You are on page 1of 14

Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144

DOI 10.1007/s10484-012-9185-2

The Effect of Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback on Performance


Psychology of Basketball Players
Maman Paul • Kanupriya Garg

Published online: 9 March 2012


 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012

Abstract Coping with pressure and anxiety is an ineluc- for dealing with anxiety. The Placebo group showed
table demand of sports performance. Heart rate variability improvement in self efficacy and performance post training.
(HRV) Biofeedback (BFB) shall be used as a tool for self The Control group showed no change in any variable except
regulating physiological responses resulting in improved performance. The results of the study support the idea that
psycho physiological interactions. For further analysis, the HRV BFB lowers the anxiety and thus there seems to be a
present study has been designed to examine the relationship potential association between HRV BFB and performance
between anxiety and performance and also effectiveness of optimization.
biofeedback protocol to create stress-eliciting situation in
basketball players. Thirty basketball players of university Keywords HRV BFB  Anxiety  Self-efficacy 
level and above (both male and female) aged 18–28 years, Basketball  STAI  Performance
who scored a minimum of 20 in state trait anxiety inventory,
were randomly divided into three equal groups- Experi-
mental (Biofeedback) group, Placebo group and Control (No Introduction
Treatment) group. The BFB group received HRV BFB
training for 10 consecutive days for 20 min that included Heart Rate Variability
breathing at individual’s resonant frequency through a pac-
ing stimulus; Placebo group was shown motivational video The term ‘‘Heart rate variability’’ (HRV) refers to the
clips for 10 consecutive days for 10 min, whereas No waveform of beat to beat changes in the duration of RR
Treatment Control group was not given any intervention. intervals (RRIs) (Lagos et al. 2008). Earlier HRV was
Two way repeated measure ANOVA was applied to analyze analyzed by means of time and frequency domain tech-
the differences within and between the groups. Anxiety, niques, referred to as spectral analysis. Time domain
coping self-efficacy, heart rate variability, respiration rate, analysis provides measures such as standard deviation of
and performance (dribbling, passing and shooting) at session Normal to Normal interval (SDNN). The frequency domain
1, 10 and 1 month follow up were statistically significant in technique analyzes the frequency information obtained in
each group along with interaction of group and time the recording and result is shown on a power spectrum,
(p \ 0.001). Also, all the measures showed statistically which depicts a breakdown of all the frequencies (Very
significant inter group difference (p \ 0.05). The findings Low Frequency, Low Frequency and High Frequency).
are harmonious with existing data on HRV BFB as a strategy A new approach to HRV monitoring and feedback is
the analysis of heart rhythm patterns which analyzes the
varying shape of the HRV waveform (Task Force of the
M. Paul (&)  K. Garg
European Society of Cardiology and the North American
Department of Sports Medicine and Physiotherapy, Guru Nanak
Dev University, Amritsar, India Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology 1996).
e-mail: maman_paul20@yahoo.co.uk HRV analysis represents the most reliable and quanti-
K. Garg tative assessment of autonomic nervous system functioning
e-mail: kannugr855@yahoo.co.in (Sutarto et al. 2010). The heart rate may be increased by

123
132 Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144

slow acting sympathetic activity or decreased by fast acting Heart Rate Variability Biofeedback Training
parasympathetic (vagal) activity. The balance between and Resonant Frequency
these systems produces an ongoing oscillation, an orderly
increase and decrease in heart rate. Higher HRV indicates Efforts to identify effective treatment that target HRV
an optimal interplay between the sympathetic and para- and baroreflex function therefore are warranted. Biofeed-
sympathetic nervous system and thus epitomize as an index back (BF) has been implemented as a method for altering
of emotional expression (Lagos et al. 2008). physiological activity which in turn results in improved
A healthy heart doesn’t beat with absolute regularity. A psychology and performance (Babiloni et al. 2008; Ham-
certain amount of variability is required so that it can adapt mond 2007; Strack 2003).
to life’s routine challenges. In recent years, potential Resonant properties in cardiovascular system result from
prognostic value of HRV has been given forethought due to heart rate baroreflex activity. Resonance frequency of an
association between HRV parameters and several physical individual can be estimated as the highest amplitude of heart
and psychological health problems. Reduced HRV is an rate oscillations elicited by breathing (Hassett et al. 2007).
indicator of cardiovascular problems, generalized anxiety The resonant characteristic of HRV is generated by paced
disorder, panic disorder and post traumatic stress disorder breathing at a frequency of about 0.1 Hz (six breaths per
(Wheat and Larkin 2010). minute). The 0.1 Hz is in the lower frequency band which
reflects the autonomic control and action of baroreflex (Lehrer
Techniques for Stress Management et al. 2006). Thus, when individuals breathe at their resonant
frequency, high-amplitude oscillations in heart rate are elic-
A variety of relaxation techniques exist in the field of sport ited and the closed loop system of the baroreflex modulates
psychology for management of stress. When practiced the respiratory effects on HRV (Lehrer et al. 2003).
regularly, techniques like deep breathing, visualization, Breathing at one’s resonant frequency aims at producing
progressive muscle relaxation, meditation and yoga negate maximal increases in respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA)
the ill effects of stress and evokes a relaxation response, a amplitude. Variation in heart rate is accompanied by
state of deep rest and/or a functionally efficient relaxed breathing (RSA) which is controlled by parasympathetic
state during performance. The psychophysiological symp- reflexes (Lagos et al. 2008). RSA usually occur in the
toms of stress can also be relieved by cognitive training frequency range of 0.15–0.4 Hz in a healthy adult (Lehrer
which includes positive self talk, mental rehearsal, mental et al. 2000).
imagery and visuo motor behavior rehearsal therapy
(Lagos et al. 2008). Most of the methods focus on psy- Heart Rate Variability and Psychological Stressors
chological symptoms of stress rather than addressing the
imbalance of sympathetic and parasympathetic system in A variety of factors can cause increase in specific rhythms
the body. of heart including anxious thinking, breathing, emotions,
Training for psychological skills has become a widely pressure sensors (baroreceptors) in the arteries, and other
accepted practice in the athletic setting now. Managing and physiological and behavioral changes (Lagos et al. 2008).
improving performance of a player involves maintenance Anxiety is important especially in a sporting context and in
of systematic records and analysis of past performances a team game such as basketball; it may be the determining
through ‘‘evaluation’’ and ‘‘feedback’’ so as to discover his/ factor between winning and losing. Anxiety is highly
her potential in pursuit of better performance. complex phenomenon acting within both the central ner-
The principle of training specificity by Zupan et al. vous system and autonomic nervous system (especially
(2006) states that athletes’ should train themselves as if cardiovascular and respiratory systems). It comprises of
they are competing in an event. Athletes when train like as two different components: State and Trait anxiety. State
if they are competing, impact their psychological processes anxiety is situational in nature, characterized by heightened
on autonomic states of their body. Wilmore and Costill autonomic nervous system activity whereas trait anxiety is
(2004) expands this concept further, the training program a world view that an individual uses when coping with
must stress the physiological systems that are critical for situations in his or her environment (Spielberger 1966).
optimal performance in the given sport. This statement Dealing with anxiety is especially important for an
highlights and characterizes the concept that body system athlete, as an anxious athlete has trouble concentrating,
specificity has a direct link with actual sport task. When focusing and in decision making (Wilson and Pritchard
athletes’ train while considering themselves in pseudo 2005). This results in detrimental performance and they
competitive situations, there is an impact on their psy- seldom reach the desired result. Reduction in anxiety will
chological processes in cognizance to the changes occur- be facilitated if one’s self efficacy is reinforced as anxiety
ring in the autonomic states of their bodies. is associated with low self efficacy (Nicholls et al. 2010).

