013; Happé &
6 months of age. In addition, Farroni et al. (2002) Frith, 2014; Baron-Cohen, Leslie, & Frith, 1985;
measured 4-month-old infants’ brain electric activ- Oberwelland et al., 2016).
ity to assess neural processing of faces with direct Several studies have shown that patients with
and averted gaze. They observed that infants neurodevelopment disorders, for example,
showed an enhanced neural processing of faces autism spectrum disorder (ASD), show early JA
with direct gaze in comparison to averted gaze. and ToM impairments (Caruana, Brock, &
52 P. Soto-Icaza and P. Billeke

Woolgar, 2015; Charman et al., 2000; Jones & blind from birth evidenced similar difficulties in
Klin, 2013; Mundy, Kim, McIntyre, Lerro, & social development as sighted ASD children. In
Jarrold, 2016; O’Nions et al., 2014). Indeed, their study, Hobson and Bishop (2003) observed
there are evidences that demonstrate that infants that some of the blind children exhibit autistic-­
with ASD show alterations in attending social like behaviors such as a tendency to be more
stimuli (Chawarska, Ye, Shic, & Chen, 2016; socially isolated in playground, less propensity to
Jones & Klin, 2013; Klin, Jones, Schultz, express pleasure, and less disposition to play or
Volkmar, & Cohen, 2002). Moreover, ASD chil- be involved in reciprocal play, manifesting a cer-
dren exhibit impairments in both social commu- tain lack of reciprocal interpersonal engagement.
nication and social interaction, as well as However, they observed that there were some
restricted and repetitive patterns of interests, blind children who were less socially impaired.
activities, or behavior that interfere in their The authors concluded that these findings might
global social functioning (APA, 2013). In fact, it highlight the importance of vision in linking chil-
is well known that alterations in the develop- dren with other people but that ASD is a specific
ment of social skills is a key feature of ASD neurodevelopment disorder that is not necessar-
(Caruana et al., 2015; Charman et al., 2000; ily related to blindness.
Jones & Klin, 2013; Mundy et al., 2016; In sum, social functioning and social skills are
O’Nions et al., 2014). the result of a complex interaction of several sen-
Neuroscience studies have illustrated EEG sory abilities that are present in human life from
correlates of JA before 1 year of age (Kopp & birth. All verbal and nonverbal abilities that form
Lindenberger, 2011; Mundy et al., 2000; Striano social skills like smiling and eye contact depend
et al., 2006). One of this is the Nc component, on widespread brain networks whose neurobiol-
which is a negative deflection that occurs around ogy is only recently being studied. The following
300–850 ms after stimulus onset (Kopp & paragraphs describe how the abilities mentioned
Lindenberger, 2011; Luyster et al., 2014; Striano in this part are combined in order to develop the
et al., 2006; Webb, Long, & Nelson, 2005). next human social ability: the capacity to be
Nelson and McCleery (2008) argue that the Nc “mind readers.”
component seems to be the first ERP to emerge in
development (present at birth). In 9-month-old
infants, this component has a higher amplitude in Perspective Taking
the midline channels in JA contexts (Kopp & and Mentalization Development
Lindenberger, 2011; Striano et al., 2006), sug-
gesting that children increase their attention to Are human beings mind readers? How can
environmental stimuli that are more salient humans know what other people are thinking,
(Striano et al., 2006). The Nc component has feeling, or planning without uttering a word? The
shown a right-side lateralization, which is consis- ability to predict how other human being is going
tent with the role of the right hemisphere in the to act is a capacity that is built from the basic
processing of faces (de Haan et al., 2007; Webb behaviors that we described above. Sensitivity to
et al., 2005) and attention (Corbetta, Pate, & eyelike stimuli, discrimination of biological
Schulman, 2008; Courchesne et al., 1981; motion, preference for faces, etc. are the founda-
Pelphrey et al., 2002; Striano et al., 2006). tions on which mentalization ability rests. The
Since the evidence revised above indicates mentalization ability or ToM has been described
that the visual system is essential for the develop- as the capacity to represent and interpret a per-
ment of social skills, we then should find evi- son’s beliefs, intentions, and feelings (Wimmer
dence supporting the idea that people who are & Perner, 1983). This ability can be first observed
born blind should show difficulties in their social around 4-year-olds, and it becomes firmly estab-
development. Hobson and Bishop (2003) lished around 4–6 years of age (Wimmer &
observed that 4- to 8-year-old children who were Perner, 1983).
Developmental Issues 53

Several studies state that the ability to share Moll & Kadipasaoglu, 2013; Southgate, Senju, &
the perception of a common object with another Csibra, 2007; Surian, Caldi, & Sperber, 2007).
person (i.e., JA; see above) is a necessary step for These evidences reveal that social development is
the development of the capacity to figure out a dynamic and progressive process of specializa-
another person’s perspective (i.e., mentalization; tion (Johnson, 2011) far from being an “all or
Sodian & Kristen-Antonow, 2015; Mundy et al., nothing” type.
2000; Charman et al., 2000; Charman, 2003; Social abilities such as VPT and ToM have
Morgan et al., 2003; Striano et al., 2006; Lachat been also a topic of interest for social neurosci-
et al., 2012; Hopkins & Taglialatela, 2013; Soto-­ ence. Most of these findings are based on imag-
Icaza et al., 2015; Happé & Frith, 2014; Baron-­ ing techniques which usually come from
Cohen et al., 1985; Oberwelland et al., 2016). magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) methods,
However, Moll and Kadipasaoglu (2013) argue specifically, the functional MRI (fMRI). This is a
that there is an intermediate ability between the technique that measures changes in the hemody-
development of JA and mentalization. These namic brain response related to neural activity
authors describe it as a level of partial compre- (blood oxygen level-dependent signal; Auer,
hension of other’s purposes, objectives, and pref- 2008). fMRI reveals the organization, distribu-
erences, called perspective taking. This includes tion, and relationship of neural networks which
taking into account the manner in which the other may be anatomically distant but linked in order to
person perceives and understands a stimulus, par- perform a specific function (Rogers, Morgan,
ticularly the visual aspect of such stimulus (visual Newton, & Gore, 2007). Using this method, sev-
perspective taking, VPT). Around 24 months of eral studies have shown that a specific brain
age, the first signs of this skill may appear as the region is involved in VPT and mentalization abil-
child is able to identify if another person can see ities, i.e., temporoparietal junction (TPJ) (Carter
an object or not (Level 1 VPT; Hamilton, & Huettel, 2013; Krall et al., 2016; Oberwelland
Brindley, & Frith, 2009). Nevertheless, there is a et al., 2016; Saxe, Whitfield-gabrieli, Scholz, &
more complex level of VPT, commonly achieved Pelphrey, 2009; Schurz, Aichhorn, Martin, &
at 4 or 5 years of age, in which children display Perner, 2013; Schurz et al., 2015). TPJ is a region
the ability to identify other’s references and per- of the cerebral cortex along the boundaries of
spectives (Hamilton et al., 2009; Moll & temporal and parietal lobes (Carter & Huettel,
Kadipasaoglu, 2013; Perner & Roessler, 2012). 2013). It includes areas of supramarginal gyrus
As a result, Level 2 VPT allows the child to and angular gyrus, and it has been related to a
understand that objects can be seen in different variety of studies in social neuroscience (Billeke
ways, depending on form of presentation and et al., 2015). A meta-analysis of the evidence of
point of view (Moll & Meltzoff, 2011). Thus, social function revealed that left TPJ was acti-
children can identify that if he/she is seeing the vated in perspective tasks (Arora et al., 2015;
front hood of a car, another person in front of Schurz & Tholen, 2016). The precuneus and left
him/her is seeing the rear hood of the same car. middle occipital gyrus were also related to VPT,
Interestingly, Level 2 VPT correlates with the revealing that mental imagery and body represen-
development of mentalization ability (Hamilton tation are necessary to consider different points
et al., 2009). of view (Schurz & Tholen, 2016). Regarding the
The development of mentalization ability was mentalization ability, Saxe et al. (2009) showed
not without controversy. There is a line of in a fMRI study that in children between 6 and
research that has found evidence about the exis- 11 years old, the brain regions involved in per-
tence of what the authors called an implicit ceiving and reasoning about other people were
ToM. This ability has been described during a the bilateral TPJ and the precuneus. An interest-
preverbal stage of development before the aver- ing finding was the fact that the medial prefrontal
age 4–5 years of age in which explicit mentaliza- cortex (MPFC) was also active but in a lower
tion appears (Baillargeon, Scott, & He, 2010; threshold than the other brain regions (Saxe et al.,
54 P. Soto-Icaza and P. Billeke

2009). According to the authors, this evidence from studies in children with language impair-
reflected a developmental change. In fact, they ments, deafness, and ASD. These three develop-
examined changes in the response patterns related ment conditions have demonstrated delays and
to age, which showed that only the right TPJ dis- alterations in the development of mentalization
played a significant correlation with age, reveal- ability, confirming that mentalization is related
ing a maturational selectivity for social to development of language skills (Peterson,
information. Moreover, they observed that the Slaughter, Moore, & Wellman, 2016; Schick, De
brain regions that were involved in ToM process- Villiers, De Villiers, & Hoffmeister, 2007;
ing did not overlap with brain regions devoted to Shield, Pyers, Martin, & Tager-Flusberg, 2016;
the perception of biological motion. In fact, they Spanoudis, 2016). On one hand, ToM impair-
found that the perception of biological motion ments are one of the most reported deficits in
was related to the recruitment of the right poste- ASD patients (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985;
rior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), which Caruana et al., 2015; Charman et al., 2000;
should not be confused with the right TPJ. The Hamilton et al., 2009; Kana, Libero, Hu,
authors concluded that this is a remarkable find- Deshpande, & Colburn, 2014; Mundy et al.,
ing for a full understanding of the social phenom- 2016; O’Nions et al., 2014). Furthermore, neuro-
ena as a developmental outcome, because it imaging studies with adults, adolescents, and
suggests that ToM comprehension may rely on a children have shown that ASD subjects evidence
distinct and later developed neural substrate. a reduced neural response in TPJ and medial pre-
Interestingly, Carter and Huettel (2013) state frontal cortex during social tasks (Castelli, Frith,
that TPJ could be related to mentalizing abilities Happé, & Frith, 2002; Kana et al., 2014;
as well as being responsible for encoding social Lombardo et al., 2011; O’Nions et al., 2014). On
information. This encoding involves thinking the other hand, evidence in school-age children
about another person’s beliefs, the interpretation with specific language impairments showed
of the physical actions of the social agent like poorer results on ToM tasks compared to typi-
gazing, biological motion or facial expressions, cally developing children (Spanoudis, 2016).
and the perception of bodies. They described a This result led the author to conclude that syn-
nexus model of TPJ, in which this brain region tax/pragmatic aspects of language impact on
works precisely as a linkage between social, ToM understanding in children with specific lan-
attention, memory, and language processing net- guage impairment. On the other hand, studies in
works. Specifically, their fMRI meta-analysis deaf children have shown elusive findings.
showed that the brain area near the occipital-­ Evidence in native signer children (deaf children
temporal border, called EBA (extra-striate body with deaf parents) showed no differences in the
area), encodes static biological stimulus, while a age of ToM apparition compared to neurotypical
more dorsal and anterior area (the superior tem- children (Peterson et al., 2016; Schick et al.,
poral sulcus) encodes biological motion, gaze 2007). On the contrary, studies in late signers
detection, and identification of facial expres- (deaf children with hearing parents) have shown
sions. Thus, following this dorsal-anterior axis, a significant delay in the age of development of
the angular gyrus is activated in tasks related to ToM ability (Peterson et al., 2016; Schick et al.,
mentalization and interpretation of intentions. 2007). In addition, other findings have shown
This model is in accordance with the notion that that deaf children have problems in social func-
the development of social skills encompasses a tioning such as social maturity, peer relations,
trajectory of specialization of several abilities and lower popularity in their peer group than
from a basic to a more complex level of expertise typical children (Peterson et al., 2016; for a
and integration. review see Peterson, 2009). However, although
In addition, mentalization ability also has late signer children achieve the same sequence
been related to language development. In this of development and proficiency in mentalization
context, an important source of evidence comes ability as typically developing children, they do
Developmental Issues 55

so later in childhood, thus showing that late sign- choice, following some assumptions (e.g., maxi-
ers make a slower but sustained progress in their mization of the monetary earning; Camerer, 2013;
mentalization understanding as they get older Lee, 2005). Interestingly, in the case of social
(Wellman, Fang, & Peterson, 2011). decision-making, it is also necessary to consider
An interesting study of Shield et al. (2016) the other persons’ earnings or preferences. Thus,
found that native signer children with a ­confirmed using social games, it is possible to infer the sub-
diagnosis of ASD (two of them were typically jective value of another person’s preference. In
hearing children with deaf parents) compared to this context, economic games that involve other
well-matched typically developing deaf native people can be understood as a stylized model of a
signer children fall behind in false-­belief under- social exchange. Specifically, these games recre-
standing tasks. Also, these ASD children scored ate social dilemmas where different interests tend
significantly lower than typically developing to clash. Therefore, several social actions and atti-
native signers in visual perspective taking tasks tudes, such as altruism, prosocial behavior, coop-
and in American Sign Language (ASL) compre- eration, and selfishness, are more prone to emerge
hension. However, this difference does not occur (Axelrod & Hamilton, 1981; Billeke et al., 2014;
in spatial cognition tasks. Regarding mentaliza- Gonzalez-Gadea et al., 2016; Steinbeis et al.,
tion ability, these results illustrate that language 2012). An example of these games is the ultima-
is strongly correlated with ToM ability. Moreover, tum game. In this experimental paradigm, two
they found that mental rotation was unrelated to players, the proposer and the responder, have to
either VPT task or language and is only weakly share an amount of money. The proposer has to
related to ToM task. They observed that the stron- make an offer about how the money should be
gest relationship is present among ASL compre- divided between both players. Only if the
hension, ToM, and VPT tasks, even when VPT responder accepts the proposer’s offer can the
and ToM were assessed with minimal language. money be distributed between the participants.
These findings could indicate that not only But, if the responder rejects the offer, both players
auditory system impairment per se is related to receive no money (Axelrod & Hamilton, 1981).
difficulties in social development but also that the Regarding the development of social decision-­
environment in which children are born is crucial making process, there is evidence that shows that
to a proper development of social skills (Bedny, social strategy changes across ages (Steinbeis
Pascual-Leone, & Saxe, 2009; Peterson et al., et al., 2012). An interesting study assessed both
2016; Shield et al., 2016). behavioral and neural trajectory during child-
hood using the “ultimatum game” and the “dicta-
tor game” (in which the responder must always
 essons from the Economic World:
L accept the offer made by the proposer; Steinbeis
Social Decision-Making et al., 2012). Children from 6 to 14 years old evi-
dence behavioral differences in their bargaining
The development of mentalization abilities allows strategies in the two games. Proposers were more
children to adapt their behavior according to their willing to give money in the ultimatum game,
partner’s intentions in order to accomplish their i.e., only in the case that the proposer’s behavior
objectives. By means of mentalization ability, could be punished by the responder if he/she is
children have the capacity to figure out other’s unsatisfied with the offer. These results evidenced
thoughts, feelings, and purposes in order to plan that children are able to adapt their behaviors
strategies to secure need. The study of choices according to their partners’ preferences.
that children and adults take in games has been Furthermore, the authors found that older chil-
useful to the comprehension of egocentric and dren offer greater amounts in comparison to
altruistic behaviors. In general terms, the partici- younger children. Astonishingly, younger chil-
pant’s choice during economic experiments can dren were more willing to accept unfair offers
be used to infer the subjective value of his/her than older children. These findings are showing
56 P. Soto-Icaza and P. Billeke

the existence of a strategic social behavior that children did not evidence any brain error moni-
integrates both personal interests and the interest toring signal cerebral modulation.
of others. In addition, fMRI results evince that These findings emphasize the fact that sensi-
age is also positively correlated with the activity tivity to social cues is just as important as mate-
and thickness of the dorsolateral prefrontal ­cortex rial cues. Since 1959 Harlow and Zimmerman’s
(DLPFC). Since DLPFC has been associated work about affective responses in infant monkeys
with self-control and strategic decisions, the to Brazelton, Tronick, Adamson, Als, and Wise’s
authors concluded that self-centered decisions (1975) study about early mother-infant reciproc-
might be related to difficulties in self-control ity, psychology and nowadays neuroscience have
rather than the ability to distinguish social norm highlighted the role of social functioning to
(e.g., fairness). human development. Decades of research have
Conditions such as ASD and attention deficit demonstrated that the construction of attachment,
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) could also be bonding, and affiliation are beyond purely mate-
informative about process of social decision-­ rial profits. Behavioral and neural evidence show
making. A recent study of cerebral activity dur- that social cues give meaningful signs to human
ing both a monetary decision and a social beings, providing valuable information to effec-
decision task showed interesting differences tive functioning in society.
among typically developing children, ASD chil-
dren, and ADHD children between 8 and
15 years old (Gonzalez-Gadea et al., 2016). Conclusions
While typically developing children showed
similar cerebral functioning compared to ASD A Specialized Social Brain
children in the monetary decision task, ADHD
children evidenced an atypical cerebral activity. Social skills are built since the beginning of life.
More interestingly, in the social decision task, All the behaviors described above contribute to
typically developing children evince a modula- ensure the interaction with another person from a
tion of their brain error monitoring signals, very early age. The ability to discriminate bio-
showing a greater cerebral response to betrayal logical motion, the ability to imitate, the prefer-
behaviors than to cooperative behaviors. This ence for eyelike stimuli, etc. seem to be
modulation was absent in the ADHD group of coordinated to guarantee, first, that the partner is
children, evincing no differences of their brain actually a living being; secondly, that he/she is a
error monitoring signals between betrayal and human being; and, finally, that the infant could
cooperative behaviors. Remarkably, ASD chil- draw the interlocutor’s attention to him/her.
dren showed a reverse pattern of cerebral modu- Thereby, infants are able to assure their own sur-
lation in this type of tasks. They exhibited a vival. For instance, mutual gaze preference is
greater cerebral response to cooperative behav- important because it informs us that another liv-
iors than to betrayal behaviors. These results ing specie could help us or harm us (Emery,
become more interesting if it is considered that 2000). If we take into account that all of those
betrayal behaviors in social decision task gener- behaviors are aiming to modify other’s actions in
ated more monetary gains to the participant. order to get what infants need to survive, those
Thus, the authors concluded that these findings actions are actually extremely challenging, even
show that typically developing children evince if they seem to be quite simple.
greater neural monitoring signals for non-proso- The evidence reviewed in this chapter allows
cial options such as betrayal although these us to consider that the complexity of social
behaviors bring them a greater monetary gain. development encompasses both a specialization
On the contrary, ASD children showed a cere- and an awareness of social functioning. In this
bral modulation given by their monetary gain context, specialization refers to the refinement of
rather than their social motivation, while ADHD the brain networks that might be directly related
Developmental Issues 57

to increasingly complex behaviors, whereas means of identification of the social agent and the
awareness refers to both the capacity of identify- social environment. These identifications are
ing one’s own thoughts and feelings and the vol- drawn from sensory and motor abilities (“S” and
untarily control of one’s own behavior. We argue “M,” respectively, in Fig. 1). Therefore, sensory
that both specialization and awareness lead to abilities enable the identification of meaningful
the construction of one’s self-identity and the social signs such as biological motion, stimuli
identity of the social partner. Over their lifetime, similar to eyes, upright faces, familiar faces, and
human beings have to deal with gradually more mutual or averted gaze. Likewise, motor abilities
complex social interactions, because social make it possible for the child to communicate
exchanges require the capacity to coordinate and interact with the surrounding environment by
one’s own needs with the needs of others. Take, means of significant social behaviors such as imi-
for example, the ultimatum game (Axelrod & tating, smiling, crying, responding and initiating
Hamilton, 1981). This social dilemma demands JA, pointing, and gaze following.
that the proposer must figure out the responder’s Thus, the increasing complexity of social
outcome. How does he/she predict this? The pro- development might reflect a specialization pro-
poser, in a very short period of time, has to not cess where these sensory and motor capacities
only identify his/her own desires but responder’s are coordinated to predict other’s behaviors. For
interests as well. He/she must then presume and instance, at a behavioral level, the process of face
predict the responder’s possible decision and detection during infancy might be the result of a
inhibit his/her impulse to keep all the money. social specialization that begins with detection of
This process is not so simple. While it requires biological versus non-biological motion; then
the refinement of the mentalization capacity, it with the identification of eyes, upright versus
also entails the coordination of all the abilities upside-down faces, and mutual versus averted
previously developed. Social abilities ranging gaze; and, finally, with familiar versus unfamiliar
from preference to biological motion to social face. Indeed, at a neural level, EEG evidence also
perspective taking are necessary to solve a describes that human face sensitivity might expe-
dilemma such as an ultimatum game. rience a cortical specialization during childhood
Moreover, the increasingly specialized social (e.g., de Haan et al., 2002; Kuefner, de Heering,
behaviors are correlated with a refinement of Jacques, Palmero-Soler, & Rossion, 2010).
brain networks. There is evidence that reveals a Moreover, there is evidence that brain structures
segregation of local connectivity together with an also experienced changes in gray matter volume
increase in the connectivity between distant brain and cortical thickness that can reflect this process
regions during development (Fair et al., 2009), of specialization (Mills, Lalonde, Clasen, Giedd,
showing increases in the brain network organiza- & Blakemore, 2014).
tion (Betzel et al., 2014; Smit et al., 2012; Neurodevelopment dysfunctions such as ASD
Tymofiyeva, Hess, Xu, & Barkovich, 2014). provide valuable evidence for the analysis of
Therefore, developmental changes at both behav- brain specialization trajectory. Specifically, there
ioral and cerebral levels might evince a process are studies that argue that ASD can be under-
of brain functioning specialization aimed to stood as a specialization disturbance (Courchesne
decode social relevant stimuli in order to adapt & Pierce, 2005), showing a reduced long-range
the behavior to a complex social environment functional brain connectivity and an increased
(Fair et al., 2009; Johnson, 2011). This decoding local functional brain connectivity (Courchesne
process could be understood as a construction & Pierce, 2005; Happé & Frith, 2006). In addi-
process of an internal representation from an tion, alterations in early visual ERPs in ASD
external social agent. As we described before (Baruth, Casanova, Sears, & Sokhadze, 2010;
(Soto-Icaza et al., 2015), the human ability to be Hileman et al., 2011) could also reveal a devia-
“mind readers” might be built upon an internal tion in the trajectory of the local circuit
representation of others, primarily established by specialization.
58 P. Soto-Icaza and P. Billeke

A model of social development

Agent detection:
Eye detection
Basic imitation
S Face detection
Low specificity in
Gaze following
brain activity

Agent identification:
Basic coordination
Increased specificity Social perspective taking
in local brain connectivity Joint attention
nternal model of the social agent: S First-order mentalization
Other's attention
Other's preferences
Other's intentions
M
Anticipation of the behaviors
of the agent:
Complex interactions:
Cooperation
Competition
Strategic social behavior

Increased long-range connectivity S


between specialized local cortical regions
Internal model of the social agent:
Other's intentions and other's believes M
Second-order mentalization

Fig. 1  A model of social development. Dotted line repre- agent is shown. On the right, the increasing complexity of
sents the interaction between neural and behavioral devel- the development of social behaviors is shown. Black lines
opment. Note that the gray arrow shows an increasing illustrate the relationship between sensory (S), motor sys-
complexity of such interaction through ages. On the left, tems (M), and cognitive systems (based on Soto-Icaza
the trajectory of the internal cognitive model of the social et al. 2015)

In summary, social development should be The authors report no biomedical financial interest or
understood as a neurodevelopmental phenome- potential conflicts of interest.
non that is modulated by both maturation and
environmental constraints. Furthermore, social
development should take into account which is References
dependent on a developing brain that determines
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
its specialization process. Conditions like ASD and statistical manual of mental disorders, fifth edi-
show that social neurodevelopment is an tion (DSM-5®). Washington, DC: Author.
extremely delicate process and that it sets out vast Arora, A., Weiss, B., Schurz, M., Aichhorn, M.,
possibilities but also offers major challenges. Wieshofer, R., & Perner, J. (2015). Left inferior-­
parietal lobe activity in perspective tasks: Identity
statements. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 360.
Acknowledgments  We thank Miki Soto for proofreading the doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00360
manuscript. This work was supported by Comisión Nacional Auer, D. (2008). Spontaneous low-frequency blood oxy-
de Investigación Científica y Tecnológica CONICYT (Grant genation level-dependent fluctuations and functional
PCHA/DoctoradoNacional/2014-21140043 to Patricia Soto-­ connectivity analysis of the ‘resting’ brain. Magnetic
Icaza and Grant FONDECYT inicio 11140535 to Pablo Resonance Imaging, 26(7), 1055–1064. doi:10.1016/j.
Billeke). mri.2008.05.008
Developmental Issues 59

Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of Carter, R., & Huettel, S. (2013). A nexus model of the tem-
cooperation. Science, 211(4489), 1390–1396. poral–parietal junction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
Baillargeon, R., Scott, R., & He, Z. (2010). False-belief 17(7), 328–336. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.05.007
understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, Caruana, N., Brock, J., & Woolgar, A. (2015). A fronto-
14(3), 110–118. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006 temporoparietal network common to initiating and
Balas, B. J., Nelson, C., Westerlund, A., Vogel-Farley, V., responding to joint attention bids. NeuroImage, 108,
Riggins, T., & Kuefner, D. (2010). Personal familiar- 34–46. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.041
ity influences the processing of upright and inverted Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002).
faces in infants. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms
1. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.001.2010 for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes.
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the Brain, 125(8), 1839–1849.
autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, Charman, T. (2003). Why is joint attention a pivotal skill
37–46. in autism? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal
Baruth, J. M., Casanova, M. F., Sears, L., & Sokhadze, Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 358,
E. (2010). Early-stage visual processing abnormali- 315–324.
ties in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder Charman, T., Baron-Cohen, S., Swettenham, J., Baird,
(ASD). Translational Neuroscience, 1(2), 177–187. G., Cox, A., & Drew, A. (2000). Testing joint atten-
doi:10.2478/v10134-010-0024-9 tion, imitation, and play as infancy precursors to lan-
Bedny, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Saxe, R. (2009). Growing guage and theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15,
up blind does not change the neural bases of theory of 481–498.
mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Chawarska, K., Ye, S., Shic, F., & Chen, L. (2016).
106(27), 11312–11317. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900010106 Multilevel differences in spontaneous social atten-
Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., & Cutting, J. E. (1984). tion in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder.
Infant sensitivity to figural coherence in biomechanical Child Development, 87(2), 543–557. doi:10.1111/
motions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, cdev.12473
37(2), 213–230. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(84)90001-8 Corbetta, M., Pate, G., & Schulman, G. L. (2008). The
Betzel, R., Byrge, L., He, Y., Goñi, J., Zuo, X., & Sporns, reorienting system of the human brain: From envi-
O. (2014). Changes in structural and functional ronment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324.
connectivity among resting-state networks across doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017
the human lifespan. NeuroImage, 102, 345–357. Courchesne, E., Ganz, L., & Norcia, M. (1981). Event-­
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.067 related brain potentials to human faces in infants.
Billeci, L., Sicca, F., Maharatna, K., Apicella, F., Narzisi, Child Development, 52, 804–811.
A., Campatelli, G., & Muratori, F. (2013). On the Courchesne, E., & Pierce, K. (2005). Why the frontal
application of quantitative EEG for characterizing cortex in autism might be talking only to itself: Local
autistic brain: A systematic review. Frontiers in Human over-connectivity but long-distance disconnection.
Neuroscience, 7, 442. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00442 Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15(2), 225–230.
Billeke, P., & Aboitiz, F. (2013). Social cognition in Csibra, G., Kushnerenko, E., & Grossmann, T. (2008).
schizophrenia: From social stimuli processing to Electrophysiological methods in studying infant cog-
social engagement. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4, 1–12. nitive development. In C. A. Nelson & M. Luciana
doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00004 (Eds.), Handbook of developmental cognitive neuro-
Billeke, P., Armijo, A., Castillo, D., López, T., Zamorano, science (pp. 1–50). Cambridge: MIT Press.
F., Cosmelli, D., & Aboitiz, F. (2015). Paradoxical Dawson, G., Webb, S., & McPartland, J. (2005).
expectation: Oscillatory brain activity reveals Understanding the nature of face processing
social interaction impairment in schizophrenia. impairment in autism: Insights from behavioral
Biological Psychiatry, 78, 421–431. doi:10.1016/j. and electrophysiological studies. Developmental
biopsych.2015.02.012 Neuropsychology, 27(3), 403–424.
Billeke, P., Zamorano, F., López, T., Rodriguez, C., de Haan, M., Johnson, M. H., & Halit, H. (2007).
Cosmelli, D., & Aboitiz, F. (2014). Someone has to Development of face-sensitive event-related potentials
give in: Theta oscillations correlate with adaptive during infancy. In M. de Haan (Ed.), Infant EEG and
behavior in social bargaining. Social Cognitive and event-related potentials. New York: Psychology Press.
Affective Neuroscience, 9, 2041–2048. doi:10.1093/ de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. (1999). Brain activity differ-
scan/nsu012 entiates face and object processing in 6-month-old
Brazelton, T. B., Tronick, E., Adamson, L., Als, H., & infants. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1113–1121.
Wise, S. (1975). Early mother-infant reciprocity. In de Haan, M., Pascalis, O., & Johnson, M. (2002).
R. Porter & M. O'Connor (Eds.), Ciba Foundation Specialization of neural mechanisms underlying face
Symposium 33-Parent-Infant Interaction (pp. 137– recognition in human infants. Journal of Cognitive
154). Chichester: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.. Neuroscience, 14(2), 199–209.
Camerer, C. F. (2013). Goals, methods, and progress in neu- Di Giorgio, E., Méary, D., Pascalis, O., & Simion,
roeconomics. Annual Review of Economics, 5(1), 425– F. (2013). The face perception system becomes
455. doi:10.1146/annurev-economics-082012-123040 species-­specific at 3 months: An eye-tracking study.
60 P. Soto-Icaza and P. Billeke

International Journal of Behavioral Development, in the anterior cingulate cortex of chimpanzees (Pan
37(2), 95–99. troglodytes). American Journal of Primatology, 75,
Elsabbagh, M., Volein, A., Csibra, G., Holmboe, K., 441–449. doi:10.1002/ajp.22120
Garwood, H., Tucker, L., … Johnson, M. H. (2009). Itier, R. (2004). N170 or N1? Spatiotemporal differences
Neural correlates of eye gaze processing in the infant between object and face processing using ERPs.
broader autism phenotype. Biological Psychiatry, 65, Cerebral Cortex, 14, 132–142.
31–38. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.034 Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological
Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: The neuroethology, motion and a model for its analysis. Perception, 14,
function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience & 201–211.
Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(6), 581–604. Johnson, M. (2006). Biological motion: A perceptual life
Fair, D., Cohen, A., Power, J., Dosenbach, N., Church, J., detector? Current Biology, 16(10), R376–R377.
Miezin, F., … Petersen, S. E. (2009). Functional brain Johnson, M. (2011). Interactive specialization: A domain-­
networks develop from a “local to distributed” organi- general framework for human functional brain devel-
zation. PLoS Computational Biology, 5. doi:10.1371/ opment? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience,
journal.pcbi.1000381 1(1), 7–21. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2010.07.003
Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. Johnson, M., & de Haan, M. (2015). Developmental
(2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. cognitive neuroscience: An introduction (4th ed.).
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, New York: John Wiley & Sons.
99(14), 9602–9605. Johnson, M., Griffin, R., Csibra, G., Halit, H., Farroni, T.,
Gauthier, I., Tarr, M., Anderson, A. W., Skudlarski, P., de Haan, M., et al. (2005). The emergence of the social
& Gore, J. (1999). Activation of the middle fusiform brain network: Evidence from typical and ­atypical
‘face area’ increases with expertise recognizing novel development. Development and Psychopathology,
objects. Nature Neuroscience, 2(6), 568–573. 17(3), 599–619.
Gonzalez-Gadea, M. L., Sigman, M., Rattazzi, A., Lavin, Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is pres-
C., Rivera-Rei, A., Marino, J., & Ibanez, A. (2016). ent but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later
Neural markers of social and monetary rewards in diagnosed with autism. Nature, 504(7480), 427–431.
children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder doi:10.1038/nature12715
and autism spectrum disorder. Scientific Reports, 6. Kana, R., Libero, L., Hu, C., Deshpande, H., & Colburn,
doi:10.1038/srep30588 J. (2014). Functional brain networks and white mat-
Hamilton, A., Brindley, R., & Frith, U. (2009). Visual ter underlying theory-of-mind in autism. Social
perspective taking impairment in children with Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 98–105.
autistic spectrum disorder. Cognition, 113, 37–44. doi:10.1093/scan/nss106
doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.07.007 Kelly, D., Quinn, P., Slater, A., Lee, K., Gibson, A., Smith,
Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence M., & Pascalis, O. (2005). Three-month-olds, but not
account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spec- newborns, prefer own-race faces. Developmental
trum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Science, 8(6), F31–F36.
Disorders, 36(1), 5–25. Kennedy, D., & Adolphs, R. (2012). Feature review: The
Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2014). Annual research review: social brain in psychiatric and neurological disor-
Towards a developmental neuroscience of atypical ders. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(11), 559–572.
social cognition. Journal of Child Psychology and doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.006
Psychiatry, 55(6), 553–577. Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen,
Harlow, H. F., & Zimmerman, R. (1959). Affectional D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of
responses in the infant monkey; orphaned baby mon- naturalistic social situations as predictors of social
keys develop a strong and persistent attachment to competence in individuals with autism. Archives of
inanimate surrogate mothers. Science, 130(3373), General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816.
421–431. Kopp, F., & Lindenberger, U. (2011). Effects of
Hileman, C., Henderson, H., Mundy, P., Newell, L., & joint attention on long-term memory in 9-month-­
Jaime, M. (2011). Developmental and individual dif- old infants: An event-related potentials study.
ferences on the P1 and N170 ERP components in Developmental Science, 14(4), 660–672.
children with and without autism. Developmental doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01010.x
Neuropsychology, 36(2), 214–236. doi:10.1080/8756 Krall, S., Volz, L., Oberwelland, E., Grefkes, C., Fink, G.,
5641.2010.549870 & Konrad, K. (2016). The right temporoparietal junc-
Hobson, P., & Bishop, M. (2003). The pathogen- tion in attention and social interaction: A transcranial
esis of autism: Insights from congenital blindness. magnetic stimulation study. Human Brain Mapping,
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of 37, 796–807. doi:10.1002/hbm.23068
London B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 335–344. Kuefner, D., de Heering, A., Jacques, C., Palmero-Soler,
Hopkins, W., & Taglialatela, J. (2013). Initiation of joint E., & Rossion, B. (2010). Early visually evoked elec-
attention is associated with morphometric variation trophysiological responses over the human brain (P1,
Developmental Issues 61

N170) show stable patterns of face-sensitivity from 4 and Developmental Disorders, 46(7), 2555–2560.
years to adulthood. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, doi:10.1007/s10803-016-2785-6
3, 67. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.067.2009 Mundy, P., Sullivan, L., & Mastergeorge, A. (2009). A par-
Lachat, F., Hugueville, L., Lemaréchal, J.-D., Conty, L., allel and distributed-processing model of joint atten-
& George, N. (2012). Oscillatory brain correlates of tion, social cognition and autism. Autism Research,
live joint attention: A dual-EEG study. Frontiers in 2(1), 2–21. doi:10.1002/aur.61
Human Neuroscience, 6(156), 1–12. doi:10.3389/ Nelson, C., & McCleery, J. (2008). Use of event-related
fnhum.2012.00156 potentials in the study of typical and atypical devel-
Lee, D. (2005). Neuroeconomics: Making risky choices opment. Journal of the American Academy of Child
in the brain. Nature Neuroscience, 8(9), 1129–1130. and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(11), 1252–1261.
doi:10.1038/nn0905-1129 doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e318185a6d8
Lombardo, M., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E., MRC O’Nions, E., Sebastian, C., McCrory, E., Chantiluke,
AIMS Consortium, & Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). K., Happé, F., & Viding, E. (2014). Neural bases of
Specialization of right temporo-parietal junction for theory of mind in children with autism spectrum dis-
mentalizing and its relation to social impairments in orders and children with conduct problems and callous
autism. NeuroImage, 56(3), 1832–1838. doi:10.1016/j. unemotional traits. Developmental Science, 17(5),
neuroimage.2011.02.067 786–796. doi:10.1111/desc.12167
Luyster, R., Powell, C., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, Oberwelland, E., Schilbach, L., Barisic, I., Krall, S.,
C. (2014). Neural measures of social attention across Vogeley, K., Fink, G., … Schulte-Rüther, M. (2016).
the first years of life: Characterizing typical devel- Look into my eyes: Investigating joint attention using
opment and markers of autism risk. Developmental interactive eye-tracking and fMRI in a ­developmental
Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 131–143. doi:10.1016/j. sample. NeuroImage, 130(15), 248–260. doi:10.1016/j.
dcn.2013.09.006 neuroimage.2016.02.026
Macchi Cassia, V., Bulf, H., Quadrelli, E., & Proietti, V. Pavlova, M., & Sokolov, A. (2000). Orientation speci-
(2014). Age-related face processing bias in infancy: ficity in biological motion perception. Perception &
Evidence of perceptual narrowing for adult faces. Psychophysics, 62(5), 889–899.
Developmental Psychobiology, 56(2), 238–248. Pelphrey, K., Sasson, N., Reznick, J., Paul, G., Goldman,
doi:10.1002/dev.21191 B., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces
Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. (1977). Imitation of facial in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, Disorders, 32(4), 249–261.
198(4312), 75–78. Perner, J., & Roessler, J. (2012). From infants’ to children’s
Mills, K., Lalonde, F., Clasen, L., Giedd, J., & Blakemore, appreciation of belief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
S. (2014). Developmental changes in the structure of 16(10), 519–525. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.004
the social brain in late childhood and adolescence. Peterson, C. (2009). Development of social-cognitive
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), and communication skills in children born deaf.
123–131. doi:10.1093/scan/nss113 Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50(5), 475–483.
Moll, H., & Kadipasaoglu, D. (2013). The primacy doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00750.x
of social over visual perspective-taking. Frontiers Peterson, C., Slaughter, V., Moore, C., & Wellman,
in Human Neuroscience, 7, 558. doi:10.3389/ H. M. (2016). Peer social skills and theory of mind
fnhum.2013.00558 in children with autism, deafness, or typical devel-
Moll, H., & Meltzoff, A. (2011). How does it opment. Developmental Psychology, 52(1), 46–57.
look? Level 2 perspective-taking at 36 months doi:10.1037/a0039833
of age. Child Development, 82, 661–673. Quinn, P., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A., & Pascalis, O.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01571.x (2002). Representation of the gender of human faces
Morgan, B., Maybery, M., & Durkin, K. (2003). Weak by infants: A preference for female. Perception, 31,
central coherence, poor joint attention, and low ver- 1109–1121.
bal ability: Independent deficits in early autism. Rogers, B., Morgan, V., Newton, A., & Gore, J. (2007).
Developmental Psychology, 39(4), 646–656. Assessing functional connectivity in the human brain
Mundy, P., Card, J., & Fox, N. (2000). EEG correlates by fMRI. Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 25(10),
of the development of infant joint attention skills. 1347–1357.
Developmental Psychobiology, 36(4), 325–338. Saxe, R., Whitfield-gabrieli, S., Scholz, J., & Pelphrey,
Mundy, P., & Jarrold, W. (2010). Infant joint attention, neu- K. (2009). Brain regions for perceiving and rea-
ral networks and social cognition. Neural Networks, soning about other people in school-aged chil-
23(8), 985–997. doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2010.08.009 dren. Child Development, 80(4), 1197–1209.
Mundy, P., Kim, K., McIntyre, N., Lerro, L., & Jarrold, doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01325.x
W. (2016). Brief report: Joint attention and informa- Schick, B., De Villiers, P., De Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister,
tion processing in children with higher function- R. (2007). Language and theory of mind: A study of
ing autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism deaf children. Child Development, 78(2), 376–396.
62 P. Soto-Icaza and P. Billeke

Schurz, M., Aichhorn, M., Martin, A., & Perner, J. (2013). Communication Disorders, 61, 83–96. doi:10.1016/j.
Common brain areas engaged in false belief reason- jcomdis.2016.04.003
ing and visual perspective taking: a meta-analysis of Steinbeis, N., Bernhardt, B., & Singer, T. (2012). Impulse
functional brain imaging studies. Frontiers in Human control and underlying functions of the left DLPFC
Neuroscience, 7, 712. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00712 mediate age-related and age-independent individual
Schurz, M., Kronbichler, M., Weissengruber, S., Surtees, differences in strategic social behavior. Neuron, 73(5),
A., Samson, D., & Perner, J. (2015). Clarifying 1040–1051. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.027
the role of theory of mind areas during visual per- Striano, T., Reid, V., & Hoehl, S. (2006). Neural mecha-
spective taking: Issues of spontaneity and domain-­ nisms of joint attention in infancy. European Journal
specificity. NeuroImage, 117, 386–396. doi:10.1016/j. of Neuroscience, 23, 2819–2823.
neuroimage.2015.04.031 Surian, L., Caldi, S., & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution
Schurz, M., & Tholen, M. (2016). What brain imaging did of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological
(not) tell us about the Inferior Frontal Gyrus in theory of Science, 18, 580–586. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.
mind-a commentary on Samson et al.,(2015). Cortex, 01943.x
74, 329–333. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.08.011 Tallon-Baudry, C., & Bertrand, O. (1999). Oscillatory
Shield, A., Pyers, J., Martin, A., & Tager-Flusberg, H. gamma activity in humans and its role in object rep-
(2016). Relations between language and cognition in resentation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 151–162.
native-signing children with autism spectrum disorder. Turati, C., Valenza, E., Leo, I., & Simion, F. (2005).
Autism Research: Official Journal of the International Three-month-olds’ visual preference for faces and its
Society for Autism Research. doi:10.1002/aur.1621 underlying visual processing mechanisms. Journal of
Simion, F., Regolin, L., & Bulf, H. (2008). A predis- Experimental Child Psychology, 90(3), 255–273.
position for biological motion in the newborn baby. Tymofiyeva, O., Hess, C., Xu, D., & Barkovich, J. (2014).
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, Structural MRI connectome in development:
105, 809–813. doi:10.1073/pnas.0707021105 Challenges of the changing brain. The British Journal
Smit, D., Boersma, M., Schnack, H., Micheloyannis, S., of Radiology, 87(1039), 20140086. doi:10.1259/
Boomsma, D., Hulshoff Pol, H., … de Geus, E. (2012). bjr.20140086
The brain matures with stronger functional connectiv- Webb, S., Long, J., & Nelson, C. (2005). Longitudinal
ity and decreased randomness of its network. PLoS development of ERPs. A longitudinal investigation of
One, 7(5), e36896. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0036896 visual event-related potentials in the first year of life.
Sodian, B., & Kristen-Antonow, S. (2015). Declarative Developmental Science, 8(6), 605–616.
joint attention as a foundation of theory of mind. Wellman, H. M., Fang, F., & Peterson, C. C. (2011).
Developmental Psychology, 51(9), 1190–1200. Sequential progressions in a theory-of-mind scale:
doi:10.1037/dev0000039 Longitudinal perspectives. Child Development, 82(3),
Soto-Icaza, P., Aboitiz, F., & Billeke, P. (2015). 780–792. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01583.x
Development of social skills in children: Neural Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs
and behavioral evidence for the elaboration of cog- representation and constraining function of wrong
nitive models. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 333. beliefs in young children’s understanding of decep-
doi:10.3389/fnins.2015.00333 tion. Cognition, 13, 103–128.
Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action Zieber, N., Kangas, A., Hock, A., Hayden, A., Collins, R.,
anticipation through attribution of false belief by Bada, H., & Bhatt, R. S. (2013). Perceptual specializa-
2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18(7), 587–592. tion and configural face processing in infancy. Journal
Spanoudis, G. (2016). Theory of mind and specific lan- of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(3), 625–639.
guage impairment in school-age children. Journal of doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.07.007
Social Competence: Consideration
of Behavioral, Cognitive,
and Emotional Factors

Karen Milligan, Annabel Sibalis, Ashley Morgan,


and Marjory Phillips

same line of reasoning, higher levels of social


Social Competence competence have also been associated with better
career success in the long term (Amdurer,
Social competence is considered an important Boyatzis, Saatcioglu, Smith, & Taylor, 2014).
resilience factor that increases positive develop- Social competence is also identified as a protec-
mental outcomes, even in the face of risk (Reich, tive factor for good mental health (Alduncin,
2016). Friendships are thought to enhance knowl- Huffman, Feldman, & Loe, 2014). It helps us to
edge about social situations, as well as provide develop strong social supports and to work effec-
emotional support, instrumental aid, affection, tively with others. More and more, we live in a
self-validation, companionship, and opportuni- complex and connected world, and the ways in
ties to learn conflict resolution skills in a support- which we connect are increasingly fast paced and
ive environment (Rose-Krasner, 1997). fragmented. The challenges of social media, living
The ability to form and maintain friendships away from extended relatives and familiar commu-
and social relationships is associated with long-­ nities, having to form new social supports, and hav-
term positive outcomes (Rose & Asher, 2017). ing to work with groups of people, all add to the
For example, a positive relationship has been need for high levels of social competence. Social
found between social competence and academic competence mitigates the impact of adverse events,
achievement in school-aged children (Del Prette, such as maltreatment (Schultz, Tharp-Taylor,
Del Prette, de Oliviera, Gresham, & Vance, 2012; Haviland, & Jaycox, 2009). Conversely, low social
Elias & Haynes, 2008; Shek & Yeung, 2016). For competence is associated with negative outcomes,
example positive relationships with peers may including school failure and dropout, alcohol and
promote better problem-solving and peer collab- substance use, social rejection, and delinquency
oration which may positively influence academic (Parker & Asher, 1987). Social competence deficits
outcomes (Del Prette et al. (2012). Along the are associated with lower social supports and
higher risk factor for physical disease (Repetti,
Taylor, & Seeman, 2002).
K. Milligan (*) • A. Sibalis
Department of Psychology, Ryerson University,
Toronto, ON, Canada
e-mail: karen.milligan@psych.ryerson.ca What Is Social Competence?
A. Morgan • M. Phillips
Integra Program, Child Development Institute, While there is agreement about the importance
Toronto, ON, Canada of social competence, what constitutes social

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 63


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_5
64 K. Milligan et al.

competence is less clear, with an abundant array mation from others, introducing oneself, responding
of operational definitions used in the extant lit- to the actions of others), empathy (showing con-
erature (Rantanen, Eriksson, & Nieminen, 2012; cern for another, taking the perspective of
Rose-Krasner, 1997). For example, in her review another), engagement (joining ongoing activities,
of the use of term social competence, Rose-­ making friends, interacting with others), and self-
Krasner (1997) concluded that the key emphasis control (taking turns, compromising, responding
is on positive social outcomes and effectiveness. appropriately to conflict; Lyons, Huber, Carter,
Social competence is defined as “the ability to Chen, & Asmus, 2016). To be considered socially
achieve personal goals in social interaction while competent, one needs to use social skills in a way
simultaneously maintaining positive relation- that adheres to social conventions and that
ships over time and across situations” (Rubin & responds appropriately to others’ emotions and
Rose-Krasnor, 1992, p. 4). Arthur, Bocher, and thoughts (Stichter et al., 2012).
Butterfield (1999), in contrast, took a develop- Each of these social skills can be seen at a
mental approach to the construct and defined behavioral level. Social interactions, however,
social competence as reflecting the evolving are complex and rarely is one enacting a single
understanding of self and other and the ability to social behavior in isolation. One must attend to
form meaningful relationships with peers. and process context cues, as well as verbal and
Gresham (2001) defined social competence as nonverbal cues from social partners. This infor-
the degree to which children and youth are able mation must then be integrated and compared
to establish and maintain satisfactory interper- with previous experiences and knowledge. The
sonal relationships, gain peer acceptance, make child must decide what information is key to
friendships, and terminate negative or pernicious responding, make a plan, draw on their verbal
interpersonal relationships. The importance of and behavioral skill repertoire, and implement.
perceiving and responding appropriately to the This complex cognitive and behavioral process is
emotional components of social interactions was further complicated in the context of strong emo-
highlighted by Halberstadt, Denham, and tion (e.g., fear, anger, excitement), which is often
Dunsmore (2001) in their understanding of present in human interactions. Emotions can
social competence. More recently, the ability to hijack cognitive processes, making it harder to
regulate emotions is considered to be an impor- perspective-take, problem-solve, and behave in a
tant component of social competence (Blair & manner that takes into account all the complex
Raver, 2015). cues of social situations (Zelazo & Lyons, 2012).
Examining all of these definitions suggests As such, there has been movement within the
that there is general agreement that social compe- social competence field away from a social skills
tence reflects more than just learning and carry- perspective that focuses on behavior to an inte-
ing out social skills. Rather, the emphasis is on grative perspective that accounts for the complex
the performance of complex and interconnected interaction of cognitive and emotional processes
skills within interpersonal environments (Lillvist, that support social competence at a behavioral
Sandberg, Bjorck-Akesson, & Granlund, 2009). level (Beauchamp & Anderson, 2010; Milligan,
Attempts have been made to disentangle and Phillips, & Morgan, 2016) (see Fig. 1).
identify the complex and interconnected set of
skills that enables us to navigate social interac-
tions and initiate and maintain relationships with  ognitive Factors and Social
C
others (Stichter, O’Connor, Herzog, Lierheimer, Competence
& McGhee, 2012). These skills are thought to
include: communication (making eye contact, Cognitive factors involved in social competence
taking turns, appropriate tone of voice), can be viewed from two inter-related p­ erspectives.
­cooperation (helping others, sharing materials, At one level, cognition reflects thoughts, includ-
following directions), assertion (requesting infor- ing one’s knowledge of social situations (e.g.,
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 65

Fig. 1 Behavioral,
cognitive, and emotional
factors interact to Behavioral
support development Social skills such as
and enactment of social taking turns, making
competence eye contact, sustaining
a conversation,
negotiating conflict

Cognitive
Knowledge of social Emotional
situations, perspective-
Emotion
taking, attributions,
understanding and
neuropsychological
regulation
cognitive processes

what is expected in terms of behavior, content, expected when they play a board game (e.g., sit
and different roles) and one’s interpretation of down, take turns, follow rules, etc.).
situations (e.g., perspective-taking and attribu- Related to schemas are attributions, cognitive
tions about the cause of events or behaviors). At a processes that reflect a child’s perception of the
more basic level, cognition also embodies neuro- cause or intent of another’s behavior (Weiner,
psychological cognitive abilities, including but 1985). Most of the research on attributions and
not limited to attention, executive functions, pro- social behavior has focused on hostile attributions
cessing speed, and visual-spatial processing. (i.e., the tendency to attribute negative intent in a
Importantly, these two levels of cognition are not benign situation). Hostile attributions are explained
independent, but rather, they interact in a transac- within the context of the social information pro-
tional manner to support social competence cessing model (SIP; Crick & Dodge, 1994). SIP
(Crick & Dodge, 1994; Dodge, 1986; Galway & breaks social problem-solving down in to a series
Metsala, 2011; Gifford-Smith & Rabiner, 2004; of steps which include interpreting cues, clarifying
Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000). goals, generating alternative responses, selecting
and implementing a specific response, and evaluat-
 ognition: Thought Processes Related
C ing the outcome. These steps are executed rapidly
to Social Competence in a non-linear manner that includes numerous
Three areas of cognition that are related to social feedback loops. Importatly, information is pro-
competence are schemas, attributions, and theory cessed within a child social schema, which includes
of mind (ToM). Schemas reflect knowledge about her social experience, as well as her beliefs and
the rules/expectations of social situations. They expectations about social situations. (e.g., Crick &
may be developed based on a child or adoles- Dodge, 1994; Dodge, 1986; Gifford-Smith &
cent’s personal experience or the observation or Rabiner, 2004; Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000).
experiences of others and essentially help chil- Research has consistently shown that children who
dren (and adults) in predicting what will occur in are socially rejected and/or engage in heightened
a given social situation so that they don’t have to levels of aggression are more likely to attribute
experience a situation as novel every time it is hostile or negative intent in benign social situa-
encountered (Kendall, 1985). An example of this tions. Further, they are more likely to experience
would include a child knowing broadly what is challenge in understanding and/or performing the
66 K. Milligan et al.

SIP steps (Dodge & Coie, 1987; Dodge & Feldman, variation found in social competence by false-­belief
1990; Dodge, Murphy, & Buchsbaum, 1984; understanding. However, it is possible that indi-
Dodge & Newman, 1981; Dodge & Tomlin, 1987). vidual differences in the flexible and appropriate
Theory of mind (ToM) is another area of cog- implementation of ToM may play a role in social
nitive understanding that has been linked with competence during this period as well. While less
social competence. ToM reflects the ability to commonly examined, advanced ToM tasks that
infer beliefs, thoughts, and desires (i.e., mental are passed later in childhood (e.g., Liddle &
states) to another person and to recognize that Nettle, 2006) have been associated with social
other’s mental states may differ from one’s own competence outcomes, including the number of
(Milligan, Astington, & Dack, 2007). The rela- friends in a child’s social network (Stiller &
tion between ToM and social competence is well Dunbar, 2007) and teacher-rated social compe-
established (for a review, see Astington, 2003). tence (e.g., Little & Nettle, 2006). This suggests a
The false-belief task is the gold standard task for continued role of ToM for school-aged social
assessing ToM in the preschool period. This task, competence. This is an area in need of further
passed by most children by age 5, assesses a exploration, particularly with neurodevelopmen-
child’s ability to reason about the behavioral tal samples who may present with more chal-
­consequences of holding a mistaken belief. Thus, lenges with ToM.
by age 5 most children can act in a way that
acknowledges that mental representations impact
on what a person says or does, even in cases Neuropsychological Processing
where they are mistaken about the situation in Abilities
reality (Milligan et al., 2007). To become socially
skilled, children must understand that desires and Disorders associated with neuropsychological or
beliefs held by peers influence their behavioral cognitive-executive weaknesses, such as schizo-
and emotional responses (Slomkowski & Dunn, phrenia, specific and nonverbal learning disabilities
1996). This knowledge assists children in under- (Galway & Metsala, 2011; Milligan et al., 2016),
standing the social behavior and verbal commu- autism spectrum disorder (Gates, Kang, & Lerner,
nications of their peers and guides their behavior 2017), and traumatic brain injury (Tlustos et al.,
in social interactions, thereby enabling them 2016), have been associated with greater social com-
to regulate and coordinate their interactions petence challenge. Within these disorders, research
(Astington & Gopnik, 1991; Lalonde & Chandler, has highlighted the key role of processing deficits in
1994). One of the first studies to examine this social competence challenge. While an exhaustive
relation, completed by Astington and Jenkins review of all neuropsychological cognitive processes
(1995), found that children who passed false- involved in social competence is beyond the scope
belief tasks were more likely to make joint pro- of this chapter, we will explore the impact of atten-
posals and to assign roles for themselves and tion control and executive functions, processing
their playmates when engaged in pretend play. speed and visual-spatial processing to exemplify the
Understanding of others’ beliefs has also been impact of processing on social competence.
related to connectedness of communications
between friends (Slomkowski & Dunn, 1996) Attention Control
and successful communication bids and coopera- Attentional control reflects the ability to orient
tive play (Dunn & Cutting, 1999). and sustain attention while filtering out irrelevant
The studies reviewed examine the relation stimuli (Derekshan & Eysenck, 2009). Challenges
between ToM and social competence in the with attention have been associated with
­preschool period. Most typically developing chil- behavioral challenges in social interactions
­
dren have developed false-belief understanding (Andrade, Brodeur, Waschbusch, Stewart, &
by middle childhood, and as such there is less McGee, 2009). Challenges with attentional
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 67

control can impact on learning social skills and & Ozonoff, 1996). The model of EF proposed by
developing one’s knowledge of social situations Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, and
and situational norms. Attentional deficits are Howerter (2000) suggests that the ability to con-
also intricately involved in the relation between trol impulses, respond flexibly (or adjust one’s
attributions and social competence. For example, approach, behavior, attention, or thinking based
research suggests that children who exhibit on feedback from the environment), and keep
aggressive behavior exhibit biased attention information in mind while working with that
toward threat cues. For example, aggressive chil- information (i.e., working memory) are the pri-
dren have difficulty attending to and remember- mary processes within the broad EF construct.
ing all important aspects of a social interaction These EF assist with problem-solving in every-
and encode fewer social cues (with preference for day life and as such are considered pivotal to suc-
those that may be most recent) before making cessful social interaction. Children and
causal attributions about the hostile intent of adolescents with weaknesses in EF experience
another person (Milich & Dodge, 1984). challenge with knowing what social information
Research examining social competence in to focus on, developing plans for social interac-
ADHD populations (where deficits in attention tions, executing their plans, controlling their
control are considered central) exemplifies the behavior in keeping with the social/situational
role of attention in social competence. Children demands, monitoring the success of their behav-
and adolescents with ADHD have fewer friends ior, and flexibly shifting their behavioral approach
and experience higher rates of rejection and based on feedback from peers and the broader
lower levels of social support compared to non- environment (Clark, Prior, & Kinsella, 2002;
ADHD peers (Humphreys, Galán, Tottenham, Dennis, Brotman, Huang, & Gouley, 2007; Nigg,
& Lee, 2016). Quamma, Greenberg, & Kusche, 1999; Riggs,
In addition to improving indices of attention control,
Greenberg, Kusché, & Pentz, 2006).
stimulant medication has been associated with Similar to attention, EF is related to thought
improvements in social functioning at home and processes, such as ToM and hostile attibutions.
school, with notable medium to large effect sizes There is a small to moderate association between
(van der Oord, Prins, Oosterlaan, & Emmelkamp, ToM and EF (d = 0.38, Devine & Hughes, 2016).
2008). Importantly, challenges with social compe- It is possible that EF enables children to attend to
tence are seen in both those with ADHD-inattentive and reflect upon the mental states of others,
and ADHD-combined (inattentive and hyperactive- thereby improving social competence.
impulsive) subtypes, suggesting that the variance in Within typically developing samples, the
social competence is related to inattention and notassociation between EF and social competence
solely due to challenges with hyperactivity/impul- appears to decrease as children age (small
sivity. In fact, research suggests that children with
effect size, Devine, White, Ensor, & Hughes,
ADHD-­inattentive subtype are more likely to show 2016). For example, Harms, Zayas, Metzloff,
deficits in the performance of social competent and Carlson (2014) found that EF at 8 and
behavior (similar to combined type) but experience 12 years was not significantly related to social
even more challenge in the acquisition of social competence as rated by teachers at age 12.
skills, possibly due to the critical role attention plays
However, within populations of children with
in learning (Wheeler & Carlson, 1994). significant EF deficits (e.g., traumatic brain
injury, disruptive behavior disorder), the asso-
Executive Functions ciation appears to be maintained across devel-
Closely associated with attention control are opmental periods. For example, in adolescents
executive functions (EF), which are the higher-­ who have experienced a traumatic brain injury,
order cognitive processes that support purposeful parent ratings of EF were significantly nega-
and effortful goal-directed behaviors (Pennington tively associated with social competence
68 K. Milligan et al.

(Tlustos et al., 2016). Further, ratings of EF Becker & Langberg, 2014), also have been shown
were found to moderate the impact of a social to have lower levels of social competence chal-
competence intervention, suggesting that EF lenge than those without these symptoms (Becker
may be a resilience factor that supports learning & Langberg, 2014). These researches suggest the
and performance of socially competent behav- sluggish cognitive tempo accounts for challenges
iors (Tlustos et al., 2016). in the initiation and working memory (EF), and
EF deficits also appear to moderate the impact of this may be one pathway by which processing
hostile attributions. For example, in their study of speed influences social competence.
83 boys, Ellis, Weiss, and Lochman (2009) found
that boys who presented with both hostile attribu- Visual-Spatial Processing
tions and EF challenges in planning and inhibi- Children and adolescents with visual-spatial pro-
tion exhibited higher rates of reactive aggression cessing deficits may also be more likely to expe-
but that EF challenges alone did not lead to rience challenges with social competence
increased rates of reactive aggression. This (Galway & Metsala, 2011; Petti, Voelker, Shore,
underscores the importance of examining the & Hayman-Abello, 2003). Effective social inter-
interaction of thought processes and neuropsy- actions depend upon the ability to attend to and
chological cognitive factors on social compe- rapidly process and integrate multiple, often sub-
tence, rather than each in isolation. tle, nonverbal social cues, as well as determine
their relative salience. This information assists
Processing Speed individuals in understanding emotional states and
The speed at which children and adolescents intentions of others (Nowicki & Duke, 1994).
­process visual and verbal information also has Research examining children with nonverbal
important implications for social competence learning disabilities (NLD) who present with
(Anderson, 2008). If it takes a child longer to take core weaknesses in visual-spatial processing has
in, process, and respond in a social context, this highlighted that in comparison to a typically
may impact on their ability to follow conversa- developing control group, children with NLD
tions, formulate responses, and be able to deliver encode fewer social cues and have more diffi-
responses in a timely manner. Further, slowed culty detecting and inferring emotion based on
processing may ultimately result in children hav- nonverbal social cues. As such, it is possible that
ing to narrow the field of perception in order to children and adolescents with visual processing
successfully process information, resulting in challenges may become overwhelmed by the
information loss and heightened possibility of amount/type of social information to encode,
social errors. leading to a narrowed focus that may distort
Certainly, research with clinical populations with understanding of a social situation. This may
marked processing speed challenges highlights result in challenges in understanding the
the relation between processing speed and social ­emotional aspects of a situation that require more
competence (e.g., schizophrenia; Bowie et al., inference and integration of information.
2008; traumatic brain injury, Rassovsky et al., Researchers suggest that children with NLD are
2006). Backenson et al. (2015) have highlighted able to generate competent/assertive responses to
that LDs marked by significant processing speed social challenges at levels that are commensurate
challenges have a greater impact on adaptive with typically developing peers; however, they
functioning (including social) than LDs associ- are less likely to believe that enactment of these
ated with working memory or executive func- responses will lead to positive outcomes (Galway
tions. Similarly, adolescents with ADHD marked & Metsala, 2011). It is possible that the genera-
by sluggish cognitive tempo, which ref­lects tion responses, while potentially accurate or
symptoms such as drowsiness, ­
­ daydreaming, competent, may be associated with a sense of
lethargy, and slowed processing speed (e.g., overload or anxiety. This in turn may impact on
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 69

performance of the response and/or attributions Miller et al., 2005; Ornaghi, Grazzani, Cherubin,
of success. Further research is needed into what Conte, & Piralli, 2015). For example, Castro
specific aspects of visual-spatial processing (vs. a et al. (2016) found that emotional knowledge
broad diagnosis such as NLD) impact on the dif- about the experience of emotion across situations
ferent components of the social interaction pro- supported positive social competence outcomes
cess, and how these challenges combine with in Grade 3 students.
other neuropsychological cognitive processes to While understanding emotions in self and
impact social behavior. other provides essential information for social
problem-solving, enacting behaviors and thought
processes associated with social competence is
Emotion Regulation dependent, in part, on emotion regulation.
Emotion regulation is defined as the “extrinsic
Social competence is not just a cognitive and and intrinsic processes responsible for monitor-
behavioral process. Social interactions are emo- ing, evaluating, and modifying emotional reac-
tional by nature, and emotion has the potential to tions, especially in their intensive and temporal
impact on learning social skills, perspective-­ features, to accomplish one’s goals” (Thompson,
taking and problem-solving, and performance of 1994, pp. 27–28). Emotion regulation is associ-
behaviors. Children who are better able to regu- ated with both cognitive processes related to
late their emotions are more likely to experience attributions and perspective-taking, as well as
positive social outcomes, including positive neuropsychological cognitive factors.
engagement with peers, greater acceptance by Emotion regulation is a significant contributor
peers, and a higher quality of friendships (Blair to effective social information processing. In a
& Raver 2015). As such, a comprehensive under- study of 100 Grade 4–6 boys, Bauminger and
standing of social competence must include fac- Kimhi-Kind (2008) found that children with LD
tors relating to emotion understanding and experienced significant challenge with social
emotion regulation, given the central role of emo- information processing, including hostile attribu-
tion in social interaction. tions. Moreover, emotion regulation was found to
Emotion understanding is a broad multidi- moderate the strength of this relation, with those
mensional construct that reflects emotion recog- with emotion regulation challenges experiencing
nition and emotional knowledge (i.e., the ability more social information processing deficits.
to attribute emotions to oneself and others based Emotional regulation is also significantly
on knowledge about emotion-eliciting situa- associated with neuropsychological cognitive
tions), as well as the integration across the skill processes (e.g., attention, language, flexibility,
areas (Castro, Halberstadt, & Garrett-Peters, processing speed, inhibition; see Diamond, 2013
2016). Emotion understanding develops across for review). Certainly, children and adolescents
childhood with emotion recognition skills devel- with neurodevelopmental disorders, such as LDs,
oping in the preschool years and emotion knowl- are at increased risk for emotion regulation
edge developing in the school-age years. More ­deficits (Bauminger & Kimhi-Kind, 2008) and
complex emotion understanding (e.g., mixed associated co-occurring mental disorders
emotions) also develops during the school-age (Milligan, Badali, & Spiroiu, 2015). From a neu-
years as developing cognitive abilities facilitate robiological perspective, the presence of a strong
the ability to analyze, interpret, and integrate emotional response limits a child’s ability to fully
emotional information (see Castro et al., 2016 for engage their cognitive processes and behavioral
review). A well-established base of research skills (e.g., impulse control, cognitive flexibility,
­support exists for the relation between emotion social knowledge, perspective-taking abilities,
understanding and social competence (e.g., social skills; Zelazo & Lyons, 2012). For children
Heinze, Miller, Seifer, Dickstein, & Locke, 2015; and adolescents who already present with
70 K. Milligan et al.

challenges in these areas, emotion may serve to as sharing toys, initiating conversations, listening
magnify these challenges. As such, many chil- quietly when others speak, and promoting help-
dren and adolescents may cope with social chal- ing behavior (e.g., Battistich, Solomon, Watson,
lenge by engaging in fight (e.g., aggression) or Solomon, & Schaps, 1989; Boyle et al., 1999;
flight (e.g., avoidance) behaviors to regulate Ialongo, Poduska, Werthamer, & Kellam, 2001;
strong emotions (Milligan et al., 2015). Further Stanton-­ Chapman, Walker, & Jamison, 2014;
research is needed to better understand the inter- Stevahn, Johnson, Johnson, Oberle, & Wahl,
action between cognitive, behavioral, and emo- 2000). As children age, the content of social
tional factors and the manner in which they competence programs mirrors the advances they
impact on the trajectory of the social interaction are making in terms of cognitive and emotional
process processes (Beelmann, Pfingsten, & Losel, 1994),
as well as the growing complexity and prominent
importance of social interactions (Brown &
Social Competence Interventions Larson, 2009). More specifically, interventions
designed for youth in Grades 5 and higher begin
Given that social competence develops and is fur- to incorporate emotional facets of social compe-
ther refined over the course of childhood and ado- tence. The focus appears to shift from behavioral
lescence, and its well-documented contribution to aspects of social skills to understanding the feel-
resilience, a number of universal social compe- ings experienced by oneself as well as others. In
tence programs have been developed, schools fact, the majority of interventions targeting mid-
being the primary setting in which these interven- dle school- and high school-aged youth in the
tions have been implemented and evaluated. extant literature contain some component reflect-
In this next section of the chapter, we take a ing socio-emotional understanding and self-­
critical look at the extent to which the social presentation, for example, emotion regulation
competence programs in the extant literature tai- (stress management, calming down when frus-
lor their content or delivery of the program to trated, expressing anger appropriately), commu-
behavioral, cognitive, and emotional processes. nicating feelings and desires to others, social
First, we examine interventions for typically assertiveness and resisting peer pressure, and
developing children and adolescents, followed by empathy and perspective-taking (e.g., Caplan
an examination of programs for clinical popula- et al., 1992; Holsen, Smith, & Frey, 2008;
tions with specific challenges in behavioral, cog- Kimber, Sandell, & Bremberg, 2008; O’Hearn &
nitive, and emotional processes. Gatz, 1999; Sarason & Sarason, 1981; Taylor,
Liang, Tracy, Williams, & Seigle, 2002).
One example of a social competence program
 ocial Competence Interventions
S for typically developing children is the Second
for Typically Developing Populations Step program (Committee for Children, 1997),
an in-class, manualized program presented by
Numerous universal social competence interven- classroom teachers; the program is adjustable for
tions have been developed for children and three different age groups: early learning
adolescents without specific cognitive, emo-
­ (­preschool), elementary (Kindergarten to Grade
tional, or behavioral challenges. The content and 5), and middle school (Grades 6–8). Depending
delivery characteristic of programs in the extant on participant age, the program is 22–28 weeks
literature appear to be moderated by age. Social in length, with 20–40 min lessons. Across all age
competence interventions designed for children groups, participants are presented with four core
ages 10 and under (including those for preschool- units: skills for learning (listening, focusing
age and kindergarten-age children) nearly attention, self-talk, being assertive), empathy
­exclusively focus content at a behavioral level, (identifying feelings, showing care and compassion,
highlighting simple, physical social actions such helping others), emotion management (managing
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 71

anxiety, disappointment, and anger), and prob- there is little emphasis on neuropsychological
lem-solving (playing fairly, thinking of solutions, cognitive processes. These processes are impor-
taking responsibility). Specific content is adjusted tant to consider given their role in learning
for age and level of cognitive ability of partici- (Milligan et al., 2015). In particular, executive
pants, with attention to what would be develop- functions are still under development throughout
mentally appropriate or salient at a given age. For the childhood and adolescent periods, and social
example, in the emotion management unit, pre- competence groups that tailor content and deliv-
schoolers discuss managing waiting, while Grade ery to the specific level of EF within a class may
5 students address avoiding making assumptions. be more successful in enhancing social compe-
Additional units addressing bullying prevention, tence. It is also important to recognize that uni-
substance abuse prevention, and goal setting are versal programs may be associated with smaller
added to the program beginning in Grade 6. effect sizes because many of the students may
There is support for the broad benefit of social already possess appropriate levels of compe-
competence interventions for typically develop- tence, leaving little room for improvement on
ing youth, across age groups. In a meta-analysis outcome measures. Regardless, it may be benefi-
of 213 studies examining social competence cial to explore if pre-intervention EF (e.g., work-
interventions for typically developing children ing memory, impulse control, flexibility)
completed between 1955 and 2007, Durlak, moderates the impact of social competence pro-
Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor, and Schellinger grams. If differences do exist, future research that
(2011) found that participation in interventions informs tailoring of social competence program
led to moderate improvement in social and emo- curriculum to support the development of these
tional skills (d = 0.57), as rated by participants executive functions or accommodate for weak-
themselves, their parents, or their teachers, as nesses in executive functions may impact on
well as small improvements in self-esteem and social competence may improve the strength of
self-efficacy (d = 0.23) and small improvements the observed effect.
in level of positive social behaviors such as coop-
eration with peers (d = 0.24; Durlak et al., 2011).
In addition, participants showed a small reduc-  ocial Competence Interventions
S
tion in conduct problems (d = 0.22) and reduced for Neurodevelopmental Disorders
emotional distress (d = 0.24; Durlak et al., 2011).
Intervention participation was also predictive of The most common neurodevelopmental disor-
small improvements in academic performance ders for which social competence interventions
(d = 0.27; Durlak et al., 2011). More recent stud- have been developed are autism spectrum disor-
ies replicate these results. Training in social com- der (ASD), ADHD and LDs. Review of this lit-
petence has led to more positive social interactions erature suggests that both content and program
with peers, as rated by children and their teach- delivery attend more explicitly to behavioral,
ers, as well as improvements in self-esteem, cognitive, and emotional factors associated with
decreases in internalizing and externalizing social competence, with the specific focus
­problems, and, for younger intervention partici- depending on the central deficits associated with
pants, increases in social initiations and coopera- each disorder. For example, social deficits are
tive play (Holsen et al., 2008; Kimber et al., central to the diagnosis of ASD. Children with
2008; Stanton-Chapman et al., 2014). ASD have difficulties experiencing and display-
While research supports the benefit of social ing empathy and engaging in reciprocal social
competence programs, effect sizes are small. interactions (APA, 2013). Often, these difficul-
While social skills are a focus across childhood ties lead to a lack of behaviors necessary to build
and adolescence, and emotion understanding and and maintain social interactions, such as main-
regulation, as well as cognitive-perspective, tak- taining eye contact, displaying engaged or wel-
ing appear to be more of a focus after age 10, coming body language, responding to direct or
72 K. Milligan et al.

indirect social advances, and engaging in coop- A similar program, Social Competence
erative play and activities. Additionally, children Intervention (SCI; Stichter et al., 2010), was devel-
with ASD can display restrictive and repetitive oped for children and adolescents with ASD and
behaviors and interests—for example, repetitive includes three separate curricula specified for chil-
motor movements such as hand flapping or obses- dren (ages 6–10), adolescents (ages 11–14), and
sion with cars—and tend to be inflexible regard- high school students (ages 14–18). All programs
ing changes to established routines (APA, 2013). are 10 weeks (1 h/week) in length and school
Such behaviors or obsessive interests can be seen based. The adolescent curriculum focuses on rec-
as confusing, frightening, or off-putting by peers ognizing facial expressions, communication skills
(Swaim & Morgan, 2001). Further, comorbid such as eye contact and nonverbal cues, turn-tak-
social anxiety is highly prevalent, affecting up to ing in conversation, recognizing emotions in one-
84% of children with ASD (White et al., 2009) self and others, stress and anxiety management,
and further impairing children from engaging in and problem-solving. As in SCEP, SCI is adapted
social situations. to suit the needs of ASD participants, using small
Review of the social competence programs group sizes (maximum six participants/ group) to
designed for ASD suggests that both the content avoid overstimulation and minimize social anxiety
and the method of delivery take into account and adhering to a strictly structured lesson format
behavioral, cognitive, and emotional processes that always begins with the practice of acknowl-
involved in learning and performing socially edging, greeting, and making eye contact with all
competent behavior. One exemplar intervention participants. A study of 27 SCI participants
is the Social Competence Enhancement Program showed that participation was associated with
(SCEP; Cotugno, 2009) for elementary school-­ improvements in parent-­ rated social skills and
aged children with ASD. This 30-week (1 h/ executive functioning, and improved performance
week) program focuses on. on measures of facial expression recognition and
eye contact and gaze sharing with others ToM (Stichter et al., 2010).
(behavioral), social initiations and social respond- ADHD is associated with a different profile of
ing (behavioral), joint attention with others neuropsychological cognitive challenge. Children
(cognitive-­attention), and flexibility and transi- often have difficulty remaining focused on the
tioning between thoughts and activities task at hand or understanding and sticking with
(cognitive-­ executive functioning). Anxiety and difficult tasks or problems (e.g., playing a com-
stress management strategies, such as visualiza- plex game, engaging in school group projects),
tion and breathing (emotion regulation), are also which often impairs cooperative work and play
integrated. Similar to other social competence with peers (Wehmeier, Schacht, & Barkley,
programs, program delivery capitalizes on mul- 2010). Due to distractibility and/or hyperactivity,
tiple instruction methods, including didactic children with ADHD often have difficulty ­waiting
instruction, discussion, modeling, and peer-based their turn in conversation or acknowledging a
practice. However, it is adapted to the neuropsy- peer’s thoughts and ideas, which can hinder con-
chological processing profiles of children with versations or budding friendships (Wehmeier
ASD, with particular attention to challenges with et al., 2010). Finally, children with ADHD may
the executive function of cognitive flexibility. For be prone to outbursts of frustration (APA, 2013),
example, (1) all sessions follow a strictly consis- which may alienate peers.
tent outline in order to accommodate need for Similar to ASD, the social competence inter-
predictable routines, (2) acknowledge and ventions for ADHD for children 6–12 tailor the
­anticipate transitional difficulty when changing content and the delivery of the program to the
activity to the next; setting aside time between behavioral, cognitive, and emotional processes of
tasks and providing transitional support, and (3) social competence. For example, the Therapeutic
predictability is increased by pre-teaching activi- Summer Day Camp for Children with ADHD
ties and breaking them down into simpler steps. (Hantson et al., 2012) is a 2-week social skills
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 73

training program offered in the milieu of a sum- program that aims to advance social competence
mer day camp that aims to increase understand- in children with LDs by tailoring programming to
ing and labeling feelings, emotional self-control, individual strengths and needs in cognitive, emo-
and positive approaches to deal with anger and tional and behavioral processes that support social
frustration (e.g., response to teasing and avoid- competence. The program will be outlined here as
ance of verbal and physical confrontations). a model for social competence programming that
Specific skills addressed include introducing successfully integrates emotional, cognitive, and
oneself (behavioral), joining social situations processing facets of social competence.
(behavioral), anger management (emotional),
and using self-control (emotional/cognitive-­
executive functioning). Program delivery is tai- Integra Social ACES Program:
lored to provide a mix of active and calm activities A Social Competence Intervention
in order to keep children engaged and introduce for LD
and practice skills across domains. Concurrent
parent training is provided to support generaliza- While we often think of LDs in the context of
tion to home (e.g., effective praise and rewards, academic achievement, challenges experienced
providing a structured day schedule, building a by children and adolescents extend beyond the
positive parent-child relationship). Participation classroom, with approximately 75% of students
in the program has been shown to be associated with LDs having lower levels of social compe-
with parent-rated improvements in peer relations, tence than typically developing children, as
as well as behavioral and emotional problems assessed by teachers, peers, and children them-
(Hantson et al., 2012). selves (Forness & Kavale, 1996). Further,
Meta-analyses examining the impact of social approximately 50% of children with LDs are
competence interventions for neurodevelopmen- rejected, neglected, or victimized by peers
tal disorders suggest that the strength of the (Baumeister, Storch, & Geffken, 2008; Mishna,
observed effect is small (d = 0.199, small effect 2003), and many have impoverished and unstable
size, Quinn, Kavale, Mathur, Rutherford, & friendships (Wiener & Schneider, 2002; Wiener
Forness, 1999; PND = 69%, low or questionable & Sunohara, 1998).
effectiveness, Bellini, Peters, Benner, & Hopf, While there is considerable current debate
2007). Despite multiple researchers noting the how LDs should be defined, we will use the con-
need for intervention programs to specifically sensus definition of the LDAO (2001), which
cater to the neuropsychological deficits of a disor- defines LDs as a disorder that (1) affects how
der (Cragar & Horvath, 2003; Rao, Beidel, & individuals acquire, understand, retain, or
Murray, 2008), this appears to be inconsistently ­ organize information; (2) resulting in specific
put into practice. While certain interventions may rather than global deficits in individuals with
address the global deficits of the population they average to above average intelligence; and (3)
seek to serve (e.g., eye contact in children with result from impairments in one or more psycho-
ASD), most interventions do not take into account logical processes related to learning (e.g., lan-
the specific needs of the subgroup attending the guage processing, visual-spatial skills, processing
intervention, and how this subgroup’s abilities speed, memory, and attention).
and deficits may vary slightly from the disorder as
a whole (Cragar & Horvath, 2003; Rao et al.,
2008). Researchers propose that by tailoring Overview of Program
interventions more specifically to the participants
attending them, interventions may have a greater The Integra Social ACES (Awareness, Competence,
positive impact (Attwood, 2000; Rao et al., 2008). Engagement and Skills) Program is a strength-
The Integra Social ACES (Awareness, based, client-centered, and experiential program
Competence, Engagement, Skills) Program is one intended to provide children and youth with LDs
74 K. Milligan et al.

with a positive social experience and increase their positively or negatively affect the group process
social competence. Unlike many manualized social and opportunities for learning for the children in
skills treatment programs, the Integra Social ACES the group. For example, a child who needs to work
Program tailors the curriculum to the child’s and on basic social competence, such as turn-­taking,
group’s treatment goals and takes an individualized eye contact, and basic conversational skills, may
approach in terms of flexibility of content, thera- be placed with other children with similar social
peutic stance, and group matching (Integra competence treatment goals. The level of self-reg-
Program, 2016). A key component of the Integra ulation may help to determine the pacing and
Social ACES Program is the tailoring of group nature of the group activities. For instance, chil-
activities to accommodate group participants’ neu- dren with low levels of regulation may need a
ropsychological processing deficits. This is accom- faster pace of activities and less talking and pro-
plished through careful group matching and cessing of the activities in order to sustain their
informed by a review of each participant’s learning attention and focus. Groups vary in size from three
profile (based on a comprehensive psychoeduca- to eight children or youth and are matched accord-
tional assessment). ing to age, developmental stage, and gender.

Group Matching Group Content

Through a multisource assessment informed by the One of the key features of the Integra Social
child’s psychological assessment report, clinical ACES Program is that there is less of an emphasis
observations of the child in an assessment group, as on teaching social skills in a didactic manner.
well as clinician and parent report, a child’s learn- Rather, the program content largely consists of
ing profile, self-regulation, and emotion regulation games and activities (e.g., tabletop games, drama
skills, in addition to their baseline level of social activities, teamwork-based activities) that provide
competence, are taken into account. Children are naturalistic and engaging opportunities for par-
categorized into group profiles on the basis of clini- ticipants to practice their skills. Children learn
cian ratings of social competence and emotion from each other and facilitated and directly
regulation, further delineated by age and gender, to coached by adult facilitators. This encourages
ensure that children with compatible goals are children to approach social situations that they
placed together and to provide a framework for tai- may normally avoid and to learn to manage the
loring group activities and lessons (see Table 1). associated emotion. In addition, the games and
Children are then matched carefully according activities allow for “in the moment” teaching
to their individual treatment goals with consider- opportunities, group discussion of the skills
ation of each child’s self-regulation and emotion learned to “real-world” situations, and direct
regulation abilities and how these abilities may modeling and coaching by the group facilitators.

Table 1  Group matching by social competence and emotion regulation


Social competence
Low Medium High
Regulation Low Low social competence/ Medium social High social competence/
low regulation competence/low regulation low regulation
High Low social competence/ Medium social High social competence/
high regulation competence/high regulation high regulation
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 75

 ample Group Session: Skills,


S speed, group leaders will ensure that each child
Information Processing Deficits, has an opportunity to engage in the movement by
and Accommodations adjusting the pace of the check-in. A feelings
check-in often involves having the participants
Given that each child brings to the group a unique discuss their current feeling state and briefly
set of social competence strengths and needs explain their choice to the group. Feelings check-
across behavioral, cognitive, and emotional ins are often adapted by having a visual c­ omponent
areas, treatment goals differ by group, and no two that includes a card with a picture of an animal and
groups are structured in the same exact manner. an associated feeling label (e.g., a bear is associ-
However, the groups follow a general structure ated with irritable). This accommodation supports
with the common elements of a form of check-in participants who may learn and express them-
and a time for “snack and chat” at the end of the selves best with visual rather than verbal informa-
group. The specific group activities that make up tion. Feelings check-ins promote emotional
the content of the group are based on several fac- awareness and conversation skills, including visual
tors including, but not limited to, individual par- tracking, sharing about oneself, the opportunity
ticipant treatment goals, group treatment goals, for group participants to ask follow-up questions,
stage of treatment, and progression toward goals. and opportunities to demonstrate empathy.
Decisions regarding the specific content of each Throughout a verbally based check-in, group lead-
group session also account for the participants’ ers will scaffold for group members how to show
specific information processing deficits. For good listening skills and how to ask follow-­up
example, all group sessions begin with an over- questions to demonstrate appropriate listening
view of the group agenda, as well as a visual skills and may provide direct feedback regarding
schedule to accommodate participants with making eye contact for participants who struggle
memory difficulties, such that they know the plan with eye contact, for example.
for the group and can refer back to the schedule The following content activities are examples
to know what is coming next. Providing a visual of activities that could be used to target skills such
schedule also helps to support group members as taking turns, compromising, and cooperation.
who struggle with transitions as they know what
to expect and what is expected of them. Squiggle Game
The Squiggle Game involves having the group
Group Check-In members draw a simple squiggle on a piece of
Most group sessions begin with an active check-­in paper and passing the paper to someone else.
or a feelings check-in as a way to ground and The next group member will turn the squiggle
reconnect the participants since their last group into a drawing, while the original participant
session together. An active check-in involves hav- who drew the squiggle has to watch their squig-
ing the group participants demonstrate a particular gle ­transform into something new. The children
movement with their bodies, while the other group who are not involved in drawing are encouraged
members mirror the movement. This type of to ask questions and show an interest in the
check-in works best for children with self-regula- drawing. For the child who is drawing, visual-
tion difficulties who would benefit from having an motor integration difficulties may make this
opportunity to release excess energy from their activity particularly challenging. To accommo-
bodies, allowing them to experience improved date for visual-motor integration difficulties,
self-regulation during the rest of the group. An group leaders may provide suggestions for how
active check-in also encourages the group to tune to turn the squiggle into a drawing or may subtly
in to the participant leading the movement, which provide a concrete example to assist the child in
involves visual tracking and shifting one’s atten- visualizing a ­potential drawing. For the group
tion to the participant, important skills in social member who originally drew the squiggle, exec-
interactions. For children with slower processing utive functioning difficulties may impact on
76 K. Milligan et al.

their ability to regulate their reaction and shift tion, and complimenting. Depending on the level
their expectations. Group leaders may provide of social competence of the group members,
verbal feedback to the group member who is more or less scaffolding is provided by the group
having a hard time seeing their squiggle turned leaders during snack and chat. For example, a
into something unexpected by labeling their group with overall low levels of social compe-
feelings and praising them for regulating their tence may require more explicit direction, model-
emotional response. ing, and coaching to practice asking questions of
one another to keep the conversation going. Over
 hange the Room
C time, group participants build their skills in this
In this activity, one group member will leave the area with the highest level of social competence
room, while the remaining group members being a conversation that begins and is main-
change three things about the room. The group tained with minimal facilitation on the part of the
member who left the room has a few guesses to group leaders. In addition to explicit instruction
figure out what is different upon re-entering the on how to maintain conversation, as well as
room. This game fosters compromise, negotia- opportunities to role play these skills and practice
tion skills, and cooperation and involves visual them in a naturalistic context, group leaders will
attention to detail. A common accommodation accommodate for memory difficulties, slow pro-
for the participant who is guessing which changes cessing speed, and executive function difficulties
have been made is that the rest of the group mem- by moderating the pace of the conversation, pro-
bers will indicate “hot” or “cold” if they are get- viding scaffolding to group members, and adjust-
ting closer to the vicinity of the change. This ing their tailored feedback according to the group
accommodation is only provided with the per- member’s level of difficulty with the skill. See
mission of the guessing participant. Group mem- Table  2 for a summary of the skills targeted
bers are instructed to discuss each change with related to the activity, the information processing
one another and to ensure that all group members deficits that may interfere with the activity, and
contribute and approve each change, thereby pro- the accommodations that are often provided in
moting the skills of negotiation, cooperation, and the Social ACES Program.
compromise. Children with executive function- Another key component of the Social ACES
ing difficulties may struggle with the emphasis Program is its focus on self-regulation and emo-
on compromise involved in this activity as a tion regulation, and how these affect the acquisi-
result of their difficulty shifting. To accommo- tion and development of social skills. Children
date for this executive functioning difficulty, with self-regulation difficulties may struggle
group leaders will have introduced the skill of with monitoring and controlling their energy
compromising prior to this activity and provided level, maintaining focused attention during
them with opportunities to practice this skill. social interactions, or providing conversational
Group leaders will provide immediate and direct space for others to participate, for example. A
feedback during the negotiation part of this activ- child’s regulation may significantly impact their
ity to support children who have executive func- ability to actively participate in group process
tioning difficulties. and may impact their social interactions. Due to
difficulties with self-regulation, a child’s ability
 nack and Chat
S to attend to and follow conversation may be
Each group session ends with “snack and chat,” a affected. For children who have some social
structured time during which the group members competence yet who struggle with self-regula-
are supported to practice their conversational tion, their difficulties may impede performance
skills while having a snack. Specific skills tar- of their social skills. Often, a focus of interven-
geted during the snack and chat include asking tion for these children is on improved awareness
on-topic questions, sharing about oneself, mak- of self and others, as well as monitoring their
ing on-topic comments to build on the conversa- self-regulation.
Table 2  Sample activity and associated skills and neuropsychological cognitive processes and accommodations
Neuropsychological cognitive processes
Activity Skills targeted involved Accommodations
Feelings check-in • Emotional awareness • Language processing • Modeling from group leaders (sharing their internal feeling
• Conversation skills • Visual-spatial processing state as well as asking follow-up questions to engage others in
• Processing speed conversation)
• Visual images to represent and match feelings listed on cards
• Allowing enough time for group members to respond (i.e.,
group leaders moderate pace of turn-taking)
Squiggle game • Communication • Language processing • Visual activity for children who may have difficulty with
• Creativity • Executive functioning (e.g., shifting) language-based activities
• Expression through art • Visual-motor integration skills • Scaffolding by group leaders to support group members to
• Adapting to change and “let it go”/adapt to unexpected changes
compromising • Repetition of instructions related to activity provided to group
members
• Check-in with group members to ensure their understanding
• Extra time (related to visual-motor integration difficulties)
Change the room • Negotiation • Language processing • Scaffolding by group leader to support group members to ask
• Compromise • Processing speed each other questions and tune in to others
• Cooperation • Executive functioning • Ensuring that each group has a chance to contribute their
• Memory ideas
• Attention • Group leaders ensuring enough time for group member who
• ToM is guessing the changes
• Repetition of rules to reduce memory demands
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors

• Didactic lesson on what it means to compromise


Snack and chat • Conversation skills, including • Language processing • Didactic lesson related to how to keep a conversation going
asking on-topic questions • Processing speed • Role plays to allow group participants to practice the skills
• Sharing about oneself • Memory • Modeling from group leaders
• Making on-topic comments to • Attention • Scaffolding (i.e., group leader asks, “does anyone have any
build on the conversation • Executive functioning skills questions about that?”)
• Complimenting
77
78 K. Milligan et al.

Similarly, for children with higher levels of ment in social competence. Effect sizes ranged
social competence and low levels of self-­ from d = 0.40–0.59, which reflects larger effects
regulation, a focus of intervention is on improv- than seen in previous research (Quinn et al., 1999)
ing their awareness of the impact of their actions and effects that approach or are medium in strength.
on others and reducing silliness. To address dif- Qualitative interviews with parents, children, and
ficulties with self-regulation, the Social ACES teachers suggested improvements in social self-­
Program uses a tool called the silly-serious scale. concept, initiation, and emotion regulation.
The goal of introducing the silly-serious scale is Tailoring treatment to the child’s information pro-
such that group members will learn that different cessing and emotion regulation abilities, as well as
activities and situations require different levels of “in the moment” feedback, was reported to support
silliness or seriousness and develop the skills to gains made and contributed to participants having a
self-monitor and adjust their behavioral output positive social experience.
accordingly. In introducing the silly-serious
scale, group leaders will elicit from the group
participants what are acceptable energy levels for Conclusion
particular activities (e.g., watching television
requires a relatively calm energy level, while This chapter highlights the complexity of social
playing outside at recess can involve more silli- competence, both in terms of its development, as
ness and less regulation). Once this tool has been well as its enactment. We have provided a
introduced in the context of a group, it is referred ­possible framework for understanding the inter-
to throughout the group so that the group partici- action of behavioral, cognitive, and emotional
pants gradually build their awareness related to factors in social competence. Review of the
their energy level and its impact on others. extant literature suggests that cognitive and emo-
In addition to difficulties with behavioral self-­ tional factors are not consistently attended to in
regulation, children with LDs often have difficulty the curriculum or delivery of social competence
managing and regulating emotional reactivity due interventions and that differences in targeting
to their executive functioning deficits. Their diffi- these factors may depend, at least in part, on the
culties with emotion regulation may impact them age and clinical characteristics of the group the
socially as they are more likely to struggle with intervention is designed for. Effect sizes for
managing their reactions to others and perspective- social competence are small. It is possible that
taking, for example. The Social ACES Program attending to behavioral, cognitive, and emotional
pays particular attention to children who demon- factors in our interventions, with flexibility to
strate rigidity and low frustration tolerance as these individualize to participants in groups (as is done
characteristics can significantly impact on a child’s in the Integra Social ACES program), may
ability to engage in and benefit from the interven- enhance the effectiveness of our social compe-
tion. For example, children who exhibit extreme tence interventions.
rigidity regarding rules of a game or the concept of
fairness will benefit most from opportunities to
interact with peers who model flexibility and who References
will be tolerant of the group member’s rigidity.
Evaluation research of the Integra Social ACES Alduncin, N., Huffman, L. C., Feldman, H. M., & Loe,
I. M. (2014). Executive function is associated with social
programs attests to its promise in enhancing the competence in preschool-aged children born preterm or
social skills of children with LDs and co-occurring full term. Early Human Development, 90, 299–306.
mental health issues (Milligan et al., 2016). The Amdurer, E., Boyatzis, R. E., Saatcioglu, A., Smith,
program was associated with significant gains in M. L., & Taylor, S. N. (2014). Long term impact of
emotional, social and cognitive intelligence compe-
initiation and engagement in positive social inter- tencies and GMAT on career and life satisfaction and
actions, foundational skills that support improve- career success. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–15.
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 79

American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic Beelmann, A., Pfingsten, U., & Losel, F. (1994). Effects
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). of training social competence in children – A meta-­
Washington, DC: Author. analysis of recent evaluation studies. Journal of
Anderson, P. J. (2008). Towards a developmen- Clinical Child Psychology, 23(3), 260–271.
tal model of executive function. In V. Anderson, Bellini, S., Peters, J. K., Benner, L., & Hopf, A. (2007).
R. Jacobs, & P. J. Anderson (Eds.), Executive func- A meta-analysis of school-based social skills inter-
tions and the frontal lobes (pp. 3–21). New York, ventions for children with autism spectrum disorders.
NY: Psychology Press. Remedial and Special Education, 28(3), 153–162.
Andrade, B. F., Brodeur, D. A., Waschbusch, D. A., Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2015). School readiness and
Stewart, S. H., & McGee, R. (2009). Selective and self-regulation: A developmental psychobiological
sustained attention as predictors of social problems in approach. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 711–731.
children with typical and disordered attention abilities. Bowie, C. R., Leung, W. W., Reichenberg, A., McClure,
Journal of Attention Disorders, 12, 341–352. M. M., Patterson, T. L., Heaton, R. K., & Harvey,
Arthur, M., Bocher, S., & Butterfield, N. (1999). P. D. (2008). Predicting schizophrenia patients’ real
Enhancing peer interactions within the context of play. world behavior with specific neuropsychological and
International Journal of Disability, Development and functional capacity measures. Biological Psychiatry,
Education, 46(3), 367–381. 63(5), 505–511.
Astington, J. W. (2003). Sometimes necessary, never Boyle, M. H., Cunningham, C. E., Heale, J., Hundert, J.,
sufficient: False-belief understanding and social MacDonald, J., Offord, D. R., et al. (1999). Helping
competence. In B. Repacholi & V. Slaughter (Eds.), children adjust – A tri-ministry study: I. Evaluation
Individual differences in theory of mind: Implications methodology. Journal of Child Psychology and
for typical and atypical development (pp. 13–38). Psychiatry, 40(7), 1051–1060.
New York, NY: Psychology Press. Brown, B. B., & Larson, J. (2009). Peer relationships in
Astington, J. W., & Gopnik, A. (1991). Theoretical expla- adolescence. In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.),
nations of children’s understanding of the mind. British Handbook of adolescent psychology (3rd ed.pp.
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 7–31. 74–103). New York: Wiley.
Astington, J., & Jenkins, J. (1995). Theory of mind devel- Caplan, M., Weissberg, R. P., Grober, J. S., Sivo, P. J.,
opment and social understanding. Cognition and Grady, K., & Jacoby, C. (1992). Social competence
Emotion, 9, 151–165. promotion with inner-city and suburban adolescents:
Attwood, T. (2000). Strategies for improving the social Effects on social adjustment and alcohol use. Journal
integration of children with Asperger Syndrome. of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60(1), 56–63.
Autism, 4(1), 85–100. Castro, V. L., Halberstadt, A. G., & Garrett-Peters, P.
Backenson, E. M., Holland, S. C., Kubas, H. A., Fitzer, (2016). A three-factor structure of emotion under-
K. R., Wilcox, G., Carmichael, J. A., … Hale, J. B. standing in third-grade children. Social Development,
(2015). Psychosocial and adaptive deficits associated 25(3), 602–622.
with learning disability subtypes. Journal of Learning Clark, C., Prior, M., & Kinsella, G. (2002). The relation-
Disabilities, 48, 511–522. ship between executive function abilities, adaptive
Battistich, V., Solomon, D., Watson, M., Solomon, J., & behaviour, and academic achievement in children with
Schaps, E. (1989). Effects of an elementary school externalizing behaviour problems. Journal of Child
program to enhance prosocial behavior on children’s Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 785–796.
cognitive-social problem-solving skills and strategies. Committee for Children. (1997). Second step: A violence-­
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 10, prevention curriculum. Seattle, WA: Author.
147–169. Cotugno, A. J. (2009). Social competence and social skills
Baumeister, A., Storch, E., & Geffken, G. (2008). Peer training and intervention for children with autism spec-
victimization in children with learning disabilities. trum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 25, 11–23. Disorders, 39(9), 1268–1277.
Bauminger, N., & Kimhi-Kind, I. (2008). Social infor- Cragar, D. E., & Horvath, L. S. (2003). The application
mation processing, security of attachment, and emo- of social skills training in the treatment of a child with
tion regulation in children with learning disabilities. Asperger’s disorder. Clinical Case Studies, 2(1), 34–49.
Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 315–332. Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and refor-
Beauchamp, M. H., & Anderson, V. (2010). SOCIAL: An mulation of social information-processing mecha-
integrative framework for the development of social nisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological
skills. Psychological Bulletin, 136(1), 39–64. Bulletin, 115, 74–101.
Becker, S. P., & Langberg, J. M. (2014). Attention-deficit/ Del Prette, Z. A. P., Del Prette, A., de Oliviera, L. A.,
hyperactivity disorder and sluggish cognitive tempo Gresham, F. M., & Vance, M. J. (2012). Role of
dimensions in relation to executive functioning in ado- social performance in predicting learning problems:
lescents with ADHD. Child Psychiatry and Human Prediction of risk using logistic regression analysis.
Development, 45(1), 1–11. School Psychology International, 33(6), 615–630.
80 K. Milligan et al.

Dennis, T. A., Brotman, L. M., Huang, K.-Y., & Gouley, meta-analysis of the research. Learning Disability
K. K. (2007). Effortful control, social competence, and Quarterly, 19, 2–13.
adjustment problems in children at risk for psycho- Galway, T. M., & Metsala, J. L. (2011). Social cogni-
pathology. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent tion and its relation to psychosocial adjustment in chil-
Psychology, 36, 442–454. dren with nonverbal learning disabilities. Journal of
Derekshan, N., & Eysenck, M. W. (2009). Anxiety, process- Learning Disabilities, 44(1), 33–49.
ing efficiency, and cognitive performance: New devel- Gates, J. A., Kang, E., & Lerner, M. D. (2017). Efficacy
opments from attentional control theory. European of group social skills interventions for youth with
Psychologist, 14, 168–176. autism spectrum disorder: A systematic review
Devine, R. T., & Hughes, C. (2016). Measuring theory of and meta-­analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 52,
mind in middle childhood: Reliability and validity of 164–181.
the silent films and strange stories tasks. Journal of Gifford-Smith, M. E., & Rabiner, D. L. (2004). Social
Experimental Child Psychology, 149, 23–40. information processing and children’s social adjust-
Devine, R. T., White, N., Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2016). ment. In J. B. Kupersmidt & K. A. Dodge (Eds.),
Theory of mind in middle childhood: Longitudinal Children’s peer relations: From development to
associations with executive function and social com- intervention (pp. 61–79). Washington, DC: American
petence. Developmental Psychology, 52, 758–771. Psychological Association.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review Gresham, F. M. (2001). Assessment of social skills in chil-
of Psychology, 64, 135–168. dren and adolescents. In J. Andrews, D. Saklosfske, &
Dodge, K. A. (1986). A social information-­processing model H. Janzen (Eds.), Handbook of psychoeducational assess-
of social competence in children. In M. Perlmutter ment (pp. 326–356). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
(Ed.), Minnesota symposium on child psychology (vol. Halberstadt, A. G., Denham, S. A., & Dunsmore, J. C.
18, pp. 77–125). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. (2001). Affective social competence. Review of Social
Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-­ Development, 10(1), 79–119.
processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression Hantson, J., Wang, P. P., Grizenko-Vida, M., Ter-­
in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and Stepanian, M., Harvey, W. J., & Joober, R. (2012).
Social Psychology, 53(6), 1146–1158. Effectiveness of a therapeutic summer camp for chil-
Dodge, K. A., & Feldman, E. (1990). Issues in social cog- dren with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders,
nition and sociometric status. In A. R. Asher & J. D. 16(7), 610–617.
Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood (pp. 119– Harms, M. B., Zayas, V., Metzloff, A. N., & Carlson,
155). New York: Cambridge University Press. S. M. (2014). Stability of executive function and pre-
Dodge, K. A., Murphy, R. R., & Buchsbaum, K. (1984). dictions to adaptive behavior from middle childhood
The assessment of intention-cue detection skills in to pre-adolescence. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–9.
children: Implications for developmental psychopa- Heinze, J. E., Miller, A. L., Seifer, R., Dickstein, S., &
thology. Child Development, 55(1), 163–173. Locke, R. L. (2015). Emotion knowledge, loneli-
Dodge, K. A., & Newman, J. P. (1981). Biased decision-­ ness, negative social experiences, and internalizing
making processes in aggressive boys. Journal of symptoms among low-income preschoolers. Social
Abnormal Psychology, 90, 375–379. Development, 24, 240–265.
Dodge, K. A., & Tomlin, A. M. (1987). Utilization of Holsen, I., Smith, B. H., & Frey, K. S. (2008). Outcomes
self-schemas as a mechanism of interpretational bias of the social competence program Second Step in
in aggressive children. Social Cognition, 5, 280–300. Norwegian elementary schools. School Psychology
Dunn, J., & Cutting, A. L. (1999). Understanding other, International, 29(1), 71–88.
and individual differences in friendship interac- Humphreys, K. L., Galán, C. A., Tottenham, N., & Lee,
tions in young children. Social Development, 8(2), S. S. (2016). Impaired social decision-making medi-
201–219. ates the association between ADHD and social prob-
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, lems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(5),
R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of 1023–1032. doi:10.1007/s10802-015-0095-7
enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A Ialongo, N., Poduska, J., Werthamer, L., & Kellam, S.
meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. (2001). The distal impact of two first-grade preven-
Child Development, 81(1), 405–432. tive interventions on conduct problems and disor-
Elias, M. J., & Haynes, N. (2008). Social competence, der in early adolescence. Journal of Emotional and
social support, and academic achievement in minority, Behavioural Disorders, 9(3), 146–160.
low-income, urban elementary school children. School Integra Program, Child Development Institute. (2016).
Psychology Quarterly, 23(4), 474–495. Integra social awareness, competence, engagement,
Ellis, M. L., Weiss, B., & Lochman, J. E. (2009). Executive and skills (ACES) program. Unpublished manual.
functions in children: Associations with aggres- Kendall, P. C., (1985). Toward a cognitive-­ behavioral
sive behavior and appraisal processing. Journal of model of child psychopathology and a critique
Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 945–956. of related interventions. Journal of Abnormal
Forness, S. R., & Kavale, K. A. (1996). Treating social Child Psychology, 13(3), 357–372. doi:10.1007/
skill deficits in children with learning disabilities: A BF00912722
Social Competence: Consideration of Behavioral, Cognitive, and Emotional Factors 81

Kimber, B., Sandell, R., & Bremberg, S. (2008). Social Nigg, J. T., Quamma, J. P., Greenberg, M. T., & Kusche,
and emotional training in Swedish schools for the C. A. (1999). A two-year longitudinal study of neu-
promotion of mental health: An effectiveness study of ropsychological and cognitive performance in rela-
5 years of intervention. Health Education Research, tion to behavioral problems and competencies in
23(6), 931–940. elementary school children. Journal of Abnormal
Lalonde, C. E., & Chandler, M. J. (1994). False belief Child Psychology, 27, 51–63.
understanding goes to school: On the social-emotional Nowicki, S., & Duke, M. P. (1994). Individual differ-
consequences of coming early or late to a first theory ences in the nonverbal communication of affect: The
of mind. Cognition and Emotion, 9(2/3), 167–185. Diagnostic Analysis of Nonverbal Accuracy Scale.
Learning Disabilities Association of Ontario. (2001). Journal of Nonverbal behavior, 18(1), 9–35.
Learning disabilities: A new definition. Retrieved O’Hearn, T. C., & Gatz, M. (1999). Evaluating a psycho-
from http://www.ldao.ca/documents/Definition_and_ social competence program for urban adolescence.
Suporting%20Document_2001.pdf The Journal of Primary Prevention, 20(2), 119–144.
Lemerise, E. A., & Arsenio, W. F. (2000). An integrated Ornaghi, V., Grazzani, I., Cherubin, E., Conte, E., & Piralli,
model of emotion processes and cognition in social infor- F. (2015). ‘Let’s talk about emotions!’ the effect of con-
mation processing. Child Development, 71(1), 107–118. versational training on preschoolers’ emotion compre-
Liddle, B., & Nettle, D. (2006). Higher-order theory of hension and prosocial orientation. Social Development,
mind and social competence in school-age children. 24, 166–183.
Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, 4(3– Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and
4), 231–244. later personal adjustment: Are low-accepted children
Lillvist, A., Sandberg, A., Bjorck-Akesson, E., & at risk? Psychological Bulletin, 102(3), 357–389.
Granlund, M. (2009). The construct of social com- Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive func-
petence – how preschool teachers define social com- tions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of
petence in young children. International Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87.
Early Childhood, 41(1), 51–68. Petti, V. L., Voelker, S. L., Shore, D. L., & Hayman-­
Lyons, G. L., Huber, H. B., Carter, E. W., Chen, R., & Abello, S. E. (2003). Perception of nonverbal emotion
Asmus, J. M. (2016). Assessing the social skills cues by children with nonverbal learning disabilities.
and problem behaviors of adolescents with severe Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities,
disabilities enrolled in general education classes. 15(1), 23–36.
American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Quinn, M. M., Kavale, K. A., Mathur, S. R., Rutherford,
Disabilities, 121(4), 317–345. R. B., & Forness, S. R. (1999). A meta-analysis of
Milich, R., & Dodge, K. A. (1984). Social information social skill interventions for students with emotional
processing in child psychiatric populations. Journal of or behavioral disorders. Journal of Emotional and
Abnormal Child Psychology, 12(3), 471–489. Behavioral Disorders, 7(1), 54–64.
Miller, A. L., Gouley, K. K., Seifer, R., Zakriski, A., Rantanen, K., Eriksson, K., & Nieminen, P. (2012). Social
Eguia, M., & Vergnani, M. (2005). Emotion knowl- competence in children with epilepsy – a review.
edge skills in low-income elementary school children: Epilepsy & Behavior, 24(3), 295–303.
Associations with social status and peer experiences. Rao, P. A., Beidel, D. C., & Murray, M. J. (2008). Social
Social Development, 14, 637–651. skills interventions for children with Asperger’s syn-
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). drome or high-functioning autism: A review and rec-
Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the ommendations. Journal of Autism and Developmental
relation between language ability and false-belief Disorders, 38, 353–361.
understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–646. Rassovsky, Y., Satz, P., Alfano, M. S., Light, R. K.,
Milligan, K., Badali, P., & Spiroiu, F. (2015). Using mind- Zaucha, K., McArthur, D. L., & Hovda, D. (2006).
fulness martial arts to address self-regulation chal- Functional outcome in TBI I: Neuropsychological,
lenges in youth with learning disabilities: A qualitative emotional, and behavioral mediators. Journal of
exploration. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24, Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 28(4),
562–575. 567–580.
Milligan, K., Phillips, M., & Morgan, A. S. (2016). Reich, S. M. (2016, February 15). Connecting offline
Tailoring social competence interventions for children social competence to online peer interactions.
with learning disabilities. Journal of Child and Family Psychology of Popular Media Culture. Advance online
Studies, 25, 856–869. publication. doi:10.1037/ppm0000111.
Mishna, F. (2003). Learning disabilities and bullying: Repetti, R. L., Taylor, S. E., & Seeman, T. E. (2002).
Double jeopardy. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, Risky families: Family social environments and the
336–347. mental and physical health of offspring. Psychological
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, Bulletin, 128(2), 330–366.
A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity Riggs, N. R., Greenberg, M. T., Kusché, C. A., & Pentz,
of executive functions and their contributions to com- M. A. (2006). The mediational role of neurocogni-
plex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. tion in the behavioral outcomes of a social-emotional
Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. prevention program in elementary school students:
82 K. Milligan et al.

Effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prevention Science, tic behaviors: Does a brief educational intervention
7, 91–102. have an effect? Journal of Autism and Developmental
Rose-Krasner, L. (1997). The nature of social competence: Disorders, 31(2), 194–205.
A theoretical review. Social Development, 6, 111–135. Taylor, C. A., Liang, B., Tracy, A. J., Williams, L. M.,
Rose, A. J., & Asher, S. R. (2017). The Social Tasks of & Seigle, P. (2002). Gender differences in middle
Friendship: Do Boys and Girls Excel in Different school adjustment, physical fighting, and social
Tasks?. Child Development Perspectives, 11(1), 3–8. skills: Evaluation of a social competency pro-
Rubin, K. H., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (1992). Interpersonal gram. The Journal of Primary Prevention, 23(2),
problem solving. In V. B. Van Hasselt & M. Hersen 259–272.
(Eds.), Handbook of social development (pp. 283– Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A
323). New York, NY: Plenum. theme in search of a defi nition. Monographs of
Sarason, I. G., & Sarason, B. R. (1981). Teaching cogni- the Society for Research in Child Development,
tive and social skills to high school students. Journal 59(2/3), 25–52.
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49(6), Tlustos, S. J., Kirkwood, M. W., Taylor, H. G., Stancin, T.,
908–918. Brown, T. M., & Wade, S. L. (2016). A randomized prob-
Schultz, D., Tharp-Taylor, S., Haviland, A., & Jaycox, L. lem-solving trial for adolescent brain injury: Changes in
(2009). The relationship between protective factors social competence. Rehabilitation Psychology, 61(4),
and outcomes for children investigated for maltreat- 347–357.
ment. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33, 684–698. Van der Oord, S., Prins, P. J., Oosterlaan, J., & Emmelkamp,
Shek, D. T. L., & Yeung, J. T. Y. (2016). Developing social P. M. (2008). Efficacy of methylphenidate, psychosocial
competence in a subject on leadership and intrapersonal treatments and their combination in school-aged chil-
development. International Journal on Disability and dren with ADHD: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology
Human Development, 15(2), 165–173. Review, 28, 783–800.
Slomkowski, C., & Dunn, J. (1996). Young children’s under- Wehmeier, P. M., Schacht, A., & Barkley, R. A. (2010).
standing of other people’s beliefs and feelings and their Social and emotional impairment in children and
connected communication with friends. Developmental ­adolescents with ADHD and the impact on quality
Psychology, 32(3), 442–447. of life. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46(3),
Stanton-Chapman, T., Walker, V., & Jamison, K. R. 209–217.
(2014). Building social competence in preschool: The Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement
effects of a social skills intervention targeting children motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92(4),
enrolled in head start. Journal of Early Childhood 548–573.
Teacher Education, 35, 185–200. Wheeler, J., & Carlson, C. L. (1994). The social func-
Stevahn, L., Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., Oberle, K., tioning of children with ADD with hyperactivity and
& Wahl, L. (2000). Effects of conflict resolution train- ADD without hyperactivity. Journal of Emotional and
ing integrated into a kindergarten curriculum. Child Behavioral Disorders, 2, 2–11.
Development, 71(3), 772–784. White, S. W., Oswald, D., Ollendick, T., & Scahill, L.
Stichter, J. P., Herzog, M. J., Visovsky, K., Schmidt, C., (2009). Anxiety in children and adolescents with
Randolph, J., Schultz, T., & Gage, N. (2010). Social autism spectrum disorders. Clinical psychology
competence intervention for youth with Asperger syn- review, 29(3), 216–229.
drome and high-functioning autism: An initial investiga- Wiener, J., & Schneider, B. (2002). A multisource explo-
tion. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, ration of friendship patterns of children with learning
40(9), 1067–1079. disabilities. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
Stichter, J. P., O’Connor, K. V., Herzog, M. J., Lierheimer, 30, 127–141.
K., & McGhee, S. D. (2012). Social competence inter- Wiener, J., & Sunohara, G. (1998). Parents’ perceptions of
vention for elementary students with Aspergers syn- the quality of friendship of their children with learn-
drome and high functioning autism. Journal of Autism ing disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research and
and Developmental Disorders, 42, 354–366. Practice, 13, 242–257.
Stiller, J., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2007). Perspective-taking Zelazo, P. D., & Lyons, K. E. (2012). The potential
and memory capacity predict social network size. benefits of mindfulness training in early childhood:
Social Networks, 29(1), 93–104. A developmental social cognitive neuroscience
Swaim, K. F., & Morgan, S. B. (2001). Children’s attitudes perspective. Child Development Perspectives, 6,
and behavioral intentions toward a peer with autis- 154–160.
Observational Methods

Susan M. Vener, Alison M. Wichnick-Gillis,
and Claire L. Poulson

We all engage in social interactions throughout of physical appearance, such as grooming, might
the course of a day. Much of our verbal and non- facilitate social interaction. The ability to initiate
verbal behavior is learned and results in either a and maintain informal conversations, to ask ques-
welcome or unwelcome response during social tions, to engage in eye contact during verbal
exchanges. Whether we are at work or in the park exchange, to maintain appropriate affect and
with a friend, a person who is socially skillful is intonation, and to remain on topic are some of the
able to navigate through an interpersonal interac- crucial components of social behavior. Voice
tion and access pleasant consequences. A verbal intensity, duration of verbal response, body lan-
initiation might result in a smile from a conversa- guage, and listening all impact social behavior.
tion recipient. A correct response to a question Social skills deficits and excesses have been
might result in social praise from a professor. correlated with many difficulties later in life,
Conversely, a young girl walking with her head including substance abuse (Green et al., 1999, as
down when entering a room might not be met cited in Matson, 2009), depression (Sato,
with a social greeting. A young man with poor Ishikawa, Arai, & Sakano, 2005, as cited in
interviewing skills might not be offered a job. Matson, 2009), aggression (Dodge et al., 2003),
Although there is no universally agreed upon and delinquency in adulthood (Roff & Wirt,
set of responses that are used to define socially 1984, as cited in Matson, 2009). Social difficul-
skillful behavior (Boisjoli & Matson, 2009), ties can also lead to peer rejection (Dodge et al.,
there are several important components of skillful 2003), poor academic functioning (Elliott,
behavior. For example, attention to basic elements Sheridan, & Gresham, 1989), and long-term defi-
cits in social problem-solving skills (Yeates et al.,
2007). As a result, it is important that disruption
S.M. Vener • A.M. Wichnick-Gillis (*) in social function be assessed and targeted for
New York Child Learning Institute, intervention.
123-14 14th Avenue, College Point, NY 11356, USA
e-mail: NYCLISMV@nycli.org; Assessments measuring social behavior serve
AlisonW319@gmail.com several important functions. They allow for a
C.L. Poulson detailed description and identification of an indi-
Queens College, The City University of New York, vidual’s strengths and weaknesses. Identifying the
65-30 Kissena Boulevard, Flushing, NY 11367, USA specific responses, dimensions, and ­environmental
The Graduate Center, The City University of conditions involved in the problem enables one to
New York, 365 Fifth Avenue, New York, set a course for treatment. Assessments are also
NY 10016, USA used to predict a later outcome, to determine the
e-mail: CLPoulson@earthlink.net

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 83


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_6
84 S.M. Vener et al.

amount of intervention needed, and to classify or Direct Observation of Behavior


diagnose an individual. The latter enables the indi-
vidual to access the needed intervention services, Direct observation requires that an observer
to access financial remuneration for the services develop an operational definition of the targeted
rendered, and to receive information regarding behavior, observe child behavior, and systemati-
progress given the particular intervention imple- cally record the behavior. An advantage of direct
mented (Merrell & Gimpel, 2014). In addition, observation is that it allows an observer to obtain
they may also be used to better understand the information about behavior as it occurs in an inter-
relationship between two or more factors such as active environment (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013).
length of utterances during a social exchange and Information about the conditions in the environ-
eye contact. If an assessment tool is used to evalu- ment that may evoke and maintain problem behav-
ate progress given a particular intervention, the ior enables the development of intervention plans.
quality of the response is important. If the assess- For example, loud noises in a lunchroom may
ment is used to accurately diagnose a condition, result in social behavior that is different from social
classification becomes important (Cordier et al., behavior that occurs in a quite kitchen. Interaction
2015; Wade, 2004). skills in the presence of a less preferred teacher
Assessments measuring social functioning in may be quite different than those in the presence of
children are often questionnaires presented in a highly preferred adult. An assessment that exam-
paper-and-pencil format. Although they often ines behavior in the environment within which it
rely on child self-report, some rely on peer, par- occurs is referred to as an “ecological assessment”
ent, and teacher reports. Self-reports alone have (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013). Nevertheless,
been found to result in low correlations with according to Cordier et al. (2015), there has been
other assessments, can be affected by social pres- limited research on the influence of external factors
sure, and rely on a child’s ability to comply with on a person’s social functioning.
directions (Frankel & Feinberg, 2002, as cited in Although direct observation relies on empiri-
Crowe, Beauchamp, Catroppa, & Anderson, cal data and minimizes the need for making qual-
2011). Crowe et al. (2011) stated that one-time itative inferences (Merrell & Gimpel, 2014), one
parent reports rely on the parent’s awareness and must be alert to the accuracy, reliability, and
willingness to identity deficits in their child’s validity of behavioral observation data obtained.
social function in comparison to other children. The operational definition itself largely impacts
A teacher report, in conjunction with a parent the behavioral data collected. A broadly defined
report, can provide essential information. response might result in observational data that
Multiple informants can provide information are quite different from a response that is nar-
regarding child performance in a variety of envi- rowly defined. For example, engaging in eye con-
ronments across peer groups (Merrell, 2001). tact during an interaction can be broadly defined
Another assessment tool is behavioral obser- as looking at the conversation recipient’s face for
vation. Although observation allows for the direct a minimum of 2 s during the verbal exchange. A
and objective measurement of behavior, there can more narrowly defined response might include
be concerns with the accuracy, reliability, and standing within 3 ft. of the conversation recipi-
validity of the data collected. ent, orienting one’s head toward the conversation
The purpose of this chapter is to identify vari- partner throughout the conversation, and looking
ous assessment strategies that have been used to at the recipient’s face for a minimum of 2 s dur-
assess social behavior and to discuss their ing the exchange. The more narrowly defined
strengths and limitations. The strategies that will response would likely result in fewer occurrences
be discussed include direct behavioral observa- of the target behavior.
tion and indirect assessments, such as interview One concern that does exist regarding direct
methods, sociometric techniques, self-reports, observation is known as observer drift, the ten-
and use of behavior rating scales. dency for the observer to gradually drift from the
Observational Methods 85

originally agreed upon definition (Kazdin, 1981, different observers might disagree on the appro-
as cited in Merrell & Gimpel, 2014). For exam- priateness of the same behavior. Different per-
ple, a verbal initiation that would have previously ceptions within the same social context can lead
been scored as inarticulate might later be scored to different interventions.
as clearly articulated as the observer becomes Dirks et al. (2007) also stated that people are
more familiar with the child’s speech pattern. not equally socially skillful across situations.
Another concern is known as observer reactiv- Children have demonstrated different levels of
ity and occurs when the child’s behavior changes competencies in different situations. In a study
in the presence of the observer (Merrell & by Dodge, Cole, and Brakke (1982) (as cited in
Gimpel, 2014). In 1982, Haynes and Horn (as Dirks et al., 2007), “popular” children were more
cited in Mayer, Sulzer-Azaroff, & Wallace, 2012) likely to initiate interactions toward peers in the
identified a number of ways to minimize reactive playground and less likely to initiate similar
effects, such as observing covertly (e.g., using a interactions in the classroom. The authors con-
golf counter to tally behavior), minimizing the cluded that social skillfulness is not exclusively a
obtrusiveness of the observers and data collection property of the behavior or the person, but rather
stimuli, minimizing interactions between the relies on interactions within the situation in
observers and the subjects, and allowing children which the person is behaving (Dirks et al., 2007).
time to adapt to the presence of the observers Matson (2009) discussed the need to consider
before formal observations begin. whether a disruption in social function is a result
A different concern is that without knowing of skill deficits or situational variables. Are the
how other children might react in a similar social needed social responses not in the child’s reper-
situation, it can be quite challenging to interpret toire? Or does the child have the needed skills,
the observational data collected. How many initia- but does not display them in the social situation?
tions should the child emit? What is the appropri- If a child does not have the social skills needed to
ate body language? To address this concern, it manage a social interaction, components of social
would be helpful to collect social comparison data behavior are taught. If a child has the needed
and to compare the data collected with that of social skills, but does not display them, then the
peers in the same environment (Merrell & Gimpel, course for treatment might be quite different. For
2014). Such data would enable inferences to be example, for a child who has the skills to engage
made about whether behavioral excesses or defi- in conversation, yet is not displaying the skill
ciencies are present. If deficiencies or excesses are while at the lunch table with peers, it might be
identified, intervention can be planned. appropriate to create a motivational system that
Merrell (2001) added the following four con- will reinforce verbal exchanges.
cerns when conducting direct behavioral obser- Although difficult to interpret, direct observa-
vations. First, he stated that direct behavioral tional data can provide useful information to deter-
observation requires extensive time and prepara- mine social skills deficits or excesses. Behavior can
tion. Second, behavior might not be adequately be recorded in its natural environment (naturalistic
recorded. Inappropriate conclusions might be observations) or in an environment designed to
reached. Third, biased expectations might influ- simulate the conditions in the natural environment
ence recording. And lastly, the number of obser- (analogue observations). Similarly, behavior can be
vations needed to make useful conclusions needs measured by the child himself or herself (self-mon-
to be considered. The reactivity of social behav- itoring) or by an observer.
ior indicates a need for multiple observations to
obtain meaningful information.
Subjective perception was raised as an issue Naturalistic Observation
by Dirks, Treat, and Weersing (2007). They
raised some interesting concerns regarding Perhaps the most efficient way to assess social
observer perceptions of social behavior. Two behavior is to do so in the environment within
86 S.M. Vener et al.

which it occurs. According to Jones, Reid, and Role-Play


Patterson in 1979 (as cited in Whitcomb & Merrell,
2013), naturalistic observation involves (a) obser- Role-play assessments occur when the observer
vation and recording of the behavior in the natural is presented with a sample situation and is instructed
environment at the time of occurrence, (b) trained to react as if it were actually occurring (Matson,
observers, and (c) an operational definition of the 2009). Such assessments enable a child’s deci-
targeted behavior. Observations in the student’s sion-making and interpersonal communication
classroom, cafeteria, or playground allow for skills to be evaluated. Behavior such as eye con-
ongoing opportunities for unobtrusive data collec- tact, facial expression, voice volume, and number
tion within a peer group. of words spoken can be recorded (Matson &
Ollendick, 1988, as cited in Matson, 2009).
Although used to assess social behavior,
Analogue Observation role-­play assessments have been found to have
relatively poor validity (Matson, Esveldt-
An analogue observation occurs in an environment Dawson, & Kazdin, 1983, as cited in Matson,
that simulates the natural environment in which 2009). Nevertheless, in a study conducted by
the behavior of concern is likely to occur (Hintze, Leaf et al. (2016), a multiple-baseline experi-
Stoner, & Bull, 2000, as cited in Whitcomb & mental design was used to assess the effects of
Merrell, 2013). For example, a child who displays role-play in addition to a discrimination program
difficulty engaging in conversation about a topic to teach three young children with autism to
of interest to a sibling may be presented with mul- engage in social communication skills. The role-
tiple opportunities to engage in similar social play task required the investigator to demonstrate
exchanges in the classroom with a peer. In this the target behavior appropriately two times and
instance, the analogue situation would be designed inappropriately two times. The discrimination
to evoke brief verbal exchanges similar to those task required the child to verbally discriminate
that might occur at home. whether the demonstration was appropriate or
Although quite useful in creating real-life sit- inappropriate. As part of the intervention package,
uations that are controlled and can be carefully the child was then asked to role-play a similar
manipulated, analogue observations make the scenario that was likely to occur in a naturalistic
assumption that the behavior observed in the sim- situation. The authors identified one component
ulated environment will be similar to the behav- of social behavior that was in need of improve-
ior observed in a naturalistic situation. To increase ment for each participant. For example, for Sally,
the likelihood that behavioral changes acquired the target response was to “chat” with a friend
in the simulated environment will generalize to while watching a short YouTube© video. The
the naturalistic situation, Stokes and Baer (1977) results of the study showed that role-play, in
suggest (a) introducing naturally maintaining addition to the discrimination-training program,
contingencies into the contrived situation (e.g., was effective in improving social communication
accessing a smile during a social exchange), (b) skills across all three participants.
training sufficient exemplars (e.g., engaging in
conversations with multiple peers/instructors in
an effort to increase conversation with mom dur- Self-Monitoring
ing dinner), (c) training loosely (e.g., leaving free
to vary the stimuli presented and the correct Self-monitoring occurs when the child observes
responses allowed), (d) using indiscriminable his or her own behavior and records the occur-
contingencies (e.g., using an intermittent sched- rence or nonoccurrence of the target social behav-
ule of reinforcement), and (e) programming com- ior. Due to the reactive effect of the procedure,
mon stimuli (e.g., arranging an analogue situation self-monitoring itself has been found to produce
in a pizzeria with a classmate). change in behavior in the desired direction
Observational Methods 87

(Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007). If the child Functional Behavior Assessment
perceives that a response is desirable or undesir-
able, that response will tend to increase or decrease The information gathered through direct observa-
accordingly (Kanfer, 1970; Kazdin, 1974). For tion can be used to guide the development of an
example, classroom teachers used self-monitoring intervention plan for social behavior. There are
to increase their use of positive statements during times, however, when a plan may be unsuccessful
a class lesson (Silvestri, 2004, as cited in Cooper in modifying behavior and, perhaps, the true
et al., 2007). Similarly, in a study by Broden, Hall, functions of the behavior are not being addressed.
and Mitts in 1971 (as cited in Cooper et al., 2007), In such cases, it might be beneficial to conduct a
a self-monitoring procedure was useful in reduc- functional behavior assessment (FBA). An FBA
ing the number of times an eighth-grade student is a direct observational method that helps to
talked out in class. Although self-monitoring can explain the function of behavior. This method
result in favorable outcomes, reinforcement con- provides information on the consequences of the
tingencies to maintain desired outcomes may be behavior or what causes the behavior to endure
necessary. Nevertheless, the effects of self-moni- over time (Mayer et al., 2012). By identifying the
toring may be temporary (Ballard & Glynn, 1975; reinforcers for a given response, one can then
Critchfield & Vargas, 1991). develop a treatment plan that alters the contin-
In addition to the desirability of the social gencies and incorporates those reinforcers to
response, additional parameters that might also decrease undesirable behavior and to increase
contribute to the reactive effects of self-­ desirable behavior (Cooper et al., 2007).
monitoring include (a) the feedback a child One of the initial stages of an FBA is the direct
receives from observing his or her own behavior observation of the behavior. This is typically con-
(Kazdin, 1974); (b) the timing of self-recording, ducted in the natural setting in which the behav-
that is, whether recording occurs prior to, dur- ior occurs, such as in school or at home, but it can
ing, or following a target response (Bellack, also be conducted in a contrived or analogue situ-
Rozensky, & Schwartz, 1974; Cavior & ation. During this initial observation, one may
Marabotto, 1976; Kanfer, 1970); (c) the obtru- gather information on the frequency of the behav-
siveness of the recording device (Kazdin, 1979; ior, the setting in which the behavior occurs, the
Kirby, Fowler, & Baer, 1991; Nelson, Lipinski, intensity of the behavior, antecedents and conse-
& Boykin, 1978); and (d) the performance stan- quences of the behavior, and duration of the
dard to which an individual adheres. A child who behavior. After this information has been col-
self-monitors may show greater behavior change lected, it can be used to help formulate a hypoth-
when working toward a specific level of perfor- esis about the function of the behavior.
mance rather than merely recording responses Subsequently, an intervention plan can be created
with no clear goal (Kazdin, 1974). that incorporates the presumed function of the
Self-monitoring has also been shown to be behavior (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013).
useful in that it enables an individual to record An FBA helps to determine the source of rein-
private events such as thoughts and physiologi- forcement maintaining behavior. The source of
cal changes, in addition to overt behavior. The reinforcement may exist in several forms, under
reactive effects of the self-monitoring strategy the main umbrellas of positive and negative rein-
result in concerns for both the reliability and forcement. One possible source of positive rein-
validity of the measures obtained. To address forcement includes social attention. Positive
these problems, one can (a) provide training on social attention may include certain facial expres-
how to collect data, (b) use formal observation sions, head turning, reprimands, or attempts to
data sheets, (c) simplify the self-monitoring soothe or distract. In a study by Christensen,
procedures, (d) conduct frequent interobserver Young, and Marchant (2004), a functional assess-
agreement checks, and (e) reinforce reliable and ment revealed that social attention had main-
valid self-­monitoring responses (Whitcomb & tained inappropriate social behavior (e.g.,
Merrell, 2013). disrupting the class, seeking peer attention, off-­
88 S.M. Vener et al.

task behavior) of two 8-year-old boys in an ele- aggressive behavior and to increase the likelihood
mentary school for typically developing students. of appropriate social behavior (Harper et al., 2013).
The authors used this reinforcer to then increase The functional assessment of behavior is a
the likelihood of appropriate classroom behavior, powerful instrument that can greatly influence
such as attending to the teacher, raising hands, the formation of a treatment plan. Although each
and complying with directions. analysis is tailored according to the individual,
Access to tangible stimuli is another form of their behavior, and the context in which the
positive reinforcement, whereby a given response behavior occurs, Hanley, Iwata, and McCord
leads to a child obtaining desired or preferred (2003) have provided several general guidelines
materials. In a study by Richman, Wacker, and for conducting an assessment. Some of the guide-
Winborn (2001), a functional analysis of aggres- lines include focusing on one or a few responses
sive behavior in a young child revealed that the at a time, programming consequences for the
behavior was maintained by access to preferred target response, limiting the amount of session
toys. This information was then used to imple- time to about 10 min, and incorporating the use
ment a treatment plan in which toys were pro- of other sources of information when conducting
vided contingently upon the child’s use of an the assessment, such as observations and interviews.
appropriate request for the toys. It is also important to consider and to weigh out
Automatic reinforcement is another possible the benefits and potential risks or dangers of
contingency maintaining behavior. Behavior conducting a functional assessment.
maintained by automatic reinforcers does not Functional assessments are frequently used in
rely upon other people, but instead produces its hospitals and other settings in which the learner
own reinforcement. This function of behavior is is not available for treatment on a daily basis for
an assumption made when social reinforcers long periods of time. In a school setting, for
have been eliminated as the source of reinforce- example, it may be less important to spend a
ment (Cooper et al., 2007). Roscoe, Iwata, and week or more to conduct a formal assessment of
Zhou (2013) conducted a functional assessment a learner’s current reinforcer preferences and the
of hand mouthing in individuals with intellec- current functions of behavior, when one can often
tual disabilities. The results of the analysis indi- provide training from day 1 to establish new
cated that hand mouthing was maintained by ­preferences and functional relations within the
automatic reinforcement. With these findings, same time frame. For example, often one can use
the authors were able to introduce a treatment pairing operations to teach preferences for more
package that successfully decreased the likeli- appropriate reinforcers within a week or 2.
hood of this response.
Behavior also may be maintained by negative
reinforcement contingencies. One possible Recording Procedures
source of negative reinforcement is the escape or
avoidance of undesirable or aversive situations. Now that we have identified different ways to
This may include escape from social interactions. observe social behavior, we need to identify differ-
Harper, Iwata, and Camp (2013) conducted func- ent ways to record the behavior observed. There
tional assessments with four individuals with are four types of recording procedures that will be
intellectual disabilities. The subjects were discussed: frequency recording; time sampling,
included for their aggressive behavior, which including whole-interval recording, partial-inter-
tended to occur during play and demand condi- val recording, and momentary time sampling;
tions. It was concluded that the function of the duration recording; and latency recording.
aggressive behavior was to escape the social
demands. With this information, the authors were Frequency Recording
able to develop a treatment plan that incorporated Frequency recording requires a tally of the number
this function to decrease the future likelihood of of times that a response occurs within a specified
Observational Methods 89

observation session (Gast & Ledford, 2014). 15-min interval during lunch, and a 15-min inter-
Frequency recording is most efficient when val during homeroom.
assessing behavior that has a discrete beginning
and ending. It is also important that the behavior  hole-Interval and Partial-Interval
W
lasts approximately the same amount of time each Recording
time it occurs (Merrell & Gimpel, 2014). Some behavior tends not to be discrete, but rather
Otherwise, the data collected might be mislead- continuous (e.g., eye contact, posture, voice vol-
ing. For example, if an observer is measuring tan- ume, intonation) (Mayer et al., 2012). Identifying
trum behavior and the behavior occurs three times when a response ends and another begins might
on the first day and once the following day, the be quite challenging. In these instances, interval
data might suggest a decrease in behavior. recording would be appropriate. Interval record-
Nevertheless, the three occurrences on the first ing involves dividing the observation session into
day might have been brief in duration, whereas time intervals and recording the presence or
the behavior on the second day might have lasted absence of the target behavior within each inter-
an hour. It is also important that the behavior does val (Cooper et al., 2007). Unlike frequency
not occur too frequently such that it is difficult to recording, with interval recording, there is an
identify the end of one occurrence and the begin- indication of when the behavior occurred. Whole-­
ning of the next (Cooper et al., 2007). Furthermore, interval recording involves recording whether the
the behavior must not occur so rapidly that the behavior occurred throughout the entire interval
observer loses count (Mayer et al., 2012). (Gast & Ledford, 2014). Responses that are con-
The challenge with frequency recording is that tinuous and conducive to this measurement pro-
a tally of behavior does not provide any informa- cedure might include walking in the park with a
tion about the temporal occurrences of the behav- friend, playing with a peer, or sitting on a chair
ior (Gast & Ledford, 2014). Nevertheless, one during a movie. Nonverbal measures that are also
might record the order in which the behavior continuous, such as eye contact or posture, would
occurs and indicate the events preceding (ante- be quite challenging to observe using a
cedents) and the events following (consequences) ­whole-­interval recording system. These nonver-
the recorded behavior. bal measures would require continuous observa-
Continuous observation and recording of the tion on the part of the observer throughout the
target behavior can provide important informa- interval. Similarly, low-frequency behavior such
tion about the child’s social behavior in the envi- as arguing with a friend and tantrum behavior
ronment within which it occurs. Continuous might require long observation sessions, such as
measurement involves a report of all instances of an entire school day, in order to be observed.
the target behavior during an observation session Continuous observation in these situations would
(Johnston & Pennypacker, 1993a, as cited in not be practical.
Cooper et al., 2007). For example, it can be pos- Partial-interval recording involves the
sible to count the number of times a child initi- recording of behavior if it occurs at any time
ates toward his peers during a 30-min recess, during the specified interval (Gast & Ledford,
usually referred to as rate per minute. 2014). Data collection of non-discrete behavior
Nevertheless, it might not be practical to count that might be fleeting (e.g., smiling, laughing,
the number of times that the child initiates toward cheering) and/or low-frequency behavior such
his peers throughout the school day. Discontinuous as tantrum behavior would also be possible
measurement involves measurement in which using a partial-interval recording system. An
some instances of the target response might not observer would score the behavior as having
be observed and recorded (Cooper et al., 2007). occurred, regardless of how long or how often it
For example, an observer can observe the occur- occurred within the interval. If the intervals are
rence or nonoccurrence of the target behavior long enough, an observer can measure the pres-
during a 15-min time interval during recess, a ence of multiple responses.
90 S.M. Vener et al.