123
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144 133

Self-efficacy is a powerful predictor for sports performance were recruited from Amritsar who met the following
which is associated with the perceived ability to cope with inclusion criteria: (a) scored 20 and above on Spielberger
fear or with anxiety-arousing events. Self-efficacy expec- state trait anxiety inventory (STAI), (b) has not received
tations are beliefs that one can successfully organize and any kind of psychological intervention previously. There
execute the action required to produce particular outcomes was no known medical or psychiatric diagnoses reported
(Anyadubalu 2010). Different individuals have distinct from the participants. The subjects represented a wide
efficacy-related beliefs. One such belief refers to a person’s range of skills from university (46.66%), state (20%) to
ability to excogitate strategies that will assist him/her in national (33.33%) standards. Ethical clearance was
coping with various fears and stressors, which is referred to obtained from the Institutional Ethics Committee of Fac-
as Coping Self Efficacy (Nicholls et al. 2010). Treating ulty of Sports Medicine and Physiotherapy, Guru Nanak
anxiety will be more difficult when evaluation of self Dev University, Amritsar, India.
efficacy is associated with poor results and self assessments The study was experimental in nature with double blind
(Dehdari et al. 2008). study design.
Basketball is a sport that requires great amount of physical The participants were randomly assigned into three
energy and does not solely rely on physical abilities for equal groups (N = 10):
performance success. For a basketball player to deliver a
1. Experimental Group received Heart rate variability
winning performance there is a firm need to cultivate strong
(HRV) biofeedback training (Male = 8, Female = 2;
psyche. In a game like basketball, where players often run up
Mean Age: 21.1 years; University: 0.4%; State: 0.2%;
and down the court, heart rate is driven by physical demands
National: 0.4%)
on the court and also by their emotional intensity. Some-
2. Placebo Group was shown motivational basketball
times, even before they begin the competition, their heart rate
visual clips (Male = 2, Female = 8; Mean Age:
is unusually high. So the aim of athletic performance training
20.6 years; University: 0.2%; State: 0.3%; National:
is to improve the cardiorespiratory efficiency thus, saving
0.5%)
cardiac and respiratory energy and helping athletes to train
3. Control Group (No Treatment) did not receive any
according to their resonant frequency (Lagos et al. 2008).
training (Male = 7, Female = 3; Mean Age:
The physical fitness of basketball players and, consequently,
21.7 years; University: 0.3%; State: 0.3%; National:
game performance can be influenced by the balance in
0.4%).
autonomic nervous system.
Thus, the current study is an attempt to further explore During the initial visit, players selected according to the
the effectiveness of HRV BFB on performance skills of inclusion criteria completed an informed consent, a
anxious basketball players. demographic questionnaire, STAI and coping self efficacy
scale. After filling the questionnaires; dribbling, passing
and 3-min shooting tests were performed. Players were
Methodology then fitted with plethysmographic sensor on the finger and
abdominal strain gauge for baseline measures of HRV and
Participants and Study Design respiration rate respectively. The study was conducted at
the Sports Psychology Lab, Department of Sports Medicine
Thirty basketball players (Male = 17, Female = 13) and Physiotherapy, Guru Nanak Dev University, Amritsar,
ranging in age from 18 to 28 years (21.13 ± 2.82 years) India.
Pre and post recording of the following measures were
done.

Psychological Measures

Anxiety was measured by STAI: It is an instrument used


for identifying anxiety in adults. This questionnaire con-
sists of two sets of 20 items each that are answered on a
4-point Likert scale. The first subscale measures state
anxiety, and the second measures trait anxiety. The test–
retest correlations for A- Trait scale ranges from 0.73 to
0.86 and 0.16 to 0.54 for A- State scale. The reliability
Fig. 1 Intergroup comparison of state and trait anxiety for experi- coefficient for A-trait ranges from 0.86 to 0.92 and 0.83 to
mental, placebo and control groups 0.92 for A- state scale (Spielberger et al. 1970) (Fig. 1).

123
134 Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144

Performance Measures (Fig. 5)

Dribbling Test

The dribbling test was part of the Harrison Basketball


Battery. It involves weaving in and around cones contin-
uously for 30 s while dribbling the ball. Each cone suc-
cessfully passed earned one point. The test–retest reliability
coefficient of battery is 0.95 (Barrow and McGee 1979).

Passing Test

The Stubbs’ Ball Handling Test was utilized. On a vertical


Fig. 2 Intergroup comparison of self efficacy for experimental,
placebo and control groups
flat wall, three adjacent circles each one 30 cm in diameter
were drawn at a distance of 160 cm from each other. The
first circle was drawn at 151 cm above the floor, the second
Self-Efficacy was measured by coping self-efficacy at 121 cm, and the third circle at 136 cm above the floor.
scale (CSES): It is a 26-item measure, with three higher- The athlete stood behind a painted line located at 450 cm
order dimensions: use problem-focused coping, stop away from the wall. On the verbal signal ‘‘Ready, … Go’’
unpleasant emotions and thoughts, and get support from the athlete threw the ball towards the first circle using a
family and friends. It is measured on 11-point scale, rating chest pass, retrieved the ball and passed to the second
the extent to which the participants feel that they can circle, retrieved the ball and passed to the third circle, then
perform behaviour important to effective coping. The retrieved the ball and passed to the second circle again,
CSES assesses the athletes’ confidence with regard to their then to the first, second, third, etc. The athlete continued
coping strategies. Thus, a higher CSES score would sug- passing the ball to the three circles for 30 s consecutively.
gest that a person is more confident in his or her ability to Each bounce, either inside the painted circle or on its
cope. The scale has adequate reliability with a Cronbach perimeter, earns one point. According to Stubb, a validity
alpha of 0.95 (Chesney et al. 2006) (Fig. 2). coefficient of 0.74 was achieved when the ratings were
correlated with best of two trials on the test (Barrow and
Physiological Measures McGee 1979).

Heart rate variability and respiration rate were measured Shooting Test
with the utilization of electrodes and a respiration strap
connected to an encoder. These measures were recorded A 3-min shooting test was used. The participant was asked
into a computer and analyzed by a specialized BFB soft- to execute as many shots as possible from any position on a
ware program (Biograph Procomp Infiniti 5.0 Thought marked perimeter of 366 cm radius from the hoop for 90 s.
technology Ltd., Canada) (Figs. 3 and 4).

Fig. 3 Intergroup comparison of total HRV, LF HRV and HF HRV Fig. 4 Intergroup comparison of respiration rate for experimental,
for experimental, placebo and control groups placebo and control groups

123
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144 135

The participant was responsible for shooting and retrieving variables on day one, day ten and 1 month follow-up along
the ball himself. Each successful shot earned one point. with inter-group comparison Two-way Repeated Measure
Test–retest reliability has been reported at 0.91 (Weinberg ANOVA was used. Statistical significance was accepted at
et al. 1991). p B 0.05. Tukey’s HSD post hoc comparison was con-
ducted to study the intergroup differences and to provide
Protocol specific information on associated variables (Table 1).

Group A: Experimental Group


Results
The protocol designed by Lehrer et al. (2000) for HRV
BFB training was implemented with the participants. Fol- Psychological Measures
lowing the pre-test measurements, the subject sat with
closed eyes in a chair for 5 min with hands resting on arm The means and standard deviation for pre, post and follow
rest in a peaceful room before commencement of HRV up state anxiety for the three groups is shown in Table 2.
biofeedback training. In the first session, the subject was Variation in state anxiety measured over time (i.e. pre, post
asked to breathe at variable respiratory rates for about and follow up) was statistically significant in each group
2 min each (6.5, 6, 5.5, 5, 4.5 breaths/minute) for deter- along with interaction of group and time (F = 66.503,
mination of resonant frequency. A ‘‘pacing stimulus’’: a p \ 0.001). The inter-group difference in state anxiety was
light display that moved up and down on the computer also statistically significant (F = 10.767, p \ 0.001).
screen at the target respiratory rate was provided. The However the post hoc analysis using Tukey’s-HSD
subject was instructed to breathe at that particular rate of revealed statistically significant difference between group
stimulus. The resonant frequency was determined as the 1(Experimental) and 2(Placebo) and group 1 and 3(Con-
maximum point in the peak amplitude signal of the reso- trol); whereas no significant difference was found between
nant frequency detection monitor on the biofeedback group 2 and 3 (Table 2).
equipment. The subject was then asked to breathe at his Similarly the means and SD for pre, post and follow up
resonant frequency and relax. The BFB sessions were trait anxiety for the three groups is shown in Table 3.
given for 10 consecutive days for 20 min each. Throughout Variation in trait anxiety measured over time (i.e. pre, post
the training, the subject was instructed for natural shallow and follow up) was statistically significant in each group
breathing, to avoid hyperventilation, as can be provoked by along with interaction of group and time (F = 157.573,
this technique. The feedback to the subject was given in the p \ 0.001). The inter-group difference in trait anxiety was
form of beat-to-beat heart rate and respiratory rate on the statistically significant (F = 6.628, p \ 0.05). However
screen. In addition, the subject was taught breathing the post hoc analysis using Tukey’s-HSD revealed statis-
through pursed lips abdominal procedure to elicit high tically significant difference between group 1 and 2 and
amplitude oscillations in heart rate at his resonant fre- group 1 and 3; whereas no significant difference was found
quency to achieve a relaxed state. between group 2 and 3 (Table 3).