Momentary Time Sampling Across these different assessments, information


Momentary time sampling involves recording the is provided on the environmental conditions sur-
presence or absence of the behavior at the end of the rounding the behavior, such as the antecedent
interval at a moment in time (Gast & Ledford, events and the consequences of behavior. This
2014). Unlike interval recording, momentary time information helps to provide more details on the
sampling does not require continuous observation. child’s behavior that may not be observable dur-
Nonverbal components of social behavior such as ing a direct assessment. Indirect assessments are
engagement in an activity and posture could be relatively easy to implement, are not risky or
measured in this manner. Nevertheless, it is impor- invasive in the child’s environment, and may pro-
tant to note that because behavior is observed for vide some valuable information. Nevertheless, in
only a moment in time, much behavior will be general, indirect assessments also have their dis-
missed. As a result, it is not recommended that low- advantages, including the subjectivity and unreli-
frequency, short duration behavior be recorded ability of the content provided. For these reasons,
using this recording method (Saudargas & Zanolli, direct measure is preferable in terms of assessing
1990, as cited in Cooper et al., 2007). behavior, and indirect measures should be used
only as an initial step in the process (Mayer et al.,
Duration 2012).
If one is interested in measuring the length of
time that a behavior occurs from start to finish,
duration is an appropriate dependent measure Interviewing Techniques
(Merrell & Gimpel, 2014). One might measure
the duration of time that a child is riding a bicycle Interviews are a form of assessment that can be
or walking on a treadmill in gym class. conducted with the child of interest or with
­people familiar with the child. Interviews provide
Response Latency some initial information to help guide an assess-
Response latency refers to the time elapsed ment. In the absence of a structured interview
between the onset of a stimulus and the begin- that can be widely used to assess social behavior,
ning of a subsequent response (Cooper et al., one must rely upon a loosely structured format
2007). The amount of time it takes for the when conducting an interview (Whitcomb &
response to begin is critical in this observation Merrell, 2013).
(Merrell & Gimpel, 2014). For example, follow- When conducting interviews with children,
ing the instruction to stand on line, the critical there are several factors that could influence the
aspect is the amount of time it takes the child to quality and extent of information obtained. First,
engage in the behavior. one must take the child’s age into account.
Typically, the younger a child is, the greater the
likelihood that information reported may not be
Indirect Assessments accurate or may be affected due to limited lan-
guage and verbal skills (Whitcomb & Merrell,
There may be times when it is not possible to 2013).
directly observe the behavior of the child of inter- In terms of social behavior, an interview is
est or when one may be interested in gathering likely to include questions pertaining to what and
information prior to direct observation. In these when information in the child’s environment,
cases, an indirect assessment may be used. rather than questions of why. Questions of why
During an indirect assessment, one does not encourage the interviewee to make inferences
directly observe the behavior, but may collect and may not be useful as reliable or valid infor-
information through interviews, sociometric mation. An interview also may include a survey
measures, or self-report measures. These may be or questionnaire or may even ask the child to self-­
completed by parents, siblings, teachers, peers, monitor their own behavior. Interviews help to
or other people who are familiar with the child. identify the variables surrounding a given
Observational Methods 91

response and help to provide information on how peer rating procedures, (c) sociometric ranking
to plan intervention (Cooper et al., 2007). procedures, (d) picture sociometrics, (e) “guess
At times, it may not be possible to interview who” procedures, (f) the class play, and (g) alter-
the child of interest, or one may want information native sociometric procedures.
from those familiar with the child to get a more
detailed picture of the social behavior. In this Peer Nomination
case, one may choose to interview the child’s The peer nomination strategy was first introduced
parents, family members, or teachers. Those in 1934 by Moreno (as cited in Merrell & Gimpel,
familiar with the child may be asked to describe 2014). Group participants are instructed to nomi-
the child’s social skills and to provide specific nate peers that can best fit specific positive and
information on the events surrounding certain negative behavioral characteristics. For example,
behavior. Interviews with significant others not participants are asked to identify classmates that
only help to provide information in terms of they would most like to play with, to eat lunch
developing an intervention plan, but they also can with, or to work with on a project. Participants
help to determine the likelihood that these people might be instructed to either (a) write the names
may be willing to be a part of the intervention of classmates on blank lines for each item or (b)
plan and help to change the child’s social behav- place an “x” under the names of the students who
ior. This support may be needed for the success are to be nominated. When the assessment is
of the child’s behavior plan (Cooper et al., 2007). completed, the number of times each name
Although interviews can provide some valuable appears is counted. The overall results can be
information, it can be difficult to assess social confidential, and the findings quite interesting.
skills through an interview because information Children can be classified as frequently nomi-
gathered is subjective and may not be a valid nated, infrequently nominated, never nominated,
report on the child’s behavior. or mutually nominated (e.g., child 1 selects child
2 and child 2 selects child 1). Similarly, preference
regarding gender choices can be investigated, and
Sociometric Techniques “cliques” can be identified (e.g., children nomi-
nated each other within a group) (Merrell &
Sociometric techniques are also used to assess Gimpel, 2014).
social behavior. These techniques allow for infor-
mation to be obtained from children within the  eer Rating Procedures
P
social context. Peer participants, as opposed to an The peer rating procedure requires that each
outside observer, are asked to observe and record member of a group assign a rating to every other
behavior. Data are obtained on measures of member in the group. For example, when asked
“social status” within the group. Qualities such as the question “Would you like to play with this
popularity, peer rejection, peer acceptance, lead- child?,” a rating from 1 to 5, 1 being the lowest
ership ability, athletic ability, academic abilities, and 5 being the highest, is assigned to each member.
social awkwardness, and aggressiveness can be An average of all the ratings he or she receives is
measured (Merrell & Gimpel, 2014). calculated to determine each child’s score
The quality of social interactions, and the dif- (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013).
ficulties that children may have with peer rela-
tionships, may have a large impact on adjustment  ociometric Ranking Procedures
S
in later life. A child’s ability to interact with peers Although the objective of sociometrics is to obtain
enables that child to (a) build friendships and (b) data from within the social group, the task of
develop social interaction skills that are useful in making social discriminations might be challeng-
other situations (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013). ing for young children and children with varying
This next section concerns the following degrees of social and intellectual impairment. As a
sociometric procedures used to assess social result, variations in procedure might be needed.
behavior: (a) peer nomination procedures, (b) The sociometric ranking procedure provides infor-
92 S.M. Vener et al.

mation on a child’s social status and peer relations egy has been found to be easy to administrate and
using information provided by an adult outside of simple to score (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013).
the social context. One way to obtain data on
sociometric measures from an outside observer is  he Class Play
T
to ask the teacher to rank each child in the class- The class play technique involves having children
room according to some criteria (e.g., a child’s pretend to direct an imaginary play and to assign
popularity in the class). A second way to obtain their classmates to roles in the play. Roles are
similar data is to identify a subgroup of children either intended to be positive (e.g., a child who
within a social context that fit a particular descrip- plays in the park with a classmate) or negative
tion (e.g., children that are “shy”) and rank the (e.g., a child who reads a book during recess). For
children according to severity with the subgroup a given child, one score was achieved by dividing
(Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013). the number of negative roles by the total number
of negative plus positive roles given to the child.
Picture Sociometrics High percentages are intended to be indicative of a
Picture sociometrics is similar to peer nomina- high degree of peer rejection. Low percentages are
tion with the exception that photographs are pre- intended to be indicative of a low degree of peer
sented to the child. Each child is presented with rejection (Merrell & Gimpel, 2014).
photographs of each child within the class. The class play procedure is viewed as positive
Questions are asked, and the child is instructed to for the following two reasons. First, children
either point to or obtain a photograph. Questions appear to enjoy assigning classmates to roles and
such as “Who is your best friend?” and ‘Who do participating in pretend play. And second, teach-
you like to play with most?” are examples of ers appear to be more receptive to having chil-
questions that can be asked. This strategy has dren participate in this type of an assessment than
been found to be useful when obtaining informa- some other types of sociometic procedures
tion from young children who are not yet profi- (Merrell & Gimpel, 2014).
cient readers. Following a series of questions, the
number of times each name appears is counted  imitations of Sociometric Procedures
L
and is tallied, and inferences regarding “social Whitcomb and Merrell (2013) encourage profes-
status” (e.g., social acceptance and social rejec- sionals using sociometric strategies to carefully
tion) can be made (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013). attend to potential racial, ethnic, and/or gender
The literature has suggested that this assess- bias that might exist. Similarities among peers
ment technique has high interrater reliability, greatly effect peer nominations and ratings. For
high short-term test-retest reliability, and ade- example, girls are more likely to nominate girls,
quate long-term test-retest reliability (Milich & and boys are more likely to nominate boys.
Landau, 1984, as cited in Merrell & Gimpel, Singleton and Asher (1977) found that black
2014). Milich and Landau (1984) have also found children were more likely to select black children
picture sociometric assessments to result in clear when identifying peers to play with, and white
discriminations between groups of aggressive, children were more likely to select white chil-
aggressive-withdrawn, and “typical” boys (as dren. In 1989, Kistner and Gatlin (as cited in
cited in Merrell & Gimpel, 2014). Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013) found that both
black and white children were more likely to
“ Guess who” Measures reject children of the opposite gender.
The “guess who” technique requires that the Similarly, although sociometric procedures
child respond to questions such as “Guess who leave children with specific labels (e.g., “a good
fights with other children?” and “Guess who has leader,” “often left out,” “often struggles”), the
so many friends?” When all questions have been specific dimensions of behavior that earned these
asked, findings are often summarized by tallying labels are unclear. When creating an intervention
counts from each question. This assessment strat- strategy, it is uncertain what components of social
behavior should be targeted.
Observational Methods 93

In addition, some sociometric procedures assessment. The results indicated that the partici-
involve asking children to nominate or rank peers pants liked the assessment process. One-third of
based on negative criteria. As a result, parents, the participants stated that they did discuss the
teachers, and/or the administration often hesitate measures with their peers. No negative or harmful
using such assessment tools. It is a concern that effects were reported.
negative nominations will further isolate children To reduce the potential risk to children by
already demonstrating social challenge. negative nominations, it is suggested that
According to Whitcomb and Merrell (2013), to researchers be sure to (a) explain confidentiality,
date, no research has been found to validate these (b) arrange for a structured activity to occur fol-
concerns. In a study by Hayvren and Hymel lowing the assessment in an effort to reduce the
(1984), no observable negative effects were likelihood of discussion after the testing, and (c)
found. Twenty-seven preschool students were conclude the testing with positive questions
asked to nominate peers to positive and negative (Mayeux, Underwood, & Risser, 2007).
characteristics. For example, a positive nomina- Another concern with sociometric assess-
tion might occur when the child was asked to ments is that the assessment relies on the
point to a picture of a peer with whom they like to participation of the entire classroom student
­
play. A negative nomination might occur when a body. Important information might be missed if a
child was asked to point to a photograph of a peer few students do not participate. It is not uncom-
with whom they would not like to play. Direct mon for a parent to refuse student participation.
observations of peer interactions were conducted
(a) 5 or 6 weeks before the assessment, (b) 5 or
6 weeks after the assessment was completed, and Behavior Rating Scales
(c) during the 10 min immediately following the
assessment. The results clearly showed that the Rating scales are a popular method of measuring
sociometric procedures did not alter patterns of behavior in the social sciences. Rating scales are
peer interaction. In addition, in the 10 min imme- available in many forms, wherein raters assign
diately following the assessment, no negative many responses a number on a scale (Johnston &
behavior was directed toward the children nomi- Pennypacker, 2009). Within the realm of child-
nated as least preferred. hood social skills, rating scales typically consist
Similarly, in a study by Mayeux, Underwood, of an itemized list of various responses being
and Risser in 2007, 91 third-grade students partici- measured, representative of the overall behavior
pated in a sociometric assessment. The investiga- of interest. Scales are typically completed by
tors interviewed the participants 15 weeks after the people familiar with the child who can provide
assessment and found that (a) the children did not information on their social skills. This may
report negative emotional reactions following the include parents, caretakers, or teachers. Each
assessment, (b) the children did not feel that they item requires the rater to indicate the likelihood
were treated differently by their peers after the that a child engages in a given response. This is
assessment, and (c) the children reported under- usually presented in a Likert-type fashion, where
standing their research rights. They understood the scale can range from 0 (never happens) to 5
their right to refuse to participate, to stop partici- (always happens). Most rating scales are com-
pating at any time, and that their responses would prised of about 25–75 items and are an efficient
be confidential. In a similar study by Iverson and means of assessing behavior, in that they gener-
Iverson in 1996, 82 children participated in a ally only take about 10–30 min to complete. The
sociometric assessment during the last week of ratings then may be compared to a normative
school that required them to associate peers with sample of children of a similar age and gender.
positive and negative characteristics. After their This comparison provides information on the
summer break, participants were interviewed extent to which the child’s social skills are
and asked to report on their experience with the adaptive or maladaptive and may assist in the
94 S.M. Vener et al.

decisions for any needed treatment planning elementary, and secondary ages. The SSRS consists
(Boisjoli & Matson, 2009). of three different forms that can be completed by
Although there are many measures that can be parents, teachers, and the child being assessed
used to assess social skills, this chapter includes (Boisjoli & Matson, 2009; Wilkins & Matson,
only a brief review of several popular rating 2007). Any of the three different versions of the
scales used to assess social behavior in children. SSRS can be completed within approximately
This is by no means an exhaustive review of child 15–25 min. The number of items across the three
social behavior scales. A more detailed descrip- different scales ranges from 34 to 57, and each
tion of behavior rating scales will be covered in a item is presented in a 3-point Likert scale. For
later chapter of this handbook. each item, the rater indicates how often the social
behavior occurs (never, sometimes, often) and the
importance of the social behavior in c­lassroom
 atson Evaluation of Social Skills
M success (not important, important, critical). The
with Youngsters (MESSY) three main scales of the SSRS are social skills,
problem behavior, and academic competence.
There are many rating scales that can be used to Teachers can complete all three scales, parents
assess social skills in children. One measure is can complete the social skills and problem behav-
the Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with ior scales, and the child can complete only the
Youngsters (MESSY; Matson, 1989). The social skills scale. The social skills scale assesses
MESSY is a scale comprised of 64 Likert-type cooperation, assertion, responsibility, empathy,
items that relates to the observable behavior of and self-control. The factors included in the prob-
children. Each item is rated on a scale from 1 to lem behavior scale include externalizing, internal-
5, with 1 being “not at all” and 5 being “very izing, and hyperactivity. The SSRS is considered
much.” This scale is available in two versions, one of the most inclusive social skills rating scales
one that can be completed by a teacher or adult and is a strong tool for predicting important social
who knows the child and the other that can be outcomes (Wilkins & Matson, 2007).
completed as a self-report by the child of interest.
The MESSY provides information on inappropri-
ate assertive or impulsive behavior, as well as Social Skills Improvement System (SSIS)
appropriate social behavior. Items on the scale
relate to behavior such as bullying, helping oth- The Social Skills Improvement System (SSIS;
ers, and engaging in conversation (Wilkins & Gresham & Elliott, 2008) is an updated version
Matson, 2007). The MESSY is an efficient tool of the SSRS. This measure assesses relationships
that takes about only 10–25 min to complete. that a child has with parents, teachers, and peers.
This scale can be very helpful in identifying Scales within this measure can be completed by
social skills deficits in children and can provide parents and teachers and also include a self-report
useful information in the development of goals scale. The SSIS is available in versions for differ-
for individualized education plans (IEPs), treat- ent age groups including preschool level (ages
ment programs, and educational curricula 3–5), elementary level (kindergarten to 6th
(Boisjoli & Matson, 2009). grade), and secondary level (7th to 12th grade).
Within the elementary level of the SSIS, 83 items
are divided among three different scales, includ-
Social Skills Rating System (SSRS) ing social skills, problem behaviors, and aca-
demic competence. The social skills scale
The Social Skills Rating System (SSRS; Gresham consists of 46 items that measure teacher and
& Elliott, 1990) is another widely used rating peer relations. The seven areas of focus within
scale that assesses social skills in children and is this scale include communication, cooperation,
available in different versions for preschool, assertion, responsibility, empathy, engagement,
Observational Methods 95

and self-control. Each item uses a 3-point rating  reschool and Kindergarten Behavior
P
scale where the likelihood of behavior is indi- Scales (PKBS)
cated with 0 as never occurring, 1 as sometimes,
and 2 as very often. In addition, teachers can rate The Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales
the extent to which the given behavior is impor- (PKBS; Merrell, 1994) is a multidimensional rat-
tant for success in the classroom by using the ing scale that consists of 76 items that measure
same 3-point scale. This component of the scale social skills and problem behavior in children
can then be used to guide plans for treatment pro- ages 3–6 years. Items on the measure are pre-
grams (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013). sented on a 4-point scale reflecting the frequency
of occurrence (Boisjoli & Matson, 2009). The
measure can be completed by adults who are
 ome and Community Social
H familiar with the child, such as a parent, teacher,
Behavior Scale (HCSBS) or daycare staff. The PKBS consists of two sub-
scales: the social skills scale and the problem
The SSBS-2 is the partner measure to the Home behavior scale. The social skills scale consists of
and Community Social Behavior Scale (HCSBS; 34 items and contains several subscales including
Merrell & Caldarella, 2002). The HCSBS is social cooperation, social interaction, and social
designed for the same age group and consists of independence. The social cooperation scale is
the same number of items and scaling format as made up of 12 items that focus on cooperative
the SSBS-2. This measure also assesses social and self-restraint behavior. The social interaction
competence and antisocial behavior but is to be scale consists of 11 items that look at social ini-
completed by parents and other home-based tiation behavior. Lastly, the social independence
informants. The items on the measure are very scale also consists of 11 items but assesses behav-
similar to those presented in the SSBS-2 but are ior related to gaining independence with peers
reworded to accommodate the home environment. (Merrell, 1996).
The information provided between the SSBS-2
and the HCSBS comprises a broad scope of the
child’s social behavior across settings and can  ocial Emotional Assets
S
be useful in the processes of screening, classi- and Resilience Scales (SEARS)
fying, and developing treatment programs
(Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013). The Social Emotional Assets and Resilience
Scales (SEARS; Merrell, 2011) is a system com-
posed of various scales that can be completed by
 aksman Social Skills Rating Scale
W parents (SEARS-P), teachers (SEARS-T), chil-
(WSSRS) dren (SEARS-C), and adolescents (SEARS-A).
The focus of these measures is to describe the
The Waksman Social Skills Rating Scale social and emotional strengths of children
(WSSRS; Waksman, 1985) is another rating between the ages of 5 and 18 years, in a way that
scale intended for children between kindergarten is socially valid. Each of the different measures
and 12th grades. The scale consists of 21 items has a 12-item form with ratings provided in a
presented on a 4-point Likert-type scale. The 4-point scale. Items on this form assess the child’s
WSSRS contains a major scale that assesses strengths in relationships with peers, empathy,
social skills and two subscales that assess aggres- responsibility, and awareness of thoughts and
sive and passive behavior. The scale is intended behavior (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013).
to be completed by teachers of the child of inter- The SEARS-C and the SEARS-A are self-­
est. Nevertheless, the WSSRS only measures report measures that will be described later in this
social skills deficits, and does not include proso- chapter. The SEARS-T consists of 41 items that
cial behavior (Demaray et al., 1995). assess factors such as responsibility, social com-
96 S.M. Vener et al.

petence, self-regulation, and empathy. Items that elementary version of the scale, plus 10 addi-
assess responsibility include “Is someone you tional items. This version of the SSCA contains
can rely on” and “Makes good decisions.” For the same subscale structure as the elementary
social competence, items may include “Asks oth- version as well as an empathy subscale that con-
ers for help when he/she needs it.” Examples of sists of six items (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013).
self-regulation items include “Can identify errors
in the way he/she thinks about things.” Lastly,
empathy contains items such as “Cares what hap- Self-Report Assessments
pens to people.” The SEARS-P is comprised of
39 items and is similar to the SEARS-T Although most rating skills for social behavior
(Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013). are completed by parents, teachers, and other
adults familiar with a child, there are some
assessments that do include a self-report measure
 alker-McConnell Scales of Social
W to be completed by the child of interest. Of the
Competence and School Adjustment behavior rating scales reviewed earlier, only two
(SSCA) include a self-report form: the SSIS and the
SEARS.
The Walker-McConnell Scales of Social As described earlier, the Social Skills
Competence and School Adjustment (SSCA; Improvement System (SSIS) assesses relation-
Walker & McConnell, 1995a, 1995b) is another ships that a child has with parents, teachers, and
measure that is completed by the child’s teachers peers. One component of this measure is a stu-
or other school-based personnel familiar with the dent self-report form that can be completed by
child. The SSCA is provided in two different ver- the child or adolescent. One version is designed
sions according to age group. The elementary ver- for children ages 8–12 years, who are in grades
sion is used with children between kindergarten 3–6. The other version is for adolescents between
and sixth grade. This version consists of 43 items the ages of 13 and 18 years, in grades 7–12.
that assess adaptive social-behavioral competen- Either version contains a social skills scale and a
cies in a school setting. Items are rated on a 5-point problem behavior scale and 11 subscales that
scale where 1 represents “never occurs” and 5 rep- assess areas such as communication, coopera-
resents “frequently occurs.” This version of the tion, responsibility, self-control, and bullying.
SSCA is comprised of three subscales. The first Items are presented on a 4-point scale, on which
subscale is the Teacher-Preferred Social Behavior students indicate “how true” items are to him or
scale and consists of 16 items that measure peer- her. The rankings include Not True, Little True,
related social behavior that teachers would deem A Lot True, and Very True. In addition to this set
valuable including empathy, sensitivity, self- of ratings, students can also indicate how impor-
restraint, and cooperative, mature relationships tant certain responses are in their relationships
with peers. The second subscale is the Peer- with other people. These ratings are presented on
Preferred Social Behavior scale and consists of 17 a scale from 0 to 2, where 0 indicates Not
items that measure peer-related social behavior Important, 1 indicates Important, and 2 indicates
that would be valuable to other children. The third Critical (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013). The inclu-
subscale is the School Adjustment Behavior scale sion of this latter set of ratings provides informa-
and includes 10 items that assess behavior impor- tion on what may be important to target for
tant in an academic setting such as following treatment with the student.
instructions, having good study habits, and work- The Social Emotional Assets and Resilience
ing in a manner that is beneficial to classroom Scales (SEARS) also includes a self-report mea-
management (Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013). sure to be completed by children (SEARS-C)
The adolescent version is used for 7th through between grades 3 and 6 and by adolescents
12th grade and includes 43 items from the (SEARS-A) between grades 7 and 12. As with
Observational Methods 97

the adult forms of this measure, the self-report social behavior, there are several limitations in
forms focus on the ratings of social and emo- the use of behavior rating scales. One limitation
tional strengths as perceived by the child about is that rating scales provide information on one’s
their own behavior. The structure and scoring is current levels of behavior. The ratings provided
very similar to the adult forms for the SEARS, at one moment in time may not reliably reflect
assessing areas such as empathy, self-regulation, the ongoing and dynamic nature of the behavior.
social competence, and responsibility (Whitcomb Therefore, follow-up evaluations are strongly
& Merrell, 2013). encouraged. Furthermore, ratings may vary
In addition to the scales reviewed in this according to the person completing the scale and,
chapter are other self-report measures that therefore, may not provide an accurate represen-
assess child social behavior. The List of Social tation of the social behavior. Ratings also may
Situation Problems (LSSP) (Spence, 1980) not provide information on the causes of the
assesses difficult social situations for children behavior or the environmental conditions sur-
and aims to identify behavior that requires treat- rounding the behavior, such as antecedents or
ment. This measure consists of 60 social prob- consequences (Demaray et al., 1995).
lem situations to which the child responds “yes” Furthermore, agreement across raters may not be
or “no” to whether or a not a situation is per- reliable given the fact that the items may cover a
ceived as a problem. Another is the Children’s long period of time during which the behavior
Self-Report Social Skills Scale (CS4) (Danielson may occur and the subjectivity of the scale values
& Phelps, 2003), which consists of 21 items (Johnston & Pennypacker, 2009). Finally, one
presented on a 5-point scale. This measure must take caution with the biases that may arise
assesses prosocial skills and poor social skills in the use of scales. For example, a rater’s opin-
(Boisjoli & Matson, 2009). ion of the child may influence the ratings that
Self-report measures can provide some useful they provide. Instead of providing objective rat-
information, but they should be interpreted with ings on the child’s behavior, they may provide
caution. When assessing a child who displays ratings based on their judgment of the subject as
maladaptive social behavior, the child may not being a “good” or “bad” child. If the rating scale
provide truly objective ratings or be able to accu- is a self-report measure, the objectivity in rating
rately report about their own behavior. It may not one’s own behavior may be difficult. The subject
be practical to use this form of assessment with may provide ratings believed to be desirable
children who are unable to identify the strengths answers, or the subject may not be completely
and weaknesses of their social behavior, or with honest in the answers. Therefore, rating scales
children who may present cognitive delays should not be used as the exclusive measure for
(Boisjoli & Matson, 2009). It is also possible that assessing social skills but, instead, should be
the child may report their own behavior more used in combination with other methods of
positively than would an outside observer assessment.
(Whitcomb & Merrell, 2013). More research is
needed in developing self-report measures for
children’s social behavior and, therefore, these Conclusion
measures should be used carefully.
Social skills enable children to interact with con-
versation recipients within their environment.
Limitations of Rating Scales The assessment of social behavior, and the iden-
tification of specific skill deficits, allows for
Multiple methods of assessment are required to needs to be targeted for intervention.
fully grasp the strengths and weaknesses of one’s This chapter identified the different assess-
social skills repertoire. Although rating scales do ment strategies that have been used to assess
provide useful information with regard to one’s social behavior. Direct behavioral observation
98 S.M. Vener et al.

and indirect assessments such as interview meth- from 1988 to 2010. Clinical Psychology Review, 31,
767–785. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2011.03.008
ods, sociometric techniques, self-reports, and use
Danielson, C. K., & Phelps, C. R. (2003). The assess-
of behavior rating scales were discussed. Because ment of children’s social skills through self-report:
each strategy has its strengths and weaknesses, A potential screening instrument for classroom use.
multiple methods of assessment can and should Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and
Development, 35, 218–229.
be used to assess social behavior.
Demaray, M. K., Ruffalo, S. L., Carlson, J., Busse, R. T.,
Regardless of whether the child identifies his or Olson, A. E., McManus, S. M., & Leventhal, A. L.
her own deficits, a parent reports about his or her (1995). Social skills assessment: A comparative evalu-
child’s social challenges, or a peer rates a child’s ation of six published rating scales. School Psychology
Review, 24, 648–671.
strengths and weaknesses in comparison to that of
Dirks, M. A., Treat, T. A., & Weersing, V. R. (2007).
his classmates, the information is important. The Integrating theoretical, measurement, and interven-
way in which a child is perceived by others, and the tion models of youth social competence. Clinical
way in which a child perceives him or herself, Psychology Review, 27, 327–347. doi:10.1016/j.
cpr.2006.11.002
greatly impacts the quality of an interaction.
Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J. E., Burks, V. S., Bates, J. E.,
Pettit, G. S., Fontaine, R., & Price, J. M. (2003).
Peer rejection and social information-processing fac-
References tors in the development of aggressive behavior prob-
lems in children. Child Development, 74, 374–393.
doi:10.1111/1467-8624.7402004
Ballard, K., & Glynn, T. (1975). Behavioral self-­
Elliott, S. N., Sheridan, S. M., & Gresham, F. M.
management in story writing with elementary school
(1989). Assessing and treating social skills defi-
children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 8,
cits: A case study for the scientist-practitioner.
387–398. doi:10.1901/jaba.1975.8-387
Journal of School Psychology, 27, 197–222.
Bellack, A. S., Rozensky, R., & Schwartz, J. (1974). A
doi:10.1016/0022-4405(89)90007-1
comparison of two forms of self-monitoring in a behav-
Gast, D. L., & Ledford, J. R. (2014). Single case research
ioral weight reduction program. Behavior Therapy, 5,
methodology: Applications in special education
523–530. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(74)80042-0
and behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York, NY:
Boisjoli, J. A., & Matson, J. L. (2009). General methods
Routledge.
of assessment. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Social behavior
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social skills rat-
and skills in children (pp. 61–75). New York, NY:
ing system. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
Springer. doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-0234-4
Service.
Cavior, N., & Marabotto, C. M. (1976). Monitoring
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (2008). The social skills
verbal behaviors in a dyadic interaction. Journal
improvement system (SSIS): Performance screening
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 44, 68–76.
guide. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson.
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.44.1.68
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003).
Christensen, L., Young, K. R., & Marchant, M. (2004).
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review.
The effects of a peer-mediated positive behavior
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185.
support program on socially appropriate classroom
doi:10.1901/jaba.2003.36-147
behavior. Education and Treatment of Children, 27,
Harper, J. M., Iwata, B. A., & Camp, E. M. (2013).
199–234.
Assessment and treatment of social avoidance.
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007).
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 147–160.
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle
doi:10.1002/jaba.18
River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Hayvren, M., & Hymel, S. (1984). Ethical issues in socio-
Cordier, R., Speyer, R., Chen, Y.-W., Wilkes-Gillan, S.,
metric testing: Impact of sociometric measures on
Brown, T., Bourke-Taylor, H., … Leicht, A. (2015).
interaction behavior. Developmental Psychology, 20,
Evaluating the psychometric quality of social skills
844–849. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.20.5.844
measures: A systematic review. PloS One, 10,
Johnston, J. M., & Pennypacker, H. S. (2009). Strategies
e0132299. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0132299
and tactics of behavioral research (3rd ed.). New York,
Critchfield, T. S., & Vargas, E. A. (1991). Self-­
NY: Routledge.
recording, instructions, and public self-graphing:
Kanfer, F. H. (1970). Self-monitoring: Methodological
Effects of swimming in the absence of coach ver-
limitations and clinical applications. Journal of
bal interaction. Behavior Modification, 15, 95–112.
Counseling and Clinical Psychology, 35, 148–152.
doi:10.1177/01454455910151006
doi:10.1037/h0029874
Crowe, L. M., Beauchamp, M. H., Catroppa, C., &
Kazdin, A. E. (1974). Reactive self-monitoring: The effects
Anderson, V. (2011). Social function assessment tools
of response desirability, goal setting, and feedback.
for children and adolescents: A systematic review
Observational Methods 99

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, Nelson, R. O., Lipinski, D. P., & Boykin, R. A. (1978).
704–716. doi:10.1037/h0037050 The effects of self-recorders’ training and the obtru-
Kazdin, A. E. (1979). Unobtrusive measures in behavioral siveness of the self-recording device on the accuracy
assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 12, and reactivity of self-monitoring. Behavior Therapy,
713–724. doi:10.1901/jaba.1979.12-713 9, 200–208. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(78)80105-1
Kirby, K. C., Fowler, S. A., & Baer, D. M. (1991). Richman, D. M., Wacker, D. P., & Winborn, L. (2001).
Reactivity in self-recording: Obtrusiveness of record- Response efficiency during functional communica-
ing procedure and peer comments. Journal of Applied tion training: Effects of effort on response allocation.
Behavior Analysis, 24, 487–498. doi:10.1901/ Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 73–76.
jaba.1991.24-487 doi:10.1901/jaba.2001.34-73
Leaf, J. B., Taubman, M., Milne, C., Dale, J., Townley-­ Roscoe, E. M., Iwata, B. A., & Zhou, L. (2013).
Cochran, D., Tsuji, K., … McEachin, J. (2016). Assessment and treatment of chronic hand mouthing.
Teaching social communication skills using a cool Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 181–198.
versus not cool procedure plus role-playing and a doi:10.1002/jaba.14
social skills taxonomy. Education and Treatment of Singleton, L. C., & Asher, S. R. (1977). Peer pref-
Children, 39, 44–63. erences and social interaction among third-­
Matson, J. L. (1989). The Matson evaluation of social grade children in an integrated school district.
skills with youngsters: Manual. Orland Park, IL: Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 330–336.
International Diagnostics Systems, Incorporated. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.69.4.330
Matson, J. L. (2009). Social behavior and Spence, S. H. (1980). Social skills training with children
skills in children. New York, NY: Springer. and adolescents: A counsellor’s manual. Windsor:
doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-0234-4 NFER Publishing.
Mayer, G. R., Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Wallace, M. (2012). Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technol-
Behavior analysis for lasting change (2nd ed.). ogy of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior
Cornwall-on-Hudson, NY: Sloan Publishing. Analysis, 10, 349–367. doi:10.1901/jaba.1977.10-349
Mayeux, L., Underwood, M. K., & Risser, S. D. (2007). Wade, D. T. (2004). Editorial: Assessment, measurement
Perspectives on the ethics of sociometrics research and data collection tools. Clinical Rehabilitation, 18,
with children: How children, peers, and teachers help 233–237. doi:10.1191/0269215504cr183ed
to inform the debate. Merrell-Palmer Quarterly, 53, Waksman, S. A. (1985). The Waksman social skills rating
53–78. doi:10.1353/mpq.2007.0002 scale. Portland, OR: ASIEP Education.
Merrell, K. W. (1994). Preschool and kindergarten Walker, H. M., & McConnell, S. R. (1995a). Walker-­
behavior scales: Test manual. Brandon, VT: Clinical McConnell scale of social competence and school
Psychology Publishing Company. adjustment (adolescent version). Belmont, CA:
Merrell, K. W. (1996). Social-emotional problems Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.
in early childhood: New directions in conceptu- Walker, H. M., & McConnell, S. R. (1995b). Walker-­
alization, assessment, and treatment. Education McConnell scale of social competence and school
and Treatment of Children, 20, 132–145. adjustment (elementary version). Belmont, CA:
doi:10.1177/105381519602000205 Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.
Merrell, K. (2001). Assessment of children’s social skills: Whitcomb, S. A., & Merrell, K. W. (2013). Behavioral,
Recent developments, best practices, and new direc- social, and emotional assessment of children and ado-
tions. Exceptionality, 9, 3–18. doi:10.1080/09362835 lescents (4th ed.). New York, NY: Routledge.
.2001.9666988 Wilkins, J., & Matson, J. L. (2007). Social skills. In J. L.
Merrell, K. W. (2011). Social emotional assets and resil- Matson (Ed.), Handbook of assessment in persons
iency scale. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment with intellectual disability (pp. 321–363). London:
Resources. Elsevier.
Merrell, K. W., & Caldarella, P. (2002). Home and com- Yeates, K. O., Bigler, E. D., Dennis, M., Gerhardt, C. A.,
munity social behavior scales (HCSBS). Eugene, OR: Rubin, K. H., Stancin, T., … Vannatta, K. (2007).
Assessment-Intervention Resources. Social outcomes in childhood brain disorder: A heuris-
Merrell, K. W., & Gimpel, G. A. (2014). Social skills tic integration of social neuroscience and developmen-
of children and adolescents. New York, NY: tal psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 535–556.
Psychology Press. doi:10.1037/0033–2909.133.3.535
Checklists and Scaling Methods

W. Jason Peters and Johnny L. Matson

accepted by their peers is said to be socially


Introduction skilled. Lastly, the social validity model defines
social skills as those skills which allow the indi-
Social interactions are encountered by individuals vidual to achieve socially important outcomes.
numerous times throughout the day in various One important distinction regarding social
locations and settings such as at their place of skills is the difference between it and social com-
employment, home, or school. Further, they petence. According to Gresham (1986), social
involve the individual exhibiting social skills so skills and social competence are two different
as to achieve a successful interaction. According constructs where social skills refer to specific
to Crowe, Beauchamp, Catroppa, and Anderson behaviors exhibited by the individual to perform
(2011), the development of fulfilling social rela- adequately on a social task, while social compe-
tionships, which are important for both psycho- tence refers to how well the individual has per-
logical and physical well-being, relies on the use formed that task and is based upon the judgements
of appropriate social skills. While adequate or of others (i.e., peers, parents, teachers, etc.). Said
poor social skills may be easily identifiable, defin- another way, social competence refers to behav-
ing what social skills are is difficult as a number ior that is successful in initiating and maintaining
of definitions for social skills exist throughout the social interactions, while social skills are the dis-
literature on assessment and treatment (Merrell & crete behaviors involved in achieving social com-
Gimpel, 1998). According to Gresham and Elliott petence (Foster & Ritchey, 1979).
(1984), three main conceptual definitions, or In the literature, the importance of assessing
models, of social skills exist including behavioral, the social skills of children is well documented as
peer acceptance, and social validity. According to impairments in social skills are associated with a
the behavioral model, social skills are learned number of undesirable outcomes. For example,
behaviors that encounter positive responses while according to Dodge et al. (2003), children with
helping to avoid negative responses which allow poor social skills were more likely to experience
for the appropriate interaction with others. In the peer rejection as well as engage in increased
peer acceptance model, the individual who is amounts of antisocial behavior. Additionally,
children with deficits in social skills are more
likely to experience other difficulties such as sub-
W.J. Peters (*) • J.L. Matson stance abuse or depression (Greene et al., 1999;
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA Sato, Ishikawa, Arai, & Sakano, 2005). Further,
e-mail: wjpeters1992@gmail.com social skills impairments are among the defining

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 101


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_7
102 W.J. Peters and J.L. Matson

characteristics for some neurodevelopmental Assessment Methods


disabilities including autism spectrum disorder
(ASD; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) Rating Scales
and are strongly associated with other forms of
psychopathology including attention-deficit/ Rating scales are a popular and widely used
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Storebø et al., method for assessing social skills. Typically,
2011) and schizophrenia (Meyer & Kurtz, 2009). rating scales are comprised of items representa-
Therefore, due to the demonstrated association tive of a particular construct. Ratings of items on
between impairments in social skills and undesir- rating scales assessing social skills are typically
able outcomes, it is of the upmost importance that based on how often an individual engages in a
accurate assessment of those deficits takes place particular behavior (e.g., 0 = “never”; 1 = “some-
so as to facilitate appropriate interventions. times”; 2 = “often”; 3 = “always”). According to
Social skills assessment and treatment have Matson and Wilkins (2009), rating scales consist
an extensive history dating back to one of the of between 25 and 75 items, on average, and take
earliest known interventions involving asser- about 10–30 min to administer. For most rating
tiveness training for shy men (McFall & scales, normative data based on age and gender
Marston, 1970). Due to the demonstrated suc- are used to evaluate an individual’s score.
cess of this study and others like it in identify- Specifically, social skills rating scales for chil-
ing targets and implementing effective dren generally include parents or teachers (i.e.,
intervention, the social skills of other groups, an individual who knows them well and can
including children, were soon targeted for report on their social behaviors) as informants.
study. Some of these early studies on social Social skills rating scales generally assess both
skills assessment focused on individuals with adaptive and maladaptive skills, and, by doing so,
more severe forms of psychopathology. useful information for planning intervention can
Research on social skills assessment and treat- be provided.
ment, therefore, became an area of clinical One advantage in using rating scales is the
interest due to the relationship between unde- ability to rate less frequent behaviors.
sirable outcomes and interpersonal difficulties Additionally, rating scales, due to their effi-
(Matson & Ollendick, 1988). Various clinical ciency, can be ideally used in settings where
populations have been subject to research on time or resources may be limited. It is important
social skills assessment including individuals to note, however, that the use of rating scales is
with ASD (Matson & Wilkins, 2007), intellec- subject to limitations. For example, information
tual disabilities (Kearney & Healy, 2011), regarding an individual’s present level of behav-
ADHD (Storebø et al., 2011), schizophrenia ior is measured by rating scales and may not
(Meyer & Kurtz, 2009), anxiety (Crawford & accurately or reliably reflect the changing nature
Manassis, 2011), depression and bipolar disor- of behavior, suggesting the need for follow-up
der (Segrin, 2000), emotional disturbance evaluations. Additionally, ratings may vary
(Milsom & Glanville, 2010), learning disabili- across informants which may provide an inac-
ties (Agaliotis & Kalyva, 2008), and typically curate representation of the individual’s social
developing children (Matson, Rotatori, & skills. Lastly, the use of rating scales may invite
Helsel, 1983). One of the earliest approaches the influence of a rater’s biases or opinions
used by researchers to identify targets for inter- regarding the individual being assessed and
vention for these groups was role-play assess- therefore providing overly subjective ratings. In
ments. More recently, however, assessment sum, it is strongly encouraged that evaluators
methods such as informant-based rating scales use rating scales in combination with other
and direct observation of behavior have been assessment methods when evaluating children’s
developed and used to evaluate social skills in social skills. What follows is a review for a
children. number of different rating scales.
Checklists and Scaling Methods 103

 atson Evaluation of Social Skills


M (i.e., hostile, adaptive/appropriate, and inappropri-
in Youngsters ately assertive/overconfident). These subscales
were determined based on an exploratory factor
The Matson Evaluation of Social Skills in analysis (Matson, Neal, Worley, Kozlowski, &
Youngsters (MESSY; Matson, 1988) is an Fodstad, 2012). Additionally, each item is rated
informant-­based measure of observable behav- on the same 5-point Likert-type scale as is the
iors representative of social skills in children original. One key difference between the two is
between the ages of 4 and 18. There are two that the MESSY-II shifted its norms down to
forms for the MESSY, self-report form and a par- include children between the ages of 2 and 16.
ent/teacher report form. The self-report form of Additionally, the MESSY-II currently does not
the MESSY is comprised of 62 items, and the have a self-report form due to difficulties popula-
parent/teacher report form of the MESSY con- tions frequently administered the MESSY may
sists of 64 items. Each item for both forms is have with poor insight (e.g., ASD; Matson et al.,
rated on a 5-point Likert-type scale (i.e., ranging 2012). Research regarding the psychometric
from 1 = “not at all” to 5 = “very much”) based properties of the MESSY-II has demonstrated
on how often the child engages in the behavior. good to excellent internal consistency, excellent
The self-report version of the MESSY is com- split-half reliability, and moderate to high inter-
prised of five subscales (i.e., appropriate social rater reliability (Matson et al., 2010, 2013).
skills, inappropriate assertiveness, impulsive, Additional research has also demonstrated good
overconfident, and jealous), while the parent/ to strong convergent and divergent validity for
teacher report version consists of only two sub- the MESSY-II with subscales of the Behavior
scales (i.e., assertiveness/impulsiveness and Assessment System for Children, Second Edition
appropriate social skills; Matson et al., 1983). (BASC-2; Matson et al., 2010; Reynolds &
Several studies have reported on the reliability Kamphaus, 2004).
and validity of the MESSY, indicating a high
internal reliability and a moderate to high test-­
retest reliability, as well as high internal consis- Social Skills Rating System
tency reliabilities across different age groups
(Matson et al., 1983, 2010). Further, the MESSY The Social Skills Rating System (SSRS; Gresham
has been demonstrated for use among children & Elliott, 1990) is a norm-referenced rating scale
with hearing and/or visual impairments as well as used to assess social skills, problem behavior,
children with ASD (Matson, Heinze, Helsel, and academic competence in children ranging
Kapperman, & Rotatori, 1986; Matson, Horovitz, from preschool age to secondary school. There
Mahan, & Fodstad, 2013; Matson, Macklin, & are three separate forms for the SSRS including
Helsel, 1985). The MESSY has also been trans- parent, teacher, and student self-report.
lated into several languages including Spanish Additionally, each form of the SSRS is further
(Méndez, Hidalgo, & Inglés, 2002), Chinese subdivided into separate versions for different
(Kee-Lee, 1997), Turkish (Bacanli & Erdogan, age groups (i.e., preschool, elementary, and sec-
2003), Dutch (Prins, 1997), Hindi (Sharma, ondary); however, there is no self-report version
Sigafoos, & Carroll, 2000), as well as several for the preschool age group. The SSRS-Parent
others with psychometric properties reported for (SSRS-P) and SSRS-Teacher (SSRS-T) forms
several of them. are comprised of 49 items each, and the SSRS-­
The MESSY recently underwent an update Student form (SSRS-S) is comprised of 34 items.
and is now the MESSY-II (Matson, 2010). Like All items across the different forms of the SSRS
the original MESSY, the MESSY-II is intended to are rated on a 3-point Likert-type scale based on
measure both appropriate and inappropriate the perceived frequency with which the individ-
social behaviors in children. The MESSY-II con- ual being assessed engages in the behavior (i.e.,
sists of 64 items, divided into three subscales 0 = “never”; 1 = “sometimes”; 2 = “very often”).
104 W.J. Peters and J.L. Matson

Both the SSRS-P and SSRS-T contain two scales prised of a higher number of factors as compared
including a social skills scale and a problem to the SSRS. The social skills scale on the
behaviors scale with the SSRS-T containing a SSIS-RS consists of seven factors including
third academic competence scale. The SSRS-S cooperation, assertion, responsibility, self-­
contains only the social skills scale. The social control, communication, empathy, and engage-
skills scale is comprised of four factors including ment. The problem behaviors scale consists of
empathy, self-control, cooperation, and assertion. five factors including externalizing, internalizing,
Factors on the problem behaviors scale include bullying, hyperactivity/inattention, and autism
externalizing, internalizing, and hyperactivity. spectrum. Each item on the SSIS-RS is rated on a
Analyses regarding the psychometric proper- 4-point Likert-type scale, ranging from
ties have demonstrated adequate reliability and 0 = “never” to 3 = “almost always.” Additionally,
validity across all forms of the SSRS (Gresham ratings are based on the perceived frequency of
& Elliott, 1990). Reliability estimates for the pre- the behavior being assessed. Research has dem-
school version of the SSRS are good; however, onstrated adequate evidence regarding reliability
researchers have failed to replicate the original and validity for the SSIS-RS (Gresham, Elliott,
social skills scale factor solution in a low-income Cook, Vance, & Kettler, 2010; Gresham, Elliott,
sample of children (Fantuzzo, Manz, & & Kettler, 2010). Additionally, comparisons
McDermott, 1998). Research regarding the ele- between the SSRS and SSIS-RS revealed strong
mentary versions of the SSRS has demonstrated relationships between the two, suggesting good
moderate to high internal consistency, test-retest convergent validity and comparability (Gresham,
reliability, and interrater agreement, as well as Elliott, Vance, & Cook, 2011).
good convergent and divergent validity (Diperna
& Volpe, 2005; Gresham & Elliott, 1990). Similar
to the MESSY, the SSRS has been translated into School Social Behavior Scales
a number of different languages and studied
across a number of different populations. The The School Social Behavior Scales (SSBS;
SSRS has been translated into Spanish (Jurado, Merrell, 1993) is a rating scale primarily used in
Cumba-Avilés, Collazo, & Matos, 2006), school settings by teachers to assess the social
Japanese (Van Horn & Tamase, 2001), Dutch behavior of children and adolescents. Recently,
(Van der Oord et al., 2005), Iranian (Shahim, the SSBS underwent a revision and is now the
2004), and Farsi (Eslami, Mazaheri, Mostafavi, SSBS, Second Edition (SSBS-2; Merrell, 2002b).
Abbasi, & Noroozi, 2014). Additionally, the The SSBS-2 consists of 64 items equally divided
SSRS has been researched and used among the into two separate scales labeled Social
following populations: ADHD (Van der Oord Competence, which assesses positive behaviors,
et al., 2005), spina bifida (Lemanek, Jones, & and Antisocial Behavior, which assesses com-
Lieberman, 2000), and neurofibrosis (Barton & mon social-related problem behaviors. Similar to
North, 2004). the original SSBS, the SSBS-2 is primarily used
The Social Skills Improvement System – by teachers to assess social competence and anti-
Rating Scales (SSIS-RS; Gresham & Elliott, social behavior of children and adolescents
2008) is a re-normed and revised version of the between the ages of 5 and 18 in the school envi-
SSRS. Like the SSRS, it assesses social skills, ronment. Each item for the SSBS-2 is rated on a
problem behaviors, and academic competence 5-point Likert-type scale (i.e., ranging from
based on ratings from parents, teachers, and stu- 1 = “never” to 5 = “frequently”), based on the fre-
dent self-report across age groups ranging from quency with which the individual being assessed
preschool (i.e., ages 3–5) to secondary (i.e., ages engaged in the behavior over the last 3 months
13–18). The SSIS-RS contains the same scales (Merrell, 2002b). Both peer- and teacher-related
(i.e., social skills, problem behaviors, and aca- behaviors are reflected on the items of the SSBS-­
demic competence); however, each scale is com- 2. According to Merrell (2002b), both forms of
Checklists and Scaling Methods 105

social behaviors are important for achieving a parent rating form of the teacher-based SSBS
social success in a school environment. for cross-informant purposes (Merrell &
Psychometric analyses for the full SSBS-2 as Caldarella, 1999). The main difference between
well as the Social Competence and Antisocial the HCSBS and the SBSS, aside from the infor-
Behavior subscales have demonstrated adequate mant, is that certain items on the HCSBS have
psychometric properties including adequate been reworded to assess behaviors reflective of
internal consistency, 1-week test-retest reliability the home and community environments as
ranging between 0.86 and 0.94, and interrater opposed to the school.
reliability ranging between 0.53 and 0.71 Previous research on the HCSBS has demon-
(Froeschle, Smith, & Ricard, 2007; Merrell, strated good psychometric properties. According
2008; Raimundo et al., 2012). Additionally, pre- to Lund and Merrell (2001), a national standard-
vious research has demonstrated both good con- ization for the HCSBS revealed that the overall
vergent and discriminant validity with other scale has strong internal consistency and that the
rating scales (Cummings, Kaminski, & Merrell, two subscales are strong measures of social com-
2008). According to Raimundo et al. (2012), the petence and antisocial behavior. Additionally, the
SSBS-2 has been translated into several lan- two subscales have strong internal consistency
guages. However, there are only two references coefficients (i.e., above 0.9; Merrell & Caldarella,
in the literature including a Turkish version of the 1999). Regarding interrater and test-retest reliabil-
full SSBS-2 (Yukay-Yuksel, 2009) and a ity, previous research indicates that the HCSBS is
Portuguese version for the Social Competence good to excellent (Merrell & Caldarella, 2002).
scale only (Raimundo et al., 2012). Both studies Further, the HCSBS has been demonstrated to
reported adequate psychometric properties for have good convergent and divergent validity with
the translated versions of the SSBS-2. Lastly, the various scales from the SSRS, Child Behavior
SSBS-2 has been used among a number of differ- Checklist, Conners Parent Rating Scale –
ent populations including a Spanish community Revised-Short form, and the BASC (Merrell,
sample (Molinuevo, Bonillo, Pardo, Doval, & Streeter, Boelter, Caldarella, & Gentry, 2001).
Torrubia, 2010), adolescent females in a school-­ Lastly, construct validity for the HCSBS has been
based drug prevention program (Froeschle et al., demonstrated for several populations including
2007), and kindergarten children who were born children with ADHD, learning disabilities, and
both extremely preterm and extremely low birth emotional-behavioral disorders (Lund & Merrell,
weight (Scott et al., 2012). 2001; Merrell & Boelter, 2001).

 ome and Community Social


H  reschool and Kindergarten Behavior
P
Behavior Scales Scales

The Home and Community Social Behavior The Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales
Scales (HCSBS; Merrell & Caldarella, 2002) is a (PKBS; Merrell, 1994) is a rating scale designed
rating scale designed to be used by parents to to assess the social skills and problem behaviors
screen and assess social competencies and behav- of children aged 3–6 years with parents or teach-
ior problems of children aged 5–18 years. The ers serving as the informant. The PKBS under-
HCSBS consists of 65 total items divided into went a recent revision and is now the PKBS,
two separate subscales measuring social compe- Second Edition (PKBS-2; Merrell, 2002a). The
tence and antisocial behavior. Each item is scored PKBS-2 is comprised of 76 total items divided
on a 5-point Likert-type scale (i.e., ranging from into two scales measuring social skills and prob-
1 = “never” to 5 = “frequently”) based on the fre- lem behavior. Based on exploratory and confir-
quency a child engages in the behavior being matory factor analyses, these two scales are
assessed. Originally, the HCSBS was designed as further subdivided into three subscales within the
106 W.J. Peters and J.L. Matson

social skills domain (i.e., social cooperation, Likert-type scale based on the social competence
social interaction, and social independence) and and maturity level of the child relative to an aver-
two subscales within the problem behavior age child of the same age (i.e., ranging from
domain (i.e., externalizing problems and inter- 1 = “very much less mature than the average
nalizing problems). Each item is scored on a child this age” to 7 = “very much more mature
4-point Likert-type scale (i.e., ranging from than the average child this age”). The PSMAT
0 = “never” to 3 = “often”), based on the fre- was designed to take advantage of teachers’
quency with which the child being assessed experiences with students with differing levels of
engages in the behavior. social competence. Further, items on the PSMAT
Previous research on the PKBS-2 has demon- are designed to assess skills such as group entry,
strated good psychometric properties. According peer leadership, and interactive social play.
to Merrell (2002a), the PKBS-2 demonstrates Research on the PSMAT indicates that it has
good internal reliability, ranging between 0.81 excellent internal consistency, as well as good
and 0.97, as well as good cross-informant agree- interrater and test-retest reliability (Fink, Rosnay,
ment. Additionally, test-retest reliability at Peterson, & Slaughter, 2013). Additionally,
3 weeks for the PKBS-2 ranged between 0.58 according to Fink et al. (2013), the PSMAT dem-
and 0.87. Convergent and divergent validity has onstrated good convergent validity with the SSRS
also been demonstrated for the PKBS-2 through as well as peer ratings of likability. Overall, the
comparisons with other widely used rating scales PSMAT is a quick and easy to use measure for
such as the SSRS, the Achenbach Teacher Report assessing the social competence of children with
Form, and the Adjustment Scales for Children sound psychometric properties.
and Adolescents (Canivez & Bordenkircher,
2002; Merrell, 2002a). Both editions of the PKBS
have been translated into other languages includ- Social Competence Inventory
ing Spanish (Carney & Merrell, 2002), German
(Al Awmleh & Woll, 2013), and Portuguese The Social Competence Inventory (SCI; Rydell,
(Major & Seabra-Santos, 2014). The PKBS and Hagekull, & Bohlin, 1997) is an informant-based
PKBS-2 have also been validated for use among questionnaire that is designed to assess social
children with ADHD (Carney & Merrell, 2005) skills and behaviors related to prosocial orienta-
as well as ASD (Major, Seabra-Santos, & tion and social initiation. The SCI can be filled
Albuquerque, 2017; Wang, Sandall, Davis, & out by either parents or teachers and is comprised
Thomas, 2011). One of the strengths of the of 25 items, each rated on a 5-point Likert-type
PKBS-2 is that it is one of only a few standard- scale. According to Rydell et al. (1997), each
ized rating scales designed specifically to assess item is rated based on how well the item applies
the socio-emotional characteristics of young chil- to the child (i.e., ranging from 1 = “doesn’t apply
dren that has sound psychometric properties and at all” to 5 = “applies very well to the child”).
makes use of content created from developmen- Based on factor analyses, the SCI is divided into
tally specific constructs appropriate for an early two scales, the Prosocial Orientation scale and
childhood population (Carney & Merrell, 2002). the Social Initiative scale, with 17 and 8 items
comprising each scale, respectively. Research
regarding the psychometric properties of the SCI
Peer Social Maturity Scale indicates that it demonstrates excellent internal
consistency and reliability (Rydell et al., 1997).
The Peer Social Maturity Scale (PSMAT; Additionally, the SCI demonstrates good interrater
Peterson, Slaughter, & Paynter, 2007) is a brief reliability as well as convergent validity with
seven-item, teacher-based measure of children’s observed peer behavior. Taken together, this
performance in peer group interactions. Teachers suggests that the SCI is a reliable and valid measure
are instructed to rate each child using a 7-point of social skills and behavior in children.
Checklists and Scaling Methods 107

Social-Emotional Assets Likewise, strong internal consistency (0.98)


and Resilience Scale has also been found for the SEARS-T (Merrell,
Cohn, et al., 2011). Convergent validity with
The Social-Emotional Assets and Resilience other rating scales, such as the SSRS, as well as
Scale (SEARS; Merrell, 2011) is an informant-­ construct validity has also been established for
based measure that assesses positive social-­ the SEARS. Overall, the SEARS appears to be a
emotional competencies of children from good strength-based measure of child and adoles-
kindergarten to 12th grade. The SEARS consists cents’ social behaviors.
of four separate forms: the SEARS, parent form
(SEARS-P); the SEARS, teacher form
(SEARS-T); the SEARS, child form (SEARS-C); Vineland Social-Emotional Early
and the SEARS, adolescent form (SEARS-A). Childhood Scales
Both the SEARS-P and the SEARS-T are based
on parent and teacher report, respectively, while The Vineland Social-Emotional Early Childhood
the SEARS-C and SEARS-A are based on self-­ Scales (Vineland SEEC Scales; Sparrow, Balla, &
report for children from 3rd to 6th grade and 7th Cicchetti, 1998) is a semi-structured interview
to 12th grade, respectively. Factor analyses were used to assess children’s social and emotional
conducted on all forms of the SEARS, from functioning from birth to age 5 years, 11 months.
which subscales were created, with the exception The interview is generally conducted by a profes-
of the SEARS-C. The SEARS-P is comprised of sional with a background in child development
39 items, each rated on a 4-point Likert-type with a parent or caregiver familiar with the child’s
scale (i.e., ranging from 0 = “never” to behavior. The Vineland SEEC Scales consists of
3 = “always”). Parent ratings are based on how 122 total items derived from the Socialization
true the item is for the child over the previous Domain of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales
6 months. The SEARS-P is divided into three (Sparrow, Balla, Cicchetti, Harrison, & Doll,
subscales labeled Self-Regulation/Responsibility, 1984) and is divided into three subscales:
Social Competence, and Empathy (Merrell, Interpersonal Relationships (44 items), Play and
Felver-Gant, and Tom, 2011). The SEARS-T is Leisure Time (44 items), and Coping Skills (34
comprised of 41 items, each rated in a similar items). Each item is scored based on a specific
fashion to the SEARS-P, with the only difference scoring criterion, with scores ranging from 0 to 2.
being that teachers are used as the informants. A score of 0 indicates that the child “never” per-
Subscales of the SEARS-T include Responsibility, forms the behavior, a score of 1 indicates that they
Social Competence, Self-Regulation, and “sometimes or partially” perform the behavior,
Empathy (Merrell, Cohn, & Tom, 2011). The and a score of 2 indicates that the child “usually”
SEARS-C and SEARS-A are both comprised of performs the behavior. According to the test
35 items and are rated in a similar fashion to the manual (Sparrow et al., 1998), the Vinland SEEC
other forms of the SEARS. Additionally, the Scales demonstrate good psychometric properties
SEARS-A is comprised of the same subscales as such as good internal consistency and reliability,
the SEARS-T (Cohn, Merrell, Felver-Grant, as well as good interrater and test-retest reliability.
Tom, & Endrulat, 2009). Short forms of each ver- Additionally, adequate construct validity as well
sion of the SEARS have also been developed as moderate convergent validity with other devel-
(Nese et al., 2012). opmental scales such as the Battelle Developmental
Strong psychometric properties have been Inventory was indicated. Overall, the Vineland
demonstrated for the SEARS. According to SEEC scales appear to be a helpful measure of
Merrell, Cohn, et al. (2011) and Merrell, Felver-­ young children’s social and emotional behaviors
Gant, and Tom (2011), the SEARS-P has a strong and can be used in clinical and educational settings
internal consistency of 0.96 in addition to strong for the screening and identification of potential
interrater reliability between pairs of parents. developmental concerns or delays.
108 W.J. Peters and J.L. Matson

Direct Behavior Observation evant activities with typical social partners in a


natural environment. After the individual is
In addition to rating scales, a popular alternative observed engaging in two or more social inter-
method for assessing social skills in children is actions, the evaluator rates each skill on the ESI
through the use of direct behavior observation. on a 4-point rating scale which indicates the
According to Nock and Kurtz (2005), the direct individual’s level of competence in that skill. The
observation of behavior can provide information, ratings of each skill are then weighted according
through the identification and evaluation of ante- to their relative difficulty.
cedents and consequences, regarding the cause or Researchers have been able to demonstrate
purpose of a given target behavior. Additionally, that the ESI has adequate psychometric proper-
direct behavior observation can provide objective ties. According to Simmons, Griswold, and Berg
measurement in regard to the frequency, inten- (2010), the ESI demonstrated good internal scale
sity, and duration of a number of behaviors as validity as well as person response validity, using
they occur in either a natural or contrived envi- Rasch analysis. Further, validity for 24 of the 27
ronment. One of the potential drawbacks of this skills was established. Sensitivity of the ESI
approach is that only external, observable behav- among different populations has also been estab-
iors may be recorded, meaning that internal, lished. In an examination of the overall quality of
mental states are unable to be evaluated in this social interaction among children with disabili-
manner. Additionally, many target behaviors may ties and those without, statistically significant
occur infrequently or only in particular environ- differences in the quality of social interactions
ments or contexts and are therefore difficult to for the two groups were found (Griswold &
observe and evaluate (Bellack, 1979). As such, Townsend, 2012). Similar results have been
multiple observations of behavior may be found when examining the quality of social inter-
required. Social skills involve a number of dis- actions among adults with and without disabili-
crete verbal and nonverbal behaviors; however, ties, where adults without disabilities demonstrate
they are complex and are subject to the influence significantly higher performance (Simmons
of one’s environment and social partner et al., 2010). One particular strength of the ESI is
(Michelson, Sugai, Wood, & Kazdin, 1983). that it is conducted in the individual’s natural
What follows is a brief review of social skills context. This allows for the evaluator to better
assessments which incorporate and take advan- assess and understand the individual’s social
tage of direct observation of behavior. skills as well as the challenges experienced by
the individual so as to better plan social skills
interventions.
Evaluation of Social Interaction

The Evaluation of Social Interaction (ESI; Fisher Interaction Rating Scale


& Griswold, 2010) is a criterion-referenced
assessment designed to evaluate verbal and non- The Interaction Rating Scale (IRS; Anme, 2009)
verbal behaviors that contribute to social interac- is an assessment designed to measure the social
tion. The evaluator using the ESI rates 27 skills, competence of children as well as the child-­
all of which relate to social interaction in some rearing competence of a child’s caregiver through
manner such as producing a social interaction, observations of child-caregiver interactions over
initiating and ending a social interaction, main- 5-min periods. This scale is appropriate to use for
taining the flow of an interaction, verbally or the assessment of interactions between children
physically supporting the interaction, adapting to from infancy to age 8 years and their caregivers.
problems within a social interaction, or shaping The IRS is comprised of 81 total items, 70 of
the content of an interaction. Typically, the evalu- which are used to obtain a behavioral score with
ator observes an individual as they engage in rel- the remaining 11 used to obtain an impression
Checklists and Scaling Methods 109

score. The items of the IRS are grouped into ten for Children (IRSC; Anme et al., 2012) was
different subscales. Of the 10 subscales, five designed as a measure of social competence for
focus on aspects of the child’s social competence children aged 3–18 years through observations of
including empathy, responsiveness, autonomy, interactions between children. The IRSC is com-
emotional regulation, and motor regulation. The prised of 43 items used to obtain a behavioral
remaining five subscales focus on the child-­ score for three separate subscales measuring self-­
rearing skills of the caregiver including respect control, assertion, and cooperation. Additionally,
for empathy development, respect for responsive- there are three impression items, one for each
ness development, respect for autonomy develop- subscale. Items for the IRSC are scored in the
ment, respect for social-emotional development, same manner as they are scored for the
and respect for cognitive development. Lastly, IRS. According to Anme et al. (2012), the IRSC
one item is used to provide a rating on the overall has an internal consistency of 0.87. The
impression of synchronous relationships. Interaction Rating Scale Advanced (IRSA; Anme
The behavior items of the IRS are scored et al., 2011) was designed as a measure of social
based on the presence of a behavior (i.e., 0 = “no”; competence for individuals over the age of 15
1 = “yes”). An overall behavior score for each through observations. The IRSA is comprised of
subscale is obtained by summing all of the behav- 92 items used to obtain a behavioral score for six
ior items of that subscale. The impression items subscales measuring assertiveness, responsive-
of the IRS, of which there is one for each sub- ness, self-control, sensitivity, regulation, and
scale and one for an overall impression, are rated expressivity with six additional impression items,
on a 5-point scale (i.e., 1 = “not evident at all”; one for each subscale. Like the IRS and IRSC,
2 = “not evident”; 3 = “neutral”; 4 = “evident”; behavior items are scored based on the presence
and 5 = “evident at a high level”). The 70 items of the behavior (i.e., 0 = “no”; 1 = “yes”), and
used to determine a behavioral score are divided impression items are scored based on a 5-point
into two separate components: 45 items are Likert-type scale. According to Anme et al.
focused on a caregiver’s behavior toward chil- (2014), the IRSA has an internal consistency of
dren and 25 items are focused on children’s 0.89. Overall, the IRS and its alternative forms
behavior toward a caregiver. The evaluator using allow for the evaluation of various features of
the IRS completes the 70-item checklist and then interactions and social skill development across
provides impression scores for each of the ten different age groups and different types of inter-
subscales as well as an overall impression. actions (Anme et al., 2010).
Previous research has demonstrated good psy-
chometric properties, with internal consistencies
for each subscale ranging from 0.43 to 0.88, as Social Profile
measured by Cronbach’s alpha, as well as an
excellent total internal consistency of 0.91 (Anme The Social Profile (SP; Donohue, 2003) is an
et al., 2010). Additionally, the IRS has demon- observational measure that evaluates levels of
strated good discriminant validity between chil- social participation in groups for individuals
dren with pervasive developmental disorders, across childhood and adulthood. Two separate
children with ADHD, and children who have forms for the SP exist, a long and a short form,
experienced abuse or maltreatment as well as and both forms are intended to measure five over-
good convergent validity as evinced by high cor- arching developmental level concepts of partici-
relations with the Strength and Difficulties pation: parallel level, associative level, basic
Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman, 1997). cooperative level, supportive cooperative level,
Two additional versions of the IRS have been and mature level. The long form of the SP is com-
developed and were designed to assess social prised of 262 items divided into seven subscales
competence outside of the interaction between a including communication, cooperation, roles,
child and caregiver. The Interaction Rating Scale norms, behavior, activity, attraction, and power.
110 W.J. Peters and J.L. Matson

The SP short form is comprised of 40 items ments regarding social skills in order to appropri-
divided into three subscales including group ately target behaviors for intervention. Social
membership, activity participation, and social skills assessment and treatment have a long his-
interaction. According to Donohue (2005), the tory and have been extensively researched across
SP is primarily intended for use among occupa- a number of different populations. Currently, it
tional therapists for the purpose of enhancing the appears that a majority of research in the assess-
observation and measurement skills of those ment of social skills has focused on the develop-
therapists in groups. Each item on the SP is ment and validation of informant-based or
scored on a Likert-type scale after descriptive, self-report rating scales, measures involving the
qualitative notes are taken during observation of direct observation of behavior, or some combina-
the individuals being assessed in a group setting. tion thereof. Additionally, many of the assess-
Scores on the SP first provide averages across the ment methods discussed focus on observable
different subscales, which then map onto the five behavior across a variety of contexts. This chap-
developmental levels providing an indication of ter reviewed a number of different social skills
how many, or how few, levels of group participa- assessments and presented evidence supporting
tion the individual being assessed has achieved. their use in children and adolescents. Overall, the
Previous research on the SP has indicated that majority of social skills assessments reviewed
it has moderate internal reliability as well as here consisted of rating scales intended to mea-
moderate to high interrater reliability for most of sure both adaptive and maladaptive social skills
the five developmental levels, depending on the or aspects of social competence with research
age group being assessed (Donohue, 2007). For indicating adequate to excellent psychometric
example, according to Donohue (2005), moder- properties.
ate to high interrater reliability was found for the
parallel and basic cooperative levels, but not the
associative level, in a sample of preschool chil- References
dren. Because this is a young age group, it is
unlikely that these individuals will achieve the Agaliotis, I., & Kalyva, E. (2008). Nonverbal social
interaction skills of children with learning disabili-
higher, adult-like supportive cooperative and ties. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 29(1),
mature levels of group participation (Donohue, 1–10.
2007). Additional research has indicated that the Al Awmleh, A., & Woll, A. (2013). Reliability of the
SP has good content and construct validity for German language version of the preschool and kin-
dergarten behavior scales second edition. Journal of
both the long and short forms. Further, the SP has Social Sciences, 9(2), 54.
been used and validated among a number of dif- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
ferent community populations across different and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
age groups, as well as psychiatric inpatient popu- Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Anme, T. (2009). Manual of interaction rating scale.
lations (Donohue, 2007). Tokyo: Japan Pediatric Press.
Anme, T., Shinohara, R., Sugisawa, Y., Tong, L., Tanaka,
E., Watanabe, T., … Yamakawa, N. (2010). Interaction
Conclusion rating scale (IRS) as an evidence-based practical index
of children’s social skills and parenting. Journal of
Epidemiology, 20(Supplement_II), S419–S426.
The appropriate use of social skills in day-to-day Anme, T., Sugisawa, Y., Shinohara, R., Matsumoto, M.,
social interactions is important for the develop- Watanabe, T., Tokutake, K., … Sadato, N. (2012).
ment of social relationships as well as psycho- Validity and reliability of the interaction rating scale
between children (IRSC) by using motion capture
logical and physical well-being (Crowe et al., analysis of head movement. Public Health Research,
2011). Additionally, social skills deficits are 2(6), 208–212.
associated with a variety of negative outcomes Anme, T., Tokutake, K., Tanaka, E., Watanabe, T.,
(e.g., depression; Sato et al., 2005) highlighting Tomisaki, E., Mochizuki, Y., … Sadato, N. (2014).
Validity and reliability of the interaction rating scale
the importance for conducting accurate assess-
Checklists and Scaling Methods 111

advanced (IRSA) as an index of social competence Donohue, M. V. (2003). Group profile studies with
development. Public Health Research, 4(1), 25–30. children: Validity measures and item analysis.
Anme, T., Watanabe, T., Tokutake, K., Tomisaki, E., Occupational Therapy in Mental Health, 19(1), 1–23.
Mochizuki, Y., Tanaka, E., … Sugisawa, Y. (2011). Donohue, M. V. (2005). Social profile: Assessment
A Pilot study of social competence assessment using of validity and reliability with preschool children.
Interaction Rating Scale Advanced. ISRN Pediatrics, Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy, 72(3),
2011, 2729316. Retrieved from http://downloads. 164–175.
hindawi.com/journals/isrn.pediatrics/2011/272913. Donohue, M. V. (2007). Interrater reliability of the social
pdf profile: Assessment of community and psychiatric
Bacanli, H., & Erdogan, F. (2003). Adaptation of the group participation. Australian Occupational Therapy
Matson evaluation of social skills with youngsters Journal, 54(1), 49–58.
(MESSY) to Turkish. Education Sciences: Theory and Eslami, A. A., Mazaheri, M. A., Mostafavi, F., Abbasi,
Practice, 3, 368–379. M. H., & Noroozi, E. (2014). Farsi version of social
Barton, B., & North, K. (2004). Social skills of chil- skills rating system-secondary student form: Cultural
dren with neurofibromatosis type 1. Developmental adaptation, reliability and construct validity. Iranian
Medicine and Child Neurology, 46(8), 553–563. Journal of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, 8(2),
Bellack, A. S. (1979). A critical appraisal of strategies 97.
for assessing social skill. Behavioral Assessment, 1, Fantuzzo, J., Manz, P. H., & McDermott, P. (1998).
157–176. Preschool version of the social skills rating system: An
Canivez, G. L., & Bordenkircher, S. E. (2002). Convergent empirical analysis of its use with low-income children.
and divergent validity of the adjustment scales for Journal of School Psychology, 36(2), 199–214.
children and adolescents and the preschool and kinder- Fink, E., Rosnay, M., Peterson, C., & Slaughter, V.
garten behavior scales. Journal of Psychoeducational (2013). Validation of the peer social maturity scale
Assessment, 20(1), 30–45. for assessing children’s social skills. Infant and Child
Carney, A. G., & Merrell, K. W. (2002). Reliability and Development, 22(5), 539–552.
comparability of a Spanish-language form of the pre- Fisher, A. G., & Griswold, L. A. (2010). Evaluation of
school and kindergarten behavior scales. Psychology social interaction (ESI). Fort Collins, CO: Three Star
in the Schools, 39(4), 367–373. Press.
Carney, A. G., & Merrell, K. W. (2005). Teacher ratings Foster, S. L., & Ritchey, W. L. (1979). Issues in the assess-
of young children with and without ADHD: Construct ment of social competence in children. Journal of
validity of two child behavior rating scales. Assessment Applied Behavior Analysis, 12(4), 625–638.
for Effective Intervention, 30(3), 65–75. Froeschle, J., Smith, R., & Ricard, R. (2007). The efficacy
Cohn, B., Merrell, K. W., Felver-Grant, J., Tom, K., & of a systematic substance abuse program for adoles-
Endrulat, N. R. (2009, February). Strength-based cent females. Professional School Counseling, 10(5),
assessment of social and emotional functioning: 498–505.
Sears-c and sears-a. In Meeting of the National Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties
Association of School Psychologists Boston, MA. questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child
Crawford, A. M., & Manassis, K. (2011). Anxiety, social Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581–586.
skills, friendship quality, and peer victimization: An Greene, R. W., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Wilens,
integrated model. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25(7), T. E., Mick, E., & Blier, H. K. (1999). Further valida-
924–931. tion of social impairment as a predictor of substance
Crowe, L. M., Beauchamp, M. H., Catroppa, C., & use disorders: Findings from a sample of siblings of
Anderson, V. (2011). Social function assessment tools boys with and without ADHD. Journal of Clinical
for children and adolescents: A systematic review Child Psychology, 28(3), 349–354.
from 1988 to 2010. Clinical Psychology Review, Gresham, F. M. (1986). Conceptual and definitional
31(5), 767–785. issues in the assessment of children’s social skills:
Cummings, K. D., Kaminski, R. A., & Merrell, K. W. Implications for classifications and training. Journal
(2008). Advances in the assessment of social com- of Clinical Child Psychology, 15(1), 3–15.
petence: Findings from a preliminary investigation Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1984). Assessment and
of a general outcome measure for social behavior. classification of children’s social skills: A review of
Psychology in the Schools, 45(10), 930–946. methods and issues. School Psychology Review, 13(3),
Diperna, J. C., & Volpe, R. J. (2005). Self-report on the 292–301.
social skills rating system: Analysis of reliability and Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social skills rat-
validity for an elementary sample. Psychology in the ing system: Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American
Schools, 42(4), 345–354. Guidance Service.
Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J. E., Burks, V. S., Bates, J. E., Gresham, F., & Elliott, S. N. (2008). Social skills improve-
Pettit, G. S., Fontaine, R., & Price, J. M. (2003). Peer ment system (SSIS) rating scales. Bloomington, MN:
rejection and social information-processing factors in Pearson Assessments.
the development of aggressive behavior problems in Gresham, F. M., Elliott, S. N., Cook, C. R., Vance, M. J.,
children. Child Development, 74(2), 374–393. & Kettler, R. (2010). Cross-informant agreement for
112 W.J. Peters and J.L. Matson

ratings for social skill and problem behavior ratings: Matson, J. L. (2010). The Matson evaluation of social
An investigation of the social skills improvement sys- skills with youngsters-II (MESSY-II). Baton Rouge,
tem—Rating scales. Psychological Assessment, 22(1), LA: Disability Consultants, LLC.
157. Matson, J. L., Heinze, A., Helsel, W. J., Kapperman, G.,
Gresham, F. M., Elliott, S. N., & Kettler, R. J. (2010). & Rotatori, A. F. (1986). Assessing social behaviors
Base rates of social skills acquisition/performance def- in the visually handicapped: The Matson evaluation
icits, strengths, and problem behaviors: An analysis of of social skills with youngsters (MESSY). Journal of
the social skills improvement system—Rating scales. Clinical Child Psychology, 15(1), 78–87.
Psychological Assessment, 22(4), 809. Matson, J. L., Horovitz, M., Mahan, S., & Fodstad,
Gresham, F. M., Elliott, S. N., Vance, M. J., & Cook, C. R. J. (2013). Reliability of the Matson evaluation of
(2011). Comparability of the social skills rating sys- social skills with youngsters (MESSY) for children
tem to the social skills improvement system: Content with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism
and psychometric comparisons across elementary and Spectrum Disorders, 7(2), 405–410.
secondary age levels. School Psychology Quarterly, Matson, J. L., Macklin, G. F., & Helsel, W. J. (1985).
26(1), 27. Psychometric properties of the Matson evaluation of
Griswold, L. A., & Townsend, S. (2012). Assessing the social skills with youngsters (MESSY) with emotional
sensitivity of the evaluation of social interaction: problems and self concept in deaf children. Journal
Comparing social skills in children with and with- of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,
out disabilities. American Journal of Occupational 16(2), 117–123.
Therapy, 66(6), 709–717. Matson, J. L., Neal, D., Fodstad, J. C., Hess, J. A., Mahan,
Jurado, M., Cumba-Avilés, E., Collazo, L. C., & Matos, S., & Rivet, T. T. (2010). Reliability and validity of
M. (2006). Reliability and validity of a Spanish ver- the Matson evaluation of social skills with youngsters.
sion of the social skills rating system–teacher form. Behavior Modification, 34(6), 539–558.
Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 24(3), Matson, J. L., Neal, D., Worley, J. A., Kozlowski, A. M.,
195–209. & Fodstad, J. C. (2012). Factor structure of the
Kearney, D. S., & Healy, O. (2011). Investigating the rela- Matson evaluation of social skills with youngsters-II
tionship between challenging behavior, co-­morbid psy- (MESSY-II). Research in Developmental Disabilities,
chopathology and social skills in adults with moderate 33(6), 2067–2071.
to severe intellectual disabilities in Ireland. Research Matson, J. L., & Ollendick, T. H. (1988). Enhancing chil-
in Developmental Disabilities, 32(5), 1556–1563. dren’s social skills: Assessment and training (p. 1988).
Kee-Lee, C. (1997). The Matson evaluation of social skills New York: Pergamon Press.
with youngsters: Reliability and validity of a Chinese Matson, J. L., Rotatori, A. F., & Helsel, W. J. (1983).
translation. Personality and Individual Differences, Development of a rating scale to measure social skills
22(1), 123–125. in children: The Matson evaluation of social skills
Lemanek, K. L., Jones, M. L., & Lieberman, B. (2000). with youngsters (MESSY). Behaviour Research and
Mothers of children with spina bifida: Adaptational and Therapy, 21(4), 335–340.
stress processing. Children’s Health Care, 29(1), 19–35. Matson, J. L., & Wilkins, J. (2007). A critical review
Lund, J., & Merrell, K. W. (2001). Social and antisocial of assessment targets and methods for social skills
behavior of children with learning and behavioral dis- excesses and deficits for children with autism spectrum
orders: Construct validity of the home and community disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,
social behavior scales. Journal of Psychoeducational 1(1), 28–37.
Assessment, 19(2), 112–122. Matson, J. L., & Wilkins, J. (2009). Psychometric test-
Major, S., & Seabra-Santos, M. J. (2014). Preschool ing methods for children’s social skills. Research in
and Kindergarten Behavior Scales - Second Edition Developmental Disabilities, 30(2), 249–274.
(PKBS-2): Adaptação e Estudos Psicométricos da McFall, R. M., & Marston, A. R. (1970). An experimental
Versão Portuguesa. Preschool and Kindergarten investigation of behavior rehearsal in assertive train-
Behavior Scales - Second Edition (PKBS-2): ing. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 76(2), 295.
Adaptation and Psychometric Studies of the Méndez, F. X., Hidalgo, M. D., & Inglés, C. (2002). The
Portuguese Version, 27(4), 689–699. https://doi. Matson evaluation of social skills with youngsters:
org/10.1590/1678-7153.201427409 Psychometric properties of the Spanish translation
Major, S., Seabra-Santos, M. J., & Albuquerque, C. P. in the adolescent population. European Journal of
(2017). Validating the preschool and kindergarten Psychological Assessment, 18(1), 30.
behavior scales-2: Preschoolers with autism spectrum Merrell, K. W. (1993). School social behavior scales: Test
disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 65, manual. Bradon, VT: Clinical Psychology.
86–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2017.04.008 Merrell, K. W. (1994). Preschool and kindergarten behav-
Matson, J. L. (1988). The Matson evaluation of social ior scales: PKBS. Bradon, VT: Clinical Psychology.
skills with youngsters (MESSY). Worthington, OH: Merrell, K. W. (2002a). Preschool and kindergarten
International Diagnostic Systems. behavior scales: PKBS-2 (2nd ed.). PRO-ED: Austin.
Checklists and Scaling Methods 113

Merrell, K. W. (2002b). School social behavior scales Nese, R. N., Doerner, E., Romer, N., Kaye, N. C., Merrell,
(2nd ed.). Eugene, OR: Assessment-Intervention K. W., & Tom, K. M. (2012). Social emotional assets
Resources. and resilience scales: Development of a strength-based
Merrell, K. W. (2008). School social behavior scales short-form behavior rating scale system. Journal for
user’s guide. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. Educational Research Online, 4(1), 124.
Merrell, K. W. (2011). Social and emotional assets and Nock, M. K., & Kurtz, S. M. (2005). Direct behavioral
resilience scales (SEARS). Lutz, FL: Psychological observation in school settings: Bringing science to
Assessment Resources. practice. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 12(3),
Merrell, K. W., & Boelter, E. (2001). An investigation of 359–370.
relationships between social behavior and ADHD in Peterson, C., Slaughter, V., & Paynter, J. (2007). Social
children and youth: Construct validity of the home maturity and theory of mind in typically developing
and community social behavior scales. Journal of children and those on the autism spectrum. Journal of
Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 9(4), 260–269. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(12), 1243–1250.
Merrell, K. W., & Caldarella, P. (1999). Social-behavioral Prins, P. J. M. (1997). Dutch version of the Matson evalu-
assessment of at-risk early adolescent students: ation of social skills with youngsters. Unpublished
Psychometric characteristics and validity of a par- Manuscript. University of Amsterdam.
ent report form of the school social behavior scales. Raimundo, R., Carapito, E., Pereira, A. I., Pinto, A. M.,
Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 17(1), Lima, M. L., & Ribeiro, M. T. (2012). School social
36–49. behavior scales: An adaptation study of the Portuguese
Merrell, K. W., & Caldarella, P. (2002). Home & com- version of the social competence scale from SSBS-­2.
munity social behavior scales user’s guide. Baltimore, The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 15(03), 1473–1484.
MD: Paul H. Brookes. Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2004). BASC-2:
Merrell, K. W., Cohn, B. P., & Tom, K. M. (2011). Behavior assessment system for children – Second edi-
Development and validation of a teacher report mea- tion. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
sure for assessing social-emotional strengths of chil- Rydell, A.-M., Hagekull, B., & Bohlin, G. (1997).
dren and adolescents. School Psychology Review, Measurement of two social competence aspects in mid-
40(2), 226. dle childhood. Developmental Psychology, 33(5), 824.
Merrell, K. W., Felver-Gant, J. C., & Tom, K. M. (2011). Sato, H., Ishikawa, S., Arai, K., & Sakano, Y. (2005).
Development and validation of a parent report mea- The relationship between childhood depression and
sure for assessing social-emotional competencies of teacher’s ratings of social skills in elementary school.
children and adolescents. Journal of Child and Family Japanese Journal of Counselling Science, 38(3), 226.
Studies, 20(4), 529–540. Scott, M. N., Taylor, H. G., Fristad, M. A., Klein, N., Espy,
Merrell, K. W., & Gimpel, G. (1998). Social skills of chil- K. A., Minich, N., & Hack, M. (2012). Behavior disor-
dren and adolescents: Conceptualization, assessment, ders in extremely preterm/extremely low birth weight
treatment. London: Psychology Press. children in kindergarten. Journal of Developmental
Merrell, K. W., Streeter, A. L., Boelter, E. W., Caldarella, and Behavioral Pediatrics, 33(3), 202.
P., & Gentry, A. (2001). Validity of the home and com- Segrin, C. (2000). Social skills deficits associated with
munity social behavior scales: Comparisons with five depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 20(3),
behavior-rating scales. Psychology in the Schools, 379–403.
38(4), 313–325. Shahim, S. (2004). Reliability of the social skills rating
Meyer, M. B., & Kurtz, M. M. (2009). Elementary neuro- system for preschool children in Iran. Psychological
cognitive function, facial affect recognition and social-­ Reports, 95(3_suppl), 1264–1266.
skills in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, Sharma, S., Sigafoos, J., & Carroll, A. (2000). Social skills
110(1–3), 173–179. assessment of Indian children with visual impair-
Michelson, L., Sugai, D. P., Wood, R. P., & Kazdin, A. E. ments. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness,
(1983). Social skills assessment and training with 94(3), 172–176.
children: An empirically based handbook. New York: Simmons, C. D., Griswold, L. A., & Berg, B. (2010).
Springer. Evaluation of social interaction during occupational
Milsom, A., & Glanville, J. L. (2010). Factors mediating engagement. American Journal of Occupational
the relationship between social skills and academic Therapy, 64(1), 10–17.
grades in a sample of students diagnosed with learning Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1998).
disabilities or emotional disturbance. Remedial and Vineland social-emotional early childhood scales:
Special Education, 31(4), 241–251. Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
Molinuevo, B., Bonillo, A., Pardo, Y., Doval, E., & Service.
Torrubia, R. (2010). Participation in extracurricular Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., Cicchetti, D. V., Harrison,
activities and emotional and behavioral adjustment in P. L., & Doll, E. A. (1984). Vineland adaptive behav-
middle childhood in Spanish boys and girls. Journal of ior scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
Community Psychology, 38(7), 842–857. Service.
114 W.J. Peters and J.L. Matson

Storebø, O. J., Skoog, M., Damm, D., Thomsen, P. H., Van Horn, K., & Tamase, K. (2001). Teachers’ expecta-
Simonsen, E., & Gluud, C. (2011). Social skills tions of high school students’ social skills in Japan.
training for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Psychologia, 44(4), 250–258.
(ADHD) in children aged 5 to 18 years. The Cochrane Wang, H.-T., Sandall, S. R., Davis, C. A., & Thomas, C. J.
Library. (2011). Social skills assessment in young children
Van der Oord, S., Van der Meulen, E. M., Prins, P. J., with autism: A comparison evaluation of the SSRS
Oosterlaan, J., Buitelaar, J. K., & Emmelkamp, P. M. and PKBS. Journal of Autism and Developmental
(2005). A psychometric evaluation of the social skills Disorders, 41(11), 1487–1495.
rating system in children with attention deficit hyper- Yukay-Yuksel, M. (2009). A Turkish version of the school
activity disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, social behavior scales (SSBS). Educational Sciences:
43(6), 733–746. Theory and Practice, 9(3), 1633–1645.
Behavior Analytic Methods

Marc J. Lanovaz, Marie-Michèle Dufour,
and Malena Argumedes

In other words, the discriminative stimulus sig-


Introduction nals the availability of the reinforcer maintaining
the social behavior. Assume that playing is a
From a behavior analytic standpoint, socials reinforcing activity for a child, Billy. When Billy
skills are typically conceptualized as behaviors asks his friend Tara to play with him, she only
or series of complex behaviors that have an agrees when they are in the schoolyard; other-
impact on the responses of others (McFall, 1982). wise, she refuses to play with him. Thus, the
The principles of operant conditioning thus apply schoolyard functions as a discriminative stimulus
to the development and generalization of social because the social behavior of asking to play is
skills in children (Allen, Hart, Buell, Harris, & more likely to be followed by reinforcement (i.e.,
Wolf, 1964; Chandler, Lubeck, & Fowler, 1992; playing) within this specific context.
Odom & McConnell, 1992). These principles are The second component of the contingency is
not only used to explain the emergence and main- the response, which is the social behavior emitted
tenance of social skills but also to treat difficul- by the child. Social behaviors may take on many
ties in both children with and without disability. forms ranging from simple nonverbal interactions
Young children learn social skills by contacting (e.g., eye contact, gesturing) to complex verbal
the social contingencies present in their environ- exchanges (e.g., conversations on abstract topics).
ment. These social contingencies typically Although social behaviors can vary widely in
include three components: a discriminative stim- form (sometimes referred to as topography), they
ulus, a response, and a social consequence share the commonality of resulting in some type
(Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007). of social consequence. More complex behaviors
The discriminative stimulus appears before can be specifically conceptualized as behavior
the response (i.e., social behavior). The response chains, which are series of responses. For exam-
is more likely to be followed by a reinforcing ple, the behavior of saying “hi” to a friend in the
consequence in its presence than in its absence. hallway may be further divided into smaller units:
(a) stopping approximately 1.5 m in front of the
friend, (b) looking at the friend, (c) saying “hi,”
M.J. Lanovaz (*) • M.-M. Dufour • M. Argumedes and (d) waiting for a response. Within a behavior
École de Psychoéducation, Université de Montréal, chain, the first response serves as the discrimina-
C.P. 6128, succursale Centre-Ville, Montreal, QC,
Canada, H3C 3J7
tive stimulus for the second response, the second
e-mail: marc.lanovaz@umontreal.ca response for the third response, and so on.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 115


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_8
116 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

The social consequence is the final component the presence of a third child increased the value
of the contingency, which is used to explain the of the game and the frequency of asking to play
development and maintenance of social skills. A tag, functioning as an establishing operation for
social consequence is a stimulus event mediated the behavior.
by another person that is provided contingent on Within a behavior analytic conceptualization,
the occurrence of the social behavior. If the conse- the practitioner generally aims to manipulate
quence increases responding, it is referred to as a these contingencies to teach children social skills.
reinforcer. Contrarily, consequences that decrease For example, a practitioner may add discrimina-
responding are referred to as punishers. For exam- tive stimuli (e.g., prompts) to facilitate the correct
ple, most mothers are more likely to talk to their execution of the behavior, use stimulus events
babies in a soothing voice when they smile. If the functioning as establishing operations to increase
infant smiles more often as a result, the mother’s the value of the reinforcer associated with the
talking in a soothing voice would be considered as social behavior, or alter the consequences contin-
a social reinforcer for smiling. In contrast, if a gent on engagement in the behavior. Multiple
mother scolds her young child when he screams interventions have been derived from the princi-
and it results in a reduction of screaming, scolding ples of applied behavior analysis to support the
would be considered as a social punisher. In both acquisition, generalization, and maintenance of
previous examples, the consequence involved the social skills in children. The next section presents
addition of stimulus (i.e., positive reinforcement common behavioral assessments that may be war-
and punishment). Social behavior may also result ranted prior to the implementation of interven-
in the removal of a stimulus (i.e., negative rein- tions for social skills. Then, we define and discuss
forcement and punishment). If a child asks a friend methods that have been used to increase interac-
to stop playing a game, the removal of the game tions and improve social skills in children.
may function as a reinforcer for the social behav-
ior of asking to stop.
Traditionally, most learned behaviors are Behavioral Assessment
explained using this three-term contingency, but
behavior analysts have been increasingly turning Assessment is the first step conducted by the
to a fourth term to supplement their analyses, the practitioner when aiming to improve social skills
motivating operation (Laraway, Snycerski, in children. Direct observation methods, check-
Michael, & Poling, 2003; Michael, 1993). lists, and scales are all options available to practi-
Motivating operations are stimulus events that tioners who need to assess social skills (Gresham
alter both the value of a consequence and the fre- & Elliott, 1984; Matson & Wilkins, 2009). As
quency of the behavior associated with it. The these assessment methods have already been
abolishing operation reduces the value of a con- reviewed previously (see Chapters “Observational
sequence, whereas the establishing operation Methods” to “Behavior Analytic Methods”), pro-
increases its value. For example, engaging in the viding a detailed description goes beyond the
same activity (e.g., game) for extended periods of scope of the current chapter. That said, we will
time may reduce its value as well as the behavior provide an overview of three behavioral assess-
of engaging in the activity. In this case, the stimu- ments that are often central to the success of
lus event (extended duration of engagement in interventions based on behavior analytic princi-
the activity) functions as an abolishing operation. ples: task analysis, preference assessment, and
As an example of establishing operation, assume functional assessment. These three assessments
that two children are playing together. When a may support practitioners in planning their inter-
third child arrives, they ask her to play tag. Even ventions and optimizing treatment effects when
though tag was available as a game beforehand, teaching social skills.
Behavior Analytic Methods 117

Task Analysis of the participants was low (functioned at levels


of 4 and 5 years old), suggesting that this method
Prior to teaching complex skills, practitioners may also be relevant to young children.
often conduct a task analysis, which involves the
division of a behavior into smaller units (i.e., a
behavior chain; Neidert, Dozier, Iwata, & Hafen, Preference Assessment
2010). According to Cooper et al. (2007), there
are three methods to construct a task analysis: Engagement in appropriate social behaviors typi-
observing skilled persons preforming the target cally generates reinforcing consequences through
task, consulting an expert of the target task in continuing interactions with others. For some
question, and performing the task yourself. By children, social consequences may not be suffi-
dividing complex behaviors into smaller units, it cient to lead to the acquisition of new social
becomes easier to measure and to teach. Once behaviors for two reasons. First, the execution of
every step of the chain is clearly defined, it is the behavior may not be correct or accurate during
essential to assess the child’s ability to perform the learning process, which may fail to lead to the
each of the chain units. The practitioner can then delivery of social reinforcement in the natural
develop a checklist that describes each unit that environment. Thus, the child may not contact the
the child must perform. Two methods may be social contingency frequently enough to increase
used to assess the units of the task analysis: sin- responding. Second, social consequences may not
gle and multiple opportunities. The single oppor- be a potent reinforcer for the child in question. In
tunity assessment consists of assessing the task in this case, an additional reinforcer should be paired
the correct order. The assessment typically ends with the social consequence in order to (a) condi-
when the child fails one of the steps because the tion the social responses of others as reinforcers
discriminative stimulus to produce the subse- and (b) strengthen the novel social behavior.
quent units of the chain is absent. During multi- Because most of the interventions for teaching
ple opportunities assessment, the instructor social skills have a reinforcement component,
assesses each unit of the chain, providing prompts assessing preferred stimuli is paramount.
if necessary so that the child has the opportunity Preference assessments are procedures
to perform each step. designed to assist practitioners in identifying pre-
In an example of single opportunity assess- ferred stimuli for treatment (Graff & Karsten,
ment, Parker and Kamps (2011) conducted a task 2012). The stimuli evaluated within preference
analysis in order to assess performance during assessments can take on many forms such as edi-
social activities in two high functioning children bles (e.g., preferred food), leisure items (e.g.,
with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). During toys, games), sensory stimuli (e.g., music), or
baseline, the instructor simply asked the partici- even other types of social stimuli (e.g., praise,
pants to complete the tasks without further tickles; Virués-Ortega et al., 2014). During treat-
prompting. Given that the tasks had to be com- ment, the practitioner can either provide pre-
pleted in a certain order, the child did not have ferred stimuli directly as reinforcers or use them
the opportunity to perform the subsequent tasks as backup reinforcers within a token economy
if the first one was not executed or was performed (Doll, McLaughlin, & Barretto, 2013). One of
incorrectly. In contrast, Haring, Kennedy, Adams, the simplest methods and least time consuming
and Pitts-Conway (1987) conducted a task analy- procedure to assess preference is the use of sur-
sis to assess community skills in young adult veys (Resetar & Noell, 2008; Rotatori, Fox, &
with ASD. During the initial assessment, the Switzky, 1979). In this type of indirect assess-
instructor presented relevant prompts so that the ment, a survey is administered to the child, a
youth could emit a step even if the previous step teacher, or parent to identify the preferred stimuli
had been failed. Although this study was con- of the child. However, studies have indicated
ducted with young adults, the level of functioning that this method does not necessarily identify the
118 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

most potent reinforcers (Hagopian, Long, & Rush, one of two stimuli and can interact with the
2004; Northup, George, Jones, Broussard, & one selected for a short period of time (e.g., 30 s).
Vollmer, 1996), which suggests that direct assess- The practitioner records the item selected on
ments methods should be used when possible. each trial (if any), and the one selected the most
During direct assessments of preference, the frequently is the most preferred. The methodol-
child has the opportunity to directly access the ogy has also been adapted to assess preference
stimuli in the assessment, and the practitioner for music and video recordings (Chebli &
measures whether the child interacts with the Lanovaz, 2016; Horrocks & Higbee, 2008). The
stimulus or the duration of interaction. Depending paired-choice method has the advantage of rank-
on the functioning of the child and type of stimu- ing the items in order of preference, but the pro-
lus, interactions can include approaching, manip- cedures can be time consuming, especially as the
ulating, consuming, picking up, or gazing at the number of items assessed increases.
item (Virués-Ortega et al., 2014). Typically, The multiple stimulus assessments are similar
direct preference assessments involve between 5 to the paired-choice method, but all stimuli are
and 15 stimuli, which will vary according to presented simultaneously (DeLeon & Iwata, 1996).
stimulus category and type of assessment, and Two versions of the multiple stimulus assessment
begin by sampling so that the child has the oppor- are available to practitioners. In the multiple
tunity to interact with the stimuli beforehand. stimulus with replacement method, the practitio-
The four most common procedures are the single- ner records the selection and replaces the selected
stimulus assessment, the paired-choice assess- item in the array following each choice. In the
ment, the multiple stimulus assessment, and the multiple stimulus without replacement method,
free-operant assessment (Graff & Karsten, 2012; the practitioner records the rank at which the item
Kang et al., 2013; Virués-Ortega et al., 2014). was selected and does not replace it in the array
During the single-stimulus assessment (Pace, following its selection. The multiple stimulus
Ivancic, Edwards, Iwata, & Page, 1985), the without replacement is generally recommended
practitioner presents each stimulus one at a time first among all the methods because of its rapid
for a brief period of time (e.g., 30 s) and records administration and its ranking of items (Kang
whether the child interacts with the stimulus or et al., 2013). Conditions in which other methods
not. The procedure is generally repeated several may be preferable include when (a) the child
times for each stimulus. The most preferred items engages in problem behaviors contingent on the
are the ones selected the most often. Alternatively, removal items, (b) assessing preference for activi-
the practitioner may measure the duration of ties, and (c) assessing preference in children with
interaction with the stimulus, which may be use- severe disabilities, which may limit the number of
ful for assessing preference for activities; in this items that can be presented simultaneously.
case, the item with which the child interacts for A final alternative is the free-operant prefer-
the longest duration is considered the most pre- ence assessment, which consists of providing
ferred (Hagopian, Rush, Lewin, & Long, 2001). access to multiple stimuli simultaneously during
The single-stimulus assessments have the advan- a period of 5–15 min and recording the duration
tage of being straightforward to implement and of interaction with each item (Roane, Vollmer,
can be rapid to complete. The main disadvantage Ringdahl, & Marcus, 1998). This method has the
is that the procedures may produce multiple false benefit of having a predictable duration and may
positives and prevent rank ordering as some chil- result in lower levels of problem behaviors as
dren may interact with all stimuli. items are not removed (Verriden & Roscoe,
The paired-choice preference assessment 2016). It should be noted that the method may
involves presenting stimuli in pairs (Fisher et al., produce false negatives as some children may
1992). Each stimulus is presented with each other only interact with one item during the entire
stimulus once, so that all stimuli are eventually duration of the session, limiting its utility when
paired together in a random order. During each multiple preferred stimuli must be identified and
presentation, the child is asked to choose between ranked.
Behavior Analytic Methods 119

In sum, practitioners should strongly consider analytic method individually in our review of
conducting a preference assessment when plan- treatments. However, nearly all treatments
ning to use reinforcers as part of their treatment. involve the implementation of multiple methods
The multiple stimulus without replacement simultaneously in order to support the develop-
method has clear advantages, especially for ment and maintenance of new social skills; we
children who do not have an intellectual disabil- thus encourage practitioners to combine these
ity and engage in few problem behaviors. That methods to meet their treatment objectives. We
said, the other procedures may prove particularly did not review self-management and behavioral
useful when it is not possible or advisable to skills training as part of the current chapter as
implement the multiple stimulus without replace- they are thoroughly covered in subsequent sec-
ment procedure. tions of this book (see Chapters “Self-Regulation
in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective” and
“Social and Emotional Learning: Recent
Functional Assessment Research and Practical Strategies for Promoting
Children’s Social and Emotional Competence in
As previously discussed in the introduction to Schools”).
this chapter, the behavior analytic conceptualiza- It should be noted that a lot of the research on
tion of social skills implies that these behaviors behavior analytic interventions to improve social
have social functions. That is, children engage in skills in children without developmental disabil-
social skills to contact social contingencies in ity has been conducted more than 20 years ago.
their environment. These functions can be numer- More recently, research has focused on social
ous such as accessing a desired item mediated by skills in children with ASD and other develop-
another person, seeking attention, or terminating mental disabilities. Our review of the interven-
an activity with a partner. As such, conducting a tions will provide an overview of both older and
functional assessment can be particularly useful more recent research on the topic. Given that the
when either identifying the contingencies main- principles of behavior apply to all (regardless of
taining an inappropriate social behavior or diagnosis), the results are most likely generaliz-
attempting to target a replacement behavior (Frea able from one population to another.
& Hughes, 1997; Maag, 2005). By identifying
the specific function of the social behavior, the
practitioner may more precisely select alterna- Prompting
tives that will allow the child to contact similar
social contingencies. Adopting a functional One of the most common components of behav-
approach may thus improve the probability of ioral interventions used to teach social skills to
success of the social skills intervention (Hurl, children is prompting. Prompting involves the
Wightman, Haynes, & Virues-Ortega, 2016; addition or modification of a stimulus prior to the
Matson, Bamburg, Cherry, & Paclawskyj, 1999). occurrence of the behavior that increases correct
For details on conducting functional assessments, responding. In other words, the parent or instruc-
we refer the reader to the Chapter “Challenging tor adds supplementary antecedent stimuli to
Behavior”, which provides a thorough review of help a child perform a skill (Odom & Strain,
the different methods. 1984; Spence, 2003). During social skills train-
ing, the use of prompting procedures aims to
reduce errors while teaching new socially appro-
Behavioral Treatment priate behaviors.
The two main types of prompts are stimulus
Many treatments to improve social skills in chil- prompts, which involve the addition or modifica-
dren have been derived from applied behavior tion of a social cue, and response prompts, which
analysis. For clarity, we present each behavior operate directly on the behavior. Stimulus
120 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

prompts are divided in two categories: extra- others. In order to support the participants, the
stimulus prompts and intra-stimulus prompts instructor provided verbal cues such as “What
(Shreibman, 1975). When providing an extra- happens when you earn all of your points?” or
stimulus prompt, the parent or instructor adds a “How many points did you earn?” Another
stimulus (prompt) to increase the child’s correct example of verbal instructions is the use of social
responding. For example, Ivy, Lather, Hatton, scripts, which involves written or audio recorded
and Wehby (2016) used automated tactile cues cues to teach social initiations and interactions
delivered by a vibrating pager to prompt children (Brown, Krantz, McClannahan, & Poulson,
with visual impairments to engage in pro-social 2008; Cowan & Allen, 2007). Social scripts have
behavior during lunchtime (i.e., eating with been shown effective in teaching children to
mouth closed). The prompting procedure was increase social initiations, to interact with their
effective at increasing the pro-social behavior in peers, and to engage in conversations about vari-
all three participants. In another example of ous topics (Krantz & McClannahan, 1993, 1998;
extra-stimulus prompts, Harrell, Kamps, and Sarokoff, Taylor, & Poulson, 2001). In most
Kravits (1997) taught three children with ASD cases, scripts are gradually faded when the chil-
strategies to maintain social interactions with dren show mastery of the socials skills so that the
others. To this end, one of the components of the newly learned behaviors are emitted in the pres-
intervention involved cue topic cards to prompt ence of natural stimuli.
conversations during lunchtime. Physical guidance refers to the instructor physi-
When implementing an intra-stimulus prompt, cally assisting the child with movements to
the instructor enhances a component of the dis- improve the accuracy of the social behavior.
criminative stimulus that helps the child respond O’Connell, Lieberman, and Petersen (2006)
correctly. In an example of intra-stimulus prompt, explain that when paired with verbal instructions
Taylor and Hoch (2008) taught a child to respond and proper feedback (i.e., adapted to the level the
to pointing. As a prompt, the instructor exagger- child’s receptive language), physical guidance is
ated the pointing gesture and accompanying ver- crucial for teaching children with visual impair-
bal command in order to increase the salience of ments and developmental delays. Physical guid-
the discriminative stimulus (i.e., the stimulus ance is often used to teach motor skills, like
[pointing] was enhanced to facilitate respond- playing games or physically requesting attention.
ing). In a study of the perception of robots by Modeling refers to providing a demonstration
children with ASD, Peca, Simut, Pintea, of the targeted social behavior prior to its perfor-
Costescu, and Vanderborght (2014) reported that mance by the child. To learn by modeling, chil-
children preferred robots with exaggerated facial dren should be able to imitate immediately after
features. Using this type of intra-stimulus prompt the stimulus has been presented (within 3–5 s).
may facilitate the initial development of receptive During modeling, the child watches a model of
nonverbal social skills as the child may be more the social behavior to be executed. This model
readily able to identify emotions and nonverbal can be presented in vivo or through video. Video
cues when the facial features are more salient. modeling is usually implemented by presenting a
Response prompts can also be further divided video recorded sample of the specific social
into three categories: verbal instructions, physi- behavior to the child. Then, the child is asked to
cal guidance, and modeling (Cooper et al., 2007). perform the sequence. In video modeling, models
Verbal instructions are frequently used to teach can either be adult models, peer models, self-
new behaviors in training contexts; they can models, point-of-view models, or mixed models
either be vocal or nonvocal instructions (e.g., (any combination of the previous models; McCoy
written). Koegel, Koegel, Hurley, and Frea (1992) & Hermansen, 2007). McCoy and Hermansen
used verbal instructions to teach four boys with (2007) have indicated that adults as models have
autism to self-manage their edible reinforcers been effective in increasing play skills, perspec-
after successfully responding to questions from tive taking skills, and conversation skills for
Behavior Analytic Methods 121

children with ASD. Peer modeling has also been Most-to-least prompting is a strategy in which
effective in increasing and generalizing commu- the instructor initially provides guidance using
nication skills in social situations. As mentioned more intrusive prompts and then gradually
by Reichow and Volkmar (2010) in a review of replaces them with less intrusive ones until the
social skills, more studies are needed to clarify child performs the skill in the absence of prompt-
what type of model may lead to better outcomes ing. The amount of guidance is gradually reduced
in teaching social skills. as the child begins to perform the social skill cor-
Video self-modeling can either involve (a) vid- rectly with less instructor assistance. Often,
eotaping children and editing out inappropriate most-to-least prompting begins with physical
behaviors to focus on the appropriate social guidance, then moves to gestural prompts fol-
behavior or (b) watching an unedited video so the lowed by verbal instruction, and ends with the
children can self-critique their performance. natural social discriminative stimulus. Jones
Video self-modeling has demonstrated encourag- (2009) implemented most-to-least prompting in
ing results in increasing socially relevant behav- order to teach joint attention skills to two chil-
iors, but more studies are needed to further support dren with ASD. In this case, the instructor began
its effectiveness (Hagiwara & Myles, 1999; with physical guidance, then replaced it by point-
McCoy & Hermansen, 2007). Point-of-view ing, and finally introduced a 4-s time delay. Most-
modeling involves showing video footage as if the to-least prompting has also been shown effective
child was engaged in the sequence. Relatively in teaching play and communication skills to
new, this approach has been effective in teaching children with ASD and other developmental dis-
play skills and other developmental skills to chil- abilities (Taylor & Hoch, 2008). Graduated guid-
dren with ASD and without developmental delay ance is a variation of most-to-least prompting,
(Norman, Collins, & Schuster, 2001; Schipley- but in this case, the practitioner only uses physi-
Benamou, Lutzker, & Taubman, 2002). Finally, cal prompts and gradually fades the different
mixed models have been used to teach conversa- forms until the learner emits the behavior without
tional skills, social initiation skills, and play skills additional prompts (Bryan & Gast, 2000;
to children with variable results (Maione & MacDuff, Krantz, & McClannahan, 1993).
Miranda, 2006; Sherer et al., 2001). When com- When implementing least-to-most prompting,
pared to in vivo modeling, video modeling seems the parent or instructor waits for the child to per-
to produce faster results and better generalization form the behavior before providing a prompt; the
of social behaviors (Charlop-Christy, Le, & prompting hierarchy moves from least-to-most
Freeman, 2000). It may also be less time consum- intrusive (Cihak, Fahrenkrog, Ayres, & Smith,
ing and more cost efficient (Graetz, Mastropieri, 2010; Kroeger, Schultz, & Newsom, 2007;
& Scruggs, 2006). Once videotaped, the sequence Murzynski & Bourret, 2007). A set amount of
may be used numerous times by different instruc- time is usually given to the learner to do so after
tors without being modified. the presentation of the social cue (e.g., 3 s). For
example, an instructor may say, “hi” and wait 3 s
for the child to respond. If the child does not
Fading respond correctly, the instructor may provide a
subtle gesture as a prompt (e.g., waving) and wait
When using prompts, the purpose is to gradually again for a response. After an additional 3 s, the
fade them until the child is able to respond in instructor may provide a more intrusive prompt
their absence (Riley, 1995). Four different proce- such as a verbal cue or physical guidance to
dures can be used to transfer control of the wave. Once the child performs the social behav-
response from the prompt to the natural social ior correctly, the instructor provides a reinforcer
discriminative stimulus: most-to-least prompt- and continues teaching. Jolly, Center, Test, and
ing, graduated guidance, least-to-most prompt- Spooner (1993) used role-play to teach social
ing, and time delay (Barton & Wolery, 2008). skills to children with ASD and integrated a
122 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

least-to-most prompting procedure to facilitate showed the children a card with the word scripted
engagement in correct responding. In a recent on it. Then, the instructor removed one third of
example, Davis-Temple, Jung, and Sainato the card at every step until no card was visible.
(2014) implemented a four-step least-to-most The script fading procedure was effective in
prompting hierarchy to teach three children with increasing child-adult social interactions for the
special needs to play social board games. The three boys and its effects also generalized to a
hierarchy involved an indirect verbal prompt, a new adult. As discussed earlier, Taylor and Hoch
direct verbal prompt, a gestural or model prompt, (2008) used fading with intra-stimulus prompts
and a physical prompt. Both the previous studies to bring a social response under the control of
are examples of how least-to-most prompting naturally occurring social stimuli; that is, they
strategies may be implemented to support chil- reduced the salience of an adult’s pointing when
dren in the development of their social skills. teaching children to respond to this social cue.
Finally, time delay refers to the amount of
time that the instructor provides between the pre-
sentation of the social request and the prompt Chaining
(Yilmaz & Birkan, 2005). Instructors can imple-
ment the delay in a constant or progressive man- Chaining involves teaching a complex behavior,
ner. For the constant time delay, the prompt is which has been divided into many simpler ones
presented after a specific amount of time (e.g., within a chain of behaviors. Every behavior
3 s). For the progressive time delay procedure, within the chain is reinforced and serves as a cue
the instructor starts by presenting the prompt for the subsequent behavior of the sequence. In
simultaneously with the social stimulus. The other words, the feedback provided from one
time delay is then systematically increased by 1 s behavior functions as the discriminative stimulus
at a time following the child’s progression. Time for the subsequent one. As for the first and the
delay procedures have been shown to be effective last unit of the chain, they serve only one func-
in teaching social and communication skills tion, either the discriminative stimulus or rein-
within children’s natural environments (Liber, forcer. Chaining is a validated procedure to teach
Frea, & Symon, 2008; Yilmaz & Birkan, 2005). self-help, adaptive, community, and domestic
For children with ASD or other disabilities, this skills to children (Rayner, 2011; Shrestha,
contextual teaching may promote generalization Anderson, & Moore, 2012; Thomson, Walters,
of social skills across individuals and settings. Martin, & Yu, 2011). Moreover, Odom, Collet-
Stimulus fading and stimulus shaping are fad- Klingenberg, Rogers, and Hatton (2010) per-
ing procedures that are implemented by modify- formed a review of evidence-based intervention
ing the discriminative stimulus presented to the in children and youth with ASD and indicated
child (Wolery & Gast, 1984). When implement- that task analysis and chaining had accumulated
ing stimulus fading, the parent or instructor intro- enough empirical support to be considered as
duces a new stimulus with enhanced evidence-based practices in teaching communi-
characteristics to increase the likelihood of an cation, play, and social skills.
errorless response (e.g., Lancioni, 1983). Then, Before implementing chaining procedures, a
the altered characteristics (e.g., color, size, shape) task analysis must be developed and validated.
are faded by the instructor. For stimulus shaping, Once the complex behavior is divided into a
relevant dimensions of a stimulus that already chain, the skills of the child are assessed and the
evokes the target behavior are gradually modified instructor selects one of the four chaining meth-
until the child responds correctly following the ods: forward, total-task, backward, or backward
presentation of the natural social stimulus only. with leap ahead (Cooper et al., 2007). Forward
Krantz and McClannahan (1998) used a script chaining consists of initially teaching the first
fading procedure to teach three boys aged 4 and 5 behavior of the chain and then every subsequent
with autism to interact with an adult by saying, unit in a sequential order. To clarify this princi-
“Look” and “Watch me.” First, the instructor ple, let’s use the simple behavior of brushing
Behavior Analytic Methods 123

teeth. The first step to be taught would be “open a can be probed while teaching the rest of the chain
tube of toothpaste.” After the child has shown (Spooner, Spooner, & Ulicny, 1986). Backward
acquisition of the first step, the behavior “apply chaining is part of the picture exchange commu-
toothpaste on toothbrush” could be taught and so nication system (PECS), a widely used program
on, until every behavior of the chain was mas- to teach social communication to children with
tered. DeQuinzio, Townsend, and Poulson (2008) developmental disabilities (Bondy & Frost,
showed that forward chaining with contingent 1994). For example, Charlop-Christy, Carpenter,
social interaction was effective at teaching a shar- Le, LeBlanc, and Kellet (2002) taught children
ing response chain to four children with ASD. In with ASD to initiate communication spontane-
another study, Libby, Weiss, Bancroft, and ously using PECS. The initial step of the pro-
Ahearn (2008) compared two prompting tech- gram, the exchange, is taught using backward
niques to teach play skills with forward chaining chaining. The behavior of giving a picture to the
to five children with ASD and other disabilities. instructor can be divided into three steps: (1) pick
Their results indicated that forward chaining led up the card, (2) move hand over the instructor’s
to play skills acquisition, regardless of the hand, and (3) let go of the card. The instructor
prompting procedure. physically prompts the two first steps, and then
Total-task chaining represents a variation of the child must release the card without prompting.
forward chaining in which the instructor teaches When this behavior meets the mastery criterion,
every unit of the chain at each training session the instructor prompts only the first step; the
until the child is able to accomplish the entire child then has to perform the last two indepen-
sequence. One example of an intervention that dently. Research on PECS suggests that back-
takes advantage of total-task chaining is video ward chaining may be useful to teach basic social
modeling. During video modeling, all the compo- communication skills to children with develop-
nents of complex social behaviors are taught mental disabilities.
simultaneously within the recording, which is a
form of total-task chaining (Kagohara et al., 2013;
Tetreault & Lerman, 2010). Similarly, Arntzen, Shaping
Halstadtrø, and Halstadtrø (2003) taught a child
with developmental disability to play appropri- Shaping is a procedure used to teach a behavior
ately by teaching all steps that he performed that is not yet in a person’s behavioral repertoire
incorrectly simultaneously. Specifically, the and consists of reinforcing the nearest approxi-
instructor provided prompts on steps performed mation of the target behavior (Cooper et al.,
incorrectly during a previous trial. 2007). The shaping procedure contains two com-
Backward chaining consists of teaching the ponents: differential reinforcement and succes-
last step of a chain and then introducing every sive approximations. The procedure involves the
unit of the chain in a reversed sequential order. differential reinforcement of behaviors that share
Using our earlier example of brushing teeth, put- some characteristics with the target behavior
ting away the toothpaste and toothbrush could be while withholding reinforcement for other behav-
the first behavior taught and after successfully iors. In doing so, the occurrence of the desirable
meeting the mastery criterion for this step, rins- behavior is likely to increase. The first step of
ing the toothbrush (i.e., the second to last step) shaping consists of identifying a behavior already
could be introduced, until the first unit of the in the repertoire of the person that shares some
chain was mastered. In backward chaining, the characteristics with the target behavior (nearest
reinforcer is always provided at the end of the approximation) and providing reinforcement
chain. Backward chaining with leap ahead is contingent on its occurrence. When the occur-
essentially the same process as backward chain- rence of the initial approximation increases, the
ing except that one would not teach every step of instructor modifies the criteria and reinforces a
the chain because the child may have already novel approximation closer to the final behavior.
mastered some units. Rather, the mastered steps Successive approximations refer to this progressive
124 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

change in reinforcement criteria. In shaping emit the target behavior, (3) the child’s response,
attending behavior, the instructor could provide (4) a consequence (reinforcing a correct response
reinforcement when the child is orienting her or implementing an error correction procedure in
head towards the instructor. When the occurrence the case of an incorrect response), and (5) a brief
of this behavior increases, the instructor could pause before presenting the discriminative stimu-
then reinforce when the child makes direct eye lus for the next trial. Discrete trial training is typi-
contact and then when the child responds to the cally applied within one-to-one teaching sessions
instructor’s question. between an instructor and a child.
As with other behavioral procedures, shaping Downs, Downs, Johansen, and Fossum (2007)
is often integrated into comprehensive interven- showed that discrete trial training brought positive
tion programs (Lovaas, 2003; Rogers, 2000). change in social-emotional and adaptive behaviors
Shaping may also represent a core intervention in young children with developmental disabilities.
strategy within a program. Allen et al. (1964) In addition, Nuzzolo-Gomez, Leonard, Ortiz,
showed that shaping was effective to teach social Rivera, and Greer (2002) demonstrated that dis-
play to a preschool girl who had a low rate of crete trial training combined with reinforcement
social interactions. Another study demonstrated could increase engagement in appropriate func-
shaping as an effective technique for increasing tional play in preschoolers with ASD. In a review
peer-to-peer interactions for children who were study, Odom et al. (2010) indicated that discrete
socially withdrawn, but that modeling appeared trial training was considered evidence-based in
to be more effective (O’Connor, 1972). In a more teaching new behaviors and communication skills,
recent example, Hall, Maynes, and Reiss (2009) but that it did not have sufficient support to be con-
used shaping with overcorrection to improve eye sidered an evidence-based practice when teaching
contact in children with fragile X syndrome. The social skills to children with ASD.
instructor only reinforced increasingly longer Lovaas (2003) presented four reasons to use
durations (i.e., approximations) of eye contact discrete trial training: (1) the nature of the
using percentile schedules. As such, shaping con- teaching format helps the children access the
tributed to increasing the duration of eye contact, discriminative stimulus, (2) it is easy to observe
an essential nonverbal social behavior. One of the when a child responds correctly, (3) it allows
benefits of implementing shaping procedures is the instructor to teach with consistency, and (4)
that it may reduce frustration by reinforcing it facilitates data collection to assess progress.
already mastered behaviors (Lovaas, 2003). That The opportunity to implement this teaching for-
said, using shaping to teach novel social behavior mat in a large range of contexts also represents
may be time consuming when compared to other an advantage (Downs et al., 2007). Although
strategies (e.g., prompting); it should mainly be discrete trial training is an efficient teaching
used when it is not possible to prompt the behav- format, some limitations should be considered.
ior (e.g., vocal behavior, eye contact) or the person Given the structured nature of this method,
is unable to execute the correct behavior despite Smith (2001) indicated that children may fail to
prompting. respond in the absence of a clear discriminative
stimulus. To address this issue, practitioners
should implement a more flexible instructional
Discrete Trial Training approach after the child has met the mastery
criterion.
Discrete trial training is a format used to teach a
variety of skills to children such as communica-
tion, play, social, self-help, and academics Reinforcement Schedules
(Hayward, Gale, & Eikeseth, 2009; Smith, 2001).
Typically, discrete trial training includes five dis- As with any other type of behavior, reinforcement
tinct parts: (1) a discriminative stimulus provided is generally an essential component of social
by the instructor, (2) a prompt to help the child skills training. With some children, the social
Behavior Analytic Methods 125

reinforcement provided by the continued interac- meet the reinforcement requirement more rapidly),
tion with others may be insufficient to teach which may be counterproductive.
novel behaviors, which is why adding other types Finally, lag schedules are often reported in
of reinforcers may be important (Reichow, studies of social skills, particularly in the acquisi-
Steiner, & Volkmar, 2013). Ratio-based sched- tion of play. Lag schedules involve reinforcing
ules involve the delivery of a reinforcer after the the variability of a behavior (Page & Neuringer,
child has emitted the behavior for a prespecified 1985). For example, a lag 5 schedule involves the
number of times (Catania, 2013). This delivery reinforcement of a response only if five consecu-
can occur after a fixed number of responses or a tive responses differ from one another. Baruni,
variable number of responses. When the rein- Rapp, Lipe, and Novotny (2014) taught children
forcer is provided every time the behavior occurs, with intellectual disability to vary play behavior
the schedule is referred to as continuous rein- by implementing lag 1 and lag 2 schedules.
forcement. For example, Russo and Koegel Interestingly, the lag 2 schedule did not signifi-
(1977) taught a young girl with ASD social skills cantly increase variability when compared to the
in the classroom by providing tokens every time lag 1 schedule for two of three participants. Using
she emitted specific skills; she could accumulate a combination of lag and interval schedules,
tokens that she later exchanged for backup rein- Lepper, Devine, and Petursdottir (2016) used lag
forcers (e.g., edible items). Intermittent ratio 1 and 2 schedules to teach varying conversational
schedules, wherein the reinforcement is provided topics in two children with ASD. Specifically, the
after a fixed or variable of response, are often conversational partner provided attention if the
used to promote maintenance of behavior over topic differed from the topics discussed in the
time (Beiers, Derby, & McLaughlin, 2016; previous one or two 10-s intervals.
Hopkins, 1968; Martins & Harris, 2006).
In contrast, interval-based schedules involve
the delivery of a reinforcer contingent on engag- Generalization Training
ing in a target behavior after a variable or fixed
period of time (Catania, 2013). In a recent exam- Generalization is the process whereby children
ple, Vallinger-Brown and Rosales (2014) taught display learned behavior within novel stimulus
basic conversational skills (i.e., intraverbal conditions or show novel responses under stimu-
responding) to children with attention deficit dis- lus conditions in which a similar response was
order. The instructor provided reinforcement for previously reinforced (Catania, 2013). For exam-
attending on a 30-s variable-interval schedule, ple, a child who learns to say “hi” to a relative
and responses during posttest were also rein- and then applies the same behavior to an instruc-
forced on a 1-min variable-interval schedule tor (without prior reinforcement or prompting) is
using tokens as reinforcers. As a variation of the said to have shown stimulus generalization.
variable-interval schedule, Matson, Fee, Coe, and Similarly, a child who learns to hold a conversa-
Smith (1991) implemented a procedure whereby tion about cars and then applies this new skill to
an instructor provided edible reinforcers to chil- discussing planes is displaying response general-
dren with developmental delay if they had ization. A child may show generalization to novel
engaged in the behavior when a timer beeped on persons, settings, contexts, or responses. Long-
a variable 4-min schedule. This procedure term maintenance of skills is also a form of gen-
increased social play for two of three partici- eralization but across time. Generalization is not
pants. We recommend interval-based schedules necessarily a passive process and should thus be
when the target social behavior may have a vari- actively programmed when teaching social skills
able duration (e.g., play, maintaining a conversa- to children (Chandler et al., 1992).
tion); using ratio-based schedules may result in In a seminal paper on generalization, Stokes
briefer social responses as the child may attempt and Baer (1977) described seven proactive strate-
to maximize reinforcement (i.e., engage in gies to promote generalization. Researchers have
shorter but more frequent bouts of the behavior to incorporated each of these strategies in prior
126 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

studies examining the effects of social skills exerts less control over the stimulus conditions
training in children (Chandler et al., 1992). The used during training. During this type of training,
first strategy, introducing naturally maintaining the child has the opportunity to contact the con-
contingencies, involves the use of contingencies tingencies under various stimulus conditions,
that maintain themselves in the child’s typical which encourages responding in the presence of
environment. Probably the best example of natu- novel stimuli. In other words, training loosely is
ral contingencies for social play is the use of similar to teaching sufficient exemplars, except
peers during training because the consequences that the instructor does not systematically control
provided by these peers are the same as those that the introduction of exemplars. La Greca and
the child will contact when emitting the behavior Santogrossi (1980) developed groups to teach
in the natural environment. For example, Laushey social skills to children without disability using
and Heflin (2000) implemented a buddy system modeling, coaching, and role-play. The results
for two children with ASD. The teacher instructed showed that the children receiving the interven-
peers to stay, play, and talk with both children. tion showed more social initiations in the class-
The contingencies in the training environment room. The intervention can be conceptualized as
(i.e., receiving social reinforcement through con- an example of training loosely because the
tinued interaction) were the same as the ones instructors exerted little control over the exem-
present in the natural environment (e.g., class- plars produced during role-play in the group con-
room, recess), which made it more likely that the text and over the questions that arose from the
children would show generalization. participants. In a more recent example, McMahon,
A second strategy to promote generalization is Vismara, and Solomon (2013) incorporated
to teach sufficient exemplars for the child to dis- unstructured play time within their social skills
play the behavior to untaught exemplars. A prac- training program, which could promote general-
titioner may train the behavior with multiple ization through the training loosely strategy.
persons, in many settings, or with different mate- To promote generalization over time, one of
rials (e.g., toys) in order to increase the likelihood the most common strategies is the use of indis-
of the learned behavior being emitted in novel criminable contingencies. These contingencies
stimulus conditions. To promote generalization involve the delivery of intermittent reinforcement
of helping behavior, Reeve, Reeve, Townsend, schedules, which have been repeatedly shown to
and Poulson (2007) taught multiple exemplars of be more resistant to extinction than continuous
helping by varying the teaching materials with reinforcement (Lerman, Iwata, Shore, & Kahng,
four children with ASD. Their results indicated 1996; MacDonald, Ahearn, Parry-Cruwys,
that teaching using multiple exemplars was effec- Bancroft, & Dube, 2013). To teach cooperative
tive in promoting multiple forms of generaliza- play to three children with intellectual disability,
tion. In an interesting variation of the peer buddy Lancioni (1982) showed that continuous edible
system, Gunter, Fox, Brady, Shores, and reinforcement was initially necessary, but that
Cavanaugh (1988) systematically introduced gradually thinning the schedule to a variable ratio
three different peers to teach social skills to two promoted the generalization of the skills.
children with ASD. Both children increased Likewise, Martins and Harris (2006) initially
appropriate responding to training peers, and used continuous reinforcement schedules to
one participant showed generalization to peers teach joint attention initiations to three children
outside training. In addition to representing the with ASD. Once each child had mastered the
use of naturally occurring contingencies (as dis- skill, the researchers changed to variable-ratio
cussed previously), this study also demonstrates schedules, which should promote both general-
the method of teaching sufficient exemplars by ization and maintenance at follow-up.
varying the peers used. A fifth strategy is to include stimuli common to
Third, practitioners may program for general- both the training and natural environments.
ization by training loosely; that is, the instructor Programming common stimuli is a relatively
Behavior Analytic Methods 127

simple strategy to promote generalization: The Finally, generalization can be conceptualized


instructor only needs to make the training envi- as an operant that can be reinforced as any other
ronment as similar as possible to the context in behavior. This strategy is typically referred to as
which the child is expected to display the social “train to generalize” (Stokes & Baer, 1977). For
skill. One common strategy to program common example, Lang et al. (2014) taught children with
stimuli is to include peers in the environment such ASD to play using lag schedules of reinforce-
as in peer-mediated treatments discussed earlier. ment. The intervention involved the reinforce-
In an interesting example, Beiers et al. (2016) ment of novel or different responses (i.e.,
taught a coach to prompt and reinforce appropri- response generalization) in order to increase vari-
ate social interactions during hockey practices. In ability in play and thus facilitate social integra-
this case, the prompting and reinforcement were tion. Another strategy can be to have parents
delivered by the same person and in the presence deliver reinforcement in the natural environment.
of the same peers as in the natural environment. In a study incorporating this strategy, Pfiffner and
The intervention effectively increased social McBurnett (1997) taught parents of children
interactions of both participants. Moreover, the with attention deficit disorder to provide social
procedures also increased the likelihood that the and token reinforcement for displaying learned
new learned skills would continue when the pro- social skills at home. In both previous examples,
cedures were faded. Another strategy is to con- generalization was reinforced as an operant,
duct the training in the environment in which the which should encourage responding under novel
skills will be used. To this end, multiple studies stimulus conditions or the production of novel
have shown that conducting training in schools responses.
may promote the generalization of learned social As with other behavior analytic methods,
skills (Bellini, Peters, Benner, & Hopf, 2007). these seven strategies are not mutually exclusive.
Children can also be taught to mediate their As an illustration, the peer buddy system is often
own generalization to promote the use of social a combination of naturally occurring contingen-
skills in novel contexts. Mediation takes on mul- cies, programming common stimuli, and multiple
tiple forms in the research literature. Notably, exemplars. Similarly, lag schedules of reinforce-
Alber and Heward (2000) recommend teaching ment are examples of both the indiscriminable
students to recruit attention in the form of praise contingencies and the train to generalize strate-
when using social skills appropriately, which gies. Practitioners should also note that the
could promote generalization. In a variation, research literature does not currently indicate
Hagopian, Kuhn, and Strother (2009) taught chil- whether one strategy is better than others.
dren to recruit attention to reduce inappropriate Therefore, we encourage practitioners to combine
social behavior; the results showed that the inter- multiple strategies together.
vention was effective, but the researchers did not
measure generalization of the new skill. Another
method of promoting generalization through Conclusions
mediation is to provide homework or handouts
following social skills training sessions in order In sum, several social skills training procedures
to prompt the child to practice the skill in other have been derived from behavior analytic princi-
contexts (La Greca & Santogrossi, 1980; ples. Most of these strategies have not been tested
Laugeson, Frankel, Gantman, Dillon, & Mogil, individually but rather as part of broader inter-
2012; Ollendick & Hersen, 1979). Self- vention packages. Given that the principles of
monitoring is an alternative form of mediation, behavior analysis should apply to most behaviors
which involves recording the frequency that the regardless of their topography, the results of stud-
skill was used outside the training setting (Ivy ies using these interventions provide sufficient
et al., 2016; Morrison, Kamps, Garcia, & Parker, support to be confident that they can also be
2001; Warrenfeltz et al., 1981). effective alone or in combination with other
128 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

interventions to improve social skills training. Bellini, S., Peters, J. K., Benner, L., & Hopf, A. (2007).
A meta-analysis of school-based social skills inter-
Social skills behavioral training generally
ventions for children with autism spectrum disorders.
involves prompts and reinforcement procedures Remedial and Special Education, 28, 153–162. doi:10
and may also include other behavior analytic .1177/07419325070280030401
strategies (Spence, 2003). As general guidelines, Bondy, A. S., & Frost, L. A. (1994). The picture
exchange communication system. Focus on Autism
we recommend that practitioners always conduct
and Other Developmental Disabilities, 9, 1–19.
an assessment prior to the implementation of doi:10.1177/108835769400900301
social skills intervention and collect data to mon- Brown, J. L., Krantz, P. J., McClannahan, L. E., &
itor its effects. When designing treatments, prac- Poulson, C. L. (2008). Using script fading to promote
natural environment stimulus control of verbal interac-
titioners should also consider combining multiple
tions among youths with autism. Research in Autism
procedures within social skills programs as is Spectrum Disorders, 2, 480–497. doi:10.1016/j.
often done in group training and peer-mediated rasd.2007.08.006
interventions. Last, generalization should not be Bryan, L. C., & Gast, D. L. (2000). Teaching on-task and
on-schedule behaviors to high-functioning children
expected to occur on its own following training
with autism via picture activity schedules. Journal of
but should rather be actively programmed. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 553–567.
Ultimately, researchers and practitioners alike doi: 10.1023/A:1005687310346
should take advantage of behavior analytic meth- Catania, A. (2013). Learning (5th ed.). Cornwall-on-
Hudson, NY: Sloan Publishing.
ods and research when implementing social skills
Chandler, L. K., Lubeck, R. C., & Fowler, S. A. (1992).
assessments and interventions with children. Generalization and maintenance of preschool chil-
dren’s social skills: A critical review and analysis.
Acknowledgement We thank Jennifer L. Cook for her Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 415–428.
comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript. doi:10.1901/jaba.1992.25-415
Charlop-Christy, M. H., Carpenter, M., Le, L., LeBlanc,
L. A., & Kellet, K. (2002). Using the picture exchange
communication system (pecs) with children with
References autism: Assessment of pecs acquisition, speech,
social-communicative behavior, and problem behavior.
Alber, S. R., & Heward, W. L. (2000). Teaching students Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 213–231.
to recruit positive attention: A review and recommen- doi:10.1901/jaba.2002.35-213
dations. Journal of Behavioral Education, 10, 177– Charlop-Christy, M. H., Le, L., & Freeman, K. A. (2000).
204. doi:10.1023/A:1012258231074 A comparison of video modeling with in vivo modeling
Allen, K. E., Hart, B., Buell, J. S., Harris, F. R., & for teaching children with autism. Journal of Autism
Wolf, M. M. (1964). Effects of social reinforcement and Developmental Disorders, 30, 537–552. doi:
on isolate behavior of a nursery school child. Child 10.1023/A:1005635326276
Development, 35, 511–518. doi:10.2307/1126714 Chebli, S. S., & Lanovaz, M. J. (2016). Using computer
Arntzen, E., Halstadtrø, A.-M., & Halstadtrø, M. (2003). tablets to assess preference for videos in children
Training play behavior in a 5-year-old boy with devel- with autism. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 9, 50–53.
opmental disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior doi:10.1007/s40617-016-0109-0
Analysis, 36, 367–370. doi:10.1901/jaba.2003.36-367 Cihak, D., Fahrenkrog, C., Ayres, K. M., & Smith, C.
Barton, E. E., & Wolery, M. (2008). Teaching pretend (2010). The use of video modeling via a video ipod
play to children with disabilities: A review of the lit- and a system of least prompts to improve transitional
erature. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, behaviors for students with autism spectrum dis-
28, 109–125. doi:10.1177/0271121408318799 orders in the general education classroom. Journal
Baruni, R. R., Rapp, J. T., Lipe, S. L., & Novotny, M. A. of Positive Behavior Interventions, 12, 103–115.
(2014). Using lag schedules to increase toy play doi:10.1177/1098300709332346
variability for children with intellectual disabilities. Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007).
Behavioral Interventions, 29, 21–35. doi:10.1002/ Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle
bin.1377 River, NJ: Pearson Education.
Beiers, K., Derby, K. M., & McLaughlin, T. F. (2016). Cowan, R. J., & Allen, K. D. (2007). Using naturalistic
Increasing social interactions using prompts and procedures to enhance learning in individuals with
rewards for adolescents with ASD in an ice hockey autism: A focus on generalized teaching within the
practice context. Educational Research Quarterly, 39, school setting. Psychology in the Schools, 44, 701–
40–56. 715. doi:10.1002/pits.20259
Behavior Analytic Methods 129

Davis-Temple, J., Jung, S., & Sainato, D. M. (2014). Hagopian, L. P., Kuhn, D. E., & Strother, G. E. (2009).
Teaching young children with special needs and their Targeting social skills deficits in an adolescent with
peers to play board games: Effects of a least to most pervasive development disorder. Journal of Applied
prompting procedure to increase independent per- Behavior Analysis, 42, 907–911. doi:10.1901/
formance. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 7, 21–30. jaba.2009.42-907
doi:10.1007/s40617-014-0001-8 Hagopian, L. P., Long, E. S., & Rush, K. S. (2004).
DeLeon, I. G., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Evaluation of a Preference assessment procedures for individuals with
multiple-stimulus presentation format for assessing developmental disabilities. Behavior Modification, 28,
reinforcer preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior 668–677. doi:10.1177/0145445503259836
Analysis, 29, 519–533. doi:10.1901/jaba.1996.29-519 Hagopian, L. P., Rush, K. S., Lewin, A. B., & Long,
DeQuinzio, J. A., Townsend, D. B., & Poulson, C. L. E. S. (2001). Evaluating the predictive validity of a
(2008). The effects of forward chaining and contin- single stimulus engagement preference assessment.
gent social interaction on the acquisition of com- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 475–485.
plex sharing responses by children with autism. doi:10.1901/jaba.2001.34-475
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 264–275. Hall, S. S., Maynes, N. P., & Reiss, A. L. (2009). Using
doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2007.06.006 percentile schedules to increase eye contact in children
Doll, C., McLaughlin, T. F., & Barretto, A. (2013). The with fragile X syndrome. Journal of Applied Behavior
token economy: A recent review and evaluation. Analysis, 42, 171–176. doi:10.1901/jaba.2009.42-171
International Journal of Basic and Applied Science, Haring, T. G., Kennedy, C. H., Adams, M. J., & Pitts-
2, 131–149. Conway, V. (1987). Teaching generalization of
Downs, A., Downs, R. C., Johansen, M., & Fossum, M. purchasing skills across community settings to
(2007). Using discrete trial teaching within a public autistic youth using videotape modeling. Journal of
preschool program to facilitate skill development in Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 89–96. doi:10.1901/
students with developmental disabilities. Education jaba.1987.20-89
and Treatment of Children, 30, 1–27. doi:10.1353/ Harrell, L. G., Kamps, D., & Kravits, T. (1997). The
etc.2007.0015 effects of peer networks on social-communicative
Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. P., behaviors for students with autism. Focus on Autism
Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparison of and Other Developmental Disabilities, 12, 241–256.
two approaches for identifying reinforcers for per- doi:10.1177/108835769701200406
sons with severe and profound disabilities. Journal of Hayward, D. W., Gale, C. M., & Eikeseth, S. (2009).
Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491–498. doi:10.1901/ Intensive behavioural intervention for young chil-
jaba.1992.25-491 dren with autism: A research-based service model.
Frea, W. D., & Hughes, C. (1997). Functional analysis and Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 571–580.
treatment of social-communicative behavior of ado- doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2008.12.002
lescents with developmental disabilities. Journal of Hopkins, B. L. (1968). Effects of candy and social rein-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 701–704. doi:10.1901/ forcement, instructions, and reinforcement schedule
jaba.1997.30-701 leaning on the modification and maintenance of smil-
Graetz, J. E., Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (2006). ing. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 121–
Show time: Using video self-modeling to decrease inap- 129. doi:10.1901/jaba.1968.1-121
propriate behavior. Teaching Exceptional Children, Horrocks, E., & Higbee, T. S. (2008). An evaluation of
38, 43–48. doi:10.1177/004005990603800506 a stimulus preference assessment of auditory stim-
Graff, R. B., & Karsten, A. M. (2012). Assessing prefer- uli for adolescents with developmental disabilities.
ences of individuals with developmental disabilities: Research in Developmental Disabilities, 29, 11–20.
A survey of current practices. Behavior Analysis in doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2006.09.003
Practice, 5, 37–48. Hurl, K., Wightman, J., Haynes, S. N., & Virues-Ortega,
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1984). Assessment and J. (2016). Does a pre-intervention functional assess-
classification of children’s social skills: A review of ment increase intervention effectiveness? A meta-
methods and issues. School Psychology Review, 13, analysis of within-subject interrupted time-series
292–301. studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 47, 71–84.
Gunter, P., Fox, J. J., Brady, M. P., Shores, R. E., & doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2016.05.003
Cavanaugh, K. (1988). Nonhandicapped peers as mul- Ivy, S. E., Lather, A. B., Hatton, D. D., & Wehby,
tiple exemplars: A generalization tactic for promoting J. H. (2016). Toward the development of a self-
autistic students’ social skills. Behavioral Disorders, management intervention to promote pro-social
13, 116–126. behaviors for students with visual impairment.
Hagiwara, T., & Myles, B. S. (1999). A multime- The Journal of Special Education, 50, 141.
dia social story intervention: Teaching skills doi:10.1177/0022466916630961
to children with autism. Focus on Autism and Jolly, A. C., Center, S. M., Test, D. W., & Spooner, F.
Other Developmental Disabilities, 14, 82–95. (1993). Using badges to increase initiations of chil-
doi:10.1177/108835769901400203 dren with severe disabilities in a play setting. Research
130 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 18, them: Some further refinements. Journal of Applied
46–51. doi:10.1177/154079699301800107 Behavior Analysis, 36, 407–414. doi:10.1901/
Jones, E. A. (2009). Establishing response and stimulus jaba.2003.36-407
classes for initiating joint attention in children with Laugeson, E. A., Frankel, F., Gantman, A., Dillon, A. R.,
autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, & Mogil, C. (2012). Evidence-based social skills
375–389. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2008.08.004 training for adolescents with autism spectrum dis-
Kagohara, D. M., Achmadi, D., van der Meer, L., Lancioni, orders: The ucla peers program. Journal of Autism
G. E., O’Reilly, M. F., Lang, R., … Sigafoos, J. (2013). and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1025–1036.
Teaching two students with Asperger syndrome to doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1339-1
greet adults using social stories™ and video modeling. Laushey, K. M., & Heflin, L. J. (2000). Enhancing social
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, skills of kindergarten children with autism through the
25, 241–251. doi:10.1007/s10882-012-9300-6 training of multiple peers as tutors. Journal of Autism
Kang, S., O’Reilly, M., Lancioni, G., Falcomata, T. S., and Developmental Disorders, 30, 183–193. doi:10.1
Sigafoos, J., & Xu, Z. (2013). Comparison of the 023/A:1005558101038
predictive validity and consistency among preference Lepper, T. L., Devine, B., & Petursdottir, A. I. (2016).
assessment procedures: A review of the literature. Application of a lag contingency to reduce perse-
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 1125– veration on circumscribed interests. Developmental
1133. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.12.021 Neurorehabilitation, 20, 313. doi:10.3109/17518423.
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Hurley, C., & Frea, W. D. 2016.1152612
(1992). Improving social skills and disruptive behav- Lerman, D. C., Iwata, B. A., Shore, B. A., & Kahng, S.
ior in children with autism through self-management. (1996). Responding maintained by intermittent rein-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 341–353. forcement: Implications for the use of extinction
doi:10.1901/jaba.1992.25-341 with problem behavior in clinical settings. Journal of
Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1993). Teaching Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 153–171. doi:10.1901/
children with autism to initiate to peers: Effects jaba.1996.29-153
of a script-fading procedure. Journal of Applied Libby, M. E., Weiss, J. S., Bancroft, S., & Ahearn, W. H.
Behavior Analysis, 26, 121–132. doi:10.1901/ (2008). A comparison of most-to-least and least-to-
jaba.1993.26-121 most prompting on the acquisition of solitary play
Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1998). Social skills. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1, 37–43.
interaction skills for children with autism: A script- Liber, D. B., Frea, W. D., & Symon, J. B. (2008). Using
fading procedure for beginning readers. Journal of time-delay to improve social play skills with peers
Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 191–202. doi:10.1901/ for children with autism. Journal of Autism and
jaba.1998.31-191 Developmental Disorders, 38, 312–323. doi:10.1007/
Kroeger, K. A., Schultz, J. R., & Newsom, C. (2007). A s10803-007-0395-z
comparison of two group-delivered social skills pro- Lovaas, O. I. (2003). Teaching individuals with develop-
grams for young children with autism. Journal of mental delays: Basic intervention techniques. Austin,
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 808–817. TX: Pro-Ed.
doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0207-x Maag, J. W. (2005). Social skills training for youth
La Greca, A. M., & Santogrossi, D. A. (1980). with emotional and behavioral disorders and learn-
Social skills training with elementary school stu- ing disabilities: Problems, conclusions, and sugges-
dents: A behavioral group approach. Journal of tions. Exceptionality, 13, 155–172. doi:10.1207/
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 48, 220–227. s15327035ex1303_2
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.48.2.220 MacDonald, J. M., Ahearn, W. H., Parry-Cruwys, D.,
Lancioni, G. E. (1982). Normal children as tutors to Bancroft, S., & Dube, W. V. (2013). Persistence dur-
teach social responses to withdrawn mentally retarded ing extinction: Examining the effects of continuous
schoolmates: Training, maintenance, and generaliza- and intermittent reinforcement on problem behavior.
tion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, 17–40. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 333–338.
doi:10.1901/jaba.1982.15-17 doi:10.1002/jaba.3
Lancioni, G. E. (1983). Using pictorial representations as MacDuff, G. S., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E.
communication means with low-functioning children. (1993). Teaching children with autism to use pho-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 13, tographic activity schedules: Maintenance and gen-
87–105. doi:10.1007/BF01531362 eralization of complex response chains. Journal of
Lang, R., Machalicek, W., Rispoli, M., O’Reilly, M., Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 89–97. doi:10.1901/
Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G., … Didden, R. (2014). jaba.1993.26-89
Play skills taught via behavioral intervention gener- Maione, L., & Miranda, P. (2006). Effects of video mod-
alize, maintain, and persist in the absence of socially eling and video feedback on peer-directed social lan-
mediated reinforcement in children with autism. guage skills of a child with autism. Journal of Positive
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8, 860–872. Behavior Interventions, 8, 106–118. doi:10.1177/109
doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2014.04.007 83007060080020201
Laraway, S., Snycerski, S., Michael, J., & Poling, A. Martins, M. P., & Harris, S. L. (2006). Teaching children
(2003). Motivating operations and terms to describe with autism to respond to joint attention initiations.
Behavior Analytic Methods 131

Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 28, 51–68. O’Connell, M., Lieberman, L. J., & Petersen, S. (2006).
doi:10.1300/J019v28n01_04 The use of tactile modeling and physical guidance as
Matson, J. L., Bamburg, J. W., Cherry, K. E., & Paclawskyj, instructional strategies in physical activity for children
T. R. (1999). A validity study on the questions about who are blind. Journal of Visual Impairement and
behavioral function (qabf) scale: Predicting treatment Blindness, 100, 471–477.
success for self-injury, aggression, and stereotypies. O’Connor, R. D. (1972). Relative efficacy of modeling,
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 20, 163–175. shaping, and the combined procedures for modifi-
doi:10.1016/S0891-4222(98)00039-0 cation of social withdrawal. Journal of Abnormal
Matson, J. L., Fee, V. E., Coe, D. A., & Smith, D. (1991). Psychology, 79, 327–334. doi:10.1037/h0033226
A social skills program for developmentally delayed Odom, S. L., Collet-Klingenberg, L., Rogers, S. J., &
preschoolers. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, Hatton, D. D. (2010). Evidence-based practices in
20, 428–433. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2004_11 interventions for children and youth with autism spec-
Matson, J. L., & Wilkins, J. (2009). Psychometric test- trum disorders. Preventing School Failure: Alternative
ing methods for children’s social skills. Research Education for Children and Youth, 54, 275–282.
in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 249–274. doi:10.1080/10459881003785506
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2008.04.002 Odom, S. L., & McConnell, S. R. (1992). Improving
McCoy, K., & Hermansen, E. (2007). Video modeling social competence: An applied behavior analysis per-
for individuals with autism: A review of model types spective. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25,
and effects. Education and Treatment of Children, 30, 239–244. doi:10.1901/jaba.1992.25-239
183–213. doi:10.1353/etc.2007.0029 Odom, S. L., & Strain, P. S. (1984). Peer-mediated
McFall, R. M. (1982). A review and reformulation of the approaches to promoting children’s social interaction:
concept of social skills. Behavioral Assessment, 4, A review. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 54,
1–33. doi:10.1007/BF01321377 544–557. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1984.tb01525.x
McMahon, C. M., Vismara, L. A., & Solomon, M. (2013). Ollendick, T. H., & Hersen, M. (1979). Social
Measuring changes in social behavior during a social skills training for juvenile delinquents.
skills intervention for higher-functioning children and Behaviour Research & Therapy, 17, 547–554.
adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of doi:10.1016/0005-7967(79)90098-6
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1843–1856. Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., Edwards, G. L., Iwata, B. A.,
doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1733-3 & Page, T. J. (1985). Assessment of stimulus prefer-
Michael, J. (1993). Establishing operations. The Behavior ence and reinforcer value with profoundly retarded
Analyst, 16, 191–206. individuals. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18,
Morrison, L., Kamps, D., Garcia, J., & Parker, D. (2001). 249–255. doi:10.1901/jaba.1985.18-249
Peer mediation and monitoring strategies to improve Page, S., & Neuringer, A. (1985). Variability is an
initiations and social skills for students with autism. operant. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 3, 237– Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 429–452.
250. doi:10.1177/109830070100300405 doi:10.1037/0097-7403.11.3.429
Murzynski, N. T., & Bourret, J. C. (2007). Combining Parker, D., & Kamps, D. (2011). Effects of task analy-
video modeling and least-to-most prompting for estab- sis and self-monitoring for children with autism
lishing response chains. Behavioral Interventions, 22, in multiple social settings. Focus on Autism and
147–152. doi:10.1002/bin.224 Other Developmental Disabilities, 26, 131–142.
Neidert, P. L., Dozier, C. L., Iwata, B. A., & Hafen, M. doi:10.1177/1088357610376945
(2010). Behavior analysis in intellectual and develop- Peca, A., Simut, R., Pintea, S., Costescu, C., &
mental disabilities. Psychological Services, 7, 103– Vanderborght, B. (2014). How do typically developing
113. doi:10.1037/a0018791 children and children with autism perceive different
Norman, J. M., Collins, B. C., & Schuster, J. W. social robots? Computers in Human Behavior, 41,
(2001). Using an instrucional package includ- 268–277. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.09.035
ing video technology to teach self-help skills Pfiffner, L. J., & McBurnett, K. (1997). Social skills
to elementary students with mental disabilities. training with parent generalization: Treatment effects
Journal of Special Education Technology, 16, 5–18. for children with attention deficit disorder. Journal of
doi:10.1177/016264340101600301 Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 749–757.
Northup, J., George, T., Jones, K., Broussard, C., & doi:10.1037/0022-006X.65.5.749
Vollmer, T. R. (1996). A comparison of reinforcer Rayner, C. (2011). Teaching students with autism to tie
assessment methods: The utility of verbal and picto- a shoelace knot using video prompting and backward
rial choice procedures. Journal of Applied Behavior chaining. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 14,
Analysis, 29, 201–212. doi:10.1901/jaba.1996.29-201 339–347. doi:10.3109/17518423.2011.606508
Nuzzolo-Gomez, R., Leonard, M. A., Ortiz, E., Rivera, Reeve, S. A., Reeve, K. F., Townsend, D. B., & Poulson,
C. M., & Greer, R. D. (2002). Teaching children with C. L. (2007). Establishing a generalized repertoire of
autism to prefer books or toys over stereotypy or pas- helping behavior in children with autism. Journal of
sivity. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4, Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 123–136. doi:10.1901/
80–87. doi:10.1177/109830070200400203 jaba.2007.11-05
132 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

Reichow, B., Steiner, A. M., & Volkmar, F. (2013). Smith, T. (2001). Discrete trial training in the treatment
Cochrane review: Social skills groups for people of autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
aged 6 to 21 with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Disabilities, 16, 86–92. doi:10.1177/1088357601
Evidence-Based Child Health: A Cochrane Review 01600204
Journal, 8, 266–315. doi:10.1002/ebch.1903 Spence, S. H. (2003). Social skills training with children
Reichow, B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2010). Social skills and young people: Theory, evidence and practice.
interventions for individuals with autism: Evaluation Child and Adolescent Mental Heath, 8, 84–96.
for evidence-based practices within a best evi- doi:10.1111/1475-3588.00051
dence synthesis framework. Journal of Autism and Spooner, F., Spooner, D., & Ulicny, G. (1986). Comparison
Developmental Disorders, 40, 149–166. doi:10.1007/ of modified backward chaining: Backward chaining
s10803-009-0842-0 with leap-aheads and reverse chaining with leap-
Resetar, J. L., & Noell, G. H. (2008). Evaluating pref- aheads. Education and Treatment of Children, 9,
erence assessments for use in the general education 122–134.
population. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit
447–451. doi:10.1901/jaba.2008.41-447 technology of generalization. Journal of Applied
Riley, G. A. (1995). Guidelines for devising a hierar- Behavior Analysis, 10, 349–367. doi:10.1901/
chy when fading response prompts. Education and jaba.1977.10-349
Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities, Taylor, B. A., & Hoch, H. (2008). Teaching children with
30, 231–242. autism to respond to and initiate bids for joint atten-
Roane, H. S., Vollmer, T. R., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus, tion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 377–
B. A. (1998). Evaluation of a brief stimulus preference 391. doi:10.1901/jaba.2008.41-377
assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, Tetreault, A. S., & Lerman, D. C. (2010). Teaching social
605–620. doi:10.1901/jaba.1998.31-605 skills to children with autism using point-of-view
Rogers, S. J. (2000). Interventions that facilitate socializa- video modeling. Education & Treatment of Children,
tion in children with autism. Journal of Autism and 33, 395–419. doi:10.1353/etc.0.0105
Developmental Disorders, 30, 399–409. doi:10.102 Thomson, K., Walters, K., Martin, G. L., & Yu, C. T.
3/A:1005543321840 (2011). Teaching adaptive and social skills to individ-
Rotatori, A. F., Fox, B., & Switzky, H. (1979). An indirect uals with autism spectrum disorders. In J. L. Matson
technique for establishing preferences for categories & P. Sturmey (Eds.), International handbook of autism
of reinforcement for severely and profoundly retarded and pervasive developmental disorders (pp. 339–354).
individuals. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 48, 1307– New York, NY: Springer.
1313. doi:10.2466/pms.1979.48.3c.1307 Vallinger-Brown, M., & Rosales, R. (2014). An inves-
Russo, D. C., & Koegel, R. L. (1977). A method for inte- tigation of stimulus pairing and listener training
grating an autistic child into a normal public-school to establish emergent intraverbals in children with
classroom1. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 30, 148–159.
579–590. doi:10.1901/jaba.1977.10-579 doi:10.1007/s40616-014-0014-y
Sarokoff, R. A., Taylor, B. A., & Poulson, C. L. (2001). Verriden, A. L., & Roscoe, E. M. (2016). A compari-
Teaching children with autism to engage in conversa- son of preference-assessment methods. Journal
tion exchanges: Script fading with embedded textual of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49, 265–285.
stimuli. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, doi:10.1002/jaba.302
81–84. doi:10.1901/jaba.2001.34-81 Virués-Ortega, J., Pritchard, K., Grant, R. L., North,
Schipley-Benamou, R., Lutzker, J. R., & Taubman, M. S., Hurtado-Parrado, C., Lee, M. S. H., … Yu, C. T.
(2002). Teaching daily living skills to children with (2014). Clinical decision making and preference
autism through instructional video modeling. Journal assessment for individuals with intellectual and
of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4(3), 166–177. doi developmental disabilities. American Journal on
:10.1177/10983007020040030501 Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 119,
Sherer, M., Pierce, K. L., Paredes, S., Kisacky, K. L., 151–170. doi:10.1352/1944-7558-119.2.151
Ingersoll, B., & Schreibman, L. (2001). Enhancing Warrenfeltz, R. B., Kelly, W. J., Salzberg, C. L., Beegle,
conversation skills in children with autism via video C. P., Levy, S. M., Adams, T. A., & Crouse, T. R.
technology: Which is better, “self” or “other” as (1981). Social skills training of behavior disordered
a model? Behavior Modification, 25(1), 140–158. adolescents with self-monitoring to promote general-
doi:10.1177/0145445501251008 ization to a vocational setting. Behavioral Disorders,
Shreibman, L. (1975). Effects of within-stimulus and 7, 18–27.
extra-stimulus prompting on discrimination learn- Wolery, M., & Gast, D. L. (1984). Effective and efficient
ing in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior procedures for the transfer of stimulus control. Topics
Analysis, 8, 91–112. doi:10.1901/jaba.1975.8-91 in Early Childhood Special Education, 4, 52–77.
Shrestha, A., Anderson, A., & Moore, D. W. (2012). doi:10.1177/0271-1214/84/0043-0052
Using point-of-view video modeling and forward Yilmaz, I., & Birkan, B. (2005). Using a constant time
chaining to teach a functional self-help skill to a child delay procedure to teach aquatic play skills to children
with autism. Journal of Behavioral Education, 22, with autism. Education and Training in Autism and
157–167. doi:10.1007/s10864-012-9165-x Developmental Disabilities, 40, 171–182.
Social Learning Instructional
Models

Renae Beaumont, Jo Hariton, Shannon Bennett,


Amy Miranda, and Elisabeth Sheridan Mitchell

their peers the skills they need to establish,


Introduction develop and maintain satisfactory friendships.
Child social skills training (SST) programs
Developing and maintaining interpersonal rela- first began to flourish in the 1970s and 1980s in
tionships with peers is a hallmark of adaptive schools and clinics. The programs that are avail-
childhood development (Foster & Bussman, able today are diverse in the social skills that they
2008). Children who fail to achieve this mile- target, their instructional methods and program
stone are at risk of a variety of negative outcomes, format (small group or whole class). Common
including dropping out of school, juvenile delin- skill targets include starting, continuing and end-
quency and impaired mental health that may ing conversations and play activities with others,
emerge or persist into adolescence and adulthood social problem-solving and preventing and man-
(Kupersmidt, Coie, & Dodge, 1990; Mikami & aging bullying and teasing. Some programs (e.g.
Hinshaw, 2006). Due to the negative long-term PATHS, Kusche & Greenberg, 1994; Secret
consequences of poor peer relationships in child- Agent Society Program, Beaumont, 2010;
hood, there has been a strong focus in the educa- Superheroes Social Skills, Jenson et al., 2011)
tional and psychological literature on methods to also teach foundational skills in emotion recogni-
teach children who are rejected or ignored by tion and regulation prior to social interaction

R. Beaumont, Ph.D. (*) A. Miranda, L.C.S.W.


Weill Cornell Medicine, New York Presbyterian Department of Child Psychiatry, Payne Whitney
Hospital, 525 East 68th Street, Box 147, New York, Clinic, New York Presbyterian Hospital and Weill
NY 10065, USA Cornell Medicine, 525 East 68th Street, Box 147,
e-mail: rbb2002@med.cornell.edu; renae@psy.uq.edu.au New York, NY 10065, USA
e-mail: amm9069@nyp.org
J. Hariton, Ph.D.
Weill Cornell Medicine, New York Presbyterian E.S. Mitchell, Ph.D.
Hospital, Westchester Division, 21 Bloomingdale Westchester Division, Department of Psychiatry,
Road, White Plains, NY 10605, USA Weill Cornell Medicine, Center for Autism and the
e-mail: jhariton@med.cornell.edu Developing Brain, New York Presbyterian Hospital,
21 Bloomingdale Road, Rogers Building,
S. Bennett, Ph.D.
White Plains, NY 10605, USA
Pediatric OCD, Anxiety, and Tic Disorders Program,
e-mail: esm9015@med.cornell.edu
Youth Anxiety Center, Weill Cornell Medicine,
New York Presbyterian Hospital, 525 East 68th
Street, Box 147, New York, NY 10065, USA
e-mail: smb9017@med.cornell.edu

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 133


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_9
134 R. Beaumont et al.

skills. These programs are founded on the prem- Tech-­tools such as apps, computer games and
ise that an impaired ability to recognise emotions virtual reality technology are also providing new
in oneself and others and manage one’s emotions and exciting ways of teaching and supporting
effectively will impair a child’s performance of children to apply social skills.
social skills. This chapter will provide an overview and
Children with a variety of mental health critique of the above social skill teaching and
conditions are likely to display social impair- application methods and the evidence support-
ments, although the factors underlying these ing them, as applied to primary school-aged
may differ. Children with an autism spectrum students. Examples of programs that employ
disorder (ASD) are generally understood to these teaching methods will be presented. The
have social skill deficits, whereas children with chapter will conclude with recommendations
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder for future research in the SST domain to further
(ADHD) or other externalising disorders may determine what social skills training methods
have intact social skills but an impaired ability are effective for children presenting with differ-
to demonstrate their social skills on a consis- ent mental health conditions and/or presenting
tent basis due to challenges with sustaining problems.
their focus during social exchanges, impulsiv-
ity and hyperactivity (Mikami, Jia, & Noa,
2014). The difficulties underlying the impaired Social Skills Instructional Methods
social interactions of children with social anxi-
ety may relate more to their distorted percep- Didactic Instruction
tions of social situations and bias to threat cues
(Cartwright-Hatton, Tschernitz, & Gomersall, Didactic instruction involves explicitly teaching
2005). Often children present with a combina- a child the steps involved in a social skill. To
tion of skills and performance deficits. A com- boost a child’s motivation to learn and apply a
prehensive assessment helps to determine the skill, a rationale for learning the skill and an
unique combination of social skill and perfor- explanation of how it can help the child are typi-
mance deficits to be targeted for a given child cally initially provided (Bourke & Van Hasselt,
and the optimal way to do this. 2001). Didactic instruction is the predominate
Most available SST programs involve a mix of focus of traditional SST approaches and is typi-
teaching methods, including didactic instruction, cally used in conjunction with other teaching
modelling (including peer-, instructor-led or methods (particularly modelling and behavioural
video-modelling of target skills), behavioural rehearsal). As such, the research literature sheds
rehearsal and the provision of feedback. Strategies little light on the effectiveness of this technique
to motivate children to engage and participate in as a stand-alone SST strategy. The specific social
skills training lessons are of critical importance, skills taught are tailored to the identified needs of
particularly in a group context, where one child’s a child. It may take a child several lessons/ses-
disruptive behaviour can impair others’ learning. sions to learn and competently demonstrate one
One of the greatest challenges with SST is sup- skill before moving on to the next.
porting children to generalise skills learned in Didactic instruction typically involves task
training to daily life. Several strategies have been analysis and chaining. Task analysis breaks down
employed to address this in contemporary SST a complex skill into its component steps. For
programs, including assigning between-session example, the social skill of preventing and man-
homework tasks, visual supports, upskilling aging bullying and teasing can be broken down
peers to help children to put their social skills into specific steps that can be taught individually:
into action when needed and training parents and (1) stay close to friends and away from others
teachers to prompt, praise and reward children who tease, (2) maintain a calm face and body, (3)
for using their social skills when needed. say something to stand up for yourself, (4) calmly
Social Learning Instructional Models 135

walk away and (5) go to a safe place. Once the help of an adult (Gray, 1994). Feelings are
individual steps are identified, each component typically illustrated with different colours.
part can be taught and demonstrated individually Well-­controlled evaluations of the effectiveness
before being chained together in a sequence. of Comic Strip Conversations are lacking,
Once the child has mastered the component steps, although clinicians and teachers often describe
he/she can integrate them to successfully execute them as a valuable teaching technique (especially
a coordinated bully management plan. for children who learn best visually). They can
Social Stories A creative example of a didac- also help to reduce a child’s level of anxiety or
tic instruction technique that has been predomi- distress when discussing a social problem that
nantly used with students with ASD is Social has occurred or an upcoming social challenge, as
Stories. Developed by Carol Gray, Social Stories the child’s focus is on drawing the stick figures,
typically involve an adult working with a child to rather than verbally describing what happened to
write a story that explains why the child needs to them in a situation.
perform a particular skill or behaviour in a cer-
tain context and the skill steps involved. Gray
(2015) provides specific guidelines for how a Modelling
social story is to be constructed to achieve opti-
mal learning outcomes. The focus of the story is Modelling involves a child observing the behav-
to be on describing a target situation and why a iour, attitudes and emotional responses of others.
skill is to be used, rather than directing a child Observation and imitation are the core compo-
what to do. The story is tailored to the develop- nents of the learning process. Research suggests
mental level of the child and can feature illustra- that this teaching technique is more effective if
tions or pictures. For example, a social story children are shown models who they perceive to
could be written about why it is important for a have characteristics similar to themselves (e.g.
child to raise his/her hand before talking in class, age, gender, interests; Kamps et al., 2002).
with specific details included about the child and Children are also more likely to imitate the
the specific class. Large-scale well-controlled behaviours of others that they believe are admi-
evaluations of social stories are lacking. However, rable or powerful (Bandura, 1977; Kazdin, 2001).
case studies suggest that Social Stories can help Models can be live or videotaped. Rather than
in reducing disruptive behaviours (Scattone, being told the component steps of a skill (didactic
Wilczynski, Edwards, & Rabian, 2002) and instruction), the child is shown the skill steps in
improving conversational abilities (Sansosti & action by an instructor, peer, caregiver or via a
Powell-Smith, 2006) in children with ASD. They video demonstration. This social skill instruc-
are also likely to have merit as a teaching tech- tional method has significant empirical support
nique for other children whose lack of under- (Gresham, 1985), although it is rarely used as a
standing as to why they should perform a social stand-alone teaching tool in SST programs.
skill in a given context (or refrain from perform- In Vivo Modelling In vivo modelling involves
ing an inappropriate behaviour) contribute to an instructor, peer or caregiver demonstrating a
their social difficulties. skill for a child in real time. One of the strengths
Comic Strip Conversations Comic strip con- of in vivo modelling is that it can be done in an
versations are a visual teaching tool (again devel- actual problematic social situation for a child,
oped by Carol Gray and drawn from the autism with the modelled response tailored to the spe-
literature) to improve children’s understanding of cific needs of the situation. For example, if a
social situations. They involve a child ideally tak- child becomes frustrated in session, the instructor
ing the lead in drawing stick figure pictures (like can pause and demonstrate for the child how to
cartoon strips) showing who was involved in an use relaxation strategies such as taking slow
event, what people said and did and what they breaths and thinking helpful thoughts (cognitive
were likely to be thinking and feeling, with the behavioural therapy techniques) to calm down.
136 R. Beaumont et al.