Group B: Placebo Group Table 1 Univariate ANOVA for pre experimental, placebo and no
treatment control group

Subjects were shown motivational basketball video clips Measures Pre


for 10 min daily for 10 days. Sum of squares df Mean square F

State anxiety 97.067 2 48.533 0.984NS


Group C: No Treatment Control Group
Trait anxiety 85.400 2 42.700 0.786NS
This group did not receive any training. Self efficacy 1,955.400 2 977.700 1.878NS
Players in all the three groups were allowed to continue Total HRV 731,752.958 2 365,876.479 1.045NS
with normal practice schedule. LF HRV 5,945.262 2 2,972.631 0.074NS
HF HRV 335,268.092 2 167,634.046 2.074NS
Respiration rate 2.651 2 1.326 0.337NS
Statistical Analyses Dribbling 471.800 2 235.900 3.628**
Passing 8.267 2 4.133 0.467NS
The data was statistically analysed using the Statistical Shooting 0.067 2 0.033 0.007NS
Package for Social Sciences (SPSS)/16.0 (Copyright NS
p [ 0.05: not significant; * p \ 0.001: highly significant;
SPSS Inc.). For examining the changes in the dependent ** p \ 0.05: significant

123
136 Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144

Table 2 Showing state anxiety


Group Time of measurement
in three groups
Pre Post Follow up

1. Experimental 40.90 ± 7.66 21.40 ± 1.43 20.10 ± 1.28


2. Placebo 38.90 ± 5.95 38.70 ± 6.12 38.60 ± 6.15
3. Control 36.50 ± 7.33 36.40 ± 7.38 36.30 ± 7.34
Test of within subject effect
Source Sum of squares df Mean square F value p value

Two-way repeated measure ANOVA for state anxiety


Time 942.200 2 471.100 70.652 \0.001*
Time 9 group 1,773.733 4 443.433 66.503 \0.001*
Error 360.067 54 6.668
Test of between subject effect
Group 2,121.867 2 1,060.933 10.767 \0.001*
Error 2,660.533 27 98.538
Comparison Mean difference p value

Post hoc analysis


Group 1 versus 2 11.27 \0.001*
NS
p [ 0.05: not significant; Group 1 versus 3 8.93 \0.05**
* p \ 0.001: highly significant;
** p \ 0.05: significant Group 2 versus 3 2.33 0.639NS

The means and SD for pre, post and follow up coping significant difference between group 1 and 2 and group 1
self efficacy for the three groups is shown in Table 4. Vari- and 3; whereas no significant difference was found between
ation in coping self efficacy measured over time (i.e. pre, post group 2 and 3 (Table 5).
and follow up) was statistically significant in each group The means and SD for pre, post and follow up LF HRV
along with interaction of group and time (F = 79.688, for the three groups is shown in Table 6. Variation in LF
p \ 0.001). The inter-group difference in coping self effi- HRV measured over time (i.e. pre, post and follow up) was
cacy was statistically non significant (F = 2.269, p = statistically significant in each group along with interaction
0.123). Post hoc analysis using Tukey’s-HSD revealed sta- of group and time (F = 12.707, p \ 0.001). The inter-
tistically no significant difference between group 1 and 2, group difference in LF HRV was statistically significant
group 1 and 3 and group 2 and 3 (Table 4). (F = 6.969, p \ 0.05). However the post hoc analysis
using Tukey’s-HSD revealed statistically significant dif-
Physiological Measures ference between group 1 and 2 and group 1 and 3; whereas
no significant difference was found between group 2 and 3
Paced stimulus breathing in response to rhythmical simu- (Table 6).
lation of individual’s resonant frequency elicits high High frequency HRV decreases after the BFB training
amplitude heart rate oscillations during HRV BFB training. whereas it increases across the sessions. The means and SD
When the participant breathes at or close to resonant fre- for pre, post and follow up HF HRV for the three groups is
quency, the respiratory component is most prominent. shown in Table 7. Variation in HF HRV measured over
Also, HRV and respiration are in phase with each other, time (i.e. pre, post and follow up) was statistically signif-
that is, heart rate increases with inhalation and decreases icant in each group along with interaction of group and
with exhalation. time (F = 17.193, p \ 0.001). The inter-group difference
The means and SD for pre, post and follow up Total in LF HRV was also statistically significant (F = 6.176,
HRV for the three groups is shown in Table 5. Variation in p \ 0.05). However the post hoc analysis using Tukey’s-
Total HRV measured over time (i.e. pre, post and follow HSD revealed statistically significant difference between
up) was statistically significant in each group along with group 1 and 2 and group 1 and 3; whereas no significant
interaction of group and time (F = 20.850, p \ 0.001). difference was found between group 2 and 3 (Table 7).
The inter-group difference in Total HRV was statistically The means and SD for pre, post and follow up Respi-
significant (F = 6.610, p \ 0.05). However the post hoc ration Rate for the three groups is shown in Table 8.
analysis using Tukey’s-HSD revealed statistically Variation in Respiration Rate measured over time (i.e. pre,

123
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144 137

Table 3 Showing trait anxiety


Group Time of measurement
in three groups
Pre Post Follow up

1. Experimental 45.30 ± 5.88 26.20 ± 2.89 25.40 ± 2.67


2. Placebo 41.30 ± 7.18 41.20 ± 7.13 41.10 ± 6.95
3. Control 42.40 ± 8.75 42.50 ± 8.84 42.40 ± 8.66
Test of within subject effect
Source Sum of squares df Mean square F value p value

Two-way repeated measure ANOVA for trait anxiety


Time 855.356 2 427.678 160.157 \0.001*
Time 9 group 1,683.111 4 420.778 157.573 \0.001*
Error 144.200 54 2.670
Test of between subject effect
Group 1,834.156 2 917.078 6.628 \0.05**
Error 3,735.800 27 138.363
Comparison Mean difference p value

Post hoc analysis


Group 1 versus 2 8.90 \0.05**
NS
p [ 0.05: not significant; Group 1 versus 3 10.13 \0.05**
* p \ 0.001: highly significant;
Group 2 versus 3 1.23 0.913NS
** p \ 0.05: significant

Table 4 Showing coping self


Group Time of measurement
efficacy in three groups
Pre Post Follow up

1. Experimental 176.30 ± 31.67 223.30 ± 24.74 224.80 ± 25.71


2. Placebo 195.80 ± 14.14 196.40 ± 14.11 196.30 ± 14.15
3. Control 188.90 ± 18.93 188.80 ± 18.91 188.80 ± 19.01
Test of within subject effect
Source Sum of squares df Mean square F value p value

Two-way repeated measure ANOVA for coping self efficacy


Time 5,166.022 2 2,583.011 81.943 \0.001*
Time 9 group 10,047.778 4 2,511.944 79.688 \0.001*
Error 1,702.200 54 31.522
Test of between subject effect
Group 5,694.689 2 2,847.344 2.269 0.123NS
Error 33,887.800 27 1,255.104
Comparison Mean difference p value

Post hoc analysis


Group 1 versus 2 11.97 0.403NS
NS
p [ 0.05: not significant; Group 1 versus 3 19.30 0.107NS
* p \ 0.001: highly significant;
** p \ 0.05: significant Group 2 versus 3 7.33 0.705NS

post and follow up) was statistically significant in each p \ 0.001). However the post hoc analysis using Tukey’s-
group along with interaction of group and time HSD revealed statistically significant difference between
(F = 202.989, p \ 0.001). The inter-group difference in group 1 and 2 and group 1 and 3; whereas no significant
Respiration Rate was statistically significant (F = 41.874, difference was found between group 2 and 3 (Table 8).

123
138 Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144

Table 5 Showing total HRV in


Group Time of measurement
three groups
Pre Post Follow up

1. Experimental 808.89 ± 817.73 2,224.11 ± 1,624.81 2,287.21 ± 1,641.80


2. Placebo 760.10 ± 482.32 760.05 ± 482.04 760.17 ± 482.16
3. Control 455.89 ± 386.51 455.75 ± 386.68 455.88 ± 386.47
Test of within subject effect
Source Sum of squares df Mean square F value p value

Two-way repeated measure ANOVA for total HRV


Time 4,657,710.465 2 2,328,855.232 20.847 \0.001*
Time 9 group 9,316,509.147 4 2,329,127.287 20.850 \0.001*
Error 6,032,317.874 54 111,709.590
Test of between subject effect
Group 2.855E7 2 1.428E7 6.610 \0.05**
Error 5.832E7 27 2,159,910.664
Comparison Mean difference p value

Post hoc analysis


Group 1 versus 2 1,013.29 \0.05**
NS
p [ 0.05: not significant; Group 1 versus 3 1,317.56 \0.05**
* p \ 0.001: highly significant;
Group 2 versus 3 304.26 0.705NS
** p \ 0.05: significant

Table 6 Showing LF HRV in


Group Time of measurement
three groups
Pre Post Follow up

1. Experimental 253.40 ± 219.30 1,220.10 ± 1,010.92 1,263.41 ± 1,027.79


2. Placebo 277.98 ± 140.97 277.86 ± 141.00 277.97 ± 140.90
3. Control 244.75 ± 227.45 244.78 ± 227.15 244.78 ± 227.16
Test of within subject effect
Source Sum of squares df Mean square F value p value

Two-way repeated measure ANOVA for LF HRV


Time 2,173,803.014 2 1,086,901.507 12.706 \0.001*
Time 9 group 4,347,964.257 4 1,086,991.064 12.707 \0.001*
Error 4,619,404.576 54 85,544.529
Test of between subject effect
Group 8,491,253.184 2 4,245,626.592 6.969 \0.05**
Error 1.645E7 27 609,234.514
Comparison Mean difference p value

Post hoc analysis


Group 1 versus 2 634.36 \0.05**
NS
p [ 0.05: not significant;
Group 1 versus 3 667.53 \0.05**
* p \ 0.001: highly significant;
** p \ 0.05: significant Group 2 versus 3 33.16 0.985NS

Sport Performance Measures statistically significant in each group along with interaction
of group and time (F = 20.444, p \ 0.001). The inter-
The means and SD for pre, post and follow up dribbling for group difference in shooting was also statistically signifi-
the three groups is shown in Table 9. Variation in dribbling cant (F = 14.181, p \ 0.001). The post hoc analysis using
measured over time (i.e. pre, post and follow up) was Tukey’s-HSD revealed statistically significant difference

123
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144 139

Table 7 Showing HF HRV in


Group Time of measurement
three groups
Pre Post Follow up

1. Experimental 385.73 ± 463.24 669.29 ± 523.28 686.68 ± 534.86


2. Placebo 223.14 ± 113.76 222.89 ± 113.96 222.75 ± 113.92
3. Control 129.90 ± 122.29 129.88 ± 122.36 129.80 ± 122.12
Test of within subject effect
Source Sum of squares df Mean square F value p value