The child can then be prompted to perform naturalistic environments (Belllini & Akullian,
these skills themselves (behavioural rehearsal— 2007). Recent advances in technology have
see below). addressed this concern and have used more natu-
Training peers as role models can be a particu- ralistic video modelling procedures. For instance,
larly powerful teaching approach, as children Simpson, Langone, and Ayres (2004) conducted
may see them as being more similar to them- a study with four children where computer-­based
selves than adult instructors. Kamps et al. (2002) video modelling was done in a classroom setting,
showed that this approach was more effective at allowing for the video to be shown within the
increasing the frequency of social initiations and context of the actual problematic social situation.
the quality of social interactions and facilitated Results suggested that the technique shows
more skill generalisation with an ASD popula- promise, with students showing increases in their
tion than adult centred training. Peer-mediated unprompted use of social skills in the classroom
interventions appear to be particularly powerful environment (Simpson et al., 2004). Clinical
in boosting the popularity of socially neglected experience suggests that children most likely to
children (Collins, Gresham, & Dart, 2016). respond to video self-modelling are those who
Video Modelling Video modelling involves a enjoy being filmed and like watching themselves
child being shown a filmed demonstration of a on film.
skill. The film clip can be stopped and replayed at Video modelling and in vivo modelling have
any time by the child, educator or instructor to been shown to be equally efficacious for pre-
allow for discussion and skill consolidation, school children with ASD (Gena, Couloura, &
deepening a child’s learning. Advantages of Kymissis, 2005). However, video modelling
video modelling over in vivo modelling include appeared to result in faster acquisition of skills
the ability to remove distractors or unnecessary than did in vivo modelling (Charlop-Christy, Le,
details from the skill demonstration and the & Freeman, 2000).
capacity for a child to review the skill demonstra- Video modelling has also been utilised suc-
tion multiple times. To optimise children’s cessfully in SST programs for children with
engagement and learning from filmed skill dem- ADHD, early-onset conduct problems and oppo-
onstrations, it is important to use motivating sitional defiant disorder. For example, it is a pri-
themes, everyday appropriate conversational lan- mary technique used in Webster-Stratton’s (2006)
guage that is used by the child’s peers and play Incredible Years Programs. In the child program,
objects or activities that interest the child. In video modelling is used in every session to pro-
recording video clips, it has been recommended mote discussion, problem solving and behav-
that 75–80% of the clip duration involve footage ioural rehearsal of prosocial behaviour. The
of the target behaviour(s) and that the child watch scenes selected represent home and school situa-
the video at least two times before assessing for tions where teasing, lying, stealing, etc. occur.
skill acquisition (Charlop-Christy, 2004). After watching them, children discuss feelings,
Video self-modelling involves the child acting generate ideas for effective responses and role-­
as the model in the video clip. The child either play solutions. Life-size puppets are also used by
receives adult prompts to successfully demon- therapists to model appropriate behaviours and
strate a behavioural sequence or video footage of thinking processes for children (Webster-Stratton
the behavioural sequence is spliced from many & Reid, 2016). Research has shown that the com-
imperfect demonstrations to create an ideal pro- bination of group discussion, a trained therapist
totype clip. For children with ASD, a meta-­ and videotape modelling produced more endur-
analysis suggested that video modelling is ing treatment gains from the Incredible Years
effective for teaching social communication skills Program than treatment that involved only one
and functional skills. However, generalisation was training component (Webster-Stratton,
limited because the skills were not targeted in Hollinsworth, & Kolpacoff, 1989).
Social Learning Instructional Models 137

Behavioural Rehearsal generalisation for children with ASD. These


children typically need additional guidance and
It is vitally important for children to practice support to apply social skills ‘in the moment’
skills taught in session before trying them out in when they need them (see the skill generalisation
daily life. Research suggests that behavioural section below for further details).
rehearsal facilitates encoding, memory and
retrieval of skill steps (King & Kirschenbaum,
1992). Behavioural rehearsal is a structured Feedback and Self-Evaluation
teaching process that involves the instructor or
other group members playing the role of charac- Feedback involves adults or peers informing a
ters in a specific scenario while the child pretends child about how they performed a skill during a
to be him/herself. For example, after discussing role-play or everyday situation. Positive feedback
how to handle teasing and bullying, an educator can be provided in the form of praise, approval or
or therapist may model the sequence of steps for tangible rewards. Constructive feedback helps to
a child using a scenario that is relevant to the shape behaviour and helps children to develop a
child’s daily life and then ask the child to demon- greater self-awareness of their social skill
strate the steps. A behavioural sequence (e.g. strengths and areas of difficulty. The feedback
saying ‘Whatever…’ to a bully with a calm face process can be enhanced by filming a child’s
and body and walking away) can be repeatedly social skill demonstrations and then watching the
practiced by the child to improve his or her skill footage with the child. The filmed skill demon-
and confidence in performing it. The child can stration can be stopped, played back and con-
also take on different roles in the vignette to cretely analysed with the child or as a small
improve his or her perspective-taking skills. For group activity with peers.
example, by taking on the role of the ‘bully’, the Research has shown that the effectiveness of
child may realise that the reason the other child feedback can be increased when it is provided
yelled ‘Get out of my way, loser!’ was because he immediately after a skill has been performed, is
or she walked in front of him in the middle of a specific and concrete, emphasises the positive
handball game. and supplies constructive information to shape
If a child is anxious about trying something future skill performances (Kazdin, 2001). One of
new or performing in front of others, he or she the risks of providing feedback to children is
may initially be reluctant to role-play a skill. In their perception of it as criticism. A way to avoid
this instance, it can be helpful to introduce role-­ this is for educators, therapists and fellow group
play through simple games like charades or a members to be trained in how to provide feed-
board game that involves role-play elements, back constructively by starting with the positives,
such as the Socially Speaking Game (Schroeder, being specific and only providing one suggestion
2003). It may also be helpful to demonstrate the for improvement. Children should also be encour-
skill first, with the child taking on a nonspeaking aged to self-evaluate their performance of social
role or a role that involves minimal dialogue that skills (in session and in daily life situations) to
is scripted. Tokens or other concrete reinforcers build their self-awareness of their behaviour and
can be offered to encourage participation and how it impacts on others.
shape the child (Rose & Edleson, 1987).
Role-plays allow for ‘in vivo’ skill practice
and provide a fun way for children to actively Behaviour Management
participate in SST. However, like all of the teach-
ing techniques reviewed in this chapter, it is not Effective behaviour management strategies are
recommended that this technique be used in iso- critical, irrespective of whether an instructor is
lation. Matson, Sevin, and Box (1995) found that delivering individual or group SST. Children who
role-plays alone are unlikely to lead to social skill have social skills deficits are more likely to have
138 R. Beaumont et al.

low frustration tolerance and emotion dysregula- and school staff involvement, delivery of programs
tion. Effective strategies need to be used both pre- within a school setting and peer buddy programs.
ventatively and reactively to help children to
engage and learn in session and to manage their
emotions and behaviours. It is recommended that Homework Tasks
clear behavioural expectations are spelled out at
the outset. These may take the form of a group A cornerstone of several evidence-based cognitive
rules chart, verbal or written contract or individu- behavioural SST interventions is homework tasks.
alised skill target card for each child. Children can Independent research trials of the Secret Agent
be reinforced for performing behavioural targets Society social skills program described later in this
(e.g. using a friendly face, voice and words, trying chapter (e.g. Beaumont & Sofronoff, 2008; Einfeld
their best, listening quietly to others’ ideas) with et al., 2017) have consistently shown that the only
praise and tokens or points that are exchanged for consistent and reliable predictor of treatment out-
tangible rewards at the end of session. come is the completion of between-session skills
It is important that social skills curricula practice tasks (‘missions’). For example, the
include self-control skills (e.g. emotion regula- ‘relaxation gadgetry’ mission in this program
tion, problem solving) so that children can be involves children using relaxation ‘gadgets’ or
prompted to use these when needed in session. strategies that they learn in session to calm down
Should a child become too behaviourally dys- when they detect low to medium levels of anxiety
regulated in the heat of the moment, it may be or anger in daily life and then answering questions
necessary to take a time out from the session or to about how helpful these strategies are in a secret
curtail the length of the session for that day. The agent journal. Every effort should be made to
break time should be used as a preventative self-­ make homework tasks fun and engaging for chil-
control technique taught to children early in the dren, to increase the chances that these tasks will
program. Children can also be encouraged to ask be completed. For example, in the Secret Agent
for it when needed and rewarded for calming Society program (Beaumont, 2010), children can
down and re-engaging in session activities. collect electronic ‘evidence’ (photos, film clips or
voice memos) of their mission completion with a
smart device with adult permission and create pic-
 trategies to Promote Social Skill
S tures using a scene generator device in the Secret
Generalisation Agent Society computer game to answer mission
questions.
The lack of generalisation of skills to environ-
ments outside of the treatment setting is a com-
mon criticism of traditional SST approaches. Visual Supports
Many old school SST programs focused mainly
on in session instruction and used a ‘train and Children vary in their learning style, with some
hope strategy’ when it came to skill generalisa- struggling to process and learn information pre-
tion (DuPaul & Eckert, 1994). Research indicates sented solely in verbal format. Visual supports
that skills learned in these outpatient clinic pro- can be particularly valuable for children who pro-
grams frequently did not generalise well to home, cess information best visually. Photographs and
school or community settings (Crager & Horvath, visual behaviour support cards (portable or dis-
2003; Marriage, Gordon, & Brand, 1995; played in a prominent place) can help children
Storebo, Gluud, Winkel, & Simonsen, 2012). By remember and implement the constituent steps of
contrast, several contemporary SST programs complex social skills in the environments where
include a variety of specific techniques to pro- they are needed. For example, in the Superheroes
mote social skills application and generalisa- Social Skills program (Jenson et al., 2011)
tion across settings. These include assigning described later in this chapter, children are given
homework activities, visual supports, caregiver a card listing three to five steps for each social
Social Learning Instructional Models 139

skill taught in the program. Parents and school programs are increasingly being offered in the
staff can prompt children to refer to pocket-sized school environment. However, until recently,
social skill cards in private to remind themselves there has been limited empirical support for the
of the steps of a target skill (e.g. talking to others) effectiveness of this approach. For example,
immediately before they enter a situation where Bellini, Peters, Benner, and Hope (2007) con-
the skill is used (e.g. lunch break). The cards can ducted a meta-analysis of school-based SST
also be used to help children review the skill steps interventions for children with ASD and found
they did well and any that they would improve on minimal treatment effects and generalisation
after a target skill has been demonstrated. across settings. By contrast, contemporary effec-
tiveness trials examining interventions that more
specifically program for skill generalisation sug-
Caregiver Involvement gest that SST conducted in schools holds promise
as an effective intervention approach (e.g.
Parents and caregivers play a vital role in facili- Beaumont, Rotolone, & Sofronoff, 2015; Kasari
tating social skill application across settings. To et al., 2016; Kenworthy et al., 2014). Group SST
promote the continued learning and generalisa- interventions implemented in schools may also
tion of skills, parents and caregivers must be have a more powerful impact on children’s
knowledgeable about specific skills and strate- friendships and social networks as child group
gies. SST programs may offer explicit parent members all experience the same class or school
training/coaching to give parents information and culture. The curriculum can be tailored to address
feedback about how to support their children to unique class- and/or school-specific concerns
practise skills learnt in session at home. (Kasari et al., 2016). Details of specific school-­
For example, in the Secret Agent Society pro- based SST programs are provided later in this
gram (Beaumont, 2010), a parent group runs con- chapter.
currently with the child treatment to teach parents Teachers and school staff can also facilitate
the specific skills that are being targeted in the child the application and generalisation of social skills
group. Parents review the skills and strategies that for children with social-emotional challenges by
children learn each week and receive instruction incorporating appropriate peer models into SST
about how to help their children complete the groups. Specifically, peer-mediated interventions
between-sessions skills practice tasks (missions). aim to engage typically developing peers as
Parents are taught how to model and role-play tar- social models to improve social responses, initia-
get skills with their children at home, prompt their tions and interactions in children with social-­
children to apply their social skills when needed in emotional challenges. Outpatient psychiatric
daily life and help their children to answer mission clinic SST groups rarely offer this opportunity, as
questions, offering praise and rewards for effort. A it can be difficult to find and engage typically
parent workbook is also included in the program developing peers in SST programs (although tar-
materials to remind parents of the skills and strate- get children’s siblings can sometimes be
gies that they can use to promote their children’s recruited).
social skill generalisation. Peer-mediated programs provide an additional
impetus for children to interact directly with their
typically developing peers by assigning the peers
 chool Involvement and Peer Buddy
S to serve as ‘peer buddies’ in cooperative learning
Programs experiences (e.g. Garrison-Harrell, Kamps, &
Kravits, 1997). Children with strong social com-
In an attempt to maximise the effectiveness of munication skills are selected by teachers to spe-
SST programs and optimise children’s social cifically act as ‘buddies’ to children with
skill application at school (where there are ample social-emotional challenges in the classroom,
incidental opportunities for peer interaction), playground and other social situations in the
140 R. Beaumont et al.

school setting. In this model, typically develop- praising and offering rewards). For example, the
ing peers are educated about the students with Secret Agent Society program (Beaumont, 2010)
whom they are paired (e.g. taught to engage in or includes weekly teacher tip sheets and a home-­
support specific social interaction behaviours) school diary that are forwarded to school staff by
and may also be reinforced by teachers for engag- parents.
ing in positive peer interactions with their bud-
dies. In this context, children with social-emotional
challenges have the opportunity to work regu- I nnovative Technologies to Enhance
larly with peer buddies that have received direct Social Skill Learning
instruction regarding ways to promote the devel- and Application
opment of their social communication skills.
While there is emerging evidence to support Recent technological advances have helped to
the efficacy of school-based interventions for optimise children’s engagement in social skill
improving social interactions in children with instruction and assist with skill generalisation.
ASD (e.g. Watkins et al., 2015), few studies have Apps like i-Modeling—Skills for autism spec-
documented the most effective methods to gener- trum disorder (Autism SA, 2016) and Social
alise skills to new situations over time and with Detective (Social Skill Builder Inc., 2016) pro-
child psychiatric populations beyond ASD. Those vide children with easy access to video model-
studies that have been conducted typically utilise ling examples of prosocial behaviours at the
a single-subject design, with the notable excep- swipe of a finger. The i-Modeling app (Autism
tion of Kasari, Rotheram-Fuller, Locke, and SA, 2016) involves an adult working with a child
Gulsrud’s (2012) randomised controlled trial that to create self-instructional video demonstrations
compared the effectiveness of different SST tech- of target pro-social skills. Children select a
niques for children with ASD. This trial com- reward image that is shown when they watch the
pared peer training, direct child instruction, and a self-instructional video. The Social Detective app
combined approach to a no treatment control (Social Skill Builder Inc., 2016) uses video clips
group of 60 children with ASD in regular educa- to teach children how to understand others’
tion settings. Target child instruction or peer behaviours and thoughts and to use their eyes,
instruction interventions were implemented for ears and brains to make smart guesses about how
6 weeks (20 min sessions occurring twice other people may be feeling and what they may
weekly). The primary social outcomes included be thinking. The app tracks gameplay data that
social network salience and peer engagement can be shared by parents and educators.
during playground observations. Overall, the While there has been a recent explosion of
peer-mediated approach resulted in better social social skills training apps and gaming-based
outcomes than direct child instruction for both approaches to social skills instruction (particu-
outcomes, and gains were maintained at 12-week larly for children with ASD), there is a lack of
follow-up. Children in the combined intervention empirical research to indicate whether tech-­
condition demonstrated the greatest improve- enhanced SST methods are superior to traditional
ments in social network salience, suggesting that teaching techniques in terms of optimising chil-
the most powerful SST method was teaching dren’s learning and application of social skills in
skills to both target children and peer buddies. daily life. Intuitively, gaming-based approaches
Outpatient SST programs can upskill school appear to have several advantages over face-to-­
staff as co-intervention agents. In this way, adults face instructional methods, including potentially
can support children in applying their social-­ being more engaging and interactive, allowing
emotional skills at school. Information for school for self-paced learning in a consistent, distraction-­
staff is provided on weekly social skill targets free environment, easy repetition of skills and
and effective school-based methods for promot- concepts taught and the provision of immediate
ing skill usage (e.g. prompting, monitoring, individualised feedback to children in an environ-
Social Learning Instructional Models 141

ment free from social demands (Moore, McGrath, Parsons, & Leonard, 2007). However, it would
& Thorpe, 2000; Murray, 1997). appear that at the present time, virtual environ-
However, without the opportunity for social ments are a potential supplement to, rather than a
skill practice in real life and the provision of replacement for, real world SST, providing the
feedback from a peer or adult, one might ques- client with a quiet, safe and nonthreatening envi-
tion the generalisation of social skills and knowl- ronment to explore social situations and
edge learnt from these tech-therapy tools. responses. The capacity of children who are
Beaumont et al. (2015) showed that improve- younger than adolescents to interpret, navigate
ments in the social-emotional functioning of chil- and learn social skills in virtual environments
dren with ASD at home and at school appeared to also remains largely unexplored in the research
be greater when a SST computer game interven- literature.
tion (the Secret Agent Society Computer Game
Pack) was supplemented with a small group skill
application curriculum, involving behavioural Integrating Social Skill Teaching
rehearsal and feedback from a school guidance and Skill Generalisation Techniques:
counsellor and peers. However, surprisingly, sig- Multicomponent SST Programs
nificant improvements in children’s social-­
emotional skills were still achieved at home and Research supports the efficacy of a variety of SST
at school for the computer game pack only condi- programs that integrate many of the teaching and
tion, where no behavioural rehearsal or feedback skill generalisation approaches described above,
was involved. A possible reason for this is that from single-case multiple baseline study designs
real-life skills practice tasks and visual supports to large-scale randomised controlled trials with
(skill code cards) were integrated into the com- longitudinal follow-up. Programs are delivered in
puter game pack intervention, helping to support a variety of formats (e.g. small group and whole
skill generalisation. class) in school and clinic contexts. A description
Virtual reality technology also holds great of some of these programs is provided below,
promise as a social skill teaching and skill prac- including the skill teaching and generalisation
tice tool. Virtual environments offer safe, three-­ enhancement techniques that each uses.
dimensional vignettes that can be built to depict
everyday social scenarios that children might
struggle with (e.g. playing cooperatively with School-Based Programs
others, group work, lunch conversations at
school, etc.). Parsons, Leonard, and Mitchell Stop and Think Social Skills Program The
(2006) used a qualitative case-study approach stop and think social skills program (Knoff,
with two teenagers with ASD to explore their 2001) is for children in preschool through 8th
capacity to understand, use and interpret the tech- grade, with content presented in four levels to
nology appropriately. The teens appeared to match the developmental and academic needs of
understand the scenes (canteen and bus), discuss- participants (preschool and 1st grade, 2nd and
ing appropriate social responses for each with a 3rd grade, 4th and 5th grade, 6th through 8th
facilitator. The teens endorsed the experience of grade). Social skills are taught in general educa-
the virtual environments as enjoyable and were tion classrooms with opportunities for small
able to explain how the technology could help to group instruction for those children with more
improve their ability to navigate social interac- academic or social challenges. In each level,
tions in daily life. A follow-up study with six children are introduced to ten core skills and ten
teenagers with ASD indicated that experiencing advanced skills that will improve their social
these virtual reality environments improved functioning while also enhancing self-­
teens’ judgements and explanations of where to sit management and academic engagement in the
in videos of real café and bus scenarios, suggest- classroom through the reinforcement of daily
ing some knowledge generalisation (Mitchell, routines.
142 R. Beaumont et al.

Targeted skills are introduced in each level, PATHS—Promoting Alternative Thinking


with opportunity to practise a learned skill in the Strategies (PATHS) The PATHS curriculum is
classroom setting throughout the year using five a program that promotes the social and emo-
universal steps. In each level, the skills are tional development of elementary-aged children
adapted to reflect changes in developmental and from kindergarten through 6th grade (Kusche &
academic expectations. The first step, ‘stop and Greenberg, 1994). This program also addresses
think!’, prompts children to approach situations aggression and other problematic behaviours in
calmly and in control. They are then encouraged the classroom. The PATHS curriculum was
to consider the consequences should they make a designed for implementation in a classroom set-
‘good choice or a bad choice’ in a given situation. ting throughout the school year by teachers and
Teachers are encouraged to guide children school counsellors. Developmentally appropri-
towards all possible good choices by focusing on ate lesson plans are introduced in 20–30 min
positive outcomes, while the children, them- segments approximately two to three times per
selves, are encouraged to consider the negative week over the course of the entire school year.
consequences of all potential bad choices. In the The curriculum supports children in the devel-
third step, ‘what are your choices or steps’, chil- opment of skills to increase emotional aware-
dren are introduced to ‘skill scripts’, including ness and regulation, to maintain self-control, to
choice skills and step skills scripts. In choice improve social competence, to develop positive
skills scripts, children learn to evaluate different peer relationships and to use problem-solving
situations to determine which choice is most skills.
appropriate. In step skills scripts, the prosocial The PATHS curriculum is organised by grade
choice is broken into sequential steps (task analy- and includes an instructor’s manual, a curriculum
sis) to ensure that the implementation of the manual, storybooks, puppets, a feelings chart,
choice is successful. In the fourth step, children cards depicting different emotions, posters, stick-
implement the prosocial skills that they have cho- ers and resources for parents. Younger children
sen using the sequential steps outlined in the step are introduced to ‘Twiggle’ a turtle who teaches
skills script (chaining). If they are unsuccessful, children how to manage feelings of anger, frus-
children are encouraged to return to step 3 and tration and disappointment.
either practice the steps in the step skills script or The PATHS program has been evaluated in
identify another prosocial skill to try from their large-scale randomised controlled trials. Twenty
choice skills script. Finally, children learn to head start classrooms totalling 246 pre-school
reward themselves for choosing the best proso- students were randomised to either implement
cial skill and implementing it correctly. The pro- the PATHS program or serve as a control class-
gram relies on teaching, modelling, role-playing, room (ten classrooms in each condition). The
performance feedback and the application of study found that children exposed to the PATHS
skills to everyday situations to teach and rein- program demonstrated higher emotion knowl-
force core and advanced social skills. edge skills, were less socially withdrawn at the
The stop and think program was designated end of the school year and were rated by parents
as an evidence-based and national model pre- and teachers as more socially competent com-
vention program by the US Department of pared to the students in the control classrooms
Health and Human Services’ Substance Abuse (Domitrovich, Cortes, & Greenberg, 2007). A
and Mental Health Services (SAMHSA) in longitudinal study of 18 special education class-
2000, a ‘promising program’ by the US rooms randomised to either the PATHS curricu-
Department of Justice’s Office of Juvenile lum or the control condition demonstrated that
Justice and Delinquency Prevention (OJJDP) in the PATHS program was associated with a reduc-
2003, and a ‘select’ program by the Collaborative tion in the rate of growth of teacher-reported
for Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning internalising and externalising behaviours from the
(CSEL) in 2002 (Knoff, 2005). students 2 years after the intervention. A reduction
Social Learning Instructional Models 143

in student-reported depression symptoms was also reinforcement for attending to and participating
maintained 2 years’ post study treatment (Chi- in the sessions.
Ming, Greenberg, & Kusche, 2004). Two single-case research design studies have
Superheroes Social Skills The superheroes demonstrated improved social skills at the indi-
social skills program (Jenson et al., 2011) is a vidual level using the Superheroes Social Skills
manualised social skills program for elementary-­ program. Two pre-school age children with
aged children with an ASD. The program was autism participated in the Superheroes program
developed for the Utah State Office of Education and showed improvements in skill accuracy and
and is distributed free for Utah educators. The social functioning following the program when
program utilises video vignettes and social narra- compared to baseline functioning (Radley,
tives to introduce each of the targeted social skills Hanglein, & Arak, 2016). Four adolescents with
and then engages the group members in role-­ intellectual disabilities participated in the pro-
playing exercises and games to reinforce those gram 2 days a week for 3 weeks specifically tar-
skills. Finally, group members are encouraged to geting expressing wants and needs, conversations
practice each targeted skill between sessions and and turn taking. All four participants demon-
to self-evaluate their experiences. The social strated improvements in skill accuracy in both
skills that are targeted in this program include the program and classroom setting based on
getting ready, following directions, reducing clinician and teacher report (O’Handly, Ford,
anxiety, participating, generalised imitation of Radley, Helbig, & Wimberly, 2016).
others, body basics, expressing wants and needs Superflex Social Skills Program The
and joint attention. Superflex teaching curriculum (Winner, Crooke,
Each session is structured similarly with nine & Knopp, 2008) has been incorporated into
main components. The sessions begin with a social skills groups, classrooms and entire
check-in during which group members are wel- schools to teach children to be better social detec-
comed, group rules are reviewed and the agenda tives, thinkers and problem solvers. The program
for that session is discussed. Any homework is paired with a series of comic books that intro-
assignments from the previous session are duce the Team of Unthinkables. The team is a
reviewed to reinforce previously taught skills. group of characters who represent ways in which
Group members watch a video of animated the brain can be inflexible when approaching
superheroes demonstrating three to five steps of a social problems. The program was designed for
social skill and then receive a self-monitoring children from kindergarten through 5th grade,
card that lists those steps. Group members then though younger children have been found to have
watch three videos of children successfully fol- more difficulty with self-regulation skills. The
lowing the steps for that targeted social skill. social thinking curriculum was evaluated using a
After watching the videos, the group members multiple baseline design with six children with
first watch the facilitators demonstrate the tar- ASD. The evaluation noted positive changes in
geted skill and provide feedback before demon- verbal and non-verbal social behaviours from
strating the skill themselves in a role-playing pretreatment to post-treatment (Crooke, Hendrix,
exercise. Finally, the skill is reinforced by view- & Rachman, 2016).
ing a social narrative in a comic book, playing a Children are first introduced to several core
game involving the targeted skill and/or complet- concepts of social learning in the comic book—
ing homework in which group members practice You are a Social Detective. Once children become
the skill in a natural setting. To aid in homework familiar with these skills, they begin to learn self-­
completion, parents are provided with a letter regulation skills while becoming more aware of
that explains the targeted skill and are encour- expectations in their social environment. This is
aged to support their children in using that skill in accomplished through a series of comic books
interactions with family and friends. In the ses- with Superflex, the social thinking superhero,
sion, group members are provided with positive who teaches children strategies to defeat the
144 R. Beaumont et al.

Unthinkables by having more flexible thinking in including children with high-functioning ASD
different social situations. As the program has (Beaumont, 2010). The social skills that are tar-
evolved, Superflex, the social thinking superhero, geted by this program include the ability to rec-
has enlisted five new friends to help him defeat ognise emotions in oneself and others; to express
the Unthinkables. These new superheroes each feelings in appropriate ways; to cope with feel-
have their own power to support children in ings of anger and anxiety; to communicate and
developing more flexible thinking in different play with others; to cope with mistakes, transi-
social situations. tions, and challenges; to build and maintain
friendships; to solve social problems; and to pre-
vent and manage bullying and teasing.
Clinic-Based Programs The program consists of small group child
‘club meetings’, concurrent parent sessions, home
Children’s Friendship Program The Children’s practice missions, weekly teacher tip sheets, a
Friendship Program is a 12-week program that multi-level computer game and a system to moni-
supports elementary school children (2nd through tor and reward skill generalisation at home and at
5th grade) in enhancing their peer relationships school. In each session, the group rules and agenda
(Frankel & Myatt, 2003). The program is deliv- items are discussed, and home missions are
ered in a clinical setting with trained mental health reviewed with rewards distributed for the comple-
clinicians, paraprofessionals and behavioural tion of assignments. A targeted skill is subse-
coaches. Both children and parents participate in quently introduced through didactics and colour
concurrent group sessions that emphasise conver- illustrations in children’s cadet handbooks, spy-
sational skills, identifying common interests, themed games and code cards, and role-playing
joining in when other children are playing, taking activities are used to consolidate the skill.
turns, being a good sport, managing rejection and A randomised-controlled trial of 49 youth
teasing and being respectful of adults. Children with ASD assigned to either the Secret Agent
are expected to practice the skills between ses- Society (SAS) program, or wait-list control dem-
sions during scheduled play dates with parents onstrated that children in the SAS program had
acting as coaches to support the generalisation of greater improvements in social skills and social
skills. Controlled studies suggest that the functioning, as rated by parents and teachers,
Children’s Friendship Program improves the peer than those in the control group, with improve-
relationships and social behaviours of children ments maintained at 5 months follow-up
with autism (Frankel & Whitham, 2011), ADHD (Beaumont & Sofronoff, 2008). Findings were
and those without any formal psychiatric diagno- similarly promising in a parent delivery format
sis (Frankel, Myatt, Cantwell, & Feinberg, 1997). with therapist assistance via Skype for 41 chil-
Long-term follow-up data was collected on close dren with high-functioning ASD. Families who
to 50% of participants in the Children’s Friendship completed the program reported improvements
Program. After an average of 35 months post in child social skills, parent self-efficacy, child
involvement in the program, parents reported that behaviour and child anxiety (Sofronoff, Silva, &
participants were invited on more play dates, Beaumont, 2015). Versions of SAS offered in
showed less conflict with peers on play dates, school settings to 69 students (ages 7–12) with
showed improvements in social skills and prob- ASD also demonstrated improvements in social
lem behaviours and reported less loneliness com- skills, emotion regulation and behavioural con-
pared to pretreatment (Mandelberg, Frankel, trol at home and at school (Beaumont et al.,
Cunningham, Gorospe, & Laugeson, 2014). 2015). Individual delivery variants of the pro-
Secret Agent Society Program The Secret gram have also shown promise in improving chil-
Agent Social Skills program is a 10-week social dren’s emotion regulation and social interaction
skills program for children between the ages of 8 skills (Tan, Mazzucchelli, & Beaumont, 2015;
and 12 with social and emotional difficulties, Thomson, Riosa, & Weiss, 2015).
Social Learning Instructional Models 145

Conclusion Beaumont, R. (2010). Secret Agent Society: Solving


the mystery of social encounters—Facilitator kit.
Brisbane, Australia: The Social Skills Training
Research suggests that contemporary SST Institute.
approaches that employ a hybrid of tried and Beaumont, R., Rotolone, C., & Sofronoff, K. (2015). The
tested social skill instructional techniques (e.g. Secret Agent Society social skills program for chil-
dren with a high-functioning autism spectrum disor-
didactic instruction, modelling, behavioural
ders: A comparison of two brief versions for schools.
rehearsal, feedback and reinforcement) and skill Psychology in the Schools, 52(4), 390–402.
generalisation strategies (e.g. assigning home- Beaumont, R., & Sofronoff, K. (2008). A multicomponent
work tasks, caregiver support, school involve- social skills intervention for children with Asperger
syndrome: The Junior Detective Training Program.
ment and peer buddy programs) appear to be
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(7),
effective at improving children’s social-­emotional 743–753. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01920.x
functioning at home and at school. However, Bellini, S., Peters, J. K., Benner, L., & Hope, A. (2007).
research identifying the core or essential ingredi- A meta-analysis of school-based social skills inter-
ventions for children with autism spectrum disorders.
ents in these programs is lacking, and there is a
Remedial and Special Education, 28, 153–162.
lack of research evaluating whether improve- Belllini, S., & Akullian, J. (2007). A meta-analysis of
ments in social-emotional functioning translate video modeling and video self modeling interventions
to improved friendships and peer acceptance. for children and adolescents with autism spectrum dis-
orders. Exceptional Children, 73, 264–287.
Most of the evidence-based SST programs
Bourke, M. L., & Van Hasselt, V. B. (2001). Social prob-
that are currently available adopt a cognitive lem solving skills for incarcerated offenders: A treat-
behavioural therapy framework and are delivered ment manual. Behavior Modification, 25, 163–188.
in a group context and have been predominantly Cartwright-Hatton, S., Tschernitz, N., & Gomersall, H.
(2005). Social anxiety in children: Social skills defi-
evaluated with children who have ASD. Therefore,
cit, or cognitive distortion? Behavior Research and
future studies are needed to evaluate if and how Therapy, 43, 131–141.
social skills training can be effectively delivered Charlop-Christy, M. H. (2004, June). Using video model-
in individual therapy, using different theoretical ing to teach perspective taking to children with autism.
Presentation at the annual Vermont Summer Autism
approaches (e.g. acceptance and commitment
Institute, Burlington, VT.
therapy, interpersonal psychotherapy) and to Charlop-Christy, M. H., Le, L., & Freeman, K. A.
children with a range of psychiatric conditions, (2000). A comparison of video modeling with in vivo
including those who are socially shy or awkward ­modeling for teaching children with autism. Journal
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(6),
but do not have clinical levels of psychopathol-
537–552.
ogy. The capacity of latest technological innova- Chi-Ming, K., Greenberg, M. T., & Kusche, C. A.
tions (e.g. apps, computer games, virtual reality (2004). Sustained effects of the PATHS curriculum
technology) to enhance social skills teaching and on the social and psychological adjustment of chil-
dren in ­special education. Journal or Emotional and
application processes also requires further empir-
Behavioral Disorders, 12, 66–78.
ical investigation. This is a field ripe for future Collins, T. A., Gresham, F. M., & Dart, E. H. (2016). The
research to determine whether the time and effects of peer-mediated check in/check out on the
money invested in the development of these tools social skills of socially neglected students. Behavior
Modification, 40(4), 611–639.
pay dividends in terms of improved SST acces-
Crager, D. E., & Horvath, L. S. (2003). The application
sibility, cost-effectiveness and outcomes for chil- of social skills training in the treatment of a child with
dren and families. Asperger’s disorder. Clinical Case Studies, 2, 34–49.
Crooke, P. J., Hendrix, R. E., & Rachman, J. Y. (2016).
Brief report: Measuring the effectiveness of teaching
social thinking to children with Asperger syndrome
References (AS) and high functioning autism. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 38(3), 581–591.
Autism SA (2016). iModeling—skills for autism spectrum Domitrovich, C. E., Cortes, R. C., & Greenberg, M. T.
disorder (version 1.3.3) [Mobile application soft- (2007). Improving young children’s social and emo-
ware]. Retrieved from https://itunes.apple.com tional competence: A randomized trial of the pre-
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood school “PATHS” curriculum. The Journal of Primary
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Prevention, 28, 67–91.
146 R. Beaumont et al.

DuPaul, G. J., & Eckert, T. L. (1994). The effects of social peer composition. Journal of Child Psychology and
skills curricula: Now you see them, now you don’t. Psychiatry, 57, 171–179.
School Psychology Quarterly, 9(2), 113–132. Kasari, C., Rotheram-Fuller, E., Locke, J., & Gulsrud,
Einfeld, S. L., Beaumont, R., Clark, T., Clarke, K. S., A. (2012). Making the connection: Randomized con-
Costley, D., Gray, K. M., ... Howlin, P. (2017). School- trolled trial of social skills at school for children with
based social skills training for young people with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology
autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Intellectual and and Psychiatry, 53, 431–439.
Developmental Disability. doi:10.3109/13668250.201 Kazdin, A. E. (2001). Behavior modification in applied
7.1326587 settings (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Foster, S. L., & Bussman, J. R. (2008). Evidence-based Kenworthy, L., Anthony, L. G., Naiman, D. Q., Cannon,
approaches to social skills training with children and L., Wills, M. C., Luong-Tran, C., & Wallace, G. L.
adolescents. In R. G. Steele, R. D. Elkin, & M. Roberts (2014). Randomized controlled effectiveness trial of
(Eds.), Handbook of evidence-based therapies for executive function intervention for children on the
children and adolescents: Bridging science and prac- autism spectrum. Journal of Child Psychology and
tice (pp. 409–427). New York, NY: Springer. Psychiatry, 55, 374–383.
Frankel, F., & Myatt, R. (2003). Children’s friendship King, C. A., & Kirschenbaum, D. S. (1992). Helping young
training. New York, NY: Brunner-Rotledge. children develop social skills: The social growth pro-
Frankel, F., Myatt, R., Cantwell, D. P., & Feinberg, D. T. gram. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing.
(1997). Parent-assisted transfer of children’s social Knoff, H. M. (2001). The Stop & Think Social Skills
skills training: Effects on children with and with- Program (preschool – grade 1, grades 2/3, grades 4/5,
out attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal middle school 6–8). Longmont, CO: Sopris West.
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Knoff, H. M. (2005). The Stop & Think Social Skills
Psychiatry, 36(8), 1056.1064. Program: Exploring its research base and rationale.
Frankel, F., & Whitham, C. (2011). Parent-assisted Little Rock, AK: Project ACHIEVE Press.
group treatment for friendship problems of children Kupersmidt, J., Coie, J., & Dodge, K. (1990). The role of
with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research, 22, peer relationships in the development of disorder. In
240–245. S. Asher & J. Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood
Garrison-Harrell, L., Kamps, D., & Kravits, T. (1997). (pp. 274–308). New York, NY: Cambridge University
The effects of peer networks on social-­communicative Press.
behaviors for students with autism. Focus on Autism Kusche, C. A., & Greenberg, M. T. (1994). The PATHS
and Other Developmental Disabilities, 12(4), curriculum. Seattle: Developmental Research and
241–256. Programs.
Gena, A., Couloura, S., & Kymissis, E. (2005). Modifying Mandelberg, J., Frankel, F., Cunningham, T., Gorospe,
the effective behavior of preschoolers with autism C., & Laugeson, E. A. (2014). Long-term outcomes
using in vivo or video modeling and reinforcement of parent-assisted social skills intervention for high-­
contingencies. Journal of Autism and Developmental functioning children with autism spectrum disorders.
Disorders, 35(5), 545–556. Autism, 18, 255–263.
Gray, C. (1994). Comic strip conversations. Arlington, Marriage, K. J., Gordon, V., & Brand, L. (1995). A social
TX: Future Horizons. skills group for boys with Asperger’s syndrome.
Gray, C. (2015). The new social story book, revised and Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,
expanded 15th anniversary edition: Over 150 social 29, 58–62.
stories that teach everyday social skills to children Matson, J. L., Sevin, J. A., & Box, M. L. (1995). Social
and adults with autism and their peers. Arlington, TX: skills in children. In W. O. O’Donohue & L. Krasner
Future Horizons. (Eds.), Handbook of psychological skills and train-
Gresham, F. M. (1985). Utility of cognitive-behavioral ing: Clinical techniques and applications (pp. 36–53).
procedures for social skills training with children: A Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
review. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 13, Mikami, A. Y., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2006). Resilient ado-
411–423. lescent adjustment among girls: Buffers of childhood
Jenson, W., Bowen, J., Clark, E., Block, H., Gabriensen, peer rejection and attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis-
T., Hood, J., … Springer, B. (2011). Superheroes order. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34,
social skills. Eugene, OR: Pacific Northwest. 823–837.
Kamps, D., Royer, J., Dugan, E., Kravits, T., Gonzalez-­ Mikami, Y. A., Jia, M., & Noa, J. J. (2014). Social
Lopez, A., Garcia, J., … Kane, L. G. (2002). Peer skills training. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric
training to facilitate social interaction for elementary Clinics of North America, 23, 775–788. http://dx.doi.
students with autism and their peers. Exceptional org/10.1016/j.chc.2014.05.007
Children, 68, 173–187. Mitchell, P., Parsons, S., & Leonard, A. (2007). Using
Kasari, C., Dean, M., Kretzmann, M., Shih, W., Orlich, F., virtual environments for teaching social understand-
Whitney, R., … King, B. (2016). Children with autism ing to 6 adolescents with autistic spectrum disorders.
spectrum disorder and social skills groups at school: A Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
randomized trial comparing intervention approach and 37(3), 589–600.
Social Learning Instructional Models 147

Moore, D., McGrath, P., & Thorpe, J. (2000). Computer for children with a high-functioning autism Spectrum
aided learning for people with autism—A frame- disorder: A parent-directed trial. Focus on Autism
work for research and development. Innovations on and Other Developmental Disabilities, 32, 55–70.
Education & Training International, 37(3), 218–228. doi:10.1177/1088357615583467
Murray, D. (1997). Autism and information technology: Storebo, O. J., Gluud, C., Winkel, P., & Simonsen, E.
Therapy with computers. In S. Powell & R. Jordan (2012). Social-skills and parental training plus stan-
(Eds.), Autism and learning: A guide to good practice dard treatment versus standard treatment for children
(pp. 100–117). London: David Fulton Publishers. with ADHD—The randomised SOSTRA trial. PloS
O’Handley, R. D., Ford, W. B., Radley, K. C., Helbig, One, 7(6), e37280. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0037280
K. A., & Wimberly, J. K. (2016). Social skills training Tan, Y. L., Mazzucchelli, T. G., & Beaumont, R. (2015).
for adolescents with intellectual disabilities: A school- An evaluation of individually delivered Secret Agent
based evaluation. Behavior Modification, 40, 541–567. Society social skills program for children with high-­
Parsons, S., Leonard, A., & Mitchell, P. (2006). Virtual functioning autism spectrum disorders: A pilot study.
environments for social skills training: Comments Behaviour Change, 32(3), 159–174.
from two adolescents with autistic spectrum disorder. Thomson, K., Riosa, P. B., & Weiss, J. A. (2015). Brief
Computers and Education, 47(2), 186–206. report of preliminary outcomes of an emotion regu-
Radley, K. C., Hanglein, J., & Arak, M. (2016). School-­ lation intervention for children with autism spec-
based social skills training for preschool-age children trum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 20(8), 938– Disorders, 45(11), 3487–3495.
951. pii: 1362361315617361 [epub ahead of print]. Watkins, L., O’Reilly, M., Kuhn, M., Gevarter, C., Lancioni,
Rose, S. D., & Edleson, J. L. (1987). Working with chil- G. E., Sigafoos, J., & Lang, R. (2015). A review of
dren and adolescents in groups. San Francisco, CA: peer-mediated social interventions for students with
Jossey-Bass. autism in inclusive settings. Journal of Autism and
Sansosti, F. J., & Powell-Smith, K. A. (2006). Using Developmental Disorders, 45(4), 1070–1083.
social stories to improve the social behavior of chil- Webster-Stratton, C. (2006). The incredible years: A
dren with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Positive trouble-­shooting guide for parents of children aged
Behavior Interventions, 8(1), 43–57. 3–8. Seattle, WA: Incredible Years Press.
Scattone, D., Wilczynski, S. M., Edwards, R. P., & Rabian, Webster-Stratton, C., Hollinsworth, T., & Kolpacoff, M.
B. (2002). Decreasing disruptive behaviors of children (1989). The long-term effectiveness and clinical sig-
with autism using social stories. Journal of Autism and nificance of three cost-effective training programs
Developmental Disorders, 32(6), 535–543. for families with conduct-problem children. Journal
Schroeder, A. (2003). Socially speaking game. School of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 550–553.
Specialty Publishing, Super Duper Publications. Webster-Stratton, C., & Reid, M. J. (2016). The incredible
Simpson, A., Langone, J., & Ayres, K. (2004). Embedded years parents, teachers and children training series: A
video and computer based instruction to improve multifaceted treatment approach for young children
social skills for students with autism. Education and with conduct problems. In J. Weisz & A. Kazdin
Training in Developmental Disabilities, 29, 240–252. (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies for children
Social Skill Builder Inc. (2016). Social detective (version and adolescents (3rd ed., pp. 194–210). New York,
1.2) [mobile application software]. Retrieved from NY: Guilford Publications.
https://itunes.apple.com Winner, M. G., Crooke, P., & Knopp, K. (2008). You are
Sofronoff, K., Silva, J., & Beaumont, R. (2015). The a social detective: Explaining social thinking to kids.
Secret Agent Society social-emotional skills program Great Barrington, MA: North River Press.
Self-Regulation in Childhood:
A Developmental Perspective

Yair Ziv, Moti Benita, and Inbar Sofri

neous operation of these multiple systems at


Introduction different levels of organization is an essential
feature of human development” (Shonkoff &
The National Research Council and the Institute Phillips, 2000, p. 26).
of Medicine of the National Academies released Notwithstanding the significance of Shonkoff
an important report in the year 2000 called From and Philips’s conclusion, it is important to note
Neurons to Neighborhoods (Shonkoff & Phillips, that the definitions of what is considered as self-­
2000). The report, which, to this date, serves as a regulatory capacities are not as clear as may be
main guideline to early childhood researchers expected from such a core concept. Moreover,
and practitioners, had emphasized ten core con- these definitions are likely to change across
cepts which are crucial to our understanding of childhood as a function of age and development.
the process of human development. One of these Consequently, it is an important aim of this chap-
concepts is self-regulation—which stands at the ter to review the concept of self-regulation from
center of the current chapter. In explaining why a developmental perspective in order to further
they see self-regulation as a factor standing at the our understanding of the similarities and differ-
core of human development, Shonkoff and ences between self-regulatory capacities as a
Philips stated that: “The growth of self-regula- function of age and developmental milestones.
tion is a cornerstone of early childhood develop- The chapter is divided into four main sections. In
ment that cuts across all domains of behavior. the first section, we look at the different defini-
Regulation is a fundamental property of all living tions of self-regulation as they appear in the lit-
organisms. It includes physiological and behav- erature and suggest an informative definition of
ioral regulations that sustain life… Regulatory that construct. The second discusses the develop-
processes modulate a wide variety of functions to ment of self-regulation from infancy to middle
keep them within adaptive ranges. The simulta- childhood. The third section presents different
methods of assessing self-regulation (again, as a
function of age and development), and the fourth
discusses the links between self-regulation and
Y. Ziv, Ph.D. (*) • M. Benita, Ph.D. • I. Sofri psychopathology and their implications to field
Faculty of Education, Department of Counseling and practitioners, focusing mainly on clinical and
Human Development, Multidisciplinary Program for
educational implications. We summarize the
Early Childhood Education and Development,
University of Haifa, Mount Carmel, Haifa, Israel chapter with a set of conclusions and recommen-
e-mail: yziz@edu.haifa.ac.il dations for future research in the field.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 149


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_10
150 Y. Ziv et al.

Defining “Self-Regulation” 2012; Zhou, Chen, & Main, 2012). For example,
there is an unclear distinction in the literature
In a highly cited article, Kopp (1982) defines self- between effortful control and inhibitory control.
regulation as “the ability to comply with a request, The former is mainly used in the context of
to initiate and cease activities according to situa- research traditions focusing on temperament and,
tional demands, to modulate the intensity, fre- as such, is defined as the regulatory component of
quency, and duration of verbal and motor acts in temperament (Rothbart & Bates, 2006). The lat-
social and educational settings, to postpone acting ter, on the other hand, is typically used by
upon a desired object or goal, and to generate researchers focusing on cognitive development
socially approved behavior in the absence of exter- and is referred to as one of the central executive
nal monitors” (Kopp, 1982, pp. 199–200). functions controlled by the prefrontal cortex
Similarly to Shonkoff and Phillips (2000), Kopp (Liew, 2012; Zhou et al., 2012). However,
emphasizes the simultaneous operation of various although these two constructs are mostly dis-
regulatory capacities and, as such, while recogniz- cussed as independent constructs (but, in some
ing the multiplicity and complexity of self-regula- cases, inhibitory control is discussed as part of
tion, seems to discuss it essentially as one entity. effortful control), their description seems to be
More recently, however, researchers and theoreti- very similar and focuses on the ability to use a
cians have argued that discussing self-regulation less desired but more appropriate response over a
as one core concept could be misleading and thus more desired but less appropriate response
it is more useful to make more specific and clear (Bridgett et al., 2013).
distinctions between separate regulatory capaci- Thus, it is important to suggest a clear defini-
ties that may be related but are still distinct tion of self-regulation that can help in what was
(Eisenberg, Hofer, Sulik, & Spinrad, 2014; recently described as a lack of clarity of this con-
Ursache, Blair, & Raver, 2012). For example, struct and its subcomponents (McClelland &
Eisenberg and colleagues suggest that it is useful Cameron, 2012). In the current chapter, we borrow
to differentiate between internally motivated and from different prominent theoreticians and
externally enforced regulation processes and researchers (e.g., Bandura, 1991; Block & Block,
between more or less volitional regulatory pro- 1979; Bridgett, Burt, Edwards, & Deater-­Deckard,
cesses (Eisenberg, Duckworth, et al., 2014; 2015; Eisenberg, Duckworth, et al., 2014;
Eisenberg, Hofer, et al., 2014), whereas Ursache Eisenberg, Hofer, et al., 2014; Kopp, 1982;
and colleagues highlight the distinction between Ursache et al., 2012) and suggest the following
emotion-related self-­ regulation and cognitive- definition: self-regulation includes a broad set of
related self-regulation capacities (Ursache et al., self-initiated behaviors that aim to regulate and
2012). For these researchers, discussing different modulate emotional, cognitive, and behavioral
regulatory capacities, such as the maintenance of arousal through conscious, deliberate, flexible, and
body temperature, the expression of feelings, and effortful inhibitory actions. There are a number of
the capacity to pay attention, from the perspective important features to this definition. First, it refers
of one core concept, necessarily assumes a con- to self-regulation as a set of distinct behaviors
necting link that does not always exist. rather than one core construct. Second, it high-
On the other hand, it was also convincingly lights the cognizant aspect of regulation (i.e., that
claimed that too many distinctions between dif- the person must be aware of what she is doing).
ferent regulatory capabilities could be confusing, Third, it emphasizes that self-regulation necessar-
as different research traditions may refer to what ily includes an important ability of restricting and
seems to be identical (or, at least, very similar) limiting actions taken by the individual. Based on
processes in different names (Bridgett, Oddi, this definition, we look next at the developmental
Laake, Murdock, & Bachmann, 2013; Liew, milestones related to these capacities.
Self-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective 151

 elf-Regulation in Childhood: Main


S regulate her emotions and arousal state is crucial
Developmental Milestones for her development. The caregiver will typically
assist the infant through interaction and routine,
When can we expect to see the first signs of and the infant will use her limited capabilities to
deliberate and effortful inhibitory actions and adjust to the caregiver’s directions. At around
how do these abilities develop across childhood? 3–4 months of age, infants gain more sensorimotor
In this section we try to answer these questions modulation and can activate a motor act and
starting with infancy, continuing in early child- change the act if needed. During this period,
hood, and concluding in middle childhood. infants add to their previous passive repertoire of
self-soothing behaviors more active behaviors
such as direct approach to caregivers and attain-
 elf-Regulation in Infancy
S ment of more control of their visuospatial orienta-
and Toddlerhood (Ages 0–3) tion (Rothbart, Ziaie, & O’boyle, 1992). At
6 months, they start redirecting their visual atten-
While there are signs of self-regulation activities tion more toward inanimate aspects in the environ-
even before birth (Florez, 2011), most develop- ment than toward their mothers (Rothbart et al.,
mental theories concur that voluntary control 1992). Then, toward the end of the first year of life,
over behavior only appears in the latter part of the infants show even more increase in inhibitory
first year of life and, even by then, it is mostly capacities, self-­soothing, and social communica-
expressed in the form of the infant’s compliance tions, which signifies an important developmental
to the caregiver’s requests (e.g., Rothbart, Sheese, period of self-regulation (Rothbart et al., 1992).
Rueda, & Posner, 2011; Ruff & Rothbart, 1996). During that period, infants not only show an abil-
Thus, in the early years, self-regulation is ity to respond to the caregiver’s control effort but
regarded many times as “guided self-regulation” also develop an ability to plan an act toward a
or “mutual regulation” because of the general desired regulatory goal (e.g., crawling to the other
conception that infants and toddlers can only side of the room to pick up the pacifier and put it in
regulate themselves through the guidance and their mouth). Notably, that already in the first year
specific instructions of their caregivers (Sroufe, of life, there are pronounced gender differences in
1995) or through co-constructions of these capac- self-regulation capabilities, with girls generally
ities (Beebe & Lachmann, 1998). As such, our showing higher capabilities of self-control and
definition of self-regulation as a set of conscious, lower levels of anger and frustration than boys
deliberate, flexible, and effortful self-initiated (Kochanska, Coy, & Murray, 2001; Weinberg,
behaviors does not adequately represent children Tronick, Cohn, & Olson, 1999). These differences
regulatory capacities in the first 3 years of life. continue to be evident throughout childhood
Still, infants as young as 2 months old (and, as (Raffaelli, Crockett, & Shen, 2005).
mentioned, even before birth) regularly engage in During the second year of life, infants start to
some form of self-regulatory activities. Their abili- show more direct signs of voluntary control and
ties are usually manifested in the form of neuro- an ability to monitor their behavior in some ways.
physiological modulation which, as mentioned, is During this phase, children are aware of social
highly unlikely to involve conscious intention or and task demands of the caregiver and can react
awareness to the meaning of a given situation. But accordingly by “initiate, maintain, modulate or
even without the conscious aspect, these more cease physical acts, communication and emo-
primitive behaviors (which are usually reflex tional signals” (Kopp, 1982, p. 204). Finally, the
movements organized in patterns of functional shift to internal monitoring, which more ade-
behavior) aim to achieve the goal of regulating and quately fits our initial definition of self-­regulation,
controlling arousal states (Gartstein, Bridgett, starts to be manifested more clearly during the
Young, Panksepp, & Power, 2013). In these first third year of life, when young toddlers begin to
months, the caregiver’s ability to help the infant acquire the ability to postpone an act if requested
152 Y. Ziv et al.

and to behave according to external standards cognitive control which is remarkably similar to
without external monitoring (Kochanska, Murray, patients with neurological prefrontal cortex (PFC)
& Harlan, 2000; Posner & Rothbart, 1998). This damage. They argue that this erratic behavior
phase depends on the emergence of representa- occurs because the PFC, which is the part of the
tional thinking and evocative memory which brain that is in charge of our ability to regulate our
allow the child to understand social standards and thoughts and behaviors, is the last part in the
to link her behavior to her caregivers’ expecta- human brain to achieve synaptic maturation. In
tions regarding acceptable and nonacceptable contrast to other mammals, this process reaches
behaviors. Importantly, throughout these first its pick only around the end of the fourth year of
years of life, children do far better in “don’t” situ- life (Huttenlocher & Dabholkar, 1997).
ations (i.e., when they are instructed by an adult Thompson-Schill and colleagues suggest that
not to engage in a pleasant task) than in “do” situ- these early years differ significantly from later
ations (i.e., when they are instructed by an adult years in being a developmental period in which
to engage in an unpleasant task) (Kochanska self-regulation may not be as important to learn-
et al., 2001). From a developmental perspective, ing as it is in later periods.
this difference between “do” and “don’t” situa- Their suggestion is fueled by the fact that dur-
tions suggests that the ability to suppress a ing these years children gain multitude of life
response develops earlier than the ability to exe- skills and world knowledge that are essential for
cute an undesired activity. It is plausible that this their development even though they cannot con-
developmental difference occurs because of trol and regulate their thoughts and behaviors as
social demands (i.e., that parents start asking efficiently as in later years. Moreover, they sug-
children to suppress a response before they ask gest that the absence of sufficient regulatory
them to initiate an undesired activity) or because capacities during the early years not only does
the latter requires more complicated coordination not interfere with learning but also serves as an
between various behavioral elements than the advantage for specific developmental tasks such
former (Kochanska et al., 2001). as language development and probability match-
It is evident from the above review that volun- ing. Thompson-Schill and colleagues summarize
tary self-regulation is an extremely hard task for by saying that, based on the Darwinian principle
children in the first 3 years of life. On the other of “trade-offs”, the advantages of PFC immatu-
hand, we also know that voluntary self-regulation rity during the first 3 years of life outstrip the dis-
capabilities are instrumental for children’s learn- advantages (Thompson-Schill et al., 2009).
ing and development (Ursache et al., 2012). Taken From our perspective, their take on cognitive
these two facts together, what does it say about development in the first 3 years of life is impor-
children’s ability to learn and develop during tant for the acknowledgment that self-regulation,
these early years? Does it mean that because they while extremely important for the acquisition of
cannot regulate efficiently, they are also not effi- adequate social, emotional, and cognitive skills,
cient learners? An interesting and provocative has also some costs and can put significant limi-
perspective that may shed a somewhat different tations on our learning. Importantly, it also puts
light on children’s limited self-regulatory capaci- in different light Shonkoff and Philips’s determi-
ties in the first 3 years of life is suggested by nation that regulatory processes modulate a wide
Thompson-Schill, Ramscar, and Chrysikou variety of functions to keep them within adaptive
(2009) in an article titled “Cognition without con- ranges (Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000) as in infancy
trol.” These authors claim that during the first and toddlerhood, it seems that adaptability is
3 years of life, children exhibit what could be more related to the absence of self-regulatory
described as severely impaired behavioral and capabilities than to its existence.
Self-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective 153

 elf-Regulation in Early Childhood


S to our ability to recall and operate distinct pieces
(Ages 3–7) of information over a very short period of time;
cognitive flexibility, which describes our ability to
The early childhood years (from preschool to the shift attention between competing tasks in the
first years of elementary school) mark an impor- most efficient way; and inhibitory control, which,
tant developmental period in which children within a specific context, enables us to select a less
make large and significant gains in their abilities desired but more appropriate response over a more
to self-regulate (Bronson, 2000). What was pre- desired but less appropriate response (Diamond,
viously highlighted by “cognition without con- Barnett, Thomas, & Munro, 2007; Miyake et al.,
trol” is changing into more controlled behaviors 2000). Researchers discuss executive functions in
and thought processes that could change based relation to self-regulation because the main role of
on the specific context (i.e., are flexible), that are these executive skills is to monitor and control
more conscious and deliberate, and that are more behavior in a flexible and adaptive manner, espe-
multidimensional (Bronson, 2000; Whitebread & cially in novel situations (Bryce, Szűcs, Soltész, &
Basilio, 2012) than in the first 3 years of life. In Whitebread, 2011).
the next few pages, we focus on the development During the fourth year of life, we see a signifi-
of self-regulation during the early childhood cant developmental leap in children’s efficient
years from two perspectives: the cognitive per- use of their working memory, in their ability to
spective and the socioemotional perspective. shift between tasks (cognitive flexibility), and in
their ability to inhibit desired responses in accor-
 elf-Regulation in Early Childhood:
S dance to environmental demands. From around
The Cognitive Perspective age 4 onward, there is a linear increase in work-
Cognitively, children after the age of 3 can ing memory capacity that continues throughout
engage in a much wider range of cognitive tasks childhood (Gathercole, Brown, & Pickering,
than before. Their perspectives on events and 2003). During the early parts of this stage (ages
objects in the world are getting to be more multi- 3–5), children use simple tactics for remember-
dimensional, they are more able to control their ing but do not use mental strategies and do not
attention and resist distractions in their environ- typically show a clear ability to differentiate
ment, they are getting to be more advanced and between what is considered as memory and what
complex problem solvers, and they start to see is considered as comprehension. Thus, in order to
the world from a more “objective” perspective. remember objects and events, they tend to ver-
Because of these more advanced abilities, they bally name or visually inspect items and use
begin to be more selective and choose tasks while memory strategies intermittently or inconsis-
taking into account their own level of skills tently even if they are aware of how they can
(Whitebread & Basilio, 2012). This means that improve recall (Henry & Norman, 1996).
they are showing the first signs of what is referred However, when they enter elementary school
to in later developmental period as metacognitive (ages 6–7), they begin to understand the advan-
thinking (Flavell, 1979). tages of memorizing and start to use more
The main construct discussed by scientists advanced techniques such as constant rehearsal
researching self-regulatory capacities in early and the use of categorization (Justice, 1985).
childhood from a cognitive perspective is “execu- These increased capacities allow them to more
tive functions” (EF). The term “executive func- fully understand social rules and apply breaks or
tions” is an umbrella term describing the ability to let go, as needed and commended by their
monitor and regulate different types of cognition environment.
and behavior to achieve specific internal goals (Xu Also at around age 4, there is an increase in
et al., 2013). It usually includes three main brain children’s ability to use rules more flexibly and to
functions that are strongly related yet considered change and shift between rules based on their
to be independent: working memory, which refers understanding of environmental demands.
154 Y. Ziv et al.

Zelazo (2006) has shown a rapid change in chil- game or a “Stroop test”) and “hot” (i.e., flexible
dren’s cognitive flexibility from age 3 to age 5. control of appetitive reward systems which is
Using the dimensional change card sort more similar to the definition of effortful control,
(DCCS)—a card sorting task in which children e.g., snack or gift delay) measures. For example,
are asked to switch their card sorting strategy whereas only half of the young 3-year-olds
(first by one dimension, e.g., color, than by passed the “cold” “bear/dragon” test (Reed, Pien,
another, e.g., shape, and, finally, either by color or & Rothbart, 1984), all of the 5-year-olds passed
by shape, depending on whether the card has a this test successfully. Similarly with the “hot”
drawn border or not)—Zelazo has found that measures, whereas 42% of the young 3-year-olds
whereas 3-year-olds could not even make the ini- passed the “gift delay” test (Kochanska, Murray,
tial switch (i.e., from color to shape), 4-year-olds Jacques, Koenig, & Vandegeest, 1996), three
had no problem doing it, but found it difficult to quarters of the 5-year-olds were able to pass this
make a conditioned decision (i.e., to decide task successfully (Carlson, 2005).
whether to sort by color or shape based on the This last point brings us to the fine line
existence of a border), whereas, by age 5, most between the cognitive and emotional aspects of
children were also able to perform the conditioned self-regulation. As Carlson and others have
switch with relative ease (Zelazo, 2006). Zelazo’s shown, there are clear associations between the
important findings converge with Deák’s (2003) emotional and cognitive aspects of regulation;
conclusion that the most rapid change in chil- however, there seem to be more variance in
dren’s ability to think more flexibly and switch emotionally-­ related self-regulation abilities,
between tasks based on environmental demands compared to the more “pure” (or “cold”) cogni-
occurs between the ages of 3 and 6 years. Similar tive regulation abilities. In the next section, we
findings to Zelazo’s, albeit with different mea- therefore review these emotional aspects of self-­
sures, were found in a number of more recent regulation, as exhibited in early childhood.
studies (e.g., Deák & Narasimham, 2014; Deák &
Wiseheart, 2015).  elf-Regulation in Early Childhood:
S
Finally, and strongly related to the other two The Socioemotional Perspective
executive functions discussed above, children The two constructs most frequently used by
from age 3 onward also show dramatic develop- researchers focusing on the socioemotional
ment in their inhibitory control (IC). As men- aspects of self-regulation are emotional self-­
tioned, inhibitory control refers to the ability to regulation (or just, emotion regulation) and
suppress or promote responses based on their effortful control. From a socioemotional perspec-
appropriateness to the environment (Bryce et al., tive, children entering their fourth year can more
2011) and, more specifically, to stop an ongoing easily control their emotions, are more capable to
thought or behavior in a sudden and complete use language to regulate their behavior, are more
manner (Williams, Ponesse, Schachar, Logan, & able to adjust their behavior based on their per-
Tannock, 1999). Until the preschool years, this ceptions of others’ behavior and state of mind,
type of restrictive behavior is virtually impossi- and, in general, seem to behave in ways that are
ble for toddlers. Only at around age 3, children based on an effort to adjust and adapt to the social
begin to use restrictive judgments in selecting demands of their environment (Bronson, 2000).
responses, with this ability rapidly developing In the literature, emotional self-regulation
until the early school years (Gagne & Hill typically refers to one’s ability to respond to
Goldsmith, 2011; Liu, Zhu, Ziegler, & Shi, environmental demands with a range of emo-
2015). In a comprehensive study assessing vari- tions (both positive and negative) in a controlled
ous EFs of children, Carlson (2005) has shown a manner (Panfile & Laible, 2012). Whereas emo-
linear increase in children’s IC from age 3 to 6 tional self-regulation continues to develop
using both “cold” (i.e., the more traditional mea- throughout the life-span, the period between
sures of PFC functions, e.g., a “Simon says” ages 3 and 6 seems to be especially important for
Self-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective 155

the development of children’s understanding of is that in all of these tasks, 3–6-year-old children
their own and others’ emotional responses and show linear and constant development, suggest-
self-control (Cole, Dennis, Smith-Simon, & ing that there is an underline construct connect-
Cohen, 2009). For example, it has been sug- ing all of them.
gested that lip compression represents a con-
scious effort to suppress high levels of (negative  elf-Regulation in Early Childhood:
S
or positive) emotional arousal (Bridges & A Summary
Grolnick, 1995). Whereas lip compression and The literature clearly shows that the early child-
other self-soothing strategies are visible already hood years are a defining period for the develop-
in infancy, there is a dramatic increase in chil- ment of self-regulation capabilities. From age 3 to
dren’s ability to control their emotions and the early elementary years, children progress in
understand the emotions of others after age 3. what seems to be a constant and linear line in their
Like in the other self-regulation capabilities working memory capacities, cognitive flexibility,
discussed thus far, the fourth year of life seems to inhibitory control, emotional self-­regulation, and
bring about an especially important developmen- effortful control. Still, even though children’s
tal change in effortful control. As mentioned ear- development during these years is impressive,
lier, there are major similarities in the definition they are far from being skillful in exhibiting self-
of inhibitory control and effortful control. Like control and regulation. These capacities continue
IC, EC is defined as the ability to suppress a dom- to develop through the middle childhood years, a
inant response to perform a subdominant period discussed in our next section.
response (Rothbart & Rueda, 2005). Perhaps the
best way to discriminate between the two is the
way mentioned earlier of differences between  elf-Regulation in Middle Childhood
S
“cold” (i.e., cognitive, PFC related) and “hot” (Ages 7–12)
(i.e., emotional, temperamentally related) inhibi-
tory actions (Carlson, 2005). Kochanska and her Although self-regulation develops most rapidly at
colleagues were instrumental in defining the spe- younger ages (Carlson & Moses, 2001), it contin-
cific effortful control skills as well as designing ues to develop throughout the life-span (Best &
appropriate measures to assess those (as will be Miller, 2010; Cicchetti & Tucker, 1994; Raffaelli
discussed in Chapter “Challenging Behavior”; et al., 2005). From both a cognitive and a socio-
e.g., Kochanska et al., 1996, 2000). The five main emotional perspective, middle childhood is a par-
effortful control skills identified by Kochanska ticularly demanding period of development, in
and colleagues were delay of gratification (mea- which children are requested to manage multiple
sured with tasks showing a candy or a gift for tasks at their homes, schools, and social lives.
which the child has to wait for before receiving Moreover, many times they receive conflicting
it), slowing motor activity (measured with tasks messages from parents, teachers, and peers, which
such as drawing a line very slowly), suppressing add another layer of complexity to their ability to
or initiating a response based on changing sig- coordinate and facilitate their mental processes
nals (measured by “go/no-go games”), effortful and behaviors. Thus, the task to self-regulate
attention (measured via shape recognition tasks), becomes more complicated and demands more
and lowering the voice (measured by tasks of advanced cognitive and emotional capabilities
changing the level of voice pitch, i.e., asking the during this stage. Therefore, self-­ regulatory
child to whisper). We will return to these con- capacities at the middle childhood ages improve
structs in Chapter “Challenging Behavior” when both qualitatively, in terms of the type of capabili-
discussing the tasks designed to measure them ties being mastered, and quantitatively, in terms of
and will see that some of these tasks may actually the degree to which s­elf-­regulatory capabilities
measure “cold” rather than “hot” traits, but, for are being mastered. Similarly to the previous sec-
the sake of this section, what is important to note tion discussing self-­regulation in early childhood,
156 Y. Ziv et al.

we turn to discuss the development of self-regula- capabilities replace the former methods of
tion in middle childhood separately from a cogni- mostly memorizing and thus afford higher
tive and socioemotional perspectives. capacities and more efficient retrieving, thus
assisting in goal-directed behaviors.
 elf-Regulation in Middle Childhood:
S Inhibitory control is also an important facet of
The Cognitive Perspective self-regulation that continues to develop through
Self-regulation capabilities during the middle middle childhood (Romine & Reynolds, 2005)
childhood years are dependent upon the develop- and particularly for tasks that combine inhibition
ment of advanced cognitive strategies that help chil- and working memory (Carlson, 2005; Gerstadt,
dren better control their arousal level (Heckhausen Hong, & Diamond, 1994). However, unlike the
& Dweck, 1998; Zimmerman, 2000). Importantly, improvements evident in preschool children,
children at these ages face much more challenging improvements in inhibitory control during the
environmental demands than in the early child- middle school years are unlikely to be fundamen-
hood years, both academically and interperson- tal qualitative changes in cognition but instead
ally. Therefore, their capacities to self-regulate seem to involve quantitative improvements in
their behaviors and cognitions are a hallmark of accuracy, perhaps due to an increasing efficiency
adaptive adjustment. Advancements in cognitive to override proponent responses. Accordingly,
processing capabilities that are typically discussed Best and Miller (2010) suggested that inhibition
in the literature in relation to self-regulation in tasks have varying sensitivities, with some being
middle childhood are the more efficient use of sensitive to the conceptual gains in early child-
memory and better inhibitory control abilities. hood and others being sensitive to the refine-
In terms of children’s use of memory, research ments in strength of the relevant cognitive skills
shows a generally linear increase in children’s or the generality of application in later
working memory capacity and efficient use from childhood.
the preschool age to early adolescence (Conklin, These advanced cognitive capabilities lay the
Luciana, Hooper, & Yarger, 2007; Gathercole, infrastructure for the child’s functioning in sev-
Pickering, Ambridge, & Wearing, 2004; Luciana, eral contexts. Specifically, they are essential to
Conklin, Hooper, & Yarger, 2005; Xu, Farver, & school functioning, which imposes growing
Zhang, 2009). However, it was suggested that demands on children in the middle childhood
the developmental course of working memory years compared to the early childhood years.
depends on the complexity of the task, namely, Self-regulation in academic settings has been
its executive demands. Indeed, several studies defined as the “active, constructive process
showed that less demanding tasks are being fully whereby learners set goals for their learning and
mastered earlier in the preschool age and more then attempt to monitor, regulate, and control
complicated tasks continue to mature until early their cognition, motivation, and behavior, guided
adolescence (Conklin et al., 2007; Luciana et al., and constrained by their goals and the contextual
2005). These findings suggest that middle child- features in the environment” (Pintrich, 2000,
hood is a stage characterized with the refinement p. 453). Models of self-regulation conceptualize
of working memory capacities. The more the self-regulating student as a motivated proac-
nuanced capabilities developed during this phase tive agent and depict the self-regulatory process
allow children to not only store more informa- as progressing along phases that include assess-
tion in their memory but also to be much better ment of task conditions and relevant personal
than in the early childhood years in retrieving resources, goal setting and selection of strategies
this information in the right context. During to pursue these goals, application of the strategies
these years, they become more proficient in and metacognitive monitoring of this application,
using advanced memory strategies like relying and evaluation of the products and metacognitive
on heuristics (like an educated guess or a rule of control of the continued use of these strategies
thumb), shortcuts, and grouping. These advanced (i.e., whether to maintain the strategies or change
Self-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective 157

them). These phases are thought to operate cycli- younger and same age children to use more or
cally, with the evaluation phase leading back to less adaptive forms of emotion regulation and the
the planning phase of goal setting and selection normative development of the capacities for such
of strategies and so on (Winne & Hadwin, 1998; forms of emotion regulation.
Zimmerman, 2000).
Thus, self-regulation in middle childhood
goes beyond holding data in short-term memory  he Assessment of Self-Regulation
T
and inhibiting unwanted responses. It is an active in Childhood
process that allows children at that stage to plan
goal-directed actions, reconceptualize the situa- In the childhood years, self-regulatory capabili-
tion, redefine their goals, or change their strategy ties are usually measured through direct assess-
in order to achieve that goal. ments and/or behavioral ratings completed by
adults (typically parents and teachers). Within
 elf-Regulation in Middle Childhood:
S this methodological framework, the following
The Socioemotional Perspective selective review highlights some frequently used
Although the capacity for effortful control devel- measures in that field. We start by reviewing
ops most rapidly in early childhood (Posner & measures of executive functions and follow by
Rothbart, 2000), there are some evidence show- reviewing measures of emotional self-regulation
ing that children further develop their regulatory and effortful control.
skills in middle childhood (Eisenberg & Morris,
2002). In addition, as children grow up, they
develop much more sophisticated ways to self-­  easures of Executive Functions
M
regulate their emotional experiences. Thus, while in Childhood
younger children rely mostly on attentional
resources and self-soothing to self-regulate, As shown by Carlson’s comprehensive review
school-aged children can rely on diverse internal (Carlson, 2005), there are numerous measures of
mental and cognitive mechanisms to regulate executive functions in childhood. These mea-
their emotional experiences (Eisenberg, sures focus mainly on the three main executive
Duckworth et al., 2014; Eisenberg, Hofer, et al., functions described earlier, working memory,
2014). In other words, in addition to the quantita- cognitive flexibility, and inhibitory control. These
tive improvement of their inhibitory control measures are discussed next.
capacities, children improve the quality of deal-
ing with emotional experiences. Working Memory
An example to a more sophisticated strategy Most measures of working memory are character-
of emotion regulation, which was explored exten- ized by having both a processing and a storage
sively in adult population, is reappraisal. component (Waters & Caplan, 2003). Performance
Reappraisal is considered an adaptive form of on these measures is usually expressed as a con-
emotion regulation, which involves the capacity tinuous score of memory span. Depending on their
to cognitively reappraise events by interpreting developmental stage, children are asked to recall
them in ways that change the emotional responses sequences of objects/numbers/letters (Gathercole
to them (Gross & Thompson, 2007). So far, only et al., 2004). An excellent example of the ways by
a few studies have explored the use of reappraisal which working memory is measured in children is
in middle childhood. For instance, McRae et al. the comprehensive Working Memory Test Battery
(2012) have demonstrated that 10–13-year-old for Children (WMTB-C; Pickering & Gathercole,
children use reappraisal to effectively deal with 2001). This measure is designed for children and
unpleasant emotions. However, much more young adults (ages 4–22) and includes nine sub-
research is needed in order to understand the tests: four tests examining verbal storage (digit
developmental pathways that enable both recall, word list recall, non-word list recall, and
158 Y. Ziv et al.

Word List Matching task), two tests focusing on information observation alone (Alloway &
visual recall (blocks recall and mazes memory), Gathercole, 2008).
and three tests examining more complex recall pat-
terns (backward digit recall, listening recall, and Cognitive Flexibility
counting recall). All of these tests can be used with Measures of cognitive flexibility typically assess
children from age 6, and five of these tests (digit children’s ability to flexibly switch between com-
recall, backward digit recall, word list recall, non- peting tasks. Among these measures, the dimen-
word list recall, and block recall) can be used with sional change card sort (DCCS; Zelazo, 2006)
children as young as 4 (Gathercole et al., 2004). mentioned above is likely the most well known
A related yet methodologically different exam- and widely used. The DCCS is an easily adminis-
ple of direct assessment measure of working mem- tered measure in which children are required to
ory in children is the Automated Working Memory sort a series of bivalent test cards, first according
Assessment (AWMA; Alloway, Gathercole, & to one dimension (e.g., color) and then according
Pickering, 2006)—a computer-­based measure of to another (e.g., shape). The child is then asked to
working memory for children age 4 and up. Like sort the cards either by color or shape, depending
the WMTB-C, it is responsive to the definition of on whether or not the card has a border. In recent
working memory as a system comprising multiple years, more complex versions of this measure
components whose coordinated activity provides such as the Three Dimension-Changes Card
the capacity for the temporary storage and manip- Sorting Photoshop-modified (Deák & Wiseheart,
ulation of information in a variety of domains 2015) were introduced. These new measures
(Baddeley, 2000). As such, it includes tests corre- include more colors and shapes and thus can pro-
sponding to each of these domains: word recall, duce higher distinguishing capabilities.
listening recall, dot matrix, and a measure of Other cognitive flexibility measures from the
visuospatial working memory called Mister same group of researchers (Deák, 2000; Deák &
X. Other examples of working memory assess- Narasimham, 2014; Deák & Wiseheart, 2015)
ments in childhood include the backward digit include the Flexible Induction of Meaning-­
span (Davis & Pratt, 1996), which serves as the Objects (FIM-Ob) and the Flexible Induction of
basis to some of the measures mentioned above, Meaning-Animates (FIM-An). These measures
and count and label (Gordon & Olson, 1998) in use novel objects (FIM-Ob) or novel animated
which children are asked to both count and label creatures (FIM-An) as stimuli. In each of these
correctly a set of objects. measures, the child has to sort these novel objects/
Although parent and teacher ratings that creatures a number of times based on different
directly tap working memory are not common, criteria. It is considered to be a strong measure of
one way to assess working memory through such flexibility because on the later trials, the child
ratings is by asking about behavior problems that must ignore matches that are perceived similarly,
were previously found to be strongly related to and, moreover, must ignore responses that were
working memory deficiencies. For example, the previously primed (Deák, 2000).
Working Memory Rating Scale (WMRS;
Alloway & Gathercole, 2008) is a 20-item, four-­ Inhibitory Control
point rating scale (from 0, not typical, to 3, very There are numerous measures that are used to
typical) of problem behaviors that are known to assess inhibitory control in children from the
differentiate children based on their working preschool years. The common thread among
­
memory abilities. The authors of this measure these measures is their attempt to measure the
report that it is particularly valuable for teachers child’s ability to postpone a preferred response in
who do not wish to use more formal assessments favor of an undesired response. Carlson (2005)
of working memory, but do want to provide a provides a comprehensive list of these measures
more systematic evaluation of the potential work- and here we review only a selection. Another
ing memory problems than can be provided by important point to be made: we discussed earlier
Self-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective 159

the unclear distinction between inhibitory control for the duration of the game. Similarly, in the pin-
and effortful control. This vagueness is also viv- ball task (Reed et al., 1984), children are asked to
idly apparent when reviewing measures of inhibi- wait for the experimenter’s direction each time it is
tory and effortful control. In order to somewhat their turn to play the game. Other tasks imitate the
clear this vagueness, we use in this chapter the well-known children game “Simon says” (Bear-
distinction used by Carlson (2005) and others Dragon; Reed et al., 1984; Simon Says, Strommen,
between “cool” and “hot” regulation that we 1973) in which the child is supposed to follow the
believe could be very useful also when trying to direction of the experimenter, but only under cer-
distinguish IC and EC measures. Using this per- tain conditions (e.g., when the experimenter says
spective, we treat measures of “cool” regulation that Simon said to do it) but not under other condi-
skills (i.e., measures that seem to include less tions (e.g., when the experimenter tells the child to
affective components) as inhibitory control mea- do the task without saying “Simon said to”).
sures, whereas we present measures of “hot” Finally, Rothbart and her colleagues have cre-
regulation skills (i.e., measures that clearly ated a series of parent and teacher reports of chil-
include affective components) as measures of dren’s temperament which include a significant
effortful control. number of items tapping inhibitory control.
The following is a selective review of IC mea- Whereas some of these items may tap effortful
sures that are frequently used with children age 4 control (based on our distinction between mea-
and older. Some of these measures are Stroop-­ sures of “cold” and “hot” regulation), we briefly
like assessments of children’s ability to follow a present these questionnaires in this section. This
direction that asks them to inhibit an automatic set of questionnaires covers almost every period
(i.e., dominant) response to a stimulus and to use of development (infancy to adulthood). The ques-
an opposite (i.e., subdominant) response instead. tionnaires relevant to the current chapter are the
For example, in the day/night assessment Infant Behavior Questionnaire (IBQ; Rothbart,
(Gerstadt et al., 1994), children are asked to say 1981; for infants ages 3–12 months); the Early
“night,” when they see a card with a sun drawn on Childhood Behavior Questionnaire (ECBQ;
it, and to say “day,” when they see a card with a Putnam, Gartstein, & Rothbart, 2006; for tod-
moon. Similarly, in the grass/snow assessment dlers ages 18–36 months); the Children’s
(Carlson & Moses, 2001), children are asked to Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ: Rothbart, Ahadi,
point to a white card when the experimenter says Hershey, & Fisher, 2001; for children ages
“grass” and to point to a green card when the 3–7 years); the Temperament in Middle
experimenter says “snow.” In other examples of Childhood Questionnaire (TMCQ; Simonds &
such tests, children are asked to make a fist when Rothbart, 2004; for children 7–10 years old); and
the experimenter points her finger and to point the Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire
their finger when the experimenter makes a fist (EATQ-R; Capaldi & Rothbart, 1992; Ellis &
(Hughes, 1998) or to tap once when the experi- Rothbart, 2001; for children ages 9–15 years).
menter tap twice and vice versa (Blair, 2003). All of these questionnaires include items that ask
Other inhibitory control measures assess chil- parents or teachers to rate children’s capacity to
dren’s ability to follow inhibiting directions for a plan ahead, as well as their ability to suppress
long time and to keep turns. For example, in the inappropriate response.
whisper task (Kochanska et al., 1996), the experi-
menter asks the child to name different cartoon
characters but to always do it in a very quiet voice.  easures of Emotional Self-­
M
In the tower game (Kochanska et al., 1996), chil- Regulation and Effortful Control
dren are asked to build a tower with an experi- in Childhood
menter but only do it on their turn. During this task,
children are never reminded on the turn-­taking rule The most well-known battery of effortful control
and are scored for their ability to maintain the rule measures is likely the one proposed by Kochanska
160 Y. Ziv et al.

and her colleagues (e.g., Kochanska et al., 1996, Other researchers designed innovative mea-
2001). As mentioned earlier, Kochanska’s mea- sures that add complexity to Mischel’s delay of
sures focus on five components of EC: (a) delay- gratification tasks. For example, in the less is
ing (e.g., waiting with an instruction not to do more task (Carlson, Davis, & Leach, 2005),
anything for a pleasant event (receiving a candy or Carlson and her colleagues added a layer of
a gift) that will only occur if waiting), (b) slowing reverse reward contingency to the task. In this
down gross and fine motor activity, (c) suppress- procedure, children are also asked to select
ing/initiating activity to a signal (e.g., games in between a larger and smaller selection of candies
which the child produces a response to one signal (put on trays) but, in addition, are told that the
and inhibits it to another), (d) effortful attention tray they will select will go to a naughty puppet,
(Stroop-like assessments, which requires ignoring whereas they will receive the other tray. Carlson
a dominant perceptual feature of a stimulus in (2005) also combined the Saarni’s disappointing
favor of a subdominant feature), and (e) lowering gift task (Saarni, 1984) with Kochanska’s gift
voice (whispering). As can be seen, based on our delay task (Kochanska et al., 1996) to create an
“hot” and “cold” definitions above, not all of these even more elaborated version of the delay of grat-
tests qualify as “real” EC measures, and some ification task. In this combined version, children
seem to tap the cognitive rather than the affective are asked to wait until an experimenter wraps a
part of control. Thus, in the following short gift that she “forgot” to wrap before. This is done
description of EC measures, we focus only on behind their back. Then, when they open the gift,
those that seem to trigger the arousal levels of spe- it turns out to be a disappointing gift, and their
cific affective systems. affective responses are being measured.
Most of these measures seem to be related to
the first of Kochanska’s definition of EC, delay-
ing, in which children are asked to delay their Self-Regulation
response to an attractive stimulus in order to and Psychopathology: Implications
receive a better reward later. For example, in the to Field Practitioners
delay of gratification task (Mischel, Shoda, &
Rodriguez, 1989), after children select their Failures of self-regulation contribute to chil-
favorite treat out of two options, two bowls with dren’s maladjustment and are manifested in vari-
their favorite treat are placed in front of them, one ous forms of children’s psychopathology.
with a large number of treats and the other with a Particularly, children’s difficulties at self-­
small number of treats. After making sure that regulation are evident in a wide range of malad-
the children prefer the bowl with the larger justed patterns of behaviors, including both
amount of treats, the children are then told that externalizing and internalizing spectrums
the experimenter needs to leave the room for a (Neuhaus & Beauchaine, 2013; Nigg, 2000). On
while but if they wait until she returns, they will the other hand, research has clearly demonstrated
receive the bowl with the large number of treats. that optimal self-regulatory capacities contribute
Children are also given the option to call the to children’s adaptive social and academic adjust-
experimenter back to the room but are being told ment (Eisenberg, Duckworth et al., 2014;
that if they do that, they will receive the bowl Eisenberg, Hofer, et al., 2014; Liew, 2012).
with the smaller amount of treats. Mischel and Therefore, informing practitioners and socializa-
his colleagues designed a number of delay of tion agents how to foster optimal self-regulatory
gratification tasks, perhaps the most well known capacities should be a primary goal of
is the Stanford Marshmallow test (Mischel, ­self-­regulation research. The present section will
Ebbesen, & Raskoff Zeiss, 1972), which is an first discuss how self-regulatory capacities are
earlier and simpler version of the delay of gratifi- involved in children’s maladjustment and then
cation measure described above. discuss implications for practitioners.
Self-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective 161

 elf-Regulation and Externalizing
S researchers (e.g., Martel, 2009; Nigg, Goldsmith,
Problems & Sachek, 2004) have suggested that deficiencies
in effortful control may account for the inatten-
Lower levels of self-regulation capabilities have tive symptoms of ADHD. Following these
been consistently linked to higher levels of exter- claims, a growing body of research has explored
nalizing problems, manifested in aggression, the links between effortful control and ADHD
impulsivity, and inattention. This association is symptoms. Although preliminary, this research
evident in the toddler and preschool years as well has consistently demonstrated that children with
as in later childhood and adolescence (for a ADHD score lower than controls on measures of
review, see Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Eggum, 2010). effortful control (De Pauw & Mervielde, 2010;
However, closer inspection at different external- Foley, McClowry, & Castellanos, 2008; Martel,
izing symptoms and different self-regulatory Gremillion, & Roberts, 2012; Martel & Nigg,
capacities reveals a more complicated picture. 2006). Similarly, studies that explored individual
Notably, failures of self-regulation are evident differences in effortful control and ADHD symp-
in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder toms found negative relations between the con-
(ADHD). ADHD has long been associated with structs, both among preschool children
impaired abilities for response suppression, man- (Papageorgiou et al., 2014) and among college
ifested in ADHD children’s difficulties at execut- students (Graziano et al., 2015).
ing goal-directed behaviors (Nigg, 2000). Thus, Another line of research has followed Shiner
large body of evidence has demonstrated that and Caspi’s (2003) suggestion that temperament
children who exhibit ADHD symptoms perform and personality traits can and should be inte-
worse than controls on tasks measuring response grated in children due to similarities between the
suppression such as the stop-signal task (Logan, two domains. Specifically, these researchers
1994) and the go/no-go task (Miller, Schäffer, & claimed that there is a certain degree of overlap
Hackley, 1991). Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, between effortful control and the trait “conscien-
and Pennington (2005) reviewed studies and tiousness,” defined as “the propensity to follow
noted a composite effect size for ADHD versus socially prescribed norms for impulse control, to
control of d = 0.61 (a medium effect size). be goal directed, to plan, and to be able to delay
Similarly, Schoemaker, Mulder, Deković, and gratification and to follow norms and rules”
Matthys (2013) reviewed 18 studies that explored (Roberts, Jackson, Fayard, Edmonds, & Meints,
the relations between executive functions and 2009, p. 369; see also Eisenberg, Duckworth,
ADHD symptoms among preschoolers and found Spinrad, & Valiente, 2014). Accordingly, several
a medium correlation effect size (ESzr = 0.21) for studies have demonstrated that children who
overall executive functions, as measured by exhibit ADHD symptoms score lower than con-
teachers’ and parents’ questionnaires. More spe- trols on measures tapping conscientiousness
cifically, small effect sizes were found for work- (Martel, 2016; Martel, Nigg, & von Eye, 2009;
ing memory (ESzr = 0.17) and for cognitive Ullsperger, Nigg, & Nikolas, 2016).
flexibility (ESzr = 0.14), and medium effect size Taken together, these results suggest that emo-
was found for inhibitory control (ESzr = 0.24). tional self-regulatory capacities play a role in
Therefore, these studies suggest that although ADHD symptomatology. However, more
children who exhibit ADHD symptoms manifest research is needed to establish this assumption.
impaired cognitive self-regulation abilities, this Specifically, longitudinal studies are required to
link may be less robust than what was commonly examine developmental trajectories and causal
argued. pathways. In addition, more direct scrutiny
Beyond cognitive control mechanisms, defi- should explore the differentiation between mea-
ciencies in emotional self-regulation have also sures supposedly tapping the same constructs
been associated with ADHD (Barkley, 1997; (i.e., effortful control, inhibitory control, and
Nigg & Casey, 2005; Wender, 1995). Accordingly, conscientiousness) and their relation to ADHD.
162 Y. Ziv et al.

Deficiencies in self-regulatory capacities are growing body of research has addressed this
also evident in disruptive behavior disorders lacuna and showed links between executive
(DBD), such as oppositional defiant disorder and functions and DBD, above and beyond the pres-
conduct disorder. In the past two decades, a ence of comorbid ADHD. Indeed, Schoemaker
growing body of research has explored the rela- et al.’ (2013) meta-analysis mentioned above
tions between effortful control and externalizing reviewed nine studies that explored the relations
problems and DBD (for a full review, see between DBD and executive functions and found
Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Eggum, 2010; Eisenberg, similar effect sizes as in studies that explored
Spinrad, Eggum, Silva, et al., 2010). This ADHD and executive functions (ESzr = 0.19,
research demonstrated that effortful control 0.15, 0.22, and 0.13 for overall executive func-
assessed at toddlerhood and early childhood neg- tions, working memory, inhibitory control, and
atively predicted externalizing symptoms both at flexibility, respectively).
preschool (Eisenberg et al., 2005; Murray & In recent years, researchers have further estab-
Kochanska, 2002; Olson, Sameroff, Kerr, Lopez, lished this link between executive functions and
& Wellman, 2005; Valiente et al., 2013) and mid- different types of aggressive behavior (Buss,
dle childhood (Choe, Olson, & Sameroff, 2014; Kiel, Morales, & Robinson, 2014; Choe, Shaw,
Eisenberg et al., 2004). Also, Woltering, Lishak, Brennan, Dishion, & Wilson, 2014; Euler,
Hodgson, Granic, and Zelazo (2016) found that Sterzer, & Stadler, 2014; Granvald & Marciszko,
7–12-year-old children diagnosed with DBD 2016; Monette, Bigras, & Guay, 2015; Sulik,
scored lower than controls on measures of effort- Blair, Mills-Koonce, Berry, & Greenberg, 2015;
ful control. Suurland et al., 2016; Verlinden et al., 2014;
However, both Spinrad et al. (2007) and Woltering et al., 2016). Overall, findings from
Eisenberg, Taylor, Widaman, and Spinrad (2015) these studies support the assumption that a strong
found that effortful control did not negatively link exits between executive functions and differ-
predict toddlers’ externalizing problems over ent types of aggression, both in children diag-
time when controlling for earlier levels of exter- nosed with DBD (e.g., Euler et al., 2014), and in
nalizing problems. Therefore, it is suggested that non-referred populations (e.g., Sulik et al., 2015).
effortful control is linked to maladjustment only
after it is fairly sophisticated and mature.
Eisenberg et al. (2015) also found that high levels  elf-Regulation and Internalizing
S
of children’s externalizing problems at both 30 Problems
and 42 months negatively predicted effortful con-
trol a year later. Thus, it is likely that, at these In internalizing problems, deviant emotion-­
ages, high levels of externalizing problems impair driven behaviors are targeted inward toward the
the development of effortful control by affecting individual (Colman, Wadsworth, Croudace, &
children’s social environment, including aspects Jones, 2007). This category encompasses a wide
of parenting. range of problems, such as anxiety, depression,
While the research exploring the relations withdrawal, and somatic complaints. At a first
between effortful control and DBD has been glance, the relations between self-regulation and
intensively explored over the years, the relations internalizing problems may seem less clear and
between executive functions, and specifically straightforward, as these have often been men-
inhibitory control, and DBD have only lately tioned as problems characterized by overcontrol.
gained researchers’ attention. This late inspec- However, Eisenberg and her colleagues have sug-
tion might have been the result of the historical gested that this overcontrol is reactive and there-
view that ascribed the impairments in executive fore could be counteracted by effortful control
functions, often found in children with DBD, to (Eisenberg, Spinrad, & Morris, 2002).
comorbid ADHD in these children (Pennington Indeed, several internalizing problems are
& Ozonoff, 1996). However, in recent years, a manifested by dysregulated emotion expression
Self-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective 163

and experience (e.g., rumination; Nolen-­ as well be the result of a self-report bias or of
Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008), as construct validity bias, wherein items from both
well as heightened emotional reactivity and the CBQ and the measurements of internalizing
impulsivity (Carver, Johnson, & Joormann, 2008; symptoms tap similar constructs (Lemery et al.,
Yap, Allen, & Sheeber, 2007). However, the rela- 2002; Lengua, West, & Sandler, 1998).
tions of self-regulatory capacities with internal- A different pattern of results emerged in mid-
izing problems have been less systematically dle childhood and adolescence, where fairly con-
explored than their relations with externalizing sistent negative relationships between effortful
problems. In addition, most studies that exam- control and internalizing symptoms emerge (Hilt,
ined this link focused on the concept of effortful Armstrong, & Essex, 2012; Hofer, Eisenberg, &
control, and less on inhibitory control. Reiser, 2010; Muris, 2006; Muris, Meesters, &
Studies that explored the relations between Blijlevens, 2007; Oldehinkel, Hartman,
effortful control and internalizing symptoms Ferdinand, Verhulst, & Ormel, 2007; Sportel,
exhibited mixed findings that tend to vary with Nauta, de Hullu, & de Jong, 2013; Sportel, Nauta,
age. Specifically, whereas several studies con- de Hullu, de Jong, & Hartman, 2011). These neg-
ducted in the toddlerhood and preschool years ative relationships were found both in relation to
found negative relations between effortful con- depression (Sportel et al., 2013), to anxiety
trol and internalizing symptoms (Carrasco, (Muris, 2006; Oldehinkel et al., 2007; Sportel
Holgado-Tello, Delgado, & González-Peña, et al., 2011; Vervoort et al., 2011), and to mixed
2016; Hopkins, Lavigne, Gouze, LeBailly, & measures of internalizing symptoms, which
Bryant, 2013; Lemery, Essex, & Smider, 2002), include withdrawal, anxiety/depression, and
others found positive relationships (Murray & somatic symptoms (Dyson, Robertson, & Wong,
Kochanska, 2002) or mild/null relationships 2015; Hofer et al., 2010; Muhtadie, Zhou,
(Dennis, Brotman, Huang, & Gouley, 2007; Eisenberg, & Wang, 2013). In addition, recently
Eisenberg, Spinrad, Eggum, Silva, et al., 2010; several studies have indicated that effortful con-
Ghassabian et al., 2014; Moran, Lengua, & trol capacities served as a moderator between
Zalewski, 2013). In addition, in all cases where several risk factors and later internalization prob-
internalizing and externalizing symptoms were lems (e.g., Gulley, Hankin, & Young, 2016; Hilt
simultaneously measured, the internalizing et al., 2012; Muhtadie et al., 2013).
symptoms’ relationships with effortful control Thus, these results suggest that effortful con-
were weaker than they were with externalizing trol capacities developed early in life might pro-
symptoms (e.g., Carrasco et al., 2016; Eisenberg, tect children against internalizing symptoms in
Spinrad, & Eggum, 2010; Eisenberg, Spinrad, adolescence. However, as the research is still pre-
Eggum, Silva, et al., 2010; Moran et al., 2013). liminary, more data is needed to determine the
Interestingly, there was a stark division developmental trajectories that follow from early
between the measurements used to assess effort- effortful control capacities to internalizing symp-
ful control in the studies that found negative rela- toms in adolescence. In addition, much less
tionships between effortful control and research has explored the relations between
internalizing symptoms and those that did not. inhibitory control and internalizing symptoms,
Thus, while the former relied solely on the CBQ and recent evidence suggests that such an explo-
(Putnam & Rothbart, 2006; Rothbart et al., 2001) ration is warranted. For instance, Ghassabian
to measure effortful control, the latter used the et al. (2014) relied on self-report measures of
battery developed by Kochanska (1996, 2001) or inhibitory control and showed positive relations
similar observational methods. This distinction between parents’ reports of inhibitory control at
casts doubt on the assumption that early child- age 4 and internalizing symptoms at age 6. As
hood internalizing problems are related to defi- mentioned, more research is needed in order to
ciencies in effortful control at these ages. Thus, establish these findings, and specifically, behav-
the negative links found in the early years might ioral measures of inhibitory control are needed.
164 Y. Ziv et al.

 elf-Regulation and Adaptive
S Diamond (2012) described three types of
Adjustment interventions aimed at improving children’s
executive functions, namely, computerized train-
Beyond their role in the development and preven- ing, school curricula, and physical exercise
tion of psychopathology, self-regulatory capaci- (including martial arts and meditation training).
ties contribute to children’s adaptive adjustment Computerized training tasks were originally
(for a review, see Liew, 2012). Research has con- designed to improve the working memory aspect
sistently demonstrated that children with good of executive functions (for a review, see Shipstead,
self-regulatory capacities do better than other Redick, & Engle, 2012). The most researched
children both socially and academically. For approach for improving children’s working
instance, several studies found positive relation- memory is Cogmed computerized training. This
ships between effortful control and adaptive indi- training was found to be successful in several
cators of social adjustment among children, such studies (e.g., Holmes, Gathercole, & Dunning,
as empathy (e.g., Eisenberg, Wentzel, & Harris, 2009; Holmes et al., 2010; Klingberg et al., 2005;
1998; Panfile & Laible, 2012; Rothbart, Ahadi, & Thorell, Lindqvist, Bergman Nutley, Bohlin, &
Hershey, 1994) and prosocial behavior (e.g., Klingberg, 2009). However, other authors
Diener & Kim, 2004; Eisenberg et al., 1997; advised more caution in the interpretation of
Luengo Kanacri, Pastorelli, Eisenberg, Zuffianò, results (Shipstead, Hicks, & Engle, 2012).
& Caprara, 2013). Specifically, these authors claimed that Cogmed
In addition, researchers have demonstrated improves performance on tasks that resemble
positive relationships between self-regularity Cogmed training, but probably does not transfer
capacities and academic skills and achievements to untrained tasks. Several studies have also tried
both in the preschool years (e.g., Blair & Razza, to implement Cogmed to also improve recipients’
2007; McClelland et al., 2007) and in grade inhibitory control abilities, with partial success.
school (e.g., Liew, McTigue, Barrois, & Hughes, Specifically, whereas gains in inhibitory control
2008; Valiente, Lemery-Chalfant, Swanson, & following Cogmed practice were observed in
Reiser, 2008; Valiente et al., 2013). middle childhood (Karbach & Kray, 2009), none
were observed in preschool children (Rueda,
Rothbart, McCandliss, Saccomanno, & Posner,
Promoting Self-Regulatory Capacities 2005; Thorell et al., 2009).
School curricula were designed to more spe-
The evidence overviewed in the previous sections cifically address children’s self-regulatory capac-
emphasized the importance of self-regulatory ities of inhibitory control. For example, the
capacities to children’s psychosocial and academic Promoting Alternative Thinking Strategies
adjustment. In addition, a large body of evidence (PATHS; Greenberg, Kusche, Cook, & Quamma,
indicates that beyond heredity, socializers’ prac- 1995) curriculum involves classroom lessons and
tices affect the development of self-­ regulatory students’ practice of inhibitory control and emo-
capacities (Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spinrad, tion identification, on children’s self-regulation.
1998). Together, this body of evidence under- Riggs, Greenberg, Kusché, and Pentz (2006)
scores the importance of developing effective found that students (second and third graders at
interventions to foster self-regulation. However, pretest) participating in PATHS performed better
the research supporting the effectiveness of such than control children on measures of executive
attempts is still limited. Thus, whereas several function (inhibitory control) and verbal fluency.
attempts were focused on the improvement of chil- However, these results were not replicated in
dren’s executive functions, including inhibitory other intervention studies that used the PATHS
control (for a review, see Diamond & Lee, 2011), (Bierman, Nix, Greenberg, Blair, & Domitrovich,
only a few have targeted effortful control. 2008; Domitrovich, Cortes, & Greenberg, 2007).
Self-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective 165

The Tools of the Mind (Tools; Bodrova & take part in either traditional taekwondo or stan-
Leong, 2007) is an intervention program that dard physical education. Students in the tae-
focuses specifically on promoting young chil- kwondo group improved more than students in the
dren’s (aged 3–6) executive functions, including standard physical education group in working
self-regulatory capacities. This program was memory and on several dimensions of inhibitory
inspired be Vygotsky (1978), who emphasized control.
the importance of social pretend play for early Nevertheless, the claim that martial arts may
development of executive functions. During pre- promote children’s executive functions was criti-
tend play, children must inhibit acting out of cized by other authors (Mercer, 2011; Strayhorn
character, remember their own and others’ roles, & Strayhorn, 2011), doubting the scientific status
and flexibly adjust as their friends improvise. of the evidence showing such links. Furthermore,
Such play exercises all three core EFs and is cen- beyond criticizing the quality of research,
tral to Tools. Diamond et al. (2007) demonstrated Strayhorn and Strayhorn (2011) espoused an
the effectiveness of the Tools curriculum to chil- educational and ethical stance and claimed that
dren’s three aspects of executive function among “in a world beset by violence, there is irony and
young children. However, more research is pathos in hoping that our children will be
needed to establish the effectiveness of Tools and improved by teaching punching, kicking, and
similar programs in improving children’s execu- tripping” (p. 310).
tive functions. These efforts should extend
beyond specific populations and age levels and
explore the mechanisms through which these  onclusions and Future Research
C
programs exert their changes. Directions
There are also several studies that explored the
effectiveness of both habitual (also referred to in There are a number of important conclusions that
the literature as “chronic”) and singular (also could be drawn from the current review:
referred to in the literature as “acute”) physical
activity on children’s executive functions (for a There is a lack of clarity in the definition of
review, see Best, 2010). It was suggested that this self-regulation and its subcomponents. Like
effect was typical to forms of exercise that are cog- many others before us (e.g., Liew, 2012;
nitively engaging and that this cognitive engage- McClelland & Cameron, 2012; Zhou et al.,
ment inherent in exercise may help explain how 2012), we identified some inconsistencies and
exercise impacts cognition (Sibley & Etnier, 2003; overlaps in the various definitions of self-­
Tomporowski, Davis, Miller, & Naglieri, 2008). regulation and its subcomponents. This may be
Along the same lines, Diamond (2012) sug- the result of self-regulation being the focus of
gested that exercise alone may be less effective in two distinct research traditions, one that views
improving children’s executive functions than self-regulation from a cognitive perspective and
activities that involve both exercise and character another focusing on the affective aspects of self-­
development (e.g., traditional martial arts) or regulation. These two research traditions have
activities that involve both exercise and mindful- identified constructs and developed measures
ness (e.g., yoga). For example, Razza, Bergen-­ independently, and this resulted in major over-
Cico, and Raymond (2015) showed that laps. Perhaps one solution to this state of affairs
preschoolers (3- to 5-year-olds), who went through is to reconstruct measures within each research
a daily mindful yoga practice for a year, performed tradition that are conscious of the definitions and
better than control children on several indices of measures created within the other tradition. For
self-regulation (including effortful control). example, conceptualize effortful control mea-
Similarly, Lakes and Hoyt (2004) randomly sures as those tapping “hot” self-regulation and
assigned children in kindergarten through fifth inhibitory control measures as those tapping
grade (5- to 11-year-olds) by homeroom class to “cold” self-regulation, as we tried to demonstrate
166 Y. Ziv et al.

in the current chapter (see “Inhibitory Control” pose today on the development of the PFC and
and “Measures of Emotional Self-Regulation and the likely evolutionary trade-off occurring during
Effortful Control in Childhood” sections). these early years (i.e., cognition without control:
Moreover, we suggest that current definitions young children cannot efficiently regulate but
of self-regulation do not do full justice to the still learn multiple skills in the most efficient
complex set of behaviors and mental representa- way; see “Self-Regulation in Infancy and
tions that children may be using to gain control Toddlerhood (Ages 0–3” section) should inform
over their own and others’ behaviors. Most defi- practitioners developing intervention programs
nitions (as is the one we suggested at the begin- for infants and toddlers perhaps not to put too
ning of this chapter) focus on self-soothing and much emphasis on teaching and enhancing regu-
attention control, yet children as young as 2 can lation abilities during these years.
use a chain of behaviors in response to actions Moreover, this knowledge should also inform
and events in their environment that require regu- our thinking and research on self-regulation in
lating their arousal levels. For example, they may later years. As mentioned earlier, traditional
fail to self-regulate based on traditional defini- thinking on self-regulation is pretty much one
tions when they do not receive the toy they directional in nature, i.e., self-regulation is
wanted (i.e., respond with a temper tantrum) but almost always considered as advantageous for
later may show prosocial behavior that hints on a the development of children, whereas dysregula-
connection they make to the previous act (e.g., tion is always considered a disadvantage. This
give the toy they received after the tantrum to line of thinking has led to the fact that there are
their sister to play with). We suggest that no studies focusing on the possible advantages
researchers should reconsider current definitions of dysregulation. For example, is it possible that
of self-regulation to include also the chain of children showing difficulties to self-regulate
reactions that may occur after the initial trigger. think more innovatively? Perhaps because they
A promising line of research that could pro- rarely fit the box, they are forced to think “out-
vide insight into such complexities is the study of side the box”? We believe that future research
different emotion regulatory strategies (Gross, should explore these possibilities by, for exam-
1998), which has been mostly applied to adults’ ple, (a) designing studies focusing on measuring
population. As several studies have demonstrated outcomes that are different from the usual school
that children use emotion regulatory strategies readiness and adjustment constructs that are typ-
such as reappraisal (e.g., McRae et al., 2012), we ically measured in relation to self-regulation and
advise a more thorough scrutiny of the different (b) studying in depth children with self-­
tactics both young and older children use to regu- regulation problems who are still successful in
late their emotions. school and in other aspects of their lives. This
type of research could certainly inform the prac-
Whereas the ability to self-regulate has clear tices of educators and clinicians committed to
developmental advantages, its limitations improve the developmental outcomes of children
should also be considered. Related to the last with self-regulation difficulties.
point we just made, the restrictive definitions of
self-regulation may prevent us from focusing not The associations between self-regulation and
only on the potential problems that are associated psychopathology should be more specifically
with dysregulation but also on the possible explored. We have reviewed above the abun-
advantages that may exist in some unregulated dance of literature on the links between
behaviors. self-­
­ regulation and psychopathology with a
We have discussed earlier that there are clear majority of studies showing links between self-
developmental benefits to the seeming inability regulation failures and various adjustment
to efficiently and independently regulate during problems. However, in order to advance the field,
the first 3 years of life. The knowledge that we the developmental pathways by which early
Self-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective 167

self-­regulation problems affect later externaliz- Block, J. H., & Block, J. (1979). The role of ego-control
and ego-resiliency in the organization of behavior. In
ing and internalizing problems should be further
W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota symposia on child psy-
explored. In addition, existing research on these chology (Vol. 13). Hillsdale, N.J: Erlbaum.
links did not control sufficiently for the possible Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2007). Tools of the mind.
overlap between different developmental prob- Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson.
Bridges, L. J., & Grolnick, W. S. (1995). The development
lems, for example, differentiating externalizing
of emotional self-regulation in infancy and early child-
problems that are or are not the result of ADHD hood. Social Development, 15, 185–211.
or differentiating anxiety and depression when Bridgett, D. J., Burt, N. M., Edwards, E. S., & Deater-­
examining the links between self-regulation and Deckard, K. (2015). Intergenerational transmission of
self-regulation: A multidisciplinary review and inte-
internalizing problems.
grative conceptual framework. Psychological Bulletin,
141(3), 602.
Bridgett, D. J., Oddi, K. B., Laake, L. M., Murdock,
K. W., & Bachmann, M. N. (2013). Integrating and
References differentiating aspects of self-regulation: Effortful
control, executive functioning, and links to negative
Alloway, T. P., & Gathercole, S. E. (2008). Working affectivity. Emotion, 13(1), 47.
Memory Rating Scale (WMRS): Manual. London: Bronson, M. (2000). Self-regulation in early childhood:
Pearson Education. Nature and nurture. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. Bryce, D., Szűcs, D., Soltész, F., & Whitebread, D. (2011).
(2006). Verbal and visuospatial short-term and work- The development of inhibitory control: An averaged
ing memory in children: Are they separable? Child and single-trial lateralized readiness potential study.
Development, 77(6), 1698–1716. NeuroImage, 57(3), 671–685.
Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new com- Buss, K. A., Kiel, E. J., Morales, S., & Robinson, E.
ponent of working memory? Trends in Cognitive (2014). Toddler inhibitory control, bold response
Sciences, 4(11), 417–423. to novelty, and positive affect predict externalizing
Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-­ symptoms in kindergarten. Social Development, 23(2),
regulation. Organizational Behavior and Human 232–249.
Decision Processes, 50(2), 248–287. Capaldi, D. M., & Rothbart, M. K. (1992). Development
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained and validation of an early adolescent temperament
attention, and executive functions: Constructing a measure. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 12(2),
unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 153–173.
121(1), 65. Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive mea-
Beebe, B., & Lachmann, F. M. (1998). Co-constructing sures of executive function in preschool children.
inner and relational processes: Self-and mutual Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 595–616.
regulation in infant research and adult treatment. Carlson, S. M., Davis, A. C., & Leach, J. G. (2005). Less
Psychoanalytic Psychology, 15(4), 480. is more executive function and symbolic representa-
Best, J. R. (2010). Effects of physical activity on chil- tion in preschool children. Psychological Science,
dren’s executive function: Contributions of experi- 16(8), 609–616.
mental research on aerobic exercise. Developmental Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differ-
Review, 30(4), 331–351. ences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of
Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental per- mind. Child Development, 72(4), 1032–1053.
spective on executive function. Child Development, Carrasco, M. Á., Holgado-Tello, F. P., Delgado, B., &
81(6), 1641–1660. González-Peña, P. (2016). Reactive temperament
Bierman, K. L., Nix, R. L., Greenberg, M. T., Blair, C., traits and behavioural problems in children: The
& Domitrovich, C. E. (2008). Executive functions mediating role of effortful control across sex and age.
and school readiness intervention: Impact, modera- European Journal of Developmental Psychology,
tion, and mediation in the Head Start REDI program. 13(2), 197–212.
Development and Psychopathology, 20(03), 821–843. Carver, C. S., Johnson, S. L., & Joormann, J. (2008).
Blair, C. (2003). Behavioral inhibition and behavioral Serotonergic function, two-mode models of self-­
activation in young children: Relations with self-­ regulation, and vulnerability to depression: What
regulation and adaptation to preschool in children depression has in common with impulsive aggression.
attending Head Start. Developmental Psychobiology, Psychological Bulletin, 134(6), 912.
42(3), 301–311. Choe, D. E., Olson, S. L., & Sameroff, A. J. (2014).
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful con- Effortful control moderates bidirectional effects
trol, executive function, and false belief understanding between children’s externalizing behavior and their
to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. mothers’ depressive symptoms. Child Development,
Child Development, 78(2), 647–663. 85(2), 643–658.
168 Y. Ziv et al.

Choe, D. E., Shaw, D. S., Brennan, L. M., Dishion, Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to
T. J., & Wilson, M. N. (2014). Inhibitory con- aid executive function development in children 4 to 12
trol as a mediator of bidirectional effects between years old. Science, 333(6045), 959–964.
early oppositional behavior and maternal depres- Diener, M. L., & Kim, D. Y. (2004). Maternal and child
sion. Development and Psychopathology, 26(4pt1), predictors of preschool children's social competence.
1129–1147. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 25(1),
Cicchetti, D., & Tucker, D. (1994). Development and self-­ 3–24.
regulatory structures of the mind. Development and Domitrovich, C. E., Cortes, R. C., & Greenberg, M. T.
Psychopathology, 6(04), 533–549. (2007). Improving young children’s social and emo-
Cole, P. M., Dennis, T. A., Smith-Simon, K. E., & tional competence: A randomized trial of the pre-
Cohen, L. H. (2009). Preschoolers’ emotion regulation school “PATHS” curriculum. The Journal of Primary
strategy understanding: Relations with emotion social- Prevention, 28(2), 67–91.
ization and child self-regulation. Social Development, Dyson, R., Robertson, G. C., & Wong, M. M. (2015).
18(2), 324–352. Brief report: Peer group influences and adolescent
Colman, I., Wadsworth, M. E., Croudace, T. J., & internalizing problems as mediated by effortful con-
Jones, P. B. (2007). Forty-year psychiatric outcomes trol. Journal of Adolescence, 41, 131–135.
following assessment for internalizing disorder in Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998).
adolescence. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164(1), Parental socialization of emotion. Psychological
126–133. Inquiry, 9(4), 241–273.
Conklin, H. M., Luciana, M., Hooper, C. J., & Eisenberg, N., Duckworth, A. L., Spinrad, T. L., &
Yarger, R. S. (2007). Working memory performance Valiente, C. (2014). Conscientiousness: Origins in
in typically developing children and adolescents: childhood? Developmental Psychology, 50(5), 1331.
Behavioral evidence of protracted frontal lobe devel- Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Murphy,
opment. Developmental Neuropsychology, 31(1), B. C., Guthrie, I. K., Jones, S., … Maszk, P. (1997).
103–128. Contemporaneous and longitudinal prediction of chil-
Davis, H. L., & Pratt, C. (1996). The development of chil- dren’s social functioning from regulation and emo-
dren’s theory of mind: The working memory explana- tionality. Child Development, 68(4), 642–664.
tion. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 25–31. Eisenberg, N., Hofer, C., Sulik, M. J., & Spinrad, T. L.
De Pauw, S. S., & Mervielde, I. (2010). Temperament, (2014). Self-regulation, effortful control, and their
personality and developmental psychopathology: A socioemotional correlates. Handbook of Emotion
review based on the conceptual dimensions under- Regulation, 2, 157–172.
lying childhood traits. Child Psychiatry & Human Eisenberg, N., & Morris, A. S. (2002). Children's emotion-­
Development, 41(3), 313–329. related regulation. Advances in Child Development
Deák, G. O. (2000). The growth of flexible problem solv- and Behavior, 30, 190–230.
ing: Preschool children use changing verbal cues to Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Eggum, N. D. (2010).
infer multiple word meanings. Journal of Cognition Emotion-related self-regulation and its relation to
and Development, 1(2), 157–191. children’s maladjustment. Annual Review of Clinical
Deák, G. O. (2003). The development of cognitive flex- Psychology, 6, 495.
ibility and language abilities. Advances in Child Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Eggum, N. D., Silva, K. M.,
Development and Behavior, 31, 273–328. Reiser, M., Hofer, C., & Michalik, N. (2010). Relations
Deák, G. O., & Narasimham, G. (2014). Young chil- among maternal socialization, effortful control, and
dren’s flexible use of semantic cues to word mean- maladjustment in early childhood. Development and
ings: Converging evidence of individual and Psychopathology, 22(03), 507–525.
age differences. Journal of Child Language, 41, Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M.,
511–542. Cumberland, A., Shepard, S. A., … Thompson, M.
Deák, G. O., & Wiseheart, M. (2015). Cognitive flexibil- (2004). The relations of effortful control and impul-
ity in young children: General or task-specific capac- sivity to children’s resiliency and adjustment. Child
ity? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 138, Development, 75(1), 25–46.
31–53. Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Morris, A. S. (2002).
Dennis, T. A., Brotman, L. M., Huang, K. Y., & Regulation, resiliency, and quality of social function-
Gouley, K. K. (2007). Effortful control, social com- ing. Self and Identity, 1(2), 121–128.
petence, and adjustment problems in children at risk Eisenberg, N., Taylor, Z. E., Widaman, K. F., &
for psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child and Spinrad, T. L. (2015). Externalizing symptoms,
Adolescent Psychology, 36(3), 442–454. effortful control, and intrusive parenting: A test of
Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve bidirectional longitudinal relations during early child-
children’s executive functions. Current Directions in hood. Development and Psychopathology, 27(4pt1),
Psychological Science, 21(5), 335–341. 953–968.
Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. Eisenberg, N., Wentzel, M., & Harris, J. D. (1998). The
(2007). Preschool program improves cognitive con- role of emotionality and regulation in empathy-related
trol. Science, 318, 1387. responding. School Psychology Review, 27(4), 506.
Self-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective 169

Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., symptomatology and risky health, driving, and finan-
Fabes, R. A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations among posi- cial behaviors in college: The mediating role of sensa-
tive parenting, children’s effortful control, and exter- tion seeking and effortful control. Journal of Attention
nalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study. Disorders, 19(3), 179.
Child Development, 76(5), 1055–1071. Greenberg, M. T., Kusche, C. A., Cook, E. T., &
Ellis, L. K., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001, April). Revision Quamma, J. P. (1995). Promoting emotional compe-
of the early adolescent temperament questionnaire. tence in school-aged children: The effects of the PATHS
In Poster presented at the 2001 Biennial Meeting curriculum. Development and Psychopathology,
of the Society for Research in Child Development, 7(01), 117–136.
Minneapolis, Minnesota. Gross, J. J. (1998). The Emerging Field of Emotion
Euler, F., Sterzer, P., & Stadler, C. (2014). Cognitive con- Regulation: An Integrative Review. Review of General
trol under distressing emotional stimulation in ado- Psychology, 2, 271–299.
lescents with conduct disorder. Aggressive Behavior, Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion
40(2), 109–119. regulation: Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3–24).
monitoring: A new area of cognitive–developmental New York: Guilford Press.
inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10), 906. Gulley, L. D., Hankin, B. L., & Young, J. F. (2016). Risk
Florez, I. R. (2011). Developing young children’s self-­ for depression and anxiety in youth: The interaction
regulation through everyday experiences. YC Young between negative affectivity, effortful control, and
Children, 66(4), 46. stressors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
Foley, M., McClowry, S. G., & Castellanos, F. X. (2008). 44(2), 207–218.
The relationship between attention deficit hyperactiv- Heckhausen, J., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Motivation
ity disorder and child temperament. Journal of Applied and self-regulation across the life span. Cambridge:
Developmental Psychology, 29(2), 157–169. Cambridge University Press.
Gagne, J. R., & Hill Goldsmith, H. (2011). A longitudinal Henry, L. A., & Norman, T. (1996). The relationships
analysis of anger and inhibitory control in twins from between memory performance, use of simple mem-
12 to 36 months of age. Developmental Science, 14(1), ory strategies and metamemory in young children.
112–124. International Journal of Behavioral Development,
Gartstein, M. A., Bridgett, D. J., Young, B. N., Panksepp, 19(1), 177–199.
J., & Power, T. (2013). Origins of effortful con- Hilt, L. M., Armstrong, J. M., & Essex, M. J. (2012). Early
trol: Infant and parent contributions. Infancy, 18(2), family context and development of adolescent rumina-
149–183. tive style: Moderation by temperament. Cognition &
Gathercole, S. E., Brown, L., & Pickering, S. J. (2003). Emotion, 26(5), 916–926.
Working memory assessments at school entry as lon- Hofer, C., Eisenberg, N., & Reiser, M. (2010). The role
gitudinal predictors of National Curriculum attain- of socialization, effortful control, and ego resiliency
ment levels. Educational and Child Psychology, in French adolescents’ social functioning. Journal of
20(3), 109–122. Research on Adolescence, 20(3), 555–582.
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Dunning, D. L. (2009).
Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of
from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, poor working memory in children. Developmental
40(2), 177. Science, 12(4), F9–F15.
Gerstadt, C. L., Hong, Y. J., & Diamond, A. (1994). Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., Place, M.,
The relationship between cognition and action: Dunning, D. L., Hilton, K. A., & Elliott, J. G. (2010).
Performance of children 312–7 years old on a stroop-­ Working memory deficits can be overcome: Impacts
like day-night test. Cognition, 53(2), 129–153. of training and medication on working memory in
Ghassabian, A., Székely, E., Herba, C. M., Jaddoe, V. W., children with ADHD. Applied Cognitive Psychology,
Hofman, A., Oldehinkel, A. J., & Tiemeier, H. (2014). 24(6), 827–836.
From positive emotionality to internalizing prob- Hopkins, J., Lavigne, J. V., Gouze, K. R., LeBailly, S. A., &
lems: The role of executive functioning in preschool- Bryant, F. B. (2013). Multi-domain models of risk fac-
ers. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 23(9), tors for depression and anxiety symptoms in preschool-
729–741. ers: Evidence for common and specific factors. Journal
Gordon, A. C., & Olson, D. R. (1998). The relation of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(5), 705–722.
between acquisition of a theory of mind and the capac- Hughes, C. (1998). Executive function in preschool-
ity to hold in mind. Journal of Experimental Child ers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability.
Psychology, 68(1), 70–83. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16(2),
Granvald, V., & Marciszko, C. (2016). Relations between 233–253.
key executive functions and aggression in childhood. Huttenlocher, P. R., & Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional
Child Neuropsychology, 22(5), 537–555. differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral
Graziano, P. A., Reid, A., Slavec, J., Paneto, A., cortex. Journal of comparative Neurology, 387(2),
McNamara, J. P., & Geffken, G. R. (2015). ADHD 167–178.
170 Y. Ziv et al.

Justice, E. M. (1985). Categorization as a preferred mem- working memory and executive control processes in
ory strategy: Developmental changes during elemen- adolescents. Child Development, 76(3), 697–712.
tary school. Developmental Psychology, 21(6), 1105. Luengo Kanacri, B. P., Pastorelli, C., Eisenberg, N.,
Karbach, J., & Kray, J. (2009). How useful is executive Zuffianò, A., & Caprara, G. V. (2013). The devel-
control training? Age differences in near and far trans- opment of prosociality from adolescence to early
fer of task-switching training. Developmental Science, adulthood: The role of effortful control. Journal of
12(6), 978–990. Personality, 81(3), 302–312.
Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Martel, M. M. (2009). Research review: A new per-
Gustafsson, P., Dahlström, K., … Westerberg, H. spective on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:
(2005). Computerized training of working memory Emotion dysregulation and trait models. Journal of
in children with ADHD-a randomized, controlled Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(9), 1042–1051.
trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Martel, M. M. (2016). Dispositional trait types of ADHD
Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(2), 177–186. in young children. Journal of Attention Disorders,
Kochanska, G., Coy, K. C., & Murray, K. T. (2001). The 20(1), 43–52.
development of self-regulation in the first four years of Martel, M. M., Gremillion, M. L., & Roberts, B. (2012).
life. Child Development, 72(4), 1091–1111. Temperament and common disruptive behavior
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harlan, E. T. (2000). problems in preschool. Personality and Individual
Effortful control in early childhood: Continuity and Differences, 53(7), 874–879.
change, antecedents, and implications for social devel- Martel, M. M., & Nigg, J. T. (2006). Child ADHD and
opment. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 220. personality/temperament traits of reactive and effort-
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. L., ful control, resiliency, and emotionality. Journal of
& Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhibitory control in Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(11), 1175–1183.
young children and its role in emerging internaliza- Martel, M. M., Nigg, J. T., & Von Eye, A. (2009). How do
tion. Child Development, 67, 490–507. trait dimensions map onto ADHD symptom domains?
Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(3),
A developmental perspective. Developmental 337–348.
Psychology, 18(2), 199–214. McClelland, M. M., & Cameron, C. E. (2012). Self-­
Lakes, K. D., & Hoyt, W. T. (2004). Promoting self-­ regulation in early childhood: Improving conceptual
regulation through school-based martial arts training. clarity and developing ecologically valid measures.
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 25(3), Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 136–142.
283–302. McClelland, M. M., Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M.,
Lemery, K. S., Essex, M. J., & Smider, N. A. (2002). Farris, C. L., Jewkes, A. M., & Morrison, F. J. (2007).
Revealing the relation between temperament and Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’
behavior problem symptoms by eliminating measure- literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental
ment confounding: Expert ratings and factor analyses. Psychology, 43(4), 947.
Child Development, 73(3), 867–882. McRae, K., Gross, J. J., Weber, J., Robertson, E. R.,
Lengua, L. J., West, S. G., & Sandler, I. N. (1998). Sokol-Hessner, P., Ray, R. D., … Ochsner, K. N.
Temperament as a predictor of symptomatology in (2012). The development of emotion regulation:
children: Addressing contamination of measures. An fMRI study of cognitive reappraisal in children,
Child Development, 69(1), 164–181. adolescents and young adults. Social Cognitive and
Liew, J. (2012). Effortful control, executive func- Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 11–22.
tions, and education: Bringing self-regulatory and Mercer, J. (2011). Martial arts research: Weak evidence.
social-­emotional competencies to the table. Child Science, 334(6054), 310–311.
Development Perspectives, 6(2), 105–111. Miller, J., Schäffer, R., & Hackley, S. A. (1991). Effects
Liew, J., McTigue, E. M., Barrois, L., & Hughes, J. N. of preliminary information in a go versus no-go task.
(2008). Adaptive and effortful control and academic Acta Psychologica, 76(3), 241–292.
self-efficacy beliefs on achievement: A longitudinal Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Raskoff Zeiss, A. (1972).
study of 1st through 3rd graders. Early Childhood Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of
Research Quarterly, 23(4), 515–526. gratification. Journal of Personality and Social
Liu, Q., Zhu, X., Ziegler, A., & Shi, J. (2015). The effects Psychology, 21(2), 204.
of inhibitory control training for preschoolers on rea- Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989).
soning ability and neural activity. Scientific Reports, Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244(4907),
5, 14200. 933–938.
Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J.,
action: A users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. In Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000).
D. Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory pro- The unity and diversity of executive functions and their
cesses in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189– contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent
239). San Diego: Academic Press. variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.
Luciana, M., Conklin, H. M., Hooper, C. J., & Monette, S., Bigras, M., & Guay, M. C. (2015).
Yarger, R. S. (2005). The development of nonverbal Executive functions in kindergarteners with high
Self-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective 171

levels of ­ disruptive behaviours. British Journal of ferences in infant fixation duration relate to attention
Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 446–463. and behavioral control in childhood. Psychological
Moran, L. R., Lengua, L. J., & Zalewski, M. (2013). The Science, 25, 1371–1379.
interaction between negative emotionality and effort- Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive func-
ful control in early social-emotional development. tions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of
Social Development, 22(2), 340–362. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87.
Muhtadie, L., Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., & Wang, Y. Pickering, S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Working mem-
(2013). Predicting internalizing problems in Chinese ory test battery for children (WMTB-C). London:
children: The unique and interactive effects of par- Psychological Corporation.
enting and child temperament. Development and Pintrich, P. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-­
Psychopathology, 25(03), 653–667. regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich,
Muris, P. (2006). Unique and interactive effects of neu- & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation
roticism and effortful control on psychopathological (pp. 451–502). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
symptoms in non-clinical adolescents. Personality and Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (1998). Attention,
Individual Differences, 40(7), 1409–1419. self–regulation and consciousness. Philosophical
Muris, P., Meesters, C., & Blijlevens, P. (2007). Self-­ Transactions of the Royal Society of London B:
reported reactive and regulative temperament in early Biological Sciences, 353(1377), 1915–1927.
adolescence: Relations to internalizing and externaliz- Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2000). Developing
ing problem behavior and “big three” personality fac- mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and
tors. Journal of Adolescence, 30(6), 1035–1049. Psychopathology, 12(3), 427–441.
Murray, K. T., & Kochanska, G. (2002). Effortful con- Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006).
trol: Factor structure and relation to externalizing and Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler tem-
internalizing behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child perament: The early childhood behavior question-
Psychology, 30(5), 503–514. naire. Infant Behavior and Development, 29(3),
Neuhaus, E., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2013). Impulsivity and 386–401.
vulnerability to psychopathology. In T. P. Beauchaine Putnam, S. P., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Development
& S. P. Hinshaw (Eds.), Child and adolescent psy- of short and very short forms of the Children’s behav-
chopathology (2nd ed., pp. 197–226). Hoboken, NJ: ior questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment,
Wiley. 87(1), 102–112.
Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in devel- Raffaelli, M., Crockett, L. J., & Shen, Y. L. (2005).
opmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and Developmental stability and change in self-­regulation
personality psychology and a working inhibition tax- from childhood to adolescence. The Journal of Genetic
onomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 220. Psychology, 166(1), 54–76.
Nigg, J. T., & Casey, B. J. (2005). An integrative theory Razza, R. A., Bergen-Cico, D., & Raymond, K. (2015).
of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder based on the Enhancing preschoolers’ self-regulation via mind-
cognitive and affective neurosciences. Development ful yoga. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24(2),
and Psychopathology, 17(03), 785–806. 372–385.
Nigg, J. T., Goldsmith, H. H., & Sachek, J. (2004). Reed, M. A., Pien, D. L., & Rothbart, M. K. (1984).
Temperament and attention deficit hyperactivity dis- Inhibitory self-control in preschool children. Merrill-­
order: The development of a multiple pathway model. Palmer Quarterly, 30(2), 131–147.
Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Riggs, N. R., Greenberg, M. T., Kusché, C. A., &
33(1), 42–53. Pentz, M. A. (2006). The mediational role of neu-
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & rocognition in the behavioral outcomes of a social-
Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. emotional prevention program in elementary school
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(5), 400–424. students: Effects of the PATHS curriculum. Prevention
Oldehinkel, A. J., Hartman, C. A., Ferdinand, R. F., Science, 7(1), 91–102.
Verhulst, F. C., & Ormel, J. (2007). Effortful control Roberts, B. W., Jackson, J. J., Fayard, J. V.,
as modifier of the association between negative emo- Edmonds, G., & Meints, J. (2009). Conscientiousness.
tionality and adolescents’ mental health problems. In M. R. Leary & R. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of indi-
Development and Psychopathology, 19(02), 523–539. vidual differences in social behavior (pp. 369–381).
Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., Kerr, D. C., Lopez, N. L., & New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Wellman, H. M. (2005). Developmental foundations of Romine, C. B., & Reynolds, C. R. (2005). A model of
externalizing problems in young children: The role of the development of frontal lobe functioning: Findings
effortful control. Development and Psychopathology, from a meta-analysis. Applied Neuropsychology,
17(01), 25–45. 12(4), 190–201.
Panfile, T. M., & Laible, D. J. (2012). Attachment security Rothbart, M. K. (1981). Measurement of temperament in
and child’s empathy: The mediating role of emotion infancy. Child Development, 52, 569–578.
regulation. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 58(1), 1–21. Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Hershey, K. L. (1994).
Papageorgiou, K. A., Smith, T. J., Wu, R., Johnson, M. H., Temperament and social behavior in childhood.
Kirkham, N. Z., & Ronald, A. (2014). Individual dif- Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 40, 21–39.
172 Y. Ziv et al.

Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., Hershey, K. L., & Fisher, Simonds, J., & Rothbart, M. K. (2004). The Temperament
P. (2001). Investigations of temperament at three to in Middle Childhood Questionnaire (TMCQ): A com-
seven years: The Children’s behavior questionnaire. puterized self-report instrument for ages 7–10. Poster
Child Development, 72(5), 1394–1408. Session Presented at the Occasional Temperament
Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. Conference. Athens, GA.
In W. Damon, R. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Gaertner, B., Popp, T.,
Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emo- Smith, C. L., Kupfer, A., … Hofer, C. (2007). Relations
tional, and personality development (pp. 99–166). of maternal socialization and toddlers’ effortful con-
New York: Wiley. trol to children’s adjustment and social competence.
Rothbart, M. K., & Rueda, M. R. (2005). The develop- Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 1170.
ment of effortful control. In U. Mayr, E. Awh, & Sportel, B. E., Nauta, M. H., de Hullu, E., &
S. Keele (Eds.), Developing individuality in the de Jong, P. J. (2013). Predicting internalizing symp-
human brain: A tribute to Michael I. Posner (pp. 167– toms over a two year period by BIS, FFFS and atten-
188). Washington, DC: American Psychological tional control. Personality and Individual Differences,
Association. 54(2), 236–240.
Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., Rueda, M. R., & Sportel, B. E., Nauta, M. H., de Hullu, E., de Jong, P. J.,
Posner, M. I. (2011). Developing mechanisms of & Hartman, C. A. (2011). Behavioral inhibition and
self-­
regulation in early life. Emotion Review, 3(2), attentional control in adolescents: Robust relation-
207–213. ships with anxiety and depression. Journal of Child
Rothbart, M. K., Ziaie, H., & O’boyle, C. G. (1992). Self-­ and Family Studies, 20(2), 149–156.
regulation and emotion in infancy. New Directions for Sroufe, L. A. (1995). Emotional development: The organi-
Child and Adolescent Development, 1992(55), 7–23. zation of emotional life in the early years. New York:
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Cambridge University Press.
Saccomanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, Strayhorn, J. M., & Strayhorn, J. C. (2011). Martial arts
maturation, and genetic influences on the development research: Prudent skepticism. Science, 334(6054),
of executive attention. Proceedings of the National 310–310.
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, Strommen, E. A. (1973). Verbal self-regulation in a
102(41), 14931–14936. Children’s game: Impulsive errors on “Simon says”.
Ruff, H. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Attention in early Child Development, 44, 849–853.
development: Themes and variations. New York: Sulik, M. J., Blair, C., Mills-Koonce, R., Berry, D.,
Oxford University Press. & Greenberg, M. (2015). Early parenting and the
Saarni, C. (1984). An observational study of children's development of externalizing behavior problems:
attempts to monitor their expressive behavior. Child Longitudinal mediation through children’s executive
Development, 55, 1504–1513. function. Child Development, 86(5), 1588–1603.
Schoemaker, K., Mulder, H., Deković, M., & Suurland, J., Heijden, K. B., Huijbregts, S. C.,
Matthys, W. (2013). Executive functions in preschool Smaling, H. J., Sonneville, L. M., Van Goozen, S. H.,
children with externalizing behavior problems: A & Swaab, H. (2016). Parental perceptions of aggres-
meta-­analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, sive behavior in preschoolers: Inhibitory control
41(3), 457–471. moderates the association with negative emotionality.
Shiner, R., & Caspi, A. (2003). Personality differences in Child Development, 87(1), 256–269.
childhood and adolescence: Measurement, develop- Thompson-Schill, S. L., Ramscar, M., &
ment, and consequences. Journal of Child Psychology Chrysikou, E. G. (2009). Cognition without control
and Psychiatry, 44(1), 2–32. when a little frontal lobe goes a long way. Current
Shipstead, Z., Hicks, K. L., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Directions in Psychological Science, 18(5), 259–263.
Cogmed working memory training: Does the evidence Thorell, L. B., Lindqvist, S., Bergman Nutley, S.,
support the claims? Journal of Applied Research in Bohlin, G., & Klingberg, T. (2009). Training and
Memory and Cognition, 1(3), 185–193. transfer effects of executive functions in preschool
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is children. Developmental Science, 12(1), 106–113.
working memory training effective? Psychological Tomporowski, P. D., Davis, C. L., Miller, P. H., &
Bulletin, 138(4), 628. Naglieri, J. A. (2008). Exercise and children’s intel-
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (2000). Committee on ligence, cognition, and academic achievement.
integrating the science of early childhood development, Educational Psychology Review, 20(2), 111–131.
board on children, youth, and families. From neurons Ullsperger, J. M., Nigg, J. T., & Nikolas, M. A. (2016).
to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood pro- Does child temperament play a role in the association
grams. Washington, DC: National Academy Press. between parenting practices and child attention defi-
Sibley, B. A., & Etnier, J. L. (2003). The relationship cit/hyperactivity disorder? Journal of Abnormal Child
between physical activity and cognition in children: Psychology, 44(1), 167–178.
A meta-analysis. Pediatric Exercise Science, 15(3), Ursache, A., Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2012). The promotion
243–256. of self-regulation as a means of enhancing school readi-
Self-Regulation in Childhood: A Developmental Perspective 173

ness and early achievement in children at risk for school Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V.,
failure. Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 122–128. & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of the executive
Valiente, C., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Haugen, R. G., function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis-
Thompson, M. S., & Kupfer, A. (2013). Effortful con- order: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry,
trol and impulsivity as concurrent and longitudinal 57(11), 1336–1346.
predictors of academic achievement. The Journal of Williams, B. R., Ponesse, J. S., Schachar, R. J.,
Early Adolescence, 33(7), 946–972. Logan, G. D., & Tannock, R. (1999). Development of
Valiente, C., Lemery-Chalfant, K., Swanson, J., & inhibitory control across the life span. Developmental
Reiser, M. (2008). Prediction of children's academic Psychology, 35(1), 205.
competence from their effortful control, relationships, Winne, P. H., & Hadwin, A. F. (1998). Studying as self-­
and classroom participation. Journal of Educational regulated learning. Metacognition in Educational
Psychology, 100(1), 67. Theory and Practice, 93, 27–30.
Verlinden, M., Veenstra, R., Ghassabian, A., Woltering, S., Lishak, V., Hodgson, N., Granic, I., &
Jansen, P. W., Hofman, A., Jaddoe, V. W., … Tiemeier, Zelazo, P. D. (2016). Executive function in chil-
H. (2014). Executive functioning and non-verbal intel- dren with externalizing and comorbid internalizing
ligence as predictors of bullying in early elementary behavior problems. Journal of Child Psychology and
school. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42(6), Psychiatry, 57(1), 30–38.
953–966. Xu, Y., Farver, J. A. M., & Zhang, Z. (2009). Temperament,
Vervoort, L., Wolters, L. H., Hogendoorn, S. M., harsh and indulgent parenting, and Chinese children’s
Prins, P. J., De Haan, E., Boer, F., & Hartman, C. A. proactive and reactive aggression. Child Development,
(2011). Temperament, attentional processes, and anxi- 80(1), 244–258.
ety: Diverging links between adolescents with and Xu, F., Han, Y., Sabbagh, M. A., Wang, T., Ren, X., &
without anxiety disorders? Journal of Clinical Child Li, C. (2013). Developmental differences in the struc-
& Adolescent Psychology, 40(1), 144–155. ture of executive function in middle childhood and
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Interaction between learning adolescence. PloS One, 8(10), e77770.
and development. Readings on the Development of Yap, M. B., Allen, N. B., & Sheeber, L. (2007). Using
Children, 23(3), 34–41. an emotion regulation framework to understand the
Waters, G. S., & Caplan, D. (2003). The reliability and sta- role of temperament and family processes in risk
bility of verbal working memory measures. Behavior for adolescent depressive disorders. Clinical Child
Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35(4), and Family Psychology Review, 10(2), 180–196.
550–564. Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The dimensional change card sort
Weinberg, M. K., Tronick, E. Z., Cohn, J. F., & (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in
Olson, K. L. (1999). Gender differences in emotional children. Nature Protocols, 1(1), 297.
expressivity and self-regulation during early infancy. Zhou, Q., Chen, S. H., & Main, A. (2012). Commonalities
Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 175. and differences in the research on children’s effort-
Wender, P. (1995). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder ful control and executive function: A call for an inte-
in adults. New York: Oxford University Press. grated model of self-regulation. Child Development
Whitebread, D., & Basilio, M. (2012). The emergence Perspectives, 6(2), 112–121.
and early development of self-regulation in young Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A
children. Profesorado: Journal of Curriculum and social cognitive perspective. In M. Boekaerts, P. R.
Teacher Education, Monograph Issue: Learn to Learn. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-­
Teaching and Evaluation of Self-Regulated Learning, regulation (pp. 13–39). San Diego, CA: Academic
16(1), 15–34. Press.
Social and Emotional Learning:
Recent Research and Practical
Strategies for Promoting
Children’s Social and Emotional
Competence in Schools

Eva Oberle and Kimberly A. Schonert-Reichl

Parents, educators, and society at large have long maintain positive relationships with peers and
agreed that, by the time young people graduate adults, and make responsible and healthy deci-
from high school, they should be independent, sions (CASEL, 2013; Greenberg et al., 2003;
socially skilled, and well-rounded citizens who Weissberg, Payton, O’Brien, & Munro, 2007).
are ready to responsibly navigate their personal Much progress has been made in the past two
and professional pathways into adulthood decades. A steady growth of research has been
(Greenberg et al., 2003). There has been wide conducted, examining central questions such as
agreement that schools play a central role in fos- “Are social and emotional competencies mallea-
tering these skills, in addition to their mandate ble and can they be taught by regular teachers in
for teaching academic competencies (Zins & typical classrooms?,” “Does teaching social and
Elias, 2006). However, until the turn of the cen- emotional competencies influence student devel-
tury, instruction in social-emotional skills was opment in other domains, such as their academic
generally missing in kindergarten to 12th grade achievement?,” and “How can instruction in
educational curricula, and schools were not social-emotional competence be incorporated
required to address children’s social-emotional into the classroom effectively, consistently, and
development explicitly and systematically. sustainably?” The research conducted in the past
Indeed, social and emotional learning (SEL) has two decades has resulted in compelling and con-
long been considered the “missing piece” in edu- sistent empirical evidence showing that, indeed,
cation (Elias, 1997; Schonert-Reichel & Hymel, social-emotional competencies can be taught and
2007). SEL encompasses processes through that teaching those competencies leads to posi-
which individuals acquire and effectively apply tive and significant improvements for other
the knowledge, attitudes, and skills necessary to important outcomes, including student behavior,
understand and manage their own emotions, health and well-being, and academic achieve-
establish and achieve positive goals, develop and ment (e.g., Domitrovich, Cortes, & Greenberg,
2007; Durlak, Weissberg, Dymnicki, Taylor, &
Schellinger, 2011; Payton et al., 2008; Sklad,
E. Oberle (*) Diekstra, Ritter, Ben, & Gravesteijn, 2012; Weare
Faculty of Medicine, University of British Columbia, & Nind, 2011; Zins, Bloodworth, Weissberg, &
Vancouver, BC, Canada Walberg, 2004a, 2004b).
e-mail: eva.oberle@ubc.ca Despite these advances and the widespread rec-
K.A. Schonert-Reichl ognition that SEL is a key contributor to positive
Faculty of Education, University of British Columbia, child and adolescent development, many educators
Vancouver, BC, Canada

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 175


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_11
176 E. Oberle and K.A. Schonert-Reichl

are still experiencing barriers for incorporating and skills necessary for identifying and achieving
SEL into schools and classrooms (Oberle, positive goals, identifying, understanding, and
Domitrovich, Meyers, & Weissberg, 2016). regulating emotions, showing empathy for
Barriers include lack of resources (e.g., financial others, initiating and maintaining positive
budget, time), little administrative support, and relationships, and making responsible decisions
lack of training and guidance in implementing SEL (Greenberg et al., 2003; Osher et al., 2008;
systematically. Because SEL is considered to be Payton et al., 2000; Zins et al., 2004a, 2004b).
the foundation of students’ well-­being and success SEL can be implemented within the family,
in school and life, researchers, educators, and pol- school, and community context. For the purpose
icy makers need to collectively advance the agenda of this chapter, we discuss SEL in schools.
of bringing SEL to all children by incorporating it The term SEL was first coined in 1994, when
consistently and effectively into school curricula the Collaborative for Academic, Social, and
(Jones & Bouffard, 2012; Osher, Sprague, Emotional Learning (CASEL; www.casel.org)
Weissberg, Keenan, & Zins, 2008). was founded as an international organization
The goal of this chapter is to present the current with the mission to establish evidence-based SEL
state of theory, research, and practices of SEL as an essential part of preschool through high
instruction in schools. The chapter has three main school education. In 1995, Daniel Goleman first
parts. First, we define SEL and introduce core described the crucial role of emotions in deter-
social-emotional competencies, provide an over- mining crucial educational and life outcomes in
view of the history of SEL in schools, and review his best-selling book Emotional Intelligence
research evidence of the impact of SEL on devel- (Goleman, 1995). The book was responsible for
opmental outcomes in children. Second, we dis- popularizing the topic of emotions and its contri-
cuss ways in which SEL can be taught successfully butions to thoughts, behaviors, and success in
in schools. We describe best practices that have life. In 1997, to catalyze the field of SEL, a group
been established through research, present estab- of scholars at CASEL published a book
lished criteria of high-quality SEL programs, and Promoting Social and Emotional Learning:
discuss different strategies of incorporating SEL Guidelines for Educators and delineated a list of
into the curriculum. Further, we provide explicit 37 guidelines to inform educational practices to
examples for evidence-based SEL programs at dif- support educators in implementing SEL in their
ferent grade levels (i.e., preschool through high classrooms (Elias et al., 1997). The guidelines for
school). Third, we discuss the importance of estab- educators were a groundbreaking contribution
lishing sustainable SEL in schools. We point out that influenced SEL research and practice for
the need for a systemic approach for SEL imple- years to come. Up to this point, the influential
mentation and for taking into account teachers’ role of children’s and adolescents’ feelings and
social and emotional competence and incorporat- understanding, thereof, emotional experiences
ing SEL instruction into both in-service and pre- and regulation, and well-being in relation to aca-
service teacher education. We conclude with demic learning had remained widely unrecog-
future directions in the field of SEL in schools and nized (Zins et al., 1998). Considering these events
highlight important next steps that need to be taken in combination, the field of school-based SEL
on a practical and a research level. was born.
What are the social-emotional competencies
that comprise SEL? In defining the outcomes that
 wo Decades of SEL in Schools:
T school-based SEL aims to achieve, CASEL has
What Is It and Why Is It Important? identified five core intra- and interpersonal and
cognitive competencies that are interrelated and
SEL is the process of providing all children and reflect the cognitive, affective, and behavioral
adolescents with the opportunities to learn, dimensions of SEL (CASEL, 2013; Elias et al.,
acquire, and practice the knowledge, attitudes, 1997; Payton et al., 2000):
SEL in Schools 177

1. Self-awareness involves the ability to identify tive outcomes in the school context in particular.
and recognize one’s own emotions, thoughts, SEL competencies facilitate effective communi-
and their influences on behavior. It includes cation with peers and teachers, help setting and
recognizing one’s own strength and chal- achieving academic goals, increase motivation to
lenges and being aware of one’s own goals learn, and increase commitment to school, all of
and values. High levels of self-awareness what are important aspects of thriving and suc-
require recognizing how thoughts, feelings, cess in the school context.
and actions are interconnected. A brief history of SEL in schools. After
2. Self-management entails the ability to regulate growing curiosity in SEL in the 1990s, interest in
one’s emotions, thoughts, and behaviors effec- school-based SEL practices exploded in the
tively, including stress management, impulse beginning of the twenty-first century (Humphrey,
control, motivating oneself, and working 2013). To date, a multitude of programs and
toward achieving personal and academic strategies designed to teach and foster SEL—
goals. It also contains self-management in classroom-­ based, school-wide, and through
social interactions. school-community partnerships—have been devel-
3. Social awareness is the ability to take the oped, implemented, and reviewed (Weissberg,
perspectives of others—including those Durlak, Domitrovich, & Gullotta, 2015). Further,
who come from a different background and many SEL interventions have been comprehen-
culture—to empathize with others, to under- sively examined and analyzed in several reviews
stand social and ethical norms, and to rec- and meta-analytic studies, resulting in strong and
ognize resources and supports in family, stable evidence that well-­implemented, high-quality
school, and community. SEL programs lead to immediate and long-term
4. Relationship skills provide children with the positive social, emotional, behavioral, and aca-
tools to form and maintain positive and demic outcomes in children (Durlak et al., 2011;
healthy relationships, communicate clearly, Sklad et al., 2012; Taylor, Oberle, Durlak, &
listen actively, cooperate, negotiate construc- Weissberg, 2017; Weare & Nind, 2011).
tively during conflict, solve problems with The overwhelming empirical support for SEL
others effectively, and offer and seek help programs has had a catalyzing effect on the
when needed. implementation of such programs. The science of
5. Responsible decision-making skills equip school-based SEL has been further advanced,
children with the ability to make constructive programs have been optimized (e.g., implemen-
and respectful choices about their own behav- tation practices, program characteristics, inter-
ior and social interactions, taking into account vention support), and educational guidelines and
safety concerns, ethical standards, social and policies that mandate, support, and prioritize
behavioral norms, consequences, and the SEL in schools have emerged (Mart, Weissberg,
well-being of self and others. & Kendziora, 2015; Oberle et al., 2016; Wright,
Lamont, Wandersman, Osher, & Gordon, 2015).
Children and adolescents who are proficient in Schools are particularly important contexts for
those core SEL competencies are able to inte- the promotion of SEL. American children spend
grate feeling, thinking, and behaving to master an average of 900–1000 h in school every year,
important tasks in school and life (Zins et al., depending on their grade level (Hull & Newport,
2004a, 2004b). Those children are competent in 2011). Canadian figures mirror these numbers
recognizing and managing their emotions, form- (OECD, 2012). This estimate does not include
ing healthy relationships with peers and adults, free time spent in school, such as lunch break,
setting realistic and positive goals, meeting per- recess, and time in after-school programs. The
sonal and social needs, and making responsible school is undoubtedly a primary ecological con-
and ethical decisions (Elias et al., 1997). text for children and adolescents to learn, grow,
Proficiency in core SEL skills is critical for posi- and develop (Bronfenbrenner & Morris, 2006),
178 E. Oberle and K.A. Schonert-Reichl

and schools are faced with numerous opportuni- Yet, it is important to acknowledge the success
ties as well as the responsibility to help children that scholars and advocates for the school-based
and adolescents to grow socially, emotionally, promotion of SEL have obtained over the past
and academically. several years. The shift from a complete absence
In addition to advances in SEL research, of formal SEL instruction in schools to an impres-
essential steps have been taken to ensure that sive accumulation of evidence, resources (e.g.,
SEL reaches the individuals and contexts for programs, guidelines), and emerging policies that
which it was developed: students, teachers, class- mandate SEL programs and practices in schools
rooms, and schools. CASEL has published two has grown considerably within less than two
guides that list and review (1) SEL programs for decades (Weissberg, Durlak, Domitrovich, &
preschools and elementary schools (CASEL, Gullotta, 2015). Even though the goal of making
2013) and (2) SEL programs for middle schools SEL in schools a national priority for all students
and high schools (CASEL, 2015b). Each guide has yet to be reached, the successes accomplished
summarizes characteristics of each program (e.g., to date are important milestone of which schol-
age group for which the program was developed, ars, educators, policy makers, families, and stu-
duration, SEL skills targeted) and reviews empir- dents must be cognizant because they provide a
ical evidence supporting program effectiveness. stepping stone for further advances in school-­
Educators, researchers, and other interested indi- based SEL.
viduals can access the CASEL guides freely SEL and positive student outcomes.
through CASEL’s website (www.casel.org). The Several reviews have documented the effective-
guides have proven to be a critical and widely ness of SEL programs. Durlak et al. (2011) have
used practical resource for the field. The guides completed the to-date largest meta-analytic
assist educators in selecting evidence-based SEL review of 213 school-based universal SEL pro-
programs that align with their own and students’ grams involving 270,034 students in kindergar-
needs and interests. ten through high school, published from 1970 to
Despite the growing interest in SEL, its strong 2007. The authors found that, compared to stu-
empirical support, and the availability of dents who did not receive an SEL program, stu-
evidence-­based programs, SEL still lags behind dents who participated in SEL programs
in its importance relative to instruction in read- demonstrated significantly improved social-­
ing, writing, and numeracy in today’s schools emotional skills and attitudes, increased proso-
(Weissberg & Cascarino, 2013). In times of man- cial and decreased in antisocial behaviors, and
dated high-stakes academic testing and academic an 11-percentile-point gain in scores on stan-
achievement pressure on students, teachers, and dardized academic achievement tests. Two mod-
schools, SEL often does not receive the necessary erating variables that predicted the positive
amount of time it requires to be firmly integrated change in student outcomes were the degree to
into day-to-day education. Instead of a system- which the SEL program was implemented with
atic approach to SEL programming that ensures fidelity and the quality of the SEL program
consistent and continuous implementation, SEL (Durlak & DuPre, 2008).
is often incorporated via a piecemeal approach in Sklad et al. (2012), in another meta-analysis,
which individual programs are chosen and imple- reviewed 75 universal school-based SEL
mented sporadically in selected classrooms programs that were evaluated through experi-
­
(Jones & Bouffard, 2012). Even though many mental or quasi-experimental research and pub-
teachers are eager to incorporate SEL in their lished in the literature between 1995 and 2008.
educational practice, tight budgets and the lack of The authors found that participation in an SEL
an overarching supportive system that facilitates program predicted significant improvements in
teaching SEL competencies often present a bar- students’ social-emotional skills, prosocial
rier difficult to cross (Bridgeland, Bruce, & behaviors, and academic achievement, and
Hariharan, 2013). significant reductions in antisocial behaviors,
SEL in Schools 179

mental health problems, and mental disorders. Taylor et al. (2017) also reported positive
Effects were strongest in the short term (i.e., up effects of SEL programs on additional impor-
to 6 months after program completion), and effect tant developmental outcomes collected up to
sizes were substantially weaker at follow-up (i.e., 18 years post-intervention, which were reported
6 months and longer since program completion), in a small subsample of studies and therefore
but they remained positive. could not be examined via a meta-analysis.
A review of previously conducted meta-­ Most notably, those students who had been
analytic and narrative reviews on SEL program- exposed to an SEL program, compared to those
ming was conducted by Weare and Nind (2011). who had not, were more likely to graduate from
The authors included 46 reviews in their study, high school and obtain a college degree (e.g.,
involving hundreds of individual program evalu- Hawkins, Kosterman, Catalano, Hill, & Abbott,
ations and more than half a million students. 2008), have improved sexual health (e.g., Hill
Based on their findings, the authors concluded et al., 2014), were less likely to have been
that school-based universal promotion programs arrested or have encounters with the justice sys-
produced positive impacts on mental health pro- tem (Cook & Hirschfield, 2008), and be diag-
motion, social-emotional skills, and academic nosed with a clinical disorder (Riggs & Pentz,
achievement immediately following interven- 2009) in adulthood. These findings are impres-
tions. The need for the study of long-term effects sive, and they provide evidence that participat-
of such interventions was raised as an important ing students and society at large can both profit
next step by the authors. from the effects achieved by SEL programs.
The latest systematic review was conducted They point at the significant economic benefits
to specifically fill the research gap of missing in society that can be achieved through school-
evidence on the long-term effectiveness of SEL based SEL programs (e.g., through reduced
programming in enhancing positive student criminal justice expenses and better employ-
outcomes (Taylor et al., 2017). Investigating ment and income for individuals with high
the degree to which positive program effects school and college degrees).
are sustained over time is a critical question for Financial benefits of SEL programs are par-
cost-­benefit analysis of SEL programs and can ticularly important to consider. Many policy
be a decisive piece of information when advo- makers and administrators shy away from
cating for resources to be allocated for SEL in implementing SEL programs because of the
school budgets. Taylor and colleagues reviewed initial costs that emerge. However, a recent
a total of 82 school-based, universal SEL pro- study that has examined the economic value of
grams involving 97,406 kindergartens to high six widely used and evidence-based SEL inter-
school students that had been published by ventions has determined that, for every dollar
2014. The students involved in the studies com- invested, there was a return of 11 dollars
prised an ethnically and sociodemographically (Belfield et al., 2015). Thus the benefits of SEL
diverse sample in urban and rural settings. Of far exceed the costs, providing further support
the 82 studies, 38 were conducted in countries that school-based SEL programming is well
outside of the United States. Results showed worth the time and the investment for individu-
that, compared to controls, students who had als and for society.
received an SEL intervention continued to show SEL in times of heightened risk. Social-­
increases in social-emotional skills, positive emotional development is considered a founda-
behaviors, and academic achievement and tion of positive development, health, and success
decreases in conduct problems, emotional dis- for all children. Hence, many SEL programs are
tress, and drug use up to almost 4 years after universal and are designed to benefit all children.
program completion. Effect sizes were moder- For young people at risk, SEL can become a par-
ate; the strongest effects were found for aca- ticularly important resource and a protective fac-
demic achievement. tor that can also promote resilience and prevent
180 E. Oberle and K.A. Schonert-Reichl

negative developmental outcomes (Elias &  inking SEL to Academic


L
Haynes, 2008; Greenberg et al., 2003; Payton Achievement in School
et al., 2000). More than ever, today’s students are
faced with severe challenges that call for As school districts are increasingly held account-
approaches to address their social-emotional able for students’ academic achievement, many
well-being and competence in schools (Centers educators are concerned that allocating time
for Disease Control and Prevention, 2013). For toward SEL would be at the sacrifice of teaching
example, the latest Youth Risk Behavior Survey core academic skills. Whereas academic and
(Kann et al., 2014) that surveyed 10- to 17-year-­ social-emotional skills have traditionally been
old youth in the United States reported that up to considered separate and distinct domains in
30% of young people are experiencing serious development, research conducted over the past
social-emotional and mental health problems. decade has shown that, in fact, social-emotional
Specifically, in 2013, 30% of students reported skills are interrelated with academic skills and,
feeling sad and hopeless every day during the moreover, explicitly foster academic learning and
30 days before the survey; 17% had seriously success (Caprara, Barbaranelli, Pastorelli,
considered attempting suicide 12 months before Bandura, & Zimbardo, 2000; Durlak et al., 2011;
the survey; 20% had been bullied on school prop- Hawkins et al., 2008; Izard et al., 2001; Oberle,
erty; 25% had been involved in at least one physi- Schonert-Reichl, Hertzman, & Zumbo, 2014).
cal fight; and 21% reported having consumed at The link between social-emotional and aca-
least five alcoholic drinks in a row on at least demic skills becomes evident considering that
1 day during the 30 days before the survey. any learning in the school context is inherently a
Canadian statistics on child and youth well-being social process (Vadeboncoeur & Collie, 2013;
are similarly concerning. One out of five children Zins et al., 2004a, 2004b). Learning occurs in
in Canada are facing social-emotional, mental, interactions with peers, teachers, and staff mem-
and behavioral health problems, which jeopar- bers and involves collaboration, negotiation, and
dize their positive development and success in cooperation across a wide spectrum of social sit-
schools, and rates are predicted to increase to uations. Students who are “fluent” in social and
30% by the year 2020 (Canadian Pediatric emotional competence tend to understand their
Society, 2009). Such alarmingly high numbers of own and others’ emotions, manage their emo-
problematic and risky behaviors and mental tions successfully even when facing stress, make
health concerns pose a significant risk for young responsible decisions, and negotiate challenging
people and emphasize the importance of SEL as situations effectively (Elias & Haynes, 2008;
an approach that promotes both well-being and Payton et al., 2000; Zins & Elias, 2006). Hence,
success in school. socially and emotionally competent children tend
Taken together, children and youth in our to be at ease in the school and classroom context
society today are faced with considerable chal- and can focus better on the academic tasks
lenges that can jeopardize their chances for suc- provided to them compared to children who
cess and positive development in the future. struggle socially and emotionally (Welsh, Parke,
SEL is a foundation for positive development Widaman, & Neil, 2001).
because it is an effective strategy to counteract Recent empirical evidence demonstrates that
those challenges, and it equips children with the children’s social and emotional skills forecast
tools and assets needed to make good, healthy, adult success. For example, a groundbreaking
and responsible decisions that navigate them study showed that children’s prosocial skills as
toward successful outcomes in life. In short, rated by classroom teachers in kindergarten were
SEL is significant for life success and can be positively related to high school and college
considered a crucial asset in the life of children graduation and stable and full-time employment
who are experiencing behavioral and mental and negatively related to the number of years that
health problems (Dryfoos, 2010). special education services were received and the
SEL in Schools 181

number of years that children repeated in school Characteristics of successful programs. In


(Jones, Greenberg, & Crowley, 2015). In addition their meta-analytic review, Durlak et al. (2011)
to these important long-term academic outcomes, found that most effective studies were character-
the authors found that early prosocial skills were ized with the acronym SAFE: (1) Does the pro-
linked to fewer mental health problems, less sub- gram contain sequenced activities that teach the
stance use, and less involvement with the crimi- targeted social-emotional skills in coordinated
nal justice system in early adulthood. and connected ways? An indicator for a
Overall, implementing high-quality SEL in sequenced program is—among others—the pres-
the classroom aids educators in establishing a ence of a program guide, which outlines activi-
positive classroom environment and provides ties that build on each other. (2) Does the program
students with core social-emotional skills that include active forms of learning? Active forms of
facilitate and drive their academic learning. learning involve participatory elements such as
Evidence-based SEL programming leads to a role-plays or active discussions engaging stu-
safe, well-managed, and caring learning envi- dents in active learning of SEL competencies. (3)
ronment with opportunities for rewards and Is the program’s main focus developing one or
positive behaviors. When becoming competent more social-emotional skills? A main focus on
in core SEL skills, students can manage their social-emotional skills can often be identified
emotions and relationships more effectively through specific program elements that are dedi-
and exhibit fewer negative and more positive cated to general social-emotional competence
behaviors. Students also grow more attached to development and time dedicated to learning spe-
school, and engagement and commitment to cific social-emotional skills. (4) Is the program
school increases. Together, these factors have a explicit about targeting social-emotional skills?
positive influence on academic learning and SEL is addressed explicitly when the program
increase students’ academic success (Zins states which of the specific social-emotional
et al., 2004a, 2004b). competencies are addressed through the interven-
tion. In the field of SEL implementation research,
SAFE has become an established acronym for
Putting SEL into Action in Schools high-quality intervention programs.
The importance of program characteristics has
When financial and structural support is in place also been discussed in the general field of pri-
for school-based SEL, educators and adminis- mary prevention science. Researchers have argued
trators are eventually faced with the decision as that successful prevention and promotion pro-
to which SEL program to implement in their grams are based on sound scientific theory and
school. Thorough background research is that their content, structure, and implementation
required before choosing a program. Effective are research-based (Bond & Hauf, 2004). Bond
SEL programs are those that have been evalu- and Hauf in fact argue that programs can fail if
ated rigorously and findings support the pro- they lack connection to a theoretical and research
gram as evidence-based. A wide range of base. Basing content of prevention and promotion
evidence-based SEL programs has been identi- efforts on research requires a careful examination
fied over the past two decades (CASEL, 2013, of empirical evidence available on the specific
2015b). Programs vary in which themes are intervention topic. In addition, theoretically sound
addressed (e.g., bullying, substance use, mind- pedagogical practices have to guide the activities
fulness), which of the five core social-­emotional and ultimately the structure of prevention and pro-
competencies are targeted, for which age groups motion programs. Successful programs also have
the program was designed, and program dura- to have a clear and attainable goal that has been
tion. Programs that have been found to be suc- agreed upon broadly by the program’s stakehold-
cessful, however, also share key characteristics. ers (Bond & Hauf, 2004; Haney & Durlak, 1998).
182 E. Oberle and K.A. Schonert-Reichl

Successful primary prevention and promotion Although the importance of implementation


programs have to be incorporated in the ecologi- fidelity is widely accepted (i.e., implementing the
cal levels in which children develop. Human program and its individual components fully and
development occurs in multiple contexts (e.g., as descried and intended in the curriculum),
school, classroom, community, family) and those adaptations of program implementation are fairly
settings reciprocally influence each other common in educational settings (Domitrovich &
(Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Bronfenbrenner & Greenberg, 2000). For example, teachers may
Morris, 2006). Hence, intervention programs choose to adapt implementation in accordance
should adopt a multisystem and multilevel per- with personal beliefs and attitudes, to match their
spective that attends to the influences and devel- teaching style or to address specific student inter-
opmental pathways in different contexts (Bond & ests and needs in their classroom. Some teachers
Hauf, 2004). This aspect of successful programs also shorten implementation due to time con-
is discussed in further detail in the end of this straints, competing projects, or financial restric-
chapter, when arguing for systemic approaches to tions. However altering implementation does not
achieve consistent, coherent, and sustainable necessarily improve program outcomes and it can
SEL in schools. Multilevel and multisystem compromise the intended program effectiveness.
implementation is arguably the most challenging Eight major components have been discussed
part of SEL in schools because it involves partici- in establishing implementation quality (Devaney
pation and collaboration of stakeholders in vari- et al., 2006; Durlak, 2016; Durlak & DuPre,
ous societal settings. However, if successful, 2008; Elias, Zins, Graczyk, & Weissberg, 2003).
multisystem and multilevel programming can Fidelity refers to the degree to which the major
also yield most benefits because it is facilitated program components are delivered as intended.
sustainably by an established infrastructure for Dosage indicates how much of the program is
programming. delivered (e.g., how many of the sessions out-
Choosing an SEL program that fulfills the lined in the curriculum were held during an inter-
characteristics of high-quality programming and vention, and how many of the activities were
that has been supported by research is a critical completed during each session?). The quality of
first step in successful school-based SEL. A fur- program delivery describes how competently a
ther essential step is understanding how the pro- program implementation is conducted. Training
gram is put into action in the classroom, ranging in delivering the program is key for high imple-
from teachers’ preparedness, buy-in, and motiva- mentation quality. Adaptation addresses the
tion to the degree to which implementation is car- question whether the program was altered or
ried out accurately and with fidelity (Devaney, adapted in any ways. Participant engagement
O’Brien, Resnick, Keister, & Weissberg, 2006). indicates to what degree attendees (i.e., students)
Implementation quality. Implementation engaged in the program and its activities. Program
refers to the ways a program is put into practice. differentiation takes into account the uniqueness
It draws a picture of how program delivery ought of the intervention compared to other programs.
to be and is an essential component of interven- Monitoring of the control condition considers the
tion effectiveness (Durlak, 2016). High-quality activities that took place in the control group
implementation of evidence-based SEL program- while the experimental group received the inter-
ming in schools is essential to achieve the spe- vention. This can reveal potential activities that
cific outcomes targeted through the SEL program. were carried out with the control group that over-
To achieve high-quality implementation, pro- lap or mirror intervention components. Last,
gram delivery needs to be facilitated through ­program reach indicates what portion of the eli-
established and theory-driven guidelines (i.e., a gible population actually participated in the
program curriculum). Implementation also needs intervention.
to be monitored throughout and support needs to The eight components of implementation can
be provided if necessary. both overlap and interact with each other (Durlak
SEL in Schools 183

& DuPre, 2008). For example, if the control group through implementing evidence-based SEL pro-
activities are not monitored, potential unintended grams that extend the regular classroom curricu-
overlap in activities with the intervention cannot lum and indirectly through effective adult
be identified and positive intervention effects modeling and infusing SEL into the existing cur-
might remain undetected. In addition, if dosage is riculum (Durlak et al., 2011; Jones & Bouffard,
low, overall implementation fidelity is affected 2012; Oberle et al., 2016). All three approaches
and the intended program outcomes might not be have strengths and they can also be used jointly
achieved. The importance of implementation has to complement each other. The advantage of
been supported empirically. In their meta-analy- implementing an evidence-based SEL program is
sis, Durlak et al. (2011) found that the positive that teachers obtain training for program imple-
effects of SEL interventions on academic gains, mentation and are provided with a curriculum
reductions in depression and anxiety, and reduc- that guides them through intervention activities
tions in conduct problems were approximately in a structured way. For many teachers, setting
twice as large when no problems with implemen- aside the time outlined in the SEL curriculum and
tation were reported compared to when the carrying out the individual activities guided by
authors indicated implementation problems. the SEL syllabus facilitate their planning in
Similarly, in a randomized controlled trial (RCT) teaching.
study evaluating the responsive classroom inter- Teaching SEL through embodiment of social-­
vention (Rimm-Kaufman et al., 2014), the authors emotional competencies in classroom interaction
initially found no significant difference in student with students, colleagues, and parents encour-
outcomes between the control group and those ages teachers to act as role models and to demon-
who had received the intervention. However, strate to children how social-emotional
when taking into account the level of intervention competencies can be incorporated in social inter-
implementation, students in the experimental actions effectively and consistently. This SEL
group showed significant increases in academic approach does not follow a specific classroom
achievement in both math and reading if imple- curriculum. Even though some teachers are com-
mentation was high. If no implementation data petent in modeling and using social-emotional
had been available, the researchers would have skills naturally, most teachers benefit from
erroneously concluded that the program did not explicit training that allows them to embody
lead to the hypothesized result of increasing aca- social-emotional skills in their classroom interac-
demic achievement in the classroom. tions and teaching style consistently and with
These findings underscore the importance of awareness. Ideally, such training is provided to
monitoring implementation in research evalua- educators in university teacher training programs
tion studies and encourage schools to support or through professional development opportuni-
high-quality implementation of SEL programs. ties (Schonert-Reichl, Hanson-Peterson, &
Ignoring implementation can come at a high cost Hymel, 2015).
of failing to achieve the desired outcomes and Many scholars have argued for infusing SEL
falsely concluding that a program is ineffective into the existing curriculum to enhance its sus-
when in fact effectiveness was jeopardized by tainability and break the perceived barrier that
poor-quality implementation (Durlak, 2016). As there is a lack of time for SEL due to the pres-
a consequence, a school’s or a teacher’s interest sures of the regular classroom curriculum.
and perceived value in a program may drop, and Integrating SEL into the existing curriculum can
the program might not be adopted for future be achieved in multiple ways, for example, by
implementation. Thus, current and future stu- drawing from literature in social studies and
dents who can benefit from SEL programs might English language studies that offers natural
not receive them. opportunities for discussing emotions, behaviors,
Integrated versus add-on approaches to and relationships (Brown, Jones, LaRusso, &
SEL in schools. SEL can be directly taught Aber, 2010; Jones, Brown, & Lawrence Aber,
184 E. Oberle and K.A. Schonert-Reichl

2011; Yoder, 2013). To date, many integrated programs are designed for implementation in
practices that are implemented outside of the elementary school. This might be due to the typi-
existence of a program or curriculum tend to be cal structure according to which elementary
ad hoc, lacking a research base, and are even car- schooling is organized. Students spend almost all
ried out largely subconsciously by teachers of their school hours with their classroom teacher
(Jones & Bouffard, 2012). Therefore, commend- and in their classroom setting. Hence, classroom
ing a shift from programs to strategies, Jones and teachers have some flexibility in organizing and
Bouffard call for integrative strategies that are planning their teaching schedule to add and inte-
designed, implemented, tested, and refined in grate SEL program elements where possible. In
order to gain empirically supported strategies high school, the traditional classroom setting is
that educators can integrate into their daily prac- replaced by a system in which different courses
tices and improve the efficiency and continuity of are constituted of different groups of students
SEL instruction. taught by different teachers. Academic pressure
also tends to increase in high school, and teachers
tend to experience larger barriers to implement-
Evidence-Based SEL Programs ing SEL in addition to their regular curriculum.
for Pre-, Elementary, and High CASEL’s guides for effective SEL in preschool
School: Examples and elementary school (CASEL, 2013) and mid-
dle school and high school (CASEL, 2015b) list
Most SEL programs are universal (i.e., designed programs that have been considered well
for all children in the classroom or school) rather designed, are accompanied by high-quality train-
than targeted to children with specific character- ing and other implementation supports, and show
istics (e.g., children at risk for mental health research evidence for effectiveness. In the fol-
problems). The goal of universal programs is to lowing sections, we briefly introduce selected
reach the entire student body rather than address- programs from each guide, exemplifying SEL
ing indicated subpopulations. The universal char- programs available for children in preschool
acteristic of SEL programs reflects the empirically through to high school.
supported assumption that school-based efforts Preschool and elementary school programs.
to promote SEL effectively enhance all children’s The 4Rs Program (reading, writing, and resolu-
success in school and life (Elias et al., 1997; Zins tion; http://www.morningsidecenter.org/4rs-pro-
& Elias, 2006). The goal of universal SEL pro- gram) was designed for use in preschool through
gramming functions as a coordinated approach to 8th grade. It includes “read-alouds,” book con-
prevention of risk and promotion of positive versations, and interactive skills lessons designed
development in all children (Payton et al., 2000). to foster children’s understanding and managing
Rather than addressing individual problems of their emotions, developing empathy and per-
through multiple fragmented programs (e.g., sub- spective taking, being assertive, resolving con-
stance use, risky behaviors, violence), universal flicts nonviolently, honoring diversity, and
SEL addresses the complex demands of growth standing up to bullying. The program consists of
and development by offering a general preven- 35 sessions. It can be implemented classroom-­
tion and promotion approach through school-­ wide and school-wide. Activities that connect the
based programming. The targeted goals of family to the program are also available. The
universal SEL are enhancing positive social units and activities are grade specific. Children
behaviors and academic success and reducing engage actively in the program. A number of
problem behaviors and emotional distress in stu- extension activities are available in addition to
dents (CASEL, 2013). the core program. Training for 4Rs is required
A burgeoning number of universal SEL pro- and typically lasts 25–30 h. Implementation sup-
grams have been developed, implemented, and port is available. The program’s effectiveness has
tested over the past two decades. The majority of been evaluated in randomized controlled trials
SEL in Schools 185

with 3rd and 4th grade students in inner-city gram for high-risk settings (Battistich et al.,
schools. Among other outcomes, significant pro- 1997).
gram effects have been found for improved aca- The Promoting Alternative Thinking Strategies
demic performance, positive social behavior, (PATHS) program (http://www.pathstraining.
reduced conduct problems, and reduced emo- com/main/curriculum/) teaches children to use
tional distress (Brown et al., 2010; Jones, Brown, peaceful conflict resolution strategies, emotional
Hoglund, & Aber, 2010; Jones et al., 2011). regulation, and responsible decision-making.
The Caring School Community (CSC) pro- The program was designed for children from pre-
gram was developed by researchers at the Center school through 6th grade. It consists of a total of
for the Collaborative Classroom (https://www. 40–52 lessons per grade. A combined set of les-
collaborativeclassroom.org/ formally called the sons is available for preschool and kindergarten
Developmental Studies Center), a nonprofit orga- and 5th and 6th grade. Separate lessons have
nization with a focus on developing and dissemi- been designed for 1st through 4th grade. Detailed
nating programs that promote children’s social, implementation support is provided to teachers
emotional, and academic development. The pro- through scripted lessons. The program offers
gram targets children in kindergarten to 6th grade opportunities for supplementary activities in
and teaches teachers to employ participatory addition to the core PATHS curriculum and for
instructional practices such as cooperative learn- connecting family members to the program. Each
ing groups, mastery teaching, and experiential lesson includes suggestions for generalizing the
activities that promote relevant, interactive class- learned skills beyond the PATHS curriculum in
room learning. The program consists of four pro- the day-to-day school context. Program training
gram elements: (a) class meeting lessons to lasts 2 days and is not mandatory. Empirical evi-
promote dialogue among students, (b) a cross-­ dence for the program’s effectiveness has been
age “buddies” program that pairs students across established widely in multiple randomized con-
grades to build relationships and trust, (c) “home- trolled trials with ethnically diverse groups of
side” activities that promote family involvement children in preschool through 5th grade (e.g.,
and inform parents of school activities while pro- Domitrovich et al., 2007; Greca, 2000).
viding them with opportunities to participate, and Intervention outcomes include improved aca-
(d) school-wide community building activities demic achievement, positive social behavior,
that involve school, home, and community. The reduced behavioral problems, and reduced emo-
CSC is unique in that it involves both extensive tional distress.
classroom-wide and school-wide efforts to create The MindUp™ program (http://thehawnfoun-
a sense of common purpose and commitment to dation.org/mindup/) is a classroom-wide pro-
prosocial norms and values such as caring, jus- gram designed with the goal to enhance
tice, responsibility, and learning. self-awareness, self-regulation, and focused
Research conducted over the past two decades attention and to reduce stress in children. Three
evaluating the effectiveness of CSC has shown separate sets of lessons are available for pre-
that students who have participated in the pro- school through 2nd grade, 3rd to 5th grade, and
gram demonstrate more prosocial and less 6th through 8th grade. A core practice of the
aggressive behaviors and a range of positive MindUp™ program is the practice of deep
school and motivation outcomes (e.g., mutual breathing and attentive listening, exercised sev-
trust in and respect for teachers and overall eral times each day. Further practices include a
increases in prosocial behavior and social skills) wide range of mindfulness exercises and activi-
compared to children who have not received it ties to create a positive and optimistic classroom
(Battistich, Schaps, & Wilson, 2004; Battistich, environment. Supporting findings from brain
Solomon, Watson, & Schaps, 1997). These posi- research is described for each activity in the pro-
tive effects remained stable in high-poverty gram manual. The program consists of 15 lessons
schools, suggesting the effectiveness of this pro- grouped into 4 units: (I) Let’s Get Focused! (1.
186 E. Oberle and K.A. Schonert-Reichl

Learning How Our Brains Work; 2. Understanding that are integrated into the regular classroom cur-
Mindful Attention; 3. Focusing Our Awareness: riculum and instruction. Taking into consider-
The Core Practices), (II) Paying Attention to Our ation the demands on teachers’ instructional
Senses (4. Mindful Listening; 5. Mindful Seeing; time, the RULER units are most applicable to
6. Mindful Smelling; 7. Mindful Tasting; 8. subject areas in English language arts (ELA) and
Mindful Moving I; 9. Mindful Moving II), (III) history because of their focus on literature, writ-
It’s All About Attitude (10. Perspective Taking; ing, and understanding the experiences of
11. Choosing Optimism; 12. Savoring Happy humans. For example, through the ELA curricu-
Experiences), and IV) Taking Action Mindfully lum, characters in literature (from children’s pic-
(13. Acting with Gratitude; 14. Performing Acts ture books to chapter books and novels) provide a
of Kindness; 15. Taking Mindful Action in Our rich opportunity for students to become cogni-
Community). A 1-day training workshop is avail- zant of a range of rich human emotional experi-
able but not mandatory for implementing ences that need to be recognized, understood,
MindUp™ in the classroom. Quasi-experimental labeled, expressed, and regulated.
and experimental research with 4th to 7th grade Research evaluating the effectiveness of
student has supported the effectiveness of RULER has provided support for its effective-
MindUP™ in enhancing academic achievement, ness (e.g., Brackett, Rivers, Reyes, & Salovey,
positive social behaviors, well-being, and peer 2010). Using a rigorous empirical design, Rivers,
relationships and decreasing problem behaviors Brackett, Reyes, Elbertson, and Salovey, (2013)
(Schonert-Reichl & Lawlor, 2010; Schonert-­ examined 62 schools that either integrated
Reichl et al., 2015). RULER into 5th and 6th grade ELA classrooms
The RULER Feeling Words Curriculum is a or served as comparison school in which only the
multi-year, structured curriculum designed for standard ELA curriculum was implemented.
elementary and middle school children to pro- They found that in schools that received the inter-
mote social, emotional, and academic learning vention, there was a higher degree of warmth and
with units and lessons centered on feeling words connectedness between teachers and students,
and related concepts (Brackett et al., 2009; more autonomy and leadership among students,
Maurer & Brackett, 2004). The primary aims of and teachers focused more on students’ interests
RULER are to enhance the social and emotional and motivations. These findings suggest that
skills of children and adolescents while creating RULER enhances classrooms in ways that can
an optimal learning environment that promotes promote students’ SEL and well-being.
academic, social, and personal effectiveness. Middle school and high school programs.
Similar to other programs designed to promote The Facing History and Ourselves program
SEL and prosocial skills, the RULER program (https://www.facinghistory.org) integrates the
uses a systemic approach to education—one in study of history, literature, and human behavior
which the learner, the learning process, and the with ethical decision-making and aims to pro-
learning environment are all incorporated mote students’ historical understanding, critical
(McCombs, 2004). thinking, and social-emotional development.
The “feeling word” units are available for kin- Students in the program engage in reflecting on
dergarten through 8th grade and include develop- history, make connections to current events, and
mentally appropriate lessons that are calibrated discuss the choices they confront and how they
for each grade level. The curriculum is designed can make a difference in the world. The program
to help students obtain a thorough and deep is available for grades 6–12 and can be embedded
understanding of the feeling words—words that within the social studies, humanities, and lan-
characterize a range of human emotions such as guage arts curriculum. The program can be
excitement, shame, alienation, and commitment. implemented classroom- or school-wide and
Each of the RULER units focuses on one feeling includes activities to involve the family and com-
word and includes a number of lessons or steps munity in activities (e.g., community members
SEL in Schools 187

come into the classroom to share their experi- functions, academic achievement, connected-
ences). There is no set number of lessons out- ness, and social-emotional skills (Lemberger,
lined in the program. The recommended amount Selig, Bowers, & Rogers, 2015).
of training for the program is 2–5 days. The advantages of evidence-based SEL pro-
Implementation support is available for teachers. grams are vast. Detailed program curricula and
In a large randomized controlled trial, Facing specific trainings and implementation support
History and Ourselves has been found effective guide educators step by step through the imple-
in enhancing students’ social-emotional skills mentation process, and many programs provide
and attitudes and in enhancing teachers’ teaching additional resources such as implementation
practices (Barr et al., 2015). coaches, booster sessions, and extension activi-
The Second Step: Student Success Through ties. Yet, many scholars have argued that in order
Prevention at Middle School (http://www.cfchil- to sustain SEL over time, schools need to shift
dren.org/second-step) aims to prepare students to away from individual programs and toward
navigate adolescence with effective communica- broader and continuous strategies through which
tion, coping, and decision-making skills that have SEL seeps into all teaching moments and interac-
them make good choices and avoid peer pressure, tions rather than occurring in isolation during
substance use, and bullying. The program was program implementation (Jones & Bouffard,
designed for students in grades 6–8. Preschool 2012; Meyers et al., 2015 ; Oberle et al., 2016). A
and elementary school versions of the program broader and more strategic approach to SEL in
that adapt the program focus and activities to stu- schools encourages both implementation of
dents’ developmental period are available. evidence-­based SEL programs and consistent uti-
Second Step consists of 48 lessons that involve lization of SEL strategies for a full infusion of
active participating in program activities and SEL into school practices.
exercises. Training for Second Step implementa-
tion consists of four virtual modules that each last
30–60 min. Findings from a large-scale random-  oward a Systemic Approach of SEL
T
ized controlled trial indicated that the program Implementation in Schools
was effective in reducing violence, aggression,
and sexual violence among socioeconomically Recent developments in school-based SEL have
disadvantaged students in 6th grade (Espelage, included a strategic move away from isolated
Low, Polanin, & Brown, 2013). implementation of individual programs towards
The aim of the Student Success Skills program a system of ongoing SEL that is fully integrated
for middle and high school (http://studentsuc- at all levels. A school-wide SEL approach
cessskills.com/programs) is to help children defines the entire school community as the unit
develop cognitive and self-management skills of change and aims to integrate SEL into daily
that improve student performance. The program interactions and practices at multiple setting
is implemented by the school counselor through levels in the school using collaborative efforts
classroom lessons. Additional group counseling that include all staff, teachers, and children
support is provided to children who need further (Jones & Bouffard, 2012; Meyers et al., 2015).
support. The program consists of eight SEL-­ School-wide SEL involves effective classroom-
specific lessons. Student Success Skills has level SEL, such as teaching and modeling
school-wide components and involves activities social-emotional competence, fostering social
that connect the family to program activities. skills in interactions among students, teachers,
Training in program implementation takes one and staff, and providing regular opportunities to
full day and some implementation support is build, advance, and practice social-emotional
available (e.g., program coaches). Experimental skills (Bierman & Motamedi, 2015; Rimm-
research with 7th grade students has found the Kaufman & Hulleman, 2015; Williamson,
program to be effective in enhancing executive Modecki, & Guerra, 2015).
188 E. Oberle and K.A. Schonert-Reichl

At the school level, SEL strategies typically Meyers et al., 2015; Weissberg et al., 2015). At
take the form of policies, practices, or structures the highest level, support can be provided through
that are in place to promote a positive school cli- national policies that allocate funding and spec-
mate and a culture that enhances development ify guidelines for SEL in schools. In alignment
across academic, personal, and social domains with national policies, state (or provincial) poli-
(Cohen, 2006; Jones & Bouffard, 2012; Meyers cies can establish learning standards for SEL and
et al., 2015). Such practices and policies can provide a framework for what students should
include formulating a code of conduct that speci- know and be able to do. At the level of school
fies social, emotional, and behavioral norms, val- districts, administrative leaders have the capacity
ues, and expectations for students and staff at to build the foundation of a well-established and
school; fair discipline policies; anti-bullying pre- well-implemented system of SEL in schools
vention guidelines; and professional learning through budget decisions and allocation of
opportunities in the domain of SEL. Educators resources required to develop and implement
and staff school-wide need to be prepared and continuous and consistent SEL programming.
trained to implement SEL strategies in- and out- Leading district administrators also have the
side of the classroom. This can be realized power to generate structural and policy changes
through high-quality professional development that warrant the sustainability of SEL program-
programs that reach current teachers and by pre- ming (Mart et al., 2015; Weissberg et al., 2015).
paring prospective teachers during their in-­ In sum, systemic support at the highest levels
service teacher training (Schonert-Reichl, enables administrators at the building level to
Hanson-Peterson, et al., 2015; Schonert-Reichl, provide the supports that are needed for the
Oberle, et al., 2015). To achieve consistency and implementation and sustainability of effective
establish a multi-tier system of support for SEL, SEL practices (CASEL, 2015a; Meyers et al.,
all staff (i.e., teachers, counselors, librarians, 2015). This systemic approach helps create a
administrators, and other school members) need supportive context for introducing and maintain-
to be included in the efforts to establish school-­ ing effective SEL programming for all students
wide practices of SEL (Weissberg et al., 2015). (Greenberg et al., 2003).
In addition to classroom- and school-based One concern of non-systematic program
approaches, family and community program- implementation is that populations of students
ming can extend SEL into the home and neigh- who could benefit from SEL programming do not
borhood context—the two developmental receive it because an adequate infrastructure that
contexts in which children spend most of their reaches all students is missing (Spoth et al.,
time when out of school (Albright & Weissberg, 2013). To aid districts and schools in ensuring
2010; Catalano, Berglund, Ryan, & Lonczak, that SEL reaches all students, CASEL has devel-
2010; Gullotta, 2015). School-family partner- oped a theory of action (ToA) that serves as a
ships can be particularly important when promot- practical blueprint for setting up and sustaining
ing SEL in younger children whose primary school-wide SEL (Meyers et al., 2015; Oberle
focus is still the family when defining their own et al., 2016). The ToA can be considered an inter-
values, social goals, and acceptable behaviors vention in itself, guiding policy makers, adminis-
and practices. Community partners can extend trators, and educators in building capacity for
school-based SEL by providing students with SEL and putting SEL into action school-wide—
additional opportunities to apply learned SEL consistently and continuously. The ToA includes
skills in various practical situations (e.g., during guidelines and activities staff can engage in at the
after-school programs and other community pro- school level to build a system for high-quality
grams) (Fagan, Hawkins, & Shapiro, 2015). SEL in their building. It also identifies where
For school-wide SEL to be successful and sus- schools require support and resources from the
tainable, systemic support is also needed at organizing school district. School administrators
higher governing levels (Jones & Bouffard, 2012; and educators can download the ToA for school-­
SEL in Schools 189

wide SEL from CASEL’s website and use it as a teachers also need support in putting SEL into
practical step-by-step resource to achieve sys- practice.
temic SEL in their school. The influence of teachers’ beliefs on imple-
Undeniably, teachers play a critical role in the mentation of SEL programs. Recent evidence
successful integration of SEL into classrooms suggests that teacher-related factors impact varia-
and schools. Nonetheless, until recently, the role tions in the implementation of SEL programs that
of teachers in the implementation of SEL pro- may influence the quality of the program and its
grams and practices into schools has been given success (Durlak & DuPre, 2008; Larsen and
scant attention. To what degree do teachers agree Samdal, 2012; Wanless & Domitrovich, 2015).
that SEL should be a part of education? How do For instance, teachers are more successful in
teachers’ attitudes and beliefs influence the suc- implementing SEL programs when they have a
cessful implementation of SEL programs? In positive attitude toward the program, are moti-
what ways do teachers’ own social and emotional vated to deliver the program with fidelity, and are
competencies and well-being influence SEL confident that they possess the skills and knowl-
implementation? What knowledge and skills edge to implement the program well (Durlak &
about SEL do teachers receive in their teacher DuPre, 2008). Teachers’ implementation fidelity
preparation programs? Answers to these ques- of SEL programs has been associated with a
tions are addressed in the following section that number of teacher beliefs: beliefs about whether
highlights the important role of teachers in SEL the SEL program activities are aligned with their
implementation and the promotion of students’ teaching approach (Domitrovich et al., 2015);
social and emotional competencies. self-efficacy beliefs about teaching (Ransford,
Greenberg, Domitrovich, Small, & Jacobson,
2009); level of comfort with delivering a SEL
I ntegrating SEL into Schools: curriculum (Brackett, Reyes, Rivers, Elbertson,
The Role of Teachers & Salovey, 2012); beliefs about behavior man-
agement practices (Rimm-Kaufman & Sawyer,
Teacher support for incorporating SEL into 2004); dedication to developing their students’
schools. Teachers are strong advocates for the SEL skills (Brackett et al., 2012); perceptions of
promotion of the social and emotional competen- whether the school leader supports an SEL pro-
cies of students. A report of a nationally repre- gram (Brown et al., 2010); and perceptions of
sentative survey including more than 600 teachers whether the school culture supports SEL instruc-
(Bridgeland et al., 2013) showed that most pre- tion (Brackett et al., 2012).
school to high school teachers believe that social A study by Reyes, Brackett, Rivers, Elbertson,
and emotional skills are teachable (95%), that and Salovey (2012) further illustrates the critical
promoting SEL will benefit students from both role of the teacher in determining an SEL pro-
rich and poor backgrounds (97%), and that SEL gram’s effectiveness. They examined whether the
has positive effects on school attendance and amount of training teachers received, the quality
graduation (80%), standardized test scores and of delivery of the SEL program, and the amount
overall academic performance (77%), college of lessons students received (dosage) were asso-
preparation (78%), workforce readiness (87%), ciated with student outcomes of social and emo-
and citizenship (87%). Additionally, these same tional competence during the initial
teachers reported that in order to effectively implementation phase of the RULER program.
implement and promote social and emotional Participants included 812 6th grade students and
skills in classrooms and schools, they need strong their teachers from 28 elementary schools in a
support from district and school leaders. These large urban school district in the northeastern
findings are important because they demonstrate United States that were part of a large random-
that there is a readiness among teachers to pro- ized controlled trial (RCT). Statistical analysis
mote social and emotional competencies and that clustered teachers into one of three groups: low-­
190 E. Oberle and K.A. Schonert-Reichl

quality implementers (i.e., teachers initially very well-being, in order to enhance teachers “fitness”
resistant to the program and delivered it poorly in implementing SEL effectively.
but became open to the program by the end of the Although limited, the past few years have seen
school year), moderate-quality implementers the emergence of interventions specifically tar-
(i.e., teachers who were moderate in their atti- geted at improving teachers’ SEL and stress man-
tudes toward the program and in their delivery of agement in school. This is an important area of
the program from beginning to end), and high-­ research given the indisputable link between
quality implementers (i.e., teachers who were teachers’ and students’ well-being and success in
consistently open to and delivered the program school (Hastings & Bham, 2003; Jennings &
very well from beginning to end). Analyses Greenberg, 2009; Oberle & Schonert-Reichl,
revealed no overall main effects for training, 2016). For example, two programs designed to
implementation quality, or dosage. There were, promote teachers’ SEL competence by incorpo-
however, more positive outcomes for students rating mindfulness-based approaches are CARE
when their teachers attended more trainings and (Cultivating Awareness and Resilience in
implemented more lessons and were classified as Education) and SMART-in-Education (Stress
either moderate or high implementers. Further Management and Resiliency Training).
analyses revealed that teachers categorized as Mindfulness is typically described as an atten-
“low implementers” were lower in their sense of tive, nonjudgmental, and receptive awareness of
teaching efficacy (i.e., beliefs about their capa- present moment experiences in terms of feelings,
bilities to modify their teaching practices to influ- images, thoughts, and sensations/perceptions
ence students’ engagement and learning even (e.g., Kabat-Zinn, 1990). Both programs aim to
among difficult and unmotivated students) than increase teachers’ mindfulness, job satisfaction,
teachers categorized as “high implementers.” compassion and empathy for students, and effi-
These findings underline the importance of not cacy for regulating emotions and decrease stress
only considering training and program fidelity and burnout. Initial research to date has sup-
when examining effective SEL program imple- ported the effectiveness of both CARE (Jennings,
mentation, it is also critical to take into account Frank, Snowberg, Coccia, & Greenberg, 2013;
teachers’ beliefs and attitudes about an SEL pro- Jennings, Snowberg, Coccia, & Greenberg, 2011)
gram and their teaching efficacy when assessing and SMART in Education (e.g., Benn, Akiva,
the influence of implementation on students’ Arel, & Roeser, 2012; Roeser et al., 2013) in pro-
SEL outcomes. moting teacher SEL competence and well-being.
The role of teachers’ own social and emo- Nonetheless, further research is needed to exam-
tional competence and well-being. Efforts to ine whether such positive changes in teacher
improve teachers’ knowledge about SEL alone well-being spill over into the classroom and lead
are not sufficient for successful SEL implementa- to improvements in students’ SEL competence.
tion. Indeed, teachers’ own SEL competence and SEL and teacher preparation. Many educa-
well-being appear to play a crucial role in influ- tors have not been adequately prepared to apply
encing the infusion of SEL into classrooms and and understand the effective implementation of
schools (Jones, Bouffard, & Weissbourd, 2013). SEL programs and practices. Given recent break-
Specifically, Jennings and Greenberg (2009) throughs in the science of SEL, it is critical now
argued that teachers’ social-emotional compe- more than ever that teacher preparation programs
tence and well-being contributes to the classroom include both the science and practice of SEL into
management strategies they use, the relationships coursework and preservice field experiences in
they form with students, student and classroom schools. To date, we have limited knowledge of
management all mediate classroom, as well as the degree to which this is occurring.
student outcomes. The authors recommend that Research on teacher attrition provides some
SEL intervention programs address teachers’ interesting insights into understanding why it is
SEL competence and the improvement of teacher essential to incorporate knowledge and skills
SEL in Schools 191

about SEL into teacher preservice education, Reichl et al. found that few teacher education
including a focus on promoting teachers’ own programs included any content on the five SEL
social and emotional competencies and well-­ competencies outlined by CASEL. Specifically,
being. Clearly, teacher burnout and attrition is a 13% had at least one course that included infor-
major problem that poses a threat to efforts to mation on relationship skills, 7% for responsible
improve teacher quality. According to a report decision-making, 6% for self-management, 2%
from the National Commission on Teaching and for social awareness, and approximately 1% for
America’s Future (2007), teacher turnover costs self-awareness.
the United States up to $7 billion a year, with the One strength of the scan conducted by
negative impact of teacher turnover being great- Schonert-Reichl et al. (2016) is that a wide cor-
est at low-performing, high-poverty, high-­ pus of data were obtained—data representing
minority schools. Stress and poor emotion each of the US states and the district of
management rank as the primary reasons why Columbia—allowing for informed decision-­
teachers become dissatisfied with the profession making for advancing the science and practice of
and leave their positions (Darling-Hammond, SEL in preservice teacher education. Nonetheless,
2001). Another contributing factor is student one limitation of the scan is that while the meth-
behavior (Ferguson, Frost, & Hall, 2012). One ods employed were high in breadth, there was a
study, for instance, indicated that of the 50% of relative absence of depth of information obtained
teachers who leave the field permanently, almost with regard to the actual ways in which SEL con-
35% report reasons related to problems with stu- tent is incorporated. For example, although the
dent discipline (Ingersoll & Smith, 2003). scan revealed the presence of SEL content in the
Problems with student discipline, classroom descriptions of courses on the websites of col-
management, and student mental health emerge leges of education, there is no way of actually
at the beginning of teachers’ careers, as first-year knowing the specific content covered in the
teachers tend to feel unprepared to manage their courses reviewed or the quality of that content.
classroom effectively and are unable to recognize Hence, future research efforts should seek to
common mental health problems such as anxiety design studies utilizing mixed methodologies
(Koller & Bertel, 2006; Siebert, 2005). On a that include both quantitative and qualitative data
more positive note, data also suggest that when in order to obtain a more complete picture of the
teachers receive training in the behavioral and precise nature of SEL efforts in teacher
emotional factors that impact teaching and learn- preparation.
ing in the classroom, they feel better equipped to
propose and implement positive, active class-
room management strategies that deter students’ Summary, Conclusions, and Future
aggressive behaviors and promote a positive Directions
classroom learning climate (Alvarez, 2007).
What is the extent to which teachers receive In this chapter, we offered a historical overview
any knowledge or skills about SEL in their pre- and the status quo on SEL instruction in elemen-
service teacher training? To address this ques- tary through to high schools, reviewed the state
tion, Schonert-Reichl, Kitil, and Hanson-Peterson of research on the effectiveness of SEL instruc-
(2016) conducted the first ever scan aimed at tion in enhancing positive developmental out-
determining the extent to which US colleges of comes in children and youth, and outlined critical
education include any course content on SEL in areas of future development for going to scale
teacher preparation programs. After conducting a with SEL.
detailed content analysis of 3916 required courses We outlined the substantial shift from a
in teacher preparation program in 304 colleges of complete absence of formal SEL in schools in the
education in the United States (representing 30% end of the twentieth century to an explosion of
of all colleges in the United States), Schonert-­ interest in SEL, development of a multitude of
192 E. Oberle and K.A. Schonert-Reichl

school-­ based SEL programs, increased SEL interventions and identifies core features (e.g.,
implementation in schools, and recent frame- school climate, classroom context) and specific
works that guide schools and districts in systemic competencies (i.e., either of the five SEL compe-
SEL. We defined SEL within the CASEL frame- tencies) that drive the success of SEL. Second,
work that outlines five core social-emotional the cultural sensitivity of SEL programs needs to
competencies that promote well-being and suc- be investigated. Although SEL is a universal
cess in- and outside of school (Payton et al., approach, it is possible that the success of indi-
2000) and discussed several practical resources vidual programs can be enhanced if programs are
CASEL has developed to facilitate program modified according to the situational context or
selection and more recently to guide administra- the ethnic and cultural background of students.
tors and district leaders in establishing a sustain- Further, the axiom about measurement “what is
able SEL school-wide (CASEL, 2013, 2015b; assessed gets addressed” also applies to SEL. A
Meyers et al., 2015; Oberle et al., 2016). The wide range of measures has been used to assess
robust link between social-emotional compe- social-emotional competence in students.
tence enhancement and outcomes in the domains Developing a coherent, psychometrically sound
of academic success, health, and well-being was measure of SEL that measures core SEL compe-
reviewed through presenting a wide range of pro- tencies is critical. Depending on age, SEL can be
gram evaluations and meta-analytic research con- assessed formatively or via self-, teacher-, peer-,
ducted within the past decade (e.g., Durlak et al., or parent report. Ideally, a multiple measures
2011). The importance of implementation quality approach is used to achieve a holistic picture of
and teachers’ buy-in, motivation, and prepared- students’ social-emotional competencies.
ness to teach high-quality SEL was emphasized. Important groundwork has been laid to move
Undoubtedly, the past two decades have wit- toward systemic SEL implementation in schools.
nessed tremendous advances in the research and Yet, going to scale with SEL requires active col-
practice of enhancing children’s social-emotional laboration with federal, state, and local policy
skills through strategic programming in school. makers, stakeholders, and funders. Cost-benefit
Several important milestones were reached: for analyses—such as the investigation completed by
example, coherent and consistent empirical Belfield et al. (2015)—are critical to provide
research has shown that SEL can be taught in transparency on the balance of costs invested into
schools and that successful high-quality SEL SEL programming and the economic returns
instruction drives children’s present and future offered for individuals and for society.
success and well-being; educational policies Finally, spreading SEL entails the prepared-
have emerged that mandate schools to address ness of educators. As illustrated above, a small
students’ SEL; practical resources and guidelines minority of preservice teachers currently receive
for implementing SEL successfully have been sufficient training for fostering their students’
developed—at the level of the classroom, school, social-emotional competencies, and very few are
and district. Yet, the goal of making SEL—along- provided with opportunities for developing
side academic learning—a national priority still their own social-emotional skills. Importantly,
has to be reached. SEL can only be successful if teachers possess
An agenda for the future of SEL is essential. the capability, motivation, and resources to put it
Weissberg and colleagues (Weissberg et al., into action.
2015) list a number of recommended next steps If addressed adequately, these and other future
that need to be addressed to ensure a sustainable developments can significantly contribute to
future of high-quality SEL. First, it is fundamen- establishing high-quality SEL and promoting its
tal to identify core components and active ingre- further growth and spread across schools and
dients that drive the success of evidence-based communities. Members of the society—includ-
SEL programs. Identifying those components ing families, parents, teachers, administrators,
contributes to the design of further effective and society leaders—have long recognized that
SEL in Schools 193

SEL skills are fundamental skills in life Brackett, M. A., Reyes, M. R., Rivers, S. E., Elbertson,
N. A., & Salovey, P. (2012). Assessing teachers’
(Greenberg et al., 2003); it is our joint responsi-
beliefs about social and emotional learning. Journal of
bility to provide children with the opportunities Psychoeducational Assessment, 30, 219–236.
to develop core SEL skills that contribute to their Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., Reyes, M. R., & Salovey,
health, well-being, and success in life. P. (2010). Enhancing academic performance and
social and emotional competence with the RULER
feeling words curriculum. Learning and Individual
Differences, 22, 218–224.
References Bridgeland, J., Bruce, M., & Hariharan, M. (2013). The
missing piece: A national survey on how social and
Albright, M. I., & Weissberg, R. P. (2010). School-family emotional learning can empower children and trans-
partnership strategies to enhance children’s social, form schools. Washington, DC: Civic Enterprises.
emotional, and academic growth. In S. L. Christenson Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). Contexts of child rearing:
& A. L. Reschley (Eds.), The handbook of school-­ Problems and prospects. American Psychologist,
family partnerships for promoting Studet competence 34(10), 844–850. doi:10.1037//0003-066X.34.10.844
(pp. 246–265). New York: Routledge/Taylor and Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. (2006). The bioecologi-
Francis Group. cal model of human development. In R. M. Lerner
Alvarez, H. K. (2007). The impact of teacher preparation (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Volume 1.
on responses to student aggression in the classroom. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed.,
Teaching and Teacher Education, 23, 1113–1126. pp. 793–828). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Barr, D. J., Boulay, B., Selman, R. L., McCormick, R. L., Brown, J. L., Jones, S. M., LaRusso, M. D., & Aber, J. L.
Lowenstein, E., Gamse, B., … Leonard, M. B. (2015). (2010). Improving classroom quality: Teacher influ-
A randomized controlled trial of professional devel- ences and experimental impacts of the 4rs program.
opment for interdisciplinary civic education: Impacts Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(1), 153–167.
on humanities teachers and their students. Teacher doi:10.1037/a0018160
College Record, 17, 1–52. Canadian Pediatric Society. (2009). Advocacy: Are we
Battistich, V., Schaps, E., & Wilson, N. (2004). Effects of doing enough? A status report on Canadian public
an elementary school intervention on students’ “con- policy and child and youth health. Retrieved August
nectedness” to school and social adjustment during 15, 2016, from http://www.cps.ca/en/status-report
middle school. The Journal of Primary Prevention, Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., Bandura,
24, 243–262. A., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2000). Prosocial foundations
Battistich, V., Solomon, D., Watson, M., & Schaps, E. of children’s academic achievement. Psychological
(1997). Caring school communities. Educational Science, 11(4), 302–306.
Psychologist, 32, 137–151. CASEL. (2013). Effective social and emotional learning
Belfield, C., Bowden, B., Klapp, A., Levin, H., Shand, programs: Preschool and elementary school edition.
R., & Zander, S. (2015). The economic value of social Chicago, IL: Author.
andemo- tional learning. New York, NY: Center for CASEL. (2015a). District guide for social and emotional
Benefit-Cost Studies in Education, Teachers College, learning. Chicago, IL: Author.
Columbia University. CASEL. (2015b). Effective social and emotional learning
Benn, R., Akiva, T., Arel, S., & Roeser, R. W. (2012). programs: Middle and high school edition. Chicago,
Mindfulness training effects for parents and educa- IL: Author.
tors of children with special needs. Developmental Catalano, R. F., Berglund, M. L., Ryan, J. A. M., &
Psychology, 48, 1476–1487. Lonczak, H. S. (2010). Positive youth development
Bierman, K. L., & Motamedi, M. (2015). Social-emotional in the United States: Findings on evaluations of posi-
learning programs for preschool children. In J. Durlak, tive youth development programs. The Annals of the
C. Domitrovich, R. P. Weissberg, & T. Gullotta (Eds.), American Academy of Political and Social Science,
The handbook of social-emotional learning: Research 591, 98–124. doi:10.1177/0002716203260102
and practice (pp. 1–36). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2013).
Bond, L. A., & Hauf, A. M. C. (2004). Taking stock and Mental health surveillance among children-United
putting stock in primary prevention: Characteristics of States, 2005-2011. MMWR Surveillance Summaries,
effective programs. Journal of Primary Prevention, 62(2), 1–35.
24(3), 199–221. Cohen, J. (2006). Academic education: Creating a climate
Brackett, M. A., Patti, J., Stern, R., Rivers, S. E., for learning, participation in democracy, and well-­
Elbertson, N., Chisholm, C., & Salovey, P. (2009). A being. Harvard Educational Review, 76(2), 201–237.
sustainable, skill-based model to building emotionally Cook, T. D., & Hirschfield, P. J. (2008). Comer’s school
literate schools. In R. Thompson, M. Hughes, & J. B. development program in Chicago: Effects on involve-
Terrell (Eds.), Handbook of developing emotional and ment with the juvenile justice system form the late
social intelligence: Best practices, case studies, and elementary through the high school years. American
tools (pp. 329–358). New York: John Wiley & Sons. Educational Research Journal, 45, 38–67.
194 E. Oberle and K.A. Schonert-Reichl

Darling-Hammond, L. (2001). The challenge of staffing (1997). Promoting social and emotional learning:
our schools. Educational Leadership, 58, 12–17. Guidelines for educators. Alexandria, VA: ASCD.
Devaney, B., O’Brien, M. U., Resnick, H., Keister, S., Espelage, D. L., Low, S., Polanin, J. R., & Brown, E. C.
& Weissberg, R. P. (2006). Sustainable schoolwide (2013). The impact of a middle school program to
social and emotional learning (SEL): Implementation reduce aggression, victimization, and sexual violence.
guide and toolkit. Chicago: Author. Journal of Adolescent Health, 52, 180–186.
Domitrovich, C. E., Cortes, R. C., & Greenberg, M. T. Fagan, A. A., Hawkins, J. D., & Shapiro, V. B. (2015).
(2007). Improving young children’s social and Taking SEL to scale in schools: The role of commu-
emotional competence: A randomized trial of the nity coalitions. In J. A. Durlak, C. E. Domitrovich,
preschool “PATHS” curriculum. The Journal of R. P. Weissberg, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Handbook of
Primary Prevention, 28(2), 67–91. doi:10.1007/ social and emotional learning: Research and practice
s10935-007-0081-0 (pp. 468–481). New York: Guilford.
Domitrovich, C. E., & Greenberg, M. T. (2000). The study Ferguson, K., Frost, L., & Hall, D. (2012). Predicting
of implementation: Current findings from effective teacher anxiety, depression, and job satisfaction.
programs that prevent mental disorders in school-aged Journal of Teaching and Learning, 8, 27–42.
children. Journal of Educational and Psychological Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence (1st ed.).
Consultation, 11(2), 193–221. doi:10.1207/ New York, NY: Bantam Books.
S1532768XJEPC1102_04 Greca, L. (2000). Sustained effects of the PATHS cur-
Domitrovich, C. E., Pas, E. T., Bradshaw, C. P., Becker, riculum on the social and psychological adjustment
K. D., Keperling, J. P., Embry, D. D., & Ialongo, N. of children in special education. Journal of Emotional
(2015). Individual and school organizational fac- and Behavioral Disorders, 12(2), 66–78.
tors that influence implementation of the PAX good Greenberg, M. T., Weissberg, R. P., O’Brien, M. U., Zins,
behavior game intervention. Prevention Science, 16, J. E., Fredericks, L., Resnik, H., & Elias, M. J. (2003).
1064–1074. Enhancing school-based prevention and youth devel-
Dryfoos, J. G. (2010). The prevalence of problem behav- opment through coordinated social, emotional, and
iors: Implications for programs. In R. P. Weissberg, academic learning. American Psychologist, 58(6–7),
T. P. Gullotta, R. L. Hampton, B. A. Ryan, & G. R. 466–474. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.6-7.466
Adams (Eds.), Enhancing children’s wellness (8th ed., Gullotta, T. P. (2015). After school programming and
pp. 17–46). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. social and emotional learning. In J. A. Durlak, C. E.
Durlak, J. A. (2016). Programme implementation in social Domitrovich, R. P. Weissberg, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.),
and emotional learning: Basic issues and research find- Handbook of social and emotional learning: Research
ings. Cambridge Journal of Education, 46, 333–345. and practice (pp. 260–266). New York: Guilford.
doi:10.1080/0305764X.2016.1142504 Haney, P., & Durlak, J. A. (1998). Changing self-esteem
Durlak, J. A., & DuPre, E. P. (2008). Implementation in children and adolescents: A meta-analytic review.
matters: A review of research on the influence of Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 423–433.
implementation on program outcomes and the fac- Hastings, R. P., & Bham, M. S. (2003). The relationship
tors affecting implementation. American Journal of between student behaviour patterns and teacher burn-
Community Psychology, 41, 327–350. out. School Psychology International, 24(1), 115–127.
Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, Hawkins, J. D., Kosterman, R., Catalano, R. F., Hill,
R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of K. G., & Abbott, R. D. (2008). Effects of social
enhancing students’ social and emotional learn- development intervention in childhood 15 years later.
ing: A meta-analysis of school-based universal Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine,
interventions. Child Development, 82(1), 405–432. 162(12), 1133–1141.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01564.x Hill, K. G., Bailey, J. A., Hawkins, J. D., Catalano, R. F.,
Elias, M. J. (1997). The missing piece: Making the case Kosterman, R., Oesterle, S., & Abbott, R. D. (2014).
for greater attention to social and emotional learning. The onset of STI diagnosis through age 30: Results
Education Week, 36–37. from the Seattle social development project interven-
Elias, M., & Haynes, N. (2008). Social competence, tion. Prevention Science, 15, 19–32. ­ doi:10.1007/
social support, and academic achievement in minor- s11121-013-0382-x
ity, low-income, urban elementary school children. Hull, J., & Newport, M. (2011). Time in school:
School Psychology Quarterly, 23(4), 474–495. How does the US compare? Retrieved July
doi:10.1037/1045-3830.23.4.474 20, 2016, from http://www.centerforpublice-
Elias, M. J., Zins, J. E., Graczyk, P. A., & Weissberg, ducation.org/Main-Menu/Organizing-a-school/
R. P. (2003). Implementation, sustainability, and scal- Time-in-school-How-does-the-US-compare
ing up of social-emotional and academic innovations Humphrey, N. (2013). Social and emotional learning: A
in public schools. School Psychology Review, 32(3), critical appraisal. London, UK: Sage.
303–319. Ingersoll, R. M., & Smith, T. M. (2003). The wrong solu-
Elias, M. J., Zins, J. E., Weissberg, R. P., Frey, K. S., tion to the teacher shortage. Educational Leadership,
Greenberg, M. T., Haynes, N. M., … Shriver, T. P. 60, 30–33.
SEL in Schools 195

Izard, C., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, study of teachers’ reactions to the Second Step pro-
B., & Youngstrom, E. (2001). Emotion knowledge as gram. School Psychology International, 33, 631–645.
a predictor of social behavior and academic compe- Lemberger, M., Selig, J., Bowers, H., & Rogers, J. (2015).
tence in children at risk. Psychological Science, 12(1), Effects of the student success skills program on the
18–23. executive functioning skills, feelings of connected-
Jennings, P. A., Frank, J. L., Snowberg, K. E., Coccia, ness, and academic achievement in in a predominantly
M. A., & Greenberg, M. T. (2013). Improving class- hispanic, low-income middle school district. Journal
room learning environments by cultivating aware- of Counseling and Development, 93, 25–37.
ness and resilience in education (CARE): Results Mart, A. K., Weissberg, R. P., & Kendziora, K. (2015).
of a randomized controlled trial. School Psychology Systemic support for social and emotional learning in
Quarterly, 28, 374–390. school districts. In J. A. Durlak, C. E. Domitrovich,
Jennings, P. A., & Greenberg, M. T. (2009). The proso- R. P. Weissberg, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Handbook of
cial classroom: Teacher social and emotional compe- social and emotional learning: Research and practice
tence in relation to student and classroom outcomes. (pp. 482–499). New York: Guilford.
Review of Educational Research, 79(1), 491–525. Maurer, M., & Brackett, M. A. (2004). Emotional literacy
doi:10.3102/0034654308325693 in the middle school: A 6-step program to promote
Jennings, P. A., Snowberg, K. E., Coccia, M. A., & social, emotional, & academic learning. Port Chester,
Greenberg, M. T. (2011). Improving classroom NY: National Professional Resources.
learning environments by cultivating awareness and McCombs, B. L. (2004). The learner-centered psycholog-
resilience in education (CARE): Results of two pilot ical principles: A framework for balancing academic
studies. Journal of Classroom Interaction, 46, 37–48. achievement and social-emotional learning outcomes.
Jones, S. M., & Bouffard, S. M. (2012). Social and emo- In J. E. Zins, R. P. Weissberg, M. C. Wang, & H. J.
tional learning in schools: From programs to strate- Walberg (Eds.), Building academic success on social
gies. Social Policy Report, 26(4), 1–33. and emotional learning: What does the research say?
Jones, S. M., Bouffard, S. M., & Weissbourd, R. (2013). (pp. 23–39). New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
Educators’ social and emotional skills vital to learn- Meyers, D. C., Gil, L., Cross, R., Keister, S., Domitrovich,
ing. Phi Delta Kappan, 94, 62–65. C. E., & Weissberg, R. P. (2015). CASEL guide for
Jones, S. M., Brown, J. L., Hoglund, W., & Aber, J. L. schoolwide social and emotional learning. Chicago,
(2010). A school-randomized clinical trial of an inte- IL: CASEL.
grated social-emotional learning and literacy inter- National Commission on Teaching and America’s
vention: Impacts after one school year. Journal of Future. (2007). The cost of teacher turnover in five
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 829–842. school districts: A pilot study. Washington, DC:
Jones, S. M., Brown, J. L., & Lawrence Aber, Author. Retrieved June 1, 2007, from www.nctaf.
J. (2011). Two-year impacts of a universal school-­ org/resources/demonstration_projects/turnover/docu-
based social-emotional and literacy intervention: ments/cttexecutivesummaryfinal.pdf
An experiment in translational developmental Oberle, E., Domitrovich, C. E., Meyers, D., & Weissberg,
research. Child Development, 82(2), 533–554. R. P. (2016). Establishing systemic social and emo-
doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01560.x tional learning approaches in schools: The need for
Jones, D. E., Greenberg, M., & Crowley, M. (2015). school-wide implementation. Cambridge Journal of
Early social-emotional functioning and public health: Education, 46, 277–297.
The relationship between kindergarten social com- Oberle, E., & Schonert-Reichl, K. A. (2016). Stress con-
petence and future wellness. American Journal tagion in the classroom? The link between classroom
of Public Health, 105, 2283–2290. doi:10.2105/ teachers’ burnout and morning cortisol in elementary
AJPH.2015.302630 school students. Social Science and Medicine, 159,
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the 30–37.
wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and Oberle, E., Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Hertzman, C., &
illness. New York, NY: Bantam Doubleday Dell. Zumbo, B. D. (2014). Social-emotional competen-
Kann, L., Kindchen, S., Shanklin, S. L., Flint, K. H., cies make the grade: Predicting academic success in
Hawkins, J., Harris, W. A., … McManus, T. (2014). early adolescence. Journal of Applied Developmental
Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance—United States , Psychology, 35(3), 138–147.
2013. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 63(4), 1–168. OECD. (2012). How long do children spend in the class-
Koller, J. R., & Bertel, J. M. (2006). Responding to today’s room? In Education at a glance 2012: Highlights.
mental health needs of children, families and schools: OECD Publishing. Retrieved from http://dx.doi.
Revisiting the pre-service training and preparation of org/10.1787/eag_highlights-2012-24-en
school-based personnel. Education and Treatment of Osher, D., Sprague, J., Weissberg, R. P., Keenan, S., &
Children, 29, 197–217. Zins, J. E. (2008). A comprehensive approach to
Larsen, T., & Samdal, O. (2012). The importance of promoting social, emotional, and academic growth
teachers’ feelings of self efficacy in developing their in contemporary schools. In A. Thomas & J. Grimes
pupils’ social and emotional learning: A Norwegian (Eds.), Best practices in school psychology (Vol. 4,
196 E. Oberle and K.A. Schonert-Reichl

pp. 1263–1278). Bethesda, MD: National Association Schonert-Reichel, K. A., & Hymel, S. (2007). Educating
of School Psychologists. the heart as well as the mind: Social and emotional
Payton, J. W., Wardlaw, D. M., Graczyk, P. A., Bloodworth, learning for school and life success. Education
M. R., Tompsett, C. J., & Weissberg, R. P. (2000). Canada, 47, 20–25.
Social and emotional learning: A framework for pro- Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Hanson-Peterson, J. L., &
moting mental health and reducing risk behavior in Hymel, S. (2015). Social and emotional learning and
children and youth. Journal of School Health, 70(5), pre-service teacher education. In J. A. Durlak, C. E.
179–185. Domitrovich, R. P. Weissberg, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.),
Payton, J., Weissberg, R. P., Durlak, J. A., Dymnicki, Handbook of social and emotional learning: Research
A. B., Taylor, R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2008). The and practice (pp. 406–421). New York: Guilford.
positive impact of social and emotional learning for Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Kitil, M. J., & Hanson-Peterson,
kindergarten to eighth-grade students findings from J. (2016). Teachers first: A national scan of teacher
three scientific reviews. Chicago, IL: Author. preparation programs and social and emotional
Ransford, C. R., Greenberg, M. T., Domitrovich, C. E., learning. A report prepared for the Collaborative for
Small, M., & Jacobson, L. (2009). The role of teach- Academic, Social, and Emotional Learning (CASEL).
ers’ psychological experiences and perceptions of cur- Vancouver, BC: University of British Columbia.
riculum supports on the implementation of a social and Schonert-Reichl, K. A., & Lawlor, M. S. (2010). The
emotional learning curriculum. School Psychology effects of a mindfulness-based education program on
Review, 38, 510–532. pre- and early adolescents’ well-being and social and
Reyes, M. R., Brackett, M. A., Rivers, S. E., Elbertson, emotional competence. Mindfulness, 1(3), 137–151.
N. A., & Salovey, P. (2012). The interaction effects of Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Oberle, E., Lawlor, M. S., Abbott,
program training, dosage, and implementation quality D., Thomson, K., Oberlander, T. F., & Diamond, A.
on targeted student outcomes for the RULER approach (2015). Enhancing cognitive and social—Emotional
to social and emotional learning. School Psychology development through a simple-to-­
administer
Review, 41, 82–99. mindfulness-­ based school program for elemen-
Riggs, N. R., & Pentz, M. A. (2009). Long term effects of tary school children: A randomized controlled trial.
adolescent marijuana use prevention on adult mental Developmental Psychology, 51(1), 52–66.
health services utilization: The Midwestern prevention Siebert, C. J. (2005). Promoting preservice teachers’ suc-
project. Substance Use and Misuse, 44, 616–631. cess in classroom management by leveraging a local
Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Hulleman, C. S. (2015). union’s resources: A professional development school
Handbook of social and emotional learning: Research initiative. Education, 125, 385–392.
and practice. In J. A. Durlak, C. E. Domitrovich, Sklad, M., Diekstra, R., Ritter, M. D., Ben, J., &
R. P. Weissberg, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Social and Gravesteijn, C. (2012). Effectiveness of school-based
emotional learning in elementary school settings: universal social, emotional, and behavioral programs:
Identifying mechanisms that matter (pp. 151–166). Do they enhance students’ development in the area
New York: Guilford. of skill, behavior, and adjustment? Psychology in the
Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., Larsen, R. A. A., Baroody, Schools, 49(9), 892–909. doi:10.1002/pits.21641
A. E., Curby, T. W., Ko, M., Thomas, J. B., … Spoth, R., Rohrbach, L. A., Greenberg, M., Leaf, P.,
DeCoster, J. (2014). Efficacy of the responsive Brown, C. H., Fagan, A., … Hawkins, J. D. (2013).
classroom approach: Results from a 3-year, lon- Addressing core challenges for the next generation
gitudinal randomized controlled trial. American of type 2 translation research and systems: The trans-
Educational Research Journal, 51, 567–603. lation science to population impact (TSci impact)
doi:10.3102/0002831214523821 framework. Prevention Science, 14(4), 319–351.
Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Sawyer, B. E. (2004). Primary-­ doi:10.1007/s11121-012-0362-6
grade teachers’ self-efficacy beliefs, attitudes toward Taylor, R., Oberle, E., Durlak, J. A., & Weissberg, R. P.
teaching, and discipline and teaching practice priori- (2017). Promoting positive youth development
ties in relation to the responsive classroom approach. through school-based social and emotional learning
Elementary School Journal, 104, 321–341. interventions: A meta-analysis of follow-up effects.
Rivers, S. E., Brackett, M. A., Reyes, M. R., Elbertson, Child Development, 88(4), 1156–1171.
N. A., & Salovey, P. (2013). Improving the social and Vadeboncoeur, J. A., & Collie, R. J. (2013). Locating
emotional climate of classrooms: A clustered random- social and emotional learning in schooled environ-
ized controlled trial testing the RULER approach. ments: A Vygotskian perspective on learning as uni-
Prevention Science, 14, 77–87. fied. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 20(3), 201–225. doi:
Roeser, R. W., Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Jha, A., Cullen, 10.1080/10749039.2012.755205
M., Wallace, L., Wilensky, R., … Harrison, J. (2013). Wanless, S. B., & Domitrovich, C. E. (2015). Readiness
Mindfulness training and reductions in teacher stress to implement school-based social-emotional learn-
and burnout: Results from two randomized, waitlist-­ ing interventions: Using research on factors related
control field trials. Journal of Educational Psychology, to implementation to maximize quality. Prevention
105, 787–804. Science, 16, 1037–1043.
SEL in Schools 197

Weare, K., & Nind, M. (2011). Mental health promotion In J. A. Durlak, C. E. Domitrovich, R. P. Weissberg,
and problem prevention in schools: What does the & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Handbook of social and emo-
evidence say? Health Promotion International, 26, tional learning: Research and practice (pp. 500–515).
i29–i69. New York: Guilford Press.
Weissberg, R. P., Payton, J. W., O’Brien, M. U., & Munro, Yoder, N. (2013). Teaching the whole child: Instructional
S. (2007). Social and emotional learning. In F. C. practices that support social and emotional learning
Power, R. J. Nuzzi, D. Narvaez, D. K. Lapsley, & T. C. in three teacher evaluation frameworks. Washington,
Hunt (Eds.), Moral education: A handbook, Volume 2: DC: American Institutes for Research Center on Great
M-Z (pp. 417–418). Greenwood Press: Westport, CT. Teachers and Leaders.
Weissberg, R. P., & Cascarino, J. (2013). Academic + Zins, J. E., Bloodworth, M. R., Weissberg, R. P., &
social-emotional learning = national priority. Phi Walberg, H. J. (2004a). The scientific base link-
Delta Kappan, 95, 8–13. ing social and emotional learning to school suc-
Weissberg, R. P., Durlak, J. A., Domitrovich, C. E., & cess. Journal of Educational and Psychological
Gullotta, T. P. (2015). Social and emotional learn- Consultation, 17(508), 191–210.
ing: Past, present, and future. In J. A. Durlak, C. E. Zins, J. E., Bloodworth, M. R., Weissberg, R. P., &
Domitrovich, R. P. Weissberg, & T. P. Gullotta Walberg, H. J. (2004b). The scientific basis link-
(Eds.), Handbook for social and emotional learning: ing social and emotional learning to school success.
Research and practice (pp. 3–19). New York, NY: In J. E. Zins, R. P. Weissberg, M. C. Wang, & H. J.
Guilford Press. Walberg (Eds.), Building academic success on social
Welsh, M., Parke, R. D., Widaman, K., & Neil, R. O. and emotional learning: What does the research say
(2001). Linkages between children’s social and aca- (pp. 3–22). New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
demic competence: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Zins, J. E., & Elias, M. J. (2006). Social and emotional
School Psychology, 39(6), 463–482. learning. In G. G. Bwar & K. M. Minke (Eds.),
Williamson, A. A., Modecki, K. L., & Guerra, N. G. Children’s needs III: Development, prevention, and
(2015). Social and emotional learning programs in intervention (pp. 1–13). Bethesda, MD: National
high school. In J. A. Durlak, C. E. Domitrovich, R. P. Association of School Psychologists.
Weissberg, & T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Handbook of Zins, J. E., Elias, M. E., Weissberg, R. P., Greenberg,
social and emotional learning: Research and practice M. T., Haynes, N. M., Frey, K. S., … Shriver, T. P.
(pp. 181–196). New York: Guilford Press. (1998). Enhancing learning through social-emotional
Wright, A., Lamont, A., Wandersman, A., Osher, D., & education. Think: The Journal of Critical and Creative
Gordon, E. (2015). Accountability and social and Thinking, 9, 18–20.
emotional learning: The getting to outcomes approach.
Using Parent Training Programmes
to Teach Social Skills

John Sharry and Caoimhe Doyle

caregivers, have a significant impact on the


 o Parents Influence Children’s
D development of children’s social competence.
Acquisition of Social Skills? Moreover, parents and primary caregivers provide
children with their earliest social relationships, as
Across nations and cultures, families, and parents well as being their first models for social roles
in particular, bear the primary responsibility for and behaviour (Kostelnik et al., 2008). Current
ensuring that children’s physical and emotional debate in the field of child development supports
needs are met and, importantly, for socialising the ideas that development results from the inter-
them (Kostelnik, Whiren, Soderman, Rupiper, & action between genetic and environmental factors
Gregory, 2008). Socialisation is the process and that children actively contribute to their own
whereby a child acquires the skills, behaviours, development (Meece & Daniels, 2008; Siegler,
values and emotional understanding needed to DeLoache, & Eisenberg, 2011). However, many
function competently within the society they are of the major theories of development, albeit to
growing up in (Maccoby, 2007). A child’s social different extents, recognise and acknowledge the
competence therefore refers to his or her ability influence parents and primary caregivers have on
to establish and maintain positive relationships with children’s and adolescent’s learning and acquisi-
others (Haven, Manangan, Sparrow, & Wilson, tion of social skills.
2014). According to Newman and Newman
(2012), early family experiences, including the
nature of family conversations, how discipline is The Psychoanalytic Perspective
addressed and the quality of attachment with
While biological maturation is central to two of
the most influential theories of social develop-
ment, namely, Freud’s theory of psychosexual
development and Erikson’s theory of psychoso-
cial development, both recognise the impact of
J. Sharry (*)
Parents Plus, Philsborough, Dublin, Ireland children’s early experiences within the family on
their subsequent relationships (Siegler et al.,
University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
e-mail: john@parentsplus.ie 2011). Freud’s theory asserts that there are three
components to personality (i.e. the id, the ego
C. Doyle
Parents Plus, Philsborough, Dublin, Ireland and the superego) which emerge and integrate
e-mail: caoimhedoyle55@gmail.com gradually as the individual passes through a

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 199


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_12
200 J. Sharry and C. Doyle

series of five developmental psychosexual stages development, particularly during the first five life
during childhood and adolescence. Freud main- stages specified in his model, namely, infancy,
tained that during each of these five stages, the toddlerhood, early school age, middle childhood
child is tasked with resolving specific conflicts and adolescence. During these stages the psycho-
related to different erogenous zones, in which social model proposes that children either
their relationships with their mother and father develop basic trust or mistrust of caregivers in
are implicated. Furthermore, Freud believed that respect to their care (from birth to 1 year), auton-
children’s relative success or failure in resolving omy or shame (from 1 to 3 years) with regard to
these conflicts has consequences for their social doing things on their own (e.g. feeding, dressing,
and emotional relationships throughout life toileting, etc.), initiative or guilt (from 3 to
(Shaffer & Kipp, 2010). For example, in Freud’s 6 years) in relation to having responsibilities,
first stage of psychosexual development, the ‘oral industry or inferiority (from 6 to 11 years) in
stage’, which begins at birth and lasts for approx- relation to mastery of social and academic skills
imately 1 year, the child’s sex instinct centres on and a sense of identity or identity confusion
the mouth, with pleasure being derived primarily (from 12 to 20 years). While Erikson’s model
from oral activities such as sucking and eating. emphasises the agency of children and adoles-
Considering this, the child’s relationships with its cents in shaping their own development, it
primary caregivers who feed and nourish him/her acknowledges that the quality of relationships
are highly significant during this stage, and any and support of parents or primary caregivers
impediments either to the caregiver-child bond or throughout these life stages influence the resolu-
the child’s weaning, for example, may have nega- tion of developmental tasks significantly (Shaffer
tive implications for their later emotional rela- & Kipp, 2010).
tionships (Shaffer & Kipp, 2010). Despite having received criticism for the
Though Erikson’s theory places much less ambiguity and dubiousness of certain theoretical
emphasis on sexual determinants of social devel- assertions which limit the extent to which their
opment than that of Freud and much more on models can be tested, both Freud and Erikson
social and cultural factors, it also considers par- have contributed significantly to our understand-
ents to be key social agents in the formative years ing of children’s social development (Shaffer &
of childhood (Shaffer & Kipp, 2010). Like Kipp, 2010). Moreover, Freud’s and Erikson’s
Freud’s theory, Erikson’s model is a sequential theories have broadened our appreciation of the
stage theory which posits that each stage of life is lasting effects of early experiences in the context
characterised by a specific set of developmental of the family on the individual’s interpersonal
tasks and personal dilemmas which the individ- relationships throughout life (Siegler et al.,
ual must resolve. Erikson’s theory proposes that 2011). This concept is the foundation of modern-­
during each of eight distinct age-related stages, day attachment research within the ethological
which span from infancy to elderhood, the devel- school, which has determined that the quality of
oping individual is tasked with reorganising their an infant’s early relationship with their primary
self-concept based on new cognitive capacities, caregivers (i.e. whether there is a secure or
relationship skills and learning, in order to inte- insecure attachment) tends to remain stable over
grate their personal needs with social and cultural time and has an enduring effect on later social
demands (Newman & Newman, 2012). ties and development.
In contrast to Freud, Erikson believed that
children are active contributors to their own
development and not merely passive reactors The Ethological Perspective
dominated by sexual instincts and shaped solely
by their parents (Shaffer & Kipp, 2010). The principal tenet of attachment theory (Bowlby,
Nonetheless, Erikson recognised the influence of 1969, 1973, 1980) is that for positive social
parents and primary caregivers on children’s development to occur, the infant needs to develop
Using Parent Training Programmes to Teach Social Skills 201

a strong emotional and physical attachment with Juffer, van IJzendoorn, Bakermans-Kranenburg,
at least one primary caregiver (most often their & Mooijaart, 2006).
mother or father). Bowlby (1982; as cited in
Ainsworth, 1989) proposed that throughout the
first year, the infant gradually develops an ‘inter- The Sociocultural and Social Learning
nal working model’ or cognitive representation Perspectives
of regularities in their physical environment,
attachment figures and self, which has a lasting Although Vygotsky’s sociocultural theory (1962;
influence on his/her interpretation and formation 1978) is concerned with the cognitive rather than
of expectations about social interactions and rela- the social domain of development (i.e. that which
tionships. Later, attachment theory was expanded involves information processing, language learn-
and empirical support for Bowlby’s assertions ing and perception), it too maintains that children
obtained by Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, and Wall acquire social and cultural values, beliefs and
(1978) and Main and Solomon (1986) with their problem-solving skills through interaction with
identification of distinct attachment patterns more competent others such as parents and care-
through observation of infants’ responses to the givers (Murphy, Scantlebury, & Milne, 2015).
behaviours of their mother and a stranger in a While Vygotsky stressed active rather than pas-
‘strange situation’—a laboratory-based proce- sive learning, he posited that children are prod-
dure for assessing attachment (Bretherton, 1992). ucts of their sociocultural context and that their
The attachment styles identified were secure cognitive development occurs when their parents
(child is visibly upset when the caregiver leaves and caregivers provide guided participation, joint
but happy to see them return), insecure-avoidant attention and social scaffolding (Siegler et al.,
(child avoids or ignores the caregiver and shows 2011). Guided participation refers to the process
little emotion both when the caregiver departs whereby a more knowledgeable other formulates
and returns), insecure-ambivalent (child is highly activities and tasks so that a novice can engage in
distressed when the caregiver leaves and is them and thus learn; joint attention is the mutual
ambivalent on their return) and insecure-­ and intentional focus of social partners on the
disorganised (child is distressed when their care- same external object which enhances children’s
giver departs but due to fear of the caregiver does ability to learn from others; and social scaffold-
not feel reassured on their return). ing refers to the expert’s provision of a temporary
Extensive longitudinal research has demon- framework to advance children’s thinking
strated that in comparison to insecurely attached (Siegler et al., 2011). This idea that cognitive
age-mates, infants who develop a secure attach- development is largely attributable to social inter-
ment with their primary caregivers attain more action and experience also underpins Bandura’s
favourable developmental outcomes, display bet- social learning theory of social development
ter social skills, enjoy better peer relations and (Bandura, 1977, 1986). Bandura (1977, 1986)
are more likely to have close friends later in asserted that by observing and imitating the
childhood and in adolescence (Shaffer & Kipp, social behaviour of others in their environment
2010). However, it is important to note that and subsequently interpreting others’ reactions to
attachment patterns in infancy and early child- their own behaviour, children develop social
hood do not guarantee either positive or negative understanding and skills.
adjustment in later life; in fact a child’s cognitive
representation of themselves, their caregivers and
their close emotional relationships can change The Bioecological Perspective
over time. Nonetheless, a large body of evidence
from attachment research strongly suggests One of the most encompassing theories of the
that early parenting and parent-child attachment general context of development across the lifes-
patterns have implications for later social pan is Urie Bronfenbrenner’s bioecological
development and functioning (Jaffari-Bimmel, model (Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Bronfenbrenner
202 J. Sharry and C. Doyle

& Morris, 1998). The bioecological model con- While the various theories of child develop-
siders human development to be an evolving ment differ in terms of the extent to which they
function of the interaction between a person and consider innate biological processes and environ-
their sociocultural environment. That is, both the mental conditions to influence learning and
individual and the environment influence one sociocultural adaptation, most acknowledge that
another bidirectionally (Bronfenbrenner, 2000). parents and primary caregivers have an important
The bioecological model emphasises the interre- role in advancing children’s social skills. In dif-
latedness and contextual variation of different ferent ways psychoanalytic, ethological, socio-
structures and processes that impact on human cultural, social learning and bioecological
development, while also highlighting the influ- perspectives on development examine and
ence of the individual’s biopsychological charac- emphasise the significance of early experiences
teristics on their environment (Darling, 2007). and emotional relationships within the family
Furthermore, Bronfenbrenner’s model conceptu- context to children’s later social functioning and
alises the environment as a number of succes- development (Feldman, Bamberger, & Kanat-­
sively nested, interdependent and dynamic Maymon, 2013). Furthermore, there is a growing
systems, namely, the micro-, meso-, exo- macro- body of evidence to suggest that positive parent-­
and chronosystems (Bronfenbrenner, 1986). child interactions are the basis of healthy devel-
For a young child, the microsystem comprises opment across the lifespan (Sanders, Kirby,
the environments, activities and relationships he Tellegen, & Day, 2014).
or she directly participates in, such as his or her
family, pre-school or neighbourhood play area;
the mesosystem refers to the interconnections Parent Training: Emergence,
among each of the child’s different microsys- Rationale and Applications
tems (e.g. the relationship between their family
and pre-school); the exosystem comprises the Prior to the late 1960s, emotional and behav-
environmental settings not directly experienced ioural problems occurring in childhood and ado-
by the individual but which affect them nonethe- lescence were predominantly addressed using
less (e.g. the child’s parent’s workplace); the child therapy, adolescent institutionalisation or
macrosystem is the wider social and cultural juvenile adjudication, all of which targeted the
context in which the other systems are embed- child’s behaviour exclusively (Kaminski, Valle,
ded; and the chronosystem refers to the histori- Filene, & Boyle, 2008). However, the realisation
cal context or changes that influence the person that parents influence and contribute to their chil-
over time (Newman & Newman, 2012). While dren’s social, emotional and behavioural tenden-
the bioecological model asserts that an individu- cies and can therefore effect change of the same
al’s development is influenced by several social saw the emergence of parent training interven-
and cultural systems and that concurrently these tions seeking to empower parents to address chil-
systems are influenced by the individual, the dren’s behavioural problems. Over time, parent
family component of the microsystem is recog- training evolved to incorporate content to help
nised as the principal context in which develop- parents to maximise their children’s cognitive
ment takes place during childhood development, physical health and social skills, in
(Bronfenbrenner, 1986). The complexity of the addition to enhancing their own self-efficacy
child’s microsystem increases as he or she grows with regard to parenting (Kaminski et al., 2008;
older and has more frequent interaction with NCCMH, 2009).
individuals outside their immediate family, for With roots in behavioural learning theory and
example, school friends, teachers and sports play therapy, the rationale for parent training was
coaches. However, the child’s family is a crucial the belief that disruptive behaviours occurring
component of the microsystem, and during across childhood are mediated and maintained by
infancy, childhood and adolescence, its influence the practices of their main socialisers and care-
predominates (Siegler et al., 2011). givers—parents (Lundahl, Risser, & Lovejoy,
Using Parent Training Programmes to Teach Social Skills 203

2006). Parent training is therefore concerned with rewarding good behaviour while largely ignoring
teaching parents the principles of child behaviour misbehaviour. According to Sanders et al. (2014),
management and facilitating their development programmes underpinned by social learning prin-
of skills and techniques for increasing the fre- ciples and which adopt this positive parenting
quency of their children’s prosocial behaviour approach have been identified as the gold stan-
(e.g. reinforcement) and reducing the frequency dard in addressing children’s emotional and
of their antisocial behaviour through ignoring, behavioural problems and promoting positive
for example (NCCMH, 2009; Forgatch & child adjustment. Numerous evaluation studies
Paterson, 2010; as cited in Carr, 2014). have produced evidence indicating the effective-
Furthermore, parent training aims to strengthen ness of both the Incredible Years and Triple P
parent-child relationships by imparting strategies programmes in terms of reducing children’s emo-
for good communication and attending positively tional and behavioural problems, decreasing par-
to children (Barlow, Smailagic, Huband, Roloff, ents’ anxiety, depression and stress and improving
& Bennett, 2012). overall family adjustment (Barlow et al., 2012;
Traditionally, parent training interventions Furlong et al., 2013; Menting, Orobio, & Matthys,
have been delivered to families on a one-to-one 2013; Sanders et al., 2014).
basis, but a group-based format is increasingly Another lesser-known but empirically sup-
being used (Furlong et al., 2013; Hand, Ni ported suite of practical manualised group-based
Raghallaigh, Cuppage, Coyle, & Sharry, 2012). parent training interventions is offered by Parents
While one-to-one parent training has greater flex- Plus. The Parents Plus Programmes aim to sup-
ibility for tailoring content and pace, group-based port families to communicate effectively, to build
parent training is more cost-effective in terms of positive relationships and to address and over-
reaching a greater number of families, and it come their children’s emotional and behavioural
facilitates greater opportunity for social support difficulties. Like Incredible Years and Triple P,
(Chronis, Chacko, Fabiano, Wymbs, & Pelham, the Parents Plus Programmes draw largely from a
2004). Typically group-based parent training pro- social learning model of how social behaviours
grammes are interactive and collaborative with are learnt and changed. In addition, the Parents
modelling, observation, behaviour rehearsal, role Plus model incorporates a solution-focused
play and group discussion being used by the most framework, whereby collaborating with parents
effective models to facilitate participants’ learn- and working on their individual goals is central to
ing (Furlong et al., 2013). Furthermore, a stan- the delivery of the programme. Rather than cor-
dard curriculum incorporating video vignette recting misbehaviour, parents attending the
content for modelling purposes is a common fea- Parents Plus Programmes are encouraged to be
ture of many widely used parent training models, goal focused and to encourage the development
and this maintains consistency in programme dis- of positive social behaviours in their children.
semination across delivery settings and providers
(Barlow et al., 2012).
A video-assisted manualised and group-based Parents Plus
format is utilised by a number of long-running
and internationally used parent training models First launched in 1998, the original Parents Plus
such as the Incredible Years (Webster-Stratton, Programme (Sharry & Fitzpatrick, 1997) was a
1998) and Triple P (Sanders, 2012) programmes. broad-based parent training intervention aimed at
Combining basic cognitive and behavioural strat- reducing children’s emotional and behavioural
egies, these programmes help parents recognise problems and promoting learning and attachment
and modify patterns of thought which may influ- in children aged 4–11 years. This programme
ence their behaviour and, in turn, that of their has since been replaced with three age-specific
children (Barlow et al., 2012). Furthermore, par- programmes: the Parents Plus Early Years
ticipants are encouraged to parent positively, Programme (Sharry, Hampson, & Fanning, 2013)
which essentially involves attending to and for parents of children aged 1–6 years, the Parents
204 J. Sharry and C. Doyle

Plus Children’s Programme (Sharry & tion and discipline strategies to positively
Fitzpatrick, 2007) for parents of children aged influence their children’s social, emotional and
6–11 years and the Parents Plus Adolescents behavioural development. The programmes are
Programme (Sharry & Fitzpatrick, 2012) for par- designed to be delivered over 8–12 weeks, with
ents of children aged 11–16 years. Two further weekly sessions lasting between 2.5 and 3 hours.
Parents Plus Programmes have also been devel-
oped—Parenting When Separated (Sharry,
Keating, & Murphy, 2012) and Working Things  heoretical Foundations of Parents
T
Out (Brosnan, Sharry, Beattie, & Fitzpatrick, Plus Programmes
2011). The former is a group-based parent train-
ing course for parents who are preparing for, who The structure and content of the Parents Plus
are going through or who have gone through a Programmes share a number of similarities with
separation or divorce, and the latter was designed other prominent group-based parent training
for use with small groups of adolescents to pro- interventions such as the Incredible Years and
mote positive youth mental health through the Triple P programmes. Firstly, like both the
development of communication, conflict resolu- Incredible Years and Triple P suites, the theoreti-
tion and coping skills. The Parents Plus cal basis of the Parents Plus Programmes is in
Programmes follow international best practice social learning theory. Social learning theory
guidelines instituted by the National Institute for posits that learning is a cognitive process which
Health and Care Excellence in the United occurs through observation or direct instruction
Kingdom and are cited by the UK Department of in a social context (Bandura, 1971). Secondly, a
Education as empirically supported parent train- standardised video-assisted curriculum is
ing programmes. employed ensuring consistency in programme
dissemination across facilitators and delivery set-
tings (Weisz, 2004). Also in common with
Goals of the Parents Incredible Years and Triple P, cognitive-­
Plus Programmes behavioural principles and concepts are incorpo-
rated and utilised in the Parents Plus Programmes
The overarching objective of the three age-­ (Sanders et al., 2014). Furthermore, the practices
specific Parents Plus Programmes, which are the of promoting peer support within the training
focus of this chapter, is to help parents foster group and balancing discipline management with
positive relationships with their children by positive parent-child relationship building are
advancing their skills for promoting prosocial shared by these three distinct intervention models
behaviour and taking a non-coercive approach to (Carr, Hartnett, Brosnan, & Sharry, 2016).
discipline. Specifically, the Parents Plus Early One of the unique aspects of the Parents Plus
Years Programme aims to help parents maximise Programmes which distinguishes them from
their children’s learning, language and social other empirically supported and widely used par-
development, to reduce behaviour problems and ent training models is that they were developed in
also to ensure that their children grow up happy partnership with parents, children and young
and emotionally secure. The Parents Plus people. Much of the DVD teaching footage that
Children’s Programme also aims to help parents is central to the programme delivery contains
curtail children’s emotional and behaviour prob- clips of real parenting scenes from families who
lems through positive communication and disci- attended child and family services and who
pline and to develop more satisfying parent-child ­volunteered to share this information for the ben-
relationships. Similarly, the aims of the Parents efit of others. The corresponding DVD for each
Plus Adolescents Programme are to help parents Parents Plus Programme features comments from
foster good relationships with their teenage chil- parents, children and young people as to how the
dren while also engaging effective communica- ideas work in their contexts. Furthermore, the
Using Parent Training Programmes to Teach Social Skills 205

Parents Plus Programmes are culturally sensitive (Carr et al., 2016). Furthermore, participant
to the Irish context. involvement is a key feature at all stages of the
Also unique to the Parents Plus Programmes programme delivery, and an interactive and col-
is their systemic solution-focused basis (Carr laborative format is adopted throughout. During
et al., 2016). Derived from the systemic family each group session, one positive parenting topic
therapy tradition, solution-focused brief ther- (e.g. using encouragement and praise) and one
apy emerged in the 1980s (see deShazer et al., positive discipline topic (e.g. using conse-
1986) and presented an alternative to the quences) are explored with group discussion, role
pathology-­centred problem-focused approaches play, observation and behavioural rehearsal being
which prevailed in psychotherapy (Sharry, used to facilitate learning. Participant feedback
2007). A principal assumption of solution- on session content, group dynamics and self-­
focused therapy is that clients possess most of perceived progress is also formally collected fol-
the strength and resources necessary to resolve lowing each session which facilitators are
their own problems (George, Iveson & Ratner, encouraged to review and adapt session content
1990; as cited in Sharry, 2007). Within this accordingly. Course outlines for the three age-­
approach, the client is encouraged to identify, specific Parents Plus Programmes are provided in
focus and draw on ‘what’s right and what’s Table 1 below.
working’ (i.e. the strengths, skills and resources
existing within them as individuals, their
families and communities), thereby enabling  esearch Base for the Parents
R
self-healing and generating a sense of empow- Plus Programmes
erment (Sharry, 2007 pp. 8). In the context of
group therapy, the solution-focused approach Several evaluation studies attest to the effective-
espouses group support (achieved through ness of the Parents Plus Programmes in a variety
affinity with and acceptance of others), group of clinical, disability and community settings
learning (through interpersonal communication (Coughlin, Sharry, Fitzpatrick, Guerin, &
and collaboration), group optimism (by instill- Drumm, 2009; Fitzpatrick, Beattie, O’Donohoe,
ing hope that change is possible), group empow- & Guerin, 2007; Gerber, Sharry, Streek, & Mc
erment (finding strength through shared Kenna, 2015; Griffin, Guerin, Sharry, & Drumm,
experiences) and facilitating the opportunity to 2010; Hand, McDonnell, Honari, & Sharry,
help others (by making meaningful contribu- 2013; Hand et al., 2012; Hayes, Siraj-Blatchford,
tions and being valued in return). Taking a Keegan, & Goulding, 2013; Keating, Sharry,
strength-­based approach to working with par- Murphy, Rooney, & Carr, 2016; Kilroy, Sharry,
ents is a key element of the Parents Plus Flood, & Guerin, 2011; Lonergan, Gerber,
Programmes. Facilitators are encouraged to Streek, & Sharry, 2015; Nitsch, Hannon, Rickard,
focus actively on participants’ strengths during Houghton, & Sharry, 2015; Rickard et al., 2015;
sessions, with a view to building their self- Sharry, Guerin, Griffin, & Drumm, 2005; Wynne,
esteem, so that the group process models the Doyle, Kenny, Brosnan, & Sharry, 2016). A 2016
positive solution-­focused attitude parents are meta-analysis (Carr et al., 2016) of 17 evaluation
encouraged to adopt with their children. studies on all five Parents Plus Programmes
In line with the principles of solution-focused involving over 1000 families demonstrated that
therapy, the Parents Plus model proposes that the Parents Plus Programmes can reduce parental
parents themselves are the initiators of positive stress, increase parental satisfaction and improve
change within their own families, and a coopera- clinical and subclinical emotional and b­ ehavioural
tive and assertive parenting style is promoted. problems in children and adolescents both with
Likewise, the intervention process for each pro- and without developmental difficulties. The
gramme begins with facilitators and participants Programmes were also found to have a signifi-
collaboratively identifying client-centred goals cant impact on participants’ therapeutic goal
206 J. Sharry and C. Doyle

Table 1  Course outlines and session content for the three age-specific Parents Plus Programmes
Parents Plus Early Years Parents Plus Children’s Parents Plus Adolescents
Session Programme Programme Programme
1 •  Tuning in to your child •  Providing positive attention •  Parenting teenagers
•  Pressing the pause button (in •  Pressing the pause button •  Positive communication
response to misbehaviour) when responding to
misbehaviour
2 •  Child-centred play and •  Setting aside play and special •  Getting to know your
communication time teenager
•  Taking the lead with children •  Using do’s rather than don’ts •  Establishing rules
3 •  Child-centred play and •  Child-centred play •  Connecting with your
communication •  Establishing routines teenager
•  Establishing routines using •  Communicating rules
rewards and picture charts positively
4 •  Encouraging and supporting •  Encouragement and praise •  Encouraging your teenager
your child •  Using consequences •  Communicating rules
•  The ‘praise-ignore’ principle positively
5 •  Ensuring encouragement gets •  Encouraging homework and •  Listening to your teenager
through learning •  Having a discipline plan
•  Dealing with misbehaviour •  Using sanction systems
using consequences
6 •  Helping children learn through •  Prevention plans •  Empowering teenagers
play and reading books •  Assertive parenting and •  Dealing with conflict and
•  Step-by-step discipline dealing with disrespect aggression
7 •  Teaching children new tasks •  Problem-solving with children • Problem-solving
•  Teaching children the skills to •  Step-by-step discipline •  Dealing with specific
behave well issues
8 •  Creative play activities •  Active listening and •  Dealing with specific
•  Solutions to specific problems problem-solving issues
and issues •  Dealing with special needs •  Parent self-care
9 •  Teaching new skills using books •  Family listening and
•  Parent self-care problem-solving
•  Parent self-care
Additional In clinical settings parents receive In clinical settings parents may In clinical settings two
sessions up to five additional individual receive two additional individual conjoint family sessions
sessions to coach them in skills sessions (which may take a may be held after sessions 3
covered in group sessions and conjoint family format) to coach and 6 to address specific
focus on attaining their specific them in skills covered in group parent-adolescent issues and
goals. Parent-child interactions sessions and focus on attaining goals. Ideally the working
are also video recorded and specific goals. Vulnerable things out programme is run
reviewed collaboratively with parents may be offered in parallel with the parents
parents who are given strength- telephone support plus adolescents Programme
focused feedback
Source: Parts of this table were adapted from Carr et al. (2016)

attainment. However, better child- and parent-­ domised controlled trials or conducted
level outcomes were achieved in smaller ran- intention-to-treat analyses, there is substantial
domised trials involving families whose children evidence to suggest that the programmes are
were younger and had less severe problems, who effective for families with children of all ages and
attended more sessions and who had less concur- for separated families (Carr et al., 2016).
rent child intervention. Furthermore, the effect size of 0.57 from Carr
While the evidence base for the Parents Plus et al.’s (2016) meta-analysis of ten controlled
Programmes is not without limitations, for exam- Parents Plus evaluation studies compared favour-
ple, few of the evaluation studies were ran- ably to those of meta-analyses of the Incredible
Using Parent Training Programmes to Teach Social Skills 207

Years (Menting et al., 2013) and Triple P (Sanders courses. Firstly, such a focus removes the need to
et al., 2014) programmes which were 0.3 and identify either the child or the parent as being to
0.47, respectively. blame for the problems experienced, and it can also
increase the parent’s empathy for the child.
Secondly, having a focus on encouraging newly
 arents Plus and Teaching
P identified skills empowers parents to take construc-
Social Skills tive action with regard to teaching their children.
Thirdly, as they make progress, both the parent and
A central principle of the Parents Plus approach child feel successful; this not only boosts their self-
to overcoming emotional and behavioural prob- esteem and confidence but also helps to improve
lems in children and teenagers is to focus on their relationship with one another.
teaching social skills. Within the programmes, To illustrate this process in action, there are
parents are invited to view their children’s misbe- case examples which are used to teach different
haviour as examples of ‘missing’ social skills. In ‘social skills’ strategies in the Parents Plus
order words, children misbehave because they Programmes detailed below.
have not yet fully learnt the skill of behaving
well. For example:
 arents Plus Early Years Programme:
P
• A child who is hitting out in frustration is Picture Schedules
doing this because he has not yet learnt the
social skill of expressing his feelings rather Within the Parents Plus Early Years Programme,
than taking them out on other people. parents are given input on a variety of strategies
• A child, who has a meltdown due to anxiety, to help their children learn new social skills and
has not yet learnt the social skills needed to to behave well. These include praising children’s
resolve the situation that caused the anxiety in engagement in the desired behaviour when it is
the first place. seen, modelling positive behaviours to the chil-
• A child who gets into trouble in the classroom dren, guiding children step by step as they
for wandering out of his seat has simply not engage in the positive behaviours themselves
yet learnt the skill of concentrating for and reading story books together that emphasise
extended periods at a table. the desired skills (e.g. such as a story about a
• A child who gets into fights with friends has little bear who learns to be gentle with his baby
not yet learnt the social skills of sharing or brother, etc.).
resolving conflict amicably. One of the challenges when teaching young
children is the fact that their language skills may
The focus of the Parents Plus Programmes is be underdeveloped, meaning that they find it
to support parents in teaching their children these more difficult to understand their parents’ and
missing social skills, rather than simply correct- caregivers’ verbal instructions and guidance.
ing misbehaviour. Parents and caregivers attend- This is especially the case for children with spe-
ing Parents Plus courses are encouraged to: cial needs, and indeed many of the behaviour
problems that these children display are related
1 . Pause and closely observe their children. to their lack of language (in comparison to their
2. ‘Tune in’ to their child and identify what they peers). A very helpful strategy, which does not
need to learn. rely on verbal language alone, involves teaching
3. Think of a plan of action as to how they can children using picture schedules (see Fig. 1 below
support their children to learn, making sure to for examples). In the Parents Plus Early Years
build on what they know already. Programme parents are shown several video
examples of parents using picture schedules with
There are a number of advantages to having this their children, and they are also given detailed
positive social skills focus throughout Parents Plus hand-outs on the principles of using picture
208 J. Sharry and C. Doyle

Playing Together

Daddy rolls Daddy’s turn Kate rolls the Kate’s turn Happy playing
the dice dice together

Getting Dressed

Pants on T-shirt on Shorts on Sandals on You’re


dressed!

Fig. 1  Examples of picture schedules for young children

schedules. Within the group sessions, the parents 3 . Thinking up solutions


are provided with the materials to make a picture 4. Agreeing on a plan
schedule, and during individual family sessions
with their children, they are coached in how to Parents are provided with a series of pictorial
explain the picture schedules to their children and worksheets which they can complete along with
put them into practice in the home. their children, hence giving their children the
opportunity to participate in the problem-solving
process and to think about the best way to resolve
Parents Plus Children’s Programme: problems through the use of good social skills.
Problem-Solving See an example of one of the worksheets in Fig. 2
below.
As the Parents Plus Children’s Programme targets Within the Parents Plus model, parents are
older children in middle childhood (from 6 to supported both in the context of the group and
11 years), there is more scope for using verbal individually in family sessions with their children
methods to teach them desired social skills. In to put the Parents Plus problem-solving model
Parents Plus Children’s Programme courses, par- into action. The dialogue given below is a real
ents are invited to utilise a simple four-step problem-­ example of a parent who used the problem-­
solving model with their children which involves: solving model to help his son learn how to man-
age conflict with other children without hitting.
1. Picking a good time and place to talk This conversation between father and son was
2. Listening to everyone’s point of view video recorded in the family’s home, and with
Using Parent Training Programmes to Teach Social Skills 209

Fig. 2  Parents Plus


Children’s Programme
problem-solving
worksheet

their permission the footage collected was used out without hitting... how do you feel when it
as part of the teaching DVD for the Parents Plus happens?
Children’s Programme so that other parents can Son: Annoyed.
observe the procedure and learn the skills Father: Do you feel very annoyed?
involved. Son: Yeah.
Father: And then what happens?
Father: Right, we’re here to talk about your Son: I just hit them.
problem about hitting... Father: And how do you think that makes them
Son: Yeah... (looks down) feel?
Father: OK? We have to sort something out. So, Son: Sore.
we need to find other ways of sorting problems Father: So, what do you think we can do about it?
210 J. Sharry and C. Doyle

Son: I don’t know. encouraging. When shown to parents during the


Father: We’ll have to think of a few ideas... can Children’s Programme course, parents are asked
you think of any? Well, what do you think you to identify the skills the father is using through-
should do when people are annoying you? out his interaction with his son and to think about
Son: Tell them to stop and then go and tell you or how they could implement a similar problem-­
Mam. solving model with their own children.
Father: You could tell them to stop and if they
don’t stop then you could call me or Mammy,
and we’ll tell them to stop. Is there anything  arents Plus Adolescents Programme:
P
else you can do? Teaching Responsibility
Son: No.
Father: Did you ever think of counting? A central objective of the Parents Plus Adolescents
Son: No. Programme is to promote and develop effective
Father: You could count to ten so you didn’t get communication skills such as listening, speaking
very angry. Would you like to try something up assertively, negotiating and collaborative
like that? problem-solving among young people aged
Son: No. 11–16 years. In a similar manner to the Parents
Father: Why not? Plus Children’s Programme, parents participating
Son: Cause it wouldn’t work. in the Adolescents Programme are provided with
Father: Why do you think it won’t work? a step-by-step problem-solving model for resolv-
Son: I don’t know. ing conflict with their adolescents and for agree-
Father: OK, any other ideas? ing on new desired ways of behaving. In addition,
Son: No. there is a focus on encouraging independence
Father: So, you ask them to stop... or you…? and giving adolescents responsibility in order to
Son: Tell you or Mammy. help them learn social skills for life. To demon-
Father: OK. Are you going to try that? strate and convey these ideas, parents are invited
Son: Yeah. to firstly view video footage of parents teaching
Father: Is it going to be hard? their children household tasks such as laundry
Son: Yeah. and ironing. Following this, group discussion is
Father: Why do you think it’ll be hard? used to consolidate learning, and parents are then
Son: I don’t know. asked to complete planning worksheets such as
Father: Well, you don’t do it all the time, you can the one in Fig. 3. By completing the worksheet
be very good, and you can play with your within the groups, parents are given time and
brothers and sisters, can’t you? space to develop a plan as to how they will teach
Son: Yeah. their teenagers identified social skills in the
Father: And you can have fun together. home. In subsequent group sessions, the parents
Son: Yeah. are invited to report on how their teens responded
Father: So when you start getting annoyed, and the progress that they made. Further group
you’re going to try to tell Mam and Dad, or support is provided as needed.
ask them to stop, aren’t you?
Son: Yeah.
Father: Good lad. Teaching Parents Skills

Within the Parents Plus Children’s Programme, The teaching skills focus at the heart of the
the scene detailed above serves as a good teach- Parents Plus Programmes is not just for the chil-
ing example of parent and child problem-solving dren, but it is also for the parents. Just as the chil-
together. The key skills involved for parents are dren are being taught social skills, the parents are
listening carefully, asking the child to come up also being taught parenting skills for communi-
with solutions themselves and being really cating effectively with their children. A ‘skills
Using Parent Training Programmes to Teach Social Skills 211

Worksheet: Helping Young People be Responsible

Look at the list of household and personal tasks below (all of which can be handed
over to teenagers as they get older) and mark those your teenagers can do
already, and those they need to learn. Add others to the list as you think of them.
Tasks I might teach:
Weekly household Changing the oil in Washing up/ironing
shopping the car Cooking meals
Painting a room Planting Wiring an electric
Washing clothes flowers/vegetables plug
Cleaning the Getting up in the Settling their own
windows Choosing morning squabbles
their own clothes Locking doors at Cleaning the
Paying bills night house
Chopping firewood Mending an Caring for a
Making their own electrical fuse younger child
life decisions Mowing the lawn
My Plan
Pick one task that you want to hand over to your teenager(s) and plan how you
might do this.
Task:
What parts of the task can your child already do?

What is the next step for him/her to learn?

Plan to teach him/her:

To make it work:
Take time out to sit down and talk to children, explain why you want to hand
it over to them (e.g. a fairer system, teaching them responsibility).
Agree to show them how to do a task if needed and agree on what the
rewards for completing the task will be, and the consequences for not.
Make sure to talk again at a later date to review how they got on.

Fig. 3  Parents Plus Adolescents Programme worksheet on helping young people be responsible

focus’ is a very helpful way to address emotional the problems they face. See an example of this
and behavioural problems in children and process below.
­adolescents as it avoids blame being placed on
either the parent or child, and it focuses both par-  xample: Coaching a Parent to Resolve
E
ents and children on concrete progress that they a Sibling Conflict
can make. In addition, within a strength-based A mother attended the Parents Plus Early Years
approach to parent training, the goal is to build on Programme with her 4-year-old son Jamie in
the parents’ strengths and on what are already order to address his tantrums and difficult behav-
doing right. The goal is to highlight that they iour, particularly during fights with his younger
already have many of the skills they need to solve sister Katie. The facilitator helped the mother
212 J. Sharry and C. Doyle

identify a clear prosocial goal for her children, of coaching) carrying out these skills. This made
namely, to help Jamie get along better with Katie. the learning process very empowering for her.
As part of the intervention, the Parents Plus facil-
itator conducted a family session during which a
video recording of the mother playing with her Summary and Conclusion
two children was made, while she was being
coached for managing a potential conflict With the widespread recognition of the influ-
between the children. Most of the work focused ence of parents and primary caregivers on chil-
on providing feedback to the mother on how she dren’s social development, parent training
was already providing positive attention to both emerged as a treatment for emotional and
children and helping them play in parallel. behavioural problems occurring in childhood
During a second family session, a second and adolescence, as well as a preventative inter-
video recording was taken while this particular vention. By equipping parents with the neces-
mother and her two children played alongside sary skills, parent training seeks to reduce
one another, and a minor tantrum occurred. Jamie children’s emotional and behavioural problems
began to grab a toy from his sister without asking and to promote positive child adjustment.
for it. The mother said ‘Don’t grab’ and gave the Extensive research has demonstrated that behav-
toy back to the sister. Jamie protested angrily and ioural and cognitive-behavioural group-based
the mother argued with him in a loud voice. Jamie parent training interventions such as Incredible
became angrier and raised his hand in a threaten- Years and Triple P are effective in terms of
ing way. The facilitator at this point intervened reducing child-focused problems and improving
(while filming) and suggested to the mother that parental mental health (Furlong et al., 2013).
she pull away from Jamie and turn her attention Another such model is offered by Parents Plus;
to Katie instead. Jamie continued to protest however, its programmes are distinguished from
somewhat and the facilitator encouraged the other evidence-based parent training interven-
mother to remain relaxed and to play with Katie. tions by their systemic and solution-focused
He suggested that the mother simply say ‘Jamie theoretical basis (Carr et al., 2016). Within the
is angry right now. When he calms down he can Parents Plus model, parents are invited to view
come back in and play’. A minute later, Jamie children’s emotional and behavioural problems
slowly moved back towards his mother. The as the absence of a particular social skill (i.e. a
facilitator said ‘I think Jamie is ready to come ‘missing’ skill). That is, children’s misbehav-
back now. Maybe show him what he can play iour is attributed to the fact that they have not
with’. The mother then turned to Jamie and asked yet fully learnt the skill of behaving well or in a
‘Come on and play with the Lego?’. Jamie sat prosocial way. Using different developmentally
close to his mother as she helped him get started. appropriate strategies and techniques such as
During a review of the video, the mother was picture schedules and step-by-step problem-
fascinated to learn that pausing and pulling back solving, the Parents Plus Early Years, Children’s
for a moment could work in terms of preventing and Adolescents Programmes help parents help
Jamie’s tantrum and difficult behaviour. She their children learn new social skills and ulti-
described how she would normally argue or shout mately how to behave well.
at her son in these situations and acknowledged
that taking that approach would often make
things worse. The facilitator highlighted the skills References
involved in her success, namely, remaining calm,
turning away completely for a moment and cru- Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy.
American Psychologist, 44(4), 709–716.
cially returning positive attention once he started Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S.
to behave well. These skills were all illustrated in (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study
the video snippet. Rather than being ‘taught’, the of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
mother could see herself on tape (with a little bit
Using Parent Training Programmes to Teach Social Skills 213

Bandura, A. (1971). Social learning theory. New York, Darling, N. (2007). Ecological systems theory: The per-
NY: General Learning Press. son in the center of the circles. Research in Human
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying Development, 4(3-4), 203–217.
theory of behavioural change. Psychological Review, deShazer, S., Berg, I. K., Lipchik, E., Nunnally, F.,
34(2), 191–215. Molnar, A., Gingerich, W. J., & Weiner-Davis, M.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and (1986). Brief therapy: Focused solution development.
action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, Family Process, 25(2), 207–221.
NJ: Prentice Hall. Feldman, R., Bamberger, E., & Kanat-Maymon, Y.
Barlow, J., Smailagic, N., Huband, N., Roloff, V., & (2013). Parent-specific reciprocity from infancy to
Bennett, C. (2012). Group-based parent training adolescence shapes children's social competence and
programmes for improving parental psychosocial dialogical skills. Attachment & Human Development,
health. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 6, 15(4), 407–423.
CD002020. Fitzpatrick, C., Beattie, D., O’Donohoe, P. & Guerin, S.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss (volume 1: (2007) Parent management training for adolescent
Attachment). New York, NY: Basic Books. mental health disorders – a controlled trial. 54th
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss (volume 2: Annual Meeting of the American Academy of Child
Separation: Anxiety and anger). New York, NY: Basic & Adolescent Psychiatry, Boston, MA, USA, 23–28th
Books. Oct 2007.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss (volume 3: Loss). Furlong, M., McGilloway, S., Bywater, T., Hutchings, J.,
New York, NY: Basic Books. Smith, S., & Donnelly, M. (2013). Behavioural and
Bretherton, I. (1992). The origins of attachment theory: cognitive-behavioural group-based parenting pro-
John Bowlby & Mary Ainsworth. Developmental grams for early-onset conduct problems in children
Psychology, 28(5), 759–775. aged 3 to 12 years. Evidence-Based Child Health: A
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human devel- Cochrane Review Journal, 8(2), 318–692.
opment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Gerber, S., Sharry, J., Streek, A., & Mc Kenna, G. (2015).
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a Parent training: Effectiveness of the parents plus
context for human development: Research perspec- early years Programme in community preschool set-
tives. Developmental Psychology, 22(6), 723–742. tings. European Early Childhood Education Research
Bronfenbrenner, U. (2000). Ecological systems theory. Journal, 24(5), 602–614.
In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology Griffin, C., Guerin, S., Sharry, J., & Drumm, M. (2010). A
(vol. 3, pp. 129–133). Washington, DC: American multi-centre controlled study of an early intervention
Psychological Association. parenting programme for young children with behav-
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology ioural and developmental difficulties. International
of developmental processes. In W. Damon & R. M. Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology, 10(2),
Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1: 279–294.
Theoretical models of human development (5th ed.pp. Hand, A., McDonnell, E., Honari, B., & Sharry, J. (2013).
993–1023). New York: John Wiley and Sons, Inc. A community-led approach to delivery of the parents
Brosnan, E., Sharry, J., Beattie, D., & Fitzpatrick, C. plus Children’s Programme for the parents of chil-
(2011). Working things out: An evidence-based mental dren aged 6-11. International Journal of Clinical and
health resource for professionals working with young Health Psychology, 13, 81–90.
people in clinical and community settings (3rd ed.). Hand, A., Ni Raghallaigh, C., Cuppage, J., Coyle, S., &
Dublin: Parents Plus. Sharry, J. (2012). A controlled clinical evaluation of
Carr, A. (2014). The evidence base for family therapy and the parents plus Children’s Programme for parents
systemic interventions for child-focused problems. of children aged 6–12 with mild intellectual disabil-
Journal of Family Therapy, 36(2), 107–157. ity in a school setting. Clinical Child Psychology and
Carr, A., Hartnett, D., Brosnan, E., & Sharry, J. (2016). Psychiatry, 18(4), 536–555.
Parents plus systemic, solution-focused parent train- Haven, E. L., Manangan, C. N., Sparrow, J. K., & Wilson,
ing programs: Description, review of the evidence B. J. (2014). The relation of parent-child interaction
base, and meta analysis. Family Process. doi:10.1111/ qualities to social skills in children with and without
famp.12225 autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 18(3), 292–300.
Chronis, A. M., Chacko, A., Fabiano, G. A., Wymbs, B. T., Hayes, N., Siraj-Blatchford, I., Keegan, S., & Goulding,
& Pelham, W. E. (2004). Enhancements to the behav- E. (2013). Evaluation of the early years Programme
ioral parent training paradigm for families of children of the childhood development initiative. Childhood
with ADHD: Review and future directions. Clinical Development Initiative (CDI): Dublin.
Child and Family Psychology Review, 7(1), 1–27. Jaffari-Bimmel, N., Juffer, F., van IJzendoorn, M. H.,
Coughlin, M., Sharry, J., Fitzpatrick, C., Guerin, S., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Mooijaart, A.
Drumm, M. (2009). A controlled clinical evaluation (2006). Social development from infancy to ado-
of the parents plus Children’s Programme: A video-­ lescence: Longitudinal and concurrent factors in an
based programme for parents of children with behav- adoption sample. Developmental Psychology, 42(6),
ioural and developmental problems. Clinical Child 1143–1153.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 14(4), 541–558.
214 J. Sharry and C. Doyle

Kaminski, J. W., Valle, L. A., Filene, J. H., & Boyle, lescent Programme in schools. Child and Adolescent
C. L. (2008). A meta-analytic review of components Psychiatry and Mental Health, 9, 43.
associated with parent training program effective- Rickard, E. D., Brosnan, E., O’Laoide, A., Wynne, C.,
ness. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36(4), Keane, M., McCormack, M. & Sharry, J. (2015). A
567–589. first-level evaluation of a school-based family pro-
Keating, A., Sharry, J., Murphy, M., Rooney, B., & gramme for adolescent social, emotional and behav-
Carr, A. (2016). An evaluation of the parents plus – ioural difficulties Clinical Child Psychology and
Parenting when separated programme. Clinical Child Psychiatry. 21(4):603–617. ISSN:1359-1045
Psychology and Psychiatry, 21(2), 240–254. Sanders, M. R. (2012). Development, evaluation, and mul-
Kilroy, S., Sharry, J., Flood, C., & Guerin, S. (2011). tinational dissemination of the triple P-positive parent-
Parent training in the community: Linking process to ing program. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology,
outcome. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 8, 1–35.
16(3), 459–473. Sanders, M. R., Kirby, J. N., Tellegen, C. L., & Day, J. J.
Kostelnik, M., Whiren, A., Soderman, A., Rupiper, M. L., (2014). The triple P-positive parenting program: A
& Gregory, K. (2008). Guiding children’s social systematic review and meta-analysis of a multi-level
development and learning: Theory and skills (8th ed.). system of parenting support. Clinical Psychology
Stanford: Cengage Learning. Review, 34(4), 337–357.
Lonergan, A., Gerber, S., Streek, A., & Sharry, J. (2015). Shaffer, D. R., & Kipp, K. (2010). Developmental psy-
Parenting groups, how long is enough? The efficacy of chology: Childhood and adolescence (8th ed.).
a community-run parents plus early years Programme Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning.
as a preschool parenting intervention of modifiable Sharry, J. (2007). Solution-focused Groupwork (2nd ed.).
duration. Global Journal of Community Psychology London: Sage.
Practice, 6(1), 1–13. Sharry, J., & Fitzpatrick, C. (1997). Parents plus
Lundahl, B. W., Risser, H. J., & Lovejoy, M. C. (2006). Programme: A practical and positive video-based
A meta-analysis of parent training: Moderators and course for managing and solving discipline problems
follow-up effects. Clinical Psychology Review, 26(1), in children. Dublin: Parents Plus.
86–104. Sharry, J., & Fitzpatrick, C. (2007). Parents plus
Maccoby, E. E. (2007). Historical overview of social- Children’s Programme: A video-based parent-
ization research and theory. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. ing guide to managing behaviour problems and
Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory ­promoting learning in children aged six to eleven.
and research (pp. 13–41). New York, NY: Guilford Dublin: Parents Plus.
Press. Sharry, J., & Fitzpatrick, C. (2012). Parents plus adoles-
Main, M., & Solomon, J. (1986). Discovery of an insecure cents Programme: A DVD-based parenting guide to
disoriented attachment pattern: Procedures, findings handling conflict, solving problems and getting on bet-
and implications for the classification of behavior. In ter with older children and young people aged 11–16
T. Brazelton & M. Youngman (Eds.), Affective devel- years (3rd ed.). Dublin: Parents Plus.
opment in infancy. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Sharry, J., Guerin, S., Griffin, C., & Drumm, M. (2005). An
Meece, J. L., & Daniels, D. H. (2008). Child and ado- evaluation of the parents plus early years Programme:
lescent development for educators. New York, NY: A video-based early intervention for parents of pre-
McGraw-Hill. school children with behavioural and developmental
Menting, A. T. A., Orobio, D. C., & Matthys, W. (2013). difficulties. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Effectiveness of the incredible years parent training 10(3), 319–336.
to modify disruptive and prosocial child behavior: A Sharry, J., Hampson, G., & Fanning, M. (2013). Parents
meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, plus early years Programme: A DVD-based parent-
901–913. ing course on promoting development and managing
Murphy, C., Scantlebury, K., & Milne, C. (2015). Using behaviour problems in young children aged 1–6 (3rd
Vygotsky’s zone of proximal development to propose ed.). Dublin: Parents Plus.
and test an explanatory model for conceptualising Sharry, J., Keating, A., & Murphy, M. (2012). Parents
co-teaching in pre-service science teacher education. plus – Parenting when separated Programme: A posi-
Asia-Pacific Journal of Teacher Education, 43(4), tive guide to helping families manage separation and
281–295. divorce. Dublin: Parents Plus.
NCCMH. (2009). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Siegler, R., DeLoache, J., & Eisenberg, N. (2011). How
The NICE guideline on diagnosis and management of children develop (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Worth
ADHD in children, young people and adults. London: Publishers.
Alden Press. Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language
Newman, B. M., & Newman, P. R. (2012). Life-span (E. Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Eds & Trans.). Cambridge,
development: A psychosocial approach (11th ed.). MA: MIT Press. (Original work published in 1934).
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development
Nitsch, E., Hannon, G., Rickard, E., Houghton, S., & of higher mental processes (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner,
Sharry, J. (2015). Positive parenting: A randomised S. Scribner & E. Souberman, Eds.). Cambridge, MA:
controlled trial evaluation of the parents plus ado-
Using Parent Training Programmes to Teach Social Skills 215

Harvard University Press. (Original work published in Wynne, C., Doyle, C., Kenny, R., Brosnan, E., & Sharry,
1930, 1933, 1935). J. (2016). A first-level evaluation of a family interven-
Webster-Stratton, C. (1998). Preventing conduct prob- tion for adolescent social, emotional and behavioural
lems in head start children: Strengthening parenting difficulties in child and adolescent mental health ser-
competencies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical vices. Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health,
Psychology, 66(5), 715–730. 28(1), 33–46.
Weisz, J. R. (2004). Psychotherapy for children and ado-
lescents: Evidence-based treatments and case exam-
ples. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum
Disorders

Chieko Kanai, Gabor Toth, Miho Kuroda,


Atsuko Miyake, and Takashi Itahashi

Autism Spectrum Disorders PDDNOS were unified as one diagnostic category


of ASD (APA, 2013). The integration of the three
Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are character- diagnostic subtypes would support a concept of
ized by markedly abnormal or impaired develop- continuous behavior and several corresponding
ment in social communication, a restricted and factors in ASD (McPartland & Law, 2016).
stereotyped repertoire of activities and interests, ASD were once thought to be extremely rare,
and atypical response to sensory stimuli. Based on affecting two to four persons in a population of
the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental 10,000 (Pickles et al., 1995), but they have
Disorders (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric recently attracted great attention. According to
Association, 2000), which is a previous version the Autism and Developmental Disabilities
of DSM-5, autism was categorized as the three Monitoring Network, as sponsored by the Centers
subtypes of autistic disorders, Asperger’s disor- for Disease Control and Prevention, the popula-
der, and pervasive developmental disorder not tion prevalence of ASD is estimated at 1 in 68
otherwise specified (PDD-NOS) (APA, 2000). children in the United States (Centers for Disease
The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Control (CDC), 2014). ASD occur in males five
Health Disorders Fifth Edition 1 (DSM-5) was times more than in females. The reasons for
published in May 2013, and the three subtypes increasing the number of diagnoses might corre-
of autistic disorders, Asperger’s disorder, and late with a change of diagnostic criteria and

C. Kanai, Ph.D. (*) • T. Itahashi, Ph.D. M. Kuroda, Ph.D.


Medical Institute of Developmental Disabilities School of Human Care Studies, Nagoya University of
Research, Showa University, 6-11-11 Arts and Sciences, 57 Takenoyama, Iwasaki-cho,
Kitakarasuyama, Setagayaku, Tokyo 157-8577, Japan Nishin-shi, Aichi 470-0196, Japan
e-mail: ckanai@med.showa-u.ac.jp; e-mail: mihok-tky@umin.ac.jp
chikanai1003320@gmail.com; itapan322@gmail.com
A. Miyake, M.S.
G. Toth, Ph.D. Department of Child and Adolescent Mental Health,
Department of Education and Child Studies, Faculty National Institute of Mental Health,
of Arts & Sciences, Sagami Women’s University, 4-1-4 Ogawahigashi-cho, Kodaira-shi, Tokyo
2-1-1 Bunkyo, Minami-ku, Sagamihara-shi, 187-8553, Japan
Kanagawa 252-0383, Japan e-mail: atsuko-m@sky.sannet.ne.jp
e-mail: gtoth.kodomo@gmail.com

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 217


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_13
218 C. Kanai et al.

increase of information from the media. On the Table 1 Early signs of ASD suspect
other hand, the number of diagnoses might Social development by the first year of life
practically increase in ASD because of environ- • Avoid or poor eye contact
mental influences such as neurotoxic chemicals • Lack of interest in other children (except siblings)
• Absence of social smile
that affect the brain and behavior development of
• Lack of response to name (lack of social
ASD (Schwatzer, Koening, & Berman, 2013). responsiveness)
In this chapter, we discuss the clinical charac- • Lack of separation and stranger anxiety
teristics, assessment, treatment, and brain func- Social development by 1½ year-old
tion of social skills in children with ASD. • Lack of showing or pointing out objects of interest
to others
• Lack of looking at things, following pointing
• Lack of looking at things, following gaze
Early Clinical Diagnosis of ASD • Lack or failure to share enjoyment, excitement, or
achievements with others
Clinical diagnosis of ASD is usually demon- • Negative affect
• Reduced ability to understand language (whether a
strated in children between 18 and 36 months child understands instructions without gesture)
(Mandell, Novak, & Zubritsky, 2005; Zachor & • Lack of gesture for movement or language
Curatolo, 2014). Early signs of ASD are poor eye (whether a child can pretend close people)
contact, lack of interest in other children, social • Quality of play (a child can play with toys
functionally, instead of dedicating sensory play
smiling, name response, stranger anxiety, point- such as dropping, getting his/her mouth around)
ing to interesting things, showing interesting • Absence of interest for other children
things to caregivers, poor ability to understand • Lack or low level of empathy and emotional
language and gestures, failure in imitation, and reactivity to others
• Limited range of facial expression
differences in feeding behavior. Although both
Social development no matter what is the
identification and diagnosis are usually made developmental level and age of a child
after the age of 3 years (Bolton, Golding, Emond, • Poor asking to be carried (holding him/her) by the
& Steer, 2012; Howlin & Asgharian, 1999), early caregiver
identification of ASD could often occur before • Concerns about sleep problems (circadian rhythm
issues)
the age of 3 years (see Table 1).
Bolton et al. (2012) indicated that differences
in social skills and fine motor were evident from 3 years of age has only partially been detected
as early as 6 months of age and were accompa- (Bolton et al., 2012; Howlin et al., 1999). The
nied by differences in communication skills and early screening and clinical assessments for ASD
concerns over vision. Caregivers continued to be will be helpful for detecting early signs, and
concerned about vision, and, by 15 months of Zachor et al. (2014) have shown practical recom-
age, this was accompanied by concerns about mendations for ASD screening and clinical
hearing, vocabulary, understanding of words, and assessments. ASD diagnosis requires three levels
problems feeding. Further characteristics of ASD of screening procedure, namely, Level 1, screen-
were apparent by 18 months of age, with caregiv- ing tools for ASD, which could be used by gen-
ers being concerned about hearing and marked eral pediatricians; Level 2, assessment of Level 1
differences in social and motor skills, listening/ abnormalities at specialized developmental clin-
response to sounds, communication, and play ics such as child and adolescent psychiatric clin-
behavior. By 15–24 months of age, repetitive and ics; and Level 3, tests used in ASD special clinics.
unusual behaviors and differences in tempera- ASD screening and clinical assessments com-
ment were apparent. prise CHAT (Checklist for Autism in Toddlers),
In contrast, clarifying the difference between M-CHAT (Modified Checklist for Autism in
ASD and typically developing (TD) children in Toddlers), CSBS DP (Communication and
earlier development stages is difficult, because Symbolic Behavior Scales Developmental
the emergence of signs of ASD during the first Profile) Infant-Toddler Checklist, and PDDSY-II
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 219

(Pervasive Developmental Disorders Screening terns of infants with ASD using video clips of
Test-II) in Level 1, SCQ (Social Communication social interaction and multidimensional scaling,
Questionnaire) and SRS (Social Responsiveness which showed that each subject with similar gaze
Scale) in Level 2, and ADI-R (Autism Diagnostic patterns would cluster together in a two-
Interview-Revised) and ADOS (Autism dimensional plane. The infants of the control
Diagnostic Observation Schedule) in Level 3 groups clustered in the center, which indicates
(ASD screening and clinical assessments are standard gaze behavior; conversely, the infants
described in detail in the chapter “Assessment”). with ASD were distributed around the periphery.
To detect early signs of ASD, pediatricians The infants with ASD looked less at faces in
should be trained to use screening tools. comparison with the control groups and preferred
Pediatricians should also recognize somatic com- letters in the caption, which is associated with
plaints including sleep and feeding problems, larger brain event-related potentials to objects in
restlessness or apathy, and general dysregulation ASD compared to TD infants (Nakano et al.,
symptoms that might be related to early symp- 2010; Webb, Dawson, Bernier, & Panagiotides,
toms of ASD (Zachor et al., 2014). Not only spe- 2006). Pierce et al. (2011) demonstrated visual
cialists in child and adolescent psychiatric clinics attention preferences of infants with ASD and
but also general pediatricians need to use the TD infants toward social images and geometric
screening and clinical assessments that could images, also showing that children with ASD
help detect early signs of ASD. preferred to look at geometric images rather than
social images. In addition, children with ASD do
not look preferentially toward social stimuli com-
Early Biomarkers of ASD pared to control groups (Campbell, Shic, Macari,
& Chawarska, 2014; Chawarska et al., 2012).
Biomarkers of ASD in infants and toddlers have Chawarska et al. (2012) showed no difference
yet to be discovered because symptom onset is between infants with ASD and TD infants in
different with each toddler; however, early bio- terms of developmental delays, but only when
markers are useful to detect early signs of ASD they looked at two specific types of social con-
(Pierce et al., 2016). Eye tracking is attractive as tent. (In the sandwich condition, the actress
a simple method in the early stages of ASD iden- looked down at the table and made a sandwich.
tification because patterns of eye gazing are There was no child-directed eye contact and
objective behaviors based on neural systems speech. In the moving toys condition, after the
known to be abnormal in ASD, such as the visual actress looked up at the camera, a toy began to
attention system (Belmonte & Yurgelun-Todd, move and make noises.) The discrepancy between
2003; Pierce et al., 2016; Shi, Wang, Peng, Wee, results might depend on differences in data anal-
& Shen, 2013; Townsend, Courchesne, & Egaas, yses and experiment designs (Fischer, Koldewyn,
1996). Recently, eye-tracking studies for ASD Jiang, & Kanwisher, 2013; Guillona, Hadjikhani,
children have highlighted many social visual Baduela, & Rogéa, 2014).
attention deficits (Jones, Carr, & Klin, 2008; The studies for children later diagnosed with
Jones & Klin, 2013; Klin, Lin, Gorrindo, Ramsay, ASD are inconsistent. Chawarska, Macari, and
& Jones, 2009; Pierce et al., 2016). ASD children Shic (2013) reported decreased looking time in
showed a deficit in social orienting and decreased response to the social scene and face of a woman
attention to social contents such as faces and at 6 months in children with ASD. In contrast,
social scenes (Chawarska, Macari, & Shic, 2012; following the original findings, Pierce et al.
Falck-Ytter, Rehnberg, & Bölte, 2013; Hosozawa, (2016) also studied visual attention preferences
Tanaka, Shimizu, Nakano, & Kitazawa, 2012; of infants consisting of diagnostic subgroups and
Klin et al., 2009; Nakano et al., 2010; Pierce, found that unaffected siblings of ASD groups did
Conant, Hazin, Stoner, & Desmond, 2011). not show preferences for geometric images. The
Nakano et al. (2010) demonstrated the gaze pat- researchers reported that abnormalities in visual
220 C. Kanai et al.

attention and preference were among the earliest is a high risk of divorce in parents of children with
emerging signs of ASD with more severe types of ASD (Freedman, Kalb, Zablotsky, & Stuart, 2012;
autism symptoms. Another study examining Ramisch, 2012), and between 2.9% and 6.7%,
social attention with no sound showed that both more marriages end in divorce for parents of chil-
high-risk siblings of ASD and TD infant groups dren with disabilities than parents of children with
preferred more dynamic social images such as no disabilities (Risdal & Singer, 2004). In other
people and faces to nonsocial contents such as words, when individuals with ASD marry and
figures (Kanai et al., 2013). Elsabbagh et al. rear children, the married couples might experi-
(2013), studying orienting response to face, ence problems within the marital and/or parent
showed no difference between infants with high- and child relationship. Therefore, some medical
risk ASD diagnosis and TD infants. This discrep- supports are needed for ASD families.
ancy could result from difference in social stimuli Our previous pilot study explored the influ-
(dynamic or static). In Kanai’s study, the high- ence of group therapy on ASD families in mar-
risk siblings of ASD groups might increase look- ried adults, observing 12 ASD individuals (mean
ing time at a face because many children may age 41.9 (8.8); 9 male, 3 female), 12 partners
love dynamic moving images such as people (mean age 43.6 (7.8); 3 male, 9 female), and 6
dancing and singing. On the other hand, in ASD children (mean age 5.8 (2.3); 3 male, 3
Elsabbagh’s study, the TD infants’ decreased female) through administration of the Lazarus-
looking time at the static social scene showed few type Stress Coping Inventory (SCI), Quality
communication styles, resulting in no difference Marriage Index (QMI), and Dysfunctional
between the two groups. Attitude Scale (DAS). The group therapy for
More studies are needed to investigate social ASD families was held once a month for
interaction in relation to gaze behavior in chil- 10 months at the university hospital. The group
dren with ASD. Such studies will be meaningful was divided into two subgroups of parents (both
for assessing children with ASD so as to provide the ASD group and the partner group) and chil-
precise and early diagnosis, leading to appropri- dren. In the group therapy for parents, the pro-
ate treatments and support such as curative edu- gram consists of ten sessions in which participants
cation and educational support. In addition, the discuss various topics in each session. In the
eye-tracking research in this field could reveal group therapy for children, participants play with
the nature of difficulties of social communication other children and specialists such as a nurse and
in children with ASD (Guillona et al., 2014). children’s nurse in a free space.
One question posed in the sessions was “What
is a hard time in your life based on family?”
ASD Family Some ASD individuals responded that they
rightly decry someone’s view and lack of flexibil-
Despite high levels of language skills, individuals ity in all areas, adding that they cannot control
who experience ASD with no intellectual deficit their feelings. Some partners, meanwhile, said
continue to encounter problems in nonverbal that they (i.e., those with ASD) have no sense of
communication and understanding of others’ time, exhibit a roller coaster ride of emotions,
minds on a lifelong basis (Senju, Southgate, and become out of control over something unex-
White, & Frith, 2009). Previous studies showed pected. When asked, “What does your partner
that only 10.8% of individuals with ASD were mean to you?” one ASD individual replied, “My
married, which indicated the difficulty of a long- partner looks like my lovely dog.” Another per-
term couple relationship (Yukawa et al., 2013). In son said, “My partner looks like my mother, who
particular, partners of those with ASD reported does practical things around the house.” In terms
that their own mental health had deteriorated due of partner responses, some women said that they
to the relationship and that they were feeling are not cognizant of the topic in its entirety. One
exhausted and neglected (Attwood, 2007). There woman said that she feels alone mentally and has
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 221

no one to depend on in her life. When asked, specialists and other children. The findings dem-
“Tell me how things can work out well in your onstrated that group therapy for ASD families
married life,” one ASD person said, “I make an could constitute a comfortable setting (Kanai,
effort to listen to her conversation.” Some part- Yokoi, Matsushita, Saito, & Kato, 2013); how-
ners said that they maintain a proper distance, ever, further study is necessary due to the small
though one woman said, “I hug him to switch his sample size and lack of a control group. Our
negative feeling.” The next question was “How study is therefore needed in order to clarify how
do you feel about your relationship with your group therapy can be useful for an ASD family.
children?” One ASD man thought that his chil-
dren must be more lost without their mother than
they are without him. Another person said that Comprehensive Assessment of ASD
the sun shines out of their child’s eyes. Regarding
the partners’ responses, some women said that As previously mentioned, comprehensive assess-
they do not know that their partners have selfless ments are necessary for supporting children with
love for their children. One woman said that her ASD. These assessments must include not only
partner plays with their child when he is not the ASD assessment but also assessment for
enthusiastic over something else. On the whole, other developmental disorders such as intellec-
as each group increases the number of sessions, tual disorder (ID), attention deficit hyperactivity
something is shown to have changed in the minds disorder (ADHD), and developmental coordina-
of its members. Some ASD individuals are able tion disorder (DCD). Moreover, evaluation of
to learn how to communicate with their wives. intellectual level and developmental level, includ-
For example, one morning, a husband with ASD ing cognitive function, as well as assessment of
became irritable over something. Although he communication skills, comorbid psychiatric
never said sorry until now, he sent an email stat- symptoms, adaptive behavior, and psychosocial
ing “I am so sorry for a little while ago” to his and environmental assessment (family, school,
wife at lunchtime. He thus learned that apologiz- workplace, community etc.), are needed. In the
ing and/or saying thank you were important for section below, I will detail adaptive behavior
communicating with his wife in the group ses- assessment, because it has become standard prac-
sions. Meanwhile, the partners became better tice to include assessments of adaptive function-
able to respond to the husbands with ASD well in ing as part of a diagnostic evaluation for ASD.
advance. Some women felt that they found this Adaptive behavior is best defined as “the per-
persistence enjoyable and kept a proper distance formance of daily activities required for personal
from the husbands with ASD. The group sessions and social sufficiency” (Sparrow, Balla, &
therefore provided the partners with some mental Cicchetti, 2005). These are skills that children
space within their relationships. should be employing independently on a daily
In questionnaire research, the scores of ability basis according to their age and intellectual abil-
of coping with a restricted and stereotyped reper- ity level. Intellectual function and adaptive
toire of activities, stable marriage, and flexibility behavior are mentioned as correlated in the gen-
were significantly higher in the last session than eral population. However, for many individuals
in the first session in the ASD group, and the with ASD, adaptive behavior is often achieved at
score of self-blame was significantly lower in the a much lower level than what is expected from
last session than in the first session in the partner the intellectual level. In particular, there is a large
group. In the group therapy for children, both discrepancy between the level of adaptive behav-
children and parents have a good opportunity to ior and IQ in high functioning individuals with
communicate with other people such as special- ASD (Kanne et al., 2011; Klin et al., 2007; Perry,
ists, other parents, and children. The ASD chil- Flanagan, Dunngeier, & Freeman, 2009).
dren can potentially learn how to play with other Among the adaptive behavior measures, the
children and finally communicate with both the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-Second
222 C. Kanai et al.

Edition (Vineland-II; Sparrow et al., 2005) is uals with ASD, adaptive behavior is achieved at a
used widely in clinical and research situations. much lower level than what is expected from
The Vineland-II is a valid and reliable measure- their cognitive function; we must consider the
ment for individuals’ adaptive level of function- evaluation of both ASD symptoms and adaptive
ing. It includes four forms: survey interview, behavior level in order to support children with
parent/caregiver rating forms, expanded inter- ASD.
view form, and teacher rating form. The
Vineland-II can be used for individuals from
birth to 90 years old, except the teacher rating  he Relationship Between Motor
T
form which is for ages 3:0–21:11. Function and Social Skill
The survey interview form is most appropriate Development in Children
for children. It is administered in a semi- with Autism Spectrum Disorders
structured interview with parents or caregivers. It
takes 25–60 min to complete the interview. The  ssential Questions and Research
E
content and scales of the Vineland-II were orga- Directions
nized within a four-domain structure: communi-
cation, daily living skills, socialization, and Early childhood is the period from prenatal
motor skills. There are sub-domains of receptive, development to 8 years of age. It is a crucial
expressive, and written in communication; per- phase of growth and development, because expe-
sonal, domestic, and community in daily living riences during early childhood can influence out-
skills; interpersonal relationships, play and lei- comes across the entire course of an individual’s
sure time, and coping skills in socialization; and life (WHO, 2007). Usually, parents or pediatri-
gross and fine in motor skills. In addition, the cians recognize developmental delays of children
Vineland-II offers an optional Maladaptive under the age of 3 years. During the last 20 years,
Behavior Index to provide more in-depth infor- screening tools and more reliable evaluation
mation on behaviors that interfere with adaptive instruments have been developed, and profes-
development. sionals have become increasingly proficient in
Regarding the evaluation method, the items recognizing and diagnosing ASD. With recent
included in each sub-domain are rated as 0 (usu- heightened public awareness, parents are more
ally), 1 (sometimes or partially), and 2 (never). likely to raise concerns related to developmental
The Vineland-II provides standard scores for issues, specifically about ASD.
each domain and an Adaptive Behavior The importance of early identification and
Composite, both calculated with M = 100, intervention highlights the need for research
SD = 15, and v-scale scores for each subdomain specifically applying the DSM-5 criteria to
calculated with M = 15, SD = 3. The standard young children (Christiansz, Gray, Taffe, &
scores on the Vineland are similar to those on Tonge, 2016).
most intelligence tests insofar as the mean and Retrospective reports on what did and did not
standard deviation. For this reason, the results of occur in early development are commonly used
intelligence tests like the Wechsler scales can be in diagnostic measures such as the Autism
compared to Vineland domain standard scores as Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and the
well as the Adaptive Behavior Composite. Also, Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS)
the profile of scores from the Vineland-II is use- (Saemundsen, Magnusson, Smari, &
ful to diagnose and classify ID and developmen- Sigurdardottir, 2003). Thus, retrospective studies
tal disorders such as ASD, ADHD, and LD. Given involving parental recollections provide several
that approximately 40% of individuals with ASD valuable insights into the early development of
also have ID and they tend to exhibit adaptive children with ASD (Goldberg, Thorsen, Osann,
deficits above and beyond their cognitive delays & Spence, 2008; Ozonoff et al., 2011; Werner,
and given that for even high functioning individ- Dawson, Osterling, & Dinno, 2000). Parents of
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 223

children later diagnosed with ASD recall devel- Recently, Bhat, Landa, and Galloway (2011)
opmental differences in the first few months of found that early motor developmental delays
life, although a larger proportion became con- within the first 2 years of life (the sensorimotor
cerned during the second year of life (Young, stage of cognitive development) play an essen-
Brewer, & Pattison, 2003). In 20–50% of chil- tial role in social and cognitive development;
dren with ASD, parents retrospectively describe thus, these delays could cause social impair-
a pattern of regression in speech or social- ments and cognitive deficiencies in children with
emotional connectedness during the second year ASD. Bhat et al. (2011) focused on five major
of life (Baranek, 1999; Werner & Dawson, 2005). issues in their study:
Moreover, there are many indications that delays
in movement development and speech usually • Types of motor impairment
prompt parents to raise concerns when visiting • A comparison between motor impairments in
pediatricians. Most parents become worried ASD and other pediatric conditions and
when an infant is between 15 and 18 months of diagnoses
age, but, in many cases, they may delay asking • A theoretical viewpoint on how motor impair-
for advice from their pediatrician for several ments might contribute to the social and com-
months. The most common developmental con- munication impairments of ASD
cerns raised by parents to their family pediatri- • Research and clinical implications of pres-
cian are delayed movement developmental ently available evidence
milestones, extremes in behavioral reactivity, dis- • Limitations of currently existing evidence on
ruption in social-communicative behavior (e.g., movement issues, motor findings, clinically
lack of responsiveness, impaired imitation), and available assessments, therapeutic approaches,
concerns related to play development (Mostofsky and interventions for ASD
et al., 2006; Turner, Stone, Pozdol, & Coonrod,
2006; Young et al., 2003). Table 1 shows a list of This study concludes that it is important to
the most common motor-related problems and address fundamental motor and sensorimotor
symptoms that indicate developmental delays or developmental issues and impairments through
deviations in children from 3 months to 6 years well-established assessments and effective early
old (Table 2). intervention programs (Bhat et al., 2011).

Table 2 A general list of symptoms related to motor functions indicating developmental delays (Lakatos & Toth, 2014)
Between 3 months and 3 years old Between the ages of 3 and 6 years old
− Poor or absent suck reflex, not swallowing food properly, − Movement coordination weaknesses
lack of eating (inaccurate, too rigid movements with too
− Appearance of defective postures, reflexes, co-movements much tension)
that stay on (e.g., ATNR) − Weakness of the ability to learn new
− Hyperreflexia, hyporeflexia, areflexia movements
− Newborn baby reflexes that will stay on too long − Dyspraxia (or developmental coordination
− Appearance of atypical muscle tone: Spasticity, hypotonic, disorder), apraxia
infantile cerebral palsy − Graphomotor dysmaturity, high risk of
− Delay or complete lack of movement imitation dysgraphia
− Appearance of bizarre, stereotypical movements − Delayed language, hindered speech
− Deviant/delayed/accelerated movement development, development or motor speech delays
hyperactivity − Hyperactivity
− Complete lack or an excessive amount of a sense of danger − Too few movement activities
− Lack of games involving movement (motor) − Disorders of body scheme, spatial
− Appearance of stereotypical game forms orientation and laterality
− Tossing around toys and objects aimlessly, etc. − At-risk of learning difficulties
− Quality of activities (slow, inefficient, or
badly organized), etc.
224 C. Kanai et al.

Moreover, the next study undertaken by Bhat, and children with ASD. These early sensorimotor
Galloway, and Landa (2012) showed that chil- and interconnected neurodevelopmental issues
dren with early motor delays (overall gross should be considered important clinical issues in
motor delays) at 3 and 6 months continued to further research (Landa & Garrett-Mayer, 2006).
exhibit a risk of communication delay at the These developmental motor deficiencies are pres-
18-month follow-up visit. The study states that ent from a very young age and are likely to affect
early motor delays could be viewed as indicators social-communicative and language development
of early disruptions related to higher risk for negatively in children with developmental disabil-
ASD; likewise, there is empirical support for the ities, especially ASD (McCleery, Elliott, Sampanis,
presence of motor delays in infants who later & Stefanidou, 2013).
develop ASD, as well as later-born siblings of In the study of Bhat et al. (2011), the authors
children with ASD. However, the sibling data conclude that their results provide evidence that
used in this study showed that only a subset of motor behavior is both quantitatively and qualita-
infant siblings of children with ASD showed tively different in children with ASD (infants,
early motor delays and communication delays in toddlers, and school-aged) compared to children
the future. Therefore, motor delays may be a fea- with TD. The study showed significant impair-
ture of ASD from early on and may present in ments in posture control, motor coordination,
infants who have a greater genetic risk of ASD praxis, and imitation. Furthermore, the study pro-
(Bhat et al., 2012). vides direct empirical evidence for a link between
Frequently reported motor findings show motor and social impairments in children with
impaired basic motor control (motor skill defi- ASD. Thus, targeted early therapeutic approaches
cits) in children with ASD (Ghaziuddin & Butler, (physical, occupational, and speech therapy) are
1998; Jansiewicz et al., 2006; Noterdaeme, needed in the future in order to improve motor
Mildenberger, Minow, & Amorosa, 2002; functioning in children with ASD.
Rinehart et al., 2006). Difficulties with basic The study of McCleery et al. (2013) also
motor control include gait, posture, balance, describes research needs and future directions for
speed, and coordination issues (see Fig. 1). the development of early intervention programs
Comprehensive early sensory perceptions with aimed at addressing the speech-language and
gross motor activities and related social communi- social communication development difficulties in
cation and language characteristics should be ASD from a motor-related perspective. Therefore,
assessed and evaluated for infants at risk of ASD from the clinical and educational viewpoint,

Fig. 1 Core deficits in


ASD; modified from
Stewart H. Mostofsky
(2013)
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 225

there is an unquestionable need to develop and dons, joints, etc.), hearing (audition, sound vibra-
provide well-established early intervention pro- tion, environmental pressure change, etc.), sight
grams and developmental theory-based therapeu- (vision: to focus on and identify images, visible
tic approaches that utilize sensory perceptions, light and colors, hues and brightness, etc.), smell
motor activities, and motor learning principles (olfaction: to detect scent), and taste (gustation:
for children with ASD. to detect taste of substances and flavor character,
etc.). Sensory integration is an essential neuro-
logical process that organizes sensation from the
I nterrelation Between Sensory, environment and one’s body (see Fig. 2).
Motor, and Executive Functioning During sensory processing, the brain receives
in Children with ASD many kinds of specific sensory information
(stimuli). Following receipt of a stimulus, the
Early detection of autism is critical for early brain must process the information, which
intervention, which has a positive effect on the includes selecting (registration), discriminating
outcome of a child with developmental delays. (orientation), interpreting, and organizing the dif-
There is a growing interest in using specified ferent impulses from the inner and outer environ-
assessment of motor development for early detec- ment. This process is called sensory processing
tion of developmental delays in infants (from or integration (Schoen, Miller, Brett-Green, &
birth to 1 year of age) and toddlers (from 1 to Nielsen, 2009) (see Fig. 3).
2 years of age) who are might be at risk of devel- The effective registration and accurate inter-
oping ASD (Landa & Garrett-Mayer, 2006; pretation of a sensory stimulus (sensory input)
Ozonoff et al., 2008; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2007). cause a person to react (motor output) and behave
During the first 2 years of life, there is a strong in a meaningful and consistent way (adequate
connection between the development of the cen- behavior). There are three types of dysfunction or
tral nervous system and sensorimotor develop- atypical responses that can occur during the sen-
ment. The sensory system includes seven primary sory integration process (Miller, Anzalone, Lane,
senses: touch (somatosensory or kinesthetic), Cermak, & Osten, 2007). The first is called sen-
vestibular (balance, gravity, acceleration, head sory modulation disorder, where the problem lies
and body position, etc.), proprioception (bodily with the adequate controlled behavioral answer
sense of body parts such as limbs, muscles, ten- to sensory input and where the motor output

Fig. 2 Primary senses


that contribute to the
sensory system
226 C. Kanai et al.

Fig. 3 The general


process of sensory
integration

Fig. 4 Categories and subtypes of sensory processing disorder; based on Kranowitz (2006)

matches the nature and intensity of the sensory within a given base of support (e.g., balance);
information (e.g., sensory sensitivity such as indeed, it is a fundamental motor skill that helps
over-responsivity, under-responsivity, or sensory a person to maintain an upright stance (Horak,
seeking). The second is called sensory discrimi- Shumway-Cook, Crowe, & Black, 1988).
nation disorder and occurs when the sensory Recently, Mache and Todd (2016) reported on the
input is poorly detected and the child cannot relationship between motor coordination skill,
sense the differences or similarities of sensory postural stability, restricted and repetitive pat-
stimuli. (Every type of sense—visual discrimina- terns of behavior, diagnosis, age, and sex in chil-
tion, tactile discrimination, etc.—has its discrim- dren with ASD. The results show that children
inatory problems.) The third dysfunction is called with ASD who participated in the study have
sensory-based motor disorder and includes pos- deficits in postural stability compared to children
tural disorders and dyspraxia or developmental without ASD (Mache & Todd, 2016). Thus, pos-
coordination disorder. In the case of sensory- tural stability appears to be an influential factor in
based motor disorders, the problem lies in the the development and performance level of gross
stabilization of one’s body (posture), motor plan- motor skills.
ning, motor answer, and an adaptive/responsive Problems at the level of sensory integration
movement series to sensory stimuli (Fig. 4). arise when a child with ASD attempts to integrate
Sensory-based motor disorders (postural sta- too many sensory inputs at the same time. The
bility and motor coordination skills) are com- difficulty with the sensory integration process
monly reported for children with ASD. Postural could be the cause of anxiety or panic that results
stability develops from a very early age and refers in rigidity or depression. If a child can control
to the ability to maintain one’s center of gravity most sensory inputs from the environment (both
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 227

from the outer environment and his/her inner the sensory system, sensory motor develop-
environment), he or she will be able to coordinate ment, perceptual motor development, and cog-
these sensory experiences. Coordinating sensory nitive development.
inputs allows the child to manage one or two sen- In many cases, inadequate motor performance
sory inputs at a time. Controlled and processed at the early preschool age is one of the first clear
sensory experiences are a gateway to executive signs of a comorbid developmental disorder with
functioning. more prominent behavioral features, such as ASD
In summary, indicators of sensory processing or ADHD (Esposito, Venuti, Maestro, & Muratori,
dysfunction might include inappropriate or prob- 2009; Van Waelvelde, Oostra, Dewitte, Van Den
lematic motor, behavioral, attentional, and adap- Broeck, & Jongmans, 2010). Thus, specified
tive responses anticipating or following sensory assessments should include the evaluation of all
stimulation (Lane, Young, Baker, & Angley, age-appropriate functions within the three “major
2010). However, sensory difficulties should be areas” of developmental difficulties:
considered a sensory processing disorder only
when they cause significant issues in daily life 1. Motor area (e.g., gross, fine motor milestones,
and interrupt tasks and routines to the extent that sensory and motor speech skills, etc.)
the child cannot compensate or cope with those 2. Psycho-cognitive area
problems (e.g., when they result in psychological 3. Social behavior and communication area
distress, anxiety, panic attacks, maladaptive
behavior, self-injurious behavior, etc.). Although motor disorders can occur in isola-
Children with developmental disabilities, tion, many studies have described frequent co-
and ASD in particular, are more frequently occurrence with ASD and ADHD (Fournier,
observed to experience notable difficulties in Hass, Naik, Lodha, & Cauraugh, 2010;
sensory processing and motor skills develop- Grzadzinski, Dick, Lord, & Bishop, 2016). In
ment (Baranek, 1999; Baranek, David, Poe, general, small children need to react to any
Stone, & Watson, 2006). The pyramid of learn- stimuli they encounter in a manner that is
ing (see Fig. 5) shows the connection between specific, precipitate, and present-oriented

Fig. 5 The pyramid of


learning; modified from
Williams and
Shellenberger (1996)
228 C. Kanai et al.