Two-way repeated measure ANOVA for HF HRV


Time 189,807.883 2 94,903.942 17.125 \0.001*
Time 9 group 381,115.324 4 95,278.831 17.193 \0.001*
Error 299,258.986 54 5,541.833
Test of between subject effect
Group 3,397,030.388 2 1,698,515.194 6.176 \0.05**
Error 7,425,026.910 27 275,000.997
Comparison Mean difference p value

Post hoc analysis


Group 1 versus 2 357.63 \0.05**
NS
p [ 0.05: not significant; Group 1 versus 3 450.70 \0.05**
* p \ 0.001: highly significant;
Group 2 versus 3 93.06 0.773NS
** p \ 0.05: significant

Table 8 Showing respiration


Group Time of measurement
rate in three groups
Pre Post Follow up

1. Experimental 15.30 ± 2.00 6.26 ± 0.26 6.00 ± 0.16


2. Placebo 15.23 ± 2.12 15.25 ± 2.09 15.33 ± 2.17
3. Control 14.64 ± 1.81 14.64 ± 1.75 14.83 ± 1.72
Test of within subject effect
Source Sum of squares df Mean square F value p value

Two-way repeated measure ANOVA for respiration rate


Time 180.741 2 90.370 192.734 \0.001*
Time 9 group 380.715 4 95.179 202.989 \0.001*
Error 25.320 54 .469
Test of between subject effect
Group 678.204 2 339.102 41.874 \0.001*
Error 218.651 27 8.098
Comparison Mean difference p value

Post hoc analysis


Group 1 versus 2 6.08 \0.001*
NS
p [ 0.05: not significant; Group 1 versus 3 5.51 \0.001*
* p \ 0.001: highly significant;
Group 2 versus 3 0.56 0.721NS
** p \ 0.05: significant

between group 1 and 2 and group 1 and 3; whereas no passing measured over time (i.e. pre, post and follow up)
significant difference was found between group 2 and 3 was statistically significant in each group along with
(Table 9). interaction of group and time (F = 115.739, p \ 0.001).
The means and SD for pre, post and follow up passing The inter-group difference in shooting was also statistically
for the three groups is shown in Table 10. Variation in significant (F = 9.860, p \ 0.001). However the post hoc

123
140 Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144

Table 9 Showing dribbling in


Group Time of measurement
three groups
Pre Post Follow up

1. Experimental 40.30 ± 10.75 50.40 ± 8.60 56.60 ± 8.24


2. Placebo 35.90 ± 6.35 37.00 ± 6.34 33.20 ± 12.40
3. Control 30.60 ± 6.25 32.60 ± 6.51 31.70 ± 6.12
Test of within subject effect
Source Sum of squares df Mean square F value p value

Two-way repeated measure ANOVA for dribbling


Time 436.200 2 218.100 17.587 \0.001*
Time 9 group 1,014.133 4 253.533 20.444 \0.001*
Error 669.667 54 12.401
Test of between subject effect
Group 5,076.267 2 2,538.133 14.181 \0.001*
Error 4,832.633 27 178.986
Comparison Mean difference p value

Post hoc analysis


Group 1 versus 2 13.73 \0.001*
NS
p [ 0.05: not significant; Group 1 versus 3 17.47 \0.001*
* p \ 0.001: highly significant;
Group 2 versus 3 3.73 0.534NS
** p \ 0.05: significant

Table 10 Showing passing in


Group Time of measurement
three groups
Pre Post Follow up

1. Experimental 9.40 ± 3.02 16.60 ± 2.27 22.10 ± 2.13


2. Placebo 9.60 ± 3.62 11.90 ± 3.75 10.80 ± 4.07
3. Control 10.60 ± 2.06 12.00 ± 2.35 11.90 ± 2.72
Test of within subject effect
Source Sum of squares df Mean square F value p value

Two-way repeated measure ANOVA for passing


Time 409.267 2 204.633 214.984 \0.001*
Time 9 group 440.667 4 110.167 115.739 \0.001*
Error 51.400 54 0.952
Test of between subject effect
Group 488.267 2 244.133 9.860 \0.001*
Error 668.500 27 24.759
Comparison Mean difference p value

Post hoc analysis


Group 1 versus 2 5.27 \0.001*
NS
p [ 0.05: not significant; Group 1 versus 3 4.53 \0.001*
* p \ 0.001: highly significant;
** p \ 0.05: significant Group 2 versus 3 0.73 0.837NS

analysis using Tukey’s-HSD revealed statistically signifi- The means and SD for the parameter of shooting at pre,
cant difference between group 1 and 2 and group 1 and 3; post and follow up for the three groups is shown in
whereas no significant difference was found between group Table 11. Variation in shooting measured over time (i.e.
2 and 3 (Table 10). pre, post and follow up) was statistically significant in each

123
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144 141

Table 11 Showing shooting in


Group Time of measurement
three groups
Pre Post Follow up

1. Experimental 5.30 ± 2.66 10.80 ± 2.34 14.70 ± 2.90


2. Placebo 5.20 ± 1.68 6.70 ± 2.11 6.00 ± 1.82
3. Control 5.30 ± 2.00 5.80 ± 2.04 5.70 ± 2.16
Test of within subject effect
Source Sum of squares df Mean square F value p value

Two-way repeated measure ANOVA for shooting


Time 198.022 2 99.011 125.507 \0.001*
Time 9 group 260.711 4 65.178 82.620 \0.001*
Error 42.600 54 0.789
Test of between subject effect
Group 404.022 2 202.011 15.206 \0.001*
Error 358.700 27 13.285
Comparison Mean difference p value