(“here and now”) to be able to adapt to changes nervous system have to “network” for successful
in their closed environment. Many studies have executive functioning to take place (Dowell,
shown that improving executive function, motor Mahone, & Mostofsky, 2009; Landrigan,
coordination, motor learning, and planning is Lambertini, & Birnbaum, 2012; Panerai et al.,
essential in children with ASD (Baranek, 1999; 2014). The more practical the executive func-
Travers, Kana, Klinger, Klein, & Klinger, 2014). tions, the more social skills become functional,
The psychological hypothesis of executive dys- and the neurological network makes stronger and
function plays a significant role in explaining newer connections. Children with ASD need a
the behavioral phenotype of persons with ASD, more reliable and more stable executive function
along with other hypotheses such as deficits in structure. Furthermore, children with ASD
theory of mind (Frith, 1993) or the weak central require a more precise executive function struc-
coherence hypothesis (Shah & Frith, 1993). ture than children with TD.
Nevertheless, none of these predictions is suffi- In her study, Hill (2004) summarized research
cient to rule out the others, and behaviors that data and related scientific evidence on executive
have their origins in one of these three hypoth- functioning and ASD. She concluded that,
eses are also determined by many developmen- although many children with ASD do have diffi-
tal processes and other factors (Martos-Perez & culties with executive function, this should not be
Paula-Perez, 2011). considered a defining feature of ASD, because
Executive function is needed to express how there are also children on the autism spectrum
someone feels, thinks, and acts within the sur- who do not have problems with executive
rounding environment. An executive function function. On the other hand, therapy approaches
can be defined as a primary type of self-regulation that focus mainly on social and communication
that involves the ability to engage in goal-directed difficulties might not have the best possible effect
activity with necessary mental processes mainly on the development of a child who has autistic
regulated by frontal lobes (Panerai, Tasca, Ferri, problems combined with executive function
Genitori D’Arrigo, & Elia, 2014). The necessary issues (Hill & Bird, 2006; Robinson, Goddard,
mental processes in executive functioning are Dritschel, Wisley, & Howlin, 2009).
attention, organization, time management, mem-
ory, flexibility, inhibition (interrupting one’s
actions and monitoring the dominant response),  he Specificity of DCD or Dyspraxia
T
personal goals, and control of emotion and and Imitation Task Performance
behavior. Young children with ASD—who have Issues in Children with ASD
not yet developed organized and conscious play
activity—engage with their environment with Problems with motor functions are more read-
sensory-based impulsivity (e.g., over-sensory, ily observable and quantifiable than complex
under-sensory, or seeking reactions; see Fig. 4), social interaction or communication.
moving from one stimulus to another (Freeman, Difficulties with motor coordination and con-
Gulsrud, & Kasari, 2015). These sensory difficul- trol can be observed as early as infancy (Landa
ties might cause repetitious and disorganized & Garrett-Mayer, 2006; MacNeil & Mostofsky,
motor and play activities. 2012) and usually include motor sequencing
Toddlers and young children at risk or with difficulties in creeping, crawling, and (later on)
ASD need to plan a course of action to be able to walking. Motor coordination ability includes
engage in a “goal-directed” way. Problems with muscle tone control, postural control, axial and
executive function are neurological in nature and limb coordination, gait movement, and the
arise from a disruption or delay in neurological speed of adequate responses to stimuli from
development (Hill, 2004). The prefrontal cortex is the environment.
considered to be primarily responsible for execu- Muscle tone is the appropriate degree of
tive functional skills, but many parts of the central muscle tension or resistance within individual
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 229

muscles and muscle groups at rest. The state of skilled gestures and difficulty recognizing the
muscle tone affects postural control and stability, gestures of others could serve as biomarkers for
which develops in the neck muscles, the shoulder the early diagnosis and therapeutic approach for
girdle muscles, and the core muscles of the trunk. ASD (Dziuk et al., 2007).
The muscles of children with high tone are tight Praxis is the ability of the brain to conceive,
and tense even when not engaged in anything. organize, and carry out a sequence of unfamiliar
High muscle tone causes the limbs to be tightly actions (Ayres, 1973). Praxis is not one action but
contorted, thus making any movement burden- a functional behavior made up of three primary
some and convulsive (spasmodic). Children with processes. The first is called ideation, which
low tone are not able to sit upright and may also refers to generating an idea (creative process)
lack endurance for gross and fine motor activi- about how one might interact with the environ-
ties; additionally, they may have difficulties with ment. The second is organizing a program of
game activities that require controlled and coor- action processes, which is termed motor plan-
dinated movements (Mandich, Polatajko, ning, while the third is the actual performance of
Macnab, & Miller, 2001). a motor act, or execution (see Fig. 3). Dyspraxia
Children with ASD show some degree of dif- means that, even though there is adequate motor
ficulty with gross and fine movements, skilled capacity, the person has a reduced ability to carry
motor gestures, imitation, and tool use. Problems out non-learned motor activities (Dejean, 2008).
in basic motor skill development are connected to The specificity of inadequate motor performance,
impairments in imitation skill (motor type), pan- imitation performance issues, and dyspraxia or
tomime, and tool use in praxis testing situations. DCD in children considered at risk of developing
Studies refer to impaired skilled motor perfor- ASD or with a diagnosis of ASD has been shown
mance as “developmental dyspraxia” (Dowell in many studies during the last 10 years. The dif-
et al., 2009; Dziuk et al., 2007; MacNeil & ficulty with imitative behavior could be a key
Mostofsky, 2012; S. H. Mostofsky et al., 2006). indicator of dyspraxia. Motor developmental
In recent decades, developmental coordination problems (gross motor development, fine motor
disorder (DCD) has been described as develop- control, visual motor control, spatial awareness,
mental dyspraxia, clumsy child syndrome, and etc.) and problems with motor imitation are more
sensory integrative dysfunction (Mostofsky et al., observable at an early age and are more easily
2006; Toussaint-Thorin et al., 2013). comparable and measurable than communication
Many studies have shown impaired perfor- and social interaction issues (May-Benson &
mance in imitation tasks in children with Koomar, 2010). These developmental issues can
ASD. Imitation is important because it can form serve as “biomarkers” (MacNeil & Mostofsky,
the developmental basis of social cognition, as in 2012) in early diagnosis and early intervention
the theory of mind (von dem Hagen, Stoyanova, approaches.
Rowe, Baron-Cohen, & Calder, 2014) and empa- The impairment of ideomotor praxis is called
thy. Being able to imitate another person involves ideomotor apraxia or dyspraxia (depending on
explicit body awareness and the cognition of self the severity of symptoms). It is characterized by
and others, as well as perceptual processing, con- the inability to imitate hand gestures correctly
trolled attention, executive functions, motor plan- (e.g., waving “bye-bye”) and voluntarily mime
ning, and comprehension of social cues and tool use and skilled gestures (e.g., pretending to
language (Mostofsky et al., 2006). Enhancing use a hammer or a scissor, to brush one’s hair,
body awareness has thus been described as an etc.). Meaningful gestures produced in the pres-
essential element or mechanism of action for ence of an object are called transitive actions
therapeutic approaches often categorized as (e.g., voluntary tool use). Situated representa-
mind-body approaches (Mehling et al., 2011). tions of gestures that are created in the absence of
Therefore, signs of developmental motor difficul- an object are called intransitive actions (e.g.,
ties together with impaired performance of communicative hand gestures).
230 C. Kanai et al.

Findings reveal that the assessment results aspects (Lane, Ivey, & May-Benson, 2014). The
related to impairments in motor functions, lower following elements should be incorporated into
performances in tool use, and difficulties with the assessment of gestures in children:
imitation tasks (postural, gestural, verbal, etc.)
are correlated with measures of the core features • Imitations related to body parts and the whole
of ASD (communication and social impairment) body aspect
(Dowell et al., 2009). Therefore, these difficulties • Moving and not moving actions
and disabilities might have a common underlying • Asymmetrical and bilateral imitations
mechanism that contributes to the issues in the • Representational versus nonrepresentational,
motor, sensorimotor, and social communication etc. (Lane et al., 2014)
skill development of children with ASD (Dziuk
et al., 2007; Srinivasan et al., 2015; Van Waelvelde On the other hand, gesture types vary by age.
et al., 2010). For example, at age 2, children should be able to
Traditional assessments of praxis employ a point to body parts (self and others), while, at age
combination of sensory integration and praxis 4, they should use their body parts as objects.
tests. These include tests of imitation of postures Later on, at around 7–8 years old, children should
and gestures, ideational praxis, oral praxis, be able to represent an object with no space,
sequencing praxis, bilateral motor coordination, while, at age 12, they should develop a mature
constructional praxis, design copying, and praxis response and be able to represent an object with
to verbal command (May-Benson & Cermak, space (Lane et al., 2014).
2007). Additional assessments can also be used, Mostofsky et al. (2006) found that children
one of which is the Miller Assessment for with high-functioning autism had significant
Preschoolers (Parush, Winokur, Goldstand, & impairments in command gestures, imitation ges-
Miller, 2002). Others are formal clinical obser- tures, and gestures with overall tool use perfor-
vations during the neurodevelopmental assess- mance compared to the control group. The study
ment (prone extension/supine flexion, of Dowell et al. (2009) showed an impaired pos-
diadochokinesis, finger identification, sequential tural knowledge and performance level in chil-
thumb-finger touching, tactile discrimination, dren with ASD compared to the control group of
oculomotor control, graphesthesia, etc.) and children with TD. MacNeil et al. (2012) found
informal clinical observations in different envi- significantly worse performance in postural
ronments and social situations. knowledge among children with ASD compared
A general praxis assessment has the following to an ADHD and a TD group. Their results
components: showed that postural knowledge impairment is
specific to ASD.
• Sensory processing and sensory discrimina- Dyspraxia in autism appears to be associated
tion ability assessments with the impaired formation of spatial represen-
• Ideational praxis assessment (through percep- tations, as well as transcoding and execution
tion and action on object affordances) (Dowell et al., 2009), during the motor organiza-
• Gestural assessment (through use of imitation tion process. Methods for altering patterns of
of postures, gestures, and following verbal skill learning in children with ASD should begin
commands) at an early age. Therapeutic intervention at a
• Motor organization (motor planning, bilateral young age might not only lead to improved social
coordination, projected action sequences) interaction with a more facile execution of com-
• Feedback and ability to make adaptive municative gestures and other social skills; it
responses to environmental demands may also help the advanced development of chil-
dren’s ability to understand the actions of others,
It is important to assess gesture praxis and with resulting improvements in social cognition
imitation performances of children in different (MacNeil & Mostofsky, 2012).
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 231

Haswell, Izawa, Dowell, Mostofsky, and  ocial Behavior and Skills


S
Shadmehr (2009) revealed that children with in Children with ASD
ASD showed a distinctive pattern of motor learn-
ing in their study. This study found a bias toward Treatment of Social Skills
reliance on proprioceptive feedback from the
internal body space, with greater dependence on Recent tendencies of treatment regarding social
cortical regions where movements are repre- skills, the effects of treatment in the inclusive
sented in intrinsic coordinates of motion (somato- classroom, the collaboration between university
sensory cortex). On the other hand, the study and the community, video modeling instruction,
suggests less dependence on regions (premotor, emotion regulation, the relation with co-occurring
posterior parietal) where movements are repre- problems of emotional control, Social Stories™,
sented in extrinsic coordinates in children with and the use of Social Stories™ in pre-school age
ASD, with relative ignorance of visual feedback children are reviewed and discussed in the fol-
from the external world (Haswell et al., 2009). lowing sections.
Furthermore, MacNeil and Mostofsky (2012)
found evidence that impairments in the recogni-
tion and performance of skilled correct gestures Recent Tendencies
are specific to children with ASD. Their results
also showed that impairments in primary motor The most important topic in terms of recent ten-
control are a generalized finding in children with dencies in social behavior and skills intervention
ASD, as they were observed in children with research is the change of methodology to estab-
ADHD as well. The authors conclude that, next lish a rigorous line of research. On this note, sev-
to the difficulties in the performance of skilled eral important reviews concerning a clear
gestures, children with ASD also have specific methodological line have been published.
problems with the ability to recognize skilled Initially, Rao and colleagues examined social
gestures in others. skills training (SST) programs for youths with
Postural knowledge and control, primary ASD, with an emphasis on critically evaluating
motor skill performance (gross motor and fine efficacy and highlighting areas of future research
motor), balance, and vestibular dysfunctions are (Rao, Beidel, & Murray, 2008). The review high-
commonly observed and well-documented dif- lights the disparity between SST programs
ficulties in children with ASD. Researchers described in the extant literature, including the
have published many studies on whether ves- lack of a universal definition of social skills, vari-
tibular dysfunction (over−/under-sensitivity or ous levels of intensity and duration of treatment,
sensory seeking; see also Fig. 4), problems in divergent theoretical backgrounds, and variety in
postural control, balance, and primary motor services provided in clinical or classroom set-
skill development are connected to the overall tings. Based on this critical review, a “roadmap”
dyspraxia diagnosis and, later on, psycho-intel- for future research, consistent with recommenda-
lectual and social developmental issues in chil- tions put forth by a leading group of autism
dren with ASD (Baranek et al., 2006; Bhat et al., researchers, is identified.
2011; Esposito et al., 2009; Mache & Todd, Reichow and Volkmar (2010) reviewed the lit-
2016; Minshew, Sung, Jones, & Furman, 2004). erature examining SST programs for youths with
Therefore, postural knowledge relates strongly ASD and placed emphasis on critically evaluat-
to basic motor skill development (praxis), while ing efficacy and highlighting areas of future
the developmental issues in social awareness research. Sixty-six studies published in peer-
strongly relate to social skill development in reviewed journals between 2001 and July 2008
children with ASD. and featuring 513 participants were included.
232 C. Kanai et al.

The authors classified the research as age-specific to be generally effective at improving social
findings into interventions for preschool children, skills, with some studies observing transfer
school-aged children, and adolescents and adults, effects to improvements in wider psychological
as well as specific findings. They also classified wellbeing.
research into applied behavior analysis, naturalis- Finally, White and colleagues summarized the
tic, parent training, peer training, and social skills state of research in group-based social skills
groups. After they had classified these criteria, training programs for school-age children and
they provided recommendations for practice and adolescents with ASD (White, Keonig, & Scahill,
areas of future research. 2007). In this review, the authors identified 14
By contrast, Cappadocia and Weiss (2011) studies published from 1985 to 2006 using a tem-
reviewed research comparing three types of plate developed by a NIMH work group. They
social skills training group studies: traditional, also mentioned that empirically supported treat-
cognitive behavioral, and parent-inclusive stud- ment (EST) must be identified as having a rigor-
ies. They provided preliminary evidence for the ous line of research. Rigorous lines of research of
efficacy of group-based social skills interven- EST are usually (1) in the form of randomized
tions; moreover, they stated that few studies used clinical trials (RCTs) and (2) a means of support-
comparison group or randomized control trial ing EST’s utility as a treatment for a particular
designs. They went on to propose some future disorder (Task Force on Promotion Dissemination
directions. of Psychological Procedures, 1995). Empirical
Using a randomized controlled trial (RCT) support for efficacy can be established through
design, Gantman and colleagues tested the (3) well-designed group experiments or several
effectiveness of an evidence-based, caregiver- (>9) single case experiments, (4) treatment man-
assisted social skills intervention known as uals, and (5) clearly defined patient samples. The
PEERS for young adults with high-functioning role of (6) systematic reviews is to evaluate the
ASD (ages 18–23) using self- and caregiver- state of the field and identify progress and gaps.
report measures (Gantman, Kapp, Orenski, & Establishing psychosocial intervention as an EST
Laugeson, 2012). They used descriptive mea- therefore clearly requires much more than pre-
sures (Autism Spectrum Quotient, AQ; Kaufman liminary evidence.
Brief Intelligence Test: Second Edition, KBIT- As White and colleagues mentioned, several
2; Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Second rigorous lines of research, randomized clinical
Edition, Survey Form, Vineland-II) and primary trials (RCTs), definition of a particular disorder,
outcome measures (Social Responsiveness patient samples, treatment manuals, and system-
Scale, SRS; Social Skills Rating System, SSRS; atic reviews must be necessary in future treat-
Social and Emotional Loneliness Scale for ment trials (White et al., 2007).
Adults, SELSA). As secondary outcome mea-
sures, they used Empathy Quotient, EQ; Quality
of Socialization Questionnaire, QSQ; Social  ffect of Treatment in the Inclusive
E
Skills Inventory, SSI; and Test of Young Adult Classroom
Social Skills Knowledge, TYASSK. Results
support the effectiveness of using this caregiver- Another important topic is whether the student
assisted, manualized intervention for young with ASD benefits from exposure to and inter-
adults with ASD. actions with typical peers. Laushey and Heflin
Hotton and Coles (2016) aimed to critically (2000) offered one answer to the argument for
evaluate studies published in the past 20 years the use of inclusive programs. They collected
that had used group-based social skills training to data on students with autism, with this data
improve the social skills of adults and/or adoles- indicating that the peer buddy approach sig-
cents with ASD. Thirteen studies were identified, nificantly increased students’ appropriate
and group-based social skills training was found social interactions.
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 233

The effects of peer role are important but com- children with ASD being educated in inclusive
plicated. Kasari and colleagues examined self, settings (Camargo et al., 2014). Characteristics
peer, and teacher reports on social relationships for and components of the interventions are summa-
60 high-functioning children with ASD (Kasari, rized, and their implications for practice and
Locke, Gulsrud, & Rotheram-Fuller, 2011), find- future research are discussed.
ing that 20% of children with ASD experienced The goal of the collaboration between a uni-
reciprocated friendships and high social network versity and two community mental health (CMH)
status. Thus, while the majority of high-function- centers was to increase the capacity among staff
ing children with ASD struggle with peer relation- serving children with ASD in the usual care
ships in general education classrooms, a small social skills groups (Bryson & Ostmeyer, 2014).
percentage appear to achieve social success. How Foundational education in behavior management
to evaluate the results of this research is very dif- may benefit successful implementation of ASD-
ficult, however. The percentage of students who specific evidence-based practices in community
achieved social success in this study is not so high, settings.
so the preliminary target is to clarify how to From these studies, the authors proposed vari-
decrease the difficulty of the 80% of children with ous types of coordination between universities
ASD and, at the same time, to ascertain why the and inclusive school settings. Their implications
other 20% of children increased the success of for practice and future research are discussed.
their social relationships and networks.
Kasari and colleagues also studied two inter-
ventions for improving the social skills of high- Video Modeling Instruction
functioning children with ASD in general
education classrooms (Kasari, Rotheram-Fuller, One effective method of social intervention,
Locke, & Gulsrud, 2012). One intervention video modeling instruction, is empirically eval-
involved a peer-mediated approach (PEER) and uated. Plavnick and colleagues adapted an effi-
the other involved a child-assisted approach cacious protocol for adolescents with ASD,
(CHILD). Findings indicated that there can be namely, video-based group instruction (VGI).
significant improvements in peer social connec- According to the intervention outcomes, long-
tions for children with ASD in general education term maintenance and generalization outcomes
classrooms with a brief intervention and that for the participants were mixed (Plavnick, Kaid,
these gains persist over time. & MacFarland, 2015). Even so, the use of these
Even though the effect of treatment in the new methods in intervention promises an impor-
inclusive classroom may vary among research tant future.
studies, basic evidence about the importance of
this treatment will increase in future research.
Emotion Regulation

 he Collaboration Between
T To increase the social behaviors of children with
University and the Community ASD, emotion regulation is essential. There are
several issues that need to be addressed in order
Another important task is to study intervention to clarify the tendency of emotion regulation
among children with ASD in inclusive settings, research, one of which is to study the relationship
with collaboration between universities and com- character of the difficulties encountered by such
munity mental health centers. children.
Camargo and colleagues answered the require- Konstantareas and Stewart (2006) examined
ments for the use of research on interventions affect regulation (AR) and temperament in chil-
in schools based on the increasing number of dren with ASD. To determine AR, children were
234 C. Kanai et al.

exposed to a mildly frustrating situation. Those Even in studies concerning adults, to assess
with ASD exhibited lower control in attention emotional functioning in ASD and TD people is
focusing, inhibitory control, and soothability. suggestive. Samson and colleagues studied 27
The results showed that fewer symptoms of ASD ASD adults (16 women) and 27 age-, gender-,
and older chronological age predicted higher and education-matched TD participants, all of
effortful control. whom completed a battery of measures of emo-
Ashburner and colleagues compared teachers’ tion experience, labeling, and regulation
perceptions of students with ASD to their percep- (Samson, Huber, & Gross, 2012). With respect
tions of students with TD regard to the capacity to emotion regulation, individuals with ASD
to perform academically and regulate emotions used reappraisal less frequently than TD indi-
and behavior in mainstream classrooms, taking viduals and reported lower levels of reappraisal
into account behavioral and emotional difficul- self-efficacy.
ties (including attention difficulties, anxiety, It is important to note that these results in
depression, and oppositional and aggressive emotion regulation became more specific; hence,
behaviors) and offering alternative models of we must be cautious in evaluating the evidence of
supporting these students in mainstream class- the result.
rooms (Ashburner, Ziviani, & Rodger, 2010).
Students with ASD seem to be underperform-
ing relative to their levels of ability and are strug-  he Relation with Co-occurring
T
gling to maintain their attention and regulate their Problems of Emotional Control
emotions and behaviors in mainstream class-
rooms. Consideration thus needs to be given to Swain and colleagues studied a mechanistic
investigating alternative models of supporting model in which anxiety culminates via emotion
these students in mainstream classrooms. In addi- dysregulation and social motivation (Swain,
tion, Samson and colleagues examined the rela- Scarpa, White, & Laugeson, 2015). However,
tionship between emotion dysregulation and the social motivation did not appear to play a moder-
core features of ASD and found the strongest ating role in the relationship between emotion
association to be with repetitive behaviors regulation and anxiety, even when controlling for
(Samson et al., 2014). social awareness.
Among the younger children, Garon and col- Regarding emotion regulation and depres-
leagues investigated early temperamental profiles sion, Rieffe and colleagues examined the
and their associations with autistic symptoms in unique contribution of two aspects of emotion
high-risk infants (N = 138) with an older sibling regulation: awareness and coping (Rieffe et al.,
with ASD and low-risk infants (N = 73) with no 2011). Depression was unrelated to positive
family history of ASD (Garon, Bryson, mental coping strategies, and the conviction
Zwaigenbaum, Smith, Brian, Roberts, & that the emotion experience helps in dealing
Szatmari, 2009). These findings suggest that tem- with the problem, suggesting that a positive
perament may be a useful framework for under- approach to the problem and its subsequent
standing the emergence of ASD early in life. emotion experience are less effective in the
Gulsrud and colleagues studied 34 toddlers high-functioning ASD group.
with autism, and the mothers participated in an Mazefsky (2015) studied mechanistic and
early intervention targeting joint engagement applied papers on emotion regulation (ER) and
(Gulsrud, Jahromi, & Kasari, 2010). An effect of emotional experiences in ASD. Important con-
intervention was found such that children cepts for future research are discussed, including
decreased their expression of negativity across how to conceptualize emotion dysregulation in
the intervention, while mothers increased their ASD, the importance of capturing variability in
emotional and motivational scaffolding. Joint emotion dysregulation in ASD studies, and the
engagement in early days therefore has a role to promise of intervention approaches that target
play in emotion regulation. ER impairments.
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 235

Samson and colleagues studied maladaptive strengths in participant description and selection
behavior, which is common in ASD (Samson, of socially important dependent variables,
Hardan, Lee, Phillips, & Gross, 2015). However, whereas weaknesses were identified in data col-
the factors that give rise to maladaptive behavior lection related to procedural reliability and social
in this context are not well understood. The pres- validity of procedures.
ent study examined the role of emotion experi- Kokina and Kaczmarek (2014) summarized
ence and emotion regulation in maladaptive and discussed the results of some meta-analy-
behavior in individuals with ASD and those with ses of Social Stories™, with particular focus
TD. It found that, by decreasing negative emo- on the outcomes of the intervention, the meth-
tions, treatments targeting adaptive emotion reg- odological quality of intervention research,
ulation may reduce maladaptive behaviors in and the role of the possible moderators of their
individuals with ASD. effectiveness (e.g., participant and intervention
As indicated by these studies, the relationship characteristics).
between co-occurring problems of ASD and Reynhout and Carter (2006) put together a
emotional control may vary, meaning that review of the empirical research literature on
evidence-based research is necessary. Social Stories™, including a descriptive review
and single-subject meta-analysis of appropriate
studies. Examination of data suggests that the
Social Stories™ effects of Social Stories™ are highly variable.
Data on maintenance and generalization are also
Social Stories™ offer one effective method of limited. Social Stories™ stand as a promising
increasing social behavior and decreasing disrup- intervention, being relatively straightforward and
tive behavior. In terms of recent research on efficient to apply to a wide range of behaviors.
Social Stories™, quite a few reviews have been However, further research is needed to determine
published. the exact nature of their contribution and the
Sansosti and colleagues pointed to Social components critical to their efficacy.
Stories™ as one method that is increasingly sug- A meta-analysis of single-subject research
gested for teaching social skills to children with was conducted, examining the use of Social
ASD (Sansosti, Powell-Smith, & Kincaid, 2004). Stories™ and the role of a comprehensive set of
This article consequently offers a synthesis of the moderator variables (intervention and participant
available research regarding Social Stories™ and characteristics) on intervention outcomes
their effectiveness in education. (Kokina & Kern, 2010). While Social Stories™
Karkhaneh and colleagues conducted a sys- had low to questionable overall effectiveness,
tematic review of the literature using pre-defined, they were more effective when addressing inap-
rigorous methods (Karkhaneh et al., 2010). propriate behaviors than when teaching social
Studies were considered eligible if they were skills. Social Stories™ also seemed to be associ-
controlled trials evaluating Social Stories™ ated with improved outcomes when used in gen-
among persons with ASD. This review under- eral education settings and with target children as
scores the need for further rigorous research and their own intervention agents.
highlights some outstanding questions regarding Bozkurt and Vuran (2014) analyzed studies in
maintenance and generalization of the benefits of which Social Stories™ were used for teaching
Social Stories™. social skills to individuals with ASD. The present
Test and colleagues’ comprehensive review of study includes a descriptive review and meta-
Social Story™ literature included (a) a descrip- analysis of single-subject studies that met the cri-
tive review, (b) analysis of research quality, and teria. Although most studies showed that Social
(c) meta-analysis using a percentage of nonover- Stories™ were effective in teaching social skills
lapping data (Test, Richter, Knight, & Spooner, to children with ASD in the descriptive study, in
2011). Analysis of research quality yielded the meta-analytic study, the mean of Percentage
236 C. Kanai et al.

of Non-overlapping Data (PND) scores for all Brain Functions and Social Skills
studies was 63.43%, with a range of 0–100%.
These results suggest that Social Stories™ should Social skills encompass a wide group of abilities
not yet be considered as evidence-based practice that facilitate interactions and communications
for teaching social skills to individuals with with others. From infancy to childhood, humans
ASD. Nevertheless, they seem to constitute a acquire several social skills, such as the detection
promising practice that warrants future research. of biological motion, sensitivity to eye-like stim-
From these research studies, Social Stories™ ulus, joint attention, and social perspective tak-
appear effective in increasing social skills, ing, to solve social situations by predicting and
decreasing inappropriate behaviors, or widening understanding others’ intentions, emotions, and
the exhibited range of behaviors. Further behaviors. Brain regions that are involved in
research is needed to determine the exact nature social cognition are collectively referred to as the
of their contribution and the components critical social brain (Blakemore, 2008), and several brain
to their efficacy. regions in the social brain undergo structural and
functional changes during development.
However, the linkages between social skills and
 se of Social Stories™ in Preschool-­
U neural mechanisms and their implications for
Aged Children social deficits in ASD are still elusive. Here, we
will first review the linkages between social skills
Thompson and Johnston (2013) used a multiple and neural mechanisms in neurotypical children.
baseline across participants design to evaluate the Then, we will describe how abnormalities in the
effects of Social Stories™ so as to help preschool- social brain network affect social deficits in chil-
aged children with the characteristics of ASD dren with ASD.
increase their engagement in functional behaviors
and use sensory integrative-based strategies to
promote self-regulation. The intervention package  eural Correlates of the Social Skills
N
included reading individualized Social Stories™ in Neurotypical Children
that discussed desired behaviors, while self-regu-
lation strategies increased the frequency of desired There are several techniques to assess functions
behaviors for all participants. The use of self-regu- in the human brain. Electroencephalography
lation strategies varied across participants. These (EEG) is a noninvasive technique that records
findings suggest that the intervention was success- electrical brain activity from scalp electrodes
ful in increasing the desired behaviors. (DeBoer, Scott, & Nelson, 2007). EEG may be
An adapted alternating treatments design was suitable for measuring brain activity in infants
used to compare mother-developed and delivered and children, because, for other techniques such
Social Stories™ and video modeling in teaching as magnetoencephalography (MEG), magnetic
social skills to children with ASD (Acar, Tekin- resonance imaging (MRI), and functional MRI
Iftar, & Yikmis, 2016). Three other-child dyads (fMRI), children must fix their head during
participated in the study. Results showed that recordings.
mothers could develop Social Stories™ and The analyses of EEG signals indicate two
video images with 100% accuracy and imple- types of neural activity. One is the response
ment them with high treatment integrity. evoked by stimuli or events. Such response is
These studies showed various new directions called event-related potential (ERP). ERPs are
for using Social Stories™, especially with young calculated from the average of several trials with
children and within the mother-infant relation- the purpose of eliminating noises related to the
ship, and proposed new intervention methods stimulus of interest. Thus, this method provides
such as video monitoring. information regarding brain waves that are phase-
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 237

locked to the stimulus presentation (i.e., event). 2002; Hileman et al., 2011), indicating that the
Another method is the analysis of oscillatory N170 might be associated with face-specific pro-
brain activity. In this case, brain activity is not cessing. While both the P1 and N170 compo-
necessarily phase-locked to the event. This nents seem to be indicative of early face
method therefore allows us to investigate sponta- processing, it has been hypothesized that the P1
neous brain activity, i.e., brain activity not related component reflects holistic face processing,
to a specific task. Here, we will first review the while the N170 component reflects early struc-
evidence from EEG (i.e., ERP and oscillatory tural encoding of a face (Cassia, Kuefner,
brain activity) and then review evidence from Westerlund, & Nelson, 2006).
neuroimaging (i.e., MRI and fMRI). The P1 and N170 components are categorized
as early ERP components that usually occur dur-
ing the first 200 ms after the stimulus presenta-
EEG Evidence of Social Skills tion. The Nc component is a late ERP component
that occurs 200 ms after the stimulus presenta-
Several ERP components are implicated in social tion, and it exhibits a peak latency decreasing
cognitive skills. For example, the P1, which is from 800 ms in 1 month old (Karrer & Monti,
also known as P100, is a positive-going deflec- 1995) to 400–600 ms in 1–3 year olds (Goldman,
tion component that arises between 90 and Shapiro, & Nelson, 2004; Parker & Nelson,
150 ms after visual stimulus onset (Luyster, 2005). The peak amplitude of this component
Powell, Tager-Flusberg, & Nelson, 2014). Since increases with age over the first year of life and
P1 is evoked by visual stimulus, this component then decreases again in the third year of life
is observed in the occipital visual cortex. In (Luyster et al., 2014; Parker & Nelson, 2005).
infants and young children, the amplitude of the This component is elicited in several different
P1 component increases between 6 and 36 months studies (Courchesne, Ganz, & Norcia, 1981;
of age (Luyster et al., 2014). In contrast, the Striano, Reid, & Hoehl, 2006). Furthermore,
amplitude of P1 decreases as age increases from Striano et al. (2006) observed that, in infants, the
2 to 4 years (Itier & Taylor, 2004; Kuefner, de Nc component exhibited higher amplitude during
Heering, Jacques, Palmero-Soler, & Rossion, the joint attention condition than in the non-joint
2010), likely reflecting synaptic pruning. In line attention condition. Therefore, the Nc component
with this notion, Hileman, Henderson, Mundy, may be associated with mandatory attentional
Newell, and Jaime (2011) revealed that the P1 processing to a visual stimulus, but not specific to
component elicited by human faces exhibited a faces (Luyster et al., 2014).
decrease in its amplitude and latency between 9 In addition to ERPs, several lines of evidence
and 17 years of age. Furthermore, they observed imply that oscillatory brain activity participates
that neurotypical children and young adolescents significantly in social functioning. The extracra-
exhibited larger P1 amplitudes for inverted faces nial EEG signals reflect the neuronal population
when compared to upright faces, suggesting that activity and can be decomposed into different
a decrease of P1 amplitude along with increasing frequency ranges: delta (~2–4 Hz), theta
age might be associated with improvement of (~4–8 Hz), alpha (~8–12 Hz), beta (~12–30 Hz),
social cognitive skills such as facial recognition. and gamma frequencies (~30–100 Hz) (Donner
Another important ERP component is N170. & Siegel, 2011).
This component is a negative-going deflection The mu rhythm occurs in the alpha range
that occurs approximately 170 ms after visual between 8 and 12 Hz, and this rhythm has been
stimulus presentation (Eimer, 2000; Hileman implicated in social skills. The mu suppression
et al., 2011) and is observed over posterior tem- occurs not only during movement execution and
poral sites. This component exhibits a shorter planning but also motor imitation and observa-
latency and larger amplitude for faces compared tion of others’ goal-directed movement
to other stimuli (de Haan, Pascalis, & Johnson, (Raymaekers, Wiersema, & Roeyers, 2009).
238 C. Kanai et al.

These findings suggest that the mu suppression For fMRI evidence, Dapretto et al. (2006)
can reflect a putative activity of the mirror neuron observed that, in children from around 10 to
system (Rizzolatti & Sinigaglia, 2010). In addi- 14 years old, the brain regions that were activated
tion, di Pellegrino, Fadiga, Fogassi, Gallese, and during the imitation of emotions were the bilat-
Rizzolatti (1992) discovered mirror neurons in eral striate and extra-striate cortices, primary
the monkey premotor cortex. These neurons in motor and premotor regions, limbic structures
the rostral part of the inferior premotor cortex in (i.e., amygdala, ventral striatum, and insula), and
the monkey brain discharge during goal-directed the cerebellum. In addition, they found activation
hand movements such as grasping, holding, and within the bilateral pars opercularis of the infe-
tearing. Furthermore, several studies using fMRI rior frontal gyrus, with the strongest peaks in the
or EEG reported evidence of the mirror neuron right hemisphere. Although the number of fMRI
activity in the human brain (Iacoboni & Dapretto, studies in childhood is limited, these brain
2006). However, the neural mechanism connect- regions have been identified in adult humans
ing mu suppression with mirror neurons is still (Buccino et al., 2001), suggesting a possible rela-
unknown. In addition, only a few studies have tionship among imitation and mirror neuron
investigated the mu rhythm in children (Lepage networks.
& Theoret, 2006; Oberman et al., 2013). Further In addition, Saxe, Whitfield-Gabrieli, Scholz,
studies are needed to elucidate the linkages and Pelphrey (2009) reported that, in children
between mu suppressions elicited by observing between 6 and 11 years of age, the bilateral TPJ
others’ goal-directed movement and the mirror were involved in perceiving and reasoning about
neuron system. other people. Interestingly, they found that only
the right TPJ exhibited a significant correlation
with age, suggesting a maturational selectivity
 euroimaging Evidence of Social
N for social information. In addition, they observed
Skills that brain regions that were involved in theory of
mind processing did not overlap with brain
In recent years, a number of studies have investi- regions dedicated to the perception of biological
gated social skills using MRI and fMRI. Mills, motion. Furthermore, the recruitment of the right
Lalonde, Clasen, Giedd, and Blakemore (2014) pSTS was associated with the perception of bio-
have investigated structural trajectories of gray logical motion, which is in line with previous
matter volume, cortical thickness, and surface findings on young adulthood (Pelphrey et al.,
area in a longitudinal sample of 288 participants 2003). These findings suggest that a theory of
(age: 7–30 years, 857 total scans). Specifically, mind comprehension may rely on a distinct and
they focused on trajectories of brain regions later developed neural substrate.
involved in the social brain (Blakemore, 2008): Recently, Yang, Rosenblau, Keifer, and
medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), temporopari- Pelphrey (2015) reviewed neural systems impli-
etal junction (TPJ), posterior superior temporal cated in social perception, action observation,
sulcus (pSTS), and anterior temporal cortex and theory of mind. Each neural system involves
(ATC). The analyses of structural MRI data different brain regions: (1) a neural system for
revealed that gray matter volume and cortical social perception involves fusiform gyrus (FG),
thickness in mPFC, TPJ, and pSTS decreased amygdala, and orbitofrontal cortex, (2) a neural
from childhood into the early twenties. These system for action observation involves inferior
changes in brain and development can reflect the parietal lobule and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG),
cortical specialization of the pre-existing cerebral and (3) a neural network for theory of mind
structures and networks as a result of the expertise encompasses TPJ, mPFC, and posterior cingulate
associated with exposure to social environment cortex (PCC). The authors then proposed an
(Davidson & McEwen, 2012). integrative model of social information processing
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 239

and emphasized that the pSTS is the core brain components, in children with ASD (Baruth,
region of the three neural systems underlying Casanova, Sears, & Sokhadze, 2010; Hileman
social information processing. Furthermore, they et al., 2011). Considering the amplitudes of these
demonstrated that pSTS is functionally con- early components, Luyster et al. (2014) observed
nected with all brain regions involved in the three that, in autism, high-risk children between 6 and
neural systems (Yang et al., 2015), raising the 36 months of age did not evidence a maturational
possibility that functional connectivity between alteration in P1, having similar mean amplitudes
pSTS and other brain regions that are involved in to children with low risk. However, they found
social information processing could be a neuro- between-group differences at later ages.
nal marker for estimating the development of Another study also reported that children with
social abilities. ASD did not exhibit differential P1 amplitudes
for upright and inverted faces (Hileman et al.,
2011). It further observed that, while for neuro-
 eural Correlates of Social Deficits
N typical individuals, smaller P1 amplitudes were
in Children with ASD associated with fewer atypical social behaviors
and better social cognitive skills, in individuals
ASD is an umbrella term for a wide variety of with ASD, there were no relations between the
disorders that share common symptoms: (1) defi- ERP components and atypical social behaviors
cits in social interaction and social communica- and social cognition. These ERP differences
tion and (2) a restricted range of interests. These might reflect a low specificity of neuronal and
symptoms become evident after the third year of cognitive processes in children with ASD. To
life. It is therefore necessary to establish reliable support this view, recent studies have reported
markers for this group of disorders, which would aberrant functional organization in the ASD brain
allow early diagnosis and more effective inter- (Barttfeld et al., 2011). Further longitudinal stud-
ventions. Here, we will first review the evidence ies are needed to elucidate the developmental tra-
from EEG and then review evidence from neuro- jectory of ERP components related to social
imaging (i.e., MRI and fMRI) associated with deficits in individuals with ASD.
social deficits in children with ASD.

 euroimaging Evidence of Social


N
EEG Evidence of Social Deficits Skills

Multiple behavioral studies reported social defi- Using MRI and fMRI, several studies have
cits, including in the areas of face discrimination, reported morphological and functional abnor-
recognition, and emotion perception, in children malities in children and adults with ASD (Abrams
and adults with ASD (Boucher & Lewis, 1992; et al., 2013; Alaerts et al., 2015; Eyler, Pierce, &
Liu, Li, & Yi, 2016), raising the possibility that Courchesne, 2012; Philip et al., 2012; Wolff
aberrant neural activity during face processing in et al., 2015). As described in the previous sec-
children with ASD could be a neural marker for tion, brain regions that are involved in social cog-
deficits in social cognition. Dawson et al. (2002) nition form a large-scale network known as the
demonstrated that, as evidenced by their P400 social brain (Blakemore, 2008). The pSTS, in
and Nc components, children with ASD did not particular, plays a central role in facilitating
differentiate between familiar and unfamiliar social information processing among different
faces, although they did show differential types of neural systems (i.e., social perception,
response to unfamiliar objects compared to action observation, and theory of mind) (Yang
familiar ones. Furthermore, several studies et al., 2015). The pSTS has been implicated in
reported alterations of early visual components biological motion, eye gaze processing, face
related to face processing, such as the P1 and Nc processing, and speech processing (Hein &
240 C. Kanai et al.

Knight, 2008; Redcay, 2008), which are acquired There are screening measures and diagnostic
during infancy and early childhood (Courchesne measures in place in order to assess for children
et al., 1981; Jones & Klin, 2013). Thus, malfunc- with ASD. Both kinds of measures are very
tion of the STS may underlie impaired social important; however, each has a different role in
information processing, such as facial emotion assessment. The screening measures are never
recognition and affective prosody recognition, in diagnostic, nor should they be used in place of a
individuals with ASD (Rosenblau, Kliemann, diagnostic evaluation. By the same token, the
Dziobek, & Heekeren, 2016). diagnosis evaluation measures are not used for
Interestingly, toddlers with ASD also failed to screening.
activate the left superior temporal gyrus (STG) in Comprehensive support for children with
response to language (Eyler et al., 2012), while ASD is important. In order to conduct this sup-
children with ASD failed to activate the bilateral port, however, comprehensive assessment is nec-
STS in response to vocal sounds but exhibited a essary. Therefore, we have to engage in the
normal activation pattern in response to nonvocal assessment battery and ascertain the relation of
sounds (Gervais et al., 2004). Furthermore, the results of each assessment. For children with
Alaerts et al. (2015) recently delineated develop- suspected ASD, we must carry out specific
mental changes of functional connectivity assessment such as ADI-R and ADOS-2. Also,
between the pSTS and the other regions in the we must assess for the many aspects of ASD in
ASD brain using resting-state functional connec- children, with the cognitive functioning and
tivity with a cross-sectional approach. In this development levels including cognitive function,
study, individuals with ASD exhibited delayed communication skill, comorbid developmental
maturation in functional connections between the disorders, comorbid psychiatric symptoms, adap-
pSTS and the neural system for action percep- tive behavior, and psychosocial and environmen-
tion, while they also exhibited an atypical devel- tal assessment.
opmental trajectory in functional connections In this chapter, we also discussed formal
between the pSTS and neural system for social assessment of ASD symptoms. However, formal
perception. These different developmental trajec- assessments cannot cover all aspects, including
tories of distinct neural systems might reflect behavior and cognition, for each child. Informal
complex age-related changes in the social cogni- assessments include developmental history, med-
tive processes in the ASD brain. These insights ical history, educational history (such as school
will provide an opportunity to develop age- records), observation of behavior in unstructured
specific interventions in ASD. settings, etc. The informal assessments provide a
lot of valuable information about ASD zymol-
ogy, as do the formal assessments. Therefore,
 urrent Status and Future
C both formal assessments and informal assess-
Directions ments are necessary.
In the light of recent research on motor func-
There has been extensive research on the early tion, it could be concluded that children with
identification of ASD during the last two decades. ASD show a bias toward proprioceptive-based
Early detection and diagnosis are critical to the learning. Consequently, children with ASD are
delivery of early intervention, which could posi- strongly associated with irregular patterns of
tively impact both the developmental outcomes motor function (Mache & Todd, 2016; McCleery
(directly) and the family outcome (indirectly) et al., 2013). The neural mechanism that under-
(Zwaigenbaum et al., 2007). Thus, developing lies the atypical development of motor skills
effective approaches to the earliest possible diag- (basic motor skill, dyspraxia, imitation, etc.) in
nosis of ASD is necessary for the long-term children with ASD might be directly relevant to
effects and benefits of early intervention the neural basis of atypical development of social
(Baranek, 1999). and communicative skills (Mostofsky, 2013).
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 241

Body awareness develops through propriocep- face processing, and speech processing, it might
tive, vestibular, and tactile senses. These sensory be possible that the STS and its functional con-
dimensions are essential for the development of nectivity to other brain regions implicated in
motor control, posture, balance, spatial aptitude, social cognition could be a predictor for matura-
and spatial perception (Baranek et al., 2006; tion of social skills in children with and without
Minshew et al., 2004). Spatial perception is fun- ASD.
damental for the development of basic grammar
and language, sense of personal space, and social
dimensions (proxemics). Self-awareness is a part References
of body image, and self-consciousness is said to
be related more to language and communication. Abrams, D. A., Lynch, C. J., Cheng, K. M., Phillips,
J., Supekar, K., Ryali, S., … Menon, V. (2013).
Body image develops through explicit mental Underconnectivity between voice-selective cortex and
representations of bodily functions and experi- reward circuitry in children with autism. Proceedings
ences in the central nervous system. Vertical, of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
horizontal, and rotational space and movement States of America, 110, 12060–12065.
Acar, C., Tekin-Iftar, E., & Yikmis, A. (2016). Effects
inside (proprioceptive) and outside (spatial, of mother-delivered social stories and video model-
physical, and social) world experiences help chil- ing in teaching social skills to children with autism
dren to develop functional sensory and motor Spectrum disorders. National Association for Special
control. Sensorimotor-dominated movement Educational Needs, May 9.
Alaerts, K., Nayar, K., Kelly, C., Raithel, J., Milham,
interventions in combination with social- M. P., & Di Martino, A. (2015). Age-related changes
interactional therapeutic approaches should be in intrinsic function of the superior temporal sulcus
included in the early intervention system for chil- in autism spectrum disorders. Social Cognitive and
dren at risk of ASD as “early” as possible. Early Affective Neuroscience, 10, 1413–1423.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic
intervention programs should be achieved and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-­
through individualized family support, parent IV-­TR). Washington, DC: Author.
education, and conjoint family approaches American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
guided by professionals. and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5).
Washington, DC: Author.
With respect to the effect of treatment in the Ashburner, J., Ziviani, J., & Rodger, S. (2010). Surviving
inclusive classroom, various types of coordina- in the mainstream: Capacity of children with autism
tion between university and inclusive school set- spectrum disorders to perform academically and regu-
tings, emotion regulation, and Social Stories™ late their emotions and behavior at school. Research in
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 18–27.
are noteworthy. However, several rigorous lines Attwood, T. (2007). The complete guide to Asperger’s
of research, randomized clinical trials (RCTs), syndrome. New York, NY: Jessica Kingsley.
and definition of a particular disorder, along with Ayres, A. J. (1973). Sensory integration and learning
patient samples, treatment manuals, and system- disorders. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological
Services.
atic reviews must be necessary in future treat- Baranek, G. T. (1999). Autism during infancy: A retro-
ment trials. spective video analysis of sensory-motor and social
As for neural correlates of social skills in neu- behaviors at 9-12 months of age. Journal of Autism
rotypical children with ASD, previous behav- and Developmental Disorders, 29(3), 213–224.
Baranek, G. T., David, F. J., Poe, M. D., Stone, W. L.,
ioral, electrophysiological, and neuroimaging & Watson, L. R. (2006). Sensory experiences ques-
studies have provided the linkages between social tionnaire: Discriminating sensory features in young
deficits and neuronal alterations. Recent advances children with autism, developmental delays, and typi-
in machine learning techniques provide powerful cal development. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 47(6), 591–601.
tools to identify neuronal markers for ASD Barttfeld, P., Wicker, B., Cukier, S., Navarta, S., Lew, S.,
(Yahata et al., 2016). However, reliable neural & Sigman, M. (2011). A big-world network in ASD:
and behavioral markers for early diagnosis are Dynamical connectivity analysis reflects a deficit in
not yet established. Since the STS is implicated long-range connections and an excess of short-range
connections. Neuropsychologia, 49, 254–263.
in several social skills, such as biological motion,
242 C. Kanai et al.

Baruth, J. M., Casanova, M. F., Sears, L., & Sokhadze, Cassia, V. M., Kuefner, D., Westerlund, A., & Nelson,
E. (2010). Early-stage visual processing abnormalities C. A. (2006). A behavioural and ERP investigation of
in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder (ASD). 3-month-olds’ face preferences. Neuropsychologia,
Translational Neuroscience, 1, 177–187. 44, 2113–2125.
Belmonte, M. K., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2003). CDC. (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder
Functional anatomy of impaired selective atten- among children aged 8 years-autism and develop-
tion and compensatory processing in autism. Brain mental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites,
research. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 651–664. United States, 2010. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly
Bhat, A. N., Galloway, J. C., & Landa, R. J. (2012). Report. Surveillance Summaries (Washington, DC:
Relation between early motor delay and later com- 2002), 63, 1.
munication delay in infants at risk for autism. Infant Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2012). Context
Behavior & Development, 35(4), 838–846. modulates attention to social scenes in toddlers with
Bhat, A. N., Landa, R. J., & Galloway, J. C. (2011). autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Current perspectives on motor functioning in infants, 53, 903–913.
children, and adults with autism spectrum disorders. Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2013). Decreased
Physical Therapy, 91(7), 1116–1129. spontaneous attention to social scenes in 6-month-old
Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disor-
Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 267–277. ders. Biological Psychiatry, 74, 195–203.
Bolton, P. F., Golding, J., Emond, A., & Steer, C. D. Christiansz, J. A., Gray, K. M., Taffe, J., & Tonge, B. J.
(2012). Autism spectrum disorder and autistic traits in (2016). Autism spectrum disorder in the DSM-
the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children: 5: Diagnostic sensitivity and specificity in early
Precursors and early signs. Journal of the American childhood. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, Disorders, 46(6), 2054–2063.
249–260.e25. Courchesne, E., Ganz, L., & Norcia, A. M. (1981). Event-
Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face rec- related brain potentials to human faces in infants.
ognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal Child Development, 52, 804–811.
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A.,
Disciplines, 33, 843–859. Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Iacoboni, M.
Bozkurt, S. S., & Vuran, S. (2014). An analysis of the (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror
use of social stories in teaching social skills to chil- neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum
dren with autism spectrum disorders. Educational disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28–30.
Sciences: Theory & Practice, 14(5), 1875–1892. Davidson, R. J., & McEwen, B. S. (2012). Social influences
Bryson, S. A., & Ostmeyer, K. F. (2014). Increasing on neuroplasticity: Stress and interventions to promote
the effectiveness of community mental health center well-being. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 689–695.
social skills groups for children with autism spec- Dawson, G., Carver, L., Meltzoff, A. N., Panagiotides, H.,
trum disorder: A training and consultation example. McPartland, J., & Webb, S. J. (2002). Neural corre-
Administration and Policy in Mental Health, 1, lates of face and object recognition in young children
808–821. with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay,
Buccino, G., Binkofski, F., Fink, G. R., Fadiga, L., and typical development. Child Development, 73,
Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., … Freund, H. J. (2001). 700–717.
Action observation activates premotor and pari- de Haan, M., Pascalis, O., & Johnson, M. H. (2002).
etal areas in a somatotopic manner: An fMRI Specialization of neural mechanisms underlying face
study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, recognition in human infants. Journal of Cognitive
400–404. Neuroscience, 14, 199–209.
Camargo, S. P. H., Rispoli, M., Ganz, J., Hong, E. R., DeBoer, T., Scott, L. S., & Nelson, C. A. (2007). Methods
Davis, H., & Mason, R. (2014). A review of the quality for acquiring and analyzing infant event-related poten-
of behaviorally-based intervention research to improve tials. In M. de Haan (Ed.), Infant EEG and event-­
social interaction skills of children with ASD in inclu- related potentials (pp. 5–37). New York: Taylor &
sive settings. Journal of Autism and Developmental Francis.
Disorders, 4, 2096–2116. Dejean, V. M. (2008). Vestibular re integration of the
Campbell, D. J., Shic, F., Macari, S., & Chawarska, K. autistic child developmental model for autism.
(2014). Gaze response to dyadic bids at 2 years New York: iUniverse, Inc.
related to outcomes at 3 years in autism spectrum dis- di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., &
orders: A subtyping analysis. Journal of Autism and Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events:
Developmental Disorders, 44, 431–442. A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain
Cappadocia, M. C., & Weiss, J. A. (2011). Review of Research, 91, 176–180.
social skills training groups for youth with Asperger Donner, T. H., & Siegel, M. (2011). A framework for
syndrome and high functioning autism research. local cortical oscillation patterns. Trends in Cognitive
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 70–78. Sciences, 15, 191–199.
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 243

Dowell, L. R., Mahone, E. M., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2009). in a high-risk infant sib cohort. Journal of Abnormal
Associations of postural knowledge and basic motor Child Psychology, 37(1), 59–78.
skill with dyspraxia in autism: Implication for abnor- Gervais, H., Belin, P., Boddaert, N., Leboyer, M., Coez, A.,
malities in distributed connectivity and motor learn- Sfaello, I., … Zilbovicius, M. (2004). Abnormal corti-
ing. Neuropsychology, 23(5), 563–570. cal voice processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience,
Dziuk, M. A., Gidley Larson, J. C., Apostu, A., Mahone, 7, 801–802.
E. M., Denckla, M. B., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2007). Ghaziuddin, M., & Butler, E. (1998). Clumsiness in autism
Dyspraxia in autism: Association with motor, social, and Asperger syndrome: A further report. Journal of
and communicative deficits. Developmental Medicine Intellectual Disability Research, 42(1), 43–48.
and Child Neurology, 49(10), 734–739. Goldberg, W. A., Thorsen, K. L., Osann, K., & Spence,
Eimer, M. (2000). Effects of face inversion on the struc- M. A. (2008). Use of home videotapes to confirm
tural encoding and recognition of faces. Evidence parental reports of regression in autism. Journal
from event-related brain potentials. Brain research. of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(6),
Cognitive Brain Research, 10, 145–158. 1136–1146.
Elsabbagh, M., Gliga, T., Pickles, A., Hudry, K., Charman, Goldman, D. Z., Shapiro, E. G., & Nelson, C. A. (2004).
T., & Johnson, M. H. (2013). The development of face Measurement of vigilance in 2-year-old children.
orienting mechanisms in infants at-risk for autism. Developmental Neuropsychology, 25, 227–250.
Behavioural Brain Research, 251, 147–154. Grzadzinski, R., Dick, C., Lord, C., & Bishop, S. (2016).
Esposito, G., Venuti, P., Maestro, S., & Muratori, F. Parent-reported and clinician-observed autism spec-
(2009). An exploration of symmetry in early autism trum disorder (ASD) symptoms in children with
spectrum disorders: Analysis of lying. Brain & attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD):
Development, 31(2), 131–138. Implications for practice under DSM-5. Molecular
Eyler, L. T., Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2012). A fail- Autism, 7, 7.
ure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language Guillona, Q., Hadjikhani, N., Baduela, S., & Rogéa, B.
is an early emerging and fundamental property of (2014). Visual social attention in autism spectrum dis-
autism. Brain, 135, 949–960. order: Insights from eye tracking studies. Neuroscience
Falck-Ytter, T., Rehnberg, E., & Bölte, S. (2013). Lack of and Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 279–297.
visual orienting to biological motion and audio visual Gulsrud, A. C., Jahromi, L. B., & Kasari, C. (2010).
synchrony in 3-year-olds with autism. PloS One, 8, The co-regulation of emotions between mothers and
e68816. their children with autism. Journal of Autism and
Fischer, J., Koldewyn, K., Jiang, Y. V., & Kanwisher, Developmental Disorders, 40, 227–237.
N. (2013). Unimpaired attentional disengagement Haswell, C. C., Izawa, J., Dowell, L. R., Mostofsky,
and social orienting in children with autism. Clinical S. H., & Shadmehr, R. (2009). Representation of
Psychological Science, 2, 214–223. internal models of action in the autistic brain. Nature
Fournier, K. A., Hass, C. J., Naik, S. K., Lodha, N., & Neuroscience, 12(8), 970–972.
Cauraugh, J. H. (2010). Motor coordination in autism Hein, G., & Knight, R. T. (2008). Superior temporal
spectrum disorders: A synthesis and meta-analysis. sulcus – it’s my area: Or is it? Journal of Cognitive
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Neuroscience, 20, 2125–2136.
40(10), 1227–1240. Hileman, C. M., Henderson, H., Mundy, P., Newell, L.,
Freedman, B., Kalb, L., Zablotsky, B., & Stuart, E. (2012). & Jaime, M. (2011). Developmental and individual
Relationship status among parents of children with differences on the P1 and N170 ERP components
autism spectrum disorders: A population-based study. in children with and without autism. Developmental
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Neuropsychology, 36, 214–236.
42(4), 539–548. Hill, E. L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism.
Freeman, S. F., Gulsrud, A., & Kasari, C. (2015). Brief Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), 26–32.
report: Linking early joint attention and play abilities Hill, E. L., & Bird, C. M. (2006). Executive processes
to later reports of friendships for children with ASD. in Asperger syndrome: Patterns of performance in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, a multiple case series. Neuropsychologia, 44(14),
45(7), 2259–2266. 2822–2835.
Frith, U. (1993). Autism. Scientific American, 268(6), Horak, F. B., Shumway-Cook, A., Crowe, T. K., &
108–114. Black, F. O. (1988). Vestibular function and motor
Gantman, A., Kapp, S. K., Orenski, K., & Laugeson, proficiency of children with impaired hearing, or
E. A. (2012). Social skills training for young adults with learning disability and motor impairments.
with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: A Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology,
randomized controlled pilot study. Journal of Autism 30(1), 64–79.
and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1094–1103. Hosozawa, M., Tanaka, K., Shimizu, T., Nakano, T., &
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., Zwaigenbaum, L., Smith, I. Kitazawa, S. (2012). How children with specific
M., Brian, J., Roberts, W., & Szatmari, P. (2009). language impairment view social situations: An eye
Temperament and its relationship to autistic symptoms tracking study. Pediatrics, 129, 1453–1460.
244 C. Kanai et al.

Hotton, M., & Coles, S. (2016). The effectiveness of Kasari, C., Rotheram-Fuller, E., Locke, J., & Gulsrud,
social skills training groups for individuals with A. (2012). Making the connection: Randomized
autism spectrum disorder. Review Journal of Autism controlled trial of social skills at school for children
and Developmental Disorders, 3, 68–81. with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child
Howlin, P., & Asgharian, A. (1999). The diagnosis of Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines,
autism and Asperger syndrome: Findings from a sur- 53(4), 431–439.
vey of 770 families. Developmental Medicine and Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones,
Child Neurology, 41, 834–839. W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-
Iacoboni, M., & Dapretto, M. (2006). The mirror neu- social contingencies rather than biological motion.
ron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nature, 459, 257–261.
Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 7, 942–951. Klin, A., Saulnier, C. A., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti,
Itier, R. J., & Taylor, M. J. (2004). Face recognition D. V., Volkmar, F. R., & Lord, C. (2007). Social
memory and configural processing: A developmen- and communication abilities and disabilities in
tal ERP study using upright, inverted, and contrast- higher functioning individuals with autism spec-
reversed faces. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, trum disorders: The Vineland and the ADOS.
487–502. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Jansiewicz, E. M., Goldberg, M. C., Newschaffer, C. J., 37, 748–759.
Denckla, M. B., Landa, R., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2006). Kokina, A., & Kaczmarek, L. A. (2014). Social Story™
Motor signs distinguish children with high function- interventions for children with autism spectrum dis-
ing autism and Asperger’s syndrome from controls. orders. Comprehensive guide to autism (Section XV,
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, pp. 2263–2281). New York, NY: Springer.
36(5), 613–621. Kokina, A., & Kern, L. (2010). Social story™ interven-
Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present tions for students with autism spectrum disorders: A
but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental
with autism. Nature, 504, 427–431. Disorders, 40(7), 812–826.
Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of pref- Konstantareas, M. M., & Stewart, K. (2006). Affect regu-
erential looking to the eyes of approaching adults pre- lation and temperament in children with autism spec-
dicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers trum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
with autism spectrum disorder. Archives of General Disorders, 36(2), 146–152.
Psychiatry, 65, 946–954. Kranowitz, C. S. (2006). The out-of-sync child:
Kanai, C., Hashimoto, R., Yamada, T., Jinbo, D., Ichihashi, Recognising and coping with sensory integration
K., Iwanami, A., & Kato N (2013). Gaze patterns in dysfunction (Revised ed., pp. 4–20). New York, NY:
infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder in Japan. Perigee.
The 36th Annual Meeting of the Japan Neuroscience Kuefner, D., de Heering, A., Jacques, C., Palmero-Soler,
Society, 328. E., & Rossion, B. (2010). Early visually evoked elec-
Kanai, C., Yokoi, H., Matsushita, Y., Saito, E., & Kato, trophysiological responses over the human brain (P1,
N. (2013). An influence of the group therapy for ASD N170) show stable patterns of face-sensitivity from 4
families of the ASD and typical developmental people years to adulthood. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience,
couple, and the ASD children -studying for focusing 3, 67.
on husband and wife relationship in ASD families. Lakatos, K., & Toth, G. (2014). An introduction of the
Psychiatry, 22, 679–686. (in Japanese). Hungarian BHRG model as a successful method in
Kanne, S. M., Gerber, A. J., Quirmbach, L. M., Sparrow, early childhood intervention. Journal of Education
S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Saulnier, C. A. (2011). The and Child Studies, 6(1), 27–38.
role of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disor- Landa, R., & Garrett-Mayer, E. (2006). Development in
ders: Implications for functional outcome. Journal of infants with autism spectrum disorders: A prospective
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1007–1018. study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Karkhaneh, M., Clark, B., Ospina, M. B., Seida, J. C., and Allied Disciplines, 47(6), 629–638.
Smith, V., & Hartling, L. (2010). Social Stories™ to Landrigan, P. J., Lambertini, L., & Birnbaum, L. S.
improve social skills in children with autism spec- (2012). A research strategy to discover the environ-
trum disorder: A systematic review. Autism, 14(6), mental causes of autism and neurodevelopmental dis-
641–662. abilities. Environmental Health Perspectives, 120(7),
Karrer, R., & Monti, L. A. (1995). Event-related poten- 258–260.
tials of 4-7-week-old infants in a visual recognition Lane, A. E., Young, R. L., Baker, A. E., & Angley, M. T.
memory task. Electroencephalography and Clinical (2010). Sensory processing subtypes in autism:
Neurophysiology, 94, 414–424. Association with adaptive behavior. Journal of Autism
Kasari, C., Locke, J., Gulsrud, A., & Rotheram-Fuller, and Developmental Disorders, 40(1), 112–122.
E. (2011). Social networks and friendships at school: Lane, S. J., Ivey, C. K., & May-Benson, T. A. (2014). Test
Comparing children with and without ASD. Journal of ideational praxis (TIP): Preliminary findings and
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 533–544. interrater and test-retest reliability with preschoolers.
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 245

American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 68(5), Mehling, W. E., Wrubel, J., Daubenmier, J. J., Price,
555–561. C. J., Kerr, C. E., Silow, T., … Stewart, A. L. (2011).
Laushey, K. M., & Heflin, L. J. (2000). Enhancing social Body awareness: A phenomenological inquiry into the
skills of kindergarten children with autism through the common ground of mind-body therapies. Philosophy,
training of multiple peers as tutors. Journal of Autism Ethics, and Humanities in Medicine, 6, 6.
and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 183–193. Miller, L. J., Anzalone, M. E., Lane, S. J., Cermak, S. A.,
Lepage, J. F., & Theoret, H. (2006). EEG evidence for the & Osten, E. T. (2007). Concept evolution in sen-
presence of an action observation-execution matching sory integration: A proposed nosology for diagnosis.
system in children. European Journal of Neuroscience, American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 61(2),
23, 2505–2510. 135–140.
Liu, W., Li, M., & Yi, L. (2016). Identifying children Mills, K. L., Lalonde, F., Clasen, L. S., Giedd, J. N., &
with autism spectrum disorder based on their face pro- Blakemore, S. J. (2014). Developmental changes
cessing abnormality: A machine learning framework. in the structure of the social brain in late childhood
Autism Research, 9, 888–898. and adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective
Luyster, R. J., Powell, C., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Neuroscience, 9, 123–131.
Nelson, C. A. (2014). Neural measures of social Minshew, N. J., Sung, K., Jones, B. L., & Furman, J. M.
attention across the first years of life: Characterizing (2004). Underdevelopment of the postural control sys-
typical development and markers of autism tem in autism. Neurology, 63(11), 2056–2061.
risk. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, Mostofsky, S. H. (2013). Motor skill and motor learning
131–143. deficits in children with autism: Implications for social
Mache, M. A., & Todd, T. A. (2016). Gross motor skills skill development and therapeutic intervention. Sharing
are related to postural stability and age in children treatment and autism research sources (STAR) parent
with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism trainings archive, The Center for Autism and Related
Spectrum Disorders, 23(3), 179–187. Disorders (CARD) at Kennedy Krieger Institute, Johns
MacNeil, L. K., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2012). Specificity of Hopkins University School of Medicine.
dyspraxia in children with autism. Neuropsychology, Mostofsky, S. H., Dubey, P., Jerath, V. K., Jansiewicz,
26(2), 165–171. E. M., Goldberg, M. C., & Denckla, M. B. (2006).
Mandell, D. S., Novak, M. M., & Zubritsky, C. D. (2005). Developmental dyspraxia is not limited to imitation
Factors associated with age of diagnosis among chil- in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
dren with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 116, the International Neuropsychological Society, 12(3),
1480–1486. 314–326.
Mandich, A. D., Polatajko, H. J., Macnab, J. J., & Miller, Nakano, T., Tanaka, K., Endo, Y., Yamane, Y., Yamamoto,
L. T. (2001). Treatment of children with developmen- T., Nakano, Y., … Kitazawa, S. (2010). Atypical gaze
tal coordination disorders: What is the evidence? In patterns in children and adults with autism spectrum
C. Missiuna (Ed.), Children with developmental coor- disorders dissociated from developmental changes in
dination disorder: Strategies for success (pp. 51–68). gaze behaviour. Proceedings of the Royal Society B:
Binghamton, NY: The Haworth Press. Biological Sciences, 277, 2935–2943.
Martos-Perez, J., & Paula-Perez, I. (2011). An approach Noterdaeme, M., Mildenberger, K., Minow, F., &
to the executive functions in autism spectrum disorder. Amorosa, H. (2002). Evaluation of neuromotor defi-
Revista de Neurologia, 52(Suppl 1), 147–153. cits in children with autism and children with a spe-
May-Benson, T. A., & Cermak, S. A. (2007). Development cific speech and language disorder. European Child &
of an assessment for ideational praxis. American Adolescent Psychiatry, 11(5), 219–225.
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 61(2), 148–153. Oberman, L. M., McCleery, J. P., Hubbard, E. M., Bernier,
May-Benson, T. A., & Koomar, J. A. (2010). Systematic R., Wiersema, J. R., Raymaekers, R., & Pineda, J. A.
review of the research evidence examining the effec- (2013). Developmental changes in mu suppression to
tiveness of interventions using a sensory integra- observed and executed actions in autism spectrum dis-
tive approach for children. American Journal of orders. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,
Occupational Therapy, 64(3), 403–414. 8, 300–304.
Mazefsky, C. A. (2015). Emotion regulation and emotional Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A. M., Young, G. S., Hepburn, S.,
distress in autism spectrum disorder: Foundations and Thompson, M., Colombi, C., … Rogers, S. J. (2011).
considerations for future research. Journal of Autism Onset patterns in autism: Correspondence between
and Developmental Disorders, 45, 3405–3408. home video and parent report. Journal of the American
McCleery, J. P., Elliott, N. A., Sampanis, D. S., & Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(8),
Stefanidou, C. A. (2013). Motor development and 796–806.e1.
motor resonance difficulties in autism: Relevance to Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Goldring, S., Greiss-Hess, L.,
early intervention for language and communication Herrera, A. M., Steele, J., … Rogers, S. J. (2008).
skills. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7, 30. Gross motor development, movement abnormalities,
McPartland, J. C., & Law, K. (2016). Autism spectrum and early identification of autism. Journal of Autism
disorder. Encyclopedia of Mental Health, 1, 124–130. and Developmental Disorders, 38(4), 644–656.
246 C. Kanai et al.

Panerai, S., Tasca, D., Ferri, R., Genitori D’Arrigo, V., with high functioning autism. Brain Research, 1304,
& Elia, M. (2014). Executive functions and adaptive 113–121.
behaviour in autism spectrum disorders with and with- Redcay, E. (2008). The superior temporal sulcus per-
out intellectual disability. Psychiatry Journal, 2014, forms a common function for social and speech per-
1–11. ception: Implications for the emergence of autism.
Parker, S. W., & Nelson, C. A. (2005). The impact of Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 123–142.
early institutional rearing on the ability to discriminate Reichow, B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2010). Social skills
facial expressions of emotion: An event-related poten- interventions for individuals with autism: Evaluation
tial study. Child Development, 76, 54–72. for evidence-based practices within a best evi-
Parush, S., Winokur, M., Goldstand, S., & Miller, L. J. dence synthesis framework. Journal of Autism and
(2002). Prediction of school performance using the Developmental Disorders, 40, 149–166.
Miller Assessment for Preschoolers (MAP): A valid- Reynhout, G., & Carter, M. (2006). Social Stories™
ity study. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, for children with disabilities. Journal of Autism and
56(5), 547–555. Developmental Disorders, 36(4), 445–469.
Pelphrey, K. A., Mitchell, T. V., McKeown, M. J., Rieffe, C., Oosterveld, P., Terwogt, M. M., Mootz, S.,
Goldstein, J., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (2003). Leeuwen, E. V., & Stockmann, L. (2011). Emotion
Brain activity evoked by the perception of human regulation and internalizing symptoms in children with
walking: Controlling for meaningful coherent motion. autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 15(6), 655–670.
Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 6819–6825. Rinehart, N. J., Tonge, B. J., Iansek, R., McGinley, J.,
Perry, A., Flanagan, H. E., Dunngeier, J., & Freeman, Brereton, A. V., Enticott, P. G., & Bradshaw, J. L.
N. L. (2009). Brief report: The Vineland Adaptive (2006). Gait function in newly diagnosed children
Behavior Scales in young children with autism spec- with autism: Cerebellar and basal ganglia related
trum disorders at different cognitive levels. Journal of motor disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1066–1078. Neurology, 48(10), 819–824.
Philip, R. C., Dauvermann, M. R., Whalley, H. C., Risdal, D., & Singer, G. H. S. (2004). Marital adjustment
Baynham, K., Lawrie, S. M., & Stanfield, A. C. in parents of children with disabilities: A historical
(2012). A systematic review and meta-analysis of review and meta-analysis. Research and Practice for
the fMRI investigation of autism spectrum disorders. Persons with Severe Disabilities, 29(2), 95–103.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 901–942. Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role
Pickles, A., Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Bailey, A., Le of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and
Couteur, A., Sim, C. H., & Rutter, M. (1995). Latent- misinterpretations. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 11,
class analysis of recurrence risks for complex pheno- 264–274.
types with selection and measurement error: A twin Robinson, S., Goddard, L., Dritschel, B., Wisley, M., &
and family history study of autism. American Journal Howlin, P. (2009). Executive functions in children
of Human Genetics, 57, 717–726. with autism spectrum disorders. Brain and Cognition,
Pierce, K., Conant, D., Hazin, R., Stoner, R., & Desmond, 71(3), 362–368.
J. (2011). Preference for geometric patterns early in Rosenblau, G., Kliemann, D., Dziobek, I., & Heekeren,
life as a risk factor for autism. Archives of General H. R. (2016). Emotional prosody processing in autism
Psychiatry, 68, 101–109. spectrum disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective
Pierce, K., Marinero, S., Hazin, R., McKenna, B., Barnes, Neuroscience, 12(2), 224–239.
C. C., & Malige, A. (2016). Eye tracking reveals Saemundsen,E.,Magnusson,P.,Smari,J.,&Sigurdardottir,
abnormal visual preference for geometric images as S. (2003). Autism diagnostic interview-revised and the
an early biomarker of an autism spectrum disorder childhood autism rating scale: Convergence and dis-
subtype associated with increased symptom severity. crepancy in diagnosing autism. Journal of Autism and
Biological Psychiatry, 79, 657–666. Developmental Disorders, 33(3), 319–328.
Plavnick, J. B., Kaid, T., & MacFarland, M. C. (2015). Samson, A. C., Hardan, A. Y., Lee, I. A., Phillips, J. M.,
Effects of a school-based social skills training pro- & Gross, J. J. (2015). Maladaptive behavior in autism
gram for adolescents with autism spectrum disorder spectrum disorder: The role of emotion experi-
and intellectual disability. Journal of Autism and ence and emotion regulation. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 45, 2674–2690. Developmental Disorders, 45(11), 3424–3432.
Ramisch, J. (2012). Marriage and family therapists work- Samson, A. C., Phillips, J. M., Parker, K. J., Shweta, S.,
ing with couples who have children with autism. Gross, J. J., & Hardan, A. Y. (2014). Emotion dysregu-
Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 38, 305–316. lation and the core features of autism spectrum disor-
Rao, P. A., Beidel, D. C., & Murray, M. J. (2008). Social der. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
skills interventions for children with Asperger’s syn- 44(7), 1766–1772.
drome or high-functioning autism: A review and rec- Samson, C. A., Huber, O., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Emotion
ommendations. Journal of Autism and Developmental regulation in Asperger’s syndrome and high-
Disorders, 38, 353–361. functioning autism. Emotion, 12(4), 659–665.
Raymaekers, R., Wiersema, J. R., & Roeyers, H. (2009). Sansosti, F. J., Powell-Smith, K. A., & Kincaid, D. (2004).
EEG study of the mirror neuron system in children A research synthesis of social story interventions for
Social Skills in Autism Spectrum Disorders 247

children with autism spectrum disorders. Focus on logical and ecological tests. Annals of Physical and
Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 19, Rehabilitation Medicine, 56(4), 268–287.
194–204. Townsend, J., Courchesne, E., & Egaas, B. (1996). Slowed
Saxe, R. R., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Scholz, J., & Pelphrey, orienting of covert visual-spatial attention in autism:
K. A. (2009). Brain regions for perceiving and reason- Specific deficits associated with cerebellar and pari-
ing about other people in school-aged children. Child etal abnormality. Development and Psychopathology,
Development, 80, 1197–1209. 8, 563–584.
Schoen, S. A., Miller, L. J., Brett-Green, B. A., & Nielsen, Travers, B. G., Kana, R. K., Klinger, L. G., Klein, C. L.,
D. M. (2009). Physiological and behavioral differences & Klinger, M. R. (2014). Motor learning in individuals
in sensory processing: A comparison of children with with autism Spectrum disorder: Activation in superior
autism spectrum disorder and sensory modulation dis- parietal lobule related to learning and repetitive behav-
order. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 3, 29. iors. Autism Research, 8(1), 38–51.
Schwatzer, J. J., Koening, C. M., & Berman, R. F. (2013). Turner, L. M., Stone, W. L., Pozdol, S. L., & Coonrod, E.
Using mouse models of autism spectrum disorders to E. (2006). Follow-up of children with autism spectrum
study the neurotoxicology of gene–environment inter- disorders from age 2 to age 9. Autism, 10(3), 243–265.
actions. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 36, 17–35. Van Waelvelde, H., Oostra, A., Dewitte, G., Van Den
Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Broeck, C., & Jongmans, M. J. (2010). Stability of
Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of motor problems in young children with or at risk of
mind in Asperger syndrome. Science, 325, 883–885. autism spectrum disorders, ADHD, and or devel-
Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals opmental coordination disorder. Developmental
show superior performance on the block design task? Medicine and Child Neurology, 52(8), 174–178.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and von dem Hagen, E. A., Stoyanova, R. S., Rowe, J. B.,
Allied Disciplines, 34(8), 1351–1364. Baron-Cohen, S., & Calder, A. J. (2014). Direct gaze
Shi, F., Wang, L., Peng, Z., Wee, C. Y., & Shen, D. (2013). elicits atypical activation of the theory-of-mind net-
Altered modular organization of structural cortical net- work in autism spectrum conditions. Cerebral Cortex,
works in children with autism. PLoS One, 8, e63131. 24(6), 1485–1492.
Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (2005). Webb, S. J., Dawson, G., Bernier, R., & Panagiotides, H.
Vineland-II adaptive behavior scales. Circle Pines, (2006). ERP evidence of atypical face processing in
MN: AGS Publishing. young children with autism. Journal of Autism and
Srinivasan, S. M., Kaur, M., Park, I. K., Gifford, T. D., Developmental Disorders, 36, 881–890.
Marsh, K. L., & Bhat, A. N. (2015). The effects of Werner, E., & Dawson, G. (2005). Validation of the phe-
rhythm and robotic interventions on the imitation/ nomenon of autistic regression using home videotapes.
praxis, interpersonal synchrony, and motor perfor- Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(8), 889–895.
mance of children with autism spectrum disorder Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N.
(ASD): A pilot randomized controlled trial. Autism (2000). Brief report: Recognition of autism spectrum
Research Treat, 736516. doi:10.1155/2015/736516). disorder before one year of age: A retrospective study
Striano, T., Reid, V. M., & Hoehl, S. (2006). Neural mech- based on home videotapes. Journal of Autism and
anisms of joint attention in infancy. European Journal Developmental Disorders, 30(2), 157–162.
of Neuroscience, 23, 2819–2823. White, S. W., Keonig, K., & Scahill, L. (2007). Social
Swain, D., Scarpa, A., White, S., & Laugeson, E. (2015). skills development in children with autism spectrum
Emotion dysregulation and anxiety in adults with disorders: A review of the intervention research.
ASD: Does social motivation play a role? Journal Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37,
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(12), 1858–1868.
3971–3977. WHO. (2007). Early childhood development: A power-
Task Force on Promotion and Dissemination of ful equalizer. Geneva: World Health Organization.
Psychological Procedures. (1995). Training in and Retrieved August 7, 2016, from http://www.who.int/
dissemination of empirically-validated psychological social_determinants/resources/ecd_kn_report_07_
treatments. The Clinical Psychologist, 48, 3–23. 2007.pdf
Test, D. W., Richter, S., Knight, V., & Spooner, F. (2011). Williams, M. S., & Shellenberger, S. (1996). How does your
A comprehensive review and meta-analysis of the engine run? Leader’s guide to the alert program for self-
social stories literature. Focus on Autism and Other regulation. Albuquerque, NM: Therapy Works.
Developmental Disabilities, 26(1), 49–62. Wolff, J. J., Gerig, G., Lewis, J. D., Soda, T., Styner,
Thompson, R. M., & Johnston, S. (2013). Use of social M. A., Vachet, C., … Piven, J. (2015). Altered corpus
stories to improve self-regulation in children with callosum morphology associated with autism over the
autism spectrum disorders. Physical and Occupational first 2 years of life. Brain, 138, 2046–2058.
Therapy in Pediatrics, 33(3), 271–284. Yahata, N., Morimoto, J., Hashimoto, R., Lisi, G., Shibata,
Toussaint-Thorin, M., Marchal, F., Benkhaled, O., Pradat- K., Kawakubo, Y., … Kawato, M. (2016). A small
Diehl, P., Boyer, F. C., & Chevignard, M. (2013). number of abnormal brain connections predicts adult
Executive functions of children with developmental autism spectrum disorder. Nature Communications, 7,
dyspraxia: Assessment combining neuropsycho- 11254.
248 C. Kanai et al.

Yang, D. Y., Rosenblau, G., Keifer, C., & Pelphrey, International Journal on Disability and Human
K. A. (2015). An integrative neural model of Development, 12, 25–29.
social perception, action observation, and theory of Zachor, D. A., & Curatolo, P. (2014). Recommendations
mind. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, for early diagnosis and intervention in autism spec-
263–275. trum disorders: An Italian-Israeli consensus confer-
Young, R. L., Brewer, N., & Pattison, C. (2003). ence. European Paediatric Neurology Society, 18,
Parental identification of early behavioural abnor- 107–118.
malities in children with autistic disorder. Autism, Zwaigenbaum, L., Thurm, A., Stone, W., Baranek,
7(2), 125–143. G., Bryson, S., Iverson, J., … Sigman, M. (2007).
Yukawa, Y., Ohta, H., Kanai, C., Tani, M., Yamada, T., Studying the emergence of autism spectrum disorders in
Hashimoto, R., … Iwanami, A. (2013). Clinical high-risk infants: Methodological and practical issues.
and psychosocial characteristics in adults with per- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
vasive development disorders: A survey in Japan. 37(3), 466–480.
Intellectual Disability and Social
Skills

Jeff Sigafoos, Giulio E. Lancioni, Nirbhay N. Singh,


and Mark F. O’Reilly

hyper-sociability, inappropriate hugging/touching,


Introduction stereotyped movements, self-injury, aggression,
and tantrums) are more frequent, severe, and per-
Intellectual disability, or mental retardation, sistent among children with intellectual disability
refers to impairment of cognitive and adaptive than children without disabilities (Hoch et al.,
behavioral functioning that negatively affects 2016; Sturmey & Didden, 2014; Wilde, Mitchell,
development and learning (National Center on & Oliver, 2016).
Birth Defects and Developmental Disabilities, Intellectual disability is often suspected when
2016). Cognitive impairments include subaver- the child shows delays in the acquisition of
age intellectual ability, deficit knowledge base, important developmental milestones, such as
and various attentional and memory problems speech, socialization, peer interaction, and play
(Kirk, Gray, Riby, & Cornish, 2015; O’Reilly & skills. Delays are also often evident in the acqui-
Carr, 2016). Adaptive functioning impairments sition of self-care skills, such as feeding, dress-
include skill deficits and excess [problem] behav- ing, and toileting (Patel, Greydanus, Merrick, &
ior (Lovaas, 2003). Skill deficits can occur Rubin, 2016). At school, children with intellec-
across a number of areas or domains, such as the tual disability generally learn more slowly than
self-­
care, communication, leisure/recreation, and typically developing peers and consequently
social skills domains. Excess behaviors (e.g., learn less and achieve a lower overall level of
academic proficiency (Nettelbeck & Wilson,
1997). However, despite their intellectual impair-
J. Sigafoos, Ph.D. (*) ment and adaptive behavior deficits, children
School of Education, Victoria University of with intellectual disability can learn to function
Wellington, P.O. Box 600, Wellington 6140, more effectively and participate more actively in
New Zealand
e-mail: jeff.sigafoos@vuw.ac.nz society (Wehmeyer, Lee, & Shogren, 2016). To
this end, the acquisition of appropriate social
G.E. Lancioni, Ph.D.
University of Bari, Bari, Italy skills is critically important to effective functioning
and participation in home, school, and commu-
N.N. Singh, Ph.D.
Medical College of Georgia, Augusta University, nity environments (Wilkins & Matson, 2007).
Augusta, GA, USA Important clinical questions include how to
M.F. O’Reilly, Ph.D. assess and teach social skills to children with
The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA intellectual disability.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 249


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_14
250 J. Sigafoos et al.

Aim of This Chapter ses. Still, all three diagnostic systems list three
main criteria for a diagnosis of intellectual dis-
The aim of this chapter is to attempt to address ability. These criteria are (a) significantly subav-
these questions by reviewing social skills assess- erage intellectual functioning, (b) substantial
ments and interventions for children with intel- deficits in adaptive behavior functioning, and (c)
lectual disability. Improving the social behavior symptom manifestation during the developmen-
and social skills of children with intellectual dis- tal period. The developmental period is generally
ability represents a major treatment priority taken to mean the period prior to reaching adult-
because impaired social functioning is one type hood (i.e., 0–18 years of age). The latter criterion
of adaptive behavior deficit that is commonly makes it clear that intellectual disability is a type
observed in such children (Wilkins & Matson, of developmental disability along with autism
2007). Indeed, the presence of impaired social spectrum disorder and cerebral palsy (Center for
skills and related adaptive behavior deficits con- Disease Control and Prevention, 2015).
stitute a defining characteristic of intellectual dis-
ability (Schalock et al., 2010). In this chapter we
review procedures for assessing social skills of  ignificant Limitations in Intellectual
S
children with intellectual disability. This is fol- Functioning
lowed by a review of evidence-based interven-
tions for addressing the social skill deficits and The nature of human intelligence has been hotly
excess behavior of children with intellectual dis- debated, and there is considerable argument over
ability. Before reviewing assessment and inter- its conceptualization, definition, and measure-
vention procedures, however, we provide ment (Fraser, 1995; Herrnstein & Murray, 1994;
background information on the definition, diag- O’Reilly & Carr, 2016). While the nature of
nosis, prevalence, and classification of intellec- intelligence is controversial, there are clearly
tual disability. individuals whose intellectual functioning
falls below the range considered typical or
average (Nouwens, Lucas, Embregts, & van
 efinition and Diagnosis
D Nieuwenhuizen, 2017). If the level of intellectual
of Intellectual Disability functioning is significantly subaverage, then the
individual would meet one of the criteria for a
Intellectual disability is characterized by signifi- diagnosis of intellectual disability. Given that IQ
cant limitations in cognitive ability and deficits in is a strong predictor of academic achievement
adaptive behavior functioning (Schalock et al., (Stetson & Stetson, 2001), it is important to iden-
2010). Intellectual and adaptive behavior impair- tify individuals with low IQ so that remedial and
ments are core features of the definition of intel- specialized educational services can be initiated.
lectual disability formulated by the American Ideally these services would be provided in the
Association on Intellectual and Developmental mainstream classroom so as to promote social
Disabilities (AAIDD; Schalock et al., 2010). inclusion (Wehmeyer et al., 2016).
AAIDD’s definition is largely consistent with The latest (i.e., 11th) edition of AAIDD’s
other diagnostic manuals, specifically: (a) The diagnostic manual described intellectual func-
International Classification of Diseases, 10th tioning as a general mental capacity that affects
edition (ICD-10; World Health Organization, learning, problem solving, and reasoning
2001) and (b) the fifth edition of the Diagnostic (Schalock et al., 2010). Intellectual functioning
and Statistical Manual of the Mental Disorders can be reasonably measured using standardized
(DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, and individually administered intelligence (i.e.,
2013). Carr and O’Reilly (2016) compared these IQ) tests (Tylenda, Beckett, & Barrett, 2007).
three diagnostic systems and noted several differ- Significantly subaverage intellectual functioning
ences in their respective terminology and empha- based on intelligence testing is typically defined
Intellectual Disability and Social Skills 251

as an IQ score that falls two standard deviations disability, that is, significant deficits in adaptive
or more below the mean (Carr & O’Reilly, 2016). behavior functioning.
Most intelligence tests are constructed with a
mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15 or 16.
This means that the cutoff score for a possible  eficits in Adaptive Behavior
D
diagnosis of intellectual disability is approximately Functioning
70–75 (Schalock et al., 2010). This five-­point
range allows for measurement error and acknowl- Although perhaps less controversial than debates
edges that the diagnosis of intellectual disability surrounding the nature of intelligence, the defini-
is not exclusively based on IQ scores but also tion, conceptualization, and measurement of
requires concurrent deficits in adaptive behavior adaptive behavior have proven to be somewhat
functioning (Carr & O’Reilly, 2016; Schalock contentious and rather complex (MacLean,
et al., 2010). Miller, & Bartsch, 2001; Staddon, 2016). Thus,
A number of IQ tests can be used to assess deciding if a child does or does not meet the sec-
intellectual functioning, and several recent ond major criterion for a diagnosis of intellectual
reviews on the use of IQ tests in the diagnosis disability (i.e., significant deficits in adaptive
of intellectual disability are available (Carr & behavior functioning) is by no means a straight-
O’Reilly, 2016; Tylenda et al., 2007). With forward process. Despite some contention and
respect to making a diagnosis of intellectual complexity, the concept of adaptive behavior
disability, the main purpose of IQ testing is to functioning is now a key component in how intel-
determine whether or not the child’s level or lectual disability is “…understood, diagnosed,
degree of intellectual functioning is signifi- classified, and approached from an intervention
cantly subaverage. As mentioned before, sig- perspective” (Buntinx, 2016, p. 107). Partly in
nificantly subaverage means an IQ score that is response to a growing recognition that low IQ did
below 70 or 75. To ensure the reliable and valid not necessarily indicate presence of a disability,
assessment, the IQ test must be appropriate for Heber (1959) argued for consideration of adap-
the child’s age, culture, and abilities. In some tive behavior functioning in the definition and
cases, specialized IQ tests may be required. For diagnosis of intellectual disability. Since then a
example, children with delayed speech and considerable amount of progress has been made
language development may be more appropri- with respect to refining the definition, conceptu-
ately assessed using an IQ test that places less alization, and measurement of adaptive behavior
reliance on verbal responding, such as the Test functioning (Dixon, 2007).
of Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI-­ 4; Brown, Reflecting this progress, contemporary defini-
Sherbenou, & Johnsen, 2010). tions and conceptualizations of adaptive behavior
It is sometimes the case that a child will score focus on the extent to which the individual has
low on an IQ test and struggle academically but acquired the skills that are needed to cope with
show few if any problems in everyday living. the demands of everyday living. The AAIDD
Reading might be a problem for the child but not (2013), for example, defined adaptive behavior
dressing, feeding, and toileting. The child might functioning as “…the collection of conceptual,
struggle with arithmetic but have no trouble social, and practical skills that are learned and
socializing with others, making friends, and play- performed by people in their everyday lives”
ing appropriately with peers. In the past, such (para. 3). Conceptual skills refer to abilities
children were often labeled mentally retarded related to self-direction, language, literacy, and
based on low IQ alone. These so-called 6-h numeracy. Social skills focus on interpersonal
retarded children (President’s Committee on interaction and social responsibility, such as
Mental Retardation, 1969) would not be labeled appropriate rule following, social problem solving,
today because they would not meet the second and maintaining a healthy degree of self-­esteem.
major criterion for a diagnosis of intellectual The extent to which the person is gullible and can
252 J. Sigafoos et al.

avoid victimization is also an important aspect the communication, daily living, social skills and
of adaptive behavior functioning.  Practical skills relationships, and physical activity domains.
cover self-care and daily living skills (washing, Assessment of adaptive behavior functioning,
feeding, dressing, toileting, cleaning, and meal in general, and social skills, in particular, are
preparation), as well as community access and covered in more detail later in this chapter. For
occupational skills, such as getting to and from now it is important to note that any assessment of
school, following schedules and routines, and adaptive behavior functioning needs to consider the
using everyday appliances and technology (e.g., individual’s age and sociocultural background.
the toaster and telephone). Additional examples The environmental context (e.g., home, school,
of conceptual, social, and practical skills are or community) and level of independence with
listed in the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric which the individual is expected to function in
Association, 2013, p. 37). those environments must also be considered
In addition to the conceptual, social, and prac- (Cory, Dattilo, & Williams, 2006; Embregts,
tical taxonomy described above, adaptive skills 2002). Wilkins and Matson (2007) noted that
have been arranged into a number of different while [social] skill deficits are prevalent among
conceptual schemes, areas, or domains. Sparrow, individuals with intellectual disability, their defi-
Cicchetti, and Sauinier (2016), for example, clas- cits can result from several sources, such as “…
sified adaptive behavior functioning into five lack of opportunity, knowledge, practice, feedback,
major domains: (a) communication, (b) daily liv- and/or reinforcement” (p. 321). Identifying the
ing, (c) social skills and relationships, (d) physi- source(s) of adaptive behavior deficits may assist
cal activity, and (e) problem behavior. Each of in designing effective interventions to promote
these domains is then further divided into two or the conceptual, social, and practical skills that the
three sub-domains. The social skills and relation- person will need to cope with the demands of
ships domain, for example, comprises three sub-­ everyday living.
domains (e.g., relating to others, playing and
using leisure time, and adapting). Each sub-­
domain, in turn, presents a series of specific age-­ Prevalence
graded skills. Adapting, for example, references
skills such as (a) seeks comfort from a loved one A meta-analytic review of 52 studies on the prev-
when hurt or upset, (b) remembers to say please alence of intellectual disability (Maulik,
when asking for something, and (c) is willing to Mascarenhas, Mathers, Dua, & Saxena, 2011)
compromise to get along with others of his/her found an overall prevalence of 1% (i.e., 10.37
age. Bruininks, Woodcock, Weatherman, and persons for every 1000 population). This 1%
Hill (1996) similarly grouped adaptive skills into prevalence figure is consistent with the results of
a number of more specific domains, specifically: another systematic review of 18 prevalence stud-
(a) gross motor, (b) fine motor, (c) social interac- ies (McKenzie, Milton, Smith, & Ouellette-­
tion, (d) language comprehension, (e) language Kuntz, 2016). However, it is important to note
expression, (f) eating and meal preparation, (g) that Maulik et al. (2011) also found that estimates
toileting, (h) dressing, (i) personal self-care, (j) of prevalence varied depending on the location
domestic skills, (k) time and punctuality, (l) and nature of the sample population (e.g., west-
money and value, (m) work skills, (n) home/com- ern versus developing countries, rural versus
munity orientation, and (o) problem behavior. urban, low versus high SES, and children versus
Regardless of how adaptive skills are categorized adults). Prevalence estimates also varied depend-
or classified, it is generally the case that the per- ing on the diagnostic system that was applied
son must show substantial deficits in more than (e.g., ICD-10 versus DSM-V) and type of study
one area of adaptive behavior functioning. For design (e.g., cohort versus cross-sectional).
example, the person would have to show deficits With an overall prevalence estimate of approx-
in their conceptual, social, and practical skills or, imately 1%, intellectual disability is nearly as
if using Sparrow et al.’s (2016) categories, across common as autism spectrum disorder (ASD;
Intellectual Disability and Social Skills 253

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, nation of the severity includes consideration of
2016) and much more common than cerebral the child’s adaptive behavior functioning across
palsy (Maenner et al., 2016). However, it is the conceptual, social, and practical domains
important to note that many individuals with (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In
ASD and cerebral palsy also meet the diagnostic either case, four categories of severity have been
criteria for intellectual disability. Indeed, based delineated: (a) mild, (b) moderate, (c) severe,
on a review on the relation between ASD and and (d) profound. These four levels of severity
intellectual disability, Matson and Shoemaker are used in both the ICD-10 (World Health
(2009) concluded that intellectual disability is Organization, 2001) and the DSM-5 (American
“perhaps the most common co-occurring disor- Psychiatric Association, 2013) but are not part
der with ASD…” (p. 1111). of AAIDD’s classification system (Schalock
This is significant because impairment of et al., 2010).
social interaction is a defining characteristic of
ASD (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Mild intellectual disability. Mild intellectual dis-
Individuals with ASD and intellectual disability ability is associated with IQ scores ranging from
are therefore highly likely to experience signifi- 50–55 to approximately 70. While considered
cant social interaction problems and numerous mild in terms of intellectual disability, this range
social skill deficits. Children with cerebral palsy of IQ scores is considered significantly subaver-
and intellectual disability are also likely to be age, and children in this range are likely to make
assessed as having impaired social functioning, relatively slower academic progress and reach
particularly if their motor impairments interfere lower levels of academic proficiency than peers.
with social interaction and the expression of However, the extent of academic achievement
social skills (Tan et al., 2016). and development of adaptive skills will also
depend on the quality of their educational experi-
ence. During early development and the pre-
Classification school years, delays in reaching developmental
milestones will occur, but these may be subtle
Intellectual disability has been classified in terms and less apparent than for children with moderate
of (a) severity, (b) etiology, and (c) the levels of and severe/profound intellectual disability
required support (Carr & O’Reilly, 2016; (MacLean et al., 2001). Indeed, many cases of
Schalock et al., 2010). These three approaches to mild intellectual disability go undetected until
classification are neither exhaustive nor mutually the child enters school and begins to fail academ-
exclusive. Each can be helpful for a range of pur- ically. Most children with mild intellectual dis-
poses, such as prevention, family planning, ability can be expected to develop the speech and
selecting appropriate assessments and interven- language skills that are foundational for many
tions, and service delivery. Assessing a child’s important social interactions, such as initiating
degree or severity of intellectual disability, for and maintaining topical conversations and asking
example, can assist in curriculum development, questions (Wilkins & Matson, 2007). Despite
whereas etiological knowledge is relevant to pre- often having this foundation, Giuliani and El
vention and family planning. Korh (2016) delineated a number of more
advanced social skills that appear to be problem-
atic for individuals with mild intellectual disabil-
Severity ities. Their list included (a) assertiveness (e.g.,
refusing non-preferred activities), (b) knowing
Intellectual disability has often been classified when and how to apologize, (c) sharing activities
on the basis of IQ score (American Psychiatric and materials, (d) recognizing and using humor
Association, 2000). More recently, the determi- and irony, and (e) responding to criticism.
254 J. Sigafoos et al.

Moderate intellectual disability. Moderate intel- Because most individuals with severe and pro-
lectual disability is associated with IQ scores that found intellectual disability fail to acquire any
range from 34–40 to 50–55. An individual with significant amount of speech and language
an IQ score of 50 or 55 could be classified as hav- (Sigafoos et al., 2016), alternative methods of
ing mild or moderate intellectual disability communication (e.g., picture-based communica-
depending on the extent of his or her adaptive tion, manual signing, and/or speech-generating
behavior deficits. Generally, children with mod- devices) are usually indicated (Beukelman &
erate intellectual disability will be identified prior Mirenda, 2013). The social skills of children with
to entering school due to more obvious delays in severe or profound intellectual disability can be
reaching developmental milestones. At school extremely rudimentary. They may indicate happi-
these children may require an adapted educa- ness and awareness of others through nonsym-
tional curriculum that includes instruction on a bolic means (e.g., facial expressions and body
range of functional, rather than purely academic, movements) but show deficits in making eye con-
skills (Wehmeyer et al., 2016). Although indi- tact and being able to show a range of emotions,
viduals with moderate intellectual disabilities can seek comfort, or show affection, at least in any
be expected to acquire good speech and language conventional way (Calculator, 2015).
skills (Sigafoos, O’Reilly, Lancioni, & Green,
2016), they are still likely to show a number of
social skill deficits. For example, they may have Etiology
difficulty making decisions, perceiving and inter-
preting others’ speech and gestures, listening to The many and varied causes of intellectual dis-
others without interrupting, acquiring social eti- ability include injury, illness, infections, genetic
quette (e.g., saying please and thank you), and mutations, and environmental deprivation.
detecting social cues (American Psychiatric Maulik and Harbour (2010) classified causes of
Association, 2013; Giuliani & El Korh, 2016). intellectual disability into prenatal and postnatal
factors. Prenatal factors included (a) genetic
Severe and profound intellectual disability. mutations/syndromes (e.g., Angelman syndrome,
Severe intellectual disability is associated with Fragile X syndrome), (b) congenital malforma-
IQ scores of 20–25 to 35–40. Profound intellec- tions (e.g., microcephaly), (c) maternal illness or
tual disability is associated with IQ scores below infections during pregnancy (e.g., hepatitis,
20 or 25 (American Psychiatric Association, rubella, diabetes, cytomegalovirus, and toxoplas-
2013). Cases of severe or profound intellectual mosis), and (d) prenatal exposure to toxins
disability can usually be identified within the first (e.g., alcohol and radiation). Postnatal factors
few months of life due to known etiology and sig- include (a) childhood illness and infections, (b)
nificant developmental delay. Individuals with malnutrition, (c) trauma/physical abuse, and (d)
severe and profound intellectual disability also environmental deprivation (e.g., unresponsive
frequently present with additional impairments parenting, lack of environmental stimulation, and
(e.g., physical, hearing, and vision impairment) limited educational opportunities). These differ-
and chronic medical problems (e.g., seizure dis- ent classes of causes are not necessarily mutually
order). These children often need an alternative exclusive. For example, genetic mutations can
curriculum (Wehmeyer et al., 2016) concentrat- cause metabolic disorders, such as phenylketon-
ing on developing age-appropriate leisure and uria or congenital hyperthyroidism. These meta-
self-care skills (e.g., feeding, dressing, toileting, bolic disorders can, in turn, cause developmental
playing with toys, self-help skills), increasing delays or intellectual disability under certain
community access, promoting social participa- environmental conditions, such as if the child’s
tion, and enabling functional communication diet is high in phenylalanine or if the child fails to
(e.g., requesting preferred objects and activities, receive proper thyroid treatment soon after birth
rejecting non-preferred objects, labeling objects). (Blau, 2016; Lain et al., 2016).
Intellectual Disability and Social Skills 255

It is important to note that in many cases, the tured observations of attention-seeking behavior
cause of a child’s intellectual disability may (e.g., looking at, approaching, reaching for, and/or
remain unknown. Indeed, Maulik and Harbour touching an adult) in 21 children with Smith-­
(2010) suggested that etiology may remain Magenis syndrome and 19 children with Down
unknown in up to 50% of cases. The percentage syndrome. Children were observed with a familiar
of cases with an unknown etiology does, how- and unfamiliar adult and under conditions of high
ever, seem to vary in relation to the degree or versus low levels of attention. Compared to the
severity of intellectual disability. Harris (2005) children with Down syndrome, children with
estimated that while etiology is known in most Smith-Magenis syndrome showed a preference
(75%) cases of moderate to profound intellectual for the familiar adult and showed more attention-­
disability, it is likely to be known in only about seeking behavior under the low-attention condi-
40% of cases of mild intellectual disability. Of tion. These results suggest syndrome-specific
course, these percentages are likely to change differences in social motivation and a possible
with the expected advances in genetic and bio- need for differing levels and types of support to
medical research, leaving fewer cases with develop appropriate attention-seeking in these
unknown etiology. two groups of children.
Etiology may have important implications for
understanding children’s social skill deficits and
excesses, which could in turn inform the selec- Levels of Required Support
tion of assessment and intervention procedures.
For example, certain genetic syndromes appear The AAIDD developed a classification system
to be associated with specific cognitive and based on the types and amounts of supports
behavioral characteristics or behavioral pheno- required by the individual (Schalock et al., 2010).
types (Dykens, 1995; Kuczynski & Udwin, The four resulting levels or categories of support
2016). Children with Angelman syndrome, for are (a) intermittent, (b) limited, (c) extensive, and
example, often show intense social excitability (d) pervasive. These categories represent a con-
that could be inappropriate and excessive in some tinuum ranging from brief periods of targeted
contexts (Kuczynski & Udwin, 2016). intervention (Intermittent) to constant, ongoing,
Assessments that can capture and rate social and life-sustaining assistance (pervasive support)
excitability under different conditions and inter- across all areas of functioning (Carr & O’Reilly,
ventions, aimed at teaching such children to 2016). An example of intermittent supports might
modulate the intensity of their social reaction be a child who requires some help reconnecting
across different environments, might therefore be with classmates after the summer holidays.
indicated. In contrast, children with Fragile X Limited supports encompass situations where a
syndrome tend toward social avoidance child needs some consistent support in order to
(Kuczynski & Udwin, 2016) and may therefore learn expected social skills but might only need
require assessment approaches that can reliably that support when entering new environments,
detect and/or rate social avoidance. Should the such as when transitioning from primary school
child’s degree or extent of social avoidance prove to middle school. Extensive supports occur regu-
problematic, then an intervention to increase spe- larly and over the long term, such as ongoing
cific social approach skills (e.g., orienting to support to ensure a child continues to make aca-
social partners and making eye contact) and demic progress and maintains positive peer rela-
decrease social avoidance might be indicated. tions over the school years. However, with
In addition to phenotypic social reactivity and effective extensive support, it is often the case
skill profiles, Wilde et al. (2016) provided data that the child will learn the required skills and
suggestive of syndrome-specific differences in become more independent over time. Pervasive
social motivation. Their data came from struc- supports are those that are required every day and
256 J. Sigafoos et al.

throughout the person’s life, such as for a person with autism and intellectual disability and 1861
who requires complete assistance with dressing, children with intellectual disability only. Their
feeding, and toileting and who is unlikely to results indicated that the SIS-C was reliable and
become independent at such tasks. valid for classification based on levels of required
It is important to note that a person’s assessed support. These positive psychometric properties
level of support does not necessarily or consis- suggest the SIS-C could be used for identifying
tently correspond to severity of intellectual dis- the nature and type of required supports.
ability. Instead, the level of required support
could be expected to reflect the perceived range
of needs at the time of the assessment, which may Assessment of Adaptive Behavior
change over time (Harris, 2005). For example, a Functioning and Social Skills
child with moderate intellectual disability might
require a 20-h structured intervention (extensive A considerable amount of research has focused
support) to overcome a specific social skill deficit on developing reliable and valid approaches
(e.g., lack of ability to effectively enter a peer for assessing adaptive behavior, including the
group). After learning this skill, however, the adaptive social skills, of children with intellec-
child might then only require intermittent sup- tual disabilities (Matson, 2007). Reliable and
ports in order to successfully maintain that newly valid measures of adaptive behavior are needed
acquired social skill. for a variety of purposes, including (a) diagno-
This system of classification represents a sis and classification, (b) studies of behavioral
social-ecological model in which intellectual dis- phenotypes, and (c) intervention planning and
ability is conceptualized as a state of functioning, evaluation.
rather than as an inherent trait (Parmenter, 2011; Ensuring a fit-for-purpose assessment of adap-
Shogren et al., 2016). However, from a practical tive behavior requires consideration of a number
perspective, an important issue is the extent to of factors, such as the child’s age, environmental
which a child’s level of required support for dif- demands, degree and types of disability, and, of
ferent environments and/or domains of function- course, the intended purpose of the assessment.
ing (e.g., home and school environmental and For example, an assessment of a child’s overall
self-care and social skills functioning) can be level of general adaptive behavior functioning
identified. Toward this end, Shogren et al. (2016) might be sufficient for diagnostic and classifica-
developed the Supports Intensity Scale-­tion purposes but would be less useful for com-
Children’s Version (SIS-C) to assess level of prehensively identifying specific social skill
required support in 5- to 16-year-old children deficits and excesses that could then form the
with developmental disabilities. The scale is basis for a social skill intervention curriculum.
administered by interviewing respondents who For example, the adaptive social skills of a
know the child well. During the interview, 7-year-old child with moderate to severe intellec-
respondents are asked to provide information tual disability and motor impairment may need to
regarding the child’s medical and behavioral sup- be assessed to identify whether the child can
port needs and indicate the type, frequency, and effectively recruit the attention of caregivers and,
amount of time needed to support the child in if so, how the child does that. In contrast, a useful
participating in a range of home, community, assessment for a 9-year-old child with mild intel-
school, health, and social activities. The social lectual disabilities who is being socially rejected
domain, for example, consists of nine items rated by peers might be one that explicitly focuses on
on a five-point scale (e.g., maintaining a conver- identifying the social skills that would promote
sation, coping with changes in routines, and tran- positive peer interactions.
sitioning across social situations). Shogren et al. Research into the assessment of individuals
(2016) evaluated the psychometric properties of with intellectual disability (Matson, 2007) has
the SIS-C from data obtained on 2124 children led to the development of several rating scales
Intellectual Disability and Social Skills 257

that can provide a reliable and valid assessment used assessment of adaptive behavior functioning
of the social skill deficits and excesses of children (Dixon, 2007). Now in its third edition
with intellectual disabilities. Existing rating (Vineland-3), the Vineland-3 can be used to
scales can be divided into those that provide a assess adaptive behavior functioning of individu-
general assessment of adaptive behavior func- als from birth to 90 years of age with a range of
tioning across a number of domains (e.g., com- disabilities, including intellectual disability,
munication, self-care, motor, social, and autism spectrum disorder, and attention deficit
community living) and those that focus more hyperactivity disorder (Sparrow et al., 2016). As
explicitly on the social skills domain (e.g., start- noted in the previous section on Deficits in
ing conversations, making friends, receiving crit- Adaptive Behavior Functioning, the Vineland-3
icism calmly, express feelings in appropriate covers five major domains: (a) communication,
ways). (b) daily living, (c) social skills and relationships,
Despite the differing emphases, both types of (d) physical activity, and (e) problem behavior,
rating scales generally share a common approach each of which has two or three sub-domains.
to the collection of assessment data. That is, the Each sub-domain consists of from 33 to 46 age-­
assessment process involves interviewing third-­ graded items, and each item is scored on a three-­
party informants (parents, teachers, and/or care- point scale (0 = never, 1 = sometimes, and
givers) who know the child well and who have 2 = usually or often). Examples of increasingly
observed the child across a number of environ- more advanced items in the relating to others
ments and over a significant period of time (e.g., sub-domain include (a) recognizing family mem-
at least 3 months). Informants meeting these cri- bers or other people he/she knows well, (b) using
teria can generally provide a reliable indication words to express his/her emotions, and (c) start-
of the extent to which a child displays specific ing conversations with others by talking about
behaviors. Interviewing third-party informants things they are interested in. Standardized scores
using standardized rating scales is generally the are calculated to compare a child’s performance
most time- and resource-efficient approach to to established norms. The discrepancy between a
social skills assessment (Wilkins & Matson, child’s scores and the norms, if any, provides an
2007). Comprehensive adaptive behavior scales indication of the child’s degree or severity of
that could be used for diagnostic, classification, adaptive functioning impairment, which is
and intervention planning purposes include the important for diagnosis and classification. The
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Sparrow assessment can also be used to identify interven-
et al., 2016), Checklist of Adaptive Living Skills tion targets. That is, intervention could focus on
(Bruininks & Moreau, 2004), Scales of teaching and/or increasing specific social skills
Independent Behavior-Revised (Bruininks et al., that were rated as occurring only sometimes or
1996), and the Diagnostic Adaptive Behavior never. Interventions might also be needed to
Scale (Tassé, Schalock, Balboni, Spreat, & address problem behaviors (e.g., avoids interact-
Navas, 2016). Commonly used rating scales for ing with others, stubborn or argumentative) if
identifying specific social skill deficits and these occur too often.
excesses include the Matson Evaluation of Social
Skills for Youngsters (Matson, Rotatori, & Helsel, Checklist of Adaptive Living Skills. The Checklist
1983), the Matson Evaluation of Social Skills for of Adaptive Living Skills (CALS) is a criterion-
Individuals with Severe Retardation (Matson, referenced measure of adaptive living skills with
1995), and the Social Skills Improvement System direct implications for intervention planning
(Gresham & Elliott, 2008) . (Bruininks & Moreau, 2004). The CALS may be
used for determining the skills that an individual
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales—Third has mastered and those that he or she might need
Edition (Sparrow et al., 2016). The Vineland to acquire in order to effectively function within
Adaptive Behavior Scales is the most commonly specific environments. It is aimed for use with
258 J. Sigafoos et al.

individuals with and without i­ ntellectual disability. determining a diagnosis of intellectual disability”
It is suitable from infancy to adulthood. The CALS (Tassé et al., 2016, p. 80). The scale follows the
canvasses approximately 800 specific adaptive conceptual model of adaptive behavior function-
behaviors covering four domains (i.e., personal ing delineated by the AAIDD. That is, function-
living skills, home living skills, community living ing is conceptualized in terms of conceptual,
skills, and employment skills). Information from social, and practical skills (Schalock et al., 2010).
the CALS is intended to assist parents, teachers, A useful feature of the scale with respect to social
and clinicians in identifying the person’s instruc- skills functioning is the inclusion of what might
tional needs, formulating individual training be seen as higher-level social skills, such as items
objectives, and monitoring progress toward those that examine a person’s degree of social responsi-
objectives. To this end, each behavioral item in bility, self-esteem, gullibility, and social problem-­
the CALS has a corresponding instructional unit solving abilities. These skills can be particularly
(instructional activity) in a parallel intervention problematic for individuals with intellectual dis-
curriculum known as the Adaptive Living Skills ability (Parmenter, 2011). Although research data
Curriculum (ALSC; Bruininks, Moreau, Gilman, is limited due to its newness, Tassé et al. (2016)
& Anderson, 2004). reported strong reliability (i.e., test-retest and
inter-rater) and convergent validity. DABS items
Scales of Independent Behavior-Revised. The also appear to be age-sensitive and seem to “max-
Scales of Independent Behavior-Revised (SIB-R) imally” differentiate between individuals with
is described as a comprehensive assessment of and without intellectual disability (Tassé et al.,
adaptive and maladaptive behavior (Bruininks 2016, p. 86), which is arguably the most impor-
et al., 1996). It can be used from infancy to old tant consideration in a diagnostic assessment.
age (80+ years). These scales assess a large num-
ber of specific responses across 12 domains (i.e., Matson Evaluation of Social Skills for Youngsters.
gross motor, fine motor, language, eating and The Matson Evaluation of Social Skills for
meal preparation, toileting, dressing, personal Youngsters (MESSY) is intended to assess the
self-care, domestic, time and punctuality, money degree of appropriate social behavior in children
management, work, and home and community). from 7 to 15 years of age (Matson et al., 1983).
There is also a section assessing the frequency The scale consists of 64 items assessing a range
and severity of eight types of problem behavior of appropriate and problematic social behavior.
(e.g., hurtful to self, hurtful to others, and socially Examples of the appropriate social behaviors in
offensive behavior). As with the Vineland-3, the the scale include (a) makes others laugh, (b)
SIB-R is comprehensive and norm-referenced helps a friend who is hurt, and (c) walks up and
and could be used for diagnostic and classifica- initiates conversations. Examples of problematic
tion purposes. The SIB-R includes items refer- social behaviors include: (a) explains things more
encing basic and advanced social skills (e.g., than necessary and (b) wants to get even with
turns head toward speaker when name is called, someone who hurt him/her. The MESSY includes
takes appropriate-­ sized portions from serving a teacher-report form and a self-report form, but
dishes, and touching others too much). It would the latter form may not be appropriate for use by
therefore seem to be a useful tool for identifying individuals with intellectual disabilities unless
specific objectives for beginning a social skills they have good comprehension skills and a suffi-
training program with individuals with mild to cient degree of self-awareness. Scores on the
severe/profound intellectual disability. MESSY were reported to be positively correlated
with the results of teachers’ ratings, with the chil-
Diagnostic Adaptive Behavior Scale. The dren’s popularity within the classroom, and with
Diagnostic Adaptive Behavior Scale (DABS) is a the children’s ability to solve social dilemmas
relatively new measure for assessing adaptive (Matson et al., 1983). MESSY scores are nega-
behavior functioning in individuals from 4 to 21 tively correlated with symptoms of
years of age. It is primarily intended for “… ­psychopathology such as anxiety and depression.
Intellectual Disability and Social Skills 259

A factor analytic study (Méndez, Hidalgo, & severe and profound intellectual disability who
Inglés, 2006) found that items in the MESSY often have extremely limited social skills and
formed four main domains: (a) aggressiveness/ who would therefore seem to require assess-
antisocial behavior, (b) social skills/assertive- ments that emphasize early and atypical social
ness, (c) conceit/haughtiness, and (d) loneliness/ development.
social anxiety. The MESSY was recently re-
normed (MESSY-II; Matson et al., 2010), and The Social Skills Improvement System. The
research on the structure of this new scale with Social Skills Improvement System (SSIS;
886 typically developing children from 2 to 16 Gresham & Elliott, 2008) is a revised version of
years of age found that scale items formed three the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS-RS;
factors: (a) hostile, (b) adaptive/appropriate, and Gresham & Elliott, 1990). It measures how often
(c) inappropriately assertive/overconfident various types of social skills are exhibited by a
(Matson, Neal, Worley, Kozlowski, & Fodstad, child. A rating of the importance of each skill is
2012). Matson et al. (2012) suggested that the also solicited from informants. Frequency is
MESSY-II could be used to identify a child’s rated on a four-point scale (0 = never, 1 = sel-
strengths and weaknesses with respect to social dom, 2 = often, and 3 = almost always).
competence. They also suggested the scale could Importance is rated on a three-point scale (0 = not
be administered repeatedly to monitor develop- important, 1 = important, and 2 = critical). There
ment and intervention effects. are teacher, parent, and student versions covering
ages 3–18 years. The scale consists of seven sub-
Matson Evaluation of Social Skills for scales: (a) communication (b) cooperation, (c)
Individuals with Severe Retardation. The engagement, (d) assertion, (e) responsibility, (f)
Matson Evaluation of Social Skills for empathy, and (g) self-control. There are also five
Individuals with Severe Retardation (MESSIER) problem behavior sub-domains: (a) externaliz-
is intended for assessing individuals with severe ing, (b) internalizing, (c) bullying, (d) hyperac-
and profound intellectual disability from child- tivity/inattention, and (e) autism spectrum.
hood to adulthood (Matson, 1995; Wilkins & Problem behavior items are also rated on a four-­
Matson, 2007). It has also been used to assess point frequency scale. The SSIS has been shown
social skills in children with autism spectrum to reliably differentiate between typically devel-
disorder (Matson, Hattier, & Turygin, 2012; oping children and those with disabilities, includ-
Tureck & Matson, 2012). The scale contains 85 ing children with intellectual disability, emotional
items derived mainly from the communication disturbance, and communication impairment.
and socialization domains of an earlier edition of Many of the specific items in the scale relate to
the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Sparrow, teacher-student relationships in classroom set-
Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984) and from the MESSY tings, such as (a) following instructions, (b) par-
(Matson et al., 1983). Additional items were ticipating in organized group activities, and (c)
nominated by experts (Wilkins & Matson, 2007). joining group activities without being told. The
The areas of functioning assessed by the scale would therefore seem particularly useful
MESSIER are (a) positive nonverbal (e.g., dis- for identifying school-based intervention targets.
criminates between persons and addresses per- A considerable amount of research has provided
sons), (b) positive verbal (e.g., thanks others), evidence to support the use of this rating system
(c) general positive (e.g., responds properly for this purpose (Elliott, 2007).
when meets others), (d) negative nonverbal (e.g.,
withdraws and isolates self), (e) negative verbal
(e.g., makes awkward comments), and (f) gen- Social Skills Intervention
eral negative (e.g., has difficulties waiting to sat-
isfy own needs). The MESSIER can be useful Results from the types of adaptive behavior and
for identifying areas of relative strength and defi- social skills assessments described in the previ-
cit in the social behavior of individuals with ous section will often reveal a number of specific
260 J. Sigafoos et al.

skill deficits and excesses that could be targeted would also have been improved by teaching
for intervention. Matson, Matson, and Rivet those social skills.
(2007) argued that target behaviors should be With respect to selecting and implementing
identified following a systematic assessment and interventions, practitioners should draw upon the
that intervention targets should be prioritized in best available research evidence regarding what
terms of (a) the potential impact on the child’s works for improving the social behavior and skills
overall adjustment, (b) ease of acquisition, (c) the of children with intellectual disabilities. That is,
extent to which skills form logical clusters, and social skills intervention for children with intellec-
(d) the extent to which the skill might replace tual disabilities should follow an evidence-based
problem behavior. To determine intervention pri- practice (EBP) approach. The EBP approach is a
orities, systematic assessment data should be broad decision-making process that aims to make
reviewed in light of such factors and in consulta- use of the best available research evidence to guide
tion with stakeholders (e.g., parents, teachers, intervention (Sackett, Rosenberg, Gray, Haynes,
peers). Once priorities have been established, & Richardson, 1996). EBPs are specific tech-
evidence-based interventions—suited to the niques or procedures that have been shown,
unique characteristics of the individual child— through a sufficient amount of high-quality experi-
are identified and implemented. mental research, to be consistently effective (Cook,
With respect to setting intervention priorities, Tankersley, & Landrum, 2013).
professionals and stakeholders should consider When implementing EBPs to improve the
the extent to which achieving the target skill/ social behavior and skills of children with
objective would improve the child’s overall qual- intellectual disability, it is important to ensure
ity of life. Improved quality of life is an impor- that intervention procedures are implemented
tant outcome to consider when evaluating the with fidelity (Johnson & McMaster, 2013). In
objectives, rationale, and effects of an interven- addition, as Johnson and McMaster (2013) noted,
tion (Keith, 2016). Cummins (2005) argued that EBPs will often need to be adapted to suit the
the quality of life concept has an objective com- child’s unique characteristics, circumstances, and
ponent and a subjective component. The objec- contexts. With respect to making such adapta-
tive component includes potentially quantifiable tions, practitioners could consider (a) the severity
aspects of a child’s life, such as his or her (a) and etiology of intellectual disability, (b) the
degree of independence and self-determination, nature of the child’s specific social skill deficits
(b) academic achievement, (c) participation in and/or excesses, (c) the child’s culture and unique
the home, school, and community, and (d) num- circumstances, (d) the environments in which he
ber and quality of friendships. The subjective or she is and will be expected to function, and (e)
component refers to the child’s expressed degree the clinical expertise of the people who will be
of satisfaction with his or her (a) independence implementing intervention. Even well-­established
and self-determination, (b) academic achieve- interventions are unlikely to work if they are not
ment, (c) participation in the home, school, and suited to the child’s unique circumstances or can-
community, and (d) friendships, for example. not be implemented with fidelity. Given the need
Certain types of social behavior and skills could to consider such factors, the specifics of social
conceivably increase one’s objective and subjec- skills intervention will vary considerably across
tive quality of life. For example, teaching a child children, even among children with the same
how to cooperate with peers during group-based degree or severity of intellectual disability, the
learning activities in the classroom might same etiology and/or support needs, and the same
improve objective quality of life by advancing or similar social skill profiles as indicated by the
the child academically and promoting positive results of a social skills assessment.
peer relations. If the child found such changes Fortunately, researchers have developed a
valuable, then his or her subjective quality of life number of intervention approaches, techniques,
Intellectual Disability and Social Skills 261

and procedures that appear to be effective for and look expectantly at the child as he or she arrives
improving the social behavior and skills of chil- in the classroom each morning. In contrast, oppor-
dren with intellectual disability. These procedures tunities to teach a child to initiate conversations
also appear to have some generality in the sense with peers might be captured as they arise naturally
that they have been successfully used to improve during lunch time, recess, and during transitions
a range of social skill deficits and excesses with within the school day.
children of varying ages and with varying degrees Whether created or captured, learning oppor-
and etiologies of intellectual and other develop- tunities begin with ensuring that natural cue/dis-
mental disabilities (Sturmey, 2014; Vaughn et al., criminative stimulus for responding is present.
2003; Watkins et al., 2016). The range of estab- For example, the natural cue for greeting another
lished EBPs for improving the social behavior is seeing that person for the first time each day.
and skills of children with intellectual disabilities While the presence of the natural cue should set
includes (a) systematic instruction, (b) Social the occasion for the desired social response, this
Stories™ (Gray & Garand, 1993), (c) video mod- is not likely to be the case during the early stages
eling, (d) social problem-­solving interventions, of intervention. Therefore, the teacher will often
(e) computer-based instruction, and (f) assistive need to prompt the occurrence of the response
technology interventions. using various prompting strategies, such as tell-
ing the child what to do, using a gestural prompt,
Systematic Instruction. Systematic instruction is and/or modeling the desired response. Over time
an umbrella term that covers a range of teaching prompts are faded by waiting longer and longer
procedures (Collins, 2012). Systematic instruc- before prompting and using less intense and
tional practices for children with intellectual dis- intrusive prompts. Correct responding needs to
ability are generally aimed at promoting be followed, at least occasionally by reinforcing
acquisition and fluent use of clearly defined tar- consequences to promote acquisition and main-
get behaviors via the provision of frequent and tain performance.
repeated learning opportunities. Examples of dis- Systematic instructional approaches have
crete social skills include (a) taking turns with the been successfully used to teach a range of func-
classroom computer, playground equipment, and tional skills, including a range of social skills, to
toys, (b) initiating a conversation with peers, and children with intellectual disability (Storey &
(c) requesting help with difficult tasks. Systematic Miner, 2011). Cipani (2009), for example,
instruction can also be used to teach response described a systematic instructional approach for
chains, such as the sequence of behaviors used to teaching children with severe intellectual dis-
initiate and then maintain a social interaction. For abilities to recruit attention. The approach begins
example: (a) gain the person’s attention, (b) with children who have already learned to make
establish joint attention, (c) make a relevant com- socially appropriate requests for preferred or
ment, and (d) respond to the person’s reply. needed objects. Opportunities to request are then
Storey and Miner (2011) provided a detailed created by withholding wanted or needed objects.
description of the steps involved in using system- Opportunities to teach attention recruitment are
atic instruction to teach functional skills to indi- then built into these requesting opportunities by
viduals with disabilities. Briefly, the teaching ensuring that the communication partner is not
process typically involves creating or capturing attending to the child at the time when a request
learning opportunities and then ensuring the needs to be made. The child is then prompted to
desired behavior occurs and is reinforced at each recruit attention before making the request by
opportunity. Learning opportunities can be created tapping the partner on the shoulder. Depending
by presenting structured discrete trials or by wait- on the culture and context, shoulder tapping
ing for opportunities to arise naturally. Opportunities could be considered a socially and culturally
to teach a greeting response, for example, might be appropriate manner of gaining the attention of a
created by having several different adults approach communicative partner. This response might also
262 J. Sigafoos et al.

be an efficient method for a child who does not Garand, 1993; Karkhaneh et al., 2010). This
have sufficient language skills to recruit attention approach involves developing a script describing
via speech or vocalizations. Once the response specific social situations, concepts, and/or behav-
occurs, which might require prompting, it is rein- iors. The script is intended to facilitate the child’s
forced by the partner facing the child and attend- ability to adapt to and cope with potentially dif-
ing to his or her subsequent request. Over time, ficult social situations (Karkhaneh et al., 2010).
the need for prompting is faded by waiting longer Scripts usually contain four content areas (a)
and longer before prompting and by gradually delineating and describing the discriminative
reducing the amount of prompting. In addition, stimuli that the child should attend to, (b) instruc-
the communication partner moves further and tions regarding how to respond to these stimuli,
further away from the child to ensure the newly (c) statements related to the behavior, thoughts,
acquired attention-getting response remains and feelings evoked or elicited by the social situ-
functional in situations when a communication ation, and (d) additional descriptions about the
partner is not in the immediate vicinity of the setting and context that might help the child bet-
child. Cipani (1990) demonstrated the effective- ter understand the situation (Barry & Burlew,
ness of this procedure for teaching a 7-year-old 2004, p. 45). A script for entering a peer group on
boy and a 10-year-old girl with severe intellectual the playground, for example, might consist of the
disability. With intervention, both children following:
acquired the social skill of approaching a non-­ When I am on the playground during recess, if I
attending adult, recruiting the adult’s attention see my classmates playing on the swing or the
using the targeted attention-recruiting response, slide, I will approach the group until I am near
and then asking for a needed, but missing item. enough to touch them. I will wait for one of them
to look at me and then I will smile and say hi. They
The attention-getting skill was maintained after are likely to say hi to me and then I will say Can I
the initial training phase, although the follow-up play with you? They will say sure and let me take
period was relatively short (i.e., 3 weeks). a turn. I will take my turn and then let someone
The general success of systematic instruc- else have a turn. When I take my turn I will be
happy. I am a good friend when I let others take
tional procedures for teaching children with their turn.
intellectual disabilities is perhaps not surpris-
ing given that the approach makes use of a A number of studies have evaluated the effects
number of empirically validated principles of of the use of Social Stories™ as an intervention
learning derived from applied behavior analytic for children with autism and other developmental
research (Collins, 2012; Storey & Miner, 2011). disabilities (Karkhaneh et al., 2010; Reynhout &
A review of social skills interventions Carter, 2006). The overall results have generally
(Gresham, 2016) concluded that interventions been positive (Karkhaneh et al., 2010; Reynhout
based on these principles appear to be the most & Carter, 2006). The approach has been success-
effective strategies for teaching social skills. fully used to teach a range of social skills, includ-
Many other approaches for teaching social ing (a) initiating verbal greetings (Reichow &
skills to children with intellectual disability Sabornie, 2009), (b) responding to the initiations
(e.g., video modeling, Social Stories™, assis- of others (Scattone, 2008), and (c) continuing a
tive technology) incorporate systematic instruc- social interaction (Delano & Snell, 2006; Sansosti
tional procedures into the overall intervention & Powell-Smith, 2008).
package. The inclusion of systematic instruc- However, Karkhaneh et al. (2010) noted that
tional procedures likely contributes to the the positive outcomes reported in many studies
effectiveness of these other approaches. into Social Stories™ interventions have often
Social Stories™. Social Stories™ is a widely been confounded by the use of other procedures,
used intervention approach for teaching social such as response prompting and feedback. Still,
skills to children with autism spectrum disorder Social Stories™ could be viewed as a promising
and other developmental disabilities (Gray & component or adjunct to the use of systematic
Intellectual Disability and Social Skills 263

instruction for increasing the social skills and (a) initiating a social interaction (Nikopoulos &
social interactions of children with developmen- Keenan, 2003), (b) using socially appropriate
tal disability. Nonetheless, this approach might be communication during peer play (Maione &
best suited to individuals with sufficient language Mirenda, 2006), (c) increasing social engage-
comprehension to understand the scripts. For ment (Bellini, Akullian, & Hopf, 2007), (d)
children with more limited comprehension skills, greeting peers (Avcioglu, 2013), and (e) teaching
Social Stories™ have been successfully presented appropriate intonation and facial expressions
using video modeling (Gül, 2016; Kagohara (Charlop, Dennis, Carpenter, & Greenberg,
et al., 2013; Sansosti & Powell-Smith, 2008; 2010). A potential prerequisite to effective use of
Scattone, 2008). video modeling appears to be the ability to attend
to the video model. This might be difficult for
Video modeling. Video modeling involves video- children with more significant attention deficits
taping a person engaged in some behavior and and/or vision impairment.
then using this video as an instructional tool
(Delano, 2007). A child might, for example, view Social problem-solving interventions. O’Reilly
a videotape of a peer greeting the teacher or play- and colleagues (O’Reilly & Chadsey-­ Rusch,
ing alongside others. After watching the video, 1992; O’Reilly et al., 2004) evaluated the effects
the child would then have an opportunity to per- of a problem-solving approach for teaching
form the modeled behavior in the natural envi- social skills to individuals with intellectual dis-
ronment. Mason, Davis, Boles, and Goodwyn ability. O’Reilly and Chadsey-Rusch (1992,
(2013) outlined several different configurations p. 324) delineated four steps in the social
that have been used in video modeling studies. problem-­solving process. These four steps are (a)
Specifically, the model could be the target child “discriminating salient social stimuli (decoding),
(video self-modeling), or another person, such as (b) identifying alternative social behaviors and
a peer or even an unfamiliar actor. In addition, the identifying the most appropriate social behavior
video could be filmed from the perspective of the for the social situation (deciding), (c) performing
actor (point-of-view video modeling) or from the that social behavior (performing), and (d) evalu-
perspective of a spectator (third-person perspec- ating the effectiveness of the social behavior once
tive). Furthermore, the child might view the it has been performed (evaluating).”
entire video before attempting to imitate the This four-step process has been successfully
modeled behavior (video priming) or might view taught to individuals with intellectual disabilities
the video in a step-by-step sequence (video using systematic instructional procedures
prompting). Each of these configurations can be (O’Reilly & Chadsey-Rusch, 1992; O’Reilly
effective depending on the child and the particu- et al., 2004). O’Reilly and Chadsey-Rusch
lar skill being modeled (McCoy & Hermansen, (1992), for example, taught three adults (23–44
2007). Achieving acquisition usually requires years of age) with moderate intellectual disability
repeated viewings of the video and repeated to use the problem-solving approach. Participants
opportunities to practice the modeled behavior. were first trained on the performance step (i.e.,
As with Social Stories™, additional instructional how to initiate a conversation by asking a ques-
components (e.g., prompting, corrective feed- tion) and then received training on the decoding,
back, and reinforcement) are often used in con- decision, and evaluation steps. The training pro-
junction with video modeling. cedures made use of modeling appropriate
Overall, video modeling can be an effective responses and using illustrative photographs to
approach for teaching social skills to individuals highlight salient social cues. For example, to
with autism and intellectual disability (Mason teach the performance step, the trainer modeled
et al., 2013; McCoy & Hermansen, 2007). four questions that the person could use to initiate
Indeed, video modeling has been effectively a conversation (e.g., What do you think about the
applied to teach a range of social skills, such as weather?). The salient social cues for this social
264 J. Sigafoos et al.

skill were illustrated in a series of photographs as well as adults (Webster-­ Stratton, Reid, &
(e.g., a photograph of the person sitting next to a Hammond, 2001) but might be best suited to indi-
co-worker). The trainer also used verbal instruc- viduals with the relatively good language compre-
tion related to the photograph (e.g., When you are hension abilities associated with mild to moderate
sitting next to a co-worker at lunch break, you intellectual disability as compared to severe/pro-
could start a conversation by asking a question, found intellectual disability.
such as what do you think about the weather?).
The results showed that these procedures were Computer-based instruction. Computer-­based
effective in training the individuals to follow the instruction makes use of personal computers and
four-step process. Importantly, once these skills related devices (e.g., iPads®, iPods®) for instruc-
had been acquired, the participants also showed tional/educational purposes. In practice, a computer
more social interaction with their co-workers, and associated instructional software is typically
that is the effects of training generalized to the used to present instructional stimuli to the child
natural environment. and provide opportunities for him or her to make
In a follow-up study, O’Reilly et al. (2004) responses. Software programs used in conjunction
compared the problem-solving approach to an with computer-based instruction also typically
alternative (external-control) approach for teach- monitor the child’s responses and provide rein-
ing two social skills (i.e., responding to corrective forcing consequences or corrective feedback as
feedback and managing conflict). The study necessary. Instructional stimuli can be presented in
involved five adults with mild intellectual disabil- a variety of modes, such as via visual, auditory,
ity. The problem-solving intervention aimed to and/or kinesthetic channels. Responses required of
teach participants to follow the four-step process the child could include touching the computer
of decoding, deciding, performance, and evalua- screen, using speech to text software, and/or com-
tion. Training procedures included (a) providing posing responses by typing out words or sentences
the participants with a verbal explanation of the with the computer keyboard.
targeted social skills and the social situations in Computer-based instruction has been success-
which these skills would be useful, (b) modeling fully used to teach a range of social skills to indi-
use of the skills, and (c) role-playing practice with viduals with developmental disabilities. Ramdoss
feedback. As part of this training, the participants et al. (2012), for example, identified 11 studies
were also taught to verbalize the social rules that with 330 participants that used computer-based
they should follow with respect to responding to instruction to teach social and emotional skills to
corrective feedback and managing conflict. The individuals with developmental disabilities. The
external-control intervention used similar proce- range of social skills addressed included: (a) ini-
dures except the participants were not taught to tiating conversations, (b) engaging in peer inter-
verbalize the social rules. The results demon- actions, and (c) generating a solution to a social
strated that both approaches were effective in problem. The range of emotional skills included
teaching the targeted social skills, although two recognizing the emotions conveyed by a person’s
participants made relatively less overall progress. facial expressions or voice intonation.
Overall the results of these two studies suggest Silver and Oakes (2001), for example, reported
that social problem-solving interventions are a on the results of a randomized controlled trial
promising approach for improving social skills of involving two groups of 11 (12- to 18-year-old)
individuals with mild to moderate intellectual dis- children with autism spectrum disorder. One
ability. A potential strength of this approach is the group received 5 h of computer-based instruction
focus on teaching generic social rules, which are aimed at teaching them to predict the responses
applicable to a range of environments and social of others and identify their emotions. For this,
situations and which might, therefore, facilitate they used a software program called Emotion
generalization of social behavior (McFall, 1982). Trainer, which presented pictures of faces,
The approach would seem applicable to children scenes, or objects and then presented the partici-
Intellectual Disability and Social Skills 265

pant with relevant questions (e.g., If Carol wanted ple, reviewed studies that used microswitch tech-
a pizza but got a hamburger, how do you think nology to increase adaptive responses and reduce
she would feel?). The target response was to socially inappropriate behavior in individuals
identify the feelings of the characters in the with severe/profound intellectual disability and
scene. Compared to the children in the control physical and sensory impairments. Their review
group, those in the computer-based instruction suggested that such interventions can be highly
group showed significant improvement in their effective in promoting a range of adaptive social
ability to correctly identify emotions. responses.
Another study (Bernard-Opitz, Sriram, & In one relevant study of this type, Lancioni
Nakhoda-Sapuan, 2001) provided computer-­ et al. (2016) configured a microswitch program
based instruction to eight children with autism. to enable nine individuals with multiple disabili-
The software adopted for this intervention pre- ties to access socially appropriate forms of stimu-
sented animated conflict scenarios (e.g., a child lation (e.g., songs, video, and picture slides).
wanting to use playground equipment) and Microswitches were also configured to enable the
offered several response options (e.g., Should the participants to initiate social conversations via
child make a polite request or have a tantrum?). text messaging and telephone calls. In addition to
This instructional program produced modest configuring the microswitch technology, the
improvements in the children’s propensity to intervention included guided practice to familiar-
select the more appropriate response option, but ize each participant with the operation of the
generalization to real conflict scenarios in the microswitches and the associated functions. With
natural environment was not assessed. It is there- this technology and the intervention program in
fore unclear if children would use the appropriate place, the participants were able to successfully
responses learned from the computer-based inter- access socially appropriate forms of stimulation
vention when they encountered similar scenarios and initiate social contact. In addition, the direct-­
in the natural environment. care staff perceived the microswitch program as
Although generalization to the natural envi- beneficial and enjoyable for the participants.
ronment could be an issue, there are data to sug- In addition to microswitches, speech-­
gest that computer-based instruction is a generating devices represent another type of
promising approach for teaching social skills and technology that is often recommended for indi-
improving emotional recognition (Ramdoss viduals with intellectual disability (Beukelman &
et al., 2012). However, most of the participants in Mirenda, 2013). Speech-generating devices typi-
these types of studies appeared to have mild/ cally include graphic symbols that the person
moderate disability. It is therefore unclear if simi- selects from a display and corresponding syn-
lar types of interventions would be effective for thetic or digitized speech output. Such devices
children with more severe intellectual disability are generally indicated as an augmentative or
and adaptive behavior deficits. alternative mode of communication for individu-
als who present with unintelligible speech or
Assistive Technology. A number of studies have insufficient speech and language development
used various types of assistive technology to pro- (Beukelman & Mirenda, 2013). The latter situa-
mote social skill development and social interac- tion is prevalent among individuals with severe
tion among individuals with disabilities (Lancioni and profound intellectual disability (Pinborough-­
& Singh, 2014). A comprehensive survey of this Zimmerman et al., 2007).
literature suggests that such interventions can be In a review of the literature on speech-­
effectively used with individuals with intellectual generating devices, Rispoli et al. (2010) found
disability, including individuals with severe/pro- that individuals with intellectual disabilities have
found intellectual disability (Lancioni, Sigafoos, been successfully taught to use this technology to
O’Reilly, & Singh, 2013). Lancioni, Singh, engage in a range of social-communicative inter-
O’Reilly, Sigafoos, and Oliva (2014), for exam- actions, such as recruiting attention, initiating
266 J. Sigafoos et al.

conversations, and requesting and protesting in other participant, however, mainly used the
socially acceptable ways. However, the mere pro- speech-generating device only when his initial
vision of a speech-generating device is usually attempts to communicate via reaching and/or
not sufficient to ensure functional use by indi- leading were ignored. This latter pattern sug-
viduals with intellectual disabilities. Rather, gests the participant was sensitive to the joint
effective intervention will often also require attention status of the communication partner.
implementation of systematic instructional pro- Overall, there is a considerable amount of data
cedures to teach the person how to use the tech- supporting the use of assistive technology inter-
nology for social-communication purposes. ventions to promote socially appropriate leisure,
Sigafoos et al. (2004), for example, used a social interaction, and communication among
systematic instructional approach to teach a individuals with intellectual disability. Based on
16-year-old male and 20-year-old woman to this literature, there appear to be three key com-
communicate requests for preferred objects in ponents to the successful use of such technology.
more socially acceptable ways. Both partici- These are (a) configuring technology that will be
pants had intellectual disability and lacked easy for the person to activate using existing
speech. They communicated mainly by reaching motor responses, (b) ensuring that microswitch
for or leading the communication partner’s hand activations lead to immediate reinforcing conse-
to objects they wanted. However, these prelin- quences, and (c) providing frequent opportunities
guistic forms were considered problematic for practice, which might also include an initial
because reaching often went unnoticed, and training phase in which the person is assisted to
leading others by the hand was viewed as use the microswitch and experience the [reinforc-
socially unacceptable, especially in the commu- ing] consequences of doing so.
nity and with unfamiliar communication part-
ners. The study therefore aimed to replace
reaching and leading by teaching the partici- Summary and Conclusion
pants to use a speech-­ generating device.
Intervention made use of systematic instruc- Intellectual disability is defined as significantly
tional procedures (response prompting, prompt subaverage intellectual ability combined with
fading, and reinforcement) and occurred during deficits in adaptive behavior functioning.
structured snack times when the participants Children with intellectual disability often have
were highly likely to attempt to request the significant social skill deficits and a high preva-
snacks by reaching and leading. During inter- lence of problematic social behaviors. The nature
vention, opportunities for teaching the use of and severity of their social skill deficits and
the speech-generating device were created by excesses vary in relation to the severity and
sometimes having the communication partner etiology of intellectual disability, opportunities
ignore and remain unresponsive to the partici- for learning, and the effectiveness of interven-
pants’ prelinguistic communication attempts. tion efforts.
At these times, participants were prompted to A number of standardized measures have been
activate the speech-generating device, which developed to assess adaptive behavior function-
was programmed to produce digitized speech ing and social skill deficits and excesses.
output (i.e., “I want more.”). Correct use of the Assessments of this type are important for
speech-generating device was reinforced by giv- diagnosis and classification purposes and for
ing participants access to more snacks. The selecting intervention priorities. A range of
results were positive in that the independent use factors need to be considered in selecting inter-
of the speech-generating device increased to the vention priorities, most importantly the potential
80–100% percent range within 18–24 learning impact on the child’s overall quality of life.
opportunities. In addition, one participant also Various intervention approaches have been
showed a decrease in reaching and leading. The developed for teaching social skills and addressing
Intellectual Disability and Social Skills 267

problematic social behaviors of children with Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnsen, S. K. (2010).
Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-4 (TONI-4). Austin,
intellectual disabilities. The range of interven-
TX: Pro-ed.
tions that have empirical support for improving Bruininks, R. H., & Moreau, L. (2004). Checklist of
the social behavior and skills of children with Adaptive Living Skills (CALS). Chicago: Riverside.
intellectual disabilities includes (a) systematic Bruininks, R. H., Moreau, L., Gilman, C. J., & Anderson,
J. L. (2004). Adaptive living skills curriculum (ALSC).
instruction, (b) Social Stories™ (Gray & Garand,
Chicago: Riverside.
1993), (c) video modeling, (d) social problem-­ Bruininks, R. H., Woodcock, R. W., Weatherman, R. F.,
solving intervention, (e) computer-based instruc- & Hill, B. K. (1996). Scales of Independent Behavior-­
tion, and (f) assistive technology interventions. Revised. Itasca, IL: Riverside.
Buntinx, W. (2016). Adaptive behavior and support needs.
By combining valid assessment data with
In A. Carr, C. Linehan, G. O’Reilly, P. Noonan Walsh,
evidence-­based instructional practices, children & J. McEvoy (Eds.), The handbook of intellectual
with intellectual disability can learn a range of disability and clinical psychology practice (pp. 107–
meaningful social skills that will enable them to 135). London: Routledge.
Calculator, S. N. (Ed.). (2015). Angelman syndrome:
more effectively interact with others and partici-
Communication, educational and related consider-
pate in society. ations. Sharjah, UAE: Bentham Science.
Carr, A., & O’Reilly, G. (2016). Diagnosis, classification
and epidemiology. In A. Carr, C. Linehan, G. O’Reilly,
P. Noonan Walsh, & J. McEvoy (Eds.), The handbook
References of intellectual disability and clinical psychology prac-
tice (pp. 3–44). London: Routledge.
American Association on Intellectual and Developmental Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (2015). Facts
Disabilities. (2013). Definition of intellectual dis- about developmental disabilities. Retrieved from
ability. Retrieved from http://aaidd.org/intellectual-­ https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/developmentaldisabili-
disability/definition#.V85PrleM8p4 ties/facts.html
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016).
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed., Autism spectrum disorder (ASD): Data and statistics.
text revision). Washington, DC: Author. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic data.html
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Charlop, M., Dennis, B., Carpenter, M., & Greenberg, A.
Washington, DC: Author. (2010). Teaching socially expressive behaviors to chil-
Avcioglu, H. (2013). Effectiveness of video modelling in dren with autism through video modeling. Education
training students with intellectual disabilities to greet & Treatment of Children, 33, 371–393. doi:10.1353/
people when they meet. Educational Sciences: Theory etc.0.0104
and Practice, 13, 466–477. Cipani, E. (1990). “Excuse me: I’ll have. ..”: Teaching
Barry, L. M., & Burlew, S. B. (2004). Using social sto- appropriate attention-getting behavior to young chil-
ries to teach choice and play skills to children with dren with severe handicaps. Mental Retardation, 28,
autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental 29–33.
Disabilities, 19, 45–51. doi:10.1177/1088357604019 Cipani, E. (2009). Practical research methods for edu-
0010601 cators: Becoming an evidence-based practitioner.
Bellini, S., Akullian, J., & Hopf, A. (2007). Increasing New York: Springer.
social engagement in young children with autism Collins, B. C. (2012). Systematic instruction for students
spectrum disorders using video self-modeling. School with moderate and severe disabilities. Baltimore: Paul
Psychology Review, 36, 80–90. H. Brookes.
Bernard-Opitz, V., Sriram, N., & Nakhoda-Sapuan, S. Cook, B. G., Tankersley, M., & Landrum, T. J. (2013).
(2001). Enhancing social problem solving in chil- Evidence-based practices in learning and behavioral
dren with autism and normal children through disabilities: The search for effective instruction. In
computer-­assisted instruction. Journal of Autism and B. G. Cook, M. Tankersley, & T. J. Landrum (Eds.),
Developmental Disorders, 31, 377–384. doi:10.102 Evidence-based practices: Advances in learning and
3/A:1010660502130 behavioral disabilities (vol. 26, pp. 1–19). Bingley,
Beukelman, D. R., & Mirenda, P. (2013). Augmentative UK: Emerald Group.
and alternative communication: Supporting children Cory, L., Dattilo, J., & Williams, R. (2006). Effects of a
and adults with complex communication needs (4th leisure education program on social knowledge and
ed.). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. skills of youth with cognitive disabilities. Therapeutic
Blau, N. (2016). Genetics of Phenylketonuria: Then and Recreation Journal, 40, 144–164.
now. Human Mutation, 37, 508–515. doi:10.1002/ Cummins, R. A. (2005). Moving from the q­ uality of life
humu.22980 concept to a theory. Journal of Intellectual Disability
268 J. Sigafoos et al.

Research, 49, 699–706. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788. Hoch, J., Spofford, L., Dimian, A., Tervo, R., MacLean,
2005.00738.x W. E., & Symons, F. J. (2016). A direct comparison of
Delano, M. (2007). Video modeling interventions for indi- self-injurious and stereotyped motor behavior between
viduals with autism. Remedial and Special Education, preschool-aged children with and without develop-
28, 33–42. doi:10.1177/07419325070280010401 mental delays. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 41,
Delano, M., & Snell, M. E. (2006). The effects of Social 566–572. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsv102
Stories™ on the social engagement of children with Johnson, L. D., & McMaster, K. L. (2013). Adapting
autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 8, research-based practices with fidelity: Flexibility by
29–42. doi:10.1177/10983007060080010501 design. In B. G. Cook, M. Tankersley, & T. J. Landrum
Dixon, D. R. (2007). Adaptive behavior scales. In J. L. (Eds.), Evidence-based practices: Advances in learn-
Matson (Ed.), Handbook of assessment in persons ing and behavioral disabilities (vol. 26, pp. 65–91).
with intellectual disability (pp. 99–140). San Diego: Bingley, UK: Emerald Group.
Academic Press. Kagohara, D. M., Achmadi, D., van der Meer, L., Lancioni,
Dykens, E. M. (1995). Measuring behavioral phenotypes: G. E., O’Reilly, M. F., Lang, R., … Sigafoos, J. (2013).
Provocations from the “new genetics”. American Teaching two students with Asperger syndrome to
Journal on Mental Retardation, 99, 522–532. greet adults using Social Stories and video modeling.
Elliott, S. N. (2007). Social skills rating system. In N. J. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities,
Salkind (Ed.), Encyclopedia of measurement and sta- 25, 241–251. doi:10.1007/s10882-012-9300-6
tistics (p. 921). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Retrieved Karkhaneh, M., Clark, B., Ospina, M. B., Seida, J. C.,
from http://methods.sagepub.com/base/download/ Smith, V., & Hartling, L. (2010). Social Stories™ to
ReferenceEntry/encyclopedia-of-measurement-and- improve social skills in children with autism spectrum
statistics/n419.xml disorder: A systematic review. Autism, 14, 641–662.
Embregts, P. J. C. M. (2002). Effect of resident and direct-­ doi:10.1177/1362361310373057
care staff training on responding during social inter- Keith, K. D. (2016). Quality of life: the journey is the
actions. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 23, reward. In A. Carr, G. O’Reilly, P. Noonan Walsh, &
353–366. J. McEvoy (Eds.), The handbook of intellectual dis-
Fraser, S. (Ed.). (1995). The bell curve wars: Race, intel- ability and clinical psychology practice (pp. 136–
ligence, and the future of America. New York: Basic 160). London: Routledge.
Books. Kirk, H. E., Gray, K., Riby, D. M., & Cornish, K. M.
Giuliani, F., & El Korh, P. (2016). Social skills groups (2015). Cognitive training as a resolution for early
for adults living with intellectual disabilities. Clinical executive function difficulties in children with intel-
Psychiatry, 2, 1–7. http://clinical-psychiatry.imedpub. lectual disabilities. Research in Developmental
com/archive.php Disabilities, 38, 145–160. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
Gray, C. A., & Garand, J. D. (1993). Social Stories™: ridd.2014.12.026
Improving responses of students with autism with Kuczynski, A., & Udwin, O. (2016). Behavioural phe-
accurate social information. Focus on Autistic notypes in genetic syndromes associated with intel-
Behavior, 8, 1–10. lectual disability. In A. Carr, G. O’Reilly, P. Noonan
Gresham, F. M. (2016). Social skills assessment and inter- Walsh, & J. McEvoy (Eds.), The handbook of intel-
vention for children and youth. Cambridge Journal lectual disability and clinical psychology practice
of Education, 46, 319–332. doi:10.1080/03057 (pp. 431–466). London: Routledge.
64X.2016.1195788 Lain, S. J., Bentley, J. P., Wiley, V., Roberts, C. L., Jack,
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social skills rat- M., Wilcken, B., & Nassar, N. (2016). Association
ing system. Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments. between borderline neonatal thyroid-stimulating hor-
Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (2008). Social skills mone concentrations and educational and developmen-
improvement system: Rating scales manual. tal outcomes: a population-based record-linkage study.
Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments. The Lancet Diabetes & Endocrinology, 4, 756–765.
Gül, S. O. (2016). The combined use of video modeling http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S2213-8587(16)30122-X
and Social Stories in teaching social skills for individ- Lancioni, G. E., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M. F., & Singh,
uals with intellectual disability. Educational Sciences: N. N. (2013). Assistive technology interventions for
Theory and Practice, 16, 83–107. doi:10.12738/ individuals with severe/profound and multiple dis-
estp.2016.1.0046 abilities. New York: Springer.
Harris, J. C. (2005). Intellectual disability: Understanding Lancioni, G. E., & Singh, N. N. (Eds.). (2014). Assistive
its development, causes, classification, evaluation and technologies for people with diverse abilities.
treatment. New York: Oxford University Press. New York: Springer.
Heber, R. (1959). A manual on terminology and classi- Lancioni, G. E., Singh, N. N., O’Reilly, M. F., Sigafoos,
fication in mental retardation. American Journal on J., Boccasini, A., Perilli, V., & Spagnuolo, C. (2016).
Mental Deficiency, 56 (Monograph Supplement, Rev.). Persons with multiple disabilities manage positive
Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve: leisure and communication engagement through a
Intelligence and class structure in American life. technology-aided program. International Journal of
New York: Free Press. Developmental Disabilities, 63, 148. doi:10.1080/204
73869.2016.1187462
Intellectual Disability and Social Skills 269

Lancioni, G. E., Singh, N. N., O’Reilly, M. F., Sigafoos, Matson, J. L., & Shoemaker, M. (2009). Intellectual
J., & Oliva, D. (2014). Assistive technology for peo- disability and its relationship to autism spectrum dis-
ple with severe/profound intellectual and multiple orders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30,
disabilities. In G. E. Lancioni & N. N. Singh (Eds.), 1107–1114. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2009.06.003
Assistive technologies for people with diverse abilities Maulik, P. K., & Harbour, C. K. (2010). Epidemiology
(pp. 277–313). New York: Springer. of intellectual disability. In J. H. Stone & M. Blouin
Lovaas, O. I. (2003). Teaching individuals with develop- (Eds.), International Encyclopedia of Rehabilitation.
mental delays: Basic intervention techniques. Austin, Retrieved from http://cirrie.buffalo.edu/encyclopedia/
TX: Pro-Ed. en/article/144/
MacLean Jr., W. E., Miller, M. L., & Bartsch, K. (2001). Maulik, P. K., Mascarenhas, M. N., Mathers, C. D., Dua,
Mental retardation. In C. E. Walker & M. C. Roberts T., & Saxena, S. (2011). Prevalence of intellectual dis-
(Eds.), Handbook of clinical child psychology (3rd ed., ability: A meta-analysis of population-based studies.
pp. 542–560). New York: Wiley. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 419–436.
Maenner, M. J., Blumberg, S. J., Kogan, M. D., Christensen, doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.12.018
D., Yeargin-Allsopp, M., & Schieve, L. A. (2016). McCoy, K., & Hermansen, E. (2007). Video modeling for
Prevalence of cerebral palsy and intellectual disability individuals with autism: A review of model types and
among children identified in two U.S. National surveys, effects. Education and Treatment of Children, 30(4),
2011–2013. Annals of Epidemiology, 26, 222–226. 183–213.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.annepidem.2016.01.001 McFall, R. M. (1982). A review and reformulation of the
Maione, L., & Mirenda, P. (2006). Effects of video mod- concept of social skills. Behavioral Assessment, 4,
eling and video feedback on peer-directed social lan- 1–33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01321377
guage skills of a child with autism. Journal of Positive McKenzie, K., Milton, M., Smith, G., & Ouellette-Kuntz,
Behavior Interventions, 8, 106–118. doi:10.1177/109 H. (2016). Systematic review of the prevalence and
83007060080020201 incidence of intellectual disability: Current trends and
Mason, R. A., Davis, H. S., Boles, M. B., & Goodwyn, issues. Current Developmental Disability Reports, 3,
F. (2013). Efficacy of point-of-view video modeling: 104–115. doi:10.1007/s40474-016-0085-7
A meta-analysis. Remedial and Special Education, 34, Méndez, F. X., Hidalgo, M. D., & Inglés, C. J. (2006). The
333–345. doi:10.1177/0741932513486298 Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters.
Matson, J. L. (1995). The Matson Evaluation of Social European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 18,
Skills for Individuals with Severe Retardation 30–42. doi: 10.1027//1015-5759.18.1.30.
(MESSIER). Baton Rouge, LA: Scientific Publishers National Center on Birth Deficits and Developmental
Inc.  Disabilities. (2016). Facts about intellectual disabil-
Matson, J. L. (Ed.). (2007). Handbook of assessment ity. Retrieved from https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/acte-
in persons with intellectual disability. San Diego: arly/pdf/parents_pdfs/intellectualdisability.pdf
Academic Press. Nettelbeck, T., & Wilson, C. (1997). Speed of information
Matson, J. L., Hattier, M. A., & Turygin, N. (2012). An processing and cognition. In W. E. MacLean Jr. (Ed.),
evaluation of social skills in adults with pica, autism Ellis’ handbook of mental deficiency, psychological
spectrum disorders, and intellectual disability. Journal theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 245–274). Mahwah,
of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 24, 505– NJ: Erlbaum.
514. doi:10.1007/s10882-012-9286-0 Nikopoulos, C. K., & Keenan, M. (2003). Promoting
Matson, J. L., Matson, M. L., & Rivet, T. T. (2007). social initiation in children with autism using video
Social-skills treatments for children with autism spec- modeling. Behavioral Interventions, 18, 87–108.
trum disorders: An overview. Behavior Modification, doi:10.1002/bin.129
31, 682–707. doi:10.1177/0145445507301650 Nouwens, P. J. G., Lucas, R., Embregts, P. J. C. M., &
Matson, J. L., Neal, D., Hess, J. A., Fodstad, J. C., Mahan, van Nieuwenhuizen, C. (2017). In plain sight but still
S., & Rivet, T. T. (2010). Reliability and validity of the visible: A structured case analysis of people with mild
Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters intellectual disability or borderline intellectual func-
(MESSY). Behavior Modification, 34, 539–558. tioning. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental
doi:10.1177/0145445510384844 Disability, 42(1), 36–44. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/13
Matson, J. L., Neal, D., Worley, J. A., Kozlowski, A. M., 668250.2016.1178220
& Fodstad, J. C. (2012). Factor structure of the O’Reilly, G., & Carr, A. (2016). Intelligence. In A. Carr,
Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters-II C. Linehan, G. O’Reilly, P. Noonan Walsh, &
(MESSY-II). Research in Developmental Disabilities, J. McEvoy (Eds.), The handbook of intellectual dis-
33, 2067–2071. ability and clinical psychology practice (2nd ed.,
Matson, J. L., Rotatori, A., & Helsel, W. J. (1983). pp. 81–106). London: Routledge.
Development of a rating scale to measure social skills O’Reilly, M. F., & Chadsey-Rusch, J. (1992). Teaching
in children: The Matson Evaluation of Social Skills a social skills problem-solving approach to workers
with Youngsters (MESSY). Behaviour Research and with mental retardation: An analysis of generalization.
Therapy, 21, 335–340. Education and Training in Mental Retardation, 27,
324–334. http://www.jstor.org/stable/23878863
270 J. Sigafoos et al.