Post hoc analysis


Group 1 versus 2 4.30 \0.001*
NS
p [ 0.05: not significant; Group 1 versus 3 4.67 \0.001*
* p \ 0.001: highly significant;
** p \ 0.05: significant Group 2 versus 3 0.37 0.920NS

group along with interaction of group and time an increase in sympathetic cardiac control, decrease in
(F = 82.620, p \ 0.001). The inter-group difference in parasympathetic control, or both. Biofeedback induced
shooting was statistically significant (F = 15.206, effects on HRV indices and respiration rate helps to mod-
p \ 0.001). However the post hoc analysis using Tukey’s- erate the heightened sympathetic activity. Total and LF HRV
HSD revealed statistically significant difference between increased post training and at follow up whereas decrease in
group 1 and 2 and group 1 and 3; whereas no significant HF HRV was observed post BFB training while it increased
difference was found between group 2 and 3 (Table 11). across the sessions for experimental group. Statistically non
significant results were acknowledged for HRV parameters
and respiration rate in placebo and no treatment control
Discussion and Future Directions groups. The desired respiration rate of six breaths per minute
was achieved in the experimental group after biofeedback
The primary objective of the present study was to examine training. The subject was trained to produce the smooth
the oppugn of heart rate variability biofeedback (HRV sinusoidal wave form (RSA) in which respiration rate and
BFB) as a stress coping tool. Application of biofeedback is heart rate varies in phase with each other, that is, heart rate
pragmatic in helping an individual modulate his/her emo- rises with inhalation and decreases with exhalation. Findings
tions. Biofeedback also monitors nervous system activation of physiological measures in the present study fortifies that
level to ascertain psychophysiological readiness of the the cardiovascular system has the property of resonance at a
body imperative for supreme performance (Jordanova and frequency near 0.1 Hz (six breaths per minute) consistent
Demerdzieva 2010). The results of the present study indi- with preliminary studies (Lagos et al. 2008; Lehrer et al.
cate that the BFB group exhibited considerable reduction in 2003, 2006; Hassett et al. 2007).
state and trait anxiety post training as compared to placebo Breathing at or near resonance frequency help subjects
and no treatment control groups, this mitigation in psy- to change their tonic level of physiological arousal by
chological stress effect persisted at 1 month follow up also. increasing HRV amplitude, hence directly resulting in
The effect of reduction in anxiety could be attributed to training and exercising the bodies’ own physiological
regulation and stimulation of baroreflexes by breathing at control mechanisms (Sutarto et al. 2010). The HRV BFB
one’s resonant frequency through HRV BFB technique. training helps to restore the autonomic balance and
The psychological states and processes can cause dra- improves autonomic control that supports emotional regu-
matic impact on autonomic control of the heart. Psycho- lation and performance coordination (Lagos et al. 2008;
logical stressors such as anxiety are often associated with Lehrer et al. 2003). The high amplitude oscillations

123
142 Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144

Placebo group also demonstrated statistically significant


improvement in self efficacy due to visualization of moti-
vational basketball videos.
Within the last decade, biofeedback practitioners have
suggested that HRV BFB may be useful in increasing the
performance of wrestlers, dancers, baseball players and
golfers (Lagos et al. 2008; Raymond et al. 2005; Strack
2003). The improved sport performance in the experi-
mental group, as indicated by the findings of the present
study, could be attributed to the HRV BFB procedure
eliciting resonance in the cardiovascular system. The HRV
BFB training monitors autonomic nervous system homeo-
Fig. 5 Intergroup comparison of dribbling, passing and shooting for stasis and reflects a positive psycho physiological shift.
experimental, placebo and control groups
Basketball performance tests including dribbling and
passing were established to be statistically significant for
stimulate and exercises autonomic reflexes, particularly the all the involved three groups post training. Shooting skill
baroreflexes, thus restoring and regulating the balance was significant for experimental and placebo groups post
between sympathetic and parasympathetic systems (Lehrer training while non significant for no treatment control
et al. 2003). group. At 1 month follow up, significant results were
Sport psychologists suggest that motivational sphere of an noticeable for experimental group for dribbling, passing
athlete is a nucleus of his/her personality which should be and shooting. Improvement in performance for placebo and
given equal importance. However, an athlete’s performance no treatment control groups could be due to the regular and
suffers when their tension exceeds the capacity for maximal consistent practice of their game during the study period.
functioning. The potential interaction of specific stress It is apparent from the findings of the present study that
responses like anxiety with performance has a debilitative HRV BFB training is a self regulatory intervention aimed
effect. Thus, it is important for athletes to be able to control at reducing psychophysiological stressors resulting in
their anxiety to accomplish peak performance during train- optimal performance. The cumulative effects of the psy-
ing and competition. According to Humara (1999), athletes chosocial processes directly target the elevated sympathetic
who score high on self efficacy interpret their anxiety as arousal, ultimately achieving the ‘‘flow state’’ of the mind
being facilitative instead of debilitative and they also achieve which is imperative for peak performance (Jordanova and
success more easily. The significantly improved scores of Demerdzieva 2010).
self efficacy suggest interesting finding as it indicates the
fostering of self confidence in subjects of the experimental Future Directions
group. The potential interdependence between raised levels
of self efficacy and self confidence with ability to cope with Different individuals gain knowledge of their specific game
diverse threats and stressors may help the subject negate the skills at different pace, and thus some players may not
deteriorating consequences of anxiety. Thus according to acquire and gain self-regulatory skills in ten sessions. Further
Anyadubalu (2010), it can be considered meaningful and research is warranted to assess the effects of HRV BFB over
impulsive for regulation of motivation. durations that would include more than ten sessions. Sec-
Nicholls et al. (2010) suggested that Coping self efficacy ondly, as individual biofeedback sessions were used in this
beliefs undoubtedly put forth strong influences on situational study, it would be helpful to investigate the effects of the
appraisals and the manner in which any personage retort to same intervention using group training sessions. The inter-
these appraisals such as anxiety. The lowered state and trait vention may be more practical to implement if athletes learn
anxiety could be attributed to restructuring of cognitive the self-regulation skills in a group setting as it could be of
processes and lowered sympathetic arousal accomplished great help to players involved in team sports. Third, the
through HRV BFB training. This finding is also concluded by resonant frequency BFB procedure may be combined with
Dahlbeck and Lightsey (2008), who showed that high self relaxation imagery intervention so as to generate physio-
efficacy, predicts lower anxiety and better psychological logical and psychological domains more consistently and
adjustment in an individual. Reduction in anxiety could also extensively. Also, future studies may aim at exploring the
be ascribed to the attention and importance given to the role of BFB relative to the advanced skills of the basketball
subject during the HRV BFB procedure. During follow up players like rebounding, moving with and without the ball,
interactions, the subjects appeared to be emotionally com- position specific skills to bring into focus the necessary
posed and felt greater control over their apprehensions. transition in the performance of the player.