O’Reilly, M. F., Lancioni, G. E., Sigafoos, J., Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 10, 162–
O’Donoghue, D., Lacey, C., & Edrisinha, C. 178. doi:10.1177/1098300708316259
(2004). Teaching social skills to adults with intel- Scattone, D. (2008). Enhancing the conversation skills
lectual disabilities: a comparison of external con- of a boy with Asperger’s disorder through Social
trol and problem-solving interventions. Research Stories™ and video modeling. Journal of Autism and
in Developmental Disabilities, 25, 399–412. Developmental Disorders, 38, 395–400. doi:10.1007/
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2003.07.003 s10803-007-0392-2
Parmenter, T. R. (2011). What is intellectual disabil- Schalock, R. L., Borthwick-Duffy, S. A., Bradley, V. J.,
ity: How is it assessed and classified? International Buntinx, W. H. E., Coulter, D. L., Craig, E. M.,.
Journal of Disability, Development and Education, .. Yeager, M. H. (2010). Intellectual disability:
58, 303–319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10349 Definition, classification, and systems of support
12X.2011.598675 (11th ed.). Washington DC: American Association on
Patel, D. R., Greydanus, D. E., Merrick, J., & Rubin, Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities.
I. L. (2016). Introduction to intellectual and develop- Shogren, K. A., Wehmeyer, M. L., Seo, H., Thompson,
mental disabilities. In I. L. Rubin, J. Merrick, D. E. J. R., Schalock, R. L., Hughes, C., ..., & Palmer, S. B.
Greydanus, & D. R. Patel (Eds.), Health care for peo- (2016). Examining the reliability and validity of the
ple with intellectual and developmental disabilities Supports Intensity Scale—Children’s Version in chil-
across the lifespan (pp. 5–12). New York: Springer. dren with autism and intellectual disability. Focus on
Pinborough-Zimmerman, J., Satterfield, R., Miller, Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. doi:
J., Bilder, D., Hossain, S., & McMahon, W. 10.1177/1088357615625060
(2007). Communication disorders: Prevalence and Sigafoos, J., Drasgow, E., Halle, J. W., O’Reilly, M. F.,
comorbid intellectual disability, autism and emo- Seely-York, S., Edrisinha, C., & Andrews, A. (2004).
tional/behavioral disorders. American Journal Teaching VOCA use as a communication repair strat-
of Speech-Language Pathology, 16, 359–367. egy. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
doi:10.1044/1058–0360(2007/039 34, 411–422.
President’s Committee on Mental Retardation. (1969, Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M. F., Lancioni, G. E., & Green, V. A.
August). The six-hour retarded child: A report on a (2016). Communication difficulties and the promo-
conference on problems of education of children in the tion of communication skills. In A. Carr, G. O’Reilly,
inner city. Washington, DC: Author. Retrieved from P. Noonan Walsh, & J. McEvoy (Eds.), The handbook of
http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED038827.pdf intellectual disability and clinical psychology practice
Ramdoss, S., Machalicek, W., Rispoli, M., Mulloy, A., (pp. 534–571). London: Routledge.
Lang, R., & O’Reilly, M. (2012). Computer-based Silver, M., & Oakes, P. (2001). Evaluation of a
interventions to improve social and emotional skills in new computer intervention to teach people with
individuals with autism spectrum disorders: A system- autism or asperger syndrome to recognize and
atic review. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 15, predict emotions in others. Autism, 5, 299–316.
119–135. doi:10.3109/17518423.2011.651655 doi:10.1177/1362361301005003007
Reichow, B., & Sabornie, E. J. (2009). Brief report: Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984).
Increasing verbal greeting initiations for a student Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales: Interview Edition
with autism via a Social Story™ intervention. Journal Survey Form. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1740– Service.
1743. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0814-4 Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Sauinier, C. A. (2016).
Reynhout, G., & Carter, M. (2006). Social Stories™ Vineland-3 Adaptive Behavior Scales (3rd ed.).
for children with disabilities. Journal of Autism and Minneapolis: Pearson.
Developmental Disorders, 36, 445–469. doi:10.1007/ Staddon, J. E. R. (2016). Adaptive behavior and learn-
s10803-006-0086-1 ing (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
Rispoli, M. J., Franci, J. H., van der Meer, L., Lang, R., Press.
Pimentel, S., & Camargo, H. (2010). The use of speech-­ Stetson, E. G., & Stetson, R. (2001). Educational assess-
generating devices in communication interventions for ment. In C. E. Walker & M. C. Roberts (Eds.),
individuals with developmental disabilities: A review Handbook of clinical child psychology (3rd ed.,
of the literature. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, pp. 124–150). New York: Wiley.
13, 276–293. doi:10.3109/17518421003636794 Storey, K., & Miner, C. (2011). Systematic instruction of
Sackett, D. L., Rosenberg, W. M. C., Gray, J. A. functional skills for students and adults with disabili-
M., Haynes, R. B., & Richardson, W. S. (1996). ties. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.
Evidence based medicine: what is it and what it isn’t. Sturmey, P. (2014). Adaptive behavior. In P. Sturmey &
British Medical Journal, 312, 71–72. http://dx.doi. R. Didden (Eds.), Evidence-based practice and intel-
org/10.1136/bmj.312.7023.71 lectual disabilities (pp. 29–61). Chichester, UK: Wiley
Sansosti, F. J., & Powell-Smith, K. A. (2008). Using Blackwell.
computer-presented Social Stories and video models Sturmey, P., & Didden, R. (Eds.). (2014). Evidence-based
to increase the social communication skills of chil- practice and intellectual disabilities. Chichester, UK:
dren with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. Wiley Blackwell.
Intellectual Disability and Social Skills 271

Tan, S. S., van der Slot, W. M. A., Ketelaar, M., Becher, & J. McEvoy (Eds.), The handbook of intellectual
J. G., Dallmeijer, A. J., Smits, D.-W., & Roebroeck, disability and clinical psychology practice (2nd ed.,
M. E. (2016). Factors contributing to the longitudinal pp. 497–533). London: Routledge.
development of social participation in individu- Wilde, L., Mitchell, A., & Oliver, C. (2016). Differences
als with cerebral palsy. Research in Developmental in social motivation in children with Smith-Magenis
Disabilities, 57, 125–135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j. syndrome and Down syndrome. Journal of Autism
ridd.2016.03.015 and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2148–2159.
Tassé, M. J., Schalock, R. L., Balboni, G., Spreat, S., doi:10.1007/s10803-016-2743-3
& Navas, P. (2016). Validity and reliability of the Wilkins, J., & Matson, J. L. (2007). Social skills. In J. L.
Diagnostic Adaptive Behaviour Scale. Journal Matson (Ed.), Handbook of assessment in persons
of Intellectual Disability Research, 60, 80–88. with intellectual disability (pp. 321–363). San Diego:
doi:10.1111/jir.12239 Academic Press.
Tureck, K., & Matson, J. L. (2012). An examination of World Health Organization. (2001). The International
the relationship between autism spectrum disorder, Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health
intellectual functioning, and social skills in children. (ICF). Geneva: Author.
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities,
24, 607–615. doi:10.1007/s10882-012-9292-2 Jeff Sigafoos is a professor in the School of Education at
Tylenda, B., Beckett, J., & Barrett, R. P. (2007). Assessing Victoria University of Wellington in Wellington, New
mental retardation using standardized intelligence Zealand. His research focuses on teaching adaptive skills
tests. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Handbook of assessment and providing communication assessment and interven-
in persons with intellectual disability (pp. 27–97). San tion to individuals with developmental and physical
Diego: Academic Press. disabilities.
Vaughn, S., Kim, A. H., Morris Sloan, C. V., Hughes,
M. T., Elbaum, B., & Sridhar, D. (2003). Social Giulio E. Lancioni is a professor in the Department of
skills interventions for young children with dis- Psychology at the University of Bari, Italy. His research
abilities. A synthesis of group design stud- interests include the development and evaluation of assis-
ies. Remedial and Special Education, 24, 2–15. tive technology, social skills training, and strategies for
doi:10.1177/074193250302400101 examining and teaching choice and preference with indi-
Watkins, L., Kuhn, M., O’Reilly, M. F., Lang, R., viduals with severe or profound and multiple disabilities.
Sigafoos, J., & Lancioni, G. E. (2016). Social
skills. In N. N. Singh (Ed.), Handbook of evidence-­ Nirbhay N. Singh is a senior scientist at ONE Research
based practices in intellectual and developmen- Institute, Midlothian, Virginia. His research interests
tal disabilities (pp. 493–509). New York: Springer. include the practice of mindfulness for self-enrichment
doi:10.1007/978-3-319-26583-4_18 and as therapy for psychological distress and learning
Webster-Stratton, C., Reid, J., & Hammond, M. (2001). technologies for improving the lives of people with
Social skills and problem-solving training for chil- disabilities.
dren with early-onset conduct problems: Who ben-
efits? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry Mark F. O’Reilly is a professor in the Department of
and Allied Disciplines, 42, 943–952. http://dx.doi. Special Education at the University of Texas at Austin.
org/10.1017/S0021963001007776 His research interests include assessment and treatment
Wehmeyer, M. L., Lee, S. H., & Shogren, K. A. (2016). of aberrant behavior, social skills training with individ-
Educating children with intellectual disability. In uals with developmental disabilities and autism, and
A. Carr, C. Linehan, G. O’Reilly, P. Noonan Walsh, staff training.
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity
Disorder

Katy E. Tresco, Jessie L. Kessler,
and Jennifer A. Mautone

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) Hyperactive/Impulsive Presentation refers to the


is among the most common psychiatric disorders least common presentation, characterized by sub-
in children (American Psychiatric Association stantial hyperactive/impulsive symptoms without
[APA], 2013), with symptoms and impairment significant symptoms of inattention. Prevalence
related to ADHD persisting into adolescence and estimates of ADHD in school-age child samples
adulthood. For example, adolescents with ADHD in the United States range from 5 to 7% (Roberts,
are at increased risk for school failure, involve- Milich, & Barkley, 2015). Survey research has con-
ment in the juvenile justice system, substance use sistently indicated that ADHD is more frequently
problems, injury, increased health care costs, and identified in boys than girls, with estimates rang-
employment problems (Bussing, Mason, Bell, ing from three times more likely in community
Porter, & Garvan, 2010; Molina & Pelham, 2001; samples to upwards of nine times more frequent in
Winston, McDonald, & McGehee, 2013). There clinical samples (Roberts et al., 2015).
are three presentations of ADHD described by Children with ADHD typically experience
the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental problems related to academic performance (e.g.,
Disorders—Fifth Edition (DSM-5; APA, 2013). lower test scores, higher rates of grade retention)
Clinically significant levels of hyperactivity/impul- and social interactions, including family relation-
sivity and difficulties with attention and focus char- ships, interactions with adults at school, and rela-
acterize ADHD, Combined Presentation. ADHD, tionships with peers (Roberts et al., 2015).
Predominantly Inattentive Presentation refers to Because of challenging classroom behavior,
individuals with significant attention problems including increased off-task behavior compared
in the absence of clinically significant hyperac- to peers, frequent violations of classroom rules,
tivity/impulsivity, and ADHD, Predominantly and failure to comply with instructions, teachers
often spend a significant amount of time provid-
ing supports to children with ADHD, which may
result in conflict in the student-teacher relation-
ship (Greene, Beszterczey, Katzenstein, Park, &
K.E. Tresco (*) • J.L. Kessler
Philadelphia College of Osteopathic Medicine, Goring, 2002). Also, due to behavioral difficulty
Philadelphia, PA, USA at home, children with ADHD frequently have
e-mail: katytr@pcom.edu stressful and conflicting interactions with their
J.A. Mautone parents, which negatively impact parent-child
Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, Perelman School relationships (Johnston & Chronis-Tuscano,
of Medicine at the University of Pennsylvania, 2015). Finally, due to disruptive and inattentive
Philadelphia, PA, USA

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 273


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_15
274 K.E. Tresco et al.

behavior, children with ADHD often have diffi- Whalen & Henker, 1985). The peer-related
culty developing and maintaining friendships ­difficulties that are most prominent in children
with peers (Hoza, 2007). with ADHD include peer victimization/bullying,
Treatments to support children with ADHD rejection, lower friendship quality, and reputation
include medication, primarily stimulants, and bias (Hoza, 2007). Further, poor peer relation-
psychosocial interventions that are implemented ships among school-aged children with ADHD
at home and school. Psychosocial interventions are predictive of later difficulties, such as sub-
address performance deficits (i.e., situations in stance abuse, delinquency, academic difficulties,
which the child knows how to perform a particu- and internalizing symptoms (Mikami & Hinshaw,
lar skill but does not do so consistently) and skills 2006; Mrug et al., 2012). Additionally, children
deficits (i.e., situations in which the child does with ADHD have difficulty assessing their own
not yet possess a skill or performs the skill inad- social competence. This lack of insight (often
equately; Eiraldi, Mautone, & Power, 2012). called “positive illusory bias”) often contributes
Interventions aimed at performance deficits to the social difficulties of children with
include behavioral strategies and environmental ADHD. In the following sections, we discuss the
adaptations to intervene at the point of perfor- relationship between ADHD symptoms/comor-
mance, such as strategies focused on contingency bid conditions and social functioning, and the
management (Pelham & Fabiano, 2008). specific domains of social impairment for chil-
Interventions to address skills deficits include dren with ADHD (i.e., peer victimization/bully-
direct instruction and increasing opportunities for ing, rejection and reputation bias, friendship
repeated practice of new skills. The social defi- quality, self-perceptions). In addition, gender and
cits of children with ADHD might be the result of developmental differences are considered.
performance deficits, skills deficits, or a combi-
nation of the two.
The purpose of this chapter is to discuss the  DHD Symptoms, Comorbid
A
social impairments for children with ADHD and Conditions, and Social Functioning
the current best practices with regard to assess-
ment of and intervention for social difficulties. ADHD Symptoms
We acknowledge that children and adolescents The symptom profile as identified by the DSM-5
with ADHD often have difficulties in their rela- includes impulsive and disruptive behaviors that
tionships with adults, as described above. The often are perceived negatively by peers (e.g., fre-
focus of this chapter, however, is on social behav- quent interruption of conversations, excessive
ior of children and adolescents with ADHD as it talking, difficulty waiting for turn) and inatten-
relates to interactions with peers and its effects tive behaviors (e.g., easily distracted, failure to
on peer relationships and functioning. First, a listen carefully) that are likely to impact the abil-
detailed discussion of the social impairments of ity of children with ADHD to engage appropri-
children with ADHD is provided. Then we ately during conversations with peers and in
review strategies for assessing social behavior in group activities (APA, 2013). The increased rates
children with ADHD. Finally, the current of negative verbalizations, rule breaking, com-
research related to treatment of social problems plaining, teasing, and noncompliance associated
in children with ADHD is discussed. with combined and hyperactive/impulsive pre-
sentations of ADHD further contribute to these
children being perceived negatively by peers
Social Impairment in ADHD (Mrug, Hoza, Pelham, Gnagy, & Greiner, 2007;
Pelham & Bender, 1982). In addition, children
The impact of ADHD symptoms on the social presenting as primarily inattentive have been
functioning of children with ADHD has long described as shy, withdrawn, and less assertive as
been recognized (Pelham & Bender, 1982; compared to children presenting with combined
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder 275

or hyperactive presentations (Hodgens, Cole, & 40% and 84% of children with ADHD are
Boldizar, 2000; Solanto, Pope-Boyd, Tryon, & ­diagnosed with a comorbid externalizing disor-
Stepak, 2009). Associated difficulties, such as der, between 25% and 50% with an anxiety disor-
deficits in executive function skills, have also der, and between 20% and 30% with comorbid
been associated with poor social outcomes depression (Pliszka, 2015). Although children
(Miller & Hinshaw, 2010; Tseng & Gau, 2013). with ADHD have challenges with peer relation-
Executive functioning deficits associated with ships regardless of the presence of comorbidities,
ADHD (e.g., planning, organization, attention, poor social relationships may be exacerbated by
memory, and response inhibition) have been the additional symptomology associated with
explored as a potential underpinning for difficul- comorbid disorders (Becker et al., 2012).
ties with application of social knowledge. In a review of the literature examining the
Research suggests that executive functioning relationship between comorbid mental health dis-
deficits are negatively associated with peer accep- orders and social functioning in children with
tance in adolescent girls, regardless of ADHD ADHD, Becker et al. (2012) found that depend-
diagnosis (Miller & Hinshaw, 2010), and mea- ing on methodology, the presence of comorbid
sures of working memory and planning are asso- externalizing disorders had either no effect or
ciated with parent and self-report ratings of social exacerbated the peer relationship problems of
competence for children with ADHD (McQuade, children with ADHD. For example, children with
Murray-Close, Shoulberg, & Hoza, 2013; Tseng ADHD and ODD or CD have been found to have
& Gau, 2013). Findings of another study examin- poorer parent- and teacher-rated social compe-
ing working memory and social competence sug- tence than children with ADHD alone (Booster,
gested that the relationship between poor central DuPaul, Eiraldi, & Power, 2012), and the addi-
executive working memory and lower levels of tion of conduct problems increases teacher rat-
social competence may be due to difficulties in ings of peer rejection (Mikami & Lorenzi, 2011).
conflict resolution, which ultimately relates to In addition, the presence of oppositional and/or
increased rates of physical aggression (McQuade conduct-related behaviors comorbid with ADHD
et al., 2013). In a study examining the relation- increases the likelihood of peer rejection and
ship of executive functioning, social competence, decreases the likelihood that an individual will be
and ADHD, however, measures of executive considered popular by their peers (Hoza et al.,
functioning did not mediate the relationship 2005). Finally, there is evidence to suggest that
between ADHD and social performance (Huang-­ the behaviors related to ODD, rather than ADHD,
Pollock, Mikami, Pfiffner, & McBurnett, 2009). contribute to engagement in bullying and rela-
It should be noted that direct assessment of peer tional aggression behaviors (Ohan & Johnston,
perceptions (i.e., sociometric nominations) was 2007; Wiener & Mak, 2009).
not included in any of the above studies. As such, Findings for internalizing disorders are less
the effect of poor executive functioning on peer clear but also indicate either no effect or an exac-
rejection and victimization is unclear. erbation of social relationship problems (Becker
et al., 2012). Specifically, anxiety has been found
Comorbid Conditions to be negatively associated with teacher-reported
Children with ADHD are often diagnosed with nominations of peer like and dislike but not on
comorbid conditions that might result in increased nominations by peers themselves (Hoza et al.,
social impairment (Becker, Luebbe, & Langberg, 2005; Mikami, Ransone, & Calhoun, 2011).
2012). Comorbid conditions commonly associ- Mikami and colleagues hypothesize that this may
ated with ADHD include other externalizing dis- be because peers do not consider the shy and ner-
orders such as oppositional defiant disorder and vous behaviors associated with anxiety to be as
conduct disorder, as well as internalizing disor- bothersome as they do the disruptive behaviors
ders, predominantly anxiety. Estimates vary associated with ADHD. Parent and child ratings,
depending on source, but it is likely that between however, also have suggested that children with
276 K.E. Tresco et al.

ADHD and anxiety have deficits in social and bullying behaviors among 9–14-year-old
competence (Becker, Langberg, Evans, Girio- children (including those that were identified as
Herrera, & Vaughn, 2015; Bowen, Chavira, both victims and bullies), Wiener and Mak found
Bailey, Stein, & Stein, 2008). A recent examina- that 40% of children with ADHD were identified
tion of self-­reported anxiety symptoms in young as victims in comparison with 9% of non-ADHD
adolescents with ADHD found increased social comparison peers. Additionally, parents and
anxiety to be associated with poorer social skills teachers of children and adolescents with ADHD
and social acceptance (Becker et al., 2015). reported higher levels of bullying behaviors
Very few studies have examined the impact of among this same sample of youth than among
comorbid depression and ADHD on social func- comparison peers without ADHD (31–10%).
tioning, and results are mixed. In general, the Notably, approximately four times as many chil-
limited findings available to date suggest that dren and adolescents with ADHD were identi-
comorbid depression either has no impact or fied as either a bully or a victim (or both) as
exacerbates the social problems of children with compared to peers without ADHD (58% vs.
ADHD (Becker et al., 2012). Consistent with 14%). It is important to note, however, that chil-
their findings on social anxiety, Becker et al. dren with ADHD have also been found to under-
(2015) also found that depressive symptoms (i.e., represent their involvement in bullying and
anhedonia) were associated with poorer social threatening behaviors; however, these children
skills and lower social acceptance. In addition, might be more accurate reporters when they are
parent- and teacher-rated social skills such as the recipient of peer victimization (Wiener &
assertion and self-control have been found to Mak, 2009).
mediate the relationship between ADHD and the Victimization by peers has been associated
development of depressive symptoms (Feldman, with social skills deficits, peer rejection, a lack of
Tung, & Lee, 2016). This suggests that poor reciprocated friendships, and increased levels of
social skills concurrent with ADHD may also comorbid symptoms in children and adolescents
increase risk for later depression. In addition, with ADHD (Scholte et al., 2009). Specifically,
individuals with ADHD who have better aware- children who are victims are more likely to be
ness of their social standings are likely to experi- rejected by peer groups, have fewer reciprocal
ence increased depressive symptoms (McQuade friendships, and experience increased levels of
et al., 2014). internalizing and externalizing symptoms
(Hodges, Boivin, Vitaro, & Bukowski, 1999;
Scholte et al., 2009). Additionally, parent and
Peer Victimization and Bullying self-reports of children with ADHD suggest links
between victimization by peers, lower self-­
Children with ADHD are both more likely to be esteem, negative affect, anxiety, and depressive
bullied or victimized by peers and to engage in symptoms in children and adolescents with
bullying behaviors toward peers (Wiener & Mak, ADHD (Becker et al., 2016; Fogelman, Walerius,
2009). Among children and adolescents with Rosen, & Leaberry, 2016; Roy, Hartman,
ADHD who have been bullied or victimized, Veenstra, & Oldehinkel, 2015). As such, it is pos-
peer victimization includes physical, verbal sible that the poor social relationships of children
(e.g., teasing), relational (e.g., exclusion from with ADHD contribute to being rejected and vic-
groups), and reputational aggression (e.g., timized by peers and to the development of and/
rumors or gossip intended to damage social or exacerbation of comorbid internalizing and
standing) with estimates as high as 57% of ado- externalizing disorders. In addition, children with
lescents with ADHD being the victim of at least ADHD who have higher levels of comorbid inter-
one of these at least once per week in the past nalizing and externalizing problems are more
year (Becker, Mehari, Langberg, & Evans, likely to be victimized by peers than those with
2016). In an examination of peer victimization ADHD alone, potentially leading to further
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder 277

impacts on self-esteem and depressive symptoms ADHD. Additionally, research has suggested


(Taylor, Saylor, Twyman, & Macias, 2010). that, over time, peers continue to view children
with ADHD negatively, regardless of whether the
symptoms of ADHD contributing to social
Peer Rejection and Reputation Bias impairment are improved (Hoza, 2007). These
findings suggest that the often negative reputa-
Peer rejection, although not often as overt as bul- tions of children with ADHD might inhibit
lying/victimization, is also of concern for chil- improvements in social standing, even if social
dren with ADHD. Peer sociometric nominations competence improves.
(i.e., peers’ indications of whether an individual
is a friend or someone with whom they would not
want to be friends) consistently reveal that chil- Friendship Quality
dren with ADHD are more often identified as
individuals with whom classmates do not want to The quality of the relationship between friends
be friends. For example, in an examination of a may be as important a consideration as the pres-
subset of children with ADHD from the MTA ence of friendships, particularly among children
study (MTA Cooperative Group, 1999) with with ADHD. Friendship intimacy, for example,
sociometric nominations available, 52% were may mitigate the relationship between ADHD
identified as being rejected by peers as compared and social problems (Becker, Fite, Luebbe,
to only 14% of same-aged classroom peers with- Stoppelbein, & Greening, 2013). Specifically,
out ADHD (Hoza et al., 2005). Furthermore, over Becker and colleagues found that levels of
half of children with ADHD (56%) lacked a ADHD symptoms were not associated with
reciprocated friendship, that is, a friendship in social problems for children ages 5–13 years who
which both children identify the other as a friend, self-reported higher ratings of friendship quality.
in comparison with 32% of a group of children Increased ADHD symptomology was associated
without ADHD. Also, only 9% of children with with increased social problems, however, for
ADHD had reciprocal friendships with more than children with ratings of low friendship quality.
one person, in comparison with 22% for children Unfortunately, friendships involving a child
without ADHD. with ADHD have been shown to be of poorer
Additionally, children with ADHD often quality than friendships between children with-
develop poor social reputations that begin early out ADHD, with evidence indicating that chil-
and, despite direct intervention, are difficult to dren with ADHD are more controlling,
change (Hoza, 2007; Hoza et al., 2005). insensitive, and self-centered than their friends
Specifically, children and adolescents perceive (Normand et al., 2011). In an effort to more fully
peers with ADHD to be disruptive and recognize understand friendships among children with
that these children often display challenging ADHD, Normand et al. (2013) explored stability,
behavior in class. In a survey of stigma associ- quality, and satisfaction in friendship dyads and
ated with ADHD, respondents (children ages 8 to found that children with ADHD had less stable
18 years), rated items about their attributions friendships over time. In addition, friends of chil-
related to a fictional classmate with ADHD, with dren with ADHD reported fewer positive friend-
a health concern (i.e., asthma), or with an inter- ship features, more negative features, and
nalizing disorder (i.e., depression). Respondents indicated less satisfaction in their friendships
indicated that the child with ADHD was likely to than friends of children without ADHD. Children
get into trouble more often than the child with a with ADHD have also been found to spend less
health concern or internalizing disorder (Walker, time with friends outside of school (Marton,
Coleman, Lee, Squire, & Friesen, 2008), thereby Wiener, Rogers, & Moore, 2015), potentially
illustrating the development of a negative social another indicator of the lack of intimacy in the
reputation among children with relationships.
278 K.E. Tresco et al.

In addition to having lower quality ­friendships, cal friendships, contributes to the likelihood of
children with ADHD appear to be more likely to delinquency, anxiety, and general impairment in
develop friendships with other children with functioning (Mrug et al., 2012). In this latter
behavior difficulties. Specifically, evidence sug- study, however, quality of friendship was not
gests that children who identify themselves as a evaluated nor were data collected regarding
friend of a child with ADHD are more likely to whether the identified reciprocal friends also
exhibit externalizing behavior problems exhibited externalizing problems. As such, future
(Normand et al., 2011). In one study, 43% of research should explore the association between
children with ADHD were identified as having friendship quality and peer rejection/peer
friends with either externalizing or learning prob- victimization.
lems, as compared to 16% of peers without
ADHD (Marton et al., 2015). It has been sug-
gested that this results from the fact that children  elf-Perceptions of Social Impairment
S
with similar behavioral difficulties are more will- (Positive Illusory Bias)
ing to befriend a child with ADHD (McQuade &
Hoza, 2015) and that children with ADHD are Children with ADHD often lack insight regard-
drawn to other children with similar characteris- ing both their social competence and the nature
tics (Normand et al., 2011). Further research is of their relationships with peers. With regard to
necessary to explore this association and its rela- social competence, children with ADHD often
tionship to peer rejection, reputation bias, and overestimate their skill more dramatically than
peer victimization. do peers without ADHD (Owens et al., 2007),
Children with ADHD also report being less although some evidence exists to suggest that this
satisfied in their friendships than peers without may be less pronounced in children with ADHD
ADHD (Normand et al., 2013). Interestingly, inattentive presentation (Owens & Hoza, 2003).
findings from the same investigation suggested This “positive illusory bias” appears to be more
that children with ADHD are less likely than prominent in children with ADHD than peers
their friends without ADHD to recognize the without ADHD (Hoza et al., 2004). Examinations
deterioration of their friendships over time. of peer relationships have also shown that chil-
This finding is consistent with previous research dren with ADHD report a more positive view of
indicating that children with ADHD generally their peers than their peers’ report of them (Hoza
lack the ability to accurately evaluate their own et al., 2005), a further indication of a lack of
social competence (see Hoza, Vaughn, awareness regarding how peers may perceive
Waschbusch, Murray-Close, & McCabe, 2012; their behavior.
Owens, Goldfine, Evangelista, Hoza, & Kaiser, Children with ADHD have also been found to
2007). self-evaluate their social performance more posi-
The impact of mutual, quality friendships on tively than their teachers, despite the presence of
healthy development is unquestionable. In chil- an intervention designed to improve their ability
dren with ADHD, reciprocal friendships may to self-evaluate (Hoza et al., 2012). Interestingly,
mitigate the effects of peer victimization and Hoza and colleagues found that the match
have also been identified as protective factors, between child and teacher evaluations was
potentially lessening the likelihood of the devel- improved through the use of positive reinforce-
opment of internalizing and externalizing prob- ment as it related to academic and behavioral
lems (Hodges et al., 1999; Laursen, Bukowski, functioning, but not social competence. Whether
Aunola, & Nurmi, 2007). Friendships have also this lack of improvement is an artifact of a lack of
been shown to be protective against peer victim- ability to accurately self-evaluate social
ization in girls (Cardoos & Hinshaw, 2011). competence or a difficulty admitting to poor
­
Other evidence suggests, however, that peer social competence is in need of further explora-
rejection, not the presence or absence of recipro- tion (McQuade & Hoza, 2015).
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder 279

The positive illusory bias common to children boys and girls with ADHD include a lack of
with ADHD may also negatively impact response reciprocated friendships and increased rates of
to treatment. For example, in a well-established peer rejection and victimization as compared to
summer treatment program for children with peers without ADHD (Becker et al., 2016; Hoza
ADHD (e.g., Pelham & Hoza, 1996), Mikami et al., 2005). Boys with ADHD, however, are
and colleagues found that children’s positive illu- more likely than girls with ADHD to (a) be vic-
sory bias was stable over time and that high levels tims of physical aggression (Becker et al., 2016),
of positive illusory bias were associated with (b) self-report higher levels of bullying others,
increased conduct problems and increased dislike and (c) be identified by adults as engaging in bul-
by peers at the end of the treatment cycle lying behaviors (Wiener & Mak, 2009). In addi-
(Mikami, Calhoun, & Abikoff, 2010). Conversely, tion, boys with ADHD may be more likely to be
children who had lower levels of positive illusory friends with peers with behavior problems than
bias improved in social preference and gained in girls with ADHD, though both boys and girls
friendship nominations over the course of with ADHD have a higher proportion of friends
treatment. with behavior problems than peers (27%–10%;
Evidence also suggests that the positive illu- Marton et al., 2015).
sory bias associated with ADHD might serve as a Conversely, girls with ADHD may be more
protective factor, limiting the likelihood of devel- likely than boys with ADHD to be victimized by
opment of depressive symptoms. That is, the peers without ADHD (Wiener & Mak, 2009). In
overestimation of self-competence and peer sta- addition, studies of friendship quality among
tus often found among children with ADHD may girls have indicated a higher rate of relational
make them less sensitive to the detrimental aggression for girls with ADHD than girls with-
effects of peer rejection and victimization out ADHD (Zalecki & Hinshaw, 2004).
(Mikami et al., 2010). An investigation utilizing Specifically, girls with ADHD are more likely to
data from the MTA study found that for individu- engage in behaviors that attempt to harm anoth-
als with lower peer preference (more negative er’s reputation, resulting in damage to their own
nominations by peers than positive), increased relationships with peers (Zalecki & Hinshaw,
self-identified social acceptance predicted lower 2004). This is likely because girls with ADHD
scores on a self-report of depressive symptoms tend to engage in these behaviors in a more overt
(McQuade et al., 2014). Conversely, McQuade manner than their peers without ADHD, and this
and colleagues also found that lower levels of overt behavior is less accepted by peers (Ohan &
self-reported social acceptance were associated Johnston, 2007). Specifically, girls without
with higher levels of self-reported depression. ADHD were found to engage in less frequent but
These findings support the protective nature of more directed, covert, and intense relational
the positive illusory bias of children and adoles- aggression than girls with ADHD. McQuade and
cents with ADHD but included only self-report Hoza (2015) suggest that the less skilled use of
measures of depression. As such, further investi- relational aggression may prevent girls with
gation into the relationship between ADHD, peer ADHD from obtaining the social benefit that has
rejection and victimization, positive illusory bias, been found for skilled and covert uses of rela-
and behaviors and outcomes associated with tional aggression in girls without ADHD
depression is warranted. (Heilbron & Prinstein, 2008).

Impact of Gender Adolescence

Boys and girls with ADHD both experience Peer relationships become even more relevant
social impairment and are at risk for problems and important in adolescence as youth develop
with peers, as noted above. Similarities among increased independence from their families and
280 K.E. Tresco et al.

typically spend more time with peers (Maccoby, Pfiffner, McBurnett, & Hangai, 2007). Subtype
1992); however, the impairments in peer relation- differences that exist in live social interactions
ships common among children with ADHD have also appear to persist online. Children with
been shown to persist into adolescence. As a ADHD combined presentation tend to exhibit
result, adolescents with ADHD may experience more disruptive and hostile responses to peers,
increased social difficulty as compared to whereas children with ADHD inattentive presen-
younger children with ADHD. Peers tend to tation respond less frequently than children with-
identify adolescents with ADHD as less well out ADHD (Mikami et al., 2007). In addition, the
liked than peers without ADHD (Sibley, Evans, poor social judgment and impulsive behavior
& Serpell, 2010). Additionally, when youth with inherent in ADHD may lead to increased engage-
ADHD, particularly girls, are rejected by peers, ment in risky online behaviors such as interacting
they are at increased risk for internalizing and with unknown individuals and posting private
externalizing problems, eating disorders, and pictures or information (McQuade & Hoza,
lower levels of academic achievement (Mikami 2015).
& Hinshaw, 2006). Cyberbullying is an increasingly common
In addition, adolescents with ADHD have online occurrence, with estimates of victimiza-
been found to be at increased risk for engagement tion between 20% and 40% for all youth
in problematic social behaviors, including drug (Aboujaoude, Savage, Starcevic, & Salame,
and alcohol use, smoking, risky sexual behaviors, 2015). Given that children with ADHD are more
and association with deviant peers (Bussing likely than their peers without ADHD to be a vic-
et al., 2010; Flory, Molina, Pelham, Gnagy, & tim and a perpetrator of peer victimization in
Smith, 2006; Marshal, Molina, & Pelham, 2003). face-to-face interactions (Wiener & Mak, 2009),
Adolescents with ADHD who become associated it is likely that adolescents with ADHD also are
with deviant peers are at additional risk for involved in cyberbullying as victims and perpe-
engaging in unsafe behaviors (Marshal & Molina, trators at higher rates than peers without
2006). Furthermore, peer rejection increases risk ADHD. Limited research suggests that conduct
for adolescent substance use and delinquency, problems and hyperactivity have been associated
beyond the risk found for ADHD alone (Mrug with perpetrating cyberbullying, whereas having
et al., 2012). poor interpersonal skills has been linked with
being the victim of cyberbullying for all youth
(see Aboujaoude et al., 2015, for review). In an
Social Media and Cyberbullying examination of cyberbullying in children with
ADHD and Asperger syndrome (diagnosed
Children and adolescents commonly interact according to DSM-IV TR; APA, 2000), Kowalski
with peers using social media; as youth increas- and Fedina (2011) found that 21% of respondents
ingly have access to their own smartphones, it is ages 10–20 years had been the victims of cyber-
challenging for parents and caregivers to care- bullying within the last 2 months and 6% indi-
fully monitor social media use. Although the lit- cated they had been a perpetrator during the same
erature in this area is underdeveloped, initial time period. The experiences of children and
investigations support the idea that the social adolescents with ADHD alone were not investi-
deficits evident in face-to-face interpersonal gated separately in the study. Although the rate of
interactions of youth with ADHD are likely to engagement in cyberbullying behavior as a per-
generalize to social media outlets (McQuade & petrator was relatively low, this investigation
Hoza, 2015). For example, online chat room included only self-report. It is possible that the
behavior is similar to behavior observed during positive illusory bias that prevents children and
live social interactions; youth with ADHD adolescents with ADHD from recognizing social
engage in negative and aversive behaviors in both impairment may also lead to decreased self-­
social situations (Mikami, Huang-Pollock, recognition of cyberbullying behaviors.
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder 281

Given the lack of literature in this area, the at a time or location detached from the original
relationship of social media to peer interactions occurrence of the behavior) measures, whereas
in children and adolescents with ADHD is in multi-informant refers to the need for input from
need of further investigation. Rates of victimiza- a variety of respondents (e.g., parents, teachers,
tion, bullying, and risky online behaviors, as well child; Odom et al., 2008; Timler & White, 2015).
as their association with self-competence, posi- The following section reviews common practices
tive illusory bias, and related social functioning, in the assessment of social behaviors and rela-
are all areas in need of additional study. The tionships for children and adolescents with
impact of these factors on adjustment and mental ADHD according to method (i.e., rating scale,
health should also be explored. sociometric nominations, and observation) and
informant (i.e., adult, peer, and self-report).

 ssessment of Social Behavior


A
in ADHD Method

When considering areas of impairment for chil- The primary methods used to assess social behav-
dren and adolescents with ADHD, assessment is iors and relationships in ADHD include rating
a crucial first step in the development of treat- scales, peer sociometric nominations, and direct
ment plans and refers to the process of evaluat- observations. A description of each, with specific
ing, measuring, or documenting the nature or examples commonly used in assessment of social
quality of a construct (Mash & Hunsley, 2005). impairment of children and adolescents with
Accurate assessment allows for improved selec- ADHD, is presented below. A further discussion
tion of treatment options through the identifica- of the importance of obtaining information from
tion of patterns of strengths and weaknesses in multiple informants and the relative value of
social competence displayed by children and obtaining information from each informant as it
adolescents with ADHD. The goal of assessment relates to specific social impairments described
is to gather qualitative and quantitative data in above will follow.
order to operationally define a problem or target
behavior (e.g., frequency, duration, setting), Rating Scale
identify corresponding skill or performance defi- Rating scales utilize a variety of structures and
cits, and highlight individual strengths that can designs to measure the informant’s perception of
be integrated in treatment. Thorough assessment the degree to which an item is representative of
practices thus allow for the development of a the child’s behavior (Thomas et al., 2011). Rating
comprehensive treatment plan that capitalizes on scales are used to gather quantitative information
strengths and includes individualized interven- for the screening, diagnosis, and progress moni-
tions that target specific areas of social perfor- toring of social behavior in children with ADHD
mance (Mash & Hunsley, 2005; Thomas, Shapiro, and allow the evaluator to assess a broad range of
DuPaul, Lutz, & Kern, 2011). impairments including difficulties sustaining
Due to the complex nature of social interac- attention, hyperactivity, interpersonal relation-
tions, it is critical to collect a comprehensive ships, emotional difficulties, and quality of life
sample of the child’s behavior in order to accu- (Zavadenko et al., 2011). Equally important, rat-
rately identify patterns of strengths and weak- ing scales can be administered across environ-
nesses. Best practices dictate multi-method, ments to assess natural variations in social
multi-informant assessment of social behavior behavior that occur in different contexts (e.g., a
(Odom, McConnell, & Brown, 2008; Timler & classroom with familiar peers and a community
White, 2015). Multi-method refers to the integra- setting with novel children; Zavadenko et al.,
tion of direct (i.e., assess behavior at the exact 2011). It is important to consider, however, that
time it occurs) and indirect (i.e., assess behavior rating scales are indirect measures of behavior
282 K.E. Tresco et al.

and are therefore susceptible to informant subjec- peers to identify classmates with whom they
tivity and responder biases (Bordens & Horowitz, would like to be friends, as well as classmates
2008; Thomas et al., 2011). Despite these criti- with whom they would least like to be friends
cisms, the relatively low cost combined with (Coie, Dodge, & Coppotelli, 1982). A score for
fairly straightforward administration, compara- each child is then determined by dividing the
tively simplistic interpretation, and minimal number of “most liked” nominations by the num-
completion time makes rating scales an efficient ber of “least liked” nominations. Ratings can be
and desirable method of assessment (Thomas used to categorize peers into “accepted” and
et al., 2011). “rejected” status, to identify social preference
Published rating scales are typically norm ref- and social impact or visibility, as well as to iden-
erenced to allow the evaluator to compare the tar- tify reciprocal friends (i.e., when two students
get child’s performance with similar peers (e.g., both identify each other as a student they “like”;
based on age and gender; Thomas et al., 2011). see, e.g., Hoza et al., 2005). Sociometric nomina-
Depending on the nature of the concern, there are tions are also typically used to classify students
several rating scales available to address relevant into groups; most common group designations
areas of social impairment associated with include “popular” (i.e., those with the most
ADHD. Although a review of all rating scales “liked” status), “rejected” (i.e., most “least
with utility in the ADHD population is beyond liked”), “neglected” (i.e., neither most nor least
the scope of this chapter, some examples that liked), and “controversial” (i.e., equal numbers of
may be used to assess areas of social impairment most and least liked; Coie et al., 1982).
include (a) the Social Skills Rating Scale Although peer nominations can provide useful
(Gresham & Elliott, 1990), which includes par- information related to the peer acceptance and
ent, teacher, and self-report of general social social status of children with ADHD, this method
skills; (b) the Revised Peer Experiences can be rather cumbersome in that it requires
Questionnaire (Prinstein, Boergers, & Vernberg, obtaining information from a large group of chil-
2001), which addresses peer victimization in dren. Also, solely focusing on one classroom
adolescence; (c) the Self-Perception of Social might not provide a comprehensive evaluation of
Acceptance Scale (Harter, 2012) to assess self-­ a target child’s social status, particularly for older
awareness of social competence; and (d) the children and adolescents, who often change
Social Competence Scales (Spence, 1995), which classrooms throughout the school day and have
includes parent and child report of interactions varying amounts of contact with peers in each
and acceptance with peer groups. It is important classroom. In addition, with increased social
to note that rating scales do not necessarily pro- media use, children and adolescents are better
vide detailed information about specific social able to maintain contact and develop friendships
skills deficits; the majority of available scales with individuals who do not attend the same
focus instead on adult or self-awareness of social schools, so it is challenging to accurately obtain
competence and acceptance. Given that children sociometric data from peers outside of the school
and adolescents with ADHD commonly experi- environment.
ence peer rejection and reputation bias, it is often
beneficial to obtain direct information regarding Observation
peer functioning. Observation is a direct assessment measure
obtained from witnessing a behavior while it is
Sociometric Nominations occurring. Structured observations can use prede-
Sociometric nominations are the direct assess- termined tasks (e.g., directing children to work
ment method most commonly used to explore together to complete a task), controlled social
peer status of children with ADHD. This method situations (e.g., child is placed with unfamiliar
of evaluation is designed to evaluate peer accep- peers in a social setting), or observation tem-
tance and rejection and typically requires asking plates (e.g., antecedent, behavior, and
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder 283

c­onsequence) to measure and/or elicit specific across multiple settings. The majority of
behaviors such as the initiation of social interac- ­children with ADHD interact with numerous
tions (Hunsley & Mash, 2007). For example, people including family members, educational
Normand et al. (2011; 2013) coded videotaped faculty, mental health service providers, and
tasks in an effort to evaluate quality of social peers across a variety of environments, includ-
interactions for children with ADHD when paired ing home, school, and community settings.
with children without ADHD. Alternatively, Given the complex nature of social impairment
Mikami et al. (2007) observed interactions detailed earlier in this chapter, it is crucial that
between children with ADHD and simulated the assessment of social difficulties includes
peers in an online chat room. This type of direct information provided by a sample of respon-
observation of social media use might become a dents familiar with the child’s typical daily
much more relevant method of assessment of interactions. For the purposes of this chapter,
social interactions as research evaluating the respondents will be discussed in the three groups
social networking use of children and adoles- (i.e., adults, peers, and self) that are primary to
cents increases. the assessment of social behavior in children
Observations do not require standardization or and adolescents with ADHD.
normative comparisons and, as such, can be con- AdultSocial behaviors are most commonly
ducted in natural environments (e.g., in class- assessed through information gathered from
rooms, during extracurricular activities) as well adult informants, typically through rating scales
as in contrived settings, such as a one-on-one such as those described above (Mikami, 2015).
testing session (Winner, 2002). Observation sys- Although the primary adult informants include
tems also allow evaluators to collect information parents and teachers, extended family members
regarding environmental factors, such as ante- (e.g., grandparents), other educational profes-
cedents and consequences that may be reinforc- sionals (e.g., classroom aides), and mental
ing target behaviors (e.g., a child has an health providers (e.g., therapists, behavior spe-
opportunity to see his friends in the hallway when cialists) can also provide valuable information.
he cuts class; LaRue, Sloman, Dashow, & When obtaining information from adult infor-
Isenhower, 2015). mants, professionals traditionally favor input
Despite the practicality, direct observation of from teachers, as they generally have the most
social behavior has limitations. First, observa- opportunity to observe the child interacting
tions are subject to biases, such as observational with peers; however, clinical and research best
bias (i.e., observers focus on areas where they practices suggest the use of multiple informants
think they will find desirable results) and confir- to gather information about behaviors across
mation bias (i.e., observers unintentionally inter- home, school, and community settings (Mikami,
pret new information to support pre-existing 2015). It is also important to note that adult
beliefs; Bordens & Horowitz, 2008). In addition, informants may be the most useful in the assess-
in research studies using observation systems, the ment of children, due to their often limited abil-
observations are often completed with a group of ity to accurately self-report, whereas self-report
children with ADHD, in a clinic, summer pro- measures are often more valuable when assess-
gram, or group treatment setting, thus preventing ing adolescents, who tend to spend more time
evaluation of social competence in natural envi- with peers and less time in the presence of
ronments, or with peers without ADHD. adults (Hunsley & Mash, 2007; Kramer et al.,
2004). In addition, adult informants are likely
unable to provide accurate information about
Informant peer regard, peer victimization, and social sta-
tus, so if these are the primary targets of an
Information regarding a child’s social behavior assessment, additional sources of information
can be obtained from a variety of respondents are necessary.
284 K.E. Tresco et al.

Self influenced by numerous variables, including


Self-report measures are particularly useful in the past experiences with the target child (Lauer &
assessment of social competence for a variety of Renk, 2013).
reasons. First, although behavior is relatively In summary, each data collection method and
easy to observe and therefore measure, it is more informant is associated with corresponding ben-
challenging to assess an individual’s cognitions efits that uniquely contribute to understanding
or self-perception (Hunsley & Mash, 2007). That social behavior; however, each option also has
said, younger children generally do not have the limitations. The strengths and weaknesses of
self-awareness and metacognitive skills neces- each method and informant vary depending on
sary to accurately self-report; these measures the nature of the social problem being assessed.
typically are most useful for the assessment of The inclusion of multiple methodologies and
adolescents’ social interactions. However, it must informants through the use of the multi-method,
be noted that even adolescents with ADHD strug- multi-informant model allows for the most com-
gle to accurately self-report their social compe- prehensive representation of the multifaceted
tence due to their tendency to overinflate their construct of social behavior in children and ado-
functioning (positive illusory bias). Although lescents with ADHD (Thomas et al., 2011;
children and adolescents with ADHD may be Winner, 2002).
able to self-report internalizing symptoms, evi-
dence suggests that many overestimate their
social competence and level of peer regard (Hoza Treatment of Social Problems
et al., 2005; Owens et al., 2007). Self-report,
however, might be a more accurate method when Despite being considered one of the core areas of
obtaining information regarding victimization by impairment for children and adolescents with
peers (Wiener & Mak, 2009). ADHD, intervention research related to social
functioning is relatively limited. Interventions for
Peer ADHD typically target symptom reduction, aca-
Peer informants are valuable sources of informa- demic functioning, and behavior at home and
tion because they spend a large amount of time school. If social impairment and functioning is
with other children and adolescents and are examined at all, it typically is included as an
familiar with the acceptable social norms in situ- ancillary part of the investigation. That is, social
ations involving other individuals who are simi- functioning may be measured but is generally not
lar in age (Newcomb, Bukowski, & Pattee, a direct target of intervention. Ideally, reductions
1993). It has been suggested that peer reports are in disruptive and intrusive behaviors common
particularly essential to increase the accuracy of among children with ADHD and improvements
the assessment of the social behavior of children in children’s ability to attend to their environment
and adolescents with ADHD (Mikami, 2015). translate to improvements in social behavior;
Given the positive illusory bias common among however, this does not appear to be the case (de
children and adolescents with ADHD, peer Boo & Prins, 2007; Hoza et al., 2005). Although
informants can provide valuable information on several investigations have established that
a variety of areas including: peer rejection, pro- behavior management strategies are effective in
social behavior, and friendships (see above sec- addressing disruptive and inattentive behavior of
tion on sociometric nominations). Research children with ADHD (e.g., Evans, Owens, &
supports the accuracy of peer informants; there- Bunford, 2014; Pelham & Fabiano, 2008), reduc-
fore, the inclusion of data collected from peers tion in social impairment and the development of
can enhance the comprehensiveness of an assess- quality friendships have rarely been included as a
ment (Renk & Phares, 2004). Nevertheless, sim- core component of intervention. Furthermore,
ilar to other indirect sources, evaluators need to findings from both behavioral and pharmacologi-
be sensitive to the fact that peer reports can be cal treatment research generally indicate that
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder 285

although parent and teacher ratings of social vided in vivo (i.e., in natural settings, such as
competence might improve as a result of inter- schools and peer group activities, during actual
vention (MTA, 1999), peer relationship deficits peer interactions) and using different formats to
continue, despite improvements in behavioral address challenges with generalization (Abikoff,
functioning (e.g., Hoza et al., 2005). 2009). Specifically, when social skills training
programs incorporate parent training to teach
parents strategies to reinforce the use of appropri-
Social Skills Training ate social behaviors during social interactions in
various settings, adult ratings of children’s social
The premise of most social skills training inter- skills show significant improvements as com-
ventions is remediation of social skills deficits. pared to skills training programs that only include
Appropriate social responses are typically taught the child as the target of intervention (Frankel,
through direct instruction and practice; however, Myatt, Cantwell, & Feinberg, 1997; Pfiffner &
most programs do not attempt to intervene McBurnett, 1997; Pfiffner et al., 2007).
directly with a child’s peers (Mikami, 2015). It has also been posited that problems with
Evidence supporting the use of this type of clinic-­ generalization of social skills could be due to
based direct socials skills training for children deficits inherent to ADHD (Abikoff, 2009).
with ADHD is limited. A number of intensive Children with ADHD have been shown to have
social skills training investigations, including difficulty modulating their social responses to
those utilizing randomized assignments and/or specific situations (Milich-Reich, Campbell,
no treatment control groups, have failed to dem- Pelham, Connelly, & Geva, 1999). This may be a
onstrate the effectiveness of social skills training result of difficulties with attention that negatively
in improving the adult- or peer-rated social per- impact the individual’s ability to select appropri-
formance of children with ADHD (see Evans ate social responses. Specifically, children with
et al., 2014 for review). ADHD have been found to utilize the most recent
The presumed utility of social skills training is social information in their interpretation of social
based on the underlying assumption that children situations rather than all relevant information
with ADHD lack the social skills necessary to from the entire interaction (Milich-Reich et al.,
respond appropriately in specific situations. 1999), indicating that these children likely are
Although social cognitive deficits have been doc- missing important information on which to base
umented to exist in children with ADHD, there is their response (McQuade & Hoza, 2015). In
little evidence that solely addressing these defi- addition, the positive illusory bias that is preva-
cits will improve the social functioning of chil- lent among youth with ADHD might result in
dren with ADHD (Mikami, 2015). Some children limited acknowledgment of their own social
with ADHD may exhibit deficits in their social competence deficits, which might interfere with
skill knowledge; however, it is likely their ability the success of skills-based interventions intended
to apply their social skills effectively and consis- to improve social competence (Mikami et al.,
tently that results in a mismatch between situa- 2010).
tion and response (de Boo & Prins, 2007). That Although direct skills training intervention
is, rather than deficits in social knowledge, chil- with children appears, on the surface, to be a log-
dren with ADHD appear to have difficulty gener- ical first step in improving peer relationships of
alizing their knowledge to everyday peer children with ADHD, there currently is little
interactions (Abikoff, 2009; Mikami, 2015). As empirical support for such interventions. Also, as
such, traditional social skills training generally noted above, peers’ perceptions of children with
does not translate to improved social compe- ADHD are resistant to change, even when the
tence. Instead of traditional social skills training, ­target child’s behavior improves after a course of
which is typically offered in analog situations, it behavioral or pharmacological intervention
has been recommended that treatment be pro- (Hoza, 2007; Mikami, Lerner, & Lun, 2010).
286 K.E. Tresco et al.

In addition, peers’ negative behavior directed playdates might foster improved peer
toward children with ADHD may further contrib- ­relationships (Hoza et al., 2003).
ute to the poor social behaviors exhibited by chil- In a more recent investigation of a friendship
dren with ADHD (Mikami, 2015). Therefore, it intervention for children with ADHD known as
is critical that interventions designed to improve parental friendship coaching (PFC), Mikami,
the social impairment of children with ADHD Lerner, Griggs, et al. (2010) taught parents of
include peers as a target of intervention. Given children with ADHD to create social opportuni-
the benefit of directly involving parents in sup- ties and to instruct their children on social skills
porting children’s appropriate use of social skills, before and during social interactions. Findings
and the difficulty in changing peers’ perceptions from this study indicated improved parent-rated
of children with ADHD, it would follow that social skills for children with ADHD who
interventions involving important adults in the received the intervention as compared to children
child’s life and directly targeting peers might be with ADHD assigned to a no treatment control
more effective than traditional social skills train- group. Teacher ratings did not improve; however,
ing targeting the child with ADHD alone. teachers did rate children receiving the interven-
tion as less disliked and more liked by peers.
Parents also reported less conflict and disengage-
Peer and Friendship Interventions ment from activities for children receiving the
intervention as compared to controls.
Little research has been conducted on interven- Observations of parental behaviors also indicated
tions specifically targeting the peer relationship an increase in parents’ facilitation of their chil-
problems of children and adolescents with dren’s appropriate behavior and a reduction in
ADHD. The inclusion of adults in improving the criticism during peer interactions. These results
peer relationships of children with ADHD, are intriguing; however, further investigation,
although in need of further study, may potentially including the addition of peer sociometric nomi-
be an effective intervention. Parent-mediated nations, is warranted. Given the reputation bias
interventions are promising; indeed, parental of children with ADHD, and the detrimental
behavior appears to impact children’s opportuni- effects of peer rejection and peer victimization,
ties for, and behavior during, social interactions. measures specifically addressing these constructs
A review of literature on parental influences on are vital to the evaluation of peer interventions
their children’s peer relationships found that par- for children with ADHD. Mikami and colleagues
ents of children with ADHD arranged for fewer currently are conducting a randomized clinical
playdates, were more critical of interactions with trial to further examine the PFC intervention and
peers, and had fewer prosocial skills themselves have included sociometric nomination as part of
(Mikami, Jack, Emeh, & Stephens, 2010). Parents their evaluation of peer relationships (Mikami,
have been utilized as intervention agents to 2015).
improve the friendships of children with ADHD, Preliminary investigations have also been con-
with findings indicating that the inclusion of par- ducted utilizing teachers to specifically address
ents in fostering relationships between children acceptance into the peer group. To this end,
with ADHD and peers may be a promising prac- Mikami et al. (2013) designed and implemented
tice (Frankel et al., 1997; Hoza, Mrug, Pelham, an intervention targeting teacher-student interac-
Greiner, & Gnagy, 2003; Mikami, Lerner, Griggs, tions, known as Making Socially Accepting
McGrat, & Calhoun, 2010). Previous investiga- Inclusive Classrooms (MOSAIC), compared to
tions have found that training parents to support traditional evidence-based contingency manage-
the generalization of social skills learned during ment training. In MOSAIC, the role of the teacher
child-focused social skills training results in is to interact positively with the children with
improvement in adult-rated social skills (Frankel ADHD in an effort to improve social reputation.
et al., 1997) and that increasing the frequency of In addition, teachers encourage inclusiveness of
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder 287

the peer group by not publically identifying those literature on peer interventions for adolescents
children who struggle to behave appropriately, with ADHD. Findings from one study evaluating
specifically targeting working together, and pub- a middle school intervention addressing social
licly valuing aspects of individual children unre- skills, without incorporating peers without
lated to their behavioral competence. Reprimands ADHD, for example, were not found to be effec-
and acknowledgment of behavioral accomplish- tive in improving adult-rated interpersonal skills
ments or deficits are offered privately so as not to (Evans, Langberg, Schultz, Vaugn, & Altaye,
create a public identity of behavior problems for 2015). Given the chronicity of social skills defi-
children with ADHD. Results from a 4-week cits and peer rejection among adolescents with
summer program evaluation, including children ADHD and the resulting deleterious outcomes,
with and without ADHD, were promising. including peer victimization, the development of
Although children with ADHD did not receive internalizing disorders, and risk for increased
more positive nominations from peers without substance use in adolescence, further investiga-
ADHD, children receiving the MOSAIC inter- tions are necessary.
vention were less likely to receive negative nomi-
nations, had more reciprocated friendships, and
received more positive comments from peers, as Conclusions
compared to children who received traditional
behavioral treatment. The group differences in Children with ADHD experience significant
socially oriented measures occurred despite the social impairment affecting relationships with
fact that there were no differences between peers and adults. Findings to date indicate that
groups on measures of disruptive behavior (i.e., children with ADHD are likely to experience
hyperactivity, inattention, oppositional behav- increased peer rejection and victimization, have
ior). In addition, as compared to peers without fewer friends, have less stable and shorter friend-
ADHD, findings were consistent with previous ships, have lower quality friendships, and are
studies indicating that children with ADHD are more likely to have friends who also have exter-
less likely than peers to have reciprocated friend- nalizing behavior problems than peers (e.g.,
ships; however, the group differences were Becker et al., 2016; Hoza, 2007; Normand et al.,
smaller after participation in MOSAIC (13% for 2011). Children with ADHD have also been
children without ADHD vs. 21% for children found to be more likely to engage in bullying and
with ADHD) as compared to participation in relationally aggressive behaviors (Ohan &
standard behavioral intervention (13–50%). Johnston, 2007; Wiener & Mak, 2009). These
Although the MOSAIC intervention (Mikami deficits in face-to-face social interactions are also
et al., 2013) appears to have influenced the peer likely to translate into challenges during online
acceptability of children with ADHD, the investi- interactions (McQuade & Hoza, 2015); however,
gation took place during a summer program, in a less is known about how social media use impacts
controlled environment, with small classroom the social relationships of children and adoles-
sizes, and novel peers without ADHD, and cents with ADHD.
included trained coaches to support teachers’ Social impairment is considerable for both
implementation of the intervention. It remains to boys and girls with ADHD; however, the social
be seen whether these outcomes could be repli- relationships of boys are more likely to include
cated during the school year, in a classroom with physical aggression, whereas the social relation-
peers already known to the child with ADHD, ships of girls are more likely to include relational
and with less consultative support from aggression (Wiener & Mak, 2009). In addition,
researchers. poor peer relationships among youth with ADHD
Although these programs are promising in may both be exacerbated by the presence of
addressing social difficulties of school-aged chil- comorbid externalizing and internalizing disor-
dren with ADHD, there is a considerable lack of ders and contribute to further emotional distress
288 K.E. Tresco et al.

and poor self-esteem (Becker et al., 2016; recent investigations have provided some
Fogelman et al., 2016). Children and adolescents ­promising preliminary outcomes and suggest that
with ADHD also tend to have challenging rela- including parents and teachers as intervention
tionships with adults, including parents and agents to directly address social skills application
teachers (Roberts et al., 2015). and peer regard can be an effective method by
With a few exceptions (e.g., Hoza et al., 2005; which to improve the peer relationships of
Mrug et al., 2012; Normand et al., 2013), much elementary-­aged children with ADHD (Mikami
of our knowledge about peer relationships and et al., 2010; 2013). Such interventions are, how-
social functioning of children with ADHD is ever, in early stages of development and evalua-
based on parent, teacher, and self-report. The lit- tion; randomized controlled trials and
erature suggests that self-report is more accurate implementation in natural environments under
than adult report of peer victimization; however, more realistic conditions are necessary.
it is important to consider self-report data in light
of the positive illusory bias common among
youth with ADHD that impacts their ability to Future Directions
accurately identify their level of social compe-
tence and the extent to which they are liked by There are four primary areas that should be of
peers (Hoza et al., 2012; Owens et al., 2007). interest to future investigations into the social
Direct report from peers of children with ADHD functioning of children and adolescents with
is likely the most accurate measure of peer-­ ADHD. First, although peer group status and
relationship information; however, these methods the presence of reciprocal friendships are asso-
inherently require access to classmates and other ciated with peer rejection and victimization, the
peers and as such are often more difficult to role of friendship quality and its risk and protec-
obtain. tive nature related to peer status and associated
Evidence-based practices for treating ADHD mental health concerns has been largely unex-
include psychosocial strategies and medication plored for children and adolescents with
management; however, these are typically tar- ADHD. Second, given the increase in social
geted at the challenging behavior associated with media use and online interactions among youth
ADHD and have been unsuccessful in improving with and without ADHD, the effects of social
social functioning (Hoza et al., 2005; 2012). media on peer relationships, social competence,
Historically, the intervention literature focused victimization, bullying, and adolescent risk tak-
on improving the social competence of children ing behavior are an area in need of further inves-
with ADHD has concentrated on social skills tigation. In addition, these factors and their
training; however, child-mediated, skills-focused relationships to mental health should also be
interventions have been largely unsuccessful at explored. Third, much of the research regarding
impacting the social competence and peer rela- social competence and ADHD has focused on
tionships of children with ADHD. Results have impairment and the teaching of social skills;
generally indicated that these interventions are additional research is necessary to evaluate
not sufficient to address children’s social prob- comprehensive peer and friendship interven-
lems, as these problems likely are not solely due tions, including regular classroom-­based inter-
to skills deficits but also to a difficulty in apply- ventions, interventions targeted at improving
ing social skills during actual interactions (de the ability of children with ADHD to generalize
Boo & Prins, 2007; Evans et al., 2014). their social skills to various situations, and
Interventions targeting improving the application interventions addressing poor peer regard and
of social skills, however, have also been shown to reputation bias. Finally, as is the case for inter-
be minimally effective likely because children vention research for adolescents with ADHD
with ADHD tend to quickly develop poor reputa- broadly, there is a significant need for the devel-
tions with peers that are difficult to alter. More opment and evaluation of interventions designed
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder 289

to improve the social functioning and peer rela- community sample. Journal of the American Academy
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 9(6), 595–605.
tionships of adolescents with ADHD.
doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2010.03.006
Cardoos, S. L., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2011). Friendship
as a protection from peer victimization for girls
References with and without ADHD. Journal of Abnormal
Child Psychology, 39, 1035–1045. doi:10.1007/
s10802-011-9517-3
Abikoff, H. (2009). ADHD psychosocial treat-
Coie, J. D., Dodge, K. A., & Coppotelli, H. (1982).
ments: Generalization reconsidered. Journal
Dimensions of social status: A cross-age perspective.
of Attention Disorders, 13, 207–210.
Developmental Psychology, 18, 557–570.
doi:10.1177/1087054709333385
de Boo, G. M., & Prins, P. J. M. (2007). Social incompe-
Aboujaoude, E., Savage, M. W., Starcevic, V., & Salame,
tence in children with ADHD: Moderators and media-
W. O. (2015). Cyberbullying: Review of an old prob-
tors in social skills training. Clinical Psychology,
lem gone viral. Journal of Adolescent Health, 57,
Review, 27, 78–97.
10–18. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.04.011
Eiraldi, R. B., Mautone, J. A., & Power, T. J. (2012).
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic
Strategies for implementing evidence-based psy-
and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.-text
chosocial interventions for children with attention-­
revision). Washington, DC: Author.
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child and Adolescent
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 21, 145–159.
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
Evans, S. E., Langberg, J. M., Schultz, B. K., Vaugn, A.,
Arlington, VA: Author.
& Altaye, M. (2015). Evaluation of a school-based
Becker, S. P., Fite, P. J., Luebbe, A. M., Stoppelbein, L.,
treatment program for young adolescents with ADHD.
& Greening, L. (2013). Friendship intimacy exchange
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology.
buffers the relation between ADHD symptoms and
doi:10.1037/ccp0000057
later social problems among children attending an
Evans, S. W., Owens, J. S., & Bunford, N. (2014). Evidence-
after-school care program. Journal of Psychopathology
based psychosocial treatments for children and ado-
and Behavioral Assessment, 35, 142–152.
lescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Becker, S. P., Langberg, J. M., Evans, S. W., Girio-Herrera,
Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,
E., & Vaughn, A. J. (2015). Differentiating anxiety
43, 527–551. doi:10.1080/15374416.2013.850700
and depression in relation to the social functioning of
Feldman, J. S., Tung, I., & Lee, S. S. (2016). Social
young adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Clinical
skills mediate the association of ADHD and depres-
Child & Adolescent Psychology, 44, 1015–1029.
sion in preadolescents. Journal of Psychopathology
Becker, S. P., Luebbe, A. M., & Langberg, J. M. (2012).
and Behavioral Assessment. doi:10.1007/
Co-occurring mental health problems and peer func-
s10862-016-9569-3
tioning among youth with attention-deficit/hyperac-
Flory, K., Molina, B. G., Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E., &
tivity disorder: a review and recommendations for
Smith, B. (2006). Childhood ADHD predicts risky sex-
future research. Clinical Child and Family Psychology
ual behavior in young adulthood. Journal of Clinical
Review, 15, 279–302. doi:10.1007/s10567-012-0122-y
Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35, 571–577.
Becker, S. P., Mehari, K. R., Langberg, J. M., & Evans,
Fogelman, N. D., Walerius, D. M., Rosen, P. J., &
S. W. (2016). Rates of peer victimization in young
Leaberry, K. D. (2016). Peer victimization linked to
adolescents with ADHD and associations with
negative affect in children with and without ADHD.
internalizing symptoms and self-esteem. European
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 46,
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. doi:10.1007/
1–10. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2016.05.003
s00787-016-0881-y
Frankel, F., Myatt, R., Cantwell, D. P., & Feinberg, D. T.
Booster, G. D., DuPaul, G. J., Eiraldi, R., & Power,
(1997). Parent-assisted transfer of children’s social
T. J. (2012). Functional impairments with
skills training: Effects on children with and with-
ADHD: Unique effects of age and comorbid sta-
out attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal
tus. Journal of Attention Disorders, 16, 179–189.
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
doi:10.1177/1087054710383239
Psychiatry, 36, 1056–1064.
Bordens, K. S., & Horowitz, I. A. (2008). Social psychol-
Greene, R. W., Beszterczey, S. K., Katzenstein, T., Park,
ogy (3rd ed.). Saint Paul, MN: Freeload Press.
K., & Goring, J. (2002). Are students with ADHD
Bowen, R., Chavira, D. A., Bailey, K., Stein, M. T., &
more stressful to teach? Patterns of teacher stress in an
Stein, M. B. (2008). Nature of anxiety comorbid
elementary school sample. Journal of Emotional and
with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in chil-
Behavioral Disorders, 10, 79–89. doi:10.1177/106342
dren from a pediatric primary care setting. Psychiatry
66020100020201
Research, 157, 201–209.
Gresham, F. M. & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social Skills
Bussing, R., Mason, D. M., Bell, L., Porter, P., & Garvan,
Rating System (SSRS). Circle Pines, MN: American
C. (2010). Adolescent outcomes of childhood
Guidance System.
attention-­ deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a diverse
290 K.E. Tresco et al.

Harter, S. (2012). Self-perception profile for children: Kowalski, R. M., & Fedina, C. (2011). Cyber bullying in
Manual and Questionnaires, Grades 3-8 (Revision of ADHD and Asperger syndrome populations. Research
the self-perception profile for children, 1985). Denver: in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 1201–1208.
University of Denver. Kramer, T. L., Phillips, S. D., Hargis, M. B., Miller, T. L.,
Heilbron, N., & Prinstein, M. J. (2008). A review and Burns, B. J., & Robbins, J. M. (2004). Disagreement
reconceptualization of social aggression: Adaptive between parent and adolescent reports of func-
and maladaptive correlates. Clinical Child and Family tional impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychology Review, 11, 176–217. Psychiatry, 45, 248–259.
Hodges, E. V. E., Boivin, M., Vitaro, F., & Bukowski, LaRue, R. H., Sloman, K. N., Dashow, E., & Isenhower,
W. M. (1999). The power of friendship: Protection R. W. (2015). Assessment and intervention for indi-
against and escalating cycle of peer victimization. viduals with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. In
Developmental Psychology, 35, 94–101. H. S. Roane, J. E. Ringdahl, & T. Falcomata (Eds.),
Hodgens, J., Cole, J., & Boldizar, J. (2000). Peer-based Clinical and organizational applications of applied
differences among boys with ADHD. Journal of behavior analysis (pp. 217–246). London, UK:
Clinical Child Psychology, 29, 443–452. American Press.
Hoza, B. (2007). Peer functioning in children with Lauer, B. M., & Renk, K. (2013). The peer informant:
ADHD. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 655– Characteristics related to the perceptions of peer behav-
663. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsm024 ior problems. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 22,
Hoza, B., Gerdes, A. C., Mrug, S., Hinshaw, S. P., 786–800. doi:10.1007/s10826-012-9633-x
Bukowski, W. M., Gold, J. A., … Wigal, T. (2005). Laursen, B., Bukowski, W. M., Aunola, K., & Nurmi,
Peer-assessed outcomes in the multi-modal treatment J.-E. (2007). Friendship moderates prospective asso-
study of children with attention deficit hyperactivity ciations between social isolation and adjustment prob-
disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent lems in young children. Child Development, 78(4),
Psychology, 34, 74–86. 1395–1404.
Hoza, B., Gerdes, A. C., Hinshaw, S. P., Arnold, L. E., Maccoby, E. (1992). The role of parents in the socializa-
Pelham, W. E., Molina, B. G., … Wigal, T. (2004). Self-­ tion of children: A historical overview. Developmental
perceptions of competence in children with ADHD Psychology, 28, 1006–1017.
and comparison children. Journal of Consulting and Marton, I., Wiener, J., Rogers, M., & Moore, C. (2015).
Clinical Psychology, 72, 382–391. Friendship characteristics of children with ADHD.
Hoza, B., Mrug, S., Gerdes, A. C., Hinshaw, S. P., Journal of Attention Disorders, 19, 872–881.
Bukowski, W. M., Gold, J. A., … Arnold, L. E. (2005). Marshal, M. P., & Molina, B. G. (2006). Antisocial behav-
What aspects of peer relationships are impaired in iors moderate the deviant peer pathway to substance
children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? use in children with ADHD. Journal of Clinical Child
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, and Adolescent Psychology, 35, 216–226.
411–423. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.411 Marshal, M. P., Molina, B. G., & Pelham, W. E. (2003).
Hoza, B., Mrug, S., Pelham, W. E., Greiner, A. R., & Childhood ADHD and adolescent substance use: An
Gnagy, E. M. (2003). A friendship intervention for examination of deviant peer group affiliation as a
children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: risk factor. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 17,
Preliminary findings. Journal of Attention Disorders, 293–302.
6, 87–98. Mash, E. J., & Hunsley, J. (2005). Developing guide-
Hoza, B., Vaughn, A., Waschbusch, D. A., Murray-Close, lines for the evident-based assessment of child and
D., & McCabe, G. (2012). Can children with ADHD adolescent disorders. Journal of Clinical Child and
be motivated to reduce bias in self-reports of compe- Adolescent Psychology, 34, 362–279.
tence? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, McQuade, J. D., & Hoza, B. (2015). Peer relationships
80, 245–254. of children with ADHD. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.),
Huang-Pollock, C. L., Mikami, A. Y., Pfiffner, L., & Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook
McBurnett, K. (2009). Can executive function explain for diagnosis and treatment (4th ed.pp. 211–222).
the relationship between attention deficit hyperactivity New York, NY: Guilford Press.
disorder and social adjustment? Journal of Abnormal McQuade, J. D., Murray-Close, D., Shoulberg, E. K.,
Child Psychology, 37, 679–691. doi:10.1007/ & Hoza, B. (2013). Working memory and social
s10802-009-9302-8 functioning in children. Journal of Experimental
Hunsley, J., & Mash, E. J. (2007). Evidence-based Child Psychology, 115, 442–435. ­ doi:10.1016/j.
assessment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, jecp.2013.03.002
3, 29–51. McQuade, J. D., Vaugh, A. J., Hoza, B., Murray-Close,
Johnston, C., & Chronis-Tuscano, A. (2015). Families D., Molina, B. S. G., Arnold, L. E., & Hechtman, L.
and ADHD. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention-deficit (2014). Perceived social acceptance and peer status
hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and differentially predict adjustment in youth with and
treatment (vol. 4th edition, pp. 191–209). New York, without ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 18,
NY: Guilford Press. 31–43. doi:10.1177/1087054712437582
Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder 291

Mikami, A. Y., Lerner, M. D., & Lun, J. (2010). Social Miller, M., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2010). Does childhood
context influences on children's rejection by their executive function predict adolescent functional out-
peers. Child Development Perspectives, 4, 123–130. comes in girls with ADHD? Journal of Abnormal
Mikami, A. Y. (2015). Social skills training for youth Child Psychology, 38, 315–326. doi:10.1007/
with ADHD. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention-deficit s10802-009-9369-2
hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis Molina, B. S., & Pelham, W. E. (2001). Substance
and treatment (4th ed.pp. 569–595). New York, NY: use, substance abuse, and LD among adoles-
Guilford Press. cents with a childhood history of ADHD. Journal
Mikami, A. Y., Calhoun, C. D., & Abikoff, H. B. of Learning Disabilities, 34(4), 333–342., 351.
(2010). Positive illusory bias and response to behav- doi:10.1177/002221940103400408
ioral treatment among children with attention-­ Mrug, S., Hoza, B., Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E. M., &
deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Greiner, A. R. (2007). Behavior and peer status in
Child and Adolescent Psychology, 39, 373–385. children with ADHD: Continuity and change. Journal
doi:10.1080/15374411003691735 of Attention Disorders, 10, 359–371.
Mikami, A. Y., Griggs, M. S., Lerner, M. D., Emeh, Mrug, S., Molina, B. S. G., Hoza, B., Gerdes, A. C.,
C. C., Reuland, M. M., Jack, A., & Anthony, M. R. Hinshaw, S. P., Hechtman, L., & Arnold, L. E.
(2013). A randomized trial of a classroom interven- (2012). Peer rejection and friendships in children with
tion to increase peers’ social inclusion of children attention-­deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Contributions
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal to long-term outcomes. Journal of Abnormal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 81, 100–112. Child Psychology, 40, 1013–1026. doi:10.1007/
doi:10.1037/a0029654 s10802-012-9610-2
Mikami, A. Y., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2006). Resilient ado- MTA Cooperative Group. (1999). A 14-month random-
lescent adjustment among girls: Buffers of childhood ized clinical trial of treatment strategies for attention-­
peer rejection and attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis- deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General
order. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, Psychiatry, 56, 1073–1086.
825–839. doi:10.1007/s10802-006-9062-7 Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993).
Mikami, A. Y., Huang-Pollock, C. L., Pfiffner, L. J., Children’s peer relations: A meta-analytic review of
McBurnett, K., & Hangai, D. (2007). Social skills popular, rejected, neglected, controversial, and aver-
differences among attention-deficit/hyperactiv- age sociometric status. Psychological Bulletin, 113,
ity disorder types in a chat room assessment task. 99–128.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 509–521. Normand, S., Schneider, B. H., Lee, M. D., Maisonneuve,
doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9108-5 M.-F., Chupelovska-Anastasova, A., Kuehn, S. M.,
Mikami, A. Y., Jack, A., Emeh, C. C., & Stephens, H. F. & Robaey, P. (2013). Continuities and changes in the
(2010). Parental influence on children with attention-­ friendships of children with and without ADHD: A
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: I. Relationships between longitudinal, observational study. Journal of Abnormal
parent behaviors and child peer status. Journal of Child Psychology, 41, 1161–1175. doi:10.1007/
Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 721–736. s10802-013-9753-9
Mikami, A. Y., Lerner, M. D., Griggs, M. S., McGrat, Normand, S., Schneider, B. H., Lee, M. D., Maisonneuve,
A., & Calhoun, C. D. (2010). Parental influence on M.-F., Kuehn, S. M., & Robaey, P. (2011). How do
children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: children with ADHD (mis)manage their real-life
II. Results of a pilot intervention training parents as dyadic friendships? A multimethod investigation.
friendship coaches for children. Journal of Abnormal Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 293–305.
Child Psychology, 38, 737–749. doi:10.1007/ doi:10.1007/s10802-010-9450-x
s10802-010-9403-4 Odom, S. L., McConnell, S. R., & Brown, W. H.
Mikami, A. Y., & Lorenzi, J. (2011). Gender and conduct (2008). Social competence of young children:
problems predict peer functioning among children Conceptualization, assessment, and influences. In
with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal W. H. Brown, S. L. Odom, & S. R. McConnell (Eds.),
of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, Social competence of young children: Risk, disabil-
777–786. doi:10.1080/15374416.2011.597089 ity, and intervention (pp. 3–30). Baltimore, MD: Paul
Mikami, A. Y., Ransone, M. L., & Calhoun, C. D. H. Brookes.
(2011). Influence of anxiety on the social func- Ohan, J. L., & Johnston, C. (2007). What is the social
tioning of children with and without ADHD. impact of ADHD in girls? A multi-method ­assessment.
Journal of Attention Disorders, 15, 473–484. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, 239–250.
doi:10.1177/1087054710369066 doi:10.1007/s10802-006-9076-1
Milich-Reich, S., Campbell, S. B., Pelham, W. E., Owens, J., Goldfine, M. E., Evangelista, N. M., Hoza,
Connelly, L., & Geva, D. (1999). Developmental and B., & Kaiser, N. M. (2007). A critical review of self-­
individual differences in children’s on-line represen- perceptions and the positive illusory bias in children
tations of dynamic social events. Child Development, with ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology
70, 413–431. Review, 10, 335–351.
292 K.E. Tresco et al.

Owens, J., & Hoza, B. (2003). The role of inattention Solanto, M. V., Pope-Boyd, S. A., Tryon, W. W., & Stepak,
and hyperactivity/impulsivity in positive illusory bias. B. (2009). Social functioning in predominantly inat-
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, tentive and combined subtypes of children with
680–691. ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 13, 27–35.
Pelham, W. E., & Bender, M. E. (1982). Peer relationships Spence, S.H. (1995). Social skills training enhanc-
in hyperactive children: Description and treatment. ing social competence with children and adoles-
Advances in Learning and Behavioral Disabilities, 1, cents. Research and Technical supplement Windsor,
365–436. Berkshire: Nfer-Nelson
Pelham, W. E., & Fabiano, G. A. (2008). Evidence-­ Taylor, L. A., Saylor, C., Twyman, K., & Macias, M.
based psychosocial treatments for attention-deficit/ (2010). Adding insult to injury: Bullying experi-
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and ences of youth with attention deficit hyperactiv-
Adolescent Psychology, 37, 184–214. ity disorder. Children’s Health Care, 39, 59–72.
Pelham, W. E., & Hoza, B. (1996). Intensive treat- doi:10.1080/02739610903455152
ment: A summer treatment program for children Thomas, L. B., Shapiro, E. S., DuPaul, G. J., Lutz, J. G.,
with ADHD. In E. Hibbs & P. S. Jensen (Eds.), & Kern, L. (2011). Predictors of social skills for pre-
Psychosocial treatments for child and adolescent dis- school children at risk for ADHD: The relationship
orders: Empirically based strategies for clinical prac- between direct and indirect measurements. Journal
tice (pp. 311–340). New York: APA Press. of Psychoeducational Assessment, 29(2), 114–124.
Pfiffner, L. J., & McBurnett, K. (1997). Social skills train- doi:10.1177/0734282910378478
ing with parent generalization: Treatment effects for Timler, G. R., & White, K. E. (2015). Social communi-
children with attention deficit disorder. Journal of cation assessment and intervention for children with
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 749–757. attention problems. In D. A. Hwa-Froelich (Ed.),
Pfiffner, L. J., Mikami, A. Y., Huang-Pollock, C., Easterlin, Social communication development and disorders
B., Zalecki, C., & McBurnett, K. (2007). A random- (pp. 252–286). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
ized, controlled trial of integrated home-school behav- Tseng, W.-L., & Gau, S. S.-F. (2013). Executive func-
ioral treatment for ADHD, predominantly inattentive tion as a mediator in the link between attention-­
type. Journal of the American Academy of Child and deficit/hyperactivity disorder and social problems.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 1041–1050. Journal of Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 996–1004.
Prinstein, M. J., Boergers, J., & Vernberg, E. M. (2001). doi:10.1111/jccp.12072
Overt and relational aggression in adolescents: Social-­ Walker, J. S., Coleman, D., Lee, J., Squire, P. N., &
psychological adjustment of aggressors and victims. Friesen, B. J. (2008). Children’s stigmatization of
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30, 479–491. childhood depression and ADHD: Magnitude and
Pliszka, S. P. (2015). Comorbid psychiatric disorders demographic variation in a national sample. Journal
in children with ADHD. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook Psychiatry, 47, 912–920.
for diagnosis and treatment (4th ed.pp. 140–159). Whalen, C. K., & Henker, B. (1985). The social worlds
New York, NY: Guilford Press. of hyperactive (ADDH) children. Clinical Psychology
Renk, K., & Phares, V. (2004). Cross-informant ratings Review, 5, 447–478.
of social competence in children and adolescents. Wiener, J., & Mak, M. (2009). Peer victimization in
Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 239–254. children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Roberts, W., Milich, R., & Barkley, R. A. (2015). Psychology in the Schools, 46, 116–131. doi:10.1002/
Primary symptoms, diagnostic criteria, subtyping, pits.20358
and prevalence of ADHD. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Winner, M. G. (2002). Assessment of social skills for stu-
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A hand- dents with Asperger syndrome and high-functioning
book for diagnosis and treatment (4th ed.pp. 51–80). autism. Assessment for Effective Intervention, 27(1),
New York, NY: Guilford Press. 73–80.
Roy, A., Hartman, C. A., Veenstra, R., & Oldehinkel, A. J. Winston, F. K., McDonald, C. C., & McGehee, D. V.
(2015). Peer dislike and victimization in pathways (2013). Are we doing enough to prevent the p­ erfect
from ADHD symptoms to depression. European Child storm? JAMA Pediatrics, 167(10), 892–894.
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 24, 887–895. doi:10.1007/ doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.2315
s00787-014-0633-9 Zalecki, C. A., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2004). Overt and
Scholte, R. H. J., Overbeek, G., Brink, G. T., Rommes, relational aggression in girls with attention deficit
E., deKemp, R. A. T., Goossens, L., & Engels, R. C. hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and
M. (2009). The significance of reciprocal and unilat- Adolescent Psychology, 33, 125–137.
eral friendships for peer victimization in adolescence. Zavadenko, N. N., Lebedeva, T. V., Schasnaya, O. V.,
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 89–100. Zavadenko, A. N., Zlobina, O. M., & Semenova, N. A.
Sibley, M. H., Evans, S. W., & Serpell, Z. N. (2010). Social (2011). Attention deficit hyperactivity syndrome: The
cognition and interpersonal impairment in young ado- role of parent and teacher questionnaires in assessing
lescents with ADHD. Journal of Psychopathology and the social and psychological adaptation of patients.
Behavioral Assessment, 32, 193–202. doi:10.1007/ Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, 41(1),
s10862-009-9152-2 52–56.
Anxiety Disorders

Thompson E. Davis III, Peter Castagna,


Georgia Shaheen, and Erin Tarcza Reuther

2008; Ollendick & H ­irshfeld-­Becker, 2002;


Introduction Rapee & Spence, 2004; Reijntjes, Kamphuis,
Prinzie, & Telch, 2010; Rubin, 2014; Spence,
According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Donovan, & Brechman-Toussaint, 1999). Children
Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM- with anxiety disorders can also be subjected to
5, American Psychiatric Association, 2013), there negative stigma (Jorm & Wright, 2008; Wright,
are nearly a dozen of anxiety disorders and pho- Jorm, & Mackinnon, 2011). Moreover, research
bias which can be diagnosed in children. Most of has shown that children with anxiety disorders
these disorders include a criterion requiring inter- also suffer from discrimination and victimization
ference in social and academic situations or, as is (e.g., Storch et al., 2006), which can cause
the case of agoraphobia, interference from embar- increased emotion dysregulation (McLaughlin,
rassment or the need of a companion (cf. DSM-5). Hatzenbuehler, & Hilt, 2009). Anxiety has also
As a diagnostic group, anxiety disorders are asso- been found to interact with social and communi-
ciated with social withdrawal, shyness, problem- cation skills in ways that impact other disorders
atic peer relations, parent-­ child interaction (e.g., autism spectrum disorder; Davis et al.,
difficulties, skills deficits, impairments in devel- 2012). As a result, it is surprising that the assess-
opmental and cognitive abilities, and cognitive ment and treatment of social skills deficits and
distortions (Davis, Hess, et al., 2011; Davis, social behavior difficulties in children with anxi-
Ollendick, & Nebel-Schwalm, 2008; Elizabeth ety disorders have received relatively little atten-
et al., 2006; Gallagher & Cartwright-Hatton, tion outside of social phobia. For example, a
decade-old review of evidence-­based treatments
The authors would like to thank Melissa Munson for her for child anxiety indicated social skills training
assistance on a prior edition of this chapter. The terms was included in less than 10% of treatment proto-
“child” and “children” are used throughout and usually cols—the least included component of the 18
meant to include adolescents.
treatment strategies selected for review (Chorpita
T.E. Davis III, Ph.D. (*) • P. Castagna • G. Shaheen & Southam-Gerow, 2006). Moreover, in a recent
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State review of the components of cognitive behavioral
University, 236 Audubon Hall, Baton Rouge,
therapy related to outcome in child anxiety disor-
LA 70803, USA
e-mail: ted@lsu.edu ders, social skills training was not included (Ale,
McCarthy, Rothschild, & Whiteside, 2015). Thus,
E.T. Reuther
LSU Health Science Center and the Children’s this ­chapter will examine the interplay of anxiety
Hospital of New Orleans, New Orleans, LA, USA disorders and social skills difficulties with a

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 293


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_16
294 T.E. Davis III et al.

p­ articular focus on social anxiety disorder (social 1998). Conceived of in this way, children having
phobia), given its relevance to the theme of this problems with social difficulties have a maladap-
volume and the pertinent research that has tive response pattern which likely incorporates
occurred in that area. Topics to be reviewed catastrophic thoughts about or misinterpretations
include the unique impact of social skills prob- of social situations, avoidance of social situations
lems on individuals with anxiety disorders, as or people, and panic-like physiological symp-
well as the assessment and treatment of social toms to social stimuli that are often interpreted as
skills deficits in anxiety-disordered children. problematic. These responses, and the prognosis
for therapeutic benefit, likely depend upon the
child’s unique presentation and the potentiation
Definition of the Population of the associative network (see sections below on
etiology and developmental psychopathology). A
A brief discussion of emotion and emotional child with anxiety and social difficulties, then, is
responding is essential to a thorough understand- a child with emotional difficulties rooted in myr-
ing of the interplay of anxiety and social behav- iad developmental, biological, environmental,
ior. Anxiety is an emotion composed of several and experiential factors. Further, this process is
constructs which are influenced by one’s memo- dynamic and reciprocal (Davis, 2009). For exam-
ries. In essence, when vast associative networks ple, a socially anxious child may display mal-
of information contained in long-term memory adaptive responses when entering a new
are stimulated, they cue an “action disposition” playgroup (e.g., thinking “other children won’t
or emotion (Lang, Cuthbert, & Bradley, 1998, like me,” acting behaviorally avoidant by hover-
p. 656; Salzman & Fusi, 2010). These networks ing awkwardly outside of the group—not engag-
are subdivided into associations between stimu- ing in conversations, and experiencing an elevated
lus, response, and meaning units of information heart rate that he or she interprets as scary). These
(Drobes & Lang, 1995; Foa & Kozak, 1998; emotional responses are then observed by his or
Lang et al., 1998). Essentially, emotion is a con- her peers, which often leads to neutral, negative,
solidation of properties which are based on sen- or even punitive responses. The peer group’s
sations associated with the stimulus, our potential responses are then taken in by the child and fur-
responses, and the meaning attributed to the stim- ther influence maladaptive emotional responding
ulus or situation which serves to further connect while potentially confirming distorted thinking
the stimulus and response units. Overall, these and expectations about social situations and so on
associative networks broadly guide our approach reciprocally (McLaughlin et al., 2009). In addi-
or avoidance of stimuli and situations based on tion, the child may be negatively reinforced for
the information activated. future avoidance as social withdrawal and shy-
Given this, anxiety and fear can be conceived ness may allow the child to avoid or reduce aver-
of as a neural pattern that facilitates emotional sive physiology and cognition, and the entire
responding or changes in physiology, behavior, experience may be associated with a sense of
and cognition (Foa & Kozak, 1986; Lang, 1979; helplessness and uncontrollability (Mineka &
for a review, see Shin & Liberzon, 2010). As a Zinbarg, 2006).
result, pathological fear or worry differs from the Children with anxiety and fear related to any
typical and more normative experiences by cue- of the DSM-5 anxiety diagnoses are likely to
ing a maladaptive network that incorporates experience social difficulties and peer rejection
exaggerated emotional responses—catastrophic (see Social Skills Problems Unique to the
cognitions and inaccurate views of the world, Population below). Most relevant and widely
unwarranted or excessive behavioral avoidance, studied, however, is the diagnosis of social anxi-
and physiologic discomfort (misinterpretation of ety disorder (also called social phobia). In
physiological sensations)—that is, problematic children, social phobia is characterized by a
­
and resistant to change (Foa & Kozak, 1986, marked and persistent fear of social performance
Anxiety Disorders 295

or evaluation when being observed by children selective mutism, can also cause an array of
and adults alike (i.e., not just fear with authority social problems (DSM-5). While it is recom-
figures or adults; DSM-5). Exposure to a feared mended for many of these disorders that one
situation typically provokes a maladaptive emo- should examine the use of a safety companion in
tional response similar to that described above diagnostic determinations, this is likely to be of
(Beidel & Turner, 2007). A physiological less value with children who frequently have par-
response, possibly even a panic attack, may be ents or caregivers nearby anyway.
present. Behaviorally, the child may withdraw,
cry, tantrum, or avoid social situations, or when
avoidance is not possible, the child may endure Etiology and Prevalence
exposure with significant discomfort (DSM-5).
Cognitively, anxious apprehension or distress Etiology
must interfere with the child’s functioning and
social relationships, and the child may believe the Anxiety disorders have generally been theorized
fearful response is warranted (i.e., no recognition to come about through four possible mechanisms,
that the emotional response is severe, excessive, usually in combination: a classical conditioning,
or unreasonable; DSM-5). For a child, the fear modeling, negative information transfer, and
must endure and be present for at least 6 months nonassociative mechanism (for more detailed
in an effort to avoid pathologizing developmen- reviews, see Fisak & Grills-Taquechel, 2007;
tally appropriate social fears. In addition, one can Mineka & Oehlberg, 2008; Muris, Merckelbach,
now specify if the fear is restricted to perform or de Jong, & Ollendick, 2002). Anxiety is thought
speak in public only by indicating “performance to be transmitted through associative means by
only” (e.g., public speaking; DSM-5). either experiencing a negative event directly, by
While social phobia may seem to be the most seeing someone else behave anxiously or afraid,
pertinent diagnostic consideration, social con- or by hearing or reading about being anxious or
cerns related to other anxiety disorders should be afraid (Mineka & Zinbarg, 2006). Nonassociative
examined as well (e.g., Schniering, Hudson, & considerations point to people having an innate,
Rapee, 2000). With separation anxiety disorder, a inborn biological (e.g., genetic) predisposition to
child is overly concerned about separation from a fear and anxiety (or potentially just not being
parent, a guardian, or the home and may experi- able to recall associative events, e.g., “I don’t
ence social disruption or embarrassment about know; I’ve always been afraid”). From these
leaving friends’ houses or needing close proxim- basic mechanisms, the etiology of anxiety has
ity to caregivers; such a child not only suffers been broadly understood to be a consideration of
from the symptoms of the disorder but also does how much association (i.e., classical condition-
not experience positive socialization experiences ing, modeling, and/or negative information trans-
(DSM-5). These symptoms may also hinder the fer) to a stimulus is necessary to bring about a
typical social developmental trajectory. In cases disorder given one’s innate predisposition
of generalized anxiety disorder, the child worries (Marks, 2002). Even so, the interaction of these
about performances, social situations, peer rela- four etiological mechanisms is poorly under-
tions, and possible embarrassment, even in the stood, and their description of coalescing into an
absence of evaluation (note: the absence of social anxiety disorder is greatly simplified.
evaluation is one factor that distinguishes it from A complete understanding of how anxiety dis-
social phobia; DSM-5). Similarly, with agorapho- orders emerge likely incorporates various etio-
bia and panic, the fear and worry over embarrass- logical processes that lead to one or more aspects
ment persist even into situations in which there is of the maladaptive emotional response, as
no evaluative component (DSM-5). As well, the opposed to the entire complex emotional
lack of social engagement and interaction from response. These etiological risks then might
the newest addition to the anxiety disorders, accumulate and interact over time. Such an
296 T.E. Davis III et al.

occurrence might be supported by notions of impressions of those around him or her. Further,
desynchrony in which only partial emotional a child’s behavior may or may not be maladap-
responses (e.g., low arousal but high distress) tive, but the caregivers’ accommodation of disor-
occur in response to an anxiety-provoking situa- der, intolerance, or misinterpretation of typical
tion or stimulus (Allen, Allen, Austin, Waldron, child behaviors may deny that a child needed
& Ollendick, 2015; Rachman & Hodgson, 1974). help or potentially contribute to the development
Even so, etiological models of anxiety need to of dysfunction (Thompson-Hollands, Kerns,
incorporate current progress in the understanding Pincus, & Comer, 2014).
of developmental psychology and developmental As a result, adequate etiological models of
psychopathology. It has long been observed that anxiety disorders increasingly need to incorpo-
many children have similar experiences but rate developmental milestones—in the case of
develop different psychological trajectories and this chapter, social development. Theorists have
that children with differing experiences can come attempted to do just that, and integrated etiologi-
to the same developmental trajectory (Cicchetti cal theories have emerged in which known mech-
& Rogosch, 1996). For example, two children anisms have been framed within a developmental
both have negative social experiences (e.g., get- psychology framework. For example, etiological
ting teased and bullied), yet only one develops an discussions of anxiety and social problems now
anxiety disorder or both develop different anxiety commonly include the topics of genetics, tem-
disorders. These observations represent multifi- perament, child-rearing and parenting, and nega-
nality (i.e., a single developmental event can have tive social experiences as well as a variety of
multiple outcomes) and equifinality (i.e., differ- other factors (Elizabeth et al., 2006; Ollendick &
ent developmental trajectories can lead to the Hirshfeld-Becker, 2002; Rapee, Schniering, &
same outcome; cf. Ollendick & Hirshfeld-Becker, Hudson, 2009; Rapee & Spence, 2004) and
2002). As a result, the goal of both research and increasingly lend themselves toward a discussion
clinical practice has been to personalize assess- of possible transdiagnostic constructs. Social
ment and treatment to better understand each anxiety has been conceptualized as a continuum
child as an individual with his or her unique pre- with a child’s risk for or resiliency to disorder
dispositions, learning histories, strengths, weak- being described as how developmental and envi-
nesses, coping abilities, etc. at a particular point ronmental factors move a child up or down the
in time (Davis, 2009). continuum from a certain initial innate set point
Developmental psychopathology seeks to (Rapee & Spence, 2004). Several of these influ-
understand the “developmental and psychologi- ential factors will be reviewed briefly below.
cal disturbances in children as the result of com- Genetics. Genetics has been tentatively linked
plex interactions over the course of development to a variety of social developmental aspects
between the biology of brain maturation and the including emotionality, sociability, and broad
multidimensional nature of experience” (Mash & internalizing tendencies (for a review, see
Dozois, 2003, p. 5). Increasingly, multifinality Gregory & Eley, 2007). In particular, genetic
and equifinality influence etiological models research has pointed to the role of both broad vul-
through providing information on developmental nerabilities (i.e., internalizing disorders) and spe-
trajectories along with the recognition that psy- cific vulnerabilities (e.g., social anxiety) but also
chopathology is influenced by both when a child does not discount the impact of environmental
is observed (e.g., developmental milestones influences (e.g., Ollendick & Hirshfeld-Becker,
negotiated, age, situation) and who is evaluating 2002; Rapee & Spence, 2004; Shimada-­
the child’s behavior (e.g., the observer’s perspec- Sugimoto, Otowa, & Hettema, 2015). For exam-
tive, orientation, and biases; Mash & Dozois, ple, social phobia has been found to be more
2003). Child psychopathology is then a multifac- common among first-degree relatives (e.g., Fyer,
eted construct which ties together environmental 1993; Fyer, Mannuzza, Chapman, Martin, &
factors, factors within the child, as well as the Klein, 1995). Moreover, heritability estimates of
Anxiety Disorders 297

approximately .48 have been found for broader ioral inhibition” (Costello, Egger, Copeland,
social anxiety constructs like the fear of negative Erkanli, & Angold, 2011). These children are
evaluation (Stein, Jang, & Livesley, 2002). thought to have a low threshold and tolerance for
Interestingly, there is evidence that the broader, arousal in novel, uncertain situations (Kagan,
more generalized type of social phobia may be Reznick, & Snidman, 1987). It may not be sur-
more heritable than the specific type (Mannuzza prising then that behaviorally inhibited children
et al., 1995; Stein, Chartier, Kozak, King, & are more susceptible to develop social phobia
Kennedy, 1998). Unfortunately, research in this (Biederman et al., 2001; Rapee et al., 2009).
area has been hampered by polygenetic influ- Parent-child interaction. Researchers to date
ences with limited impact, and in the end, the have failed to determine if socially anxious and
genetic component may be more useful for deter- inhibited children are the result of particular par-
mining psychopathological risk than response to enting styles or elicit those parenting behaviors
treatment (Gregory & Eley, 2007). Moreover, a (for a review, see McLeod, Wood, & Weisz,
recent genome-wide association study was not 2007; Ollendick & Hirshfeld-Becker, 2002;
able to find any associations, and attempts to rep- Rapee & Spence, 2004). It is likely a bidirec-
licate the top associations did not yield signifi- tional interaction of both possibilities, where a
cant results (Trzaskowski et al., 2013). In child and parent dyads impact each other recip-
addition, even genes must be understood as resid- rocally, with other variables such as tempera-
ing in cellular environments which can act to ment, environmental factors, and genetics having
switch them “on” or “off,” an area of investiga- long-­standing contributions as well. Even so, in
tion termed epigenetics (e.g., Szyf, McGowan, & examining non-retrospective studies, Wood,
Meaney, 2008). McLeod, Sigman, Hwang, and Chu (2003) found
Temperament. For decades children have been that anxious children had parents who were
understood to have different temperaments. A observed to be less accepting, more critical,
child’s temperament is understood to involve sev- overcontrolling, and overprotective. Moreover,
eral dimensions that include, but are not limited parents who are more likely to model anxiety
to, his or her emotionality, activity, and sociabil- tend to have children with increased anxiety;
ity (Buss & Plomin, 1984). For example, children however, such observations make it difficult to
classified into groups such as “easy” or “diffi- tease apart genetic contribution. Also, parents
cult” find that children receiving difficult classifi- are frequently influential in arranging and super-
cations (e.g., children with poor adaptability, vising play, with socially phobic parents poten-
withdrawal from novelty, intense reactivity) are tially less proficient at these tasks themselves
more strongly associated with anxiety and behav- (Masia & Morris, 1998; Ollendick & Hirshfeld-
ior problems (Thomas & Chess, 1977; Thomas, Becker, 2002). Parents of anxious children have
Chess, & Birch, 1968). In contrast, children clas- been observed to interact with their children in
sified as “easy” are less likely to be associated ways that present the world as hostile, danger-
with emotional dysregulation later in life. ous, and anxiety provoking while also demand-
Subsequent refinements have found the related ing compliance and being less accepting and
concept of behavioral inhibition to be a particu- more critical for deviation from their directives
larly important temperamental construct, and risk (Ollendick & Benoit, 2012). Moreover, these
factor, for social anxiety and social withdrawal. parents often provide maladaptive social model-
Behavioral inhibition refers to a relatively stable ing as well as an inherited genetic component.
pattern of behavioral and emotional responses in Such interactions may be most impairing as risk
which a child is tentative, shy, and withdrawn in factors for younger children who have other dia-
strange or novel situations (Kagan, Reznick, theses (Ollendick & Horsch, 2007); as a result,
Clarke, Snidman, & Garcia Coll, 1984; Rapee & there is likely an unfortunate interaction of mul-
Spence, 2004). During early childhood, approxi- tiple familial ­factors that increase the likelihood
mately 15% of young children exhibit “behav- of a child developing social anxiety (e.g., inher-
298 T.E. Davis III et al.

ited traits which affect parents and children develop social anxiety disorder, with a lifetime
alike, parenting styles, modeling, socioeconomic prevalence of 28% versus 14% (Hirshfeld-Becker
status, etc.). et al., 2007). Age of onset is usually in preadoles-
cence to adolescence with more generalized
social worries beginning earlier (Ollendick &
Prevalence Hirshfeld-Becker, 2002; Rapee & Spence, 2004).
Also suggesting a later onset, a review of preado-
Prevalence estimates of anxiety disorders in chil- lescent children (defined to be under 12 years of
dren have considerable variability, ranging from age) has found prevalence rates to be less than
roughly 3 to 32% of children depending on the 1% (Cartwright-Hatton et al., 2006), similar to
disorders included, sample, methodology, and the slightly younger-aged sample in Costello
time period (Cartwright-Hatton, McNicol, & et al. (2003).
Doubleday, 2006; Merikangas et al., 2010).
According to one group of researchers, 36.7% of
children will meet the criteria for at least one  ocial Skills Problems Unique
S
DSM-IV disorder, and 10% will have an anxiety to the Population
disorder by 16 years of age, with the 3-month
prevalence of anxiety disorders being 2.4% As alluded to above, children with anxiety disor-
(Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, & Angold, ders and phobias experience a variety of social
2003). Also, in 22.8% of cases, children report- problems that interact bidirectionally with psy-
ing “childhood fears” had a diagnosable anxiety chopathology, further complicating both.
disorder (Muris, Merkelbach, Mayer, & Prins, Moreover, a threat-related attentional bias may
2000). High comorbidity with other disorders exacerbate avoidance and the perception of situa-
(including anxiety disorders) also has been con- tions as potentially anxiety provoking (Puliafico
sistently found (e.g., 41% comorbid, Beidel, & Kendall, 2006). Childhood anxiety has been
Turner, & Morris, 2000; 54% comorbid, Spence, associated with numerous risks including social
Donovan, & Brechman-Toussaint, 2000; 72% withdrawal, social skills deficits, peer rejection
comorbid, Silverman et al., 1999). Research on and neglect, dysfunctional parent-child interac-
the prevalence rates of anxiety disorders with the tions, maladaptive social strategies, and cognitive
new edition of the DSM has yet to be published. distortions (e.g., Elizabeth et al., 2006; Ollendick
Relevant to this chapter, the 3-month preva- & Hirshfeld-Becker, 2002; Rapee & Spence,
lence of social anxiety disorder in the general 2004; Sondaite & Zukauskiene, 2005; Spence
population has been found to be 0.5% for a et al., 1999; Strauss, Lease, Kazdin, Dulcan, &
younger sample of children and adolescents Last, 1989). Moreover, an attention bias toward
(Costello et al., 2003; 9–13 years initially), while threat is linked to behavioral inhibition in young
1-year prevalence estimates of child and adoles- children, which is more likely to lead to social
cent social phobia have been suggested to be withdrawal over time (Pérez-Edgar et al., 2011).
6.8% in primary care facilities (Chavira, Stein, As a result, anxious children have been some-
Bailey, & Stein, 2004). Similarly, the adult 1-year what consistently described as being socially
prevalence in the population has been found to be maladjusted by parents, teachers, peers, and even
6.8–7.4%, the second most prevalent mental dis- the children themselves (e.g., Strauss et al., 1989;
order (Kessler, Chiu, Demler, & Walters, 2005; Verduin & Kendall, 2008). These factors may be
Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky, & placed them upon a developmental trajectory
Wittchen, 2012). Overall, lifetime prevalence toward further social withdrawal and dysfunction
rates for children and adolescents have been sug- (Oh et al., 2008). Unfortunately, social anxiety
gested to vary between 5 and 15% (Heimberg, has been described as moderately stable across
Stein, Hiripi, & Kessler, 2000). Behaviorally the life-span (Chronis-Tuscano et al., 2009;
inhibited preschoolers are twice as likely to Rapee & Spence, 2004).
Anxiety Disorders 299

The influence of peers and their relationships disordered, conduct-disordered, and non-referred
have been one of the larger areas investigated by control children. Broadly, they found that chil-
researchers. Shy children have been suggested to dren with anxiety and conduct disorder were
be as likely to have friends as other children; similarly disliked compared to a group of typi-
however, there are important differences in these cally functioning peers. Children with anxiety
relationships (Rubin, Wojslawowicz, Rose-­ disorders were also more likely to be classified as
Krasnor, Booth-LaForce, & Burgess, 2006). socially neglected (i.e., few “like most” or “like
Examinations of adolescents with social anxiety least” nominations; Strauss et al., 1988).
have suggested friendships may be impacted— Additionally, Strauss et al. (1989) found that,
particularly for girls (Erath, Flanagan, & compared to typically developing peers, anxious
Bierman, 2007; La Greca & Lopez, 1998). children reported more loneliness and less social
According to Rubin et al. (2006), withdrawn chil- competence. Parents and teachers reported that
dren’s friends were more likely to be withdrawn the anxious children were more withdrawn, mal-
and victimized themselves by peers, and the adjusted, socially deficient, and lacking in social
quality of these friendships was poorer than con- skills (Strauss et al., 1989).
trol children’s relationships. La Greca and Lopez Examining the differences between two
(1998) found that socially anxious adolescent groups of anxious children, Ginsburg, La Greca,
boys and girls reported feeling less supported, and Silverman (1998) found that children diag-
accepted by, and attractive to peers; girls espe- nosed with an anxiety disorder with high social
cially were found to report fewer friendships and anxiety, compared to anxious children with low
less intimacy and support in existing relation- social anxiety, had more negative peer interac-
ships. Moreover, social anxiety and loneliness tions and lower self-esteem and social accep-
have been found to impact online communication tance. Children diagnosed with social phobia in
(Bonetti, Campbell, & Gilmore, 2010). Generally, particular have also been found to have deficits in
it has been surmised that social anxiety in chil- their social skills and social competence and to
dren is associated with long-standing social and engage in a more negative self-talk in socially
peer problems which lead children to experience evaluative situations when compared to a matched
peer rejection, neglect, and exclusion (Rapee & group of typically developing children (Spence
Spence, 2004). Similarly, Rudy, May, Matthews, et al., 1999). Also, direct observations of children
and Davis (2013) found through structural equa- interacting with peers at school have determined
tion modeling that the frequency of youth nega- that children with social phobia experience simi-
tive self-statements was associated with less lar percentages of interaction that were negative
social self-efficacy. The expected relationship or in which the child was ignored but fewer peer
was also found: more frequent negative self-­ social interactions with positive outcomes
statements were associated with less social self-­ (Spence et al., 1999).
efficacy. However, this relationship was only held Verduin and Kendall (2008) examined chil-
for the youth’s sense of social self-efficacy with dren’s ratings of videotaped anxious and typi-
peers and adults, not strangers. cally developing children. Child raters were able
Examinations of social functioning, relation- to perceive anxiety in videotaped children, as
ships, and competence in children with other indicated by positive correlations between the
anxiety disorder diagnoses are more limited, but raters’ ratings of anxiety in the videotaped chil-
findings have been similar to those obtained with dren and the videotaped children’s ratings of
socially anxious children. For example, Strauss, themselves. The effect was stronger for ratings of
Lahey, Frick, Frame, and Hynd (1988) used a children diagnosed with an anxiety disorder than
peer nomination procedure (i.e., children wrote those children without an anxiety disorder.
down the names of the three children they liked Children rated anxious children as significantly
most and the three children they liked least) to less likeable; however, further analyses indicated
determine peer social status among anxiety-­ that these differences were “wholly attributable
300 T.E. Davis III et al.

to the presence of” social phobia and not diagno- tigation found that relational victimization pre-
ses of generalized anxiety disorder or separation dicted social phobia symptoms appearing at
anxiety disorder (p. 465). Socially anxious chil- 1 year, but not the reverse, and also did not pre-
dren were even rated less likeable when the peer-­ dict more general symptoms of social anxiety
raters’ ratings of anxiety were controlled—in (Storch et al. 2005). These results suggest a uni-
other words, socially phobic children were dis- directional influence of relational aggression;
liked even if they were not perceived to be anx- however, it may be that socially anxious children
ious (Verduin & Kendall, 2008). As a result, it are already avoidant and excluded to the extent
would seem that the combination of anxiety and that little more relational aggression can occur. In
social problems causes peer difficulties for chil- contrast, Siegel, La Greca, and Harrison (2009)
dren independent of the overt presentation of found peer victimization was both a predictor and
anxious symptomatology. consequence of social anxiety over time using a
There is evidence that anxious children’s 2-month prospective research design. It is clear
problems with peer relations may even be stig- that more research is needed to better understand
matizing and associated with victimization by the interplay between social anxiety and peer vic-
peers. Jorm and Wright (2008) surveyed 3746 timization. Storch et al. (2006) found that chil-
children, adolescents, and young adults as well as dren with obsessive-compulsive disorder were
2005 parents in Australia by phone. Participants victimized more than controlled children or even
were interviewed after being read several the children with diabetes who were included.
vignettes of hypothetical clinically diagnosed Victimization was associated with a number of
15-year-olds—one of which described the hypo- factors including depression and loneliness and
thetical teen as having symptoms of social pho- has fully or partially mediated the effects between
bia. Youth’s ratings of the teen with social phobia obsessive-compulsive disorder severity and
were associated with higher scores on scales stig- depression, externalizing behaviors, and loneli-
matizing the teen as “weak not sick” and “stigma ness. Boys with comorbid social anxiety and
perceived in others” indicating perceptions that depression have been shown to have the highest
the hypothetical teen was weak-minded, stigma- rates of both overt and covert victimization,
tized, and to be avoided (Jorm & Wright, 2008). whereas girls had the highest rate of covert vic-
Moreover, youth “weak not sick” beliefs were timization (Ranta, Kaltiala-Heino, Pelkonen, &
associated with parents’ increased “weak not Marttunen, 2009). Taken together, it may be that
sick” beliefs and decreased “stigma perceived in socially anxious and awkward children are iden-
others” beliefs. Overall, the authors concluded tified, disliked, and targeted for victimization by
that “social phobia was more likely to be seen as peers, even before they show significant overt
a weakness rather than a sickness and was per- symptoms of anxiety (cf. Storch et al., 2005;
ceived as being more stigmatised [sic] by others Verduin & Kendall, 2008), and it is possible that
in society” (p. 147). this victimization leads to an exacerbation of
Peer victimization is also a problem for chil- social anxiety symptomatology (Siegel et al.,
dren with anxiety and anxiety-related disorders 2009). Moreover, the social and anxiety prob-
(e.g., obsessive-compulsive disorder). For exam- lems these children experience may be viewed by
ple, Storch, Masia-Warner, Crisp, and Klein peers and adults as abnormal and indicative of
(2005) and Storch et al. (2006) examined the vic- weakness instead of as symptoms of a treatable
timization of adolescents with social anxiety and psychiatric condition (cf. Jorm & Wright, 2008;
children and adolescents with obsessive-­ Storch et al., 2006).
compulsive disorder. Victimization was defined From this review, it is apparent that children
as both overt (e.g., hitting, yelling) and relational with comorbid anxiety and social problems face a
(e.g., spreading rumors and gossip, using rela- difficult and multifaceted developmental
tionships to isolate individuals) peer aggression ­ trajectory. Children experiencing loneliness, a
(Storch et al., 2005, 2006). A longitudinal inves- lack of friendship or stability in friendships, and
Anxiety Disorders 301

peer exclusion have been found to be on a trajec- Assessment


tory of increasing social withdrawal, as well as
other mood disorders, across the preadolescent to Anxiety disorders do not generally seem to
early adolescent years (Chronis-Tuscano et al., improve over time (Beidel, Fink, & Turner,
2009; Oh et al., 2008). Moreover, in significant 1996), and they often lead to long-term problems
percentages of adolescents, avoidant and helpless for children having moderate to high detrimental
social strategies have been observed which may impacts on their functioning (Demyttenaere
serve to maintain social problems and anxiety et al., 2004; Kendall, Safford, Flannery-­
(Gazelle, Workman, & Allan, 2010; Sondaite & Schroeder, & Webb, 2004). As a result, proper
Zukauskiene, 2005). Even so, the principles of diagnosis is essential to ensure that the correct
developmental psychopathology need to be taken treatments are initiated and appropriate supple-
into consideration, and trajectories should not be ments are included as soon as possible. To this
viewed as absolute or even themselves single fac- end, accurate assessment using evidence-based
eted. For example, high levels of familial stress measures is crucial to ensure that the correct
have been associated with shyness, anxiety, and diagnosis is made (Silverman & Ollendick,
social skills deficits in urban youth; however, 2005). Researchers have made a great deal of
additional factors such as parental warmth and progress in the development and validation of
strong familial support have been suggested to be evidence-based measures of anxiety (see
protective factors even in families experiencing Silverman & Ollendick, 2005 for a more com-
high stress (for a review on resiliency factors, see plete review), and the current best-practice strat-
Benzies & Mychasiuk, 2009; McCabe, Clark, & egy for assessment is a multi-method,
Barnett, 1999). Overall, though, children with multi-informant approach to provide the most
anxiety are subjected to many socially related comprehensive diagnostic picture possible. This
difficulties (i.e., peer issues, stigma, parent-child type of evaluation also ensures that important
interaction factors, deficits/distortions, and areas of emotional functioning or differing physi-
more), and these problems extend beyond anxi- cal environments are not overlooked (see De Los
ety symptoms and varied diagnostic criteria. In Reyes, 2011 for a review; Achenbach,
addition, more research examining the extent to McConaughy, & Howell, 1987).
which social skills difficulties represent actual Even so, the lingering issue of multi-­informant
social skills deficits and production deficits (the agreement, and more so disagreement, is a com-
child has the skill but does not implement it), or plex problem for child practitioners (Davis,
the interaction of the two, would provide a much 2009). There has commonly been a problem of
needed clarification about the underlying psycho- multi-informant disagreement as to the presence
pathology as well as possible avenues for inter- and severity of child anxiety disorders (e.g.,
vention. As a result, a complex multi-method, Brown-Jacobsen, Wallace, & Whiteside, 2011;
multi-informant, evidence-based assessment of Grills & Ollendick, 2003; Jenson et al., 1999;
both anxiety and comorbid disorders, as well as Silverman & Ollendick, 2005). While discussion
peer relations and social skills, is important when of this issue is beyond the space allotted, it is
working with anxious children, especially when important to note that disagreements should be
determining the best evidence-based treatment taken seriously and fully considered. For exam-
approach (for a review, see Silverman & Hinshaw, ple, Muris and Merkelbach (2000) found that
2008; Davis, May, & Whiting, 2011; Silverman almost 20% of children with parent-reported
& Ollendick, 2005). Such an assessment needs to “childhood fears” met the full criteria for specific
examine both the child’s symptomatology and phobia, while 23% of children reporting their
the possible presence of social skills deficits own “childhood fears” met the criteria for an
(both the lack of a particular skill and the possi- anxiety disorder (Muris et al., 2000). Moreover,
bility of just a lack of implementation). DiBartolo and Grills (2006) had children, ­parents,
302 T.E. Davis III et al.

and teachers completed measures of social anxi- address many of the challenges posed by clinical
ety in an effort to predict children’s anxiety dur- interviews (i.e., asking a set of scripted questions
ing a social evaluation task. Results indicated in the same way, in the same order to each indi-
poor agreement across informants; only chil- vidual as opposed to open, unscripted, free-­
dren’s report predicted their own anxious feel- flowing interviews). Several of these commonly
ings on a social evaluation task. Further, used interviews are detailed in Table 1. The
verification of diagnostic information from par- Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for
ents and children by trained clinicians has indi- DSM-IV: Child and Parent Versions (ADIS: C/P;
cated that children were accurate in reporting Silverman & Albano, 1996) is generally consid-
anxiety disorders, while their parents did not and ered the most popular by anxiety researchers
vice versa in 59% and 65% of cases, respectively (Silverman & Ollendick, 2005); however, at the
(Jenson et al., 1999). As a result, careful attention time of this chapter, the updated edition for DSM-­
should be paid to the discrepant information, and 5 is not widely available. The ADIS contains
a thorough assessment is strongly separate modules for each of the anxiety disor-
recommended. ders (e.g., separation anxiety disorder, specific
Common methods of assessment in anxiety phobia, generalized anxiety disorder, social anxi-
include structured and semi-structured inter- ety disorder) and other common psychological
views, self-reports, parent and other reports, and disorders and problems in youth (e.g., attention
analogue behavior observation methods (ABO; deficit hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defi-
for a review of ABO methods, see Mori & ant disorder, major depressive disorder). Also
Armendariz, 2001). Assessments should also be included are a number of abbreviated screening
constructed to probe the different components of modules for other problems (e.g., eating disor-
the anxiety response (i.e., physiology, behavior, ders, pervasive developmental disorders, schizo-
and cognition; Davis, 2009; Davis, May, et al., phrenia, enuresis). The ADIS offers a great deal
2011; Davis & Ollendick, 2005). The following of flexibility in its administration as the entire
brief review will include information on several interview can be given or selected modules may
commonly used measures from each of these cat- be given to probe specific disorders only. The
egories. While several of the measures discussed questions in each module closely model the
include measurement of social functioning, few DSM-IV criteria, and most of the modules have
actually measure social skills. Therefore, a sepa- the child rate fear and avoidance for various situ-
rate discussion of measures of social skills com- ations that are commonly problematic on a scale
monly used in anxiety is also included. More from 0 (no problems or fear) to 8 (very severe or
information on the assessment of social skills is disturbing). Youth also rates the overall interfer-
also included in other chapters in this volume. ence that each disorder is causing using a 0–8
scale. A visual fear thermometer with numeric
and qualitative descriptors helps younger chil-
Structured Diagnostic Interviews dren grasp the scale and allows for developmen-
tally sensitive responses. At the end of the
The most widely employed assessment method is interview, the clinician also assigns clinical
likely the open, clinical interview (Lyneham, severity ratings on the 0 (none) to 8 (very severely
Abbott, & Rapee, 2007; Ollendick & Hersen, disturbing/impairing) scale to each of the disor-
1993; Silverman, 1994; Silverman & Ollendick, ders that were endorsed based on the information
2005); however, a variety of problems and limita- provided by the informant, with clinical signifi-
tions are associated with their use (e.g., reliabil- cance indicated by scores of 4 or higher.
ity, validity, diagnostic specificity, and While the ADIS does not include a scored
comprehensiveness; cf. Brown-Jacobsen et al., assessment of social skills per se, there are a
2011). Hence, several structured and semi-­ number of portions that assess similar and/or rel-
structured interviews have been developed to evant content. For example, there is a screener
Anxiety Disorders 303

Table 1  Diagnostic interviews


Instrument Description Psychometric properties
Anxiety Disorders Interview A semi-structured clinical interview Kappa coefficients for the anxiety
Schedule – Child/Parent Schedules designed for use with children ages disorders from parent and child
(ADIS-C/P; Silverman & Albano, 6 to 18 years, used to diagnose a combined assessment are as
1996; Silverman, Saavedra, & Pina, range of internalizing and follows: GAD = 0.80, SAD = 0.84,
2001) externalizing disorders SOP = 0.92, SP = 0.81. Kappa
coefficients from the mood
disorders and externalizing
disorders range from 0.62 to 1.00
NIMH Diagnostic Interview A structured clinical interview Kappa coefficients for the anxiety
Schedule for Children Version IV designed for use with children ages disorders from parent and child
(NIMH DISC-IV; Shaffer, Fisher, 9–17 years, used to diagnose a combined assessment are as
Lucas, Dulcan, & Schwab-Stone, range of internalizing and follows: GAD = 0.58, SAD = 0.51,
2000) externalizing disorders SOP = 0.48, SP = 0.86. Kappa
coefficients from the mood
disorders and externalizing
disorders range from 0.55 to 0.86
Schedule for Affective Disorders and A semi-structured clinical interview Kappa coefficients range from 0.55
Schizophrenia for School-Age designed for use with children ages to 0.80 for specific anxiety
Children (K-SADS; Ambrosini, 6–18 years, used to diagnose a disorders from parent and child
2000; Current Version: K-SADS-PL range of internalizing and combined assessment (Ambrosini,
2009 Working Draft; Axelson, externalizing disorders 2000)
Birmaher, Zelazny, Kaufman, & Gill,
2009)
Note: GAD generalized anxiety disorder, SAD separation anxiety disorder, SOP social phobia, SP specific phobia

that inquires of the extent and quality of the to aid in the diagnosis of anxiety disorders and
interpersonal relationships of the child. This sec- social skills/abilities. These instruments are
tion includes questions such as “Compared to often collected through a multi-informant
other kids, do you feel you have more friends, approach, typically including parents and
less friends, or about the same,” “Do you have a teachers along with the child’s self-report.
best friend,” and “Given the choice, would you Many of the most frequently used question-
prefer to spend most of your time alone or with naires are presented in Table 2. Questionnaires
other kids?” There are also questions in the have several advantages over interviews includ-
social phobia and school refusal modules that ing being easier and cheaper to administer and
ask the child to rate fear and avoidance of spe- providing the opportunity to collect more infor-
cific social situations, including starting or join- mation from multiple informants in an efficient
ing in on a conversation, working or playing in a manner. The speed and efficiency of question-
group, and having difficulties with assertiveness. naires make them valuable as screening tools
It is unclear, however, whether these problems and can add to the cost-­effectiveness of services
are due to a lack of skill or just a general fear of (Silverman & Ollendick, 2005). A multi-infor-
negative evaluation. mant approach whereby a clinician includes
parents and teachers may be particularly impor-
tant for very young children or children with
 elf-Report and Other Report
S social skills deficits—these children may be
Questionnaires unable to fully express their ­symptoms or con-
cerns, and so other informants may help com-
Parent-, teacher-, and self-report questionnaires plete a profile of the child’s symptoms
offer another expedient method of assessment (Choudhury, Pimentel, & Kendall, 2003).
304 T.E. Davis III et al.

Table 2  Self-, parent-, and other report questionnaires for anxiety assessment
Measure Description Subscales Psychometric properties
Behavior Assessment A 175-item parent Clinical scales (aggression, Internal consistency ranges
System for Children, 3rd report measure of anxiety, attention problems, from 0.84 to 0.89
ed. (BASC-3; Reynolds adaptive functioning atypicality, conduct problems,
& Kamphaus, 2015) and behavior depression, hyperactivity,
problems in children learning problems,
aged 2–21 years somatization, and withdrawal),
adaptive scales (activities of
daily living, adaptability,
functional communication,
leadership, social skills, and
study skills), and content scales
(anger control, bullying,
developmental social disorders,
emotional self-control,
executive functioning, negative
emotionality, and resiliency)
Child Anxiety Sensitivity An 18-item Disease concerns, unsteady Internal consistency for the
Index (CASI; Silverman, questionnaire for concerns, mental incapacitation total score = 0.87. Test-­
Fleisig, Rabian, & children aged 6–17 concerns, and social concerns retest reliability for the total
Peterson, 1991) that has the child rate score = 0.76
how disturbing
various anxiety
symptoms are to
them
Child Behavior Checklist A 120-item measure Two broad scales (internalizing Internal consistency for the
(CBCL; Achenbach & that asks parents to and externalizing problems) subscales ranges from 0.78
Rescorla, 2001) rate the frequency of and eight subscales to 0.97. Test-retest
various problem (withdrawn/depressed, somatic reliability ranges from 0.82
behaviors that their complaints, anxious/depressed, to 0.92
child (age 6–18) may social problems, thought
experience problems, attention problems,
rule-breaking behavior, and
aggressive behavior)
Children’s Automatic A 40-item Internal consistency = 0.94.
Thoughts Scale (CATS; questionnaire for Test-retest reliability = 0.79
Schniering & Rapee, children aged 7–16
2002) that asks the child to
rate the frequency of
each automatic
thought about
physical threat,
personal failure, and
hostility in the last
week
Fear Survey Schedule for An 80-item measure Fear of failure and criticism, Internal consistency for the
Children – Revised for children aged fear of the unknown, fear of subscales ranges from 0.92
(FSS-R; Ollendick, 1983) 7–18 that has the danger and death, medical to 0.95. Test-retest
child stating the fears, and small animals reliability for the total
amount of fear they scale = 0.82. Has been
experience for each shown to be able to
object or situation discriminate between the
different types of phobias
(Weems, Silverman,
Saavedra, Pina, & Lumpkin,
1999)
(continued)
Anxiety Disorders 305

Table 2 (continued)
Measure Description Subscales Psychometric properties
Multidimensional A 50-item measure Total score and subscales for Internal consistency for the
Anxiety Scale for for children age 8–19 separation anxiety, GAD, OCD, total score = 0.89. Test-­
Children, 2nd ed. that measures a range harm avoidance, physical retest for the total score and
(MASC-2; March, 2013) of anxiety symptoms symptoms (panic and tense/ the subscales range from
restlessness), social anxiety 0.80 to 0.94. Good
(humiliation/rejection and convergent validity.
performance fears), and an Excellent discriminative
inconsistency index validity
Negative Affect A 31-item measure Internal consistency for the
Self-Statement for children aged total score ranges from 0.89
Questionnaire (Ronan, 7–15 that assesses to 0.96. Test-retest
Kendall, & Rowe, 1994) how often the child reliability ranges from 0.78
experiences negative to 0.96
automatic thoughts
Penn State Worry A 14-item Internal consistency = 0.89.
Questionnaire for questionnaire that Test-retest reliability = 0.92
Children (PSWQ; has children aged
Chorpita, Tracey, Brown, 6–18 rate the
Collica, & Barlow, 1997) frequency and
controllability of
worry
Revised Children’s A 49-item measure Total anxiety, physiological Internal consistency for total
Manifest Anxiety Scale, for children aged anxiety, worry, social anxiety, score and subscales range
2nd ed. (RCMAS-2; 6–19 that assesses defensiveness, and inconsistent from 0.68 to 0.89
Reynolds & Richmond, anxiety symptoms in responding index
2008) a yes/no format
Screen for Child Anxiety A 38-item measure Somatic/panic, general anxiety, Internal consistency ranges
Related Emotional for children aged separation anxiety, social from 0.74 to 0.93. Test-­
Disorders ( Birmaher 9–18 that measures phobia, and school phobia retest reliability ranges from
et al., 1997, 1999) symptoms of 0.70 to 0.90. Good
separation anxiety discriminant validity
disorder, general
anxiety disorder,
social phobia, and
school phobia
Screen for Child Anxiety A 66-item measure Separation anxiety disorder, Internal consistency = 0.94.
Related Emotional for children aged generalized anxiety disorder, Good convergent and
Disorders – Revised 6–18 that measures panic disorder, social phobia, discriminate validity
(SCARED-R; Muris, symptoms of anxiety obsessive-compulsive disorder,
Merckelbach, Schmidt, disorders based on traumatic stress disorder, and
& Mayer, 1999; Muris & the DSM-IV specific phobias
Steerneman, 2001)
Social Anxiety Scale for A 26-item measure Fear of negative evaluation, Internal consistency for the
Children (SAS-C; La that asks children social avoidance and distress in subscales ranges from 0.69
Greca, Dandes, Wick, aged 8–18 to rate new situations, and general to 0.86. Test-retest
Shaw, & Stone, 1988) how true each social avoidance and distress reliability ranges from 0.69
experience of social to 0.86
anxiety is for them
(continued)
306 T.E. Davis III et al.

Table 2 (continued)
Measure Description Subscales Psychometric properties
Social Phobia and A 26-item measure Assertiveness/general Good internal consistency,
Anxiety Inventory for for children aged conversation, traditional social test-retest reliability
Children (SPAI-C; 8–14 years that encounters, and public
Beidel, Turner, & Morris, measures performance
1995) physiological,
cognitive, and
behavioral symptoms
of social phobia on a
3-point Likert scale
The Social Worries A measure of the Internal consistency for the
Questionnaire (SWQ; degree of worry the parent version = 0.82.
Spence, 1995) child experiences in Internal consistency for the
various social pupil version = 0.85
situations. A 10-item
parent version and a
13-item pupil version
are available
State-Trait Anxiety A 20-item measure Anxiety trait, anxiety state Internal consistency for the
Inventory for Children for children aged subscales ranges from 0.80
(STAI-C; Spielberger, 8–15 that measures to 0.90. Test-retest
1973) chronic and reliability ranges from 0.31
transitory symptoms to 0.71
of anxiety
Teacher Report Form A 120-item teacher Two broad scales (internalizing Internal consistency for the
(TRF; Achenbach & report measure that is and externalizing problems) subscales ranges from 0.72
Rescorla, 2001) comparable to the and eight subscales to 0.95. Test-retest
CBCL described (withdrawn/depressed, somatic reliability ranges from 0.60
above complaints, anxious/depressed, to 0.96
social problems, thought
problems, attention problems,
rule-breaking behavior, and
aggressive behavior)

 nalogue Behavioral Observation


A observation of a child’s behavior in various con-
(ABO) texts (e.g., in school classrooms, psychiatric
facilities, research settings, and the home). For
ABO is another important method of assess- example, the BAT involves asking the child to
ment with children, as it provides a direct oppor- engage in some feared behavior or situation
tunity to objectively view how the child responds (e.g., touching a spider, riding an elevator) and
in various situations. A variety of behavioral then measuring the extent to which the child
assessment methods and tasks have been devel- complies and usually other variables as well—
oped and include many techniques such as role- for example, ­ subjective units of distress the
plays, interaction tasks, think-aloud procedures, child experiences during the task, heart rate, etc.
functional assessments, and behavioral avoid- (Castagna, Davis, & Lilly, 2016; Davis et al.,
ance tasks (BATs; Haynes, 2001). ABOs can 2013). This task is commonly used in phobia
also be conducted in multiple settings, allowing research (Ollendick, Davis, & Muris, 2004)
Anxiety Disorders 307

but has also been used in the assessment of  easures Specific to Social Skills/
M
obsessive-compulsive disorder (Barrett, Healy, Social Competence
& March, 2003) and social phobia (Coles &
Heimberg, 2000). An adaptation of this task to a In addition to instruments and paradigms dis-
role-play format has also been used to measure cussed to this point, it is also important to note
social skills directly on the Revised Behavioral that there are several instruments designed spe-
Assertiveness Test for Children (BAT-CR; cifically to assess social skills or social compe-
Ollendick, 1981). The BAT-CR involves asking tence (see Table 3 for more detailed descriptions).
the child to participate in a series of role-plays The Matson Evaluation of Social Skills for
of both positive and negative social situations. Youngsters (MESSY; Matson, Rotatori, & Helsel,
They can then be coded on things such as eye 1983) and the Social Skills Rating System Child
contact, latency of response, and length of and Parent Version (SSRS; Gresham & Elliot,
response. This task has been used to evaluate the 1990) are two of the most frequently used of
social skills of children with social phobia these measures (and their updates to newer edi-
(Spence et al., 1999). Similarly, BATs have been tions). Both are described in greater detail in the
developed that incorporate parents and caregiv- broader social skills assessment chapters of this
ers to observe the degree to which their social volume. Other less frequently used measures
influence may affect a child’s performance include the Social Skills Questionnaire (Parent)
(Ollendick, Lewis, Cowart, & Davis, 2012). (SSQ-P, Spence, 1995), the Social Competence
In addition, other types of direct observations Questionnaire (Parent) (SCQ-P; Spence, 1995),
of behavior are commonly used to assess anxi- the Friendship Questionnaire (Bierman &
ety. These protocols allow the observer to view McCauley, 1987), and the Children’s Assertive
the anxious behaviors in the child’s natural envi- Behavior Scale (CABS; Michelson & Wood,
ronment (i.e., home, classroom) and are coded 1982).
based on the protocol being used (e.g., Glennon
& Weisz, 1978). Other forms of ABO also pro-
vide valuable information. For example, fami-  ummary and Recommendations
S
lies can be asked to complete interaction tasks for Assessment
in which they are observed talking freely about
a prescribed topic or situation; these paradigms For assessment, an evidence-based, multicompo-
can aid in determining how parental influence or nent (i.e., physiology, behavior, and cognition),
patterns of dysfunctional interaction may con- multi-method (e.g., questionnaires, analogue
tribute to a disorder (Haynes, 2001). Functional behavioral observations, clinical interviews), and
assessments are also useful in which the poten- multi-informant (e.g., self, parent, teacher, other
tial operant maintaining factors of anxiety are caregivers) assessment is crucial. Given the
observed, codified, recorded (Davis, 2009; numerous instruments and methods available to
Haynes, 2001), or discussed indirectly through assess anxiety (cf. Silverman & Ollendick, 2005),
detailed interviewing with the child and/or par- it is difficult to create a single, one-size-fits-all
ent (Davis, 2009; Nebel-Schwalm & Davis, battery, and generally a clinician is better served
2011; Ollendick et al., 2004). Overall, direct pulling together the various methodologies and
observation procedures allow the clinician an instruments based on a particular client’s needs.
opportunity to directly observe how a child Finally, clinicians remain aware of the influence
behaves in certain anxiety-provoking and/or that the assessment process itself can have on a
social situations, and while at times difficult to client: i.e., the social anxiety, deficits, and
arrange, they can generate a great deal of useful ­difficulties they are attempting to assess and treat
information. may be exacerbated by the actual assessment and
308 T.E. Davis III et al.

Table 3  Measures of social skills and social competence


Instrument Description Psychometric properties
The Children’s Assertive A 27-item child-report measure of social Internal consistency = 0.78.
Behavior Scale (CABS; behavior. Each item represents a social Test-retest reliability = 0.86. Good
Michelson & Wood, 1982) situation, and children indicate how they discriminant and convergent validity
would respond on a 5-point scale from
passive to aggressive
The Friendship Questionnaire A 40-item child-report measure of peer Internal consistency ranges from
(Bierman & McCauley, 1987) interactions. Includes three subscales: 0.72 to 0.82
Positive interactions, negative
interactions, and extensiveness of peer
network
The Matson Evaluation of A 64-item parent- and teacher-report Internal consistency ranged from
Social Skills for Youngsters, measure of social skills in children aged 0.86 to 0.92 for the factors. Has
2nd ed. (MESSY-II; Matson 2–16. Various social behaviors are listed, been found to have good convergent
et al. 2010; Matson, Neal, and respondents indicate how often the and discriminant validity
Worley, Kozlowski, & Fodstad, behavior is performed on a scale from 1
2012) (“not at all”) to 5 (“very much”). The
scale yields three factors, hostile,
adaptive/appropriate, and inappropriately
assertive/overconfident
The Social Competence Contains nine items in which parents rate Guttman split-half reliability has
Questionnaire – Parent a child’s social competence with peers been reported to be 0.87
(Spence, 1995) from 0 (not true) to 2 (mostly true)
The Social Skills Contains 30 items in which parents assess Split-half reliability has been
Questionnaire – Parent a child’s perceived social skills reported to be 0.90
(Spence, 1995)
The Social Skills Rating The SSRS (used the most with anxiety) Internal consistency ranges from
System Child and Parent includes parent, teacher, and child (grade 0.65 to 0.95. Test-retest reliability
Version (SSRS; Gresham & 3 and above) measures of social skills and ranges from 0.65 to 0.87. Has been
Elliot, 1990; Updated to the problem behaviors. There are five social shown to have good construct and
Social Skills Improvement skills factors: cooperation, assertion, criterion validity
System-Rating Scales, responsibility, empathy, and self-control
SSIS-RS; Gresham & Elliott,
2008)

treatment process (e.g., a child with selective peutic and researched in their own rights, have
mutism may have more difficulty participating in been combined into increasingly efficacious
assessment and treatment until becoming com- behavioral and cognitive-­ behavioral therapies
fortable with the clinician). As a result, clinicians (CBT). These treatment techniques include
should be mindful to allow more time to develop exposure, systematic desensitization, modeling,
rapport and be especially mindful of the child’s and contingency management. Each of these
progress, anxiety, and frustration (Davis, 2009). procedures is explained and evaluated below
regarding its use in the treatment of symptoms of
anxiety and in particular for the treatment of
Treatment social skills problems in children with anxiety.
Additionally, a particular focus will be the com-
Currently, a variety of efficacious treatment monly used combination of behavioral and cog-
techniques have been examined for use with nitive-behavioral treatments for child social
anxious children (Davis, May et al., 2011; Davis anxiety. Overall, however, it is important to
& Ollendick, 2005; Higa-McMillan, Francis, remember that efficacious treatments for child
Rith-Najarian, & Chorpita, 2016; and e.g., anxiety have typically been designed to alleviate
Ollendick et al., 2009; Walkup et al., 2008). anxious symptomatology and not necessary
Over time, these various techniques, while thera- social problems per se. While progress has been
Anxiety Disorders 309

made, many child treatments still need to account tered all at once (e.g., flooding or implosive ther-
for child development and developmental psy- apy) using the most feared situation; however,
chopathology more thoroughly, and the field exposure is more commonly implemented by
should be wary of overly simplistic downward gradually creating a hierarchy with the child and
extensions of adult therapies (Barrett, 2000; moving up that list of steps from the least to most
Davis, 2012). Also, the newcomer to the anxiety fearful stimuli or situations. The decision to
disorders, selective mutism, seems well fitted to move on to the next step typically occurs when
the diagnostic category, but its rarity has meant fear and anxiety have decreased significantly and
that research into its symptoms, assessment, and the child becomes comfortable with moving for-
treatment still lags comparatively behind many ward (i.e., the progression through steps is not
of the other disorders (Muris & Ollendick, forced). This gradual progression is considered
2015). While space prohibits a full review of the the more humane of the two doses of exposure
research that does exist for this disorder in par- and is preferred by most clients, parents, and pro-
ticular, CBT and its techniques seem to be very fessionals (King & Gullone, 1990). Controlled
promising, and so it does lend itself to the sec- exposure is believed to provide safe experiences
tions that follow as well (e.g., Bergman, with the feared stimulus or situation allowing for
Gonzalez, Piacentini, & Keller, 2013; Reuther, habituation and extinction of fear anxiety. New
Davis, Moree, & Matson, 2011; for a review, see learning occurs as well which competes with the
Muris & Ollendick, 2015). Overall, given the previous fearful responding, and exposure ses-
reciprocal influence of anxiety and social skills, sions provide a structured environment in which
the treatment of both problematic anxiety and to practice and develop coping skills and compe-
social skills deficits or social skills production tency (Davis, 2009). In addition, recent research
deficits may be necessary and advisable, even in has suggested that approaches which incorporate
children having anxiety disorders other than inhibitory learning may enhance the outcomes of
social anxiety disorder. exposure (Craske, Treanor, Conway, Zbozinek,
& Vervliet, 2014).

Exposure
Systematic Desensitization
Exposure-based therapy involves the anxious
child experiencing their fear either in vivo or As developed by Wolpe (1958), systematic
imaginally. This ideally involves remaining desensitization is one form of treatment that uti-
exposed until the anxiety or fear decreases sig- lizes exposure. At its inception, this treatment
nificantly. In in vivo exposure, the child is was grounded in classical conditioning and
exposed to the actual feared stimulus, for exam- founded on the idea that fear could be counter-
ple, being around a lizard or interacting with a conditioned by pairing the feared stimulus with
stranger (e.g., who is a therapist’s confederate). an activity that is reciprocal to and incongruent
In imaginal exposure, the therapist guides the with anxiety. As theory and research have
child through imagining the feared stimulus or advanced, these processes are better understood
situation, for example, imagining what would and currently described as the creation of context-­
happen if the child were to talk with a stranger at specific learning that competes with previous
a restaurant or have a confrontation with peers at information instead of the idea that new learning
school. In vivo exposure is commonly included actually overwrites the old (Bouton, 2004). Even
in the treatment of child anxiety disorders, and as so, the actual procedure remains largely
many as 90–100% of anxiety treatments incorpo- unchanged. The competing activity most often by
rate exposure (Chorpita & Southam-Gerow, researchers and practitioners alike is progressive
2006). Exposure can also be adjusted as to the muscle relaxation (PMR) and diaphragmatic
intensity of the experience. It can be adminis- breathing: it should be noted, however, that in
310 T.E. Davis III et al.

theory any behavior or act could be used as long would be having a child watch a peer introduce
as the response is incompatible with anxiety himself on the playground and then having that
(e.g., holding a favorite toy or eating). After peer go with the child to introduce himself.
relaxation skills are taught, a fear hierarchy is Subsequently, the effectiveness of models during
developed ranging from the least or easiest expo- child treatment has been suggested to increase
sure to the most feared stimulus or situation. The across media/experience from using filmed mod-
client then begins to engage in the competing els to live models, to participant modeling
activity (becomes relaxed) and is then gradually (Ollendick, 1979).
exposed to situations on the hierarchy. If imple-
mented correctly, the client should remain relaxed
and should not become anxious in the situation. Contingency Management
Under these circumstances, according to classi-
cal conditioning theory, the association between In contingency management (also known as rein-
the stimulus and the fearful response should forced practice), children are reinforced for desir-
decrease, and the client’s fear of the stimulus able behavior in anxiety-provoking situations.
should also decrease. Systematic desensitization Reinforcers can be tangible items or even verbal
can be administered either in vivo or imaginally. praise from therapists or caregivers. Silverman
A slight modification for imaginal exposures et al. (1999) examined the efficacy of contin-
usually involves some sort of safety signal (e.g., gency management in children with anxiety dis-
raising a hand or a finger) during the progression orders. In this study, the children’s parents
along the hierarchy to designate when an indi- provided reinforcement for completing tasks on a
vidual begins to experience fear or anxiety and fear hierarchy. Parents were also educated in
the exposure needs to be paused or slightly basic behavioral principles such as positive and
decreased. negative reinforcement and extinction.
Subsequently, children showed significant
improvement on outcome measures assessing
Modeling fear, anxiety, and depression, and 55% of chil-
dren who received contingency management
Modeling is grounded in social learning theory treatment no longer met the criteria for an anxiety
and involves the child observing another person disorder (Silverman et al., 1999).
(e.g., therapist, peers, parent, or other confeder-
ates) demonstrating non-fearful or appropriate
behavior in the feared situation (either on video  ehavioral and Cognitive-Behavioral
B
or directly) to promote imitation. A model may Techniques
perform a variety of behaviors to be learned
including conversation skills, ordering food at a Behavioral treatments make use of a variety of
restaurant, or asking a teacher questions in class. techniques previously discussed and are based
Models can also be broken into two types: mas- upon operant and classical conditioning.
tery models demonstrate mastery of interacting Cognitive-behavioral treatments (CBT) further
in the situation with ease, and coping models combine techniques such as cognitive restructur-
evince initial anxiety in the situation but over- ing and changing expectations about what will
come their fear and worry (Chorpita & Southam-­ happen in a feared situation combined with
Gerow, 2006). In a different, classic variation, behavioral techniques like exposure, contingency
Ritter (1965, 1968) developed participant model- management, or social skills training. A 50-year
ing. In participant modeling, the model demon- review of research on child and adolescent anxi-
strates appropriate behavior for the child and then ety treatment done by Higa-McMillan et al.
goes a step further by interacting with the child to (2016) reported that CBT and exposure-based
help him perform the skills. An example of this therapy continue to be the most well-established
Anxiety Disorders 311

treatments for this population. CBT has been competency scale of the CBCL as an outcome
found to have strong effect sizes for reducing measure. Children in the treatment group showed
social anxiety and moderate effect sizes for significant improvement in ratings on this scale
increasing social competence (Segool & Carlson, compared to the wait-list group, and these effects
2008). As a result, several of the more popular were maintained at 1-year follow-up (Kendall,
behavioral and cognitive-behavioral treatments 1994). Additionally, an adaptation of Coping Cat
are discussed below. was used in one of the largest trials to date, the
Coping Cat. The Coping Cat program is a Child/Adolescent Anxiety Multimodal Treatment
manualized childhood anxiety treatment Study (CAMS). Results from this comparison of
(Kendall, Kane, Howard, & Siqueland, 1990). CBT to sertraline, their combination, or placebo
There is a therapist manual and also a Coping Cat suggested 80.7% improvement on the combina-
Workbook that is given to children and used in tion therapy, 59.7% on CBT, 54.9% on sertraline,
each session (Kendall, 1990). The treatment con- and 23.7% on placebo (Walkup et al., 2008).
sists of 16 individual sessions, 2 of which are par- Similarly, Settipani and Kendall (2013) found
ent meetings with the therapist. The first eight that poorer social functioning was associated
sessions focus on psychoeducation, cognitive with a more severe anxiety in children and that
skills, and healthy coping skills; the last eight poor response to CBT may be associated with
sessions focus on working through an exposure poor social competence.
hierarchy (Kendall et al., 1990). Overall, the pro- It has also been suggested that flexible appli-
gram uses techniques such as relaxation, cogni- cations of the manual-based treatment can be
tive restructuring, problem-solving, and exposure implemented (see Podell et al., 2010 for review).
tasks in an effort to help youth learn to identify Several authors have examined whether adding a
and cope with their anxious arousal (Podell, family component increases efficacy of the treat-
Mychailyszyn, Edmunds, Puleo, & Kendall, ment (Barrett, 1998; Barrett, Dadds, & Rapee,
2010). 1996; Kendall, Hudson, et al., 2008; Nauta,
Several RCTs have been conducted evaluating Scholing, Emmelkamp, & Minderaa, 2003).
the Coping Cat program (Kendall, 1994, 1997; Some results indicated that there may be a mar-
Kendall et al., 2008). In the first RCT, Kendall ginal effect of adding parent training and parent
(1994) evaluated the effectiveness of this pro- education components to the Coping Cat and that
gram for children ages 9–13 years, with primary these changes will have a greater efficacy with
anxiety disorder diagnoses including overanx- younger treatment clients than adolescents
ious disorder, separation anxiety disorder, and (Barrett, 1998; Barrett et al., 1996), while Nauta
avoidant disorder. Children who received the et al. (2003) found no outcome differences
Coping Cat treatment were compared with a between treatment groups with and without par-
wait-list control group. Diagnostically, at post- ent training components. Kendall et al. (2008)
treatment 64% of children in the Coping Cat reported that individual treatment outperformed
group no longer met the criteria for an anxiety family treatment on teacher reports of child anxi-
disorder, compared with 5% of children in the ety, while family treatment outperformed the
wait-list group (Kendall, 1994). The children individual treatment when both parents had an
who received the Coping Cat treatment also anxiety disorder. The Coping Cat has also been
improved on a number of measures assessing implemented successfully in group formats,
anxiety (i.e., both self- and parent-report mea- showing that it is superior to a psychological pla-
sures and behavioral observations); these results cebo procedure and wait-list control (Muris,
were maintained at 1-year follow-up (Kendall, Meesters, & van Melick, 2002). When compared
1994). Similar results for treatment outcome with individual treatment, few differences were
were found by Kendall (1997) in another RCT found, and both treatments were superior to the
comparing the Coping Cat to wait-list control. wait-list control (Flannery-Schroeder & Kendall,
Further, Kendall (1994) also included the social 2000). Flannery-Schroeder and Kendall (2000)
312 T.E. Davis III et al.

included several measures of social skills in an can engage in social activities and individual
RCT comparing an individual treatment, a group in vivo exposure to feared social situations
treatment, and a wait-list control. This study (Beidel et al., 2000). In an RCT, Beidel et al.
failed to show, however, that the treatment groups (2000) compared SET-C to an attentional control
differed from the wait-list control at posttreat- called Testbusters. Children in the Testbusters
ment on both child and parent measures of social group were taught study skills and test-taking
skills (Flannery-Schroeder & Kendall, 2000). strategies for similar amounts of time. At post-
FRIENDS. The FRIENDS program is a group treatment, 67% of children in the SET-C group
format CBT procedure for children ages no longer met the criteria for social phobia, com-
6–16 years with anxiety disorders. FRIENDS is pared to 5% in the Testbusters group (Beidel
an acronym for the coping skills taught in the et al., 2000). Children in the SET-C group were
treatment (i.e., F, feeling worried?; R, relax and also rated as being more skilled during a role-­
feel good; I, inner thoughts; E, explore plans; N, play task and read-aloud task than children in the
nice work so reward yourself; D, don’t forget to Testbusters group by independent observers post-
practice; S, stay calm, you know how to cope treatment. At 6-month follow-up, 80% of chil-
now). The treatment consists of ten child group dren who received SET-C no longer met the
sessions, two booster sessions, and ten parent criteria for social phobia (Beidel et al., 2000),
sessions. The treatment is similar to the Coping and at 3-year follow-up, 72% of children who
Cat in that it includes cognitive and coping skills, received SET-C no longer met the criteria for
training in family management and communica- social phobia. Ratings of children’s skills during
tion skills to facilitate practice of the skills chil- the role-play task decreased to pretreatment lev-
dren learn in the session, and a peer component in els following the posttreatment assessment, but
which children are taught basic social skills effectiveness in performance during the read-­
including how to make friends (Shortt, Barrett, & aloud task was maintained (Beidel, Turner,
Fox, 2001). The program includes a therapist Young, & Paulson, 2005). Another trial of SET-C
manual, children’s workbook, and parent book- by Scharfstein, Beidel, Finnell, Distler, and
let. Shortt et al. (2001) examined the efficacy of Carter (2011) compared SET-C to fluoxetine and
the FRIENDS treatment in an RCT with a wait-­ pill placebo. Their conclusions were that SET-C
list control. Children in the study met the criteria led to important, lasting improvements across
for a primary anxiety disorder diagnosis (includ- several social skills not seen in the fluoxetine or
ing generalized anxiety disorder, separation anxi- placebo conditions. Also, children treated with
ety disorder, or social phobia). At posttreatment, fluoxetine did not differ in pragmatic or paralin-
69% of children in the FRIENDS group no lon- guistic skills compared to placebo. In addition,
ger met the diagnosis, compared with 6% of chil- Öst, Cederlund, and Reuterskiöld (2015) found
dren in the wait-list control. These treatment that parent education training did not improve
effects were maintained at 1-year follow-up, with treatment outcomes.
68% of children who received treatment no lon- Skills for Academic and Social Success
ger meeting the diagnosis for a primary anxiety (SASS). Fisher, Masia-Warner, and Klein (2004)
disorder (Shortt et al., 2001). studied a school-based social skills intervention
Social Effectiveness Therapy for Children for adolescents with social phobia. Skills for
(SET-C). SET-C is a behavioral group treatment Academic and Social Success (SASS) is a
for social anxiety disorder in children and adoles- cognitive-­behavioral treatment group composed
cents. The treatment includes group sessions for of psychoeducation, cognitive skills, social skills
education and social skills training which training (including conversation skills), listening
includes conversation skills, skills for joining skills, and assertiveness, gradual exposure, and
groups, assertiveness, and telephone skills. The relapse prevention. It was developed from Social
treatment also incorporates peer generalization Effectiveness Therapy for Children (SET-C).
sessions, so anxious and non-anxious children Treatment was administered as 12 weekly group
Anxiety Disorders 313

meetings at school, 2 individual meetings, social compared to only 4% of those in the no treatment
events (including the use of peer assistants who control group (Hayward et al., 2000). At 1-year
were non-anxious adolescent classmates who follow-up, however, there was no statistical dif-
assisted group members at school events), 2 par- ference between the CBGT-A group and the no
ent meetings which included education about treatment control group. The treatment group
social anxiety, and 2 teacher meetings which continued to improve, and the control group
included education about anxiety and aid in set- improved as well. The authors suggested that this
ting up and working through the fear hierarchy effect might have been due to children in the con-
for students (Fisher et al., 2004). Masia-Warner, trol group receiving community treatment
Fisher, Shrout, Rathor, and Klein (2007) exam- (Hayward et al., 2000). Similarly, a study of a
ined the efficacy of SASS in a randomized clini- CBT group for social anxiety disorder led to the
cal trial (RCT) comparing it to an attention finding that children with higher social anxiety
control (Masia-Warner et al., 2007). Results indi- prior to treatment had the greatest treatment
cated that after treatment, 59% of the SASS treat- gains, with reductions in self-consciousness and
ment group no longer met the criteria for social poor anxiety regulation predicting reductions in
phobia, compared to 0% of the attention control social anxiety (Kley, Heinrichs, Bender, &
group, with gains maintained at 6-month follow- Tuschen-Caffier, 2012).
­up (Masia-Warner et al., 2007). Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment with and
Cognitive-Behavioral Group Treatment for Without Parent Involvement. Spence et al. (2000)
Adolescents (CBGT-A) and Other Group CBTs. used a social skills-based cognitive-behavioral
CBGT-A is a treatment for social anxiety disor- treatment (CBT) to treat children ages 7–14 with
der in adolescents. It was developed from an social phobia. The children were divided into
adult treatment for social phobia that followed a three treatment groups—CBT with parent
similar format (Albano & Barlow, 1996). The involvement, CBT without parent involvement,
treatment was administered in 16 group sessions and wait-list control. Treatment consisted of 12
with the first 8 sessions focused on education, group sessions, followed by a half hour of games,
skill building including cognitive restructuring, so children could practice their skills with peers.
social skills including those necessary for main- The treatment included social skills training that
taining social relationships, and problem-solving. included conversation skills, listening skills, and
The last eight sessions focused on exposure to identifying social cues in others, and the children
feared social situations. Parents were also edu- were assigned with weekly homework tasks.
cated about the disorder and helped with expo- Parents observed their children’s group sessions
sure exercises (Albano & Barlow, 1996). Albano, and were taught about modeling and reinforce-
Marten, Holt, Heimberg, and Barlow (1995) ment. This treatment successfully reduced anxi-
tested the efficacy of the protocol in five adoles- ety symptoms—87.5% of CBT with parent
cents who met the criteria for social phobia. At involvement and 58% of CBT without parent
3-month follow-up, four of the five participants involvement no longer met the criteria at post-
no longer met the criteria for social phobia, and at treatment compared with 7% of children in the
12-month follow-up, none of the five participants wait-list group. Importantly, the authors also
met the criteria for social phobia (Albano et al., examined social skills, using the Social Skills
1995). Hayward et al. (2000) tested the efficacy Questionnaire, parent version (SSQ-P); Social
of the treatment on a larger scale. These authors Competence Questionnaire, parent version
included 35 adolescent females who met the cri- (SCQ-P); direct observation of social skills in the
teria for social phobia. Half of the participants classroom and on the playground; and the
were assigned to CBGT-A, and the other half Revised Behavioral Assertiveness Test for
were assigned to a no treatment control group. Children (BAT-CR). Parent report of social
Following treatment, 45% of girls in the CBGT-A ­competence (SCQ-P) and performance in role-
group no longer met the criteria for social phobia, play tasks for the BAT-CR improved for both
314 T.E. Davis III et al.

treatment groups as well as the wait-list control (Chorpita et al., 2004). A pilot study examining
group. Ratings of competence from direct obser- the efficacy of this treatment was conducted with
vation by independent observers did not differ seven children suffering from primary diagnoses
between the treatment groups and the wait-list of anxiety disorders. Following their individual
control and did not improve over time (Spence treatments, none of the children met the criteria
et al., 2000). At 6- and 12-month follow-up for their primary anxiety diagnoses. These effects
assessments, treatment gains were maintained were maintained at a 6-month follow-up
(Spence et al., 2000). (Chorpita et al., 2004). Modular treatment has
Brief Group Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment also been suggested for disorders other than anxi-
for Social Phobia. Similar to other CBT studies, ety such as depression (Chorpita, Daleiden, &
Gallagher, Rabian, and McCloskey (2004) exam- Weisz, 2005) and has been used successfully
ined the effects of a small group CBT interven- with selective mutism (Reuther et al., 2011).
tion composed of psychoeducation, cognitive
strategies, and exposure in 23 children with social
phobia. Children were either assigned to a wait-­ Summary and Caution
list or to small groups of five to seven children for
three 3-h sessions of CBT. At a 3-week follow- It is encouraging that there are many treatment
­up, children treated with CBT generally showed options available for childhood anxiety in general
improvement over those in the wait-list condi- and childhood social anxiety disorder in particu-
tion; however, no change in social competence lar. Many of these interventions are often focused
was evident as measured by the CBCL (Gallagher on reducing the symptoms of fear and anxiety,
et al., 2004). but unfortunately several ignore the social skills
Modular Treatment of Anxiety. Modular CBT and production deficits common to children with
for anxiety is an individual treatment for anxiety these problems. Not surprisingly then, behavioral
disorders in children (Chorpita, Taylor, Francis, and cognitive-behavioral packages have been
Moffitt, & Austin, 2004). It consists of 13 mod- found to be particularly effective at treating anxi-
ules that therapists can pick and choose from and ety disorder diagnoses and symptoms. While it
arrange to meet the needs of their clients. There depends on the evidence-based criteria used to
are four core modules that all children receive: summarize the treatment effects, CBT has been
self-monitoring (fear ladder), psychoeducation, found to meet well-established criteria for allevi-
exposure, and maintenance/relapse prevention. ating anxiety disorders, and both behavioral
These modules were thought to be essential prin- and cognitive-behavioral programs have been
ciples in the treatment of childhood anxiety found to be effective for social anxiety disorder
(Chorpita et al., 2004). Depending on the needs (i.e., Davis, 2009; Davis, May et al., 2011).
of the individual child, therapists can choose to Unfortunately, there is still a sizeable minority of
include other modules: cognitive restructuring, children who do not respond to even the best
social skills training, rewards, differential rein- interventions present at this time. Even for treat-
forcement, and time-out. The order of use for the ments that do include social skills training as a
modules is indicated by a flowchart in which component in treatment, and similar to the con-
modules for the basic skills of self-monitoring clusions drawn in this chapter in 2009 (Davis,
and psychoeducation are done first, then other Munson, & Tarcza), few RCTs include outcome
optional modules are completed for behaviors measures of social skills or social competency,
that may interfere with exposure, and then expo- and those that do suggest it is unclear whether
sure and relapse prevention modules are com- social skills improvements are evident (e.g.,
pleted. To the extent that parents or other adults Flannery-­Schroeder & Kendall, 2000; Gallagher
are involved in the maintenance of the disorder, et al., 2004) or maintained over time (Beidel
they are also involved in treatment such as the et al., 2000, 2005; Spence et al., 2000). Because
differential reinforcement and reward modules impairments in social skills often impact the lives
Anxiety Disorders 315

of children with anxiety disorders, RCTs for relationships and functioning, as it is possible
childhood anxiety should include outcome mea- that these have long-lasting effects beyond the
sures of social skills. Social skills training should end of treatment and may possibly be one reason
also be included in the treatment of anxiety to the that outcomes for children with social anxiety
extent that it is relevant for individual cases (i.e., disorder are consistently poorer. Researchers
if social skills are deficient versus if there is a should be encouraged to include measures of
production deficit—children have the skills but social functioning child anxiety treatment stud-
do not use them). More recent explorations of the ies and then report on those outcomes. The lit-
moderators and mediators of treatment outcomes erature on peer relations and social anxiety
have born this out, as social anxiety disorder has should increasingly be infused into broader
repeatedly been found to be a stumbling block to treatment strategies to continue to develop com-
anxiety treatment outcome. Across recent effi- prehensive treatment packages or, as is the case
cacy and effectiveness trials for childhood anxi- with the work by Chorpita, modular options
ety and across different anxiety disorders, the which can be included as needed. Some of this
presence of social anxiety disorder has consis- push has come to fruition since our initial con-
tently been found to be associated with poorer clusions in Davis et al. (2009). Transdiagnostic
outcomes (e.g., Compton et al., 2015; Hudson, CBT for children emphasizes the underlying
Keers et al., 2015; Hudson, Rapee et al., 2015; mechanisms which may influence outcome as
Wergeland et al., 2016). common targets for intervention across disorders
(Ehrenreich-May & Chu, 2014). Additionally,
researchers should continue to target the poten-
Conclusion and Future Directions tial moderators and mediators of treatment out-
come as well and further illuminate the means by
Children with anxiety suffer from a variety of dif- which social anxiety seems to be associated with
ficulties that extend beyond their anxiety symp- poorer outcomes. For example, incorporating
toms including strained peer relationships, parents into treatment may be important, but
stigma, and victimization. Given the debilitating parental influence may be greatest for certain
interaction of anxiety and social problems, it is genders or ages (e.g., Ollendick & Horsch,
surprising that as few as 10% of child anxiety 2007), and this may help explain the lack of con-
treatment protocols include a social skills com- sistent findings for including parents. Finally,
ponent (Chorpita & Southam-Gerow, 2006). treatments should still incorporate development
Similar to the conclusions drawn many years and developmental psychopathology into assess-
ago, anxiety treatments still seem geared toward ment and treatment methods, eschewing the one-
alleviating anxious emotional responding and size-fits-all approach.
even then targeting behavioral symptoms in par-
ticular (Davis, Munson, & Tarcza, 2009; cf.
Davis & Ollendick, 2005; Davis, May et al., References
2011). While assessing avoidant and dysfunc-
tional behavior is important, more research Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T.
(1987). Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional
should be conducted using a comprehensive problems: Implications of cross-informant correla-
assessment of anxiety symptoms (i.e., physiol- tions for situational specificity. Psychological Bulletin,
ogy, behavior, and cognition; Davis & Ollendick, 101, 213–232.
2005) as well as indicators of social functioning Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for
the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles. Burlington,
across a child’s day-to-day environments (e.g., VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for
school, home, peers), even with the existing mul- Children, Youth, and Families.
tiple-informant disagreements. Albano, A. M., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Breaking the
Specifically, more research and work need to vicious cycle: Cognitive-behavioral group treatment
for socially anxious youth. In E. D. Hibbs & P. S.
target the interference and impairment in social
316 T.E. Davis III et al.

Jensen (Eds.), Psychosocial treatments for child and Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., & Morris, T. L. (2000).
adolescent disorders: Empirically based strategies Behavioral treatment of childhood social phobia.
for clinical practice (pp. 43–62). Washington, DC: Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68,
American Psychological Association. 1072–1080.
Albano, A. M., Marten, P. A., Holt, C. S., Heimberg, Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., Young, B., & Paulson, A.
R. G., & Barlow, D. H. (1995). Cognitive-behavioral (2005). Social effectiveness therapy for children:
group treatment for social phobia in adolescents: A Three- year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and
preliminary study. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Clinical Psychology, 73, 721–725.
Disease, 183, 649–656. Benzies, K., & Mychasiuk, R. (2009). Fostering fam-
Ale, C. M., McCarthy, D. M., Rothschild, L. M., & ily resiliency: A review of the key protective factors.
Whiteside, S. P. (2015). Components of cogni- Child & Family Social Work, 14(1), 103–114.
tive behavioral therapy related to outcome in child- Bergman, R., Gonzalez, A., Piacentini, J., & Keller, M.
hood anxiety disorders. Clinical Child and Family (2013). Integrated behavior therapy for selective mut-
Psychology Review, 18(3), 240–251. ism: A randomized controlled pilot study. Behaviour
Allen, K. B., Allen, B., Austin, K. E., Waldron, J. C., Research and Therapy, 51, 680–689.
& Ollendick, T. H. (2015). Synchrony–desynchrony Biederman, J., Hirshfeld-Becker, D., Rosenbaum, J.,
in the tripartite model of fear: Predicting treatment Herot, C., Friedman, D., Snidman, N., … Faraone,
outcome in clinically phobic children. Behaviour S. V. (2001). Further evidence of association between
Research and Therapy, 71, 54–64. behavioral inhibition and social anxiety in children.
Ambrosini, P. J. (2000). Historical development and pres- American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1673–1679.
ent status of the schedule for affective disorders and Bierman, K. L., & McCauley, E. (1987). Children’s
schizophrenia for school-age children (K-SADS). descriptions of their peer interactions: Useful informa-
Journal of the American Academy of Child and tion for clinical child assessment. Journal of Clinical
Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 49–58. Child Psychology, 16, 9–18.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic Birmaher, B., Khetarpal, S., Brent, D. A., Cully, M.,
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Balach, L., Kauffman, J., & Neer, S. M. (1997). The
Washington, DC: Author. Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders
Axelson, D., Birmaher, B., Zelazny, J., Kaufman, (SCARED): Scale construction and psychometric
J., & Gill, M. (2009). K-SADS-PL: 2009 work- characteristics. Journal of the American Academy of
ing draft. Retrieved from http://www.psychiatry. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 545–553.
pitt.edu/sites/default/files/Documents/assessments/ Birmaher, B., Brent, D. A., Chiappetta, L., Bridge, J.,
KSADS-PL_2009_working_draft_full.pdf Monga, S., & Baugher, M. (1999). Psychometric
Barrett, P. M. (1998). Evaluation of cognitive-behavioral properties of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related
group treatments for childhood anxiety disorders. Emotional Disorders (SCARED): a replication
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 459–468. study. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Barrett, P. M. (2000). Treatment of childhood anxiety: Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(10), 1230–1236.
Developmental aspects. Clinical Psychology Review, Bonetti, L., Campbell, M. A., & Gilmore, L. (2010). The
20, 479–494. relationship of loneliness and social anxiety with
Barrett, P. M., Dadds, M. R., & Rapee, R. M. (1996). children’s and adolescents’ online communication.
Family treatment of childhood anxiety: A controlled Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking,
trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 13(3), 279–285.
64, 333–342. Bouton, M. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in
Barrett, P., Healy, L., & March, J. S. (2003). Behavioral extinction. Learning and Memory, 11, 485–494.
avoidance test for childhood obsessive-compulsive Brown-Jacobsen, A. M., Wallace, D. P., & Whiteside,
disorder: A home-based observation. American S. P. (2011). Multimethod, multi-informant agree-
Journal of Psychotherapy, 57, 80–100. ment, and positive predictive value in the identifica-
Beidel, D. C., Fink, C. M., & Turner, S. M. (1996). tion of child anxiety disorders using the SCAS and
Stability in anxious symptomatology in children. ADIS-C. Assessment, 18(3), 382–392.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 24, 257–269. Buss, A., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early
Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (2007). Behavioral and developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ:
cognitive-behavioral treatment of social anxiety dis- Erlbaum.
order in children and adolescents. In Shy children, Cartwright-Hatton, S., McNicol, K., & Doubleday, E.
phobic adults: Nature and treatment of social anxiety (2006). Anxiety in a neglected population: Prevalence
disorders (2nd ed., pp. 261–313). Washington, DC: of anxiety disorders in pre-adolescent children.
American Psychological Association. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 817–833.
Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., & Morris, T. L. (1995). A Castagna, P., Davis III, T. E., & Lilly, M. (2016).
new inventory to assess childhood social anxiety and Behavioral Avoidance Tasks with anxious youth:
phobia: The social phobia and anxiety inventory for A review of procedures, properties, and criticisms.
children. Psychological Assessment, 7, 73–79. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Anxiety Disorders 317

Chavira, D., Stein, M., Bailey, K., & Stein, M. (2004). childhood psychopathology and developmental disor-
Childhood anxiety disorders in primary care: Prevalent ders. New York: Springer.
but untreated. Depression and Anxiety, 20, 155–164. Davis, T. E., III. (2012). Where to from here for ASD and
Chorpita, B. F., Daleiden, E. L., & Weisz, J. R. (2005). anxiety? Lessons learned from child anxiety and the
Modularity in the design and application of therapeu- issue of DSM-5. Clinical Psychology: Science and
tic interventions. Applied and Preventive Psychology, Practice, 19, 358–363.
11, 141–156. Davis, T. E., III, Hess, J. A., Matthews, R., Fodstad, J. C.,
Chorpita, B., & Southam-Gerow, M. (2006). Fears and Dempsey, T., Jenkins, W., … Matson, J. L. (2011). The
anxieties. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), moderating effect of anxiety on development in atypi-
Treatment of child disorders (3rd ed., pp. 271–335). cally developing toddlers. Journal of Psychopathology
New York: Guilford. and Behavioral Assessment, 33, 171–177.
Chorpita, B. F., Taylor, A. A., Francis, S. E., Moffitt, C., Davis, T. E., III, May, A. C., & Whiting, S. E. (2011).
& Austin, A. A. (2004). Efficacy of modular cogni- Evidence-based treatment of anxiety and phobia in
tive behavior therapy for childhood anxiety disorders. children and adolescents: Current status and effects on
Behavior Therapy, 35, 263–287. the emotional response. Clinical Psychology Review,
Chorpita, B. F., Tracey, S. A., Brown, T. A., Collica, 31, 592–602.
T. J., & Barlow, D. H. (1997). Assessment of worry Davis, T. E., III, Moree, B. N., Dempsey, T., Hess, J. A.,
in children and adolescents: An adaptation of the Penn Jenkins, W. S., Fodstad, J. C., & Matson, J. L. (2012).
State Worry Questionnaire. Behavior Research and The effects of communication deficits on anxiety
Therapy, 35, 569–581. symptoms in infants and toddlers with autism spec-
Choudhury, M. S., Pimentel, S. S., & Kendall, P. C. trum disorders. Behavior Therapy, 43, 142–152.
(2003). Childhood anxiety disorders: Parent-child Davis, T. E., III, Munson, M., & Tarcza, E. (2009).
(dis)agreement using a structured interview for the Anxiety disorders and phobias. In J. Matson (Ed.),
DSM-IV. Journal of the American Academy of Child Social behavior and social skills in children (pp. 219–
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 957–964. 244). New York: Springer.
Chronis-Tuscano, A., Degnan, K. A., Pine, D. S., Perez-­ Davis, T. E., III, & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). A critical
Edgar, K., Henderson, H. A., Diaz, Y., … Fox, N. A. review of empirically supported treatments for specific
(2009). Stable early maternal report of behavioral phobia in children: Do efficacious treatments address
inhibition predicts lifetime social anxiety disorder in the components of a phobic response? Clinical
adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Psychology: Science and Practice, 12, 144–160.
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(9), 928–935. Davis, T. E., III, Ollendick, T. H., & Nebel-Schwalm,
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1996). Equifinality and M. (2008). Intellectual ability and achievement in
multifinality in developmental psychopathology. anxiety-disordered children: A clarification and exten-
Development and Psychopathology, 8(04), 597–600. sion of the literature. Journal of Psychopathology and
Coles, M. E., & Heimberg, R. G. (2000). Patterns of anx- Behavioral Assessment, 30, 43–51.
ious arousal during exposure to feared situations in Davis, T. E., III, Reuther, E., May, A., Rudy, B., Munson,
individuals with social phobia. Behaviour Research M., Jenkins, W., & Whiting, S. (2013). The behavioral
and Therapy, 38, 405–424. avoidance task using imaginal exposure (BATIE): A
Compton, S., Peris, T., Almirall, D., Birmaher, B., Sherrill, paper-and-pencil version of traditional in vivo behav-
J., Kendall, P., … Albano, A. (2015). Predictors and ioral avoidance tasks. Psychological Assessment, 25,
moderators of treatment response in childhood anxi- 1111–1119.
ety disorders: Results from the CAMS trial. Journal De Los Reyes, A. (2011). Introduction to the special
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 82, 212–224. section: More than measurement error: Discovering
Costello, E. J., Egger, H. L., Copeland, W., Erkanli, A., meaning behind informant discrepancies in clinical
& Angold, A. (2011). The developmental epidemiol- assessments of children and adolescents. Journal of
ogy of anxiety disorders: Phenomenology, prevalence, Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 40(1), 1–9.
and comorbidity. In W. K. Silverman & A. P. Field Demyttenaere, K., Bruffaerts, R., Posada-Villa, J.,
(Eds.), Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents Gasquet, I., Kovess, V., Lepine, J. P., … Chatterji,
(pp. 56–75). New York, NY: Cambridge University S. (2004). Prevalence, severity, and unmet need for
Press. treatment of mental disorders in the World Health
Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., & Organization World Mental Health Surveys. The
Angold, A. (2003). Prevalence and development of Journal of the American Medical Association,
psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. 291(21), 2581–2590.
Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 837–844. DiBartolo, P. M., & Grills, A. E. (2006). Who is best at
Craske, M., Treanor, M., Conway, C., Zbozinek, T., & predicting children’s anxiety in response to a social
Vervliet, B. (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy: An evaluative task? A comparison of child, parent, and
inhibitory learning approach. Behaviour Research and teacher reports. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20(5),
Therapy, 58, 10–23. 630–645.
Davis, T. E., III. (2009). PTSD, anxiety, and phobias. In Drobes, D. J., & Lang, P. J. (1995). Bioinformational theory
J. Matson, F. Andrasik, & M. Matson (Eds.), Treating and behavior therapy. In W. O’Donohue & L. Krasner
318 T.E. Davis III et al.

(Eds.), Theories of behavior therapy: Exploring dren. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
behavior change (pp. 229–257). Washington, DC: 46, 1246–1257.
American Psychological Association. Gregory, A., & Eley, T. (2007). Genetic influences on
Ehrenreich-May, J., & Chu, B. (2014). Transdiagnostic anxiety in children: What we’ve learned and where
treatments for children and adolescents: Principles we’re heading. Clinical Child and Family Psychology
and practice. New York: The Guildford Press. Review, 10, 199–212.
Elizabeth, J., King, N., Ollendick, T. H., Gullone, E., Gresham, F. M., & Elliot, S. N. (1990). Social skill rat-
Tonge, B., Watson, S., & Macdermott, S. (2006). ing system. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
Social anxiety disorder in children and youth: A Service.
research update on aetiological factors. Counselling Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (2008). Social skills
Psychology Quarterly, 19, 151–163. improvement system. Bloomington, MN: Pearson
Erath, S. A., Flanagan, K. S., & Bierman, K. L. (2007). Assessments.
Social anxiety and peer relations in early adolescence: Grills, A., & Ollendick, T. H. (2003). Multiple infor-
Behavioral and cognitive factors. Journal of Abnormal mant agreement and the Anxiety Disorders Interview
Child Psychology, 35(3), 405–416. Schedule for Parents and Children. Journal of
Fisak, B., & Grills-Taquechel, A. E. (2007). Parental mod- the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
eling, reinforcement, and information transfer: Risk Psychiatry, 42, 30–40.
factors in the development of child anxiety? Clinical Haynes, S. N. (2001). Introduction to the special section
Child and Family Psychology Review, 10, 213–231. on clinical applications of analogue behavioral obser-
Fisher, P. H., Masia-Warner, C., & Klein, R. G. (2004). vation. Psychological Assessment, 13, 3–4.
Skills for social and academic success: A school-­ Hayward, C., Varady, S., Albano, A. M., Thienemann, M.,
based intervention for social anxiety disorder in Henderson, L., & Schatzberg, A. F. (2000). Cognitive-­
adolescents. Clinical Child and Family Psychology behavioral group therapy for social phobia in female
Review, 7, 241–249. adolescents: Results of a pilot study. Journal of the
Flannery-Schroeder, E. C., & Kendall, P. C. (2000). Group Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39,
and individual cognitive-behavioral treatments for 721–726.
youth with anxiety disorders: A randomized clinical Heimberg, R., Stein, M., Hiripi, E., & Kessler, R. (2000).
trial. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 251–278. Trends in the prevalence of social phobia in the United
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional process- States: A synthetic cohort analysis of changes over
ing of fear: Exposure to corrective information. four decades. European Psychiatry, 15, 29–37.
Psychological Bulletin, 99, 20–35. Higa-McMillan, C. K., Francis, S. E., Rith-Najarian, L.,
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1998). Clinical applications of & Chorpita, B. F. (2016). Evidence base update: 50
bioinformational theory: Understanding anxiety and years of research on treatment for child and adoles-
its treatment. Behavior Therapy, 29, 675–690. cent anxiety. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
Fyer, A. (1993). Heritability of social anxiety: A brief Psychology, 45(2), 91–113.
review. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 54, 10–12. Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Biederman, J., Henin, A.,
Fyer, A., Mannuzza, S., Chapman, T., Martin, L., & Klein, Faraone, S. V., Davis, S., Harrington, K., &
D. (1995). Specificity in familial aggregation of pho- Rosenbaum, J. F. (2007). Behavioral inhibition in pre-
bic disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, school children at risk is a specific predictor of mid-
564–573. dle childhood social anxiety: A five-year follow-up.
Gallagher, B., & Cartwright-Hatton, S. (2008). The rela- Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics,
tionship between parenting factors and trait anxiety: 28(3), 225–233.
Mediating role of cognitive errors and metacognition. Hudson, J., Keers, R., Roberts, S., Coleman, J., Breen,
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22(4), 722–733. G., Arendt, K., … Eley, T. (2015). Clinical predictors
Gallagher, H., Rabian, B., & McCloskey, M. (2004). A of response to cognitive-behavioral therapy in pediat-
brief cognitive-behavioral intervention for social pho- ric anxiety disorders: The genes for treatment (GxT)
bia in childhood. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18, study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
459–479. Adolescent Psychiatry, 54, 454–463.
Gazelle, H., Workman, J. O., & Allan, W. (2010). Anxious Hudson, J., Rapee, R., Lyneham, H., McLellan, L.,
solitude and clinical disorder in middle childhood: Wuthrich, V., & Schniering, C. (2015). Comparing
Bridging developmental and clinical approaches to outcomes for children with different anxiety disorders
childhood social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Child following cognitive behavioral therapy. Behaviour
Psychology, 38(1), 1–17. Research and Therapy, 72, 30–37.
Ginsburg, G., La Greca, A., & Silverman, W. (1998). Jenson, P. S., Rubio-Stipec, M., Canino, G., Bird, H. R.,
Social anxiety in children with anxiety disorders: Dulcan, M. K., Schwab-Stone, M. E., & Lahey, B. B.
Relation with social and emotional functioning. (1999). Parent and child contributions to diagnosis of
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 175–185. mental disorder: Are both informants always neces-
Glennon, B., & Weisz, J. R. (1978). An observational sary? Journal of the American Academy of Child and
approach to the assessment of anxiety in young chil- Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1569–1579.
Anxiety Disorders 319

Jorm, A., & Wright, A. (2008). Influences on young peo- rent validity. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,
ple’s stigmatising attitudes towards peers with mental 17, 84–91.
disorders: National survey of young Australians and La Greca, A., & Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among
their parents. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 192, adolescents: Linkages with peer relationships and
144–149. friendships. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
Kagan, J., Reznick, J., Clarke, C., Snidman, N., & Garcia 26, 83–94.
Coll, C. (1984). Behavioral inhibition to the unfamil- Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emo-
iar. Child Development, 55, 2212–2225. tional imagery. Psychophysiology, 16, 495–512.
Kagan, J., Reznick, J., & Snidman, N. (1987). The physi- Lang, P. J., Cuthbert, B. N., & Bradley, M. M. (1998).
ology and psychology of behavioral inhibition. Child Measuring emotion in therapy: Imagery, activation,
Development, 58, 1459–1473. and feeling. Behavior Therapy, 29, 655–674.
Kendall, P. C. (1990). The coping cat workbook. Ardmore, Lyneham, H. J., Abbott, M. J., & Rapee, R. M. (2007).
PA: Workbook. Interrater reliability of the Anxiety Disorders
Kendall, P. C. (1994). Treating anxiety disorders in chil- Interview Schedule for DSM-IV: Child and parent
dren: Results of a randomized clinical trial. Journal version. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 100–110. Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(6), 731–736.
Kendall, P. C. (1997). Therapy for youths with anxiety Mannuzza, S., Schneier, F., Chapman, T., Liebowitz,
disorders: A second randomized clinical trial. Journal M., Klein, D., & Fyer, A. (1995). Generalized social
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, 366–380. phobia: Reliability and validity. Archives of General
Kendall, P. C., Kane, M., Howard, B., & Siqueland, L. Psychiatry, 52, 230–237.
(1990). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for anxious chil- March, J. S. (2013). Multidimensional anxiety scale for
dren: Treatment manual. Ardmore, PA: Workbook. children (2nd ed.). Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-­
Kendall, P. C., Safford, S., Flannery-Schroeder, E. C., & Health Systems.
Webb, A. (2004). Child anxiety treatment: Outcomes in Marks, I. (2002). Innate and learned fears are at oppo-
adolescence and impact on substance use and depres- site ends of a continuum of associability. Behaviour
sion at 7.4 year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Research and Therapy, 40, 165–167.
Clinical Psychology, 72, 276–287. Mash, E., & Dozois, D. (2003). Child psychopathology:
Kendall, P. C., Hudson, J. L., Gosch, E., Flannery-­ A developmental-systems perspective. In E. Mash &
Schroeder, E., & Suveg, C. (2008). Cognitive– R. Barkley (Eds.), Child psychopathology (2nd ed.,
Behavioral Therapy for Anxiety Disordered Youth: pp. 3–71). New York: Guilford Press.
A Randomized Clinical Trial Evaluating Child and Masia, C., & Morris, T. (1998). Parental factors associ-
Family Modalities. Journal of Consulting and Clinical ated with social anxiety: Methodological limitations
Psychology, 76(2), 282–297. and suggestions for integrated behavioral research.
Kendall, P. C., Gosch, E., Furr, J. M., & Sood, E. (2008). Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5,
Flexibility within Fidelity. Journal of the American 211–228.
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(9), Masia-Warner, C., Fisher, P. H., Shrout, P. E., Rathor,
987–993. S., & Klein, R. G. (2007). Treating adolescent with
Kessler, R., Chiu, W., Demler, O., & Walters, E. (2005). social anxiety disorder in school: An attention control
Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-months trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48,
DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity sur- 676–686.
vey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, Matson, J. L., Neal, D., Hess, J. A., Fodstad, J. C., Mahan,
617–627. S., & Rivet, T. T. (2010). Reliability and validity of the
Kessler, R. C., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters
A. M., & Wittchen, H. (2012). Twelve-month and life- (MESSY). Behavior Modification, 34, 539–558.
time prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety Matson, J. L., Neal, D., Worley, J. A., Kozlowski, A. M.,
and mood disorders in the United States. International & Fodstad, J. C. (2012). Factor structure of the
Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 21(3), Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters-II
169–184. (MESSY-II). Research in Developmental Disabilities,
King, N. J., & Gullone, E. (1990). Acceptability of 33, 2067–2071.
fear reduction procedures with children. Journal of Matson, J. L., Rotatori, A. F., & Helsel, W. J. (1983).
Behavioral Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 21, Development of a rating scale to measure social skills
1–8. in children: The Matson Evaluation of Social Skills
Kley, H., Heinrichs, N., Bender, C., & Tuschen-Caffier, with Youngsters (MESSY). Behaviour Research and
B. (2012). Predictors of outcome in a cognitive-­ Therapy, 21, 335–340.
behavioral group program for children and a­ dolescents McCabe, K., Clark, R., & Barnett, D. (1999). Family pro-
with social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety tective factors among urban African American youth.
Disorders, 26, 79–87. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28, 137–150.
La Greca, A. M., Dandes, S. K., Wick, P., Shaw, K., & McLaughlin, K. A., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., & Hilt, L. M.
Stone, W. L. (1988). Development of the Social (2009). Emotion dysregulation as a mechanism link-
Anxiety Scale for Children: Reliability and concur- ing peer victimization to internalizing symptoms
320 T.E. Davis III et al.

in adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical ing. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Psychology, 77(5), 894. Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1270–1278.
McLeod, B. D., Wood, J. J., & Weisz, J. R. (2007). Nebel-Schwalm, M., & Davis, T. E., III. (2011).
Examining the association between parenting Preliminary factor and psychometric analysis of the
and childhood anxiety: A meta-analysis. Clinical Motivation for Fear (MOTIF) Survey. Journal of
Psychology Review, 27(2), 155–172. Anxiety Disorders, 25, 731–740.
Merikangas, K. R., He, J. P., Burstein, M., Swanson, Oh, W., Rubin, K., Bowker, J., Booth-LaForce, C., Rose-­
S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., … Swendsen, J. (2010). Krasnor, L., & Laursen, B. (2008). Trajectories of
Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in US ado- social withdrawal from middle childhood to early ado-
lescents: Results from the National Comorbidity lescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36,
Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-­ 553–566.
A). Journal of the American Academy of Child & Ollendick, T. H. (1979). Fear reduction techniques with
Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(10), 980–989. children. In M. Hersen, R. M. Eisler, & P. M. Miller
Michelson, L., & Wood, R. (1982). Development and (Eds.), Progress in behavior modification (vol. 8,
psychometric properties of the Children’s Assertive pp. 127–168). New York: Academic.
Behavior Scale. Journal of Behavioral Assessment, 4, Ollendick, T. H. (1981). Assessment of social interac-
3–13. tion skills in school children. Behavioral Counseling
Mineka, S., & Oehlberg, K. (2008). The relevance of Quarterly, 1, 227–243.
recent developments in classical conditioning to Ollendick, T. H. (1983). Reliability and validity of the
understanding the etiology and maintenance of anxi- Revised Fear Survey Schedule for Children (FSSC-R).
ety disorders. Acta Psychologica, 127(3), 567–580. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 21, 395–399.
Mineka, S., & Zinbarg, R. (2006). A contemporary learn- Ollendick, T. H., & Benoit, K. E. (2012). A parent–child
ing theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety dis- interactional model of social anxiety disorder in youth.
orders. American Psychologist, 61, 10–26. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 15(1),
Mori, L. T., & Armendariz, G. M. (2001). Analogue 81–91.
assessment of child behavior problems. Psychological Ollendick, T. H., Davis, T. E., & Muris, P. (2004).
Assessment, 13(1), 36. Treatment of specific phobia in children and ado-
Muris, P., Meesters, C., & van Melick, M. (2002). lescents. In P. Barrett & T. H. Ollendick (Eds.), The
Treatment of childhood anxiety disorders: A pre- handbook of interventions that work with children and
liminary comparison between cognitive-behavioral adolescents: From prevention to treatment (pp. 273–
group therapy and a psychological placebo interven- 299). West Sussex, England: Wiley.
tion. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Ollendick, T. H., & Hersen, M. (1993). Child and ado-
Psychiatry, 33, 143–158. lescent behavioral assessment. In T. H. Ollendick &
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., de Jong, P., & Ollendick, T. H. M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of child and adolescent
(2002). The etiology of specific fears and phobias in assessment (pp. 3–14). New York: Pergamon.
children: A critique of the non-associative account. Ollendick, T. H., & Hirshfeld-Becker, D. (2002). The
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 185–195. developmental psychopathology of social anxiety dis-
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Schmidt, H., & Mayer, B. order. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 44–58.
(1999). The revised version of the Screen for Child Ollendick, T. H., & Horsch, L. (2007). Fears in clinic-­
Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED-R): referred children: Relations with child anxiety sen-
Factor structure in normal children. Personality and sitivity, maternal overcontrol, and maternal phobic
Individual Differences, 26, 99–112. anxiety. Behavior Therapy, 38, 402–411.
Muris, P., & Merkelbach, H. (2000). How serious are Ollendick, T. H., Lewis, K., Cowart, M., & Davis, T. E.,
common childhood fears? II. The parent’s point of III. (2012). Prediction of child performance on a
view. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 813–818. parent-­ child behavioral approach test with animal
Muris, P., Merkelbach, H., Mayer, B., & Prins, E. (2000). phobic children. Behavior Modification, 36, 509–524.
How serious are common childhood fears? Behaviour Ollendick, T. H., Öst, L. G., Reuterskiöld, L., Costa, N.,
Research and Therapy, 38, 217–228. Cederlund, R., Sirbu, C., … Jarrett, M. (2009). One-­
Muris, P., & Ollendick, T. (2015). Children who are anx- session treatment of specific phobias in youth: A ran-
ious in silence: A review on selective mutism, the new domized clinical trial in the USA and Sweden. Journal
anxiety disorder in DSM-5. Clinical Child and Family of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 504–516.
Psychology Review, 18, 151–169. Öst, L., Cederlund, R., & Reuterskiöld, L. (2015).
Muris, P., & Steerneman, P. (2001). The revised version Behavioral treatment of social phobia in youth: Does
of the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional parent education training improve the outcome?
Disorders (SCARED-R): First evidence for its reliabil- Behaviour Research and Therapy, 67, 19–29.
ity and validity in a clinical sample. British Journal of Pérez-Edgar, K., Reeb-Sutherland, B. C., McDermott,
Clinical Psychology, 40, 35–44. J. M., White, L. K., Henderson, H. A., Degnan, K. A.,
Nauta, M. H., Scholing, A., Emmelkamp, P. M. G., & … Fox, N. A. (2011). Attention biases to threat link
Minderaa, R. B. (2003). Cognitive-behavioral therapy behavioral inhibition to social withdrawal over time
for children with anxiety disorders in a clinical set- in very young children. Journal of Abnormal Child
ting: No additional effect of a cognitive parent train- Psychology, 39(6), 885–895.
Anxiety Disorders 321

Podell, J. L., Mychailyszyn, M., Edmunds, J., Puleo, to social self-efficacy among differing relation-
C. M., & Kendall, P. C. (2010). The Coping Cat ships. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Program for anxious youth: The FEAR plan comes Assessment, 35, 106–112.
to life. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 17(2), Salzman, C. D., & Fusi, S. (2010). Emotion, cognition,
132–141. and mental state representation in amygdala and pre-
Puliafico, A., & Kendall, P. C. (2006). Threat-related frontal cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33,
attentional bias in anxious youth: A review. Clinical 173–202.
Child and Family Psychology Review, 9, 162–180. Scharfstein, L., Beidel, D., Finnell, L., Distler, A., &
Rachman, S., & Hodgson, R. (1974). Desynchrony in Carter, N. (2011). Do pharmacological and behav-
measures of fear. Behaviour Research and Therapy, ioral interventions differentially affect treatment
12, 319–326. outcome for children with social phobia? Behavior
Ranta, K., Kaltiala-Heino, R., Pelkonen, M., & Marttunen, Modification, 35, 451–467.
M. (2009). Associations between peer victimization, Schniering, C. A., Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2000).
self-reported depression and social phobia among Issues in the diagnosis and assessment of anxi-
adolescents: The role of comorbidity. Journal of ety disorders in children and adolescents. Clinical
Adolescence, 32(1), 77–93. Psychology Review, 20(4), 453–478.
Rapee, R. M., Schniering, C. A., & Hudson, J. L. (2009). Schniering, C. A., & Rapee, R. M. (2002). Development
Anxiety disorders during childhood and adolescence: and validation of a measure of children’s automatic
Origins and treatment. Annual Review of Clinical thoughts: The Children’s Automatic Thoughts Scale.
Psychology, 5, 311–341. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1091–1109.
Rapee, R., & Spence, S. (2004). The etiology of social Segool, N. K., & Carlson, J. S. (2008). Efficacy of
phobia: Empirical evidence and an initial model. cognitive-­behavioral and pharmacological treatments
Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 737–767. for children with social anxiety. Depression and
Reijntjes, A., Kamphuis, J. H., Prinzie, P., & Telch, M. J. Anxiety, 25, 620–631.
(2010). Peer victimization and internalizing problems Settipani, C., & Kendall, P. (2013). Social functioning in
in children: A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. youth with anxiety disorders: Association with anxi-
Child abuse & Neglect, 34(4), 244–252. ety severity and outcomes from cognitive-behavioral
Reuther, E., Davis, T. E., III, Moree, B., & Matson, J. L. therapy. Child Psychiatry and Human Development,
(2011). Treating selective mutism using modular CBT 44, 1–18.
for anxiety: A case study. Journal of Clinical Child Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C., Dulcan, M. K., &
and Adolescent Psychology, 40, 156–163. Schwab-Stone, M. E. (2000). NIMH Diagnostic
Reynolds, C., & Kamphaus, R. (2015). Behavior assess- Interview Schedule for Children Version IV (NIMH
ment system for children, third edition (BASC-3). DISC-IV): Description, differences from previous
Bloomington, MN: Pearson. versions, and reliability of some common diagno-
Reynolds, C. R., & Richmond, B. O. (2008). Revised ses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Children’s manifest anxiety scale–second edition Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 28–38.
(RCMAS-2). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Shimada-Sugimoto, M., Otowa, T., & Hettema, J. (2015).
Services. Genetics of anxiety disorders: Genetic epidemiologi-
Ritter, B. (1965). The treatment of a dissection phobia. cal and molecular studies in humans. Psychiatry and
Unpublished manuscript, Queens College. Clinical Neurosciences, 69, 388–401.
Ritter, B. (1968). The group desensitization of children’s Shin, L. M., & Liberzon, I. (2010). The neuro-
snake phobias using vicarious and contact desensitiza- circuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders.
tion procedures. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 6, Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 169–191.
1–6. Shortt, A. L., Barrett, P. M., & Fox, T. L. (2001).
Ronan, K. R., Kendall, P. C., & Rowe, M. (1994). Negative Evaluating the FRIENDS program: A cognitive-­
affectivity in children: Development and validation of behavioral group treatment for anxious children and
a self-statement questionnaire. Cognitive Therapy and their parents. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,
Research, 18, 509–528. 30, 525–535.
Rubin, K. H. (2014). The Waterloo longitudinal project: Siegel, R. S., La Greca, A. M., & Harrison, H. M. (2009).
Correlates and consequences of social withdrawal Peer victimization and social anxiety in adolescents:
from childhood to adolescence. In K. H. Rubin & J. B. Prospective and reciprocal relationships. Journal of
Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and Youth and Adolescence, 38(8), 1096–1109.
shyness in childhood (pp. 291–314). Hillsdale, NJ: Silverman, W. K. (1994). Structured diagnostic inter-
L. Erlbaum Associates. views. In T. H. Ollendick, N. J. King, & W. Yule
Rubin, K., Wojslawowicz, J., Rose-Krasnor, L., Booth-­ (Eds.), International handbook of phobic and anxiety
LaForce, C., & Burgess, K. (2006). The best friend- disorders in children and adolescents (pp. 293–315).
ships of shy/withdrawn children: Prevalence, stability, New York: Plenum.
and relationship quality. Journal of Abnormal Child Silverman, W. K., & Albano, A. M. (1996). Anxiety
Psychology, 34, 143–157. Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV (child and
Rudy, B., May, A., Matthews, R., & Davis, T. E., III. parent versions). San Antonio, TX: Psychological
(2013). Youth’s negative self-statements as related Corporation.
322 T.E. Davis III et al.

Silverman, W. K., Fleisig, W., Rabian, B., & Peterson, Storch, E. A., Masia-Warner, C., Crisp, H., & Klein, R. G.
R. A. (1991). Childhood anxiety sensitivity index. (2005). Peer victimization and social anxiety in ado-
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20, 162–168. lescence: A prospective study. Aggressive Behavior,
Silverman, W. K., Kurtines, W. M., Ginsburg, G. S., 31, 437–452.
Weems, C. F., Rabian, B., & Serafini, L. T. (1999). Strauss, C., Lahey, B., Frick, P., Frame, C., & Hynd,
Contingency management, self-control, and education G. (1988). Peer social status of children with anxi-
support in the treatment of childhood phobic disorders: ety disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Psychology, 56, 137–141.
Clinical Psychology, 67, 675–687. Strauss, C., Lease, C., Kazdin, A., Dulcan, M., & Last,
Silverman, W., & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). Evidence-based C. (1989). Multimethod assessment of the social com-
assessment of anxiety and its disorders in children and petence of children with anxiety disorders. Journal of
adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Clinical Child Psychology, 18, 184–189.
Psychology, 34, 380–411. Szyf, M., McGowan, P., & Meaney, M. (2008). The social
Silverman, W. K., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2008). The second environment and the epigenome. Environmental and
special issue on evidence-based psychosocial treat- Molecular Mutagenesis, 49, 46–60.
ments for children and adolescents: A 10-year update. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and devel-
Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, opment. New York: Brunner/Mazel.
37(1), 1–7. Thomas, A., Chess, S., & Birch, H. (1968). Temperament
Silverman, W. K., Saavedra, L. M., & Pina, A. A. (2001). and behavior disorders in children. New York:
Test-retest reliability of anxiety symptoms and diag- New York University Press.
noses using the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule Thompson-Hollands, J., Kerns, C. E., Pincus, D. B., &
for DSM-IV: Child and Parent Versions (ADIS for Comer, J. S. (2014). Parental accommodation of child
DSM-IV: C/P). Journal of the American Academy of anxiety and related symptoms: Range, impact, and cor-
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 937–944. relates. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 28(8), 765–773.
Sondaite, J., & Zukauskiene, R. (2005). Adolescents’ Trzaskowski, M., Eley, T. C., Davis, O. S., Doherty,
social strategies: Patterns and correlates. Scandinavian S. J., Hanscombe, K. B., Meaburn, E. L., … Plomin,
Journal of Psychology, 46, 367–374. R. (2013). First genome-wide association study on
Spence, S. H. (1995). Social skills training: Enhancing anxiety-­related behaviours in childhood. PLoS One,
social competence with children and adolescents. 8(4), e58676.
Windsor, Berkshire, England: NFER-Nelson. Verduin, T. L., & Kendall, P. C. (2008). Peer perceptions
Spence, S., Donovan, C., & Brechman-Toussaint, M. and liking of children with anxiety disorders. Journal
(1999). Social skills, social outcomes, and cogni- of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 459–469.
tive features of childhood social phobia. Journal of Walkup, J. T., Albano, A. M., Piacentini, J., Birmaher,
Abnormal Psychology, 108, 211–221. B., Compton, S. N., Sherrill, J. T., … Kendall, P. C.
Spence, S. H., Donovan, C., & Brechman-Toussaint, M. (2008). Cognitive behavioral therapy, sertraline, or a
(2000). The treatment of childhood social phobia: combination in childhood anxiety. The New England
The effectiveness of a social skills training-based, Journal of Medicine, 359, 2753–2766.
cognitive-behavioural intervention, with and without Weems, C. F., Silverman, W. K., Saavedra, L. M., Pina,
parent involvement. Journal of Child Psychology and A. A., & Lumpkin, P. W. (1999). The discrimination
Psychiatry, 41, 713–726. of children’s phobias using the Revised Fear Survey
Spielberger, C. D. (1973). Manual for the state-trait anxi- Schedule for Children. Journal of Child Psychology
ety inventory for children. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting and Psychiatry, 40, 941–952.
Psychologists Press. Wergeland, G., Fjermestad, K., Marin, C., Bjelland, I.,
Stein, M., Chartier, M., Kozak, M., King, N., & Kennedy, Haugland, B., Silverman, W., … Heiervang, E. (2016).
J. (1998). Genetic linkage to the serotonin transporter Predictors of treatment outcome in an effectiveness
protein and 5HT2A receptor genes excluded in gen- trial of cognitive behavioral therapy for children with
eralized social phobia. Psychiatry Research, 81, anxiety disorders. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
283–291. 76, 1–12.
Stein, M., Jang, K., & Livesley, W. (2002). Heritability Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition.
of social anxiety-related concerns and personality Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
characteristics: A twin study. Journal of Nervous and Wood, J. J., McLeod, B. D., Sigman, M., Hwang, W. C.,
Mental Disease, 190, 219–224. & Chu, B. C. (2003). Parenting and childhood anxi-
Storch, E. A., Ledley, D., Lewin, A., Murphy, T., Johns, ety: Theory, empirical findings, and future directions.
N., Goodman, W., & Geffken, G. R. (2006). Peer vic- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44,
timization in children with obsessive-compulsive dis- 134–151.
order: Relations with symptoms of psychopathology. Wright, A., Jorm, A. F., & Mackinnon, A. J. (2011).
Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, Labeling of mental disorders and stigma in young
35, 446–455. people. Social Science & Medicine, 73(4), 498–506.
Evidence-Based Methods
of Dealing with Social Difficulties
in Conduct Disorder

Kimberly Renk, J’Nelle Stephenson, Maria Khan,


and Annelise Cunningham

A significant number of children are diagnosed Keenan, Shaw, Delliquadri, Giovannelli, &
with Conduct Disorder today, with each of these Walsh, 1998; Keenan et al., 2011; Kim-Cohen
children experiencing significant difficulties in et al., 2009).
their emotional and behavioral functioning and Given the difficulties that are associated
in their social relationships. Estimates suggest with Conduct Disorder, health service provid-
that the incidence of Conduct Disorder in young ers should have several goals in mind when
children may be as high as 35% (Webster- they work with families who have a child with
Stratton & Hammond, 1998). A significant per- Conduct Disorder. First, health service provid-
centage of older children and adolescents also ers must understand the diagnostic criteria
are affected (2–3%, Maughan, Rowe, Messer, used to identify Conduct Disorder. Next, health
Goodman, & Meltzer, 2004; 9.5%, Nock, service providers should use appropriate
Kazdin, Hiripi, & Kessler, 2006; 2–10%, APA, assessment measures to identify Conduct
2013), with males showing higher rates of diag- Disorder. Finally, health service providers
nosis than females (12% of males versus 7.1% should implement interventions that have the
of females; APA, 2013; Nock et al., 2006). strongest evidence base as they work to achieve
Given the emotional, behavioral, and social dif- positive outcomes for children with Conduct
ficulties that accompany Conduct Disorder, this Disorder. Given the importance of these goals
diagnosis often is cited as the most common for bettering the outcomes of children with
reason for mental health service referrals (e.g., Conduct Disorder, this chapter will examine
in preschoolers, Luby & Morgan, 1997; in the diagnostic criteria from the Diagnostic and
school-age children, Foster, Kelsch, Kamradt, Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-Fifth
Sosna, & Yang, 2001). Of even greater clinical Edition (APA, 2013) that are used to identify
significance, the behaviors that are associated Conduct Disorder. Also, we briefly survey eti-
with Conduct Disorder (e.g., aggression) show ological factors that may contribute to conduct
significant stability over time (Frick, 2016; problems in children and adolescents, high-
light relevant assessment instruments for mak-
ing a diagnosis of Conduct Disorder, and
K. Renk, Ph.D. (*) • J. Stephenson • M. Khan identify the interventions that can be used to
A. Cunningham promote the best emotional, behavioral, and
Department of Psychology, University of Central psychosocial outcomes for children and ado-
Florida, P.O. Box 161390, Orlando, FL 32816, USA
e-mail: Kimberly.Renk@ucf.edu
lescents with Conduct Disorder.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 323


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_17
324 K. Renk et al.

 onduct Disorder Diagnostic


C 18 years of age and older when behaviors demon-
Criteria strating a persistent disregard for and violation of
the rights of others are present; APA, 2013).
As described in the most recent version of the When making a Conduct Disorder diagnosis in a
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental child or adolescent, age is also important when
Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5; APA, 2013), considering descriptive specifiers for the Conduct
Conduct Disorder consists of a repetitive or per- Disorder diagnosis. In fact, specific Conduct
sistent pattern of behavior in which the basic Disorder subtypes describe the age when symp-
rights of others and/or major age-appropriate toms are first manifested. A Childhood-Onset
societal norms or rules are violated. Given this Type of Conduct Disorder is diagnosed when
description, difficulties with emotional and children display at least one criterion prior to the
behavioral functioning and in social relationships age of 10 years, whereas an Adolescent-Onset
are inherent in a Conduct Disorder diagnosis. Type of Conduct Disorder is diagnosed when
More specifically, to meet criteria for Conduct children do not exhibit any of the criteria prior to
Disorder, the DSM-5 requires that children the age of 10 years. Further, an Unspecified-­
exhibit at least 3 of 15 criteria. These criteria fall Onset Type of Conduct Disorder is diagnosed
into four categories, including aggressive behav- when the age of criteria onset is unknown (APA,
ior that threatens or causes physical harm to other 2013). Generally, research suggests that those
individuals or animals (e.g., bullying, threaten- who are diagnosed with the Childhood-Onset
ing, or intimidating other individuals; initiating Type of Conduct Disorder exhibit a more prob-
physical fights; using weapons to cause physical lematic course of aggressive and violent symp-
harm; being physically cruel to animals and/or to toms as well as a poorer outcome over time
other individuals; engaging in confrontational relative to those who are diagnosed with the
stealing; forcing another individual into sexual Adolescent-Onset Type (Dandreaux & Frick,
activity), destruction of property (e.g., deliber- 2009; Frick & Loney, 1999; Moffitt, 1993;
ately engaging in fire setting, deliberately Moffitt & Caspi, 2001).
destroying others’ property), deceitfulness or When diagnosing Conduct Disorder, the
theft (e.g., breaking into another individual’s severity of the disorder also is specified. Conduct
home or property, lying to obtain goods or avoid Disorder is considered to be “mild” if few con-
obligations, stealing items without confronta- duct problems are exhibited in excess of those
tion), and serious rule violations (e.g., staying out required to make the diagnosis and if these prob-
at night despite parental prohibitions, running lems cause only minor harm to other individuals.
away from home, being truant; APA, 2013). In contrast, Conduct Disorder is considered to be
These criteria must occur persistently, being “moderate” if the number of conduct problems
present for at least 12 months and with at least and their effect on other individuals are between
one criterion occurring in the past 6 months. The mild and severe. Lastly, Conduct Disorder is con-
presence of these criteria also must cause clini- sidered to be “severe” if many conduct problems
cally significant difficulty in social, academic, or are exhibited in excess of those required to make
occupational functioning (APA, 2013), suggest- the diagnosis and if these problems cause consid-
ing inherent impairments in the social interac- erable harm to other individuals (APA, 2013).
tions of children and adolescents. Thus, children’s impact on other individuals is
When identifying Conduct Disorder, age is an critical in determining the degree of Conduct
important consideration. In particular, the DSM-5 Disorder severity.
states that, if the individual is 18 years of age or Finally, a specifier for callous-unemotional
older, the individual being diagnosed with traits (i.e., “With Limited Prosocial Emotions”)
Conduct Disorder must not meet criteria for now is included with the DSM-5 Conduct
Antisocial Personality Disorder (i.e., a personal- Disorder criteria (APA, 2013). To be assigned
ity disorder diagnosed in individuals who are this specifier, the child or adolescent must display
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 325

at least two of four specific criteria over the last mechanism. The most common etiological fac-
12 months, with these criteria occurring across tors associated with Conduct Disorder are dis-
multiple relationships and settings. These criteria cussed briefly here.
include lack of remorse or guilt (e.g., the indi- As part of this discussion, it is important to note
vidual shows a general lack of concern about the that etiological factors may interact differentially
negative consequences of his or her actions), cal- with the age of symptom onset and with sex
lous lack of empathy (e.g., the individual is more (Silverthorn & Frick, 1999). For example, biologi-
concerned about the effects of his or her actions cal makeup and individual characteristics (e.g.,
on himself or herself, rather than on others), temperament) as well as psychosocial factors (e.g.,
being unconcerned about performance (e.g., the familial dysfunction, poverty) are associated with
individual does not put forth the effort necessary the development of the Childhood-Onset Type of
to perform well or blames others for his or her Conduct Disorder (Moffitt & Caspi, 2001). As a
performance), and showing shallow or deficient result, the behaviors associated with the Childhood-
affect (e.g., the individual does not express feel- Onset Type are more likely to increase during the
ings or show emotions to others). Individuals adolescent years and to persist into adulthood
who meet criteria for this specifier are thought to (Moffitt, 1993; Moffitt & Caspi, 2001). In contrast,
have a more severe and aggressive form of the Adolescent-Onset Type of Conduct Disorder is
Conduct Disorder (APA, 2013) and to be at associated with increased socialization with devi-
greater risk for poorer developmental outcomes ant peers and the need to gain autonomy (Frick,
(Frick, Ray, Thornton, & Kahn, 2014). Although 2004a; Moffitt, 1993; Moffitt & Caspi, 2001). As a
studies still are needed to determine the utility of result, children with the Adolescent-Onset Type
this new specifier, some studies are noting some may benefit more so from interventions that are
limited incremental utility for predicting reactive designed to improve their social skills. Further,
aggression (i.e., 7% of the variance; Jambroes research suggests that Conduct Disorder is diag-
et al., 2016). Nonetheless, with the addition of nosed rarely in girls during childhood (Silverthorn
this specifier to the Conduct Disorder criteria, the & Frick, 1999). Although girls may not display
social functioning of children and adolescents behavior that is consistent with Conduct Disorder
has become even more crucial for understanding until adolescence, their risk factors are similar to
the Conduct Disorder diagnosis. those of the Childhood-Onset Type (Moffitt &
Caspi, 2001). Given such findings, age and sex
may be important considerations for identifying
Conduct Disorder Etiology potential etiological mechanisms, identifying help-
ful assessment instruments, and selecting the most
Although health service providers must be effective interventions.
knowledgeable about Conduct Disorder criteria
to make an accurate diagnosis, they also should
seek to understand the potential etiological fac- Biological Factors
tors that may promote conduct problems in chil-
dren and adolescents. In fact, several biological, Genetics. Genetic factors are key in understand-
individual, and psychosocial risk factors associ- ing the etiology of Conduct Disorder. Although it
ated with the etiology of Conduct Disorder have can be difficult to separate genetic contributions
been identified (e.g., see Frick, 2004a, for a from psychosocial risk factors, research suggests
review). In considering these factors, it is impor- that genetic factors account for a considerable
tant to note that the etiology of Conduct Disorder amount of variance in the development of
usually involves several interacting factors (Frick Conduct Disorder (Arseneault et al., 2003;
& Ellis, 1999), rather than one simple underlying Holmes, Slaughter, & Kashani, 2001; Silberg,
326 K. Renk et al.

Maes, & Eaves, 2012). For example, research when combined with psychosocial factors, such
examining Conduct Disorder symptoms in twins as child abuse or neglect, however (Caspi et al.,
indicates that monozygotic twins display more 2002; Fergusson et al., 2011; Ficks & Waldman,
similarities in the degree of their conduct prob- 2014). Research suggests that adrenal androgen
lems and antisocial behavior relative to dizygotic functioning also may be higher in children who
twins (Reid, Dorr, Walker, & Bonner, 1986; Rhee exhibit the oppositional behaviors (Shenk et al.,
& Waldman, 2002). Similarly, Arseneault et al. 2012; van Goozen et al., 2000) that may be
(2003) describe a stronger genetic contribution related to conduct problems.
for the severe, pervasive conduct problems of Further, the relationship between specific neu-
5-year-old children relative to those of children roanatomical structures and the development of
with an older age of onset. A more recent study Conduct Disorder has been examined as well.
(i.e., Children of Twins) also suggests that there For example, research documents differences in
is a strong genetic relationship between parents’ gray matter volume between children with
antisocial behavior and children’s conduct prob- Conduct Disorder and those who do not have
lems, even after controlling for a variety of other such a diagnosis (Fairchild et al., 2011, 2015;
genetic influences and environmental factors Matthys, Vanderschuren, Schutter, & Lochman,
(Silberg et al., 2012). Collectively, research doc- 2012). Specifically, research describes cortical
uments a significant genetic contribution for con- thinning in the superior temporal gyrus, increased
duct problems, particularly when those problems insula cortical folding, and reduced orbitofrontal
have an early onset. cortex (OFC) surface area, with OFC surface area
Neurophysiological Factors. The contribu- being correlated negatively with the number of
tion of neurophysiological factors to the develop- Conduct Disorder symptoms that are exhibited
ment of conduct problems also has been examined (Fairchild et al., 2015). Further, differences in
(e.g., Kim-Cohen et al., 2006; Manuck et al., white matter structure also have been docu-
1999), particularly with regard to the relationship mented. For example, Passamonti et al. (2012)
between the neurotransmitter serotonin and dis- note a relationship between Conduct Disorder
plays of aggression. For example, Kruesi et al. and abnormalities in the uncinate fascicle (i.e.,
(1990) report that children who exhibit conduct the anatomical tract that connects the amygdala
problems and physical aggression have low lev- to the orbitofrontal cortex). Such findings may
els of 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA; a suggest that maturation of white matter pathways
metabolite of serotonin) in their cerebrospinal (i.e., those that are critical for emotional and
fluid (CSF). Further, CSF 5-HIAA levels predict behavioral regulation) is atypical (Passamonti
the severity of children’s subsequent physical et al., 2012). Overall, research suggests a com-
aggression (i.e., 2 years later; Kruesi et al., 1992). plex picture for the neurophysiological factors
There also are conflicting results related to CSF that may contribute to Conduct Disorder.
5-HIAA and aggression (Duke, Begue, Bell, & Prenatal Exposure to Substances. Exposure
Eisenlohr-Moul, 2013), however, suggesting that to certain substances in the prenatal environment
serotonin’s role in conduct problems may depend has been linked to the development of Conduct
on other child-specific factors. Disorder as well (Dodge & Pettit, 2003). Research
Other neurotransmitters may be important in suggests that fetal exposure to opiates or metha-
predicting conduct problems as well. For exam- done in utero may lead to conduct problems 10 to
ple, research suggests that low monoamine oxi- 13 years later (de Cubas & Field, 1993). Maternal
dase A (MAO-A) appears to act as a biological smoking during pregnancy also has been exam-
risk factor for the development of conduct prob- ined, with a link between smoking during preg-
lems (e.g., Caspi et al., 2002; Fergusson, Boden, nancy and child conduct problems being noted
Horwood, Miller, & Kennedy, 2011; Godar, Fite, consistently (Desrosiers et al., 2013; Fergusson,
McFarlin, & Bortolato, 2016; Kim-Cohen et al., Woodward, & Horwood, 1998; Gaysina et al.,
2003). MAO-A only is implicated as a risk factor 2013; Knopik, 2009). For example, Gatzke-Kopp
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 327

and Beauchaine (2007) report that mothers who problems due to their increased tendency to be in
smoke and those who are exposed to environ- unsafe and novel situations and due to their dif-
mental tobacco smoke during pregnancy (but ficulty with managing impulses and regulating
who do not smoke themselves) tend to have chil- emotions (Yaman, Mesman, van Ijzendoorn, &
dren with more Conduct Disorder symptoms. Bakermans-Kranenburg, 2010). Further, a
Further, prenatal exposure to alcohol, marijuana, 12-year longitudinal study suggests that the char-
and/or tobacco places the fetus at a considerably acteristics of children with difficult tempera-
higher risk for developing Conduct Disorder in ments at the ages of 3 and 5 years predict
the future (relative to those who are not exposed adolescent behaviors that are consistent with
to these substances prenatally; Day, Richardson, Conduct Disorder (Caspi, Henry, McGee, Moffitt,
Goldschmidt, & Cornelius, 2000). & Silva, 1995). Although the link between diffi-
The consumption of alcohol during preg- cult temperament and later conduct problems
nancy, in particular, has been associated with an may be indirect (e.g., dependent on psychosocial
early onset of persistent conduct problems in factors), these characteristics continually are
children (D’Onofrio et al., 2007; Murray et al., noted as risk factors for later conduct problems
2015). In other words, when comparing children (see Frick & Morris, 2004, for a review).
with prenatal alcohol exposure to children who Callous-Unemotional Traits. Research long
would be considered typically developing, higher has suggested that children who exhibit callous-­
rates of Oppositional Defiant Disorder and unemotional (CU) traits (e.g., a lack of empathy
Conduct Disorder have been found in those chil- and guilt) are more likely to develop severe con-
dren with prenatal exposure (Ware et al., 2013). duct problems in childhood (hence, the addition
The effects of prenatal exposure to substances of the new “With Limited Prosocial Emotions”
must be considered in relation to the develop- specifier to the current Conduct Disorder crite-
ment of cognitive deficits associated with each ria). In fact, it has been estimated that 10–46%
substance, however (Dodge & Pettit, 2003). Such and 21–59% of children and adolescents with
findings highlight the critical importance of the Conduct Disorder have CU traits in community
prenatal period for the development of Conduct and clinic samples, respectively (Kahn, Frick,
Disorder. Youngstrom, Findling, & Youngstrom, 2012;
Kolko & Pardini, 2010; Pardini & Frick, 2013).
Further, CU traits are linked to severe and persis-
Children’s Individual Characteristics tent behavior problems, suggesting an increased
likelihood for the symptoms to continue 1 year
Temperament. Temperament, or biologically later (Dadds, Fraser, Frost, & Hawes, 2005) and
based behavioral approaches and emotional dis- into adolescence and adulthood (Frick, Cornell,
positions that appear early in life (Bates, 2001; Barry, Bodin, & Dane, 2003; Pardini & Frick,
Calkins, Hungerford, & Dedmon, 2004), also 2013). For example, Frick et al. (2003) indicate
may be important to consider in relation to that children who exhibit both conduct problems
Conduct Disorder. In fact, research suggests that and CU traits show greater levels of conduct
children with difficult temperaments are at problems, aggression, and delinquent acts 1 year
greater risk for developing conduct problems and later relative to children who exhibit conduct
aggression in childhood (Frick & Viding, 2009), problems alone. Accordingly, CU traits are a
adolescence (Frick, 2012), and the future (see strong predictor of the development, severity,
Frick & Morris, 2004, for a review; Shaw, Owens, and persistence of Conduct Disorder.
Giovannelli, & Winslow, 2001). Generally, chil- Low Verbal Intelligence. Children who dis-
dren with difficult temperaments are character- play poorer language skills also have been identi-
ized as irritable, highly active, rigid, fied to be at risk for developing externalizing
unaffectionate, and aversive (Shaw et al., 2001). behavior problems (Menting, Van Lier, & Koot,
They also are at greater risk for later behavior 2011). Research further indicates that children
328 K. Renk et al.

with Conduct Disorder exhibit poor performance of children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity


on standardized tests of verbal ability as well as Disorder (ADHD) meet criteria for Conduct
poor verbal scores on intelligence tests (Moffitt Disorder (relative to 1.8% of children without
& Lynam, 1994). These performance difficulties ADHD). Moreover, relative to children who
extend to more generalized measures as well, exhibit only conduct problems, children who
with children who have conduct problems exhib- exhibit comorbid conduct problems and ADHD
iting a higher likelihood of deficits in their gen- symptoms are at greater risk for developing more
eral verbal skills (Lynam & Henry, 2001) and severe, persistent conduct problems (Lynam,
pragmatic use of language (Gilmour, Hill, Place, 1998) and later antisocial behavior (Mordre,
& Skuse, 2004). Research also suggests that boys Groholt, Kjelsberg, Sandstad, & Myhre, 2011).
with conduct problems have the greatest deficits Beyond ADHD symptoms, children who
in both verbal skills and verbal memory and are exhibit conduct problems display symptoms
more likely to perform poorly on tests of verbal commonly associated with anxiety and depres-
intelligence beginning at the age of 5 years (rela- sive disorders as well (Miller-Johnson, Lochman,
tive to boys without conduct problems; Moffitt, Coie, Terry, & Hyman, 1998; Polier, Vloet,
1990, 1993). Relative to children who do not Herpertz-Dahlmann, Laurens, & Hodgins, 2012).
exhibit conduct problems, the verbal intelligence Based on longitudinal studies, conduct problems
scores of children with Conduct Disorder are that co-occur with mood disorders appear stable
notably lower, even when variables such as socio- over time and are linked to a variety of risk fac-
economic status, academic achievement, and tors, including poor social skills and poor aca-
motivation are controlled (Lynam, Moffitt, & demic achievement (Ingoldsby, Kohl, McMahon,
Stouthamer-Loeber, 1993; Pajer et al., 2008). Lengua, & Conduct Problems Prevention
Although verbal deficits may result from con- Research Group, 2006; Olsson, 2009; Polier
duct problems, verbal deficits also may contrib- et al., 2012). Thus, identifying and treating
ute to the development of children’s conduct comorbid concerns in children with Conduct
problems. For example, Murray and Farrington Disorder may be beneficial to remediating their
(2010) suggest that poor manipulation of abstract conduct symptoms.
concepts may be the underlying connection
between low intelligence test scores and delin-
quent behavior, with children who perform Psychosocial Factors
poorly on intelligence tests also struggling with
being able to foresee the consequences of their Family. The relationship that children have with
actions. Likewise, verbal deficits have been their parents is associated closely with children’s
linked to peer rejection, suggesting another behavior in general (Madigan, Atkinson, Laurin,
explanation for the link between verbal ability & Benoit, 2013; Patterson, 1982) and to the
and subsequent conduct problems (Menting development of Conduct Disorder in particular
et al., 2011). As a result of such findings, Frick (see Pardini, Waller, & Hawes, 2015, for a review
(2012) suggests that the combination of verbal of different family variables). For example,
ability deficits with poor socializing could create research indicates that children’s high levels of
problems with delaying gratification, anticipation CU traits and early conduct problems are associ-
of negative consequences, and difficulties with ated with disorganized attachment with parents
the executive control of behavior. (Pasalich, Dadds, Hawes, & Brennan, 2012). In
Comorbid Psychological Factors. Research contrast, stronger emotional connections between
further suggests that inattention, impulsivity, and adolescents and their parents predict a reduced
hyperactivity can be prominent factors in the risk of antisocial behavior (Sousa et al., 2011).
development of conduct problems (see Holmes Given these findings, it may be that children with
et al., 2001, for a review). For example, Larson, conduct problems may begin experiencing
Russ, Kahn, and Halfon (2011) report that 27.4% ­difficulties in their social relationships very early,
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 329

as problematic attachment with parents sets the controlling for sociodemographic factors (Afifi,
stage for difficult social interactions as children McMillan, Asmundson, Pietrzak, & Sareen,
proceed through development. 2011; Maniglio, 2015). Children who experience
Specific parenting behaviors and practices multiple forms of childhood maltreatment may
also have been related to conduct problems in be at the greatest risk for exhibiting violently
children and adolescents. For example, parents’ delinquent behavior (Crooks, Scott, Wolfe,
warmth and positive, proactive parenting (e.g., Chiodo, & Killip, 2007). Further, research indi-
parents and children spending time playing cates that children who experience abuse or
together) may diminish children’s risk of devel- neglect have a 50% increased probability of
oping CU traits and conduct problems over time engaging in future criminal behavior (Widom,
(Gardner, Ward, Burton, & Wilson, 2003; 1989, 1997). For example, adolescents who expe-
Kroneman, Hipwell, Loeber, Koot, & Pardini, rience childhood maltreatment are 2.19 times
2011; Waller et al., 2014). Research also suggests more likely to commit a felony assault, 2.67
that problematic parenting behaviors, such as times more likely to commit minor assault, 2.47
harsh or erratic discipline, low warmth, coercion, times more likely to commit delinquency, and
and poor supervision or minimal involvement, 4.57 times more likely to commit a status offense
contribute greatly to children’s development of relative to those with no exposure to childhood
Conduct Disorder (e.g., Barker, Oliver, Viding, maltreatment (Sousa et al., 2011). Other forms of
Salekin, & Maughan, 2011; Murray & Farrington, familial discord [e.g., domestic violence (Sousa
2010; Pasalich, Dadds, Hawes, & Brennan, 2011; et al., 2011), inconsistent parental figures
Patterson, 1982; Stormshak, Bierman, McMahon, (Ackerman, Brown, D’Eramo, & Izard, 2002),
Lengua, & Conduct Problems Prevention marital conflict (Rutter, Giller, & Hagell, 1998),
Research Group, 2000). For example, Patterson parental incarceration, familial stress (Campbell,
(1982) indicates that parents of children with Pierce, Moore, Marakovitz, & Newby, 1996;
Conduct Disorder (relative to those of typically Murray, Farrington, & Sekol, 2012)] also place
developing children) are more inconsistent, use children at a heightened risk for the development
harsher commands, are less involved in their par- of Conduct Disorder.
enting practices, and are more likely to engage in Further, parents’ own emotional and behav-
coercive processes when interacting with their ioral functioning is an area of concern when
children. More recently, Barker et al. (2011) sug- examining the development of Conduct Disorder.
gest that harsh parenting practices when children For example, having a parent who exhibits anti-
are 4 years of age predict CU traits and conduct social behavior greatly increases the likelihood
problems in children at 13 years of age. Similarly, that children will develop Conduct Disorder
lower levels of positive reinforcement from a pri- (Murray & Farrington, 2010; Tiet et al., 2001).
mary adoptive parent are associated with CU Research also suggests that parents’ alcohol and
traits in adopted children (Waller et al., 2014). In substance abuse (Loeber, Green, Keenan, &
particular, negative reinforcement of children’s Lahey, 1995; Marmorstein, Iacono, & McGue,
conduct problems may maintain and exacerbate 2009) and psychological symptoms (e.g., depres-
these problems. sion; Barker et al., 2011; Campbell, 1990) are
Child maltreatment (i.e., abuse and neglect) each associated with CU traits and conduct prob-
also contributes to the development of Conduct lems in children. Thus, familial factors, in addi-
Disorder symptoms. For example, Johnson et al. tion to children’s individual characteristics,
(2002) demonstrate that children who experience should be considered when Conduct Disorder is a
child abuse early in life display heightened levels concern.
of conduct problems and aggression. Other Peers. In addition to family characteristics,
research demonstrates that childhood emotional, social relationships with peers are an important
physical, and sexual maltreatment is associated factor to consider in the development of Conduct
significantly with Conduct Disorder, even after Disorder. In particular, research indicates that the
330 K. Renk et al.

development of conduct problems is associated Murray & Farrington, 2010). For example,
with early peer rejection and increased socializa- Gorman-Smith and Tolan (1998) report that 65%
tion with peers with conduct problems or antiso- of boys from a low SES neighborhood described
cial behavior (Gooren, van Lier, Stegge, Terwogt, exposure to severe violence and significant
& Koot, 2011; Murray & Farrington, 2010; increases in their level of aggression during the
Poulin & Boivin, 2000; Vitaro, Brendgen, & course of a year. Moreover, residing in high-­
Tremblay, 2000). Consistently, children who are crime neighborhoods may desensitize children’s
aggressive are more likely to be rejected by their cognitions regarding violent behavior and place
peers and to display increasing conduct problems them at a greater risk for associating with deviant
over time (Kim & Cicchetti, 2010; Vitaro et al., peers (Frick, 2012). For example, Mrug and
2000). Further, following rejection from their Windle (2010) report that 79% of adolescents
nondeviant peers, children who are aggressive from a low SES neighborhood witnessed vio-
are more likely to associate with peers who also lence and experience increased aggression and
are aggressive (Poulin & Boivin, 2000). Attention delinquency during the course of a year. It is
from such peers often acts as a reinforcer for con- important to note that, although poverty is associ-
duct problems (Kiesner, Dishion, & Poulin, ated with conduct problems in children, it also is
2001), allowing children’s conduct problems to associated with familial conflict and parenting
be maintained and exacerbated (Vitaro et al., problems (e.g., Pinderhughes et al., 2001; Zhang
2000). Thus, overall, these problems occur partly & Anderson, 2010). Thus, neighborhood events
because of the rejection that children experience and families’ subsequent responses may contrib-
from their nondeviant peers (Vitaro et al., 2000) ute to Conduct Disorder.
and partly because of increased socialization
with peers who are aggressive. Associations with
peers such as those described here are related Summary of Etiological Factors
most closely to the Adolescent-Onset Type of
Conduct Disorder (Frick, 2016; Moffitt, 2003). It Taken together, there are various etiological fac-
also is noteworthy that children who are aggres- tors that may promote the development of
sive tend to interpret ambiguous situations in Conduct Disorder in children and adolescents. In
hostile ways, suggesting that they maintain a hos- fact, much research combines these factors into
tile attribution bias. Unfortunately, this bias may multifactorial models that describe the etiology
exacerbate children’s aggressive behaviors and of conduct problems, as it is unlikely that any
negative feelings, resulting in further peer rejec- single risk factor is a necessary or sufficient cause
tion (MacBrayer, Milich, & Hundley, 2003). (Rockhill, Collett, McClellan, & Speltz, 2006).
Neighborhood. Although peers may contrib- For example, Liaw and Brooks-Gunn (1994)
ute to the development of Conduct Disorder, the examine 13 risk factors in conjunction with chil-
neighborhoods in which children reside may dren’s behavior problems, with it being noted
make a contribution as well. For example, that the incidence of behavior problems increases
research suggests that children who live in as the number of risk factors increases. Further,
impoverished neighborhoods with a lower socio- Greenberg, Speltz, DeKlyen, and Jones (2001)
economic status (SES) are at a heightened risk use many different factors (i.e., child characteris-
for developing conduct problems (Leventhal & tics, parenting practices, parent-child attachment,
Brooks-Gunn, 2000; Murray & Farrington, and family ecology variables) to differentiate
2010). In particular, research indicates that these families seeking services for boys who meet cri-
children are more likely to be exposed to higher teria for Oppositional Defiant Disorder, a diagno-
rates of criminal acts and violence in their neigh- sis that is related to Conduct Disorder (Borduin,
borhoods, both of which are predictors of Henggeler, & Manley, 1995), from matched com-
Conduct Disorder in adolescents (Frick, 2016; parison boys. This model boasts 81% sensitivity
McCabe, Lucchini, Hough, Yeh, & Hazen, 2005; and 85% specificity, and a dramatic increase in
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 331

clinical status occurs when three or more risk fac- conduct problems are likely to be persistent over
tors are present. time (e.g., Smith et al., 2014). For example,
Rather than examining only the number of Christophersen and Mortweet (2002) suggest that
risk factors that children experience, other mod- 73% of boys, compared to 48% of girls, when
els are beginning to examine the implications of assessed at 4 years of age continue to have persis-
interactions among etiological factors. For exam- tent and severe symptoms when they are 8 years
ple, McKinney and Renk (2007) propose the of age. Such a persistent Conduct Disorder symp-
Interactional-Developmental-Etiological tom profile would suggest the importance of
Approach to understanding the etiology of dis- identifying and intervening early and over time.
ruptive behavior disorders. This approach con- Effective assessment and intervention for
siders a variety of pathways to conduct problems, Conduct Disorder becomes even more important
including genetic factors, dispositional factors, when the costs of Conduct Disorder are consid-
and environmental factors, each of which may ered in terms of psychosocial and financial
interact to promote the occurrence of conduct expenditures. With regard to psychosocial costs,
problems at different stages of development. Conduct Disorder is related to criminal activities,
Given the implications of such models, it may be use and abuse of illegal substances (Brook,
more important for future research to examine Whiteman, Finch, & Cohen, 1996; see Bukstein,
the manner in which etiological factors interact to 2015, for a discussion), and consequences of
promote the development of Conduct Disorder early sexual activity (e.g., unwanted pregnancy;
and how such interactions may be related to Capaldi, Crosby, & Stoolmiller, 1996; Conduct
implementation of successful interventions for Problems Prevention Research Group, 2014).
children and adolescents with Conduct Disorder. Certainly, such costs have implications for the
The following sections will review useful tools emotional and behavioral functioning and for the
for the assessment of Conduct Disorder and social relationships of children and adolescents
interventions that have been designed to treat with Conduct Disorder. In addition to these psy-
Conduct Disorder. chosocial costs, the financial expenditures of the
services provided to children and adolescents
with Conduct Disorder also can be great. In fact,
 ationale for Assessment
R some estimates suggest a cost of $130,000 or
and Intervention with Conduct more per child over the course of a 6-month
Disorder period (Foster et al., 2001). Other researchers
(e.g., Cohen, 1998) estimate that children who
Although it is important for health service pro- follow the path of the Childhood-Onset Type of
viders to understand the diagnostic criteria and Conduct Disorder and who persist in their crimi-
etiological factors related to Conduct Disorder, nal behavior may cost society at least $1.3 mil-
the practicalities of working with children and lion per child. Thus, the costs of Conduct
adolescents with Conduct Disorder will require a Disorder are quite great.
good understanding of assessment measures that Another important rationale for improving
can be used to identify symptoms and the ability health service providers’ knowledge of effective
to implement evidence-based interventions. assessment and intervention for Conduct Disorder
Given that externalizing behavior problems, such is the usual rate of service usage by children and
as those involving conduct problems (e.g., defi- adolescents with Conduct Disorder. In a study
ance, anger, noncompliance), are primary rea- examining the public expenditures of Conduct
sons for children to be referred for mental health Disorder, findings suggest that children with
services (e.g., in preschool age; Gadow, Sprafkin, Conduct Disorder have a high rate of service
& Nolan, 2001; Renk, 2005), it is likely that usage (i.e., 5% receive inpatient services, 15%
health service providers will encounter children receive outpatient services, 18% receive special
and adolescents with Conduct Disorder. Further, education, and 21% have contact with the police;
332 K. Renk et al.

Foster, Jones, & Conduct Problems Prevention tion (Meyer et al., 2001). Further, a comprehen-
Research Group, 2005). In addition, children sive assessment can provide predictive
with Conduct Disorder incur a significantly information regarding prognosis and the likeli-
higher average total cost for services by the time hood of intervention success (Meyer et al., 2001).
they graduate from high school (i.e., exceeding As Conduct Disorder develops through several
$140,000 for the average child with Conduct different pathways and manifests in many forms,
Disorder or a cost that is over six times greater conceptualization of this disorder is difficult but
than that for the average child who does not have vital to intervention planning (McMahon &
Conduct Disorder). In particular, inpatient and Frick, 2005). When assessing children for
outpatient mental health costs account for Conduct Disorder, the examiner must ask several
approximately 70% of the difference in costs for questions: (1) How many symptoms does the
those with Conduct Disorder versus those with- child exhibit?, (2) What types of problematic
out Conduct Disorder (Foster et al., 2005). Given behaviors does the child exhibit?, (3) To what
these estimates, it is imperative that Conduct degree is the child’s functioning impaired?, and
Disorder be identified accurately with appropri- (4) How appropriate is this referral? (McMahon
ate assessment instruments, be addressed effec- & Frick, 2005). Such questions will help health
tively with evidence-based interventions, and be service providers to select appropriate assess-
prevented when possible. ment methods, to include important consider-
ations in their thinking about Conduct Disorder,
and to plan effectively for future intervention
Assessment of Conduct Disorder efforts.

An evidence-based comprehensive assessment is


a crucial first step in diagnosing and effectively Methods of Assessment
treating Conduct Disorder (McMahon & Frick,
2005). In general, evaluations of children and Several methods exist for the assessment of con-
adolescents who exhibit conduct problems should duct problems, including clinical interviews,
assess the topography of these behaviors behavior rating scales, and behavior observa-
(Rockhill et al., 2006). The following section tions, among other methods. It is important to
describes the overarching goals for assessment of note that, although each method is described
Conduct Disorder as well as multiple methods independently and distinctly, the use of multiple
that can be used to diagnose Conduct Disorder. In assessment methods is the norm when assessing
addition, several diagnostic considerations will for Conduct Disorder and is particularly impor-
be noted, including comorbidity, age and sex tant given the complexity of the symptoms
considerations, risk factors, and effects of a described throughout this chapter (McMahon &
Conduct Disorder diagnosis. Frick, 2005).
Clinical Interviews. Clinical interviews pro-
vide an important avenue through which a large
 urposes of Assessment
P amount of information about children’s conduct
in the Context of Intervention problems can be collected. In particular, clinical
interviews allow for the gathering of information
Clinical assessment is a tool used for obtaining a about the types of behaviors that children are
clear picture of clients’ emotional and behavioral manifesting and their severity, the level of impair-
functioning in the context of complex systems. In ment in functioning that children are experienc-
general and as applied to the assessment of ing, and the nature of typical parent-child
Conduct Disorder, the purposes of clinical assess- interactions (McMahon & Frick, 2005). Further,
ment are to describe clients’ current functioning, clinical interviews also allow for information
confirm clients’ diagnoses, and inform interven- about children’s medical, academic, and social
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 333

history to be gathered, and they facilitate the use social strategies for approaching these situations,
of clinical expertise and judgment about chil- with the assumption that social objectives and
dren’s level of impairment (Hartung, McCarthy, problem-solving techniques would differ for
Milich, & Martin, 2005). In addition, such inter- children who are popular versus those who are
views are useful in obtaining a precise picture of not (Landau & Milich, 1990). Consistently, chil-
children’s clinical diagnosis and a description of dren who are more popular tend to have sponta-
the clinical course and severity of children’s neous social strategies that are more friendly,
symptoms (Rockhill et al., 2006). It also is note- positive, and outgoing relative to children who
worthy that interviews can be used for assessing hold a lower status among their peers (Landau &
children’s social skills (Merrell, 2001). Milich, 1990). Interviews regarding social skills
Clinical interviews may vary in the structure also can provide information about the environ-
and level of flexibility used. They can be used ment in which children’s behavior problems
with parents, children and adolescents, and teach- occur, providing a more direct linkage to inter-
ers (in some cases). They also can be structured, vention (Merrell, 2001).
semi-structured, or unstructured. Structured Although structured clinical interviews are
interviews provide an organized method for considered a vital part of a Conduct Disorder
obtaining information (McMahon & Frick, assessment, they are not without limitations.
2005), are designed typically to collect informa- Aside from being time-consuming to administer,
tion in such a way that DSM (APA, 2013) diagno- structured clinical interviews typically do not
ses can be made, and are often comprehensive include normative data. Further, as the interview
enough to allow for the assessment of comorbid proceeds, informants tend to report fewer and
disorders. Given these characteristics, structured fewer symptoms (Jensen, Watanabe, & Richters,
clinical interviews are considered to be the pre- 1999). In other words, informants tend to report
miere method of assessment for Conduct more symptoms earlier in the interview and then
Disorder because they have adequate convergent decrease the number of symptoms that are
validity across informants (e.g., children and par- reported later in the interview. This response pat-
ents) and are considered to have superior reliabil- tern tends to occur regardless of the order in
ity and validity when compared to rating scales which symptoms are assessed. It also is impor-
(Hartung et al., 2005). The Diagnostic Interview tant to note that children under the age of 9 years
Schedule for Children (Shaffer, Fisher, Lucas, are not considered reliable informants when clin-
Dulcan, & Schwab-Stone, 2000) and the ical interviews are administered (Loney & Frick,
Diagnostic Interview for Children and 2003).
Adolescents (Reich, 2000) are two widely used Rating Scales. A second method of assess-
structured clinical interviews that can be helpful ment for Conduct Disorder is the use of behavior
in the diagnosis of Conduct Disorder. In addition, rating scales (McMahon & Frick, 2005). Behavior
the Kiddie Schedule of Affective Disorders and rating scales cover an extensive range of conduct
Schizophrenia (Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, & problems and are helpful as screening devices
Rao, 1997) is a semi-structured interview (i.e., an because they are brief. Some evidence even sug-
interview that has more flexibility but that still gests that self-report behavior rating scales may
thoroughly assesses diagnostic criteria) that can converge with clinical interviews when consider-
be used with children and parents and that is ing the rank order of Conduct Disorder symptom
helpful for differential diagnosis. severity. In particular, self-report behavior rating
Interviews also may be helpful for assessing scales may provide more information about
the social relationships and social skills of chil- Conduct Disorder at low levels of severity
dren and adolescents with Conduct Disorder. In (although clinical interviews appear to provide
particular, it may be helpful to assess children’s more information at high levels of severity;
responses to hypothetical social situations Kelley et al., 2016). They also can assist in the
(Renshaw & Asher, 1983) and their goals and assessment of other adjustment problems and
334 K. Renk et al.

often are accompanied by normative data, allow- that rating scales should be considered a first-line
ing for comparison to peers of the same age. choice for social skills assessment. Although
In addition, behavior rating scales generally there is less research on the utility of self-reports
allow for the inclusion of multiple informants, for social skills assessment (relative to that for
with parents, children and adolescents, and teach- self-reports of emotional and behavioral func-
ers being able to complete such scales. For exam- tioning; Merrell, 2001; Renk & Phares, 2004),
ple, the Child Behavior Checklist, Youth Gresham & Elliott’s, 1990 Social Skills Rating
Self-Report, and Teacher’s Report Form are three System is a well-researched, cross-informant
premiere broad-based behavior rating scales that measure of children’s social skills that can be
can be administered to parents, adolescents, and completed by teachers, parents, and children
teachers, respectively. In addition to measuring themselves. Certainly, there are a variety of mea-
broad-based internalizing, externalizing, and sures that can be used generally to assess chil-
total behavior problems, these scales measure dren’s social skills and competence, such as
more specific narrow-band and DSM-oriented sociometric measures (Hymel, 1983) and the
behavior problem domains relevant to conduct Self-Perception Profile for Children (Harter,
problems as well as scales addressing areas of 1985). Other measures of children’s social skills
competence (Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001). are designed specifically for assessing perfor-
Beyond measuring emotional and behavioral mance in school settings. For example, teachers
difficulties, attention is now being given to rat- can complete many measures, including the
ings meant to identify children and adolescents Social Competence Scale (Kohn & Rosman,
who should be assigned the “With Limited 1972), the Social Behavior Assessment-Revised
Prosocial Emotions” specifier. In particular, some (Byrne & Schneider, 1985), the Social
research has begun to examine the use of item Competence and Behavior Evaluation Scale
response theory to identify the best method for (LaFreniêre, Dumas, Capuano, & Dubeau, 1992),
distinguishing children and adolescents who the School Social Behavior Scales (Merrell,
exhibit CU traits. For example, Kimonis et al. 1993), and the Walker-McConnell Scales of
(2015) suggest that considering the most extreme Social Competence and School Adjustment
responses for the presence of CU traits may pro- (Walker & McConnell, 1995). Given that there is
vide the best discrimination, show reasonable only small to moderate agreement across infor-
prevalence rates for this particular specifier, and mants in ratings of children’s social skills and
identify those who are high in antisocial charac- competence (Renk & Phares, 2004), there may
teristics. When assessing CU traits, the use of be occasions when it would be helpful to collect
multiple informants also may be helpful, as dif- information about children’s social skills from
ferent informants may emphasize different con- multiple informants. In these cases, measures
stellations of CU traits. For example, Gao and specific to particular informants could be used,
Zhang (2016) suggest that confirmatory factor such as the Teacher Rating of Social Skills-­
analyses of self-reports provided by 8- to 10-year-­ Children (a measure completed by teachers
olds on the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional regarding children’s social skills; Clark, Gresham,
Traits (Frick, 2004b) appear to result in uncaring & Elliott, 1985) or Harter’s (1985) Rating Scale
and callousness factors but that confirmatory fac- of Actual Behavior (a measure that can be com-
tor analyses of parent reports on this measure pleted by teachers and/or parents and linked to
appear to result in uncaring, callousness, and the Self-Perception Profile for Children).
unemotional factors. Thus, parents may have Observations. Finally, behavior observations
insights into other characteristics relevant to the allow for behavior to be observed in a natural or
CU traits important to this new specifier. immediate environment (McMahon & Frick,
Rating scales also can be used to assess the 2005), allowing for valuable conclusions to be
social relationships and social skills of children made about children and adolescents separately
and adolescents. In fact, Merrell (2001) suggests from the reports of other informants (e.g., par-
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 335

ents, teachers; McMahon & Frick, 2005). Such make observations of the children. Observing
observations may be particularly important for covert behaviors also may be a challenge
examining social skills, especially when children (McMahon & Frick, 2005) unless special proce-
can be observed in settings where they interact dures are used. For example, temptation-­
with peers (e.g., school; Merrell, 2001). When provocation tasks can be used to measure covert
conducting behavior observations, it is helpful to behaviors (Hinshaw, Zupan, Simmel, Nigg, &
use the same observer across multiple observa- Melnick, 1997). Otherwise, health service pro-
tions and to minimize conspicuous recording viders will have to use alternative assessment
equipment (Aspland & Gardner, 2003). The methods to gain information regarding covert
Dyadic Parent-Child Coding System II (Robinson behaviors.
& Eyberg, 1981) is an example of a structured
coding system for behavior observations in which
children and their parents are observed engaging Important Considerations
in a series of structured play tasks. For classroom in Assessment of Conduct Disorder
observations, Achenbach and Rescorla (2001)
have developed the Direct Observation Form to Dimensions of Conduct Disorder. In order to
assist in coding teacher-child interactions as well make a valid diagnosis of Conduct Disorder and
as the amount of time that a student engages in in order to most effectively inform intervention,
academic activity and on-task versus off-task an understanding of the dimensions of Conduct
behaviors. Disorder is helpful (McMahon & Frick, 2005).
With regard to observing children’s social First, conduct problems manifest in either an
skills, the Peer Social Behavior Code (part of the overt or covert fashion (or both). Overt behaviors
Systematic Screening for Behavior Disorders; are confrontational in nature and include bully-
Walker & Severson, 1992) can be used to catego- ing, arguing with adults, and being aggressive
rize children’s social behaviors (e.g., social toward other individuals and/or animals, among
engagement, parallel play) during free play situa- other things. In contrast, covert behaviors are
tions. Further, some may use observations of considered nonconfrontational (e.g., stealing,
role-play situations to assess children’s social truancy). Second, behaviors can be divided into
skills. In such situations, children are presented those that are destructive and those that are not
with a standard set of situations that involve destructive. This dimension can be combined
social interactions and are asked to respond to a with the overt-covert dimension to form four cat-
provided prompt as if the situations were real. egories: overt-destructive (e.g., physical aggres-
One such role-play situation is the role-play test sion), overt-nondestructive (e.g., oppositional
(Hughes et al., 1989). behaviors), covert-destructive (e.g., destruction
A possible limitation of behavior observation of property), and covert-nondestructive (e.g.,
is the reactivity (i.e., changes in behaviors result- substance use). See McMahon and Frick (2005)
ing from being observed) that children (or par- for a comprehensive review. Further, Nock et al.
ents) may experience during the observation. As (2006) provide an alternative, yet similar, con-
long as the children (or parents) are given ample ceptualization that may prove helpful in develop-
time to become accustomed to the observation ing intervention plans. This conceptualization
procedure, however, reactivity is typically not a includes five subtypes of Conduct Disorder, three
problem (McMahon & Frick, 2005). It also may specialized subtypes (i.e., rule violations, deceit/
be difficult or unrealistic for health service pro- theft, and aggression) and two more general but
viders to conduct extensive behavior observa- severe subtypes (i.e., severe covert and
tions of the children (or parents) being assessed. pervasive).
To address this particular limitation, adults who McMahon and Frick (2005) point out that
have regular contact with the children being these dimensions of conduct problems are
assessed (e.g., parents, teachers) can be trained to important for several reasons. First, conduct
­
336 K. Renk et al.

problems are associated strongly with delin- (Frick & Dantagnan, 2005). Further, longitudinal
quency and involvement with the criminal justice research finds that antisocial traits and detach-
system (Moffitt, 1993). Therefore, the congru- ment are moderately stable over time and may be
ence between the psychological and criminal jus- predictive of a more severe life-course persistent
tice schools of thought can be helpful in allowing pattern of antisocial behavior (Loney, Taylor,
communication to occur across professionals in Butler, & Iacono, 2007). Given the importance of
the mental health and criminal justice fields. CU traits to the course of Conduct Disorder, it is
Second, it is helpful to note whether children are recommended that CU traits be examined early in
exhibiting behaviors consistent with only one the assessment process so that the remainder of
conduct dimension or are more variable in their the assessment can be structured accordingly
pattern of conduct problems. In particular, chil- (McMahon & Frick, 2005). In particular, gaining
dren who exhibit a more heterogeneous pattern of information about CU traits will allow for the
conduct problems tend to experience worse out- prediction of later emotional, behavioral, and
comes than those who only exhibit one dimen- social difficulties. This information also will be
sion of conduct problems (Frick & Loney, 1999; important for confirming a diagnosis of Conduct
Loeber et al., 1993). Finally, these dimensions Disorder as well as for planning for intervention
can provide important clues regarding the role of (Loney et al., 2007; McMahon & Frick, 2005).
genetics in the development of children’s diffi- Comorbidity. In addition to assessing the dif-
culties. In particular, research shows that destruc- ferent dimension of Conduct Disorder, it is
tive behaviors are likely inherited, whereas important to note any comorbid diagnoses that
nondestructive behaviors are not (Simonoff, may be present. In particular, intervention impli-
Pickles, Meyer, Silberg, & Maes, 1998). This dis- cations may differ depending on the diagnoses
tinction may be important as more stable or that are comorbid with Conduct Disorder
inherited traits will likely require different types (McMahon & Frick, 2005). Specifically, it is
of intervention than those traits that are learned. important to assess for Attention-Deficit/
An equally important dimension to examine Hyperactivity Disorder, anxiety disorders, and
when assessing children with conduct problems mood disorders (i.e., disorders that most com-
is whether or not they possess CU traits (e.g., monly co-occur with Conduct Disorder;
lacking empathy or guilt), particularly with the Waschbusch, 2002). Substance use also is associ-
addition of the “With Limited Prosocial ated highly with Conduct Disorder (Hawkins,
Emotions” specifier. Such traits are associated Catalono, & Miller, 1992) and should be noted
with increasingly severe conduct problems and during the assessment process. Most structured
aggression (Frick et al., 2003). Children who clinical interviews and many behavior rating
possess CU traits and who exhibit conduct prob- scales can facilitate the assessment of these
lems also tend to experience more life stressors comorbid diagnoses.
(e.g., peer rejection, family dysfunction, harsh Age Considerations. It also is important to
and inconsistent discipline) and a more stable, note children’s age at the onset of their conduct
severe pattern of conduct problems (Frick & problems, as this information can be helpful in
Dantagnan, 2005). They also are at greater risk the development of assessment guidelines and in
for a variety of individual, behavioral, and con- designing an intervention plan that will be suit-
textual problems, including symptoms of anxiety able for their individual needs (McMahon &
and depression, narcissism, proactive and reac- Frick, 2005). As noted previously, children who
tive aggression, and low self-esteem (Eisenbarth, begin exhibiting conduct problems before the
Demetriou, Kyranides, & Fanti, 2016). age of 10 years tend to have more severe conduct
Interestingly, children with CU traits tend to problems in adolescence and are more likely to
associate less with deviant peer groups, suggest- continue to display such problems into adult-
ing that these children experience a greater level hood (Frick & Loney, 1999; Moffitt & Caspi,
of social rejection than those in other groups 2001). This early age of onset tends to be associ-
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 337

ated with children’s stable temperament and functioning commonly co-occur with Conduct
often is related to criminal behavior and involve- Disorder (Lynam & Henry, 2001; Lynam et al.,
ment with the criminal justice system. Further, 1993). As a result, it is helpful to administer tests
the development of symptoms before the age of of intellectual functioning (e.g., using the
10 years (i.e., the Child-Onset Type of Conduct Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fifth
Disorder) is correlated with other stable risk fac- Edition; Wechsler, 2014) and academic achieve-
tors as well (e.g., lower intellectual functioning, ment (e.g., using the Woodcock-Johnson Tests of
family dysfunction; McMahon & Frick, 2005). Academic Achievement-Fourth Edition; Schrank,
In contrast, children who begin showing symp- Mather, & McGrew, 2014) when assessing chil-
toms after the age of 10 years (i.e., the dren for Conduct Disorder. Collecting informa-
Adolescent-­ Onset Type of Conduct Disorder) tion regarding children’s intellectual and
are more likely to develop conduct problems as a academic functioning may prove useful in pre-
result of their affiliation with deviant peers and dicting a prognosis for children’s conduct prob-
are more likely to be described as conflicting lems, as lower intellectual functioning is
with authority or as being “rebellious” (Moffitt associated with the persistence of conduct prob-
& Caspi, 2001; Moffitt, Caspi, Dickinson, Silva, lems and is predictive of adolescent delinquency
& Stanton, 1996). Given this information, the (Frick & Loney, 1999). Further, the presence or
age at the onset for conduct problems can pro- absence of CU traits may be particularly infor-
vide helpful information about the measures that mative for the type of intervention that is pursued
should be used in the assessment process, prob- (McKinney & Renk, 2006). Coercive parent-­
able components of effective interventions, and child relationships, family factors (e.g., marital
a likely prognosis. and financial stress; McMahon & Estes, 1997),
Sex Considerations. In addition, it is impor- and peer relationships (e.g., association with
tant to note that boys and girls who are diag- deviant peers; Fergusson, Swain-Campbell, &
nosed ultimately with Conduct Disorder may Horwood, 2004) are several additional factors
manifest different types of conduct problems. associated with the development of Conduct
First, girls are more likely to engage in rela- Disorder that should be assessed. In particular,
tional forms of aggression (e.g., gossip, slander) observation of the parent-­ child relationship is
as opposed to overt aggression (e.g., fighting, vital, as understanding this relationship can help
cruelty; Frick, O’Brien, Wootton, & McBurnett, determine whether the focus of intervention
1994; Xie, Cairns, & Cairns, 2005). As a result, should be on children’s individual characteris-
McMahon and Frick (2005) suggest that a mea- tics, on parent-child interactions, and/or on par-
sure of relational aggression should be adminis- enting practices (McMahon & Frick, 2005).
tered during the assessment process, so as to not Effects of Labeling. Unfortunately, labeling
“miss” girls who meet Conduct Disorder crite- children with antisocial characteristics may lead
ria. Second, girls with conduct problems are at to unnecessary stigmatization and to adults mak-
higher risk for comorbid anxiety and depression ing punitive decisions regarding these children
diagnoses. Therefore, it is important for health (e.g., the type of intervention that may be
service providers to closely screen for such dis- required, the types of punishment that may be
orders so that the most effective and compre- warranted for conduct problems; Rockett, Murie,
hensive intervention can be implemented & Boccaccini, 2007). Therefore, Conduct
(McMahon & Frick, 2005). Disorder-related terminology should be used
Risk Factors. The assessment of risk factors cautiously and conservatively when describing
also is important for informing future interven- children and should always be presented in the
tions. For example, as discussed previously, lan- context of children’s developmental, social,
guage impairments and lower intellectual familial, and academic experiences.
338 K. Renk et al.

Assessment Summary toms, individual differences in presenting


symptoms, parents’ psychological symptoms,
Although assessment and intervention are two poor parenting practices), have not allowed one
distinct processes, the assessment process is an particular intervention to be identified as best or
essential, therapeutic part of intervention (Meyer most efficacious (Frick, 2001). Further, some
et al., 2001). Assessment can be a time when all interventions are effective in some studies but not
family members are allowed to provide input, others, with some even resulting in iatrogenic
and it even may be the first time that all family effects (Dishion, McCord, & Poulin, 1999).
members are able to talk about or process their Thus, even after carefully identifying the pres-
experiences and difficulties in a safe, validating ence of Conduct Disorder criteria, considering
environment. Assessment also allows family the etiological factors that may be present, and
members to work with health service providers in conducting a careful assessment of children and
a collaborative way to ameliorate the negative adolescents with conduct problems, it still may
effects that children’s conduct problems may be be difficult to identify the intervention that may
having on the children themselves, the family, be most beneficial for remediating Conduct
and other individuals in the community Disorder symptoms. To provide an overview of
(McMahon & Frick, 2005). Further, receiving interventions that are available, the next section
assessment-based feedback often is relieving and will discuss those interventions that have empiri-
therapeutic for these children and their families, cal support and those interventions that may
especially if the family’s initial attempts to prove problematic for children and adolescents
decrease children’s conduct problems have failed with conduct problems.
(Meyer et al., 2001).
In general, when conducting assessments for
children and adolescents with conduct problems, Interventions with Empirical Support
the assessment process should be flexible, should
use multiple methods and multiple informants, To better understand the wide range of possible
and should examine the conduct problems, interventions for Conduct Disorder, several
comorbid conditions, risk factors, and other char- reviews discuss a number of evidence-based psy-
acteristics. By conducting assessments in this chosocial treatments (EBTs) that address the
fashion, interventions can be selected carefully emotional, behavioral, and social difficulties
and informatively so that the best possible out- experienced by children with Conduct Disorder
comes can occur for these children and adoles- and other disruptive behavior disorders (Bakker,
cents and their families. Greven, Buitelaar, & Glennon, 2016; Brestan &
Eyberg, 1998; Eyberg et al., 2008; Frick, 2001;
McCart & Sheidow, 2016). These reviews indi-
Interventions for Conduct Disorder cate that identified EBTs are effective for specific
age groups and vary based on the method of
Clearly, children and adolescents with conduct delivery (e.g., individual versus group interven-
problems are a challenge for parents, teachers, tion, the degree to which the intervention is child-­
and health service providers given their usual focused versus parent-focused) although most
behaviors (e.g., disruptive behavior, noncompli- fall within behavior, cognitive-behavioral, and
ance, defiance, aggression, oppositionality, social family systems orientations (e.g., McCart &
deficits). As a result, identifying effective inter- Sheidow, 2016). Although it is clear that more
ventions for these children and adolescents is dif- research is needed to better address gaps in the
ficult. Further, a lack of replicable findings and current literature regarding interventions for chil-
generalized intervention effects (Eyberg, Nelson, dren with Conduct Disorder, useful information
& Boggs, 2008), as well as a myriad of confound- is currently available to health service providers
ing factors (e.g., comorbid disorders and symp- as they select interventions. Some of this
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 339

i­nformation is provided here, but readers are situational factors (Frick, 2001). Differences in
encouraged to examine the review papers noted children’s ages at the time of their symptom onset
above. may be one reason that parent-focused interven-
As an example, a recent meta-analytic review tions, or child-focused interventions with accom-
of 17 studies published between 2004 and 2014 panying parent components, tend to be more
that examined 19 psychological and nonpharma- effective with younger children (Eyberg et al.,
cological interventions for Conduct Disorder 2008; Kazdin & Weisz, 2003). In fact, Eyberg
suggests that many studies are of poor to fair et al. (2008) suggest that parent-focused inter-
quality (Bakker et al., 2016). Based on the stud- ventions be used first for younger children and
ies examined (which included many of the inter- that more individually focused cognitive-­
ventions discussed below), parent-reported behavioral interventions be used with adoles-
outcomes, teacher-reported outcomes, and blind cents. As children often do not receive immediate
observer outcomes following intervention services for conduct problems, children’s ages
resulted in small, significant effects, but self-­ when they begin an intervention also should be
reported outcomes resulted in nonsignificant considered. Various interventions are effective,
effects. Given the findings of this meta-analysis, but their effectiveness is often specific to the ages
no one intervention was identified as better than of children and adolescents at the time of inter-
all the others (Bakker et al., 2016). Another vention and to the intervention formats that are
recent review of EBTs for children who have dis- used (Eyberg et al., 2008). As a result, children’s
ruptive behavior (McCart & Sheidow, 2016) ages will be used as an organizer for the interven-
identifies ten EBTs (two well-established, three tions described briefly here.
probably efficacious, and five possibly effica-
cious interventions) for children and adolescents
involved with the justice system. This review Interventions for Young Children
examines the literature from 2007 to 2014 as well
as all adolescent-focused articles that were When intervening on behalf of young children
included in two previous reviews (Brestan & with conduct problems, the inclusion of parents
Eyberg, 1998; Eyberg et al., 2008). The EBTs in the intervention is vital. For example, Parent-­
identified in this review are categorized using Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT; Hembree-­
established criteria for identifying well-­Kigin & McNeil, 1995) is a probably efficacious
established, probably efficacious, and possibly intervention for young children (i.e., children
efficacious interventions (for a complete review who are 2–7 years of age; based on Eyberg et al.,
of the criteria used to identify these interventions, 2008) that focuses particularly on parenting skills
see Chambless & Hollon, 1998, or Chambless & and parent-child interactions. Specifically, the
Ollendick, 2001). tenets of this program are based in both attach-
Further, reviews of the literature identify many ment theory (Herschell, Calzada, Eyberg, &
moderators that may prove beneficial when McNeil, 2002) and operant conditioning
choosing interventions. For example, Frick (Shillingsburg, 2005). As a result, this program
(2001) suggests that the developmental trajectory provides parents the opportunity to learn respon-
of conduct problems differs as a function of chil- sive parenting skills as they meet the needs of
dren’s age at the time of their symptom onset. As their young children, to positively attend to
already noted, conduct problems consistent with appropriate child behaviors, and to actively
the Child-Onset Type of Conduct Disorder may ignore negative child behaviors. Parents and their
reflect a more severe and pervasive disturbance, young children attend sessions together, allowing
whereas symptoms developing in adolescence for in vivo training to occur. In other words, par-
(i.e., those consistent with the Adolescent-Onset ents practice child behavior management skills
Type of Conduct Disorder) may reflect a negative with their young children while being provided
exaggeration of typical adolescent behaviors or feedback from a health service provider
340 K. Renk et al.

(Brinkmeyer & Eyberg, 2003; Kazdin, 2005). (e.g., group therapy; Niec, Barnett, Prewett, &
Parents also are expected to continue practicing Shanley Chatham, 2016) over time (i.e.,
these skills at home, allowing for the generaliza- 3–6 years; Hood & Eyberg, 2003). It also is effec-
tion and mastery of these skills (Capage, Foote, tive in decreasing the likelihood that children will
McNeil, & Eyberg, 1998). meet criteria for their initially diagnosed disor-
As part of PCIT, parents and their young chil- ders upon completion of the intervention (Boggs
dren participate in two sequential treatment mod- et al., 2005). Given this support for PCIT’s effec-
ules. First, parents and their young children tiveness, this intervention would likely benefit
participate in a Child-Directed module where families who have young children with conduct
parents are coached in how to interact attentively problems.
with their young children. This module is similar Another useful intervention is the Helping the
to play therapy (Eyberg, 2003), in that parents are Noncompliant Child (HNC; Forehand &
asked to engage in special playtime with their McMahon, 1981) program. Similar to PCIT,
young children. During this playtime, parents HNC is a probably efficacious intervention for
describe, imitate, and praise their young chil- addressing the conduct problems of young chil-
dren’s appropriate behavior, while they use dren who are between 3 and 8 years of age
appropriate speech, ignore inappropriate behav- (Eyberg et al., 2008). HNC focuses on parenting
ior, and avoid criticism, commands, and ques- behaviors that are similar to those targeted by
tions (Greco, Sorrell, & McNeil, 2001). Overall, PCIT (e.g., attending to positive child behaviors,
the purpose of this module is to improve parent-­ ignoring negative child behaviors, employing
young child interactions by fostering a more nur- effective discipline). HNC also utilizes in vivo
turing relationship (Eyberg & Bussing, 2010). strategies, where parents and young children are
Second, parents and their young children partici- seen together in the clinic or at home (McMahon
pate in a Parent-Directed module where parents & Forehand, 2003). There are two phases
are coached in effective contingency manage- included in this program as well. During the first
ment skills that can be used to manage child non- phase, differential attention, parents learn to
compliance (Hembree-Kigin & McNeil, 1995). increase the frequency and range of their social
In particular, the purpose of this module is to help attention to their children, to decrease the fre-
parents increase their young children’s prosocial quency of their competing verbal behaviors, and
behaviors and decrease their young children’s to ignore minor inappropriate behaviors. To reach
problematic behavior (Eisenstadt, Eyberg, these goals, parents are assisted in creating posi-
McNeil, Newcomb, & Funderburk, 1993; Eyberg tive and mutually reinforcing relationships with
& Bussing, 2010). Generally, PCIT is designed to their young children by practicing skills in ses-
improve young children’s social relationships sion and at home during 10–15minute time peri-
(particularly parent-young child relationships), ods (i.e., Child’s Game; McMahon & Forehand,
mothers and fathers’ parenting skills, and young 2003). During the second phase, compliance
children’s emotional and behavioral functioning. training, parents are coached in the use of appro-
Overall, research suggests that PCIT meets priate commands so that their young children’s
these goals. In particular, research shows that compliance can be increased (i.e., Parent’s
PCIT is effective in improving parent-young Game). Parents also are coached in the use of
child interactions and in decreasing young chil- standing rules as a supplement to clear instruc-
dren’s behavior problems (Eyberg & Robinson, tions both in and out of the home (McMahon &
1982; Eyberg et al., 2001; Herschell et al., 2002). Forehand, 2003).
Further, PCIT is effective in decreasing chil- Similar to PCIT, HNC is designed to improve
dren’s conduct problems across a variety of set- young children’s social relationships (particu-
tings (e.g., school; Eyberg, Boggs, & Algina, larly the parent-young child relationship), moth-
1995; McNeil, Eyberg, Eisenstadt, Newcomb, & ers and fathers’ parenting skills, and young
Funderburk, 1991) and with a variety of formats children’s emotional and behavioral functioning.
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 341

McMahon and Forehand (2003) summarize seven children), young children who are between
many of the outcome studies that examine the the ages of 3 and 8 years discuss how they would
HNC program, with these studies providing sup- feel during the videotaped situations and provide
port for this program (e.g., Forehand & King, suggestions for appropriate responses. Modeling
1974, 1977). For example, previous controlled and feedback are utilized through a variety of
studies find that HNC is effective in increasing games, activities, and role-playing, with the goal
young children’s compliance in response to par- of teaching young children the basic skills of
ents’ demands and in improving secondary con- empathy, communication, friendship, anger con-
duct problems (e.g., aggression, tantrums; Wells, trol, and problem-solving (Webster-Stratton &
Forehand, & Griest, 1980). Given these findings, Reid, 2003). Thus, IY-CT focuses specifically on
the HNC program also would benefit young chil- the social deficits that young children with con-
dren with conduct problems and their families. duct problems exhibit.
A third intervention, the Incredible Years When compared to young children participat-
(Webster-Stratton & Reid, 2003) program, is also ing in other programs and to those serving as wait
probably efficacious for parents and their young list controls, young children who participate in
children (based on Eyberg et al., 2008). Similar IY-CT demonstrate significant improvements in
to the first two programs described in this sec- aggressive and noncompliant behavior (Webster-­
tion, this program includes a parent intervention Stratton, Reid, & Hammond, 2001) and reduc-
component. In addition, other components can be tions in oppositionality and conduct problems
included for young children (i.e., social skills over time (i.e., at 1-year follow-up, Hobbel &
training) and for teachers (Reid & Webster-­ Drugli, 2013; McGilloway et al., 2014; Webster-­
Stratton, 2001; Webster-Stratton, 2001). The Stratton & Hammond, 1997; up to 2-year follow-
Incredible Years Parent Training (IY-PT) compo- up, Posthumus, Raajmakers, Maassen, van
­
nent utilizes videotaped model vignettes teach Engeland, & Matthys, 2012; Trotter & Rafferty,
parents about positive parent-child interactions, 2014). Further, when used in conjunction with
effective discipline techniques, and the fostering the Incredible Years Teacher Training program,
of appropriate problem-solving skills and spur young children exhibit more social competence,
discussion. Parents then complete homework more emotional self-regulation, and lower levels
assignments to practice these skills (Webster-­ of conduct problems (Webster-Stratton, Reid, &
Stratton, 1981b). A supplemental component of Stoolmiller, 2008). The combination of IY-PT
IY-PT (ADVANCE) often is offered at the com- and IY-CT also results in improvements in young
pletion of the BASIC parent program. This children’s social competence (Brotman et al.,
ADVANCE program allows parents to learn 2005; Drugli, Larsson, & Clifford, 2007).
more effective communication, self-control, and These findings for young children may be pro-
problem-solving skills for use in their marital moted by changes noted for mothers, who tend to
relationship and encourages parents to strengthen decrease their directive behaviors (Webster-­
their social support network (Webster-Stratton & Stratton, 1981b, 1982), demonstrate increased
Reid, 2003). confidence (Webster-Stratton, 1981a), and show
Although typically administered in conjunc- more positive and significantly less negative
tion with IY-PT, the Incredible Years Child interactions with their young children (Webster-­
Training (IY-CT; Webster-Stratton & Reid, 2003) Stratton, 1981b, 1982) in conjunction with this
component is probably efficacious as an individ- intervention. With regard to the ADVANCE
ual treatment (as is IY-PT; based on Eyberg et al., IY-PT program, significant reductions in the
2008). Similar to IY-PT, IY-CT employs the use behavior problems of young children and
of videotaped vignettes; however, with IY-CT, improvements in the prosocial behaviors of
the videotaped vignettes depict social situations young children have been shown to be related to
that young children are likely to encounter at improved parent communication, collaboration,
home and at school. In small groups (i.e., six to and problem-solving skills. Results also suggest
342 K. Renk et al.

that improvements in parents’ marital relation- This program also can be offered in a group for-
ship, as a result of better communication and sup- mat (Group Triple P; Sanders, 1999) and now is
port skills, is related to improvements in young being examined in an online format (Triple P
children’s conduct problems (Webster-Stratton & Online; Sanders, Baker, & Turner, 2012). In the
Reid, 2003). In particular, young children whose group format, which also is a possibly efficacious
parents participate in this program exhibit treatment intervention (based on Eyberg et al.,
decreases in negative affect and submissive 2008), parents practice newly acquired parenting
behavior and increases in positive affect (Webster-­ skills in small groups.
Stratton, 1981b, 1982). These findings suggest These different levels of Triple P result in
that the Incredible Years program would provide fewer child disruptive behaviors, greater parent-
benefits to young children and their families as ing competence, and less dysfunctional parenting
well. over time (e.g., at 1-year follow-up, Sanders,
Markie-Dadds, Tully, & Bor, 2000; at 3-year fol-
low-­up, Sanders, Bor, & Morawska, 2007). Other
Interventions for Children studies suggest that Triple P is beneficial in
reducing children’s problematic behaviors, even
Similar to the interventions described for young when confounding factors (e.g., mothers’ psy-
children, interventions for school-age children chological symptoms, marital discord) are pres-
address mothers and fathers’ parenting strategies. ent (Sanders, 1999). Although the focus of this
For example, the Positive Parenting Program intervention is on improving mothers and fathers’
(Triple P; Sanders, 1999) offers two parent-­ parenting behaviors, it is likely that resulting
focused approaches that are considered to be improvements in children’s conduct problems
probably efficacious EBTs (based on Eyberg will promote improvements in these children’s
et al., 2008) and five levels of preventive inter- social relationships as well.
vention for children who are 12 years of age and Another well-established parent-focused
younger. These levels increase in intensity, rang- intervention (Eyberg et al., 2008) for parents of
ing from the dissemination of basic information children who are 12 years of age and younger is
on parenting strategies to individual and group the Parent Management Training Oregon Model
training sessions for parents of children who have (PMTO; Patterson, Reid, Jones, & Conger, 1975).
severe behavior problems. PMTO appears to be a variant of a previously
With the availability of these different levels well-established treatment called Living with
of intervention, this intervention can be individu- Children (Patterson & Gullion, 1968). This inter-
alized for different families and their children vention employs behavior modification tech-
with conduct problems. As an example, the Triple niques that are based on tenets of operant
P Standard Individualized Treatment (i.e., level conditioning (Brestan & Eyberg, 1998). PMTO,
4) is a ten-session program that utilizes model- like other parent programs, teaches parents basic
ing, rehearsal, and feedback to teach parents core behavior modification principles, such as how to
parenting skills (e.g., managing misbehavior, better monitor children’s behaviors and imple-
preventing problematic behavior, teaching and ment effective discipline. Based on a long history
encouraging new and appropriate behaviors, of research regarding Conduct Disorder and anti-
improving the parent-child relationship). Further, social behavior, PMTO accounts for individual-
the Triple P Enhanced Treatment is the most ized differences in children’s symptoms,
intensive level of treatment (i.e., level 5) and children’s developmental trajectory, and basic
incorporates home visits where therapists attempt principles of positive and negative reinforcement
to improve characteristics of the home environ- (Patterson, Reid, & Eddy, 2002). Previous con-
ment (e.g., parenting stress, communication, cop- trolled studies indicate that PMTO is effective in
ing, mood management, partner support) and reducing deviant behaviors in children who are
increase effective parenting (Sanders, 1999). younger than 12 years of age (Patterson,
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 343

Chamberlain, & Reid, 1982) and can reduce con- described as probably efficacious when com-
duct problems whether or not symptoms of bined with PSST (based on Eyberg et al., 2008).
attention-­deficit/hyperactivity disorder are pres- Further, the simultaneous combination of PSST
ent (Bjømebekk, Kjøbli, & Ogden, 2015). Given and PMT is superior to either program alone
these findings, PMTO also may have secondary (Kazdin, 2003, 2010; Kazdin, Siegel, & Bass,
benefits for children’s social relationships. 1992), suggesting that this intervention would
For school-age children, individual and group benefit school-age children with conduct
therapy programs also become an option. For problems.
example, Problem Solving Skills Training (PSST; A number of group interventions also are
Kazdin, 2003, 2010) is labeled as a probably effi- described as EBTs for the reduction of conduct
cacious intervention (based on Eyberg et al., problems in school-age children. These group
2008). PSST is a cognitive approach that teaches interventions vary in the amount of involvement
children with conduct problems to use problem-­ expected from children and their parents. For
solving skills meant to foster accurate appraisals example, Anger Control Training (Lochman,
of social situations. Intervention strategies Barry, & Pardini, 2003) is a child-focused group
include the modeling of appropriate behaviors, intervention that can be completed in school.
games, activities, role-playing, and the use of a This intervention was labeled as being probably
token economy reward system (Kazdin, 1996, efficacious previously (Eyberg et al., 2008) but
2003, 2010). The original PSST also may be listed as an experimental treatment in a more
complemented with an in vivo component, a recent review (McCart & Sheidow, 2016). This
combination that also is labeled as being proba- program allows children to discuss social situa-
bly efficacious (based Eyberg et al., 2008). As tions, identify the potential social cues and
part of this combined program (which also is motives of the individuals in each situation, and
called Supersolvers), parents, who also learn the practice appropriate problem-solving strategies.
problem-solving steps taught to their children, Through discussion, role-playing, and the video-
help their children apply these skills in everyday taping of practice interactions, children learn to
situations (Kazdin, 1996, 2003, 2010). identify how they feel in social situations and to
A third variation of PSST includes Parent better control their feelings while being provided
Management Training (PMT; Kazdin, 2003, with feedback on their performance (Lochman,
2005, 2010). PMT uses operant conditioning 1992; Lochman et al., 2003). In general, anger
principles to change parents’ behavior, children’s coping programs improve self-esteem and
adaptive functioning, and parent-child interac- problem-­solving skills, decrease substance use,
tions. In particular, parents learn to better identify and have long-term benefits (i.e., 3-year follow-
problem behaviors and implement effective rein- ­up; Lochman, 1992). Thus, such interventions
forcement and discipline. Randomized control directly address the social difficulties of children
trials of this program describe statistically sig- with conduct problems.
nificant changes following intervention (Kazdin,
2005), with approximately 79% of clinically
referred children and adolescents who complete Interventions for Adolescents
the program making changes that their parents
label as being important (Kazdin & Wassell, Currently, more research is being done to identify
1998). In particular, research suggests that the individualized interventions specifically for ado-
use of PSST and PMT decreases children’s anti- lescents with conduct problems and related social
social behavior and increases their prosocial difficulties. One group intervention, Group
behavior. These changes also seem to be main- Assertiveness Training (Huey & Rank, 1984),
tained for 1–2 years following intervention was labeled previously as being probably effica-
(Kazdin, 2005). Based on several controlled stud- cious for African American adolescents in the
ies, PMT is effective (Kazdin, 1996) and is eighth and ninth grades (Eyberg et al., 2008) but
344 K. Renk et al.

was listed as experimental in a more recent macological interventions). MST is based on


review (McCart & Sheidow, 2016). Group ecological and family systems theories, support-
Assertiveness Training is a highly structured, ing the idea that interventions should incorporate
short school-based program (i.e., consisting of and generalize to children and adolescents’ many
eight 1-hour sessions provided over the course of interconnected systems (e.g., families, peers,
a 4-week period). As part of this program, trained neighborhoods, schools, larger community con-
counselors or peers lead reflective group discus- texts). Services often are provided in multiple
sions on a variety of topics (e.g., anger, aggres- settings (e.g., at home, in the school setting).
sion, rules) for adolescents with conduct The core principles of MST involve tailoring
problems and maintain a strong emphasis on interventions to meet the needs of the children
emotional awareness and feelings (Huey & Rank, and adolescents being treated, focusing on posi-
1984). Research suggests that this program is tive aspects of family involvement, evaluating
more effective than group discussion in reducing gains continually and making modifications as
classroom aggression (Huey & Rank, 1984). they are needed, and generalizing the positive
The Rational-Emotive Mental Health Program effects of the interventions that are being used
(REHM; Block, 1978) is a second school-based across various settings (Eyberg et al., 2008;
EBT that was labeled previously as probably effi- Henggeler & Lee, 2003). Thus, MST interven-
cacious for older adolescents (i.e., Hispanic and tions are individualized to meet the needs of chil-
African American adolescents in the 11th and dren and adolescents and their families.
12th grades; Eyberg et al., 2008) but now is listed Controlled studies report that MST is more effec-
as experimental in a more recent review (McCart tive than individual therapy in reducing behavior
& Sheidow, 2016). REHM is a highly structured problems, increasing family relationships, and
and directive group program that is based on the preventing criminal behavior 4 years following
rational-emotive therapy model and that uses spe- treatment (Borduin, Mann, & Cone, 1995). MST
cific cognitive-behavioral techniques (e.g., in vivo is a well-established intervention (van der
activities, group discussion, homework assign- Stouwe, Asscher, Stams, Deković, & van der
ments). With this program, students are taught to Laan, 2014; Zajac, Randall, & Swenson, 2015).
be introspective and self-aware as they practice Given the comprehensive nature of MST, it would
the rational appraisal of social situations. Relative benefit children and adolescents with significant
to Group Assertiveness Training, REHM is a conduct problems.
­longer, more intensive program, with adolescents
meeting 5 days per week for 12 weeks (Block,
1978). Research suggests that REHM promotes  ther Interventions (Not Based
O
improvements in adolescents’ grade point aver- on Age)
ages and decreases their truancy and disruptive
behavior over time (i.e., at 4-month follow-up; Another example of a well-respected EBT is
Block, 1978). Overall, these programs address Multidimensional Treatment Foster Care (MTFC;
directly the social difficulties of adolescents with Chamberlain & Smith, 2003; Smith &
conduct problems. Chamberlain, 2010; now known as Treatment
Multisystemic Therapy (MST; Henggeler & Foster Care Oregon, McCart & Sheidow, 2016).
Lee, 2003) is a third EBT that is used with ado- MTFC is a community-based intensive program
lescents who exhibit severe antisocial and delin- for children of all ages with disruptive and antiso-
quent behavior. This program has been labeled cial behavior problems. As part of this program,
consistently as probably efficacious (Eyberg children are placed in foster care for 6–9 months
et al., 2008; McCart & Sheidow, 2016). MST is a with foster parents who are provided training in
very intensive but flexible program that utilizes a the use of positive reinforcement, discipline prac-
variety of established interventions (e.g., tices, positive feedback for appropriate behavior,
cognitive-­behavioral therapies, parenting, phar- and daily behavior management via token
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 345

e­conomy procedures. During the foster place- et al., 2005; Pelham, 1993). As there is overlap in
ment, children in MTFC attend individual ther- the symptoms exhibited by children with ADHD
apy sessions designed to improve their anger and those with Conduct Disorder (e.g., impulsiv-
management, problem-solving, and social skills ity, noncompliance, verbal and physical aggres-
(Chamberlain & Smith, 2003; Smith & sion), psychostimulants also may be beneficial in
Chamberlain, 2010). These children also are pro- treating some conduct problems present with
vided with in vivo training in the community so Conduct Disorder (Frick, 2001; Gurnani, Ivanov,
that they can practice their prosocial behaviors & Newcorn, 2016). Further, a reduction in such
while being provided with direct reinforcement conduct problems may improve other behaviors
and feedback. Finally, during the foster place- that are associated tangentially with Conduct
ment, biological parents are provided with inten- Disorder (e.g., poor peer interactions, problem-­
sive parent management training where they solving skills; Frick, 2001).
learn and practice effective communication, par- Beyond psychostimulants, other medications,
enting, and disciplining skills (Chamberlain & such as alpha-2 adrenoreceptor agonists, mood
Smith, 2003; Smith & Chamberlain, 2010). stabilizers, and antipsychotic medications, some-
Research suggests that several core compo- times are used to treat aggression in children
nents of MTFC account for the effectiveness of (Steiner, Saxena, & Chang, 2003). For example,
this intervention. These components include the Olfson, Blanco, Liu, Moreno, and Laje (2006)
children being closely supervised by and having examine trends in the usage of antipsychotic
a close relationship with an adult (i.e., their foster medications as part of outpatient visits for chil-
parents), foster parents setting clear limits for the dren in the United States. This study documents
children during foster placement, and foster par- an increase in the use of antipsychotic medica-
ents preventing children’s interactions with devi- tions from 1993 to 2003, particularly in visits for
ant peers (Chamberlain, Leve, & DeGarmo, males and with regard to disruptive behavior dis-
2007). In addition, biological parents are pro- orders. As these medications are indicated for use
vided with intensive intervention, while trained with adults only, any use of these medications in
foster parents care for their children. Controlled children would be considered off label, with lim-
studies of MTFC indicate that this intervention is ited research noting their utility (Findling, 2003).
more effective than group care for adolescent As some suggest that the more severe symptoms
boys (Chamberlain & Reid, 1998) and girls of Conduct Disorder require more intensive psy-
(Chamberlain et al., 2007), resulting in fewer chosocial interventions (Eyberg et al., 2008),
criminal and delinquent acts in 1- and 2-year fol- psychotropic medications may be beneficial as a
low-­up periods. Given these findings, positive supplement to psychosocial interventions for
outcomes are promoted by providing intensive some children or when psychosocial interven-
training to biological parents and addressing the tions are unsuccessful (Eyberg et al., 2008; Frick,
behavioral and social deficits of children with 2001; Pelham, 1993). Overall, use of psychotro-
conduct problems. pic medications as part of Conduct Disorder
Although many consider psychotropic medi- interventions should be examined further.
cations to be an effective intervention for chil- Finally, there are other interventions that show
dren with conduct problems, there is less research promise but that require more research regarding
available on these medications than for psycho- their effectiveness for the treatment of Conduct
social interventions (Farmer, Compton, Burns, & Disorder. For example, Barkley (1997) devel-
Robertson, 2005). Although no psychotropic oped the Defiant Children program for parents of
medications are approved for conduct problems children up to the age of 12 years. This program
and aggression at this time (Rockhill et al., 2006), focuses on improving parents’ management skills
psychostimulants are used regularly and are when dealing with their children’s behavior prob-
effective in reducing symptoms of attention-­ lems, improving parents’ knowledge of the
deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Farmer potential causes for their children’s behavior
346 K. Renk et al.

problems, improving children’s compliance with school relationships, and effective communica-
parents’ commands and rules, and increasing tion and discipline skills. Thus far, studies
family harmony. A second but related program, indicate that Fast Track promotes positive out-
the Defiant Teen program (Barkley, Edwards, & comes for children and adolescents (e.g.,
Robin, 1999), adapts the Defiant Children pro- improvements in child behavior, social skills,
gram for use with adolescents and includes emotion recognition, social problem-solving, and
problem-­ solving communication training. language skills) and for parents (e.g., less use of
Although these programs do not have extensive physical punishment, improvements in parenting
outcome research, the skills taught to parents as behaviors and satisfaction; Pasalich, Witkiewitz,
part of these programs have received extensive McMahon, Pinderhughes, & Conduct Problems
research support (McMahon & Forehand, 2003). Prevention Research Group, 2016; Slough et al.,
In addition, comprehensive classroom-based 2008). Further, Fast Track appears to be cost-­
interventions may hold promise for reducing the effective in terms of reducing the diagnosis of
conduct problems and social difficulties of chil- Conduct Disorder in children who are at the high-
dren and adolescents over the long term. Most est risk (Foster, Jones, & Conduct Problems
notably, comprehensive preschool programs Prevention Research Group, 2006).
(e.g., Head Start) promote decreases in behavior A second prevention program that has positive
problems and juvenile delinquency (Yoshikawa, results thus far is the Early Risers Preventive
1994). Other research supports classroom-based Intervention program (Bernat et al., 2007;
interventions that include behavior training and Hektner, August, Bloomquist, Lee, & Klimes-­
consultation for teachers as well as implementa- Dougan, 2014). As part of this program, kinder-
tion of token economy systems and response-cost gartners in 23 elementary schools were screened
interventions (Filcheck, McNeil, Greco, & for aggressive behavior and then were assigned
Bernard, 2004). Given these preliminary find- randomly to either the Early Risers program or a
ings, future research should examine the utility of control condition. Those who were assigned to
these school-based interventions for decreasing the program were provided an intensive interven-
the conduct problems and social difficulties of tion in their kindergarten year through the sum-
children and adolescents. mer after their third grade year. These children
then participated in a booster phase in the fourth
and fifth grades. The preventive intervention con-
Prevention sisted of five components, including a 6-day
summer wilderness program emphasizing com-
Prevention programs, particularly if they are ini- munity building and peer support activities, a
tiated early enough and are designed to address “Circle of Friends” group in which children met
multiple etiologies, also may prove beneficial independently in 6 monthly groups per year, a
children and adolescents with conduct problems Family Skills parent group in which expert speak-
(Bernat, August, Hektner, & Bloomquist, 2007; ers presented topics and tips to parents, and
Slough, McMahon, & Conduct Problems Monitoring and Mentoring School Support and
Prevention Research Group, 2008). The Fast Family Support programs (Bernat et al., 2007).
Track Project, a multisite, collaborative research Children who participate in this program experi-
project examining a comprehensive, multicom- ence significant increases in their academic
ponent intervention for preventing serious con- achievement as well as in their cognitive compe-
duct problems, is one such prevention program tence and concentration (as rated by teachers)
(Slough et al., 2008). Fast Track includes many following the first 2 years of participation and
different components depending on the age and into the third program year (August, Realmuto,
needs of the child or adolescent, including the Hektner, & Bloomquist, 2001). Children who
provision of a curriculum that promotes social participated in the intervention show lower rates
and emotional competence, positive family-­ of conduct problems at the end of their sixth
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 347

grade year (Bernat et al., 2007) and when they are lack the ability to appropriately appraise these
in high school (Hektner et al., 2014). In particu- situations and to identify their own feelings and
lar, participation in this program appears to those of others (Kazdin, 2003, 2005). As a result,
decrease nondeviant peer relations and improve these children often are impulsive and unable to
social skills. These improvements then are related predict the consequences of their actions,
to decreases in children’s symptoms (Bernat responding with negative problem-solving skills
et al., 2007). Thus, preventive interventions may (Kazdin, 2005). Thus, all the child-focused inter-
have benefits for the emotional, behavioral, and ventions described previously address social and/
social functioning of children and adolescents. or problem-solving skills. Overall, the important
component related to effectiveness may be the
inclusion of emotional awareness and increasing
 ommonalities Among EBTs: Why Are
C accurate perceptions of social situations.
They Effective?

The EBTs described here for children and ado- Potentially Problematic Treatments
lescents with conduct problems may differ in for-
mat but overlap greatly in terms of the skills As described previously, EBTs for adolescents
being taught and the behaviors being addressed. often utilize a group format and are employed in
For children and adolescents with conduct prob- school settings. The group interventions that
lems, it appears that parents need to take an active incorporate social skills training are considered
role in the interventions provided (Eyberg et al., an effective means of treating adolescents with
2008; Kazdin & Weisz, 2003), particularly when Conduct Disorder (Eyberg et al., 2008).
it is noted that parent-child dyads are at signifi- Therefore, it would make sense that the most eco-
cant risk for negative interactions (Frick, 1998). nomical and convenient way to conduct interven-
Accordingly, all the effective parent-focused tions for adolescents would be to always use a
interventions described here teach effective par- group format. Research on group interventions
enting skills while encouraging positive parent-­ provides mixed results, however. Although a
child interactions, consistency, and structure. meta-analysis by Weiss et al. (2005) suggests that
They also encourage or implement modeling and group interventions are generally not iatrogenic,
active practice of newly learned behaviors, other studies suggest that group interventions
thereby creating opportunities for positive parent-­ may actually cause more harm than benefit.
child interactions. Many theories may explain why many group
Although there are some promising child-­ interventions, especially with older children and
focused interventions, the most effective inter- adolescents, may have iatrogenic effects. For
ventions at this time are those that provide example, De-Haan and MacDermid (1999) posit
parent-focused components and/or that encour- that the general stigma of receiving interventions
age parent involvement. In other words, child-­ and the resulting influence on adolescents’ self-­
focused interventions that teach problem-solving, concepts may be related to increases in conduct
anger management, and social skills seem to be problems. Iatrogenic effects also may be ampli-
enhanced when parents are taught how to foster fied by “deviancy training” or the reinforcement
these skills in their children and adolescents at of deviant behavior by other group members
home, making them generalizable across set- (Dishion et al., 1999). In particular, social skills
tings. Thus, a second commonality among the interventions with high-risk youth may increase
EBTs described here is that they target the identi- the amount of contact that individuals have with
fied deficits often found with conduct problems. deviant peers, further contributing to maladjust-
For example, children and adolescents with con- ment (Dishion & Andrews, 1995).
duct problems often have difficulty in social situ- Given the cost-effectiveness and convenience
ations and with handling conflict because they of group interventions but the mixed findings
348 K. Renk et al.

regarding their effectiveness and/or potential interventions may be enough to produce changes
harm to children and adolescents with conduct in behavior (Eyberg et al., 2008). For adolescents
problems, certain factors should be considered with a long history of conduct problems, how-
before implementing group interventions. First, ever, early problematic behaviors have the chance
deviancy training is likely to be most concerning to worsen over time, likely resulting in poor
for adolescents (Dishion et al., 1999), as adoles- parent-­child relationships. As a result, incorpo-
cents are progressing from primarily identifying rating parents into interventions may be neces-
with their family to seeking an identity through sary to counteract this history of conduct
peer relationships. Second, iatrogenic effects are problems, as evidenced by more intensive pro-
likely to occur when group interventions are grams, such as MST and MTFC (Eyberg et al.,
lacking structure (Dodge, 1999). Third, low-risk 2008).
children or children who do not have a history of Beyond the controversy regarding group inter-
antisocial behavior are considered the most vul- ventions, other interventions also have problem-
nerable to deviancy training influences (Dishion atic results, such as Scared Straight programs,
et al., 1999). These children may have experi- drug abuse resistance education, and boot camp
enced overall peer rejection but have not associ- programs (see Lilienfeld, 2007). Scared Straight
ated with deviant peers until participating in such programs expose children who are at high risk for
group interventions (Weiss et al., 2005). Further, conduct problems to prison conditions in the
Mager, Millich, Harris, and Howard (2005) indi- hope that such an experience would be upsetting
cate that group interventions that include both enough to deter them from committing acts of
high- and low-risk children may have greater iat- delinquency and crime in the future. Results indi-
rogenic effects and poorer outcomes than group cate that children who take part in this type of
interventions that include only adolescents who program experience a significantly higher likeli-
are at high risk. It may be that group leaders hood of offending and a significant increase in
unknowingly provide a greater amount of prefer- arrests, however (Lilienfeld, 2007; Petrosino,
ential treatment and positive reinforcement to Turpin-Petrosino, & Buehler, 2003). Similarly,
those adolescents who are at low risk. This dif- drug abuse resistance education, such as that
ferential treatment may lead high-risk adoles- included in the DARE program, are counterpro-
cents to mentally or emotionally withdraw from ductive. For example, Werch and Owen (2002)
the group process and form a minority out-group indicate that such programs are ineffective in
in which reinforcement is sought from deviant teaching the social skills needed to resist peer
peers rather than from positive influences (Mager pressure to use drugs and are related to increased
et al., 2005). substance intake. Finally, boot camp interven-
In contrast, the interventions described here tions have mixed results regarding their effective-
appear to be effective with very small groups ness. These particular social skills programs
(i.e., six to eight adolescents) and with specific emphasize discipline and obedience but are asso-
targeted populations (e.g., Assertiveness Training ciated with a number of deaths and other related
for adolescents who are in the eighth and ninth problems (e.g., dehydration; Lilienfeld, 2007).
grades; REMH for adolescents in the 11th and Thus, these interventions are not recommended.
12th grades). Further, adolescents may benefit
from more direct interventions as opposed to par-
enting programs (Eyberg et al., 2008). Thus, with  onsiderations for Successful
C
these group interventions, it appears that highly Interventions
structured, very small groups are most effective.
It is important, however, to consider children’s Although effective EBTs are described for chil-
age at the onset of their symptoms. For those dren and adolescents with conduct problems,
children whose problematic behaviors do not many of these interventions only are efficacious
emerge until adolescence, less intense group with specific age groups and in specific formats.
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 349

Further, these interventions may decrease, but tured and should prevent the formation of deviant
not always eliminate, conduct and related prob- peer networks with reinforcement for negative
lems. Therefore, other confounding factors must behavior (Dodge, 1999).
be considered when choosing the most appropri- Second, children who have conduct problems
ate interventions for children and adolescents vary greatly in their presenting symptoms, with a
with conduct problems. Age is an important con- large number of children experiencing other
founding factor to consider. As already noted, the comorbid disorders (e.g., ADHD, ODD; Kazdin,
developmental trajectories for children with the 1996). As a result, these children often have a
Child-Onset Type of Conduct Disorder versus myriad of co-occurring symptoms and other
those with the Adolescent-Onset Type of Conduct behavior problems (e.g., impulsivity, inattention,
Disorder vary greatly, resulting in individual dif- oppositionality, poor peer relationships) that
ferences in presenting symptoms (Frick, 2001). complicate intervention. By treating only the
As noted previously, the Child-Onset Type of symptoms of Conduct Disorder and not chil-
Conduct Disorder tends to be more chronic in dren’s individualized presentation, problematic
nature and is linked to more severe, disruptive, behaviors may not be treated fully and/or ineffec-
and antisocial behaviors. Further, early conduct tive interventions may be implemented. As the
problems, when untreated, often are met with EBTs described here are noted to decrease spe-
coercive, ineffective parenting strategies, leading cific conduct problems that are characteristic of
to a deterioration of the parent-child relationship Conduct Disorder (e.g., noncompliance, disrup-
and a worsening of children’s behavior. Over tive behavior, aggression, oppositionality, delin-
time, this coercive cycle may be an impetus for quency), rather than addressing multiple
more severe antisocial and delinquent behaviors diagnoses, these interventions may not always be
(Reid & Patterson, 1989). Although the the most beneficial for children and adolescents
Adolescent-Onset Type of Conduct Disorder with comorbid conditions. In contrast, Kazdin
may be situational or more normative in nature and Whitley (2006) examine the effectiveness of
(Frick, 2001), presenting conduct problems still PMT, PSST, or both (both categorized as proba-
are concerning. Further, adolescence is a period bly efficacious treatments by Eyberg et al., 2008)
marked by a striving for autonomy as well as a in children and adolescents with comorbid condi-
need to belong (Vander Zanden, Crandell, & tions. Their results indicate that children and ado-
Crandell, 2006). As such, deviant adolescent peer lescents with Conduct Disorder or ODD and with
groups may lead to a worsening of symptoms, up to four additional diagnoses (i.e., comorbid
including engagement in dangerous, illegal activ- conditions) actually have greater behavior
ities with grave repercussions. changes relative to children with Conduct
Therefore, although the Child-Onset Type and Disorder or ODD alone (i.e., no additional diag-
the Adolescent-Onset Type of Conduct Disorder noses). Thus, comorbid conditions may compli-
may differ in presentation, they both require cate intervention, but positive effects are still
early, structured intervention to prevent degrada- possible when an appropriate intervention is
tion of children’s behavior, degradation of the used.
parent-child relationship, and the prevention of Finally, confounding variables related to the
deviant peer networks. As such, the most effec- environments of children and adolescents may
tive interventions for children with long histories interfere with their gains. For example, a number
of conduct problems appear to be those that of confounding variables moderate or mediate
include a parent-focused component (Eyberg the effects of psychosocial interventions (e.g.,
et al., 2008; Kazdin & Weisz, 2003). In these parents’ psychological symptoms and substance
cases, individualized interventions may be a ben- abuse, parents’ marital adjustment, harsh and
eficial supplement to many parent-focused inter- ineffective parenting practices; Beauchaine,
ventions. Finally, when utilized, group Webster-Stratton, & Reid, 2005). Stressors in the
interventions should be small and highly struc- home environment (e.g., low SES, poor
350 K. Renk et al.

e­ducational opportunities, living in high-risk viders can remain flexible and open to alternative
neighborhoods) likely are related to the effective- and/or supplemental methods of intervention. As
ness of various interventions and the generaliza- part of this endeavor, future research should
tion of gains across settings (Frick, 1998, 2001). examine the differential effectiveness of the
Parents’ negative attitudes also decrease the many components included in the interventions
effectiveness of interventions (Kazdin & Whitley, described here as well as the utility of using dif-
2006). Overall, these factors are important to ferent intensities of intervention (e.g., interven-
consider, especially with families who exhibit tion length, inclusion of one versus many system
many of these characteristics and who have lost levels). Finally, research should continue to
hope that the conduct problems exhibited by their develop and examine evidence-based prevention
child or adolescent will improve. programs so that families and the communities in
which they live can foster environments that
enrich the experiences of their children and ado-
Conclusions lescents. In this way, children and adolescents
who are at risk for or who already are exhibiting
Based on the various characteristics of children conduct problems can show the greatest improve-
and adolescents with conduct problems, the ments in their emotional and behavioral function-
assessment options that are available, and the ing and their social relationships.
evidence-based interventions that are beneficial
for these children and adolescents, it appears that Acknowledgments  Special thanks and acknowledgment
no one approach will be best for all families who to Rachel S. Wolfe, Samantha Scott, and Melissa
Middleton, who were authors on the first version of this
seek intervention for their children and adoles- chapter that appeared in the first edition of this book.
cents with conduct problems. It is clear, however, Their initial contributions continue to shape the current,
that children and adolescents with conduct prob- revised chapter.
lems likely will benefit from early interventions
that are tailored individually to their specific
needs and those of their families. As a result, it References
behooves the field to move in the direction of
matching the characteristics of families who Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for
the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles. Burlington:
present for intervention for the conduct problems University of Vermont, Research Center for Children,
of their children and adolescents to the interven- Youth, and Families.
tions that they actually receive. Research should Ackerman, B. P., Brown, E. D., D’Eramo, K. S., & Izard,
examine these issues carefully so as to maximize C. E. (2002). Maternal relationship instability and the
school behavior of children from disadvantaged fami-
the effectiveness of the interventions that are lies. Developmental Psychology, 38, 694–704.
implemented. As the research literature contin- Afifi, T. O., McMillan, K. A., Asmundson, G. J., Pietrzak,
ues to develop this area of interest, special con- R. H., & Sareen, J. (2011). An examination of the
siderations should be given to age of onset for relation between conduct disorder, childhood and
adulthood traumatic events, and posttraumatic stress
conduct problems and for commencement of disorder in a nationally representative sample. Journal
intervention, the presence of comorbid disorders, of Psychiatric Research, 45, 1564–1572.
and other potential barriers to potential interven- American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2013).
tion effectiveness (e.g., parents’ psychological Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disor-
ders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric
symptoms, ineffective parenting, high degree of Publishing.
daily stressors, parents’ attitudes toward inter- Arseneault, L., Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Taylor, A.,
vention) as assessment and intervention proto- Rijsdijk, F. V., Jaffee, S. R., … Measelle, J. R. (2003).
cols are selected and implemented. Strong genetic effects on cross-situational antiso-
cial behavior among 5-year-old children accord-
Once the most appropriate intervention is ing to mothers, teachers, examiner-observers, and
identified and implemented, gains should be twins’ self-reports. Journal of Child Psychology and
monitored continually so that health service pro- Psychiatry, 44, 832–848.
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 351

Aspland, H., & Gardner, F. (2003). Observational mea- Van Hasselt & M. Hersen (Eds.), Handbook of ado-
sures of parent-child interaction: An introductory lescent psychopathology (pp. 349–383). New York:
review. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 8, Lexington Books.
136–143. Borduin, C. M., Mann, B. J., & Cone, L. T. (1995).
August, G. J., Realmuto, G. M., Hektner, J. M., & Multisystemic treatment of serious juvenile offend-
Bloomquist, M. L. (2001). An integrated components ers: Long-term prevention of criminality and violence.
preventive intervention for aggressive elementary Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63,
school children: The Early Risers program. Journal 569–578.
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 614–626. Brestan, E. V., & Eyberg, S. M. (1998). Effective psy-
Bakker, M. J., Greven, C. U., Buitelaar, J. K., & Glennon, chosocial treatments of conduct-disordered children
J. C. (2016). Practitioner review: Psychological treat- and adolescents: 29 years, 82 studies, and 5,272 kids.
ments for children and adolescents with conduct Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 180–189.
disorder problems—A systematic review and meta-­ Brinkmeyer, M. Y., & Eyberg, S. M. (2003). Parent-child
analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, interaction therapy for oppositional children. In A. E.
58(1), 4–18. Kazdin & J. R. Weisz (Eds.), Evidence-based psycho-
Barker, E. D., Oliver, B. R., Viding, E., Salekin, R. T., & therapies for children and adolescents (pp. 204–223).
Maughan, B. (2011). The impact of prenatal maternal New York: Guilford.
risk, fearless temperament and early parenting on ado- Brook, J. S., Whiteman, M., Finch, S. J., & Cohen,
lescent callous-unemotional traits: A 14-year longitu- P. (1996). Young adult drug use and delinquency:
dinal investigation. Journal of Child Psychology and Childhood antecedents and adolescent mediators.
Psychiatry, 52, 878–888. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Defiant children: A clinician’s Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1584–1592.
manual for assessment and parent training (2nd ed.). Brotman, L. M., Gouley, K. K., Chesir-Teran, D., Dennis,
New York: The Guilford Press. T., Klein, R. G., & Shrout, P. (2005). Prevention
Barkley, R. A., Edwards, G. H., & Robin, A. L. (1999). for preschoolers at high risk for conduct problems:
Defiant teens: A clinician’s manual for assessment and Immediate outcomes on parenting practices and child
family intervention. New York: The Guilford Press. social competence. Journal of Clinical Child and
Bates, J. E. (2001). Adjustment style in childhood as a Adolescent Psychology, 34, 724–734.
product of parenting and temperament. In T. D. Wach Bukstein, O. G. (2015). Conduct disorder and delinquency
& G. A. Kohnstamm (Eds.), Temperament in context and substance use disorders. In Y. Kaminer (Ed.),
(pp. 173–200). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Youth substance abuse and co-occurring disorders
Beauchaine, T. P., Webster-Stratton, C., & Reid, M. J. (pp. 81–102). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric
(2005). Mediators, moderators, and predictors of Publishing.
1-year outcomes among children treated for early-­ Byrne, B. M., & Schneider, B. H. (1985). Factorial valid-
onset conduct problems: A latent growth curve analy- ity of Stephens’ social behavior assessment. Journal
sis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 259–260.
73, 371–388. Calkins, S. D., Hungerford, A., & Dedmon, S. E. (2004).
Bernat, D. H., August, G. J., Hektner, J. M., & Bloomquist, Mothers’ interactions with temperamentally frustrated
M. L. (2007). The Early Risers Preventive Intervention: infants. Infant Mental Health Journal, 25, 219–239.
Testing for six-year outcomes and mediational pro- Campbell, S. B. (1990). Behavior problems in pre-
cesses. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35, school children: Clinical and developmental issues.
605–617. New York: Guilford Press.
Bjømebekk, G., Kjøbli, J., & Ogden, T. (2015). Children Campbell, S. B., Pierce, E. W., Moore, G., Marakovitz,
with conduct problems and co-occurring ADHD: S., & Newby, K. (1996). Boys’ externalizing prob-
Behavioral improvements following parent manage- lems at elementary school age: Pathways from early
ment training. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 37, behavior problems maternal control, and family stress.
1–19. Development and Psychopathology, 8, 701–719.
Block, J. (1978). Effects of a rational-emotive mental Capage, L. C., Foote, R., McNeil, C. B., & Eyberg, S. M.
health program on poorly achieving, disruptive high (1998). Parent-child interaction therapy: An effective
school students. Journal of Counseling Psychology, treatment for young children with conduct problems.
25, 61–65. The Behavior Therapist, 21, 137–138.
Boggs, S. R., Eyberg, S. M., Edwards, D. L., Rayfield, Capaldi, D. M., Crosby, L., & Stoolmiller, M. (1996).
A., Jacobs, J., Bagner, D., & Hood, K. K. (2005). Predicting the timing of first sexual intercourse for
Outcomes of parent-child interaction therapy: A com- at-risk adolescent males. Child Development, 67,
parison of treatment completers and study dropouts 344–359.
one to three years later. Child and Family Behavior Caspi, A., Henry, B., McGee, R. O., Moffitt, T. E., &
Therapy, 26, 1–22. Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental origins of child
Borduin, C. M., Henggeler, S. W., & Manley, C. M. and adolescent behavior problems: From age three to
(1995). Conduct and oppositional disorders. In V. B. age fifteen. Child Development, 66, 55–68.
352 K. Renk et al.

Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., Mill, J., Martin, J., & childhood and adolescent-onset distinction. Journal of
Craig, I. W. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle Abnormal Child Psychology, 37, 375–385.
of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, Day, N. L., Richardson, G. A., Goldschmidt, L., &
851–854. Cornelius, M. D. (2000). Effects of prenatal tobacco
Chamberlain, P., Leve, L. D., & DeGarmo, D. S. (2007). exposure on preschoolers’ behavior. Journal of
Multidimensional treatment foster care for girls in the Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21,
juvenile justice system: 2-Year follow-up of a random- 180–188.
ized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical de Cubas, M. M., & Field, T. (1993). Children of
Psychology, 75, 187–193. methadone-­ dependent women: Developmental out-
Chamberlain, P., & Reid, J. B. (1998). Comparison of two comes. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 63,
community alternatives to incarceration for chronic 266–276.
juvenile offenders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical De-Haan, L. G., & MacDermid, S. M. (1999). Identity
Psychology, 66, 624–633. development as a mediating factor between urban pov-
Chamberlain, P., & Smith, D. K. (2003). Antisocial erty and behavioral outcomes for junior high school
behavior in children and adolescents: The Oregon students. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 20,
Multidimensional Treatment Foster Care Model. 123–148.
In A. E. Kazdin & J. R. Weisz (Eds.), Evidence-­ Desrosiers, C., Boucher, O., Forget-Dubois, N., Dewailly,
based psychotherapies for children and adolescents E., Ayotte, P., Jacobson, S. W., … Muckle, G. (2013).
(pp. 282–300). New York: Guilford. Associations between prenatal cigarette smoke expo-
Chambless, D. L., & Hollon, S. D. (1998). Defining sure and externalized behaviors at school age among
empirically supported therapies. Journal of Consulting Inuit children exposed to environmental contaminants.
and Clinical Psychology, 66, 7–18. Neurotoxicology and Teratology, 39, 84–90.
Chambless, D. L., & Ollendick, T. H. (2001). Empirically Dishion, T. J., & Andrews, D. W. (1995). Preventing
supported psychological interventions: Controversies escalation in problem behaviors with high-risk young
and evidence. Annual Review in Psychology, 52, adolescents: Immediate and 1-year outcomes. Journal
685–716. of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 538–548.
Christophersen, E. R., & Mortweet, S. L. (2002). Dishion, T. J., McCord, J., & Poulin, F. (1999). When
Treatments that work with children: Empirically interventions harm: Peer groups and problem behav-
supported strategies for managing childhood prob- ior. American Psychologist, 54, 755–764.
lems. Washington, DC: American Psychological Dodge, K. A. (1999). Cost effectiveness of psychotherapy
Association. for child aggression. First, is there cost effectiveness?
Clark, L., Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1985). Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 3,
Development and validation of a social skills 275–278.
assessment measure: The TROSS-C. Journal of Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biophysiological
Psychoeducational Assessment, 4, 347–356. model of the development of chronic conduct prob-
Cohen, M. A. (1998). The monetary value of saving a lems in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 39,
high-risk youth. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 349–371.
14, 5–33. Drugli, M. B., Larsson, B., & Clifford, G. (2007).
Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2014). Changes in social competence in young children
Trajectories of risk for early sexual activity and early treatment because of conduct problems as viewed by
substance use in the Fast Track prevention program. multiple informants. European Child and Adolescent
Prevention Science, 15(Suppl 1), S33–S46. Psychiatry, 16, 370–378.
Crooks, C. V., Scott, K. L., Wolfe, D. A., Chiodo, D., Duke, A. A., Begue, L., Bell, R., & Eisenlohr-Moul, T.
& Killip, S. (2007). Understanding the link between (2013). Revisiting the serotonin-aggression relation
childhood maltreatment and violent delinquency: in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,
What do schools have to add? Child Maltreatment, 12, 139, 1148–1172.
269–280. Eisenbarth, H., Demetriou, C. A., Kyranides, M. N., &
D’Onofrio, B. M., Van Hulle, C. A., Waldman, I. D., Fanti, K. A. (2016). Stability subtypes of callous-­
Rodgers, J. L., Rathouz, P. J., & Lahey, B. B. (2007). unemotional traits and conduct disorder symptoms and
Causal inferences regarding prenatal alcohol exposure their correlates. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45,
and childhood externalizing problems. Archives of 1889–1901.
General Psychiatry, 64, 1296–1304. Eisenstadt, T. H., Eyberg, S., McNeil, C. B., Newcomb,
Dadds, M. R., Fraser, J., Frost, A., & Hawes, D. J. (2005). K., & Funderburk, B. (1993). Parent-child interac-
Disentangling the underlying dimensions of psychopa- tion therapy with behavior problem children: Relative
thy and conduct problems in childhood: A community effectiveness of two stages and overall treatment
study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, outcome. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 22,
73, 400–410. 42–51.
Dandreaux, D. M., & Frick, P. J. (2009). Developmental Eyberg, S. M. (2003). Parent-child interaction ther-
pathways to conduct problems: A further test of the apy. In T. H. Ollendick & C. S. Schroeder (Eds.),
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 353

Encyclopedia of clinical child and pediatric psychol- Filcheck, H. A., McNeil, C. B., Greco, L. A., & Bernard,
ogy. New York: Plenum. R. S. (2004). Using a whole-class token economy and
Eyberg, S. M., Boggs, S. R., & Algina, J. (1995). New coaching of teacher skills in a preschool classroom
developments in psychosocial, pharmacological, and to manage disruptive behavior. Psychology in the
combined treatments of conduct disorders in aggres- Schools, 41, 351–361.
sive children. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 31, Findling, R. (2003). Dosing of atypical antipsychotics in
83–91. children and adolescents. Primary Care Companion
Eyberg, S. M., & Bussing, R. (2010). Parent-child inter- Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 5, 10–13.
action therapy for preschool children with conduct Forehand, R., & King, H. E. (1974). Pre-school children’s
problems. In R. C. Murrihy, A. D. Kidman, & T. H. non-compliance: Effects of short-term behavior ther-
Ollendick (Eds.), Clinical handbook of assessing and apy. Journal of Community Psychology, 2, 42–44.
treating conduct problems in youth (pp. 139–162). Forehand, R., & King, H. E. (1977). Noncompliant chil-
New York, NY: Springer. dren: Effects of parent training on behavior and atti-
Eyberg, S. M., Funderburk, B. W., Hembree-Kigin, T. L., tude change. Behavior Modification, 1, 93–108.
McNeil, C. B., Querido, J. G., & Hood, K. K. (2001). Forehand, R., & McMahon, R. J. (1981). Helping the non-
Parent-child interaction therapy with behavior prob- compliant child: A clinician’s guide to parent training.
lem children: One and two year maintenance of treat- New York: Guilford.
ment effects in the family. Child and Family Behavior Foster, E. M., Jones, D. E., & Conduct Problems
Therapy, 23, 1–20. Prevention Research Group. (2005). The high costs of
Eyberg, S. M., Nelson, M. M., & Boggs, S. R. (2008). aggression: Public expenditures resulting from con-
Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for children duct disorder. American Journal of Public Health, 95,
and adolescents with disruptive behavior. Journal of 1767–1772.
Child and Adolescent Psychology, 37, 215–237. Foster, E. M., Jones, D. E., & Conduct Problems
Eyberg, S. M., & Robinson, E. A. (1982). Parent-child Prevention Research Group. (2006). Can a costly
interaction training: Effects on family functioning. intervention be cost-effective? An analysis of vio-
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 11, 130–137. lence prevention. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63,
Fairchild, G., Passamonti, L., Hurford, G., Hagan, 1284–1291.
C. C., von dem Hagen, E. A., van Goozen, S. H., … Foster, E. M., Kelsch, C. C., Kamradt, B., Sosna, T., &
Calder, A. J. (2011). Brain structure abnormalities in Yang, Z. (2001). Expenditures and sustainability in
early-onset and adolescent-onset conduct disorder. systems of care. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral
American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 624–633. Disorders, 9, 53–62.
Fairchild, G., Toschi, N., Hagan, C. C., Goodyer, I. M., Frick, P. J. (1998). Conduct disorders and severe antiso-
Calder, A. J., & Passamonti, L. (2015). Cortical thick- cial behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
ness, surface area, and folding alterations in male Frick, P. J. (2001). Effective interventions for children and
youths with conduct disorder and varying levels of adolescents with conduct disorder. Canadian Journal
callous–unemotional traits. NeuroImage: Clinical, 8, of Psychiatry, 46, 597–608.
253–260. Frick, P. J. (2004a). Developmental pathways to conduct
Farmer, E. M. Z., Compton, S. N., Burns, B. J., & disorder: Implications for serving youth who show
Robertson, E. (2005). Review of the evidence severe aggressive and antisocial behavior. Psychology
base for treatment of childhood psychopathology: in the Schools, 41, 823–834.
Externalizing disorders. Journal of Consulting and Frick, P. J. (2004b). The inventory of callous-unemotional
Clinical Psychology, 70, 1267–1302. traits. Unpublished rating scale. New Orleans, LA:
Fergusson, D. M., Boden, J. M., Horwood, L. J., Miller, University of New Orleans.
A. L., & Kennedy, M. A. (2011). MAOA, abuse expo- Frick, P. J. (2012). Developmental pathways to conduct
sure and antisocial behaviour: 30-Year longitudinal disorder: Implications for future directions in research,
study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 198, 457–463. assessment, and treatment. Journal of Clinical Child
Fergusson, D. M., Swain-Campbell, N., & Horwood, & Adolescent Psychology, 41, 378–389.
L. J. (2004). How does childhood economic disad- Frick, P. J. (2016). Current research on conduct disorder
vantage lead to crime? Journal of Child Psychology & in children and adolescents. South African Journal of
Psychiatry, 45, 956–966. Psychology, 46, 160–174.
Fergusson, D. M., Woodward, L. J., & Horwood, L. J. Frick, P. J., Cornell, A. H., Barry, C. T., Bodin, S. D., &
(1998). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and psy- Dane, H. A. (2003). Callous-unemotional traits and
chiatric adjustment in late adolescence. Archives of conduct problems in the prediction of conduct prob-
General Psychiatry, 55, 721–727. lem severity, aggression, and self-report of delin-
Ficks, C. A., & Waldman, I. D. (2014). Candidate genes quency. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 31,
for aggression and antisocial behavior: A meta-­ 457–470.
analysis of association studies of the 5HTTLPR and Frick, P. J., & Dantagnan, A. L. (2005). Predicting the
MAOA-uVNTR. Behavioral Genetics, 44, 427–444. stability of conduct problems in children with and
354 K. Renk et al.

­ ithout callous-unemotional traits. Journal of Child


w Gooren, E. M., van Lier, P. A., Stegge, H., Terwogt,
and Family Studies, 14, 469–485. M. M., & Koot, H. M. (2011). The development of
Frick, P. J., & Ellis, M. L. (1999). Callous-unemotional conduct problems and depressive symptoms in early
traits and subtypes of conduct disorder. Clinical Child elementary school children: The role of peer rejection.
and Family Psychology Review, 2, 149–168. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,
Frick, P. J., & Loney, B. R. (1999). Outcomes of children 40, 245–253.
and adolescents with conduct disorder and opposi- Gorman-Smith, D., & Tolan, P. (1998). The role of
tional defiant disorder. In H. C. Quay & A. Hogan exposure to community violence and developmental
(Eds.), Handbook of disruptive behavior disorders problems among inner-city youth. Development and
(pp. 507–524). New York: Plenum. Psychopathology, 10, 101–116.
Frick, P. J., & Morris, A. S. (2004). Temperament and Greco, L. A., Sorrell, J. T., & McNeil, C. B. (2001).
developmental pathways to conduct problems. Understanding manual-based behavior therapy: Some
Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, theoretical foundations of parent-child interaction
33, 54–68. therapy. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 23,
Frick, P. J., O’Brien, B. S., Wootton, J. M., & McBurnett, 21–36.
K. (1994). Psychopathy and conduct problems in chil- Greenberg, M. T., Speltz, M. L., DeKlyen, M., & Jones, K.
dren. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 103, (2001). Correlates of clinic referral for early conduct
700–707. problems: Variable- and person-oriented approaches.
Frick, P. J., Ray, J. V., Thornton, L. C., & Kahn, R. E. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 255–276.
(2014). Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social skills rat-
understanding, diagnosis, and treatment of serious ing system. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance
conduct problems in children and adolescents? A com- Service.
prehensive review. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1–57. Gurnani, T., Ivanov, I., & Newcorn, J. H. (2016).
Frick, P. J., & Viding, E. (2009). Antisocial behavior Pharmacotherapy of aggression in child and ado-
from a developmental psychopathology perspective. lescent psychiatric disorders. Journal of Child &
Development and Psychopathology, 21, 1111–1131. Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 26, 65–73.
Gadow, K. D., Sprafkin, J., & Nolan, E. E. (2001). Harter, S. (1985). Manual for the self-perception profile
DSM-IV symptoms in community and clinic pre- for children. Denver, CO: University of Denver.
school children. Journal of the American Academy of Hartung, C. M., McCarthy, D. M., Milich, R., & Martin,
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1383–1392. C. A. (2005). Parent-adolescent agreement on disrup-
Gao, Y., & Zhang, W. (2016). Confirmatory factor analy- tive behavior symptoms: A multitrait-multimethod
ses of self- and parent-report inventory of callous-­ model. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
unemotional traits in 8- to 10-year-olds. Journal of Assessment, 27, 159–168.
Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 38, Hawkins, J. D., Catalono, R. F., & Miller, J. Y. (1992).
331–340. Risk and protective factors for alcohol and other
Gardner, F., Ward, S., Burton, J., & Wilson, C. (2003). drug problems in adolescence and early adult-
The role of mother-child joint play in the early devel- hood: Implications for substance abuse prevention.
opment of children’s conduct problems: A longitu- Psychological Bulletin, 112, 64–105.
dinal observational study. Social Development, 12, Hektner, J. M., August, G. J., Bloomquist, M. L., Lee, S.,
361–378. & Klimes-Dougan, B. (2014). A 10-year randomized
Gatzke-Kopp, L. M., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2007). Direct controlled trial of the Early Risers conduct problems
and passive prenatal nicotine exposure and the devel- preventive intervention: Effects on externalizing and
opment of externalizing psychopathology. Child internalizing in late high school. Journal of Consulting
Psychiatry & Human Development, 38, 255–269. and Clinical Psychology, 82, 355–360.
Gaysina, D., Fergusson, D. M., Leve, L. D., Horwood, Hembree-Kigin, T. L., & McNeil, C. B. (1995). Parent-­
J., Reiss, D., Shaw, D. S., … Harold, G. T. (2013). child interaction therapy. New York: Plenum Press.
Maternal smoking during pregnancy and offspring Henggeler, S. W., & Lee, T. (2003). Multisystemic treat-
conduct problems: Evidence from 3 indepen- ment of serious clinical problems. In A. E. Kazdin
dent genetically sensitive research designs. JAMA & J. R. Weisz (Eds.), Evidence-based psychothera-
Psychiatry, 70, 956–963. pies for children and adolescents (pp. 301–322).
Gilmour, J., Hill, B., Place, M., & Skuse, D. H. (2004). New York: Guilford.
Social communication deficits in conduct disorder: Herschell, A. D., Calzada, E. J., Eyberg, S. M., & McNeil,
A clinical and community survey. Journal of Child C. B. (2002). Parent-child interaction therapy: New
Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 967–978. directions in research. Cognitive and Behavioral
Godar, S. C., Fite, P., McFarlin, K. M., & Bortolato, M. Practice, 9, 9–16.
(2016). The role of monoamine oxidase A in aggres- Hinshaw, S. P., Zupan, B. A., Simmel, C., Nigg, J. T.,
sion: Current translational developments and future & Melnick, S. (1997). Peer status in boys with and
challenges. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology without attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder:
& Biological Psychiatry, 69, 90–100. Predictions from overt and covert antisocial behavior,
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 355

social isolation, and authoritative parenting beliefs. Kazdin, A. E. (1996). Problem solving and parent
Child Development, 68, 880–896. management in treating aggressive and antiso-
Hobbel, S., & Drugli, M. B. (2013). Symptom changes cial behavior. In E. D. Hibbs & P. S. Jensen (Eds.),
of oppositional defiant disorder after treatment with Psychosocial treatments for child and adolescent
the Incredible Years Program. Nordic Journal of disorders: Empirically based strategies for clinical
Psychiatry, 67, 97–103. practice (pp. 377–408). Washington, DC: American
Holmes, S. E., Slaughter, J. R., & Kashani, J. (2001). Psychological Association.
Risk factors in childhood that lead to the development Kazdin, A. E. (2003). Problem-solving skills training
of conduct disorder and antisocial personality disor- and parent management training for conduct disor-
der. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 31, der. In A. E. Kazdin & J. R. Weisz (Eds.), Evidence-­
183–193. based psychotherapies for children and adolescents
Hood, K. K., & Eyberg, S. M. (2003). Outcomes of (pp. 241–262). New York, NY: Guilford.
parent-child interaction therapy: Mothers’ reports of Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Parent management training:
maintenance three to six years after treatment. Journal Treatment for oppositional, aggressive and antisocial
of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 32, behavior in children and adolescents. New York, NY:
419–429. Oxford University Press.
Huey, W. C., & Rank, R. C. (1984). Effects of counselor Kazdin, A. E. (2010). Problem-solving skills training and
and peer-led group assertive training on black adoles- parent management training for conduct disorder. In
cent aggression. Journal of Counseling Psychology, A. E. Kazdin & J. R. Weisz (Eds.), Evidence-based
31, 95–98. psychotherapies for children and adolescents (2nd ed.,
Hughes, J. N., Boodoo, G., Alcala, J., Maggio, M., pp. 211–226). New York, NY: Guilford.
Moore, L., & Villapando, R. (1989). Validation of a Kazdin, A. E., Siegel, T. C., & Bass, D. (1992). Cognitive
role-play measure of children’s social skills. Journal problem-solving skills training and parent manage-
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 633–646. ment training in the treatment of antisocial behav-
Hymel, S. (1983). Preschool children’s peer relations: ior in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Issues in sociometric assessment. Merrill-Palmer Psychology, 60, 733–747.
Quarterly, 29, 237–260. Kazdin, A. E., & Wassell, G. (1998). Treatment comple-
Ingoldsby, E. M., Kohl, G. O., McMahon, R. J., Lengua, tion and therapeutic change among children referred
L., & Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. for outpatient therapy. Professional Psychology:
(2006). Conduct problems, depressive symptomatol- Research and Practice, 29, 332–340.
ogy and their co-occurring presentation in childhood Kazdin, A. E., & Weisz, J. R. (2003). Introduction:
as predictors of adjustment in early adolescence. Context and background of evidence-based psycho-
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34, 602–620. therapies for children and adolescents. In A. E. Kazdin
Jambroes, T., Jansen, L. M. C., Vermeiren, R. R. J. M., & J. R. Weisz (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies
Doreleijers, T. A. H., Colins, O. F., & Popma, A. for children and adolescents (pp. 3–20). New York:
(2016). The clinical usefulness of the new LPE speci- Guilford.
fier for subtyping adolescents with conduct disorder in Kazdin, A. E., & Whitley, M. K. (2006). Comorbidity,
the DSM 5. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, case complexity, and effects of evidence-based treat-
25, 891–902. ment for children referred for disruptive behavior.
Jensen, P. S., Watanabe, H. K., & Richters, J. E. (1999). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74,
Who’s up first? Testing for order effects in structured 455–467.
interviews using a counterbalanced experimental design. Keenan, K., Boeldt, D., Chen, D., Coyne, C., Donald,
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 439–445. R., Duax, J., … Humphries, M. (2011). Evidence for
Johnson, R. M., Kotch, J. B., Catellier, D. J., Dufort, V., the predictive validity of DSM-IV oppositional defi-
Hunter, W., & Amaya-Jackson, L. (2002). Adverse ant and conduct disorders diagnosed in a clinically
behavioral and emotional outcomes from child abuse referred sample of preschoolers. Journal of Child
and witnessed violence. Child Maltreatment, 7, Psychology & Psychiatry, 52, 47–55.
179–186. Keenan, K., Shaw, D., Delliquadri, E., Giovannelli, J., &
Kahn, R. E., Frick, P. J., Youngstrom, E., Findling, R. L., Walsh, B. (1998). Evidence for the continuity of early
& Youngstrom, J. K. (2012). The effects of includ- problem behaviors: Application of a developmental
ing a callous-unemotional specifier for the diagnosis model. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26,
of conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and 441–452.
Psychiatry, 53, 271–282. Kelley, S. E., Balsis, S., Smith, S. T., Edens, J. F., Douglas,
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., & Rao, U. (1997). K. S., & Poythress, N. G. (2016). A dimensional com-
Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia parison of a self-report and a structured interview
for school-age children-present and lifetime ver- measure of conduct disorder. Journal of Personality
sion (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity Disorders, 30, 232–241.
data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Kiesner, J., Dishion, T. J., & Poulin, F. (2001). A rein-
Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 980–988. forcement model for conduct problems in children
356 K. Renk et al.

and adolescence: Advance in theory and interven- autonomic measures? Archives of General Psychiatry,
tion. In J. Hill & B. Maughan (Eds.), Conduct dis- 49, 429–435.
orders in childhood and adolescence (pp. 264–291). Kruesi, M. J., Rapoport, J. L., Hamburger, S., Hibbs, E.,
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Potter, W. Z., & Lenane, M. (1990). Cerebrospinal
Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways fluid monoamine metabolites, aggression, and impul-
linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer sivity in disruptive behavior disorders of children
relations, and psychopathology. Journal of Child and adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47,
Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 706–716. 419–426.
Kim-Cohen, J., Arseneault, L., Newcombe, R., Adams, LaFreniêre, P. J., Dumas, J., Capuano, F., & Dubeau,
F., Bolton, H., Cant, L., … Moffitt, T. E. (2009). D. (1992). The development and validation of the
Five-year predictive validity of DSM-IV conduct preschool socio-affective profile. Psychological
disorder research diagnosis in 4½-5-year-old chil- Assessment, 4, 442–450.
dren. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 18, Landau, S., & Milich, R. (1990). Assessment of children’s
284–291. social status and peer relations. In A. M. LaGreca
Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, H., (Ed.), Through the eyes of the child: Obtaining self-­
Milne, B. J., & Poulton, R. (2003). Prior juvenile diag- reports from children and adolescents. Boston: Allyn
noses in adults with mental disorders: Developmental and Bacon.
follow-back of a prospective-longitudinal cohort. Larson, K., Russ, S. A., Kahn, R. S., & Halfon, N. (2011).
Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 709–717. Patterns of comorbidity, functioning, and service use
Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Williams, B., for US children with ADHD, 2007. Pediatrics, 127,
Newcombe, R., & Craig, I. W. (2006). MAOA, mal- 462–470.
treatment, and gene-environment interactions pre- Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). The neigh-
dicting children’s mental health: New evidence and borhoods they live in: The effects of neighborhood
a meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 11, 903–913. and residence on child and adolescent outcomes.
Kimonis, E. R., Fanti, K. A., Frick, P. J., Moffitt, T. E., Psychological Bulletin, 126, 309–337.
Essau, C., Bijttebier, P., & Marsee, M. A. (2015). Using Liaw, F., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1994). Cumulative famil-
self-reported callous-unemotional traits to cross-­ ial risks and low birth weight children’s cognitive and
nationally assess the DSM-5 ‘with limited prosocial behavioral development. Journal of Clinical Child
emotions’ specifier. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychology, 23, 360–272.
Psychiatry, 56, 1249–1261. Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Psychological treatments that
Knopik, V. S. (2009). Maternal smoking during preg- cause harm. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
nancy and child outcomes: Real or spurious effect? 2, 53–70.
Developmental Neuropsychology, 34, 1–36. Lochman, J. E. (1992). Cognitive-behavioral interven-
Kohn, M., & Rosman, B. L. (1972). A social compe- tion with aggressive boys: Three-year follow-up and
tence sale and symptom checklist for the preschool preventive effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
child: Factor dimensions, their cross-instrument gen- Psychology, 60, 426–432.
erality, and longitudinal persistence. Developmental Lochman, J. E., Barry, T. D., & Pardini, D. (2003). Anger
Psychology, 6, 430–444. control training for aggressive youth. In A. E. Kazdin
Kolko, D. J., & Pardini, D. A. (2010). ODD dimensions, & J. R. Weisz (Eds.), Evidence-based psychothera-
ADHD, and callous–unemotional traits as predic- pies for children and adolescents (pp. 263–281).
tors of treatment response in children with disruptive New York: Guilford.
behavior disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Keenan, K., & Lahey, B. B.
119, 713–725. (1995). Which boys will fare worse? Early predic-
Kroneman, L. M., Hipwell, A. E., Loeber, R., Koot, H. M., tors of conduct disorder in a six-year longitudinal
& Pardini, D. A. (2011). Contextual risk factors as pre- study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
dictors of disruptive behavior disorder trajectories in Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 499–509.
girls: The moderating effect of callous-unemotional Loeber, R., Wung, P., Keenan, K., Giroux, B., Stouthamer-­
features. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Loeber, M., Van Kammen, W. B., & Maughan, B.
52, 167–175. (1993). Developmental pathways in child disrup-
Kruesi, M. J. P., Hibbs, E. D., Zahn, T. P., Keysor, tive behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 5,
T. S., Hamburger, S. D., Bartko, J. J., & Rapoport, 101–131.
J. L. (1992). A 2-year prospective follow-up study Loney, B. R., & Frick, P. J. (2003). Structured diagnostic
of children and adolescents with disruptive behav- interviewing. In C. R. Reynolds & R. W. Kamphaus
ior disorders: Prediction by cerebrospinal fluid (Eds.), Handbook of educational assessment of chil-
5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid, homovanillic acid, and dren (2nd ed., pp. 235–247). New York: Guilford.
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 357

Loney, B. R., Taylor, J., Butler, M. A., & Iacono, W. G. McCabe, K. M., Lucchini, S. E., Hough, R. L., Yeh, M., &
(2007). Adolescent psychopathy features: 6-year Hazen, A. (2005). The relation between violence expo-
temporal stability and the prediction of ­externalizing sure and conduct problems among adolescents: A pro-
symptoms during the transition to adulthood. spective study. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
Aggressive Behavior, 33, 242–252. 75, 575–584.
Luby, J., & Morgan, K. (1997). Characteristics of an infant/ McCart, M. R., & Sheidow, A. J. (2016). Evidence-based
preschool psychiatric clinic sample: Implications psychosocial treatments for adolescents with disrup-
for clinical assessment and nosology. Infant Mental tive behavior. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
Health Journal, 18, 209–220. Psychology, 45, 529–563.
Lynam, D. R. (1998). Early identification of the fledg- McGilloway, S., NiMhaille, G., Bywater, T., Leckey,
ling psychopath: Locating the psychopathic child Y., Kelly, P., Furlong, M., … Donnelly, M. (2014).
in the current nomenclature. Journal of Abnormal Reducing child conduct disordered behaviour and
Psychology, 107, 566–575. improving parent mental health in disadvantaged
Lynam, D. R., & Henry, V. (2001). The role of neuropsy- families: A 12-month follow-up and cost analysis of a
chological deficits in conduct disorders. In J. Hill & parenting intervention. European Child & Adolescent
B. Maughan (Eds.), Conduct disorders in childhood Psychiatry, 23, 783–794.
and adolescence. Cambridge: Cambridge University McKinney, C., & Renk, K. (2006). Similar presentations
Press. of disparate etiologies: A new perspective on oppo-
Lynam, D. R., Moffitt, T. E., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. sitional defiant disorder. Child and Family Behavior
(1993). Expanding the relationship between IQ and Therapy, 28, 37–49.
delinquency: Class, race, test motivation, school fail- McKinney, C., & Renk, K. (2007). Emerging research and
ure, or self-control? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, theory in the etiology of oppositional defiant disorder:
102, 187–196. Current concerns and future directions. International
MacBrayer, E. K., Milich, R., & Hundley, M. (2003). Journal of Behavior Consultation and Therapy, 3,
Attributional biases in aggressive children and their 349–371.
mothers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, McMahon, R. J., & Estes, A. M. (1997). Conduct prob-
698–708. lems. In E. J. Mash & L. G. Terdal (Eds.), Assessment
Madigan, S., Atkinson, L., Laurin, K., & Benoit, D. of childhood disorders (3rd ed., pp. 130–193).
(2013). Attachment and internalizing behavior in New York: Guilford.
early childhood: A meta-analysis. Developmental McMahon, R. J., & Forehand, R. L. (2003). Helping the
Psychology, 49, 672–689. noncompliant child: Family-based treatment for oppo-
Mager, W., Millich, R., Harris, M. J., & Howard, A. sitional behavior (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.
(2005). Intervention groups for adolescents with con- McMahon, R. J., & Frick, P. J. (2005). Evidence-based
duct problems: Is aggregation harmful or helpful? assessment of conduct problems in children and ado-
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 349–362. lescents. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
Maniglio, R. (2015). Significance, nature, and direction Psychology, 34, 477–505.
of the association between child sexual abuse and con- McNeil, C. B., Eyberg, S., Eisenstadt, T. H., Newcomb,
duct disorder: A systematic review. Trauma, Violence, K., & Funderburk, B. (1991). Parent-child inter-
& Abuse, 16, 241–257. action therapy with behavior problem children:
Manuck, S. B., Flory, J. D., Ferrell, R. E., Dent, K. M., Generalization of treatment effects to the school
Mann, J. J., & Muldoon, M. F. (1999). Aggression setting. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 20,
and anger-related traits associated with a polymor- 140–151.
phism of the tryptophan hydroxylase gene. Biological Menting, B., Van Lier, P. A., & Koot, H. M. (2011).
Psychiatry, 45, 603–614. Language skills, peer rejection, and the development
Marmorstein, N. R., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2009). of externalizing behavior from kindergarten to fourth
Alcohol and illicit drug dependence among parents: grade. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Associations with offspring externalizing disorders. 52, 72–79.
Psychological Medicine, 39, 149–155. Merrell, K. W. (1993). School social behavior scales.
Matthys, W., Vanderschuren, L. J., Schutter, D. J., & Iowa City: Assessment Intervention Resources.
Lochman, J. E. (2012). Impaired neurocognitive func- Merrell, K. W. (2001). Assessment of children’s social
tions affect social learning processes in oppositional skills: Recent developments, best practices, and new
defiant disorder and conduct disorder: Implications for directions. Exceptionality, 9, 3–18.
interventions. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L. D., Kay, G. G.,
Review, 15, 234–246. Moreland, K. L., Dies, R. R., … Read, G. M. (2001).
Maughan, B., Rowe, R., Messer, J., Goodman, R., & Psychological testing and psychological assessment: A
Meltzer, H. (2004). Conduct disorder and oppositional review of evidence and issues. American Psychologist,
defiant disorder in a national sample: Developmental 56, 128–165.
epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Miller-Johnson, S., Lochman, J. E., Coie, J. D., Terry, R.,
Psychiatry, 45, 609–621. & Hyman, C. (1998). Comorbidity of conduct and
358 K. Renk et al.

depressive problems at sixth grade: Substance use out- of conduct problems in young children. Journal of
comes across adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 84, 682–698.
Psychology, 26, 221–232. Nock, M. K., Kazdin, A. E., Hiripi, E., & Kessler, R. C.
Moffitt, T. E. (1990). The neuropsychology of delin- (2006). Prevalence, subtypes, and correlates of
quency: A critical review of theory and research. In DSM-IV conduct disorder in the national comorbid-
M. Morris & M. Tonry (Eds.), Crime and justice: ity survey replication. Psychological Medicine, 36,
An annual review of research (vol. 12, pp. 99–169). 699–710.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Olfson, M., Blanco, C., Liu, L., Moreno, C., & Laje, G.
Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course (2006). National trends in the outpatient treatment of
persistant antisocial behavior: A developmental tax- children and adolescents with antipsychotic drugs.
onomy. Psychological Review, 100, 674–701. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 679–685.
Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Life-course persistant and adolescent-­ Olsson, M. (2009). DSM diagnosis of conduct disorder
limited antisocial behavior: A 10-year research review (CD)—A review. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 63,
and research agenda. In B. B. Lahey, T. E. Moffitt, & 102–112.
A. Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct disorder and juve- Pajer, K., Chung, J., Leininger, L., Wang, W., Gardner, W.,
nile delinquency (pp. 49–75). New York: Guilford. & Yeates, K. (2008). Neuropsychological function in
Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2001). Childhood predictors adolescent girls with conduct disorder. Journal of the
differentiate life-course persistant and adolescent-­ American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
limited antisocial pathways in males and females. 47, 416–425.
Development and Psychopathology, 8, 399–424. Pardini, D., & Frick, P. J. (2013). Multiple developmen-
Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Dickinson, N., Silva, P., & Stanton, tal pathways to conduct disorder: Current conceptu-
W. (1996). Childhood-onset versus adolescent-­onset alizations and clinical implications. Journal of the
antisocial conduct problems in males: Natural his- Canadian Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
tory from ages 3 to 18 years. Development and 22, 21–25.
Psychopathology, 8, 399–342. Pardini, D. A., Waller, R., & Hawes, S. W. (2015).
Moffitt, T. E., & Lynam, D. R. (1994). The neuropsychol- Familial influences on the development of serious
ogy of conduct disorder and delinquency: Implications conduct problems and delinquency. In J. Morizot &
for understanding antisocial behavior. In D. Fowles, L. Kazemian (Eds.), The development of criminal
P. Sutker, & S. Goodman (Eds.), Psychopathy and and antisocial behavior: Theory, research and prac-
antisocial personality: A developmental perspective tical applications (pp. 201–220). Cham, Switzerland:
(vol. 18, pp. 233–262). New York: Springer. Springer.
Mordre, M., Groholt, B., Kjelsberg, E., Sandstad, B., & Pasalich, D. S., Dadds, M. R., Hawes, D. J., & Brennan,
Myhre, A. M. (2011). The impact of ADHD and con- J. (2011). Do callous-unemotional traits moderate
duct disorder in childhood on adult delinquency: A the relative importance of parental coercion versus
30 year follow-up study using official crime records. warmth in child conduct problems? An observational
BMC Psychiatry, 11, 57. study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Mrug, S., & Windle, M. (2010). Prospective effects of vio- 52, 1308–1315.
lence exposure across multiple contexts on early adoles- Pasalich, D. S., Dadds, M. R., Hawes, D. J., & Brennan,
cents’ internalizing and externalizing problems. Journal J. (2012). Attachment and callous-unemotional traits
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 953–961. in children with early-onset conduct problems. Journal
Murray, J., Burgess, S., Zuccolo, L., Hickman, M., Gray, of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(8), 838–845.
R., & Lewis, S. J. (2015). Moderate alcohol drinking Pasalich, D. S., Witkiewitz, K., McMahon, R.,
in pregnancy increases risk for children’s persistent Pinderhughes, E., & Conduct Problems Prevention
conduct problems: Causal effects in a Mendelian ran- Research Group. (2016). Indirect effects of the Fast
domization study. Journal of Child Psychology and Track intervention on conduct disorder symptoms and
Psychiatry, 57, 575–584. callous-unemotional traits: Distinct pathways involv-
Murray, J., & Farrington, D. P. (2010). Risk factors for ing discipline and warmth. Journal of Abnormal Child
conduct disorder and delinquency: Key findings Psychology, 44, 587–597.
from longitudinal studies. The Canadian Journal of Passamonti, L., Fairchild, G., Fornito, A., Goodyer,
Psychiatry, 55, 633–642. I. M., Nimmo-Smith, I., Hagan, C. C., & Calder, A. J.
Murray, J., Farrington, D. P., & Sekol, I. (2012). Children's (2012). Abnormal anatomical connectivity between
antisocial behavior, mental health, drug use, and edu- the amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in conduct dis-
cational performance after parental incarceration: A order. PLoS One, 7, e48789.
systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene,
Bulletin, 138, 175–210. OR: Castalia.
Niec, L. N., Barnett, M. L., Prewett, M. S., & Shanley Patterson, G. R., Chamberlain, P., & Reid, J. B. (1982). A
Chatham, J. R. (2016). Group parent-child interaction comparative evaluation of a parent-training program.
therapy: A randomized control trial for the treatment Behavior Therapy, 13, 638–650.
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 359

Patterson, G. R., & Gullion, M. E. (1968). Living with Renk, K. (2005). Reasons young children are referred for
children: New methods for parents and teachers. psychological services. Child and Family Behavior
Champaign, IL: Research Press. Therapy, 27, 61–71.
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Eddy, J. M. (2002). A Renk, K., & Phares, V. (2004). Cross-informant ratings
brief history of the Oregon model. In J. B. Reid, G. R. of social competence in children and adolescents.
Patterson, & J. Snyder (Eds.), Antisocial behavior in Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 239–254.
children and adolescents: A developmental analysis Renshaw, P. D., & Asher, S. R. (1983). Children’s goals
and model for intervention (pp. 3–21). Washington, and strategies for social interaction. Merrill-Palmer
DC: American Psychological Association. Quarterly, 29, 353–375.
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., Jones, R. R., & Conger, R. E. Rhee, S. H., & Waldman, I. D. (2002). Genetic and envi-
(1975). A social learning approach to family inter- ronmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-­
vention: Families with aggressive children (vol. 1). analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological
Eugene, OR: Castalia. Bulletin, 128, 490–529.
Pelham, W. E. (1993). Pharmocotherapy for children Robinson, E. A., & Eyberg, S. M. (1981). The
with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. School dyadic parent-­ child interaction coding system:
Psychology Review, 22, 199–227. Standardization and validation. Journal of Consulting
Petrosino, A., Turpin-Petrosino, C., & Buehler, J. (2003). and Clinical Psychology, 49, 245–250.
Scared Straight and other juvenile awareness pro- Rockett, J. L., Murie, D. C., & Boccaccini, M. T. (2007).
grams for preventing juvenile delinquency: A system- Diagnostic labeling in juvenile justice settings: Do
atic review of the randomized experimental evidence. psychopathy and conduct disorder findings influence
Annals of the American Academy of Political and clinicians? Psychological Services, 4, 107–122.
Social Science, 589, 41–62. Rockhill, C. M., Collett, B. R., McClellan, J. M., & Speltz,
Pinderhughes, E. E., Nix, R., Foster, E. M., Dones, D., M. L. (2006). Oppositional defiant disorder. In J. L.
Bierman, K. L., Coie, J. D., … McMahon, R. J. (2001). Luby (Ed.), Handbook of preschool mental health:
Parenting in context: Impact of neighborhood poverty, Development, disorders, and treatment (pp. 80–114).
residential stability, public services, social networks, New York: The Guilford Press.
and danger on parental behaviors. Journal of Marriage Rutter, M., Giller, H., & Hagell, A. (1998). Antisocial
and Family, 63, 941–953. behavior by young people. New York: Cambridge
Polier, G. G., Vloet, T. D., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., University Press.
Laurens, K. R., & Hodgins, S. (2012). Comorbidity Sanders, M. R. (1999). Triple P-positive parenting pro-
of conduct disorder symptoms and internalising gram: Towards an empirically validated multilevel
problems in children: Investigating a community parenting and family support strategy for the preven-
and a clinical sample. European Child & Adolescent tion of behavior and emotional problems in children.
Psychiatry, 21, 31–38. Clinical Child and Family Psychological Review, 2,
Posthumus, J. A., Raajmakers, M. A. J., Maassen, G. H., 71–90.
van Engeland, H., & Matthys, W. (2012). Sustained Sanders, M. R., Baker, S., & Turner, K. M. T. (2012). A
effects of Incredible Years as a preventive intervention randomized controlled trial evaluating the efficacy of
in preschool children with conduct problems. Journal Triple P Online with parents of children with early-­
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 487–500. onset conduct problems. Behaviour Research and
Poulin, F., & Boivin, M. (2000). The role of proactive and Therapy, 50, 675–684.
reactive aggression in the formation and development Sanders, M. R., Bor, W., & Morawska, A. (2007).
of boys’ friendships. Developmental Psychology, 36, Maintenance of treatment gains: A comparison of
233–240. enhanced, standard, and self-directed Triple P-positive
Reich, W. (2000). Diagnostic interview for children parenting program. Journal of Abnormal Child
and adolescents (DICA). Journal of the American Psychology, 35, 983–998.
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, Sanders, M. R., Markie-Dadds, C., Tully, L. A., & Bor, W.
59–66. (2000). The Triple P-parenting program: A compari-
Reid, W. H., Dorr, D., Walker, J. I., & Bonner, J. W. son of enhanced, standard, and self-directed behav-
(1986). Unmasking the psychopath: Antisocial per- ioral family intervention for parents of children with
sonality and related syndromes. New York: Norton. early onset conduct problems. Journal of Consulting
Reid, J. B., & Patterson, G. R. (1989). The development of and Clinical Psychology, 68, 624–640.
antisocial behavior patterns in childhood and adoles- Schrank, F. A., Mather, N., & McGrew, K. S. (2014).
cence. European Journal of Personality, 3, 107–119. Woodcock-Johnson IV tests of achievement. Rolling
Reid, M. J., & Webster-Stratton, C. (2001). The Incredible Meadows, IL: The Riverside Publishing Company.
Years parent, teacher, and child intervention: Targeting Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., &
multiple areas of risk for a young child with pervasive Schwab-Stone, M. E. (2000). NIMH diagnostic
conduct problems using a flexible, manualized treat- interview schedule for children version IV (NIMH
ment program. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 8, DISC-IV): Description, differences from previous
377–386. versions, and reliability of some common diagno-
360 K. Renk et al.

ses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and tary school. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29,
Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1228–1231. 17–29.
Shaw, D. S., Owens, E. B., Giovannelli, J., & Winslow, Tiet, Q. Q., Bird, H. R., Hoven, C. W., Moore, R., Wu,
E. B. (2001). Infant and toddler pathways leading to P., & Wicks, J. (2001). Relationship between specific
early externalizing disorders. Journal of the American adverse life events and psychiatric disorders. Journal
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 153–164.
36–43. Trotter, H., & Rafferty, H. (2014). A follow-up to the
Shenk, C. E., Dorn, L. D., Kolko, D. J., Susman, E. J., Incredible Years Parenting programme: The reflec-
Noll, J. G., & Bukstein, O. G. (2012). Predicting tions of mothers one to two years later. Educational &
treatment response for oppositional defiant disorder Child Psychology, 31, 40–57.
and conduct disorder using pre-treatment adrenal van der Stouwe, T., Asscher, J. J., Stams, G. J. J. M.,
and gonadal hormones. Journal of Child and Family Deković, M., & van der Laan, P. H. (2014). The effec-
Studies, 21, 973–981. tiveness of multisystemic therapy (MST): A meta-­
Shillingsburg, M. A. (2005). The use of the establishing analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 34, 468–481.
operation in parent-child interaction therapies. Child van Goozen, S. H., van den Ban, E., Matthys, W., Chen-­
and Family Behavior Therapy, 26, 43–58. Kettenis, P. T., Thijsse, J. H., & van Engeland, H.
Silberg, J., Maes, H., & Eaves, L. (2012). Unraveling the (2000). Increased adrenal androgen functioning in
effect of genes and environment in the transmission children with oppositional defiant disorder: A com-
of parental antisocial behavior to children’s conduct parison with psychiatric and normal controls. Journal
disturbance, depression and hyperactivity. Journal of of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 668–677. Psychiatry, 39, 1446–1451.
Silverthorn, P., & Frick, P. J. (1999). Developmental path- Vander Zanden, J. W., Crandell, T. L., & Crandell, C. H.
ways to antisocial behavior: The delayed-onset path- (2006). Human development (8th ed.). New York:
way in girls. Development and Psychopathology, 27, McGraw-Hill.
383–392. Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Tremblay, R. E. (2000).
Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Meyer, J., Silberg, J., & Maes, Influence of deviant friends on delinquency: Searching
H. (1998). Genetic and environmental influences for moderator variables. Journal of Abnormal Child
on subtypes of conduct disorder behavior in boys. Psychology, 28, 313–325.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 495–509. Walker, H. M., & McConnell, S. (1995). Walker-­
Slough, N. M., McMahon, R. J., & Conduct Problems McConnell scale of social competence and school
Prevention Research Group. (2008). Preventing seri- adjustment, elementary version. San Diego, CA:
ous conduct problems in school-age youth: The Fast Singular.
Track Program. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, Walker, H. M., & Severson, H. (1992). Systematic screen-
15, 3–17. ing for behavior disorders. Longmont, CO: Sopris
Smith, D. K., & Chamberlain, P. (2010) Multidimensional West.
treatment foster care for adolescents: Processes and Waller, R., Gardner, F., Viding, E., Shaw, D. S.,
outcomes. In J. R. Weisz & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Dishion, T. J., Wilson, M. N., & Hyde, L. W. (2014).
Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and Bidirectional associations between parental warmth,
adolescents (2nd ed., pp. 243–258). New York, NY: callous unemotional behavior, and behavior problems
Guilford Press. in high-risk preschoolers. Journal of Abnormal Child
Smith, J. D., Dishion, T. J., Shaw, D. S., Wilson, M. N., Psychology, 42, 1275–1285.
Winter, C. C., & Patterson, G. R. (2014). Coercive fam- Ware, A. L., O’Brien, J. W., Crocker, N., Deweese, B. N.,
ily process and early-onset conduct problems from age Roesch, S. C., Coles, C. D., … Jones, K. L. (2013).
2 to school entry. Development and Psychopathology, The effects of prenatal alcohol exposure and attention-­
26, 917–932. deficit/hyperactivity disorder on psychopathology
Sousa, C., Herrenkohl, T. I., Moylan, C. A., Tajima, E. A., and behavior. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental
Klika, J. B., Herrenkohl, R. C., & Russo, M. J. (2011). Research, 37, 507–516.
Longitudinal study on the effects of child abuse and Waschbusch, D. A. (2002). A meta-analytic examination
children’s exposure to domestic violence, parent-child of comorbid hyperactive-impulsive-attention prob-
attachments, and antisocial behavior in adolescence. lems and conduct problems. Psychological Bulletin,
Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 26, 111–136. 128, 118–150.
Steiner, H., Saxena, K., & Chang, K. (2003). Webster-Stratton, C. (1981a). Modification of mothers’
Psychopharmacologic strategies for the treatment of behaviors and attitudes through a videotape model-
aggression in juveniles. CNS Spectrum, 8, 298–308. ing group discussion program. Behavior Therapy, 12,
Stormshak, E. A., Bierman, K. L., McMahon, R. J., 634–642.
Lengua, L. J., & Conduct Problems Prevention Webster-Stratton, C. (1981b). Videotape modeling: A
Research Group. (2000). Parenting practices and method of parent education. Journal of Clinical Child
child disruptive behavior problems in early elemen- Psychology, 10, 93–98.
Evidence-Based Methods of Dealing with Social Difficulties in Conduct Disorder 361

Webster-Stratton, C. (1982). Teaching mothers through Wells, K. C., Forehand, R. L., & Griest, D. L. (1980).
videotape modeling to change their children’s behav- Generality of treatment effects from treated to untreated
ior. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 7, 279–294. behaviors resulting from a parent training program.
Webster-Stratton, C. (2001). The Incredible Years: Parents, Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 9, 217–219.
teachers, and children training series. Residential Werch, C. E., & Owen, D. M. (2002). Iatrogenic effects
Treatment for Children and Youth, 18, 31–45. of alcohol and drug prevention programs. Journal of
Webster-Stratton, C., & Hammond, M. (1997). Treating Studies on Alcohol, 63, 581–590.
children with early-onset conduct problems: A com- Widom, C. S. (1989). Child abuse, neglect, and adult
parison of child and parent training interventions. behavior: Research design and findings on criminal-
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65, ity, violence, and child abuse. American Journal of
93–109. Orthopsychiatry, 59, 355–367.
Webster-Stratton, C., & Hammond, M. (1998). Conduct Widom, C. S. (1997). Child abuse, neglect, and witness-
problems and level of social competence in Head Start ing violence. In D. Stoff, J. Breiling, & J. D. Maser
children: Prevalence, pervasiveness, and associated (Eds.), Handbook of antisocial behavior (pp. 159–
risk factors. Clinical Child and Family Psychology 170). New York: Wiley.
Review, 1, 101–124. Xie, H., Cairns, B. D., & Cairns, R. B. (2005). The
Webster-Stratton, C., & Reid, M. (2003). The Incredible development of aggressive behaviors among girls:
Years parents, teachers, and children training series: Measurement issues, social functions, and differential
A multifaceted treatment approach for young children trajectories. In D. J. Pepler, K. C. Madsen, C. Webster,
with conduct problems. In A. E. Kazdin & J. R. Weisz & K. S. Levene (Eds.), The development and treatment
(Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies for children of girlhood aggression (pp. 105–136). Mahwah, NJ:
and adolescents (pp. 224–240). New York: Guilford. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Webster-Stratton, C., Reid, J., & Hammond, M. (2001). Yaman, A., Mesman, J., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., &
Social skills and problem-solving training for chil- Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2010). Parenting and
dren with early-onset conduct problems: Who ben- toddler aggression in second-generation immigrant
efits? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, families: The moderating role of child temperament.
943–952. Journal of Family Psychology, 24(2), 208–211.
Webster-Stratton, C., Reid, M. J., & Stoolmiller, M. Yoshikawa, H. (1994). Prevention as cumulative protec-
(2008). Preventing conduct problems and improving tion: Effects of early family support and education
school readiness: Evaluation of the Incredible Years on chronic delinquency and its risks. Psychological
Teacher and Child Training Programs in high-risk Bulletin, 115, 28–54.
schools. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Zajac, K., Randall, J., & Swenson, C. C. (2015).
49, 471–488. Multisystemic therapy for externalizing youth. Child
Wechsler, D. (2014). WISC-V administration and scoring and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America,
manual. Bloomington, MN: PsychCorp. 24, 601–616.
Weiss, B., Caron, A., Ball, S., Tapp, J., Johnson, M., & Zhang, S., & Anderson, S. G. (2010). Low-income single
Weisz, J. R. (2005). Iatrogenic effects of group treat- mothers’ community violence exposure and aggres-
ment for antisocial youth. Journal of Consulting and sive parenting practices. Children and Youth Services
Clinical Psychology, 73, 1036–1044. Review, 32(6), 889–895.
Depression and (Hypo)mania

Patrick Pössel and Thomas D. Meyer

In light of these facts, it seems justified to


Introduction review the literature about social skills and com-
petency focusing on minors diagnosed with a
Mood disorders including depression and (hypo) mood disorder. The following review is not aim-
mania are among the most prevalent mental ing to be fully systematic but to highlight some
health problems which often start early and cause research trends and to show gaps in our
a lot of impairment and disability, even if symp- knowledge.
toms might not reach diagnostic threshold for a
formal diagnosis. Furthermore during adoles-
cence, there is a steep increase in prevalence Definition of the Population
which is especially pronounced for depressive
symptoms and in girls. The latter has been dis- Contrary to hypomania and mania, the core
cussed potentially related to specific interper- symptoms of depression impair more the internal
sonal roles and expectations that girls and women experiences, emotions, and affect of an individ-
are faced which increase the likelihood of experi- ual. Depression is therefore often difficult to rec-
encing stress. Furthermore, there have been theo- ognize from the outside and to identify by
retical models which saw the origins of depression external sources (e.g., friends, relatives, teach-
rooted in interpersonal problems, such as skill ers). Thus, depression in children and adolescents
deficits, loss of social reinforcements, or role is one of the “internalizing” disorders. Hypomania
transitions (e.g., Hammen & Shih, 2014; Mundt, and mania, however, are usually first recognized
Goldstein, Hahlweg, & Fiedler, 1996). by others, especially in younger people.
Furthermore, while symptoms of (hypo)mania
are more closely related to what is usually labeled
as “externalizing” such as distractibility, psycho-
motor activation, or irritability, the associated
formal diagnosis of a bipolar disorder (BD) has
P. Pössel (*) traditionally not been categorized in that way.
Department of Counseling and Human Development,
University of Louisville, Louisville, KY, USA One reason is that until recently BD was not
e-mail: Patrick.possel@louisville.edu diagnosed in young people, especially prepuber-
T.D. Meyer tal children.
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Following the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric
McGovern Medical School, University of Texas Association, 2013), the core symptoms of
Health Science Center, Houston, TX, USA ­depression or more precisely an episode of major
e-mail: Thomas.d.meyer@uth.tmc.edu

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 363


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6_18
364 P. Pössel and T.D. Meyer

depression is (1) depressed—in minors also irrita- When it comes to hypomania and mania, the
ble and cranky—mood and/or (2) loss of interest description of symptoms of both syndromes is
or pleasure. Additional symptoms of depression fairly similar in the DSM-5 (APA, 2013). An epi-
are: sode of persistent elevated, expansive, or irritable
mood must be present which is accompanied by
1. Significant weight loss or decrease in appetite an increased level of energy or activity. Additional
(more than 5% of body weight in a month or symptoms of hypomania and mania are:
failure to meet expected weight gains)
2. Insomnia or hypersomnia 1 . Increased self-esteem or grandiosity
3. Psychomotor agitation or retardation 2. Decreased need for sleep but still feeling

4. Fatigue or lack of energy rested
5. Feelings of worthlessness or guilt 3. Being more talkative than usual or pressure to
6. Decreased concentration or indecisiveness keep talking
7. Recurrent thoughts of death or suicide 4. Flight of ideas or subjective experience that
thoughts are racing
While the symptoms for depression in adults 5. Distractibility
and minors are generally identical, age and devel- 6. Increase in goal-directed activity or psycho-
opment influence the clinical presentation of motor activation
depression in children and adolescents (Fu-I & 7. Excessive involvement in activities that have a
Pang Wang, 2008). For example, an epidemio- high potential for painful consequences
logical study of symptom profiles in clinically
depressed youth ages 7–14 found that irritability As pointed out before, hypomania and mania do
was the most prevalent symptom (84%; Liu et al., not differ so much in the kind of symptoms but in
2006) but closely followed by depressed mood the minimum duration of the episodes and severity
(78.1%), diminished ability to concentrate of impairment. Hypomanic episodes formally need
(76.5%), fatigue (71.6%), insomnia (63.7%), and to last at least 4 days, and mania is diagnosed if the
feelings of worthlessness (62.7%) which are typi- symptoms last at least 1 week or shorter if a hospi-
cal for depression in older age groups as well. In talization is required. From a clinical point of view,
the same study, about the half of the children the severity of impairment is more relevant for dif-
aged 7–10 had recurrent thoughts of death (Liu ferentiating hypomania and mania. Hypomania is
et al., 2006). In a study of boys ages 6–11 at a diagnosed if the distinct episode of altered mood
child psychiatric center, 59% of depressed and energy level is observed by others (e.g., friends,
patients report sadness, with 71% reporting sui- relatives, teachers) and would be described as
cidal ideation, highlighting the severity of dis- unusual and uncharacteristic for the individual’s
tress these children are experiencing (Breton personality. However, there is no significant or
et al., 2012). severe impairment in social, occupational, or other
The DSM-5 considers the impact of age and areas of functioning. The latter is, however,
development on the expression of depression required to give a diagnosis of mania.
(APA, 2013). The possibility that not sadness but As mentioned above, irritability can be an
irritable or cranky mood is a core symptom in expression of depression in minors, but irritabil-
minors was already mentioned above. Further, ity is also seen a lot in children with mania.
the duration of a persistent depressive disorder Furthermore, compared to mania in adults, irrita-
(PDD, formerly known as dysthymia), a less bility is much more prevalent than euphoria in
severe (only two symptoms are required) but children (Geller et al., 2002; Serra et al., 2016).
longer-­ lasting depressive disorder, is reduced Van Meter, Burke, Kowatch, Findling, and
from 2 years in adults to 1 year in children and Youngstrom (2016) looked meta-analytically at
adolescents. the frequency of symptoms of mania in minors
Depression and (Hypo)mania 365

diagnosed with BD. As the most common symp- more times each week for 1 year or more.
toms, they cited increased distractibility, energy, Between outbursts, minors with DMDD display a
goal-directed activity, irritability, and mood labil- persistently irritable or angry mood, most of the
ity with each being present in over 70% of cases; day and nearly every day, which is observable by
euphoric mood was observed on average in 64%. parents, teachers, or peers. It is likely that—
However, they also raise the issue of a large het- before this category was introduced—some stud-
erogeneity between studies. ies about mania in minors were including
While the validity of a diagnosis of (hypo) individuals who would now qualify for this new
mania in adolescence is not really controversial, diagnosis using the former label “BD not other-
the situation is slightly different when it comes to wise specified (NOS).” However, Althoff et al.
prepubertal children. One unresolved issue is the (2016) assume that mood dysregulation disorder
reliability of the diagnosis of mania and hypoma- is a rare condition in the community and highly
nia in children, especially outside of specialty comorbid with other conditions.
clinics. There has been a debate whether mania The terms (hypo)mania and BD will often be
or more generally BD should even be diagnosed, used interchangeably throughout this chapter. The
especially when there is a lack of distinct epi- reason is that a diagnosis of BD is primarily based
sodes, when irritability is the predominant mood on the presence of a hypomanic or manic episode.
and/or when daily ultra-ultra-rapid cycling of This is especially the case in bipolar I disorder
symptoms (i.e., extreme mood instability— where only one lifetime episode of mania but no
sometimes within minutes) is observed (Baroni, additional episode of depression is required (APA,
Lunsford, Luckenbaugh, Towbin, & Leibenluft, 2013). Contrary, for a diagnosis of bipolar II dis-
2009; Roy, Lopes, & Klein, 2014; Youngstrom, order, at least one episode of depression must
Findling, Youngstrom, & Calabrese, 2005). As a have been observed in addition to a hypomanic
side note, compared to the USA, European stud- episode. Especially in minors, formerly a DSM-4
ies suggest that this diagnosis is hardly ever given diagnosis of “BD, not otherwise specified” was
in children (Holtmann, Boelte, & Poustka, 2008; not uncommon which often meant that either the
Meyer, Koßmann-Böhm, & Schlottke, 2004; duration or number of symptoms for (hypo)mania
Reichart, Nolen, Wals, & Hillegers, 2000). has not reached the formal threshold. In DSM-5
Contrary to younger children, the presentation of these can either be diagnosed as “other specified
mania in adolescence is similar to the one in BD” (e.g., “short-­duration hypomanic episodes”)
adults, and the application of the clinical criteria or “unspecified BD.”
as outlined in DSM-5 for adults (APA, 2013) is
justified in individuals on this developmental
stage. Prevalence, Comorbidity,
Some of this discussion about the validity of a and Consequences
diagnosis of BD in children, especially with
regard to repeated outburst of anger and chronic Research suggests that depressive disorders do
irritability, has led the authors of DSM-5 to intro- exist in children as young as age 3 and that the
duce the new category of “disruptive mood dys- prevalence rate for depression in preschoolers
regulation disorder (DMDD)” in the chapter of may be as high as 3% (Bufferd, Dougherty,
“depressive disorders” (Roy et al., 2014). This Carlson, Rose, & Klein, 2012; Egger & Angold,
diagnosis also acknowledges the significant over- 2006; Luby, Belden, Pautschen, Si, & Spitznagel,
lap between depression and externalizing symp- 2009). In school-aged children, the prevalence
toms in children and adolescents to an age of up rate increases significantly, particularly around
to 12 years. A DMDD is characterized by severe ages 9–11 years, and up to 9% of youth experi-
and recurrent temper outbursts that are out of ence a minimum of one depressive episode by the
proportion in intensity or duration to the situa- age of 14 (Abela & Hankin, 2008; Mash &
tion. These outbursts occur, on average, three or Barkley, 2006). This number further increases to
366 P. Pössel and T.D. Meyer

approximately 20% by the age of 18 (Hankin apply as well to those who will develop a
et al., 1998). BD. Third, even when there seems to be an obvi-
Depressive disorders and even the so-called ous genetic vulnerability for hypomania and
subsyndromal depression are associated with risk mania, many children of individuals with BD
for recurring depressive episodes throughout life remain healthy, some develop anxiety and depres-
(Georgiades, Lewinsohn, Monroe, & Seeley, sion, and only about 10% will develop mania and
2006; Rutter, Caspi, & Moffitt, 2003). Early therefore BD themselves (Goodwin & Jamison,
onset of depression also comes with a host of 2007). Some of the studies we will include
additional problems. For example, depression in explicitly refer to mania and BD in minors but
minors is associated with decreased quality of actually look at “high-risk” samples and espe-
life, serious emotional disturbances, and poor to cially offspring of patients with BD. Therefore,
severe functional impairment (Bertha & Balázs, the research question of those studies is often
2013; Kessler, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky, whether social competency and related skills are
& Wittchen, 2012). Adolescents with depressive different in at-risk individuals from what would
symptoms have been shown to have higher sui- be expected in people who are not affected by a
cidality, academic failure, delinquency, interper- family history of a mood disorder. We decided to
sonal distress, substance abuse, and include such studies because in these studies
unemployment (Klein, Torpey, & Bufferd, 2008; often a substantial proportion of those offspring
Patel, Flisher, Hetrick, & McGorry, 2007). have already been diagnosed with depression or
Before talking about prevalence, comorbidity, even a subthreshold BD (Vance, Jones, Espie,
and consequences of (hypo)mania in childhood Bentall, & Tai, 2008; Whitney et al., 2013).
and adolescence, it seems necessary to empha- Therefore, they will provide evidence whether
size some points which are highly relevant for the social skills and behavior are affected across the
research and evidence presented in the context of different forms and stages of (hypo)mania.
BD in general. First of all, much of the research Reliable data about the prevalence of BD in
done in the area of depression in minors might be children in epidemiological and clinical popula-
applicable and relevant to BD because more than tions is still not available (Serra et al., 2016). This
half of the adults diagnosed with BD were in is partially due to the high overlap with symp-
their youths diagnosed with other mental health toms of other conditions and comorbidity with
problems (Duffy, 2014; Goodwin & Jamison, such conditions. Especially comorbidity with
2007). The diagnosis most often found in the ADHD, conduct disorder, and substance use
records of those adults is one of (unipolar) problems is often estimated to be high
depression (Drancourt et al., 2013; Vedel (Marangoni, De Chiara, & Faedda, 2015; Serra
Kessing, Vradi, & Kragh Andersen, 2015). et al., 2016). There is an almost twofold increase
Second, although it is known that yearly about in prevalence of BD during adolescence
1–2% of individuals with depression need to be (Merikangas et al., 2012) which might explain
re-diagnosed as having BD (Angst, Sellaro, the wide range in prevalence estimates for BD in
Stassen, & Gamma, 2005; Ostergaard et al., adolescence between 1% and 3% across studies
2014) and that early onset of recurrent depression (Kim-Cohen et al., 2003; Lewinsohn, Klein, &
increases the likelihood of a future diagnosis of Seeley, 2000; Merikangas et al., 2010). If one
BD (Goodwin & Jamison, 2007), currently we counts subsyndromal manifestations, the preva-
cannot reliably predict who will develop BD over lence rates are even higher (Lewinsohn et al.,
time. While variables such as family history of 2000). However, there is also some evidence
BD or emotional dysregulation have been found from epidemiological studies that stability of BD
to be predictive (Ostergaard et al., 2014; Uchida diagnoses in adolescence over time might be low
et al., 2015), they are not sensitive and specific and that those who have recurrent episodes are a
enough. This means that many of the results very specific subgroup (Shankman et al., 2009;
found in depressed children and adolescents Tijssen et al., 2010). Although the stability of the
Depression and (Hypo)mania 367

diagnosis seems to be low, those who have recur- this pattern as indication that individuals with
rent episodes report more suicide attempts and depression not only self-disclose more negatively
other indicators of psychosocial impairment than individuals without depression but that the
(Lewinsohn et al., 2000; Merikangas et al., 2012). timing of the self-disclosures is inappropriate.
This pattern is crucial as self-disclosures of indi-
viduals with depression are a key reason why
 efinition of Social Skills
D they get rejected by their interaction partners
and Behavior (Gurtman, 1987). In addition, individuals with
depression seem to interpret topics that make per-
Social skills can be defined in very different sons unhappy and uncomfortable and lead to
ways. From a behavioral perspective, they might negative interactions more likely as appropriate
be defined as the ability to maximize the positive topics than individuals without depression
interactions (positive reinforcement) and mini- (Kuiper & McCabe, 1985), while there is no dif-
mize negative interactions (negative reinforce- ference between both groups regarding their
ment, punishment) with others (Libet & interpretation of the appropriateness of topics
Lewinsohn, 1973). Other definitions focus more that make persons comfortable and lead to posi-
on the ability to express feelings and/or interests tive interactions (see also Breznitz, 1992). This
and desires toward others (Liberman, King, pattern of verbal social behavior seems to be par-
DeRisi, & McCann, 1975) without pushing oth- ticularly pronounced when individuals with
ers away (Hersen & Bellack, 1977). Segrin depression interact with intimate interaction part-
(2000) highlights that the common core of all ners (Hautzinger, Linden, & Hoffman, 1982;
definitions of social skills is that they describe the Ruscher & Gotlib, 1988; Segrin & Flora, 1998).
ability to interact with others in an appropriate Studies regarding nonverbal social behavior
and effective way. Following Segrin, “appropri- demonstrate that children with depression are
ateness” means that an individual does not vio- more often alone, initiate fewer interactions, and
late social expectations, norms, and values or in show more aggressive and negative behaviors
other words interaction partners of the individual than their peers without depression (Altmann &
do not see their behavior as negative or odd. Gotlib, 1988). On an even more molecular level,
“Effectiveness” means that an individual reaches minors with depression demonstrate more frown-
his or her goals1 in that interaction (Segrin, 2000). ing, longer response times, and less smiling in
This definition of social skills raises the ques- interactions with adults than their peers without
tion of how verbal and nonverbal social behaviors depressive symptoms (Kazdin, Sherick, Esveldt-­
are related to mood symptoms. Regarding verbal Dawson, & Rancurello, 1985).
social behavior, individuals with depression show
in conversations more negative self-statements
than their peers without depressive symptoms  ocial Support, Skills, and Behavior
S
(Gibbons, 1987; Gurtman, 1987; Jacobson & and Their Associations with Mood
Anderson, 1982), particularly after an interaction Symptoms
partner self-disclosed. Segrin (2000) interprets
The association between social support and
depression in minors is clearly supported by a
It should to be pointed out that many, if not all, social
1 

trainings help the clients to select the “right” goals (e.g.,


recent meta-analysis that summarizes and inte-
Pössel et al., 2004). For example, having the goal of “get- grates 342 studies with 273,430 participants
ting” what one wants (e.g., someone else’s love, being (Rueger, Malecki, Pyun, Aycock, & Coyle,
allowed to stay up the whole night on a school day) is 2016). Rueger and colleagues found an r of .29 in
unrealistic and inappropriate. Worse, such a goal might
even contribute to the depression if it is not reached. A
cross-sectional and .17 in longitudinal studies
more realistic and appropriate goal is the expression of between social support and depression in minors.
one’s own desires, feelings, and interests. Similarly the connection between social skills
368 P. Pössel and T.D. Meyer

and behavior on the one hand and depression on tious versus being hostile, Wolkenstein & Meyer,
the other hand in youth has been established. For 2010). Hypersexuality is a symptom often
example, minors with depression experience observed in adolescents with BD, and when addi-
themselves as lacking social skills; to be more tional substance use problems are present, this
precise, they report to be less able to provide often is linked with increased unplanned preg-
emotional support, make friends, and solve con- nancies (Basco & Cells-de Hoyos, 2012; Geller
flicts (Hammen, Shih, & Brennan, 2004; et al., 2002; Goldstein, Strober, et al., 2008).
Rudolph, Kurlakowsky, & Conley, 2001; Zahn-­ Beyond the classification of social behavior as
Waxler, Klimes-Dougan, & Slattery, 2000). symptom of depression and (hypo)mania, a lack
Further, children and adolescents who rate them- of social skills has been conceptualized as a cru-
selves as socially incompetent are more likely to cial vulnerability for depression for some time
report depressive symptoms (Chan, 1997; Cole, (Lewinsohn, Weinstein, & Shaw, 1969). To be
Martin, Powers, & Truglio, 1996), and both par- more specific, from a behavioral perspective, a
ent and teacher ratings of social competence have lack of social skills is responsible for low rates of
been found to be lower for children and adoles- positive interactions with others (positive rein-
cents who are depressed (or self-report depres- forcement), which is one form of social support
sion) compared with those who are not depressed and causes depression. Segrin and Abramson
(Dalley, Bolocofsky, & Karlin, 1994; Hamilton, (1994) branded this proposed association as the
Asarnow, & Thompson, 1997; Shah & Morgan, “social skill-stress hypothesis.” This hypothesis is
1996). Thus, the nature of the associations supported by a longitudinal study demonstrating
between social support, skills, and behavior and that individuals with a lack of social skills experi-
depression seems to be more interesting than the ence chronic interpersonal stressors (Herzberg
fact that such association exists. et al., 1998), and meta-analysis mentioned before
The associations between social skills and also underscores the negative association between
behavior and depression are to be expected which social support and depression in youth (Rueger
becomes even obvious when one looks at the et al., 2016). However, a variety of other longitu-
above-described diagnostic criteria for depres- dinal studies examining the temporal relationship
sive disorders following DSM-5 (APA, 2013). In between social skills and depression did not find
the DSM-5, the severity of a depressive episode that a lack of social skills predicted later depres-
can be quantified in symptoms like withdrawal, sion (Lewinsohn et al., 1994; Segrin, 1996, 1999).
reduced verbal communication, and decreased Consistent with this seemingly inconsistent pic-
socialization. Similarly, hypomania and mania ture in minors, Cole et al. (1996) found that social
affect interpersonal behavior which is also skills predicted depression in sixth graders but not
reflected in their diagnostic criteria. Individuals in third graders. Thus, the age or developmental
are more talkative and show increased interest in level of youth might play an important role regard-
goal-directed activities which especially involve ing social behavior being a vulnerability for
social and sexual interactions. While the actual depression or not.
behavior can be damaging to the individual them- Further, a lack of social support and/or social
selves (e.g., spending a lot of money for phone skills and behavior might also be a consequence
calls, gifts for others, sexually transmitted dis- of depression. For example, psychomotor retar-
eases), it will depend on the predominant mood dation (including long response latencies,
(euphoria vs. irritability) and the severity of the reduced eye contact, and slowed speech) is
episode (hypomanic vs. manic) how much this another symptom of depression, and those psy-
will in the short and long term affect their rela- chomotor behaviors can easily be interpreted by
tionships (Bowie et al., 2010). Vulnerability for others as indicators of a lack of social skills
hypomania or mania can be associated with (Ellgring & Scherer, 1996; Segrin, 1992;
increased or decreased social skills perceived by Talavera, Saiz-Ruiz, & Garcia-Toro, 1994; for a
the person and others (being funny or more flirta- summary of the research with youth, see Rudolph
Depression and (Hypo)mania 369

& Clark, 2001). Further, other symptoms of manic state can actually enhance perceived social
depression like feelings of worthlessness and skills and behavior in persons who are, for exam-
decreased concentration or indecisiveness can ple, very shy and socially anxious, while depres-
interrupt social behavior and decreased the moti- sion is likely to exacerbate shyness and social
vation to even interact with others (Segrin, 2000). anxiety into more social withdrawal.
In addition, when they do not withdraw, depressed Prospectively, depressive symptoms in BD show
individuals tend to continuously demand reassur- a moderate but stable relationship with interper-
ance from others to substantiate their sense of sonal behavior over time, but once again this is so
self-worth and verify that others care about them far only examined in adults with BD (Morriss
(Joiner & Metalsky, 2001). This continuous et al., 2013). Looking at offspring of individuals
demand may cause others to avoid interacting with BD of which 70% are currently qualified for
with them and therefore diminish social support. a diagnosis of depression and 23% for a bipolar
Thus, it seems obvious that depression is capable spectrum disorder, Whitney et al. (2013) found
of affecting social skills and behavior and even significant impairment in social reciprocity,
social support in a negative way. However, including impairments in social awareness, com-
empirical findings regarding depression dimin- munication, and social motivation based on their
ishing social support and causing a lack or the parents’ ratings but not in objective measures.
impression of a lack of social skills are inconsis- There is evidence that almost 50% of patients
tent. To be more precise, depression does not with BD fully recovered socially and this is spe-
seem to impact social support by adults like par- cifically related to younger age and reporting
ents (Stice, Ragan, & Randall, 2004) and teach- fewer depressive symptoms (Wingo, Baldessarini,
ers (Pössel, Rudasill, Sawyer, Spence, & Bjerg, Compton, & Harvey, 2010). Siegel et al. (2015)
2013; Reddy, Rhodes, & Mulhall, 2003) but by also found that in youth with BD, interpersonal
peers (Prinstein, Borelli, Cheah, Simon, & relationships were generally good and described
Aikins, 2005; Stice et al., 2004). Regarding social as “poor” or worse in less than 20% when it came
skills, Cole et al. (1996) found in the already to peers, siblings, or parents. Furthermore, they
abovementioned longitudinal study no significant reported that overall interpersonal functioning
associations between depression and later social remained fairly stable over time and did only
skills in third or sixth graders, therefore also change before and after the onset of (hypo)mania
questioning whether social skill problems are a but not depression. However, in another study,
long-lasting consequence in youth. parents observed that the onset of BD in their
When looking at BD and (hypo)mania less, children had a detrimental effect on social func-
research has so far focused on social skills and tioning with their kids losing confidence and
behavior in general and in minors with BD in par- feeling alienated from others (Crowe et al., 2011).
ticular. Furthermore, since both depression and Furthermore, Quackenbush, Kutcher, Robertson,
(hypo)mania can be present, it is often not clear Boulos, and Chapan (1996) found that peer rela-
whether any associations with social skills and tionships at school declined after onset of the dis-
behaviors are correlates of current depressive or order among bipolar adolescents, potentially
(hypo)manic symptoms, are a consequence of indicating a change related to the onset of
having experienced mood episodes, or are reflect- BD. Kutcher, Robertson, and Bird (1998) also
ing an underlying vulnerability to (hypo)mania found that according to parents and school
or to depression. Extrapolating from research reports, premorbid academic performance and
done in adults with BD, only a subgroup shows peer relationships were overall good to excellent
social skill deficits. If patients had social skill before the onset of bipolar I disorder in adoles-
deficits, this increased the negative effect depres- cents. Obviously, the onset of current symptoms
sive symptoms (not manic symptoms) had on changes things because Lewinsohn, Klein, and
social functioning (Depp et al., 2010). This shows Seeley (1995) found that (a) adolescents with
the complexity because getting into a (hypo) bipolar, unipolar, and subsyndromal bipolar dis-
370 P. Pössel and T.D. Meyer

orders reported comparable levels of social for this increased risk for adolescent girls to
impairment during episodes of mood disorders, develop depression is that they start to depend
but (b) prospectively there was a decline in social more heavily on peer relationships for their self-
functioning as well in all groups. However, it esteem (Nolen-Hoecksema & Girgus, 1994).
might also depend on the current mood state, Further, Rose and Rudolph (2006) propose based
because Goldstein et al. (2009) found that inter- on their review of the literature to gender differ-
personal functioning was especially impaired ences in peer relationships that girls tend to be
when there were manic and psychotic symptoms more self-­disclose and show stronger interper-
present. Despite the latter result, it seems more sonal engagement and greater concerns about
that depression, rather than (hypo)mania, is asso- their relationship status. However, while gender
ciated with social deficits. differences in various aspects of peer relation-
ships are supported, evidence for gender differ-
ences in the association between social support
Developmental Changes and depression is less clear. To be more specific,
and Gender Differences in Social Rueger and colleagues found in their recent
Support, Behavior, and Skills meta-analysis (2016) that social support is sig-
and Their Associations with Mood nificantly related with depressive symptoms in
Symptoms both male and female youth but also that there
was a significant difference between boys and
When examining the relationships between social girls. However, the difference is so small that the
support, behavior, and skills and mood symptoms authors of the meta-analysis suggest that the dif-
in youth, developmental changes need to be con- ference is only statistically significant due to the
sidered. First, social skills become more relevant large number of studies they could include in
when nonparental adults and peers become more their meta-analysis rather than representing a
important in the lives of minors. To be more spe- real gender difference in youth.
cific, nonparental adults become important sources
of social support in middle childhood, same-­
gender peers become more important during the  ssessment of Social Support
A
transition into early adolescence (Buhrmester, and Social Skills and Behavior
1996; Levitt, Guacci-Franco, & Levitt, 1993), and
in late adolescence romantic partners become rel- Based on the above-reported associations of
evant sources of social support (Collins & Laursen, social support and social skills and behavior with
2004). However, it should be pointed out that this mood disorders in minors, in particular in the
does not mean that social support from adults is context of depressive symptoms, it becomes clear
diminished as peer sources grow in importance how important the assessment of social support
(Colarossi & Eccles, 2003; Rueger, Malecki, & and social skills and behavior is. Their assess-
Demaray, 2010). Nevertheless, while parents ment should be an integral part whenever study-
might forgive a lack of social skills, impaired or ing or working therapeutically with children and
dysfunctional social skills are likely to be answered adolescents with depression or (hypo)mania.
with peer rejection (Murray-Close et al., 2010). We are not aware of formal assessments that
Second, parallel to the increased focus on are exclusively designed to measure social rela-
peer relationships during puberty, rates of tionships or social skills and behavior in minors
depression in female adolescents increase and with depression and (hypo)mania. However, a
more so than in males (Angold, Erkanli, Silberg, variety of instruments to measure social support
Eaves, & Costello, 2002; Ge, Conger, & Elder, and social skills and behavior in children and
2001; Hankin, Mermelstein, & Roesch, 2007). adolescents from the general population are used
Thus, one explanation discussed in the literature in research with youth with mood disorders.
Depression and (Hypo)mania 371

Social Support Summarized, all seven instruments are self-­


report tools for youth and measure multiple
Based on Rueger et al.’s (2016) meta-analysis, sources of support. The number of different
most studies about social support and depression sources ranges from two in the PSSS (Procidano
in minors used one of the following seven instru- & Heller, 1983) and the SSQ (Sarason et al.,
ments measuring social support (Table 1): Child 1983) to eight in the NRI (Furman & Buhrmester,
and Adolescent Social Support Scale (CASSS, 1985). This range of measured sources of support
Malecki, Demaray, & Elliott, 2000), goes hand in hand with the abovementioned
Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social changes in relevance of different sources of social
Support (MSPSS, Zimet, Dahlem, Zimet, & support with the age of the youth. Associated
Farley, 1988), Network of Relationships with that, the age range for which the individual
Inventory (NRI, Furman & Buhrmester, 1985), instruments are evaluated varies widely with the
Perceived Social Support Scale (PSSS, Procidano CASSS (Malecki et al., 2000), SSSCA (Harter,
& Heller, 1983), Social Support Questionnaire 1986), and SOCSS (Dubow & Ullman, 1989)
(SSQ; Sarason, Levine, Basham, & Sarason, covering the widest range from grades 3 to 12
1983), Social Support Scale for Children and and ages 8 to 18 years. Further, while only four of
Adolescents (SSSCA, Harter, 1986), and Survey the instruments were originally developed for
of Children’s Social Support (SOCSS, Dubow & minors (CASSS, Malecki et al., 2000; NRI,
Ullman, 1989). Furman & Buhrmester, 1985; SSSCA, Harter,

Table 1  Most commonly used instruments of social support including age range of validation samples, number of
items, sources of support, and whether network size is measured based on Rueger et al. (2016)
Instrument and authors Age range No. of items Sources of support/network
Child and Adolescent Social Developed and validated 60 Classmate, close friend,
Support Scale (CASSS; Malecki for use in grades 3–12 parent, school, teacher
et al., 2000)
Multidimensional Scale of Developed for adults 12 Family, friend, significant
Perceived Social Support Validated for use with other
(MSPSS; Zimet et al., 1988) youth (mean age
15.8 years)
Network of Relationships Developed and validated 249 Mother, father, sibling,
Inventory (NRI; Furman & for use in ages relative, boy/girlfriend,
Buhrmester, 1985) 11–13 years other-sex friend, another
individual
Perceived Social Support Scale Developed for adults 40 Family, friends
(PSSS; Procidano & Heller, 1983) Validated for use in ages
12–18
Social Support Questionnaire Developed for adults 27 Family
(SSQ; Sarason et al., 1983) Validated for use in Network
grades 8 and 11
Social support scale for children Developed and validated 24 Classmates, friends, parents,
and adolescents (SSSCA; Harter, for use in ages 8–18 years teachers
1986)
Survey of Children’s social Developed and validated 72 Family, peers, teacher
support for use in grade 3 and Network
(SOCSS; Dubow & Ullman, higher
1989)
Based on Rueger, S. Y., Malecki, C. K., Pyun, Y., Aycock, C., & Coyle, S. (2016). A meta-analytic review of the asso-
ciation between perceived social support and depression in childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 142,
1017–1067. Published by APA and adapted
372 P. Pössel and T.D. Meyer

1986; SOCSS, Dubow & Ullman, 1989), all d­ifference in self-­ reports by depressed and
seven were evaluated in minors. Finally, only non-depressed adults, this difference is still
­
three instruments assess the size of the social net- ­significant reflecting a “real” difference in social
work (NRI, Furman & Buhrmester, 1985; SSQ; skills between depressed and non-depressed indi-
Sarason et al., 1983; SOCSS, Dubow & Ullman, viduals. A “real” lack of social skills seems to exist
1989). Besides selecting the instrument that best in youth as well because parents (Hamilton et al.,
fits the purpose of the research and the age group 1997), peers (Kennedy, Spence, & Hensley, 1989),
studied, when designing a study, the lengths of teachers (Dalley et al., 1994; Shah & Morgan,
the selected instrument are crucial to avoid over- 1996), and trained observers (Fauber, Forehand,
burden the participants and to allow to measure Long, Burke, & Faust, 1987) all report a lack of
all necessary constructs. Thus, that the number of social skills in depressed minors.
items ranges from 12 items in the MSPSS (Zimet Related to this topic, studies comparing self-
et al., 1988) to 249 in the NRI (Furman & and other reports of social skills and behavior by
Buhrmester, 1985) is likely relevant for many cli- depressed and non-depressed individuals found
nicians and researchers. significant differences between self- and other
reports of social skills, but not social behavior
(Ducharme & Bachelor, 1993; for a review, see
Social Skills and Behavior Segrin & Dillard, 1993). However, it should be
mentioned that Ducharme and Bachelor (1993)
Before we can examine instruments to assess found the following: when trying to differentiate
social skills and behavior in minors with depres- between depressed and non-depressed clients of
sion or (hypo)mania, we need to discuss another a university counseling center by looking at ver-
crucial topic. Compared to individuals without bal (amount of speech, ratio of time spent talking
depression, depressed individuals evaluate their about interaction partner vs. self) and nonverbal
own social skills more negatively, and currently social behavior (eye contact, smiling), neither the
hypomanic individuals will likely overestimate self-ratings of the clients nor the ratings of their
their social skills and be disinhibited (Benarous, interaction partners or external observers are
Mikita, Goodman, & Stringaris, 2016). A meta-­ allowed to do so. In other words, they found no
analysis quantified the difference in self-ratings of differences in social behavior between college
social skills between depressed and non-­depressed students with and without depression. Thus, it is
adults as r = .30–.61 (Segrin, 1990). This differ- possible that the overall lack of significant find-
ence in self-ratings is also consistently found in ings regarding social behavior is responsible for
depressed and non-depressed minors (Chan, 1997; the similarities between self- and other ratings in
Dalley et al., 1994). This points to a negative self- regard to social behavior in this study.
evaluation bias in adults and minors with depres- When focusing on adolescents with BD,
sion. This might raise the question whether such a Goldstein, Miklowitz, and Mullen (2008) differ-
bias can completely explain the repeatedly found entiate between social skill knowledge and actual
deficits in self-reported social skills in depressed social skill performance and argue that this could
individuals. However, according to Dykman, be especially relevant in the context of experienc-
Horowitz, Abramson, and Usher (1991), the nega- ing different mood states and showing different
tive self-ratings of social skills by depressed indi- behaviors (e.g., socially withdrawn when
viduals reflect both the described self-evaluation depressed, people seeking when hypo(manic)).
bias and an actual lack in social skills. This hypoth- Adolescents with BD who were experiencing
esis is supported by the already abovementioned minimal symptoms at the time of testing as well
meta-analysis. In this meta-analysis, Segrin (1990) as their parents were interviewed. Compared to
quantified the effect for depressed-non-depressed adolescents without BD, adolescents with BD did
differences in other ratings of social skills as not differ in their social skill knowledge, but they
r = .22 (Segrin, 1990). While weaker than the reported more social skill deficits. For example,
Depression and (Hypo)mania 373

they rated themselves as more inappropriately therapist or—in case of a group therapy—with a
assertive, impulsive, jealous, withdrawn, and peer. Those informal assessments of social skills
overconfident than their peers without BD. and behavior translate almost immediately into
Consistently, with the rating of the adolescents, the training of social skills and behavior that ther-
parents of adolescents with BD rated their apist and client see as in need of improvement
offspring as being more socially impaired.
­ (for therapy manuals demonstrating this, see
However, ratings of social interactions with the Clarke, Lewinsohn, & Hops, 1990; Pössel, Horn,
interviewers could not differentiate between ado- Seemann, & Hautzinger, 2004).
lescents with and without BD. Based on the Before summarizing the information pre-
­findings presented here, it seems advisable for sented in Table 2, two details regarding the listed
researchers and clinicians to consider both self- instruments need to be discussed. First, the origi-
and other ratings when measuring social skills in nal Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with
depressed minors, while self-ratings might be Youngsters-II (MESSY-II; Matson, Rotatori, &
sufficient when assessing social behavior. The Helsel, 1983; Matson, Neal, Hess, et al., 2010;
same might be highly informative in currently Matson, Neal, Worley, Kozlowski, & Fodstad,
(hypo)manic minors, but it might be more diffi- 2012) had separated versions for adolescents,
cult to get valid self-reports from manic patients parents, and teachers. The MESSY-II, however,
depending on the severity of the mania. solely includes a scale for parents/caregivers. As
Irrespective of the value of self-reports in Matson et al. (2010) point out, the version for the
depressed minors, social skills and behavior can minors themselves was dropped from the
be assessed formally and informally, depending MESSY-II due to low insight of the subgroups of
on the purpose of the assessment and the setting. minors that are usually asked to fill out the
The different forms of evidence-based psycho- MESSY (e.g., minors with developmental dis-
therapy which are effective in minors with mood ability, intellectual disability, mental health
disorders, especially depression (e.g., behavioral issues). Further, the teacher scale was not
therapy, cognitive behavioral therapy, interper- included in the MESSY-II; it was mainly used in
sonal psychotherapy; for a more detailed descrip- community and clinic settings, and teachers were
tion, see Implications for Treatment), might focus barely asked to fill out the MESSY.
on different aspects of social skills and behavior, In summary, six out of the seven instruments
but they usually involve how to establish relation- measuring youths’ social skills and behaviors
ships with others and how to behave more effec- (including original MESSY) are or include a ver-
tively in interpersonal situations. When it comes sion for the youths themselves. Four instruments
to (hypo)mania and BD, the same applies, but include versions for parents and two (including
often the parents or the families are involved in the original MESSY) for teachers, and one is an
the treatment, and one of the core themes is on instrument asking classmates for the social skills
communication skills in families (Fristad & and behaviors of their peers. Unfortunately, there
MacPherson, 2014; Miklowtiz, 2016). Thus, is no instrument using all four sources of infor-
those areas should be the main focus when mation. Furthermore, one of the two instruments
assessing social behavior and social skills in that includes information from three sources
minors, no matter if the assessment is formal or dropped two sources in the most recent updates,
informal. leaving the Social Skills Improvement System,
While formal assessments of social skills and Social Skills subscale (SSIS; Gresham & Elliott,
behavior could be used within psychotherapy, 2008) as the only instrument utilizing informa-
informal assessments are more common. tion from the youths themselves, parents, and
Informal assessments sometimes include obser- teachers. Contrary to the instruments measuring
vations of the client in real-world settings like social support, all instruments measuring social
schools but are more often focused on the spe- skills and behaviors are developed for minors.
cific behavior of a client in role-plays with the However, all but the MESSY-II (Matson, Neal,
374 P. Pössel and T.D. Meyer

Table 2  Instruments to measure social skills and behavior previously used in relationship with depressive and/or
(hypo)manic symptoms in youth including age range of validation samples, number of items, and sources of
information
No. of Sources of
Instrument and authors Age range items Subscales information
Adjustment Scales for Validated for use 41 Academic difficulty, aggression-­ Classmates
Sociometric in grades 9–12 disruptiveness, anxiety, social
Evaluation of competence, withdrawal
Secondary-School
Students (ASSESS;
Prinz, Swan, Liebert,
Weintraub, & Neale,
1978)
Children’s Self-­ Validated for use 21 Likeability, social ingenuousness, Self
Report Social Skills in grades 4–6 social rules
Scale (CS; Danielson
& Phelps, 2003)
Interpersonal Validated for use 40 Conflict management, emotional Self
Competence with youth (age supportiveness, initiation of
Questionnaire (ICQ; range 16–19; relationships, negative assertion,
Buhrmester, Furman, mean age self-disclosure
Wittenberg, & Reis, 18.3 years)
1988)
Matson Evaluation of Validated for use 62 (S), 64 MESSY: Parents, MESSY
Social Skills with in ages 2–16 years (P, T) Appropriate social skills, only: Self,
Youngsters-II (MESSY), inappropriate assertiveness teachers
(MESSY-II; Matson 4–18 years MESSY-II: Adaptive/appropriate,
et al., 1983; Matson, (MESSY-II) hostile, inappropriate assertive/
Neal, Hess, et al., overconfident
2010; Matson, Neal,
Worley, Kozlowski, &
Fodstad, 2012)
Social Competence Validated for use 9 (S), 10 No subscales Self, parents
with Peers in ages (P)
Questionnaire (SCPS; 12–19 years
Spence, 1995)
Social Skills Validated for use 46 Assertion, communication, Self, parents,
Improvement System, in ages cooperation, empathy (S only), teachers
Social Skills subscale 3–18 years; engagement, responsibility (P
(SSIS; Gresham & self-report, only), self-control
Elliott, 2008) 8–18 years
Social Skills Validated for use 30 No subscales Self, parents
Questionnaire (SSQ; in ages
Spence, 1995) 12–19 years

Worley, et al., 2012) and the SSIS (Gresham & all necessary constructs. Thus, that the number of
Elliott, 2008) are evaluated for a relatively nar- items ranges from 9 items in the SCPS (Spence,
row age range. In addition, all instruments with a 1995) to 62/64 in the MESSY-II (Matson, Neal,
narrow age range are developed for adolescents Worley, et al., 2012) is likely relevant for many
but not for children. Finally, as discussed regard- clinicians and researchers.
ing the instruments to measure social support, Fodstad and Matson (2009) point out how
when designing a study, the lengths of the important direct behavioral observations of social
selected instrument are crucial to avoid overbur- behavior of a minor with depression can be to
dening the participants and to allow to measure understand the underlying problems and to moni-
Depression and (Hypo)mania 375

tor the progress of a treatment. Thus, they sug- can be seen on the example of affect-related
gest a more formal assessment in which the target expression. Frowning and smiling basically
behavior is operationally defined, and this behav- measure (simple) facial expressions, while argu-
ior is broken down into discrete, observable ing and complaining measure verbal expression
(overt), and simple to be described behaviors of negative emotions toward someone else. Thus,
which can be reliably observed by multiple the level and complexity of assessed behavior
observers. Further, Fodstad and Matson (2009) are very different. Obviously, other behavioral
highlight the importance of selecting the most observation systems have to handle similar
appropriate settings in which the social behavior challenges.
should be observed. Finally, ways to conduct
independent and reliable assessments of the tar-
geted social behavior need to be established. Implications for  Treatment
Typical methods to measure behavior include
duration recording (measuring the time for how As stated above, the different forms of evidence-­
long the targeted social behavior occurred), event based psychotherapy for minors with mood disor-
recording (counting the number of times the tar- ders (e.g., behavioral therapy, cognitive behavioral
geted social behavior occurred in a predefined therapy, interpersonal psychotherapy) all consider
time span), and time sampling (indicating social support and social skills and behavior as
whether or not the targeted social behavior relevant. They usually focus on how to establish
occurred during a predefined time span). As a relationships with others and how to behave more
good starting point for observation systems effectively in interpersonal situations. When it
developed for a particular patient or research comes to BD and (hypo)mania, interpersonal
study, Fodstad and Matson (2009) recommend a issues are likely to be addressed alongside other
system developed by Kazdin (1990), as this core topics such as sleep-wake rhythms, medica-
observation system (a) includes affect-related tion, and so on, and furthermore when the focus is
expression, social activity, and solitary behavior on interpersonal situations, it is mainly on interac-
as broad categories of social behavior which are tions with and within the family.
relevant for minors with depression and (b) can There are multiple variables described in this
be adapted for the use in a wide range of settings chapter that should be considered by clinicians.
including but not limited to classroom behavior, First, the associations between social support,
lunch time, and leisure time activities. Affect-­ skills, and behavior and depression seem to be
related expression includes frowning, smiling, bidirectional. In other words, a lack of social sup-
arguing, and complaining. Social activity port, skills, and behavior seems to be risk factors
includes interaction with staff during a conversa- for the development but also symptoms and con-
tion or play activity as well as talking, playing a sequences of depression. Thus, a lack of social
game, and participating in a group activity with support, skills, and behavior does not only
peers. Finally, solitary behavior includes groom- increase the likelihood of developing depression
ing oneself, listening and watching, playing a but also worsens a current depression and
game alone, straightening the own room, and increases the likelihood of a relapse after recover-
working on an academic task (Kazdin, 1990). ing from depression. Thus, working on social sup-
Obviously, this kind of observation can be done port, skills, and behavior seems to be a promising
regardless of whether the mood of the minor is target on all stages of intervention (primary pre-
depressed, (hypo)manic, or euthymic to identify vention, treatment, relapse prevention of depres-
the triggers, maintaining factors, or consequences sion). Although most of the evidence is based on
of behaviors. patients who experienced only depressive symp-
Kazdin’s (1990) observation system clearly toms, the assumption of bidirectionality is most
highlights some of the challenges that are associ- likely to be true for bipolar depression as well
ated with measures of actual social behaviors as (e.g., Johnson, Meyer, Winett, & Small, 2000).
376 P. Pössel and T.D. Meyer

Second, when a clinician considers working skills but also for the prevention and therapy of
with their client on improving their social support depression. One limitation all definitions listed
by others, the age of the client is crucial. The age above seem to have is that they assume that social
will affect the sources of social support they can behavior is solely determined by the ability of the
and want to focus on. While parents seem to be individual. One other important factor why indi-
an important source of social support throughout viduals do not always behave to the best of their
childhood and adolescence (Colarossi & Eccles, abilities is that they may not have the motivation
2003; Rueger et al., 2010), other sources come to do so and, thus, they might not appear as
and go. To be more precise, same-gender peers skilled as they are. This is relevant for the treat-
become important during early adolescence ment of depression as reduced motivation or bet-
(Buhrmester, 1996; Levitt et al., 1993), and ter loss of interest is one of the core symptoms of
romantic partners become relevant sources of depression. This leads us back to the question of
social support in late adolescence (Collins & how to measure social skills as only self-report,
Laursen, 2004). Thus, while working on improv- but not other report or external behavioral obser-
ing social support by parents seems appropriate vations allow separating out if a youth lacks
in all age groups, a clinician should consider that social skills or the motivation to use available
impaired or dysfunctional social skills are likely skills. A further factor impacting the actually
to be answered with peer rejection (Murray-­ demonstrated social behavior is the emotional
Close et al., 2010). Thus, social support by peers dysregulation or reactivity to situational cues in
might be more lacking than social support by par- some individuals vulnerable to mood disorders.
ents. Therefore, a clinician might want to focus This dysregulation or reactivity might inhibit the
on working on social skills and behavior that are display of the most appropriate social behavior in
relevant in interactions with peers. favor of a more emotionally influenced reaction.
Third, when measuring the social support, In conclusion, a social skill training is not in all
skills, and behavior of a client, a clinician should cases the method of choice for the treatment of
consider how to assess those variables. While depression. This hypothesis is also supported by
youth with mood disorders have been shown to research demonstrating that interventions includ-
have problems regarding social support, skills, ing a social skill component not always show
and behavior, they themselves will likely evalu- effects on depressive symptoms, even when they
ate their own social skills more negatively when improve social outcome variables (Pössel, Horn,
currently depressed and overestimate their social & Hautzinger, 2003).
skills when (hypo)manic (Benarous et al., 2016).
However, this self-evaluation bias seems not to
impact the self-description of social behavior References
(Ducharme & Bachelor, 1993; for a review, see
Segrin & Dillard, 1993). While there is not much Abela, J. R., & Hankin, B. L. (2008). Handbook of
depression in children and adolescents. New York,
research with regard to the self-evaluation bias NY: Guilford Press.
and social support, it is likely that the self-­ Althoff, R. R., Crehan, E. T., He, J. P., Burstein, M.,
evaluation bias impacts the self-rating of social Hudziak, J. J., & Merikangas, K. R. (2016). Disruptive
support as well. Thus, it seems clinicians might mood dysregulation disorder at ages 13–18: Results
from the National Comorbidity Survey-Adolescent
want to consider using both self- and other rat- Supplement. Journal of Child and Adolescent
ings when measuring social support and skills, Psychopharmacology, 26, 107–113. doi:10.1089/
while self-ratings might be sufficient when cap.2015.0038
assessing social behavior. Altmann, E. O., & Gotlib, I. H. (1988). The social behav-
ior of depressed children: An observational study.
Forth, and as Segrin (2000) points out, the Journal of Abnormal Child Psychiatry, 16, 29–44.
definition of social skills as the ability to interact American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
with others in an appropriate and effective way and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
has implications for the assessment of social Washington, DC: Author.
Depression and (Hypo)mania 377

Angold, A., Erkanli, A., Silberg, J., Eaves, L., & Costello, Clarke, G. N., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Hops, H. (1990).
E. J. (2002). Depression scale scores in 8–17-year-­ Instructor’s manual for the adolescent coping with
olds: Effects of age and gender. Journal of Child depression course. The therapist manual and the ado-
Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 1052–1063. lescent workbook may be downloaded for free from
Angst, J., Sellaro, R., Stassen, H. H., & Gamma, A. the Internet at http://www.kpchr.org/acwd.html
(2005). Diagnostic conversion from depression to Colarossi, L. G., & Eccles, J. S. (2003). Differential
bipolar disorders: Results of a long-term prospective effects of support providers on adolescents’ mental
study of hospital admissions. Journal of Affective health. Social Work Research, 27, 19–30. doi:10.1093/
Disorders, 84, 149–157. swr/27.1.19
Baroni, A., Lunsford, J. R., Luckenbaugh, D. A., Towbin, Cole, D. A., Martin, J. M., Powers, B., & Truglio, R.
K. E., & Leibenluft, E. (2009). Practitioner review: The (1996). Modeling causal relations between academic
assessment of bipolar disorder in children and adoles- and social competence and depression: A multitrait-­
cents. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & multimethod longitudinal study of children. Journal of
Allied Disciplines, 50, 203–215. Abnormal Psychology, 105, 258–270.
Basco, M. R., & Cells-de Hoyos, C. E. (2012). Collins, W. A., & Laursen, B. (2004). Changing relation-
Biopsychosocial model of hypersexuality in adoles- ships, changing youth: Interpersonal contexts of ado-
cent girls with bipolar disorder: Strategies for inter- lescent development. Journal of Early Adolescence,
vention. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric 24, 55–62. doi:10.1177/0272431603260882
Nursing, 25, 42–50. Crowe, M., Inder, M., Joyce, P., Luty, S., Moor, S., &
Benarous, X., Mikita, N., Goodman, R., & Stringaris, A. Carter, J. (2011). Was it something I did wrong? A
(2016). Distinct relationships between social aptitude qualitative analysis of parental perspectives of their
and dimensions of manic-like symptoms in youth. child’s bipolar disorder. Journal of Psychiatric and
European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 25, 831– Mental Health Nursing, 18, 342–348.
842. doi:10.1007/s00787-015-0800-7 Dalley, M. B., Bolocofsky, D. N., & Karlin, N. J. (1994).
Bertha, E. A., & Balázs, J. (2013). Subthreshold depres- Teacher-ratings and self-ratings of social competency
sion in adolescence: A systematic review. European in adolescents with low- and high-depressive symp-
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 22, 589–603. toms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22,
doi:10.1007/s00787-013-0411-0 477–485.
Bowie, C. R., Depp, C. A., McGrath, J. A., Wolyniec, Danielson, C. K., & Phelps, C. R. (2003). The assess-
P., Mausbach, B. T., Thornquist, M. H., … Pulver, ment of children’s social skills through self-report:
A. E. (2010). Prediction of real world functional dis- A potential screening instrument for classroom use.
ability in chronic mental disorders: A comparison of Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and
schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. American Journal Development, 35, 218–229.
of Psychiatry, 167, 1116–1124. doi:10.1176/appi. Depp, C. A., Mausbach, B. T., Harvey, P. D., Bowie, R. R.,
ajp.2010.09101406 Wolyniec, P. S., Thornquist, M. H., … Patterson, T. L.
Breton, J. J., Labelle, R., Huynh, C., Berthiaume, C., (2010). Social competence and observer-rated social
St-Georges, M., & Guilé, J. M. (2012). Clinical functioning in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorder, 12,
characteristics of depressed youths in child psychia- 843–850. doi:10.1111/j.1399-5618.2010.00880.x
try. Journal of the Canadian Academy of Child and Drancourt, N., Etain, B., Lajnef, M., Henry, C., Raust, A.,
Adolescent Psychiatry, 21, 23–29. Cochet, B., … Bellivier, F. (2013). Duration of untreated
Breznitz, Z. (1992). Verbal indicators of depression. bipolar disorder: Missed opportunities on the long road
Journal of General Psychology, 119, 351–363. to optimal treatment. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,
Bufferd, S. J., Dougherty, L. R., Carlson, G. A., Rose, 127, 136–144. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2012.01917.x
S., & Klein, D. N. (2012). Psychiatric disorders in Dubow, E. F., & Ullman, D. G. (1989). Assessing social
preschoolers: Continuity from ages 3 to 6. American support in elementary school children: The survey of
Journal of Psychiatry, 169, 1157–1164. children’s social support. Journal of Clinical Child
Buhrmester, D. (1996). Need fulfillment, interpersonal com- Psychology, 18, 52–64.
petence, and the developmental contexts of early adoles- Ducharme, J., & Bachelor, A. (1993). Perception of social
cent friendship. In W. M. Bukowski, A. F. Newcomb, & functioning in dysphoria. Cognitive Therapy and
W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they keep: Friendship Research, 17, 53–70.
in childhood and adolescence (pp. 158–185). New York, Duffy, A. (2014). Toward a comprehensive clinical stag-
NY: Cambridge University Press. ing model for bipolar disorder: Integrating the evi-
Buhrmester, D., Furman, W., Wittenberg, M. T., & Reis, dence. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 59, 659–666.
H. T. (1988). Five domains of interpersonal compe- Dykman, B. M., Horowitz, L. M., Abramson, L. Y., &
tence in peer relationships. Journal of Personality and Usher, M. (1991). Schematic and situational deter-
Social Psychology, 55, 991–1008. minants of depressed and nondepressed students’
Chan, D. W. (1997). Depressive symptoms and perceived interpretation of feedback. Journal of Abnormal
competence among Chinese secondary school students Psychology, 100, 45–55.
in Hong Kong. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 26, Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2006). Common emotional
303–319. and behavioral disorders in preschool children:
378 P. Pössel and T.D. Meyer

Presentation, nosology, and epidemiology. Journal of adolescents with bipolar spectrum disorders. Bipolar
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 313–337. Disorder, 10, 469–478.
Ellgring, H., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Vocal indicators Goodwin, F. K., & Jamison, K. R. (2007). Manic-­
of mood change in depression. Journal of Nonverbal depressive illness. Bipolar disorders and recurrent
Behavior, 20, 83–110. depression (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University
Fauber, R., Forehand, R., Long, N., Burke, M., & Faust, Press.
J. (1987). The relationship of young adolescent Gresham, F. M. A., & Elliott, S. N. (2008). Social Skills
Children’s Depression Inventory scores to their social Improvement System manual. New York City, NY:
and cognitive functioning. Journal of Psychopathology Pearson.
and Behavioral Assessment, 9, 161–172. Gurtman, M. B. (1987). Depressive affect and disclo-
Fodstad, J. C., & Matson, J. L. (2009). Major depres- sures as factors in interpersonal rejection. Cognitive
sion. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Social behavior and skills Therapy and Research, 11, 87–100.
in children (pp. 245–265). New York, NY: Springer. Hamilton, E. B., Asarnow, J. R., & Thompson, M. C.
doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-0234-412 (1997). Social, academic, and behavioral competence
Fristad, M. A., & MacPherson, H. A. (2014). Evidence-­ of depressed children: Relationship to diagnostic sta-
based psychosocial treatments for child and adoles- tus and family interaction style. Journal of Youth and
cent bipolar spectrum disorders. Journal of Clinical Adolescence, 26, 77–87.
Child and Adolescence Psychology, 43, 339–355. Hammen, C., & Shih, J. H. (2014). Depression and inter-
Fu-I, L., & Pang Wang, Y. (2008). Comparison of demo- personal processes (Chapter 15). In I. H. Gotlib &
graphic and clinical characteristics between children C. L. Hammen (Eds.), Handbook of depression (3rd
and adolescents with major depressive disorder. ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 30, 124–131. Hammen, C., Shih, J. H., & Brennan, P. A. (2004).
Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children’s per- Intergenerational transmission of depression: Test of
ceptions of the personal relationships in their social an interpersonal stress model in a community sample.
networks. Developmental Psychology, 21, 1016–1024. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72,
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.21.6.1016 511–522. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.72.3.511
Ge, X., Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (2001). Pubertal Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Moffitt, T. E., McGee, R.,
transition, stressful life events, and the emergence of Silva, P. A., & Angell, K. E. (1998). Development of
gender differences in adolescent depressive symp- depression from preadolescence to young adulthood:
toms. Developmental Psychology, 37, 404–417. Emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.3.404 study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 128–
Geller, B., Zimerman, B., Williams, M., DelBello, 140. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.107.1.128
M. P., Bolhofner, K., Craney, J. L., … Nickelsburg, Hankin, B. L., Mermelstein, R., & Roesch, L. (2007). Sex
M. J. (2002). DSM-IV mania symptoms in a prepu- differences in adolescent depression: Stress exposure
bertal and early adolescent bipolar disorder pheno- and reactivity models. Child Development, 78, 279–
type compared to Attention-Deficit-Hyperactive and 295. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00997.x
normal controls. Journal of Child and Adolescent Harter, S. (1986). Manual for the social support scale for
Psychopharmacology, 12, 11–25. children and adolescents. Denver, CO: Author.
Georgiades, K., Lewinsohn, P. M., Monroe, S. M., & Hautzinger, M., Linden, M., & Hoffman, N. (1982).
Seeley, J. R. (2006). Major depressive disorder in Distressed couples with and without a depressed part-
adolescence: The role of subthreshold symptoms. ner: An analysis of their verbal interaction. Journal of
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 13,
Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 936–944. doi:10.1097/01. 307–314.
chi.0000223313.25536.47 Hersen, M., & Bellack, A. S. (1977). Assessment of social
Gibbons, F. X. (1987). Mild depression and self-­disclosure skills. In A. R. Cininero & K. S. Calhoun (Eds.),
intimacy: Self and others’ perceptions. Cognitive Handbook of behavior assessment (pp. 509–554).
Therapy and Research, 11, 361–380. New York: Wiley.
Goldstein, T. R., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D., Goldstein, Herzberg, D. S., Hammen, C., Burge, D., Daley, S. E.,
B. I., Kay Gill, M., Esposito-Smythers, C., … Keller, Davila, J., & Lindberg, N. (1998). Social competence
M. (2009). Psychosocial functioning among bipolar as a predictor of chronic interpersonal stress. Personal
youth. Journal of Affective Disorders, 114, 174–183. Relationships, 5, 207–218.
Goldstein, T. R., Miklowitz, D. J., & Mullen, K. L. (2008). Holtmann, M., Boelte, S., & Poustka, F. (2008). Rapid
Social skills knowledge and performance among ado- increase in rates of bipolar diagnosis in youth: “True”
lescents with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 8, bipolarity or misdiagnosed Severe Disruptive Behavior
350–361. Disorders? Archives of General Psychiatry, 65, 477.
Goldstein, B. I., Strober, M. A., Birmaher, B., Axelson, Jacobson, N. S., & Anderson, E. A. (1982). Interpersonal
D. A., Esposito-Smythers, C., Godstein, T. R., … skill and depression in college students: An analysis of
Keller, M. B. (2008). Substance use disorders among the timing of self-disclosures. Behavior Therapy, 13,
271–282.
Depression and (Hypo)mania 379

Johnson, S. L., Meyer, B., Winett, C., & Small, J. (2000). pathology: II. Psychosocial risk factors for depression.
Social support and self-esteem predict changes in Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 302–315.
bipolar depression but not mania. Journal of Affective Lewinsohn, P. M., Weinstein, M. S., & Shaw, D. A.
Disorders, 58, 79–86. (1969). Depression: A clinical research approach.
Joiner, T. E., Jr., & Metalsky, G. I. (2001). Excessive In R. D. Rubin & C. M. Franks (Eds.), Advances in
reassurance seeking: Delineating a risk factor behavior therapy (pp. 231–240). New York, NY:
involved in the development of depressive symptoms. Academic Press.
Psychological Science, 5, 371–378. Liberman, R. P., King, L. W., DeRisi, W. J., & McCann,
Kazdin, A. E. (1990). Childhood depression. Journal M. (1975). Personal effectiveness. Champaign, IL:
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 121–160. Research Press.
doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb02276.x Libet, J., & Lewinsohn, P. M. (1973). The concept of
Kazdin, A. E., Sherick, R. B., Esveldt-Dawson, K., social skill with special reference to the behavior of
& Rancurello, M. D. (1985). Nonverbal behavior depressed persons. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
and childhood depression. Journal of the American Psychology, 40, 304–312.
Academy of Child Psychiatry, 24, 303–309. Liu, X., Gentzler, A. L., Tepper, P., Kiss, E., Kothencné,
Kennedy, E., Spence, S. H., & Hensley, R. (1989). An V., Tamás, Z., … Kovacs, M. (2006). Clinical features
examination of the relationship between childhood of depressed children and adolescents with various
depression and social competence amongst primary forms of suicidality. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
school children. Journal of Clinical Psychology and 67, 1442–1450. doi:10.4088/JCP.v67n0917
Psychiatry, 30, 561–573. Luby, J. L., Belden, A. C., Pautschen, J., Si, X., &
Kessler, R. C., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, Spitznagel, E. (2009). The clinical significance
A. M., & Wittchen, H.-U. (2012). Twelve-month and of preschool depression: Impairment of function-
lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety ing and clinical markers of the disorder. Journal of
and mood disorders in the United States. International Affective Disorders, 112, 111–119. doi:10.1016/j.
Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 21, 169– jad.2008.03.026
184. doi:10.1002/mpr.1359 Malecki, C. K., Demaray, M. K., & Elliott, S. N. (2000).
Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, H. L., The child and adolescent social support scale.
Milne, B. J., & Poulton, R. (2003). Prior juvenile diag- DeKalb, IL: Northern Illinois University.
nosis in adults with mental disorder: Developmental Marangoni, C., De Chiara, L., & Faedda, G. L. (2015).
follow-back of a prospective-longitudinal cohort. Bipolar disorder and ADHD: Comorbidity and diag-
Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 709–717. nostic distinctions. Current Psychiatry Reports, 17,
Klein, D. N., Torpey, D. C., & Bufferd, S. J. (2008). 604. doi:10.1007/s11920-015-0604-y
Depressive disorders. In T. P. Beauchaine & S. P. Mash, E. J., & Barkley, R. A. (Eds.). (2006). Treatment of
Hinshaw (Eds.), Child and adolescent psychopathol- childhood disorders. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
ogy (pp. 477–509). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Matson, J. L., Neal, D., Hess, J. A., Fodstad, J. C., Mahan,
Kuiper, N. A., & McCabe, S. B. (1985). The appropri- S., & Rivet, T. T. (2010). Reliability and validity of the
ateness of social topics: Effects of depression and Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters
cognitive vulnerability on self and other judgments. (MESSY). Behavior Modification, 34, 539–558.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 371–379. doi:10.1177/0145445510384844
Kutcher, S., Robertson, H. A., & Bird, D. (1998). Matson, J. L., Neal, D., Worley, J. A., Kozlowski, A. M.,
Premorbid functioning in adolescent onset bipolar I & Fodstad, J. C. (2012). Factor structure of the
disorder: A preliminary report from an ongoing study. Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters-II
Journal of Affective Disorders, 51, 137–144. (MESSY-II). Research in Developmental Disabilities,
Levitt, M. J., Guacci-Franco, N., & Levitt, J. L. 33, 2067–2071. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.09.026
(1993). Convoys of social support in child- Matson, J. L., Rotatori, A. F., & Helsel, W. J. (1983).
hood and early adolescence: Structure and func- Development of a rating scale to measure social skills
tion. Developmental Psychology, 29, 811–818. in children: The Matson Evaluation of Social Skills
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.29.5.811 with Youngsters (MESSY). Behaviour Research
Lewinsohn, P. M., Klein, D. N., & Seeley, J. R. (1995). Therapy, 21, 335–340.
Bipolar disorders in a community sample of older Merikangas, K. R., Cui, L., Kattan, G., Carlson, G. A.,
adolescents: Prevalence, phenomenology, comorbidity Youngstrom, E. A., & Angst, J. (2012). Mania with
and course. Journal of the American Academy of Child and without depression in a community sample of
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 454–463. US adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69,
Lewinsohn, P. M., Klein, D. N., & Seeley, J. R. (2000). 943–895.
Bipolar disorders during adolescence and young adult- Merikangas, K. R., He, J. P., Burstein, M., Swanson,
hood in a community sample. Bipolar Disorders, 2, S. A., Avenevoli, S., Cui, L., … Swendsen, J. (2010).
281–293. Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. ado-
Lewinsohn, P. M., Roberts, R. E., Seeley, J. R., Rohde, P., lescents: Results from the National Comorbidity
Gotlib, I. H., & Hops, H. (1994). Adolescent psycho- Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-­
380 P. Pössel and T.D. Meyer

A). Journal of the American Academy of Child and personal vulnerability to depressive symptoms: A lon-
Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 980–989. gitudinal examination of reassurance-seeking and peer
Meyer, T. D., Koßmann-Böhm, S., & Schlottke, P. F. relationships. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114,
(2004). Do child psychiatrists in Germany diagnose 676–688. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.114.4.676
bipolar disorders in children and adolescents?— Prinz, R. J., Swan, G., Liebert, D., Weintraub, S., &
Results from a survey. Bipolar Disorders, 6, 426–431. Neale, J. M. (1978). ASSESS: Adjustment scales for
Miklowtiz, D. J. (2016). Evidence-based family interven- sociometric evaluation for secondary-school students.
tions for adolescents and young adults with bipolar Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6, 493–501.
disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 77(Suppl Procidano, M. E., & Heller, K. (1983). Measures of
E1), e5. doi:10.4088/JCP.15017su1c.05 perceived social support from friends and from fam-
Morriss, R., Yang, M., Chopra, A., Bentall, R., Paykel, ily: Three validation studies. American Journal of
E., & Scott, J. (2013). Differential effects of depres- Community Psychology, 11, 1–24.
sion and mania symptoms on social adjustment: Quackenbush, D., Kutcher, S., Robertson, H. A., Boulos,
Prospective study in bipolar disorder. Bipolar C., & Chapan, P. (1996). Premorbid and postmorbid
Disorders, 15, 80–91. school functioning in bipolar adolescents: Description
Mundt, C., Goldstein, M. J., Hahlweg, K., & Fiedler, P. and suggested academic interventions. Canadian
(1996). Interpersonal factors in the origin and course Journal of Psychiatry, 41, 16–22.
of affective disorders. London: Gaskell. Reddy, R., Rhodes, J. E., & Mulhall, P. (2003). The influ-
Murray-Close, D., Hoza, B., Hinshaw, S. P., Arnold, ence of teacher support on student adjustment in the
L. E., Swanson, J., Jensen, P. S., … Wells, K. (2010). middle school years: A latent growth curve study.
Developmental processes in peer problems of chil- Development and Psychopathology, 15, 119–138. doi:
dren with attention-deficit/hyper-activity disorder 10.1017.S0954579403000075
in the multimodal treatment study of children with Reichart, C. G., Nolen, W. A., Wals, M., & Hillegers,
ADHD: Developmental cascades and vicious cycles. M. H. (2000). Bipolar disorder in children and adoles-
Developmental Psychopathology, 22, 785–802. cents: A clinical reality? Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 12,
Nolen-Hoecksema, S., & Girgus, J. S. (1994). The emer- 132–135.
gence of gender differences in depression during ado- Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex
lescence. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 424–443. differences in peer relationship processes: Potential
Ostergaard, S. D., Straszek, S., Petrides, G., Skadhede, trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral develop-
S., Jensen, S. O. W., Munk-Jorgensen, P., & Nielsen, ment of girls and boys. Psychological Bulletin, 132,
J. (2014). Risk factors for conversion from unipo- 98–131. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.98
lar psychotic depression to bipolar disorder. Bipolar Roy, A. K., Lopes, V., & Klein, R. B. (2014). Disruptive
Disorder, 16, 180–189. doi:10.1111/bdi.12152 Mood Dysregulation Disorder (DMDD): A new
Patel, V., Flisher, A. J., Hetrick, S., & McGorry, P. (2007). diagnostic approach to chronic irritability in youth.
Mental health of young people: A global public-health American Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 918–924.
challenge. Lancet, 369, 1302–1313. doi:10.1016/ Rudolph, K. D., & Clark, A. G. (2001). Conceptions of
S0140-6736(07)60368-7 relationships in children with depressive and aggres-
Pössel, P., Horn, A. B., & Hautzinger, M. (2003). Erste sive symptoms: Social-cognitive distortion or reality?
Ergebnisse eines Programms zur schulbasierten Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 41–56. doi
Praevention von depressiven Symptomen bei :10.1023/A:1005299429060
Jugendlichen. [First results of a school-based preven- Rudolph, K. D., Kurlakowsky, K. D., & Conley, C. S.
tion program of depressive symptoms among adoles- (2001). Developmental and social-contextual origins
cents.] Zeitschrift fuer Gesundheitspsychologie, 11, of depressive control-related beliefs and behaviors.
10–20. doi:10.1026/0943-8149.11.1.10 Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 447–475. doi:10
Pössel, P., Horn, A. B., Seemann, S., & Hautzinger, M. .1023/A:1005538704056
(2004). Trainingsprogramm zur Praevention von Rueger, S. Y., Malecki, C. K., & Demaray, M. K. (2010).
Depressionen bei Jugendlichen. LARS&LISA: Lust An Relationship between multiple sources of perceived
Realistischer Sicht & Leichtigkeit Im Sozialen Alltag. social support and psychological and academic adjust-
[A training to prevent depressions among adolescents. ment in early adolescence: Comparisons across gen-
LARS&LISA: Joy of realistic view & Easiness in der. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 47–61.
social everyday] Hogrefe, Germany: Göttingen. doi:10.1007/s10964-008-9368-6
Pössel, P., Rudasill, K. M., Sawyer, M. G., Spence, Rueger, S. Y., Malecki, C. K., Pyun, Y., Aycock, C., &
S. H., & Bjerg, A. C. (2013). Associations between Coyle, S. (2016). A meta-analytic review of the associ-
teacher emotional support and depressive symptoms ation between perceived social support and depression
in Australian adolescents: A 5-year longitudinal in childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin,
study. Developmental Psychology, 49, 2135–2146. 142, 1017–1067. doi:10.1037/bul0000058
doi:10.1037/a0031767 Ruscher, S. M., & Gotlib, I. H. (1988). Marital interac-
Prinstein, M. J., Borelli, J. L., Cheah, C. S. L., Simon, tion patterns of couples with and without a depressed
V. A., & Aikins, J. W. (2005). Adolescent girls’ inter- partner. Behavior Therapy, 19, 455–470.
Depression and (Hypo)mania 381

Rutter, M., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Using Research and technical supplement. Windsor:
sex differences in psychopathology to study causal NFER-Nelson.
mechanisms: Unifying issues and research. Journal Stice, E., Ragan, J., & Randall, P. (2004). Prospective
of Child Psychiatry and Psychology, 44, 1092–1115. relations between social support and depression:
doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00194 Differential direction of effects for parent and peer
Sarason, I. G., Levine, H. M., Basham, R. B., & Sarason, support? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 155–
B. R. (1983). Assessing social support: The Social 159. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.113.1.155
Support Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Talavera, J. A., Saiz-Ruiz, J., & Garcia-Toro, M. (1994).
Social Psychology, 44, 127–139. Quantitative measurement of depression through
Segrin, C. (1990). A meta-analytic review of social skill speech analysis. European Psychiatry, 9, 185–193.
deficits in depression. Communication Monographs, Tijssen, M. J. A., van Os, J., Wittchen, H. U., Lieb, R.,
57, 292–308. Beesdo, K., Mengelers, R., & Wichers, M. (2010).
Segrin, C. (1992). Specifying the nature of social Persistence of transition from common adolescent
skill deficits associated with depression. Human bipolar experiences to bipolar disorders: 10-year
Communication Research, 19, 89–123. study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 196, 102–108.
Segrin, C. (1996). The relationship between social Uchida, M., Serra, G., Zayas, L., Kenworthy, T., Hughes,
skills deficits and psychosocial problems: A test of B., Koster, A., … Biederman, J. (2015). Can manic
a vulnerability model. Communication Research, 23, switches be predicted in pediatric major depression?
425–450. A systematic literature review. Journal of Affective
Segrin, C. (1999). Social skills, stressful life events, and Disorders, 172, 300–306.
the development of psychosocial problems. Journal of Van Meter, A. R., Burke, C., Kowatch, R. A., Findling,
Social and Clinical Psychology, 18, 14–34. R. L., & Youngstrom, E. A. (2016). Ten-year updated
Segrin, C. (2000). Social skills deficits associated with meta-analysis of the clinical characteristics of pedi-
depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 379–403. atric mania and hypomania. Bipolar Disorder, 18,
Segrin, C., & Abramson, L. Y. (1994). Negative reac- 19–32. doi:10.1111/bdi.12358
tions to depressive behaviors: A communication theo- Vance, Y. H., Jones, S. H., Espie, J., Bentall, R., & Tai,
ries analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, S. (2008). Parental communication style and family
655–668. relationships in children of bipolar parents. British
Segrin, C., & Dillard, J. P. (1993). The complex link Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 355–359.
between social skill and dysphoria: Conceptualization, Vedel Kessing, L., Vradi, E., & Kragh Andersen, P.
perspective, and outcome. Communication Research, (2015). Diagnostic stability in pediatric bipolar disor-
20, 76–104. der. Journal of Affective Disorders, 172, 417–421.
Segrin, C., & Flora, J. (1998). Depression and verbal Whitney, J., Howe, M., Shoemaker, V., Li, S., Sanders,
behavior in conversations with friends and strang- E. M., Dijamco, C., … Chang, K. (2013). Socio-­
ers. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 17, emotional processing and functioning of youth at
494–505. high risk for bipolar disorder. Journal of Affective
Serra, G., Uchida, M., Battaglia, C., Casini, M. P., De Disorders, 148, 112–117.
Chiiara, L., Biederman, J., … & Wozniak, J. (2016, Wingo, A. P., Baldessarini, R. J., Compton, M. T., &
June 6). Pediatric mania: The controversy between Harvey, P. D. (2010). Correlates of recovery of social
euphoria and irritability. Current Neuropharmacology functioning in type I and II bipolar disorder patients.
[Ahead of EPub]. Psychiatry Research, 177, 131–134.
Shah, F., & Morgan, S. B. (1996). Teachers’ ratings of Wolkenstein, L., & Meyer, T. D. (2010). Is one’s mood
social competence of children with high versus low affected when interacting with people putatively at
levels of depressive symptoms. Journal of School risk for affective disorders? Journal of Nervous and
Psychology, 34, 337–349. Mental Disease, 198, 576–579.
Shankman, S. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., Klein, D. N., Youngstrom, E. A., Findling, R. L., Youngstrom, J. K.,
Small, J. W., Seeley, J. R., & Altmanm, S. E. (2009). & Calabrese, J. R. (2005). Toward an evidence-based
Subthreshold conditions as precursors for full syn- assessment of pediatric bipolar disorder. Journal
drome disorders: A 15-year longitudinal study of mul- of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34,
tiple diagnostic classes. Journal of Child Psychology 433–448.
and Psychiatry, 50, 1485–1494. Zahn-Waxler, C., Klimes-Dougan, B., & Slattery, M. J.
Siegel, R. S. P., Hoeppner, B., Yen, S., Stout, R. L., (2000). Internalizing problems of childhood and ado-
Weinstock, L. M., Hower, H. M., … Keller, M. B. lescence: Prospects, pitfalls, and progress in under-
(2015). Longitudinal associations between interper- standing the development of anxiety and depression.
sonal relationship functioning and mood episode Development and Psychopathology, 12, 443–446.
severity in youth with bipolar disorder. Journal of Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley,
Nervous and Mental Disease, 203, 194–204. G. K. (1988). The multidimensional scale of perceived
Spence, S. H. (1995). Social skills training: Enhancing social support. Journal of Personality Assessment, 52,
social competence with children and adolescents. 30–41.
Index

A IRS, 108, 109


Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment MESSY, 103
(ASEBS), 12 PKBS, 105, 106
Action disposition, 294 PSMAT, 106
Adaptive behavior composite, 222 rating scales, 102
Adaptive behavior functioning, 251, 252 SCI, 106
Adult social skills, 48 SEARS, 107
Affect regulation (AR), 233 SP, 109, 110
American Association on Intellectual and Developmental SSBS, 104, 105
Disabilities (AAIDD), 250 SSRS, 103, 104
American Psychiatric Association, 217, 250, 253 Vineland SEEC Scales, 107
Analogue behavioral observation (ABO), 306, 307 Assessment of Basic Language and Learning Skills –
Anger control training, 343 Revised (ABLLS-R), 26
Anterior temporal cortex (ATC), 238 Attentional control, 66, 67
Anxiety disorders, 302, 303, 306–315 Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), 4, 15,
assessment, 301, 302, 304–306 56, 71, 134, 161, 162, 221, 227, 274–287
ABO, 306, 307 social behavior assessment, 281
recommendations, 307, 308 adult, 283
self-report and questionnaires, 303 informant, 283
social skills/competence, 307 method, 281
structured diagnostic interviews, 302, 303 observation, 282, 283
definition, population, 294, 295 peer informants, 284
diagnostic interviews, 303 rating scale, 281, 282
etiology, 295–297 self, 284
genetics, 296 sociometric nominations, 282
parent-child interaction, 297, 298 social impairment, 274
prevalence, 298 ADHD symptoms, 274, 275
social skills and competence, 308 adolescence, 279, 280
social skills problems, 298–301 comorbid conditions, 275, 276
temperaments, 297 friendship quality, 277, 278
treatment, 308, 309 impact of gender, 279
behavioral and cognitive-behavioral techniques, peer rejection and reputation bias, 277
310–314 peer victimization and bullying, 276
caution, 314, 315 self-perceptions, 278, 279
contingency management, 310 social media and cyberbullying, 280, 281
exposure, 309 treatment of social problems, 284
modeling, 310 peer and friendship interventions, 286, 287
systematic desensitization, 309, 310 social skills training, 285, 286
Applied behavior analysis (ABA), 21 treatments, 274
Asperger’s disorder, 217 Augmentative alternative communication (AAC), 28
Assessment methods Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring
direct behavior observation, 108 Network, 217
ESI, 108 Autism diagnostic interview-revised (ADI-R), 219, 222
HCSBS, 105 Autism diagnostic observation schedule (ADOS), 219

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 383


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Social Behavior and Skills in Children, Autism and Child
Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-64592-6
384 Index

Autism social skills profile (ASSP), 28 self-management and behavioral skills training, 119
Autism spectrum disorders(ASD), 4–6, 14, 51, 71, 117, shaping, 123, 124
134, 217 Behavior analytic methods
ADI-R and ADOS-2, 240 discriminative stimulus signals, 115
biomarkers, 219, 220 functional assessment, 119
comprehensive assessments, 221, 222 preference assessment, 117–119
core deficits, 224 socials skills, 115
DCD/dyspraxia, 228–231 task analysis, 117
developmental motor deficiencies, 224 three-term contingency, 116
early clinical diagnosis, 218, 219 Behavior Assessment System for Children, Third Edition
early motor delays, 224 (BASC-3), 12
early signs, 218 Behavior skills training (BST), 35
infant siblings, 224 Behavioural rehearsal, 137
influence of group therapy, 220 Biological motion, 49, 54
influence outcomes, 222 Bipolar disorder (BD), 363
informal assessments, 240
language skills, 220
machine learning techniques, 241 C
mental health, 220 Callous-unemotional (CU) traits, 327
motor functions, 223 Caring School Community (CSC) program, 185
motor learning, 231 Center for the Collaborative Classroom, 185
movement development and speech, 223 Centers for Disease Control (CDC), 217, 253
neurological development, 228 Challenging behavior, 21–24
psychological hypothesis, 228 direct assessments, 20, 21
self-awareness, 241 functional analysis
sensorimotor and interconnected neurodevelopmental alone and ignore conditions, 22
issues, 224 attention, 21, 22
sensorimotor-dominated movement interventions, 241 brief functional analysis, 23
sensory dimensions, 241 demand/escape, 22
sensory integration, 226 extended alone conditions, 24
sensory processing disorder, 226, 227 latency functional analysis, 24
sensory system, 225 play condition, 23
social communication, 217, 223, 240 precursor functional analysis, 23
typical development (TD), 228 tangible condition, 22
Automated Working Memory Assessment (AWMA), 158 trial-based functional analysis, 24
Automatic reinforcement, 88 indirect assessment, 20
intellectual and developmental disabilities, 19
social skills, 19, 20
B Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (CHAT), 218
Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development – Third Checklist of Adaptive Living Skills (CALS), 257
Edition (Bayley-III), 27, 28 Child and Adolescent Social Support Scale (CASSS), 371
Behavioral and cognitive-behavioral Child behavior checklist (CBCL), 12
techniques, 310 Childhood, 48, 51, 55, 57
CBGT-A, 313 Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS), 222
Coping Cat program, 311, 312 Childhood Autism Rating Scale, Second Edition
FRIENDS program, 312 (CARS-2), 13
modular treatment, 314 Child maltreatment, 329
SASS, 312, 313 Child psychopathology, 296
SET-C, 312 Child raters, 299
for social phobia, 314 Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC), 28
with and without parent involvement, 313, 314 Children’s Friendship program, 144
Behavioral inhibition, 297 Child social skills training (SST) programs, 133
Behavioral intervention, 119 Class play technique, 92
Behavioral treatment Clinical-based programs
chaining, 122, 123 Children’s Friendship program, 144
discrete trial training, 124 Secret Agent Society program, 144
fading, 121, 122 Cognition, 65, 66
generalization training, 125–127 Cognitive-Behavioral Group Treatment for Adolescents
prompting, 119–121 (CBGT-A), 313
reinforcement schedules, 124, 125 Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), 4
Index 385

Cognitive flexibility, 153 social support, skills and behavior, 367–370


Cognitive impairments, 249 assessment, 370–375
Collaborative for Academic, Social, and Emotional developmental changes and gender
Learning (CASEL), 176, 178 differences, 370
Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales instruments, 371, 374
Developmental Profile (CSBS DP), 218 Descriptive assessment, 21
Communication deficits, 13 Developmental coordination disorder (DCD), 221, 229
Conduct disorder, 324–346, 348–350 Developmental Studies Center, 185
assessment, 332 Diagnostic Adaptive Behavior Scale (DABS), 258
age considerations, 336, 337 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
clinical interviews, 332, 333 (DSM-IV-TR), 217
comorbidity, 336 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health
dimensions, 335, 336 Disorders Fifth Edition 1 (DSM-5), 217, 250
effects of labeling, 337 Dimensional change card sort (DCCS), 154
methods, 332 Direct behavior observation, 108
observations, 334, 335 Discrete trial teaching (DTT), 32
purposes, 332 Discriminative stimuli (SDs), 37
rating scales, 333, 334 Disruptive behavior disorders (DBD), 162
rationale for, 331, 332 Disruptive mood dysregulation disorder (DMDD), 365
risk factors, 337 Down syndrome, 255
sex considerations, 337 Dysfunctional Attitude Scale (DAS), 220
biological factors Dyspraxia, 229
genetics, 325, 326
neurophysiological factors, 326
prenatal exposure to substances, 326, 327 E
children’s individual characteristics Early adolescent temperament questionnaire
comorbid psychological factors, 328 (EATQ-R), 159
CU traits, 327 Economic world, 55, 56
low verbal intelligence, 327, 328 Electroencephalographic (EEG) studies, 50, 52, 57,
temperament, 327 236–238
commonalities, EBTs, 347 Emotional intelligence, 176
diagnostic criteria, 324, 325 Emotion regulation (ER), 69, 70, 234
etiology, 325, 330, 331 Emotion Trainer, 264
health service providers, 323 Empirically supported treatment (EST), 232
interventions, 338 Establishing operations (EOs), 37
for adolescents, 343, 344 Evaluation of social interaction (ESI), 108
for children, 342, 343 Event-related potentials (ERPs), 50, 236, 260
considerations, 348–350 Evidence-based psychosocial treatments (EBTs), 338
with empirical support, 338, 339 Executive functions (EF), 67, 68, 153
not based on age, 344–346
rationale for, 331, 332
for young children, 339–342 F
potentially problematic treatments, 347, 348 Flexible Induction of Meaning-Animates (FIM-An), 158
prevention, 346, 347 Flexible Induction of Meaning-Objects (FIM-Ob), 158
psychosocial factors Free-operant preference assessment, 118
family, 328, 329 Friendships, 274, 276, 277, 279, 282, 284, 288
neighborhoods, 330 interventions, 286, 287
peers, 329, 330 quality, 277, 278
Contingency management, 310 Functional assessment interview (FAI), 20
Functional behavior assessment (FBA), 87, 88
Functional communication response (FCR), 36
D Functional communication training (FCT), 37
Defiant Children program, 345 Functional Independence Skills Handbook (FISH), 27
Defiant Teen program, 346 Functional MRI (fMRI), 53, 54, 56, 236
Deficits in Adaptive Behavior Functioning, 257
Depression and (hypo)mania, 363, 370–375
definition of population, 363–365 G
implications for treatment, 375, 376 Gestures, 230
prevalence, comorbidity and consequences, 365–367 Gilliam autism rating scale (GARS-3), 13
social skills and behavior, 367 Guess who technique, 92
386 Index

H M
High-functioning autism (HFA), 230 Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), 53, 236
High-quality implementers, 190 Magnetoencephalography (MEG), 236
Home and community social behavior scales (HCSBS), Making Socially Accepting Inclusive Classrooms
95, 105 (MOSAIC), 286
Hypersexuality, 368 Maladaptive social skills, 110
Matson Evaluation of Social Skills for Individuals with
Severe Retardation (MESSIER), 27, 259
I Matson Evaluation of Social Skills for Youngsters
Ideomotor apraxia, 229 (MESSY), 27, 94, 103, 258, 259, 307
Infant behavior questionnaire (IBQ), 159 Matson Evaluation of Social Skills with Youngsters-II
Infant-Toddler Checklist, 218 (MESSY-II), 373
Inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), 238 Medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), 53, 238
Inferior parietal lobule, 238 Mental retardation, 249
Inhibitory control (IC), 154 Mentalization development
Integra social ACES (Awareness, Competence, perspective taking, 52–55
Engagement, Skills) Program, 73 social decision-making, 55, 56
group content, 74 Miller Assessment for Preschoolers, 230
group matching, 74 Mind readers, 52
overview, 73 MindUpTM program, 185, 186
Intellectual disorder (ID), 1, 3, 6, 14, 15, 221, 249 Moderate intellectual disability, 254
adaptive behavior functioning and deficits, 249, 251, Moderate-quality implementers, 190
252, 256–259 Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT), 218
assistive technology, 265, 266 Monoamine oxidase A (MAO-A), 326
classification, 253–256 Mood disorders, 363
computer-based instruction, 264, 265 Multidimensional scale of perceived social support
definition and diagnosis, 250–252 (MSPSS), 371
etiology, 254, 255 Multidimensional treatment foster care (MTFC), 344
evidence-based interventions, 250 Multi-informant, 281
extensive supports, 255 Multiple stimulus assessments, 118
intellectual functioning, 250, 251 Multisystemic therapy (MST), 344
intermittent supports, 255
limited supports, 255
mild intellectual disability, 253 N
moderate intellectual disability, 254 National Center on Birth Defects and Developmental
pervasive supports, 255 Disabilities, 2016, 249
prevalence, 252, 253 National Commission on Teaching and America’s Future
self-care skills, 249 (2007), 191
severe and profound, 254 Neurodevelopmental disorders, 57, 69, 71–73
severity, 253–254 Neuropsychological processing abilities, 66
social problem-solving interventions, 263, 264 Neurotoxic chemicals, 218
social skills, 256–259 Nonverbal learning disabilities (NLD), 68
assessments and interventions, 250
deficit, 256
intervention, 259–266 O
Social Stories™, 262 Observational methods, 85–97
social-ecological model, 256 behavior rating scales, 93, 94
systematic instruction, 261, 262 HCSBS, 95
video modeling, 263 limitations, 97
Interaction rating scale (IRS), 108, 109 MESSY, 94
The International Classification of Diseases, 10th edition PKBS, 95
(ICD-10), 250 SEARS, 95, 96
In vivo modelling, 135, 136 self-report assessments, 96, 97
SSCA, 96
SSIS, 94, 95
L SSRS, 94
Language development survey (LDS), 13 WSSRS, 95
Learning disability (LD), 71 direct observation of behavior, 84, 85
List of Social Situation Problems (LSSP), 97 analogue observation, 86
Localization, 47 functional behavior assessment, 87, 88
Index 387

naturalistic observation, 85 Pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified


recording procedures, 88–90 (PDD-NOS), 217
role-play, 86 Pervasive Developmental Disorders Screening Test-II
self-monitoring, 86, 87 (PDDSY-II), 219
indirect assessments, 90 Picture exchange communication system (PECS), 123
class play technique, 92 Pivotal response treatment (PRT), 32
guess who measures, 92 Positive illusory bias, 274
interviewing techniques, 90, 91 Positive parenting program, 342
peer nomination, 91 Posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), 238
peer rating procedures, 91 Posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS), 238
picture sociometrics, 92 Praxis, 229
sociometric procedures, limitations, 92, 93 Preschool and Kindergarten Behavior Scales (PKBS), 95,
sociometric ranking procedures, 91 105, 106
sociometric techniques, 91 Problem solving skills rraining (PSST), 343
Profile of social difficulty (POSD), 28
Promoting Alternative Thinking Strategies (PATHS),
P 142, 143, 164, 185
Paired-choice preference assessment, 118 Promoting social and emotional learning, 176
Parental friendship coaching (PFC), 286 Psychometrics, 103–110
Parent-child interaction therapy (PCIT), 339
Parent management training (PMT), 343
Parent management training oregon model (PMTO), 342 Q
Parents Plus model, 203 Questions About Behavioral Function (QABF), 20
Parents Plus Programmes Quality Marriage Index (QMI), 220
context of group therapy, 205
goals, 204
parent training models, 204 R
research base, 205–207 Randomized controlled trial (RCT), 189, 232
social learning theory, 204 Rational-Emotive Mental Health Program (REHM), 344
solution-focused therapy, 205 Recording procedures, social behavior, 88
strength-based approach, 205 duration, 90
systemic family therapy, 205 frequency recording, 88, 89
Parent training programmes momentary time sampling, 90
bioecological perspective, 201, 202 response latency, 90
child development supports, 199 whole-interval and partial-interval recording, 89
child’s social competence, 199 Residential cognitive therapy (RCT), 4
emergence, rationale and applications, 202, 203 Residential interpersonal psychotherapy (RIPT), 4
ethological perspective, 200, 201 Response latency, 90
parents and primary caregivers, 199 The RULER Feeling Words Curriculum, 186
picture schedules, 207, 208
problem-solving, 208–210
psychoanalytic perspective, 199, 200 S
socialisation, 199 Scales of Independent Behavior-Revised (SIB-R), 258
sociocultural and social learning perspectives, 201 Schizophrenia, 2
teaching parents skills, 210–212 School-based programs
teaching responsibility, 210 PATHS, 142, 143
Peer-mediated approach (PEER), 233 stop and think social skills program, 141, 142
Peer nomination strategy, 91 superflex social skills program, 143, 144
Peer Social Behavior Code, 25, 335 superheroes social skills program, 143
Peer social maturity scale (PSMAT), 106 School Social Behavior Scales (SSBS), 104, 105
Peer victimization, 300 Secret Agent Society Program, 144
Percentage of nonoverlapping data (PND), 31, 33, 235, 236 Self-regulation, 153–157
Performance deficits adaptive adjustment, 164
antecedent manipulations, 37 clear developmental benefits, 166
differential reinforcement, 36 cognitive flexibility, 158
extinction, 36 definition, 150
FCT, 37 early childhood
function-based interventions, 36–38 cognitive perspective, 153, 154
Persistent depressive disorder (PDD), 364 development, 149
Personality disorder, 2 socioemotional perspective, 154–155
388 Index

Self-regulation (cont.) Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ), 219


effortful control measures, 159, 160 Social competence, 63, 70–76, 78
externalizing problems, 161, 162 attentional control, 66, 67
infancy and toddlerhood, 151, 152 cognitive factors, 64
inhibitory control, 158, 159, 164 emotion regulation, 69, 70
internalizing problems, 162, 163 executive functions, 67, 68
measures of executive functions, 157 importance, 63, 64
middle childhood interventions, 70
cognitive and socioemotional perspective, 155 developing populations, 70, 71
cognitive perspective, 156, 157 for LDs, 73, 74
socioemotional perspective, 157 neurodevelopmental disorders, 71–73
psychopathology, 160–166 neuropsychological processing abilities, 66
subcomponents, 165, 166 processing speed, 68
types of interventions, 164 sample group session, 75, 77
working memory, 157, 158 change the room, 76
Sensory-based motor disorder, 226 group check-in, 75
Sensory discrimination disorder, 226 snack and chat, 76, 78
Sensory integration, 226 Squiggle Game, 75, 76
Sensory modulation disorder, 225 support development and enactment, 65
Sensory processing disorder, 227 thought processess, 65, 66
Single-stimulus assessment, 118 visual-spatial processing, 68, 69
Skill deficits, 249 Social competence intervention (SCI), 72, 106
Skills for Academic and Social Success (SASS), 312, 313 Social decision-making, 55, 56
Smith-Magenis syndrome, 255 Social development, 47
Social abilities, 53 model of, 58
Social and emotional learning (SEL) neurodevelopmental phenomenon, 58
academic achievement , school, 180, 181 Social-ecological model, 256
antisocial behaviors, 178 Social effectiveness therapy for children (SET-C), 312
characteristics, 181, 182 Social Emotional Assets and Resilience Scales (SEARS),
classroom- and school-based approaches, 188 95, 96, 107
collaborative efforts, 187 Social functioning, 52
community partners, 188 Social interactions, 101
cost-benefit analysis, 179 Social learning instructional models, 135, 136, 138–144
evidence-based programs, 178, 181 behaviour management, 137, 138
family and community programming, 188 behavioural rehearsal, 137
implementation quality, 182, 183 didactic instruction, 134, 135
integrated vs. add-on approaches, 183, 184 comic strip conversations, 135
middle and high school programs, 186, 187 social stories, 135
non-systematic program implementation, 188 feedback and self-evaluation, 137
positive student outcomes, 178, 179 innovative technologies, 140, 141
practices and policies, 188 modelling, 135, 136
pre-, elementary and high school, 184–187 multicomponent SST programs, 141
relationship skills, 177 clinical-based programs, 144
research and practical strategies, 175, 176 school-based programs, 141–144
responsible decision-making skills, 177 strategies to promote generalisation of skills, 138
school-based SEL programs, 178 caregiver involvement, 139
school-community partnerships, 177 homework tasks, 138
school-wide SEL, 187–189 school involvement and peer buddy programs,
SEL intervention, 179, 183, 190 139, 140
self-awareness, 177 visual supports, 138, 139
self-management, 177 Social phobia, 294, 314
social and emotional competence, 190 Social (pragmatic) communication disorder (SCD), 14
social awareness, 177 Social Profile (SP), 109, 110
teacher preparation, 190, 191 Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), 219
teacher-related factors, 189, 190 Social Responsiveness Scale II (SRS II), 28
teacher support, 189 Social skill assessment
Social anxiety disorder (SAD), 14, 134 ABLLS-R, 26
Social behavior, 47 ASSP, 28
Social brain, 47, 56–58 Bayley-III, 27, 28
Social cognition, 11, 47 behavior observation codes, 25
Index 389

behavioral rating scales, 26 Social skills improvement system (SSIS), 11, 28, 94,
CCC, 28 95, 259
FISH, 27 Social skills interventions
learning, 25 behavioral rehearsal and behavior skills training, 35
MESSIER, 27 coaching, 30
MESSY, 27 modeling, 30, 31
observational checklists, 25 peer-mediated interventions, 35
performance deficit, 25 priming procedure, 34
POSD, 28 prompting procedures, 32, 33
sociometric assessment, 26 PRT, 32
SRS, 28 social and/or communication skills, 30
SSIS, 28 social skills groups, 34
tool, 29, 30 social stories, 33
VB-Mapp, 26 technology-based instruction, 31, 32
Vineland TM-II, 26 Social Skills Rating Scale, 282
Social skills, 9, 20, 101, 102, 104–106, 108, 110, Social Skills Rating System (SSRS), 94, 103, 104
370–375 Social skills training (SST) programs, 30, 231
adult, 48 Social Stories™, 235, 236, 262
analysis, 47 Social support scale for children and adolescents
anxiety and cognitive function, 5 (SSSCA), 371
assessment and training, 4 Sociometric techniques, 91
and behavior, 367 Specialization, 47
CBT model, 5 Specialized social brain, 56–58
classroom, 6, 232, 233 Squiggle Game, 75, 76
deficits, 83 Stress Coping Inventory (SCI), 220
definition, 9–11 Stress Management and Resiliency Training (SMART-in-­
and disability, 14–15 Education), 190
EEG, 237, 238 Subjective perception, 85
emotion regulation, 233, 234 Subsyndromal depression, 366
emotional control, 234–235 Superflex social skills program, 143, 144
generalization, 286 Superheroes social skills program, 143
during infancy, 49–52 Superior temporal gyrus (STG), 240
intervention strategies, 5 Supports Intensity Scale-Children’s Version (SIS-C), 256
neuroimaging evidence, 238–240 Systematic desensitization, 309, 310
neurotypical children, 236, 237
operationalizing social skills, 11–14
peer acceptance, 10, 13 T
peer-mediated intervention, 5 Teacher report form (TRF), 12
populations, 2–4 Temperament in Middle Childhood Questionnaire
preschool-aged children, 236 (TMCQ), 159
problems to population, 298–301 Temporoparietal junction (TPJ), 238
recent tendencies, 231 Test of Nonverbal Intelligence (TONI-4), 251
SCD, 14 Theory of mind, 51
and social acceptance, 276 Three Dimension-Changes Card Sorting Photoshop, 158
social competence, 10, 13, 15 Tools of the Mind, 165
social deficits, 239 Triad Social Skills Assessment, Second Edition
Social Stories™, 235, 236 (TSSA©), 29
social support, 367–370 Typically developing (TD), 234, 235
assessment, 370–375
developmental changes and gender
differences, 370 V
sociometric ratings, 13, 14 Verbal Behavior Milestone Assessment and Placement
special populations, 1–2 Program, Second Edition (VB-Mapp), 26
SSIS, 11 Victimization, 300
training, 1, 285, 286 Video modeling (VM) technique, 31, 120, 136, 263
treatment, 231 Video self-modeling (VSM), 31, 121
two-phase treatment approach, 2 Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-Second Edition
university and community, 233 (Vineland-II), 26, 221
victimization, 276 Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales—Third Edition,
video modeling instruction, 233 12, 257
390 Index

Vineland Social-Emotional Early Childhood Scales, 107 Working Memory Rating Scale
Virtual reality (VR), 31 (WMRS), 158
Visual sensory system, 49 Working Memory Test Battery for Children
Visual-spatial processing, 68, 69 (WMTB-C), 157
World Health Organization, 250, 253

W
Waksman Social Skills Rating Scale (WSSRS), 95 Y
Walker-McConnell Scales of Social Competence and Youth Risk Behavior Survey, 180
School Adjustment (SSCA), 96 Youth self-report (YSR), 12

You might also like