123
Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144 143

Strength and Limitation of the Study International Journal of Human and Social Sciences, 5(3),
193–198.
Babiloni, C., Percio, C. D., Iacoboni, M., Infarinato, F., Lizio, R.,
A major strength of the current study was inclusion of Marzano, N., et al. (2008). Golf putt outcomes are predicted by
placebo group to compare and evaluate the efficacy of sensorimotor cerebral EEG rhythms. Journal of Physiology,
HRV BFB more specifically on the trained players. Pre- 58(1), 131–139.
vious studies which examined the effect of HRV BFB on Barrow, H., & McGee, R. (1979). A practical approach to measure-
ment in physical education. Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.
sport performance did not incorporate the placebo condi- Chesney, M. A., Neilands, T. B., Chambers, D. B., Taylor, J. M., &
tions. Placebo group in this study helped in minimizing Folkman, S. (2006). A validity and reliability study of the coping
possible Hawthorne effects. Further, potential threats to the self-efficacy scale. British Journal of Health Psychology, 11,
internal validity of the current study were addressed 421–437.
Dahlbeck, D. T., & Lightsey, O. R., Jr. (2008). Generalized self-
through random distribution of players into three equal efficacy, coping, and self-esteem as predictors of psychological
groups. Inclusion of both male and female players with adjustment among children with disabilities or chronic illnesses.
skills ranging from university, state to national standards Children’s Health Care, 37(4), 293–315.
ensures that findings are generalized for both gender and Dehdari, T., Heidarnia, A., Ramezankhani, A., Sadeghian, S., &
Ghofranipour, F. (2008). Anxiety, self efficacy expectation and
different levels of the game. Recording of the tested social support in patients after coronary angioplasty and
measures after a period of 1 month helped in the assess- coronary bypass. Iranian journal of public health, 3(4), 119–125.
ment of long term effects of HRV BFB on sports perfor- Hammond, D. C. (2007). Neurofeedback for the enhancement of
mance and predicted whether training effects remain athletic performance and physical balance. The Journal of the
American Board of Sport Psychology, 1, 1–9.
consistent over time. Limitation of the present study was Hassett, A. L., Radvanski, D. C., Vaschillo, E. G., Vaschillo, B.,
the small sample size and retrieving the ball himself/herself Sigal, L. H., Karavidas, M. K., et al. (2007). A pilot study of the
during shooting test that might influence the player’s per- efficacy of heart rate variability (HRV) biofeedback in patients
formance during pre and post recording. with fibromyalgia. Applied Psychophysiology Biofeedback, 32,
1–10.
Humara, M. (1999). The relationship between anxiety and perfor-
mance: A cognitive-behavioral perspective. Athletic insight, the
Conclusion online journal of sport psychology, 1(2), 1–14.
Jordanova, N. P., & Demerdzieva, A. (2010). Biofeedback training
for peak performance in sport: Case study. Macedonian journal
The existing study was sufficiently powered to examine the of medical sciences, 3(2), 113–118.
effect of HRV BFB on performance skills of anxious Lagos, L., Vaschillo, E., Vaschillo, B., Lehrer, P., Bates, M., &
basketball players. Pandina, R. (2008). Heart rate variability biofeedback for dealing
In spite of the limitations inherent in the present study, with competitive anxiety: A case study. Applied Psychophysi-
ology and Biofeedback, 36(3), 109–115.
the findings of the study suggest that cardiac variability Lehrer, P. M., Vaschillo, E. G., & Vaschillo, B. (2000). Resonant
biofeedback (HRV BFB) training focalizes on simulating frequency biofeedback training to increase cardiac variability:
physiological requirements with psychological thus, help- Rationale and manual for training. Applied Psychophysiology
ing athletes find their ‘‘zone of excellence’’. An interesting and Biofeedback, 25, 177–191.
Lehrer, P., Vaschillo, E., Vaschillo, B., Lu, S. E., Eckberg, D. L.,
finding which emerged from results of the present study Edelberg, R., et al. (2003). Heart rate variability biofeedback
was the improvement in the construct of self efficacy after increases baroreflex gain and peak expiratory flow. Psychoso-
the biofeedback training which was persistent even after a matic Medicine, 65, 796–805.
month. This suggests the profound influence of biofeed- Lehrer, P., Vaschillo, E., Lu, S. E., Eckberg, D., Vaschillo, B.,
Scardella, A., et al. (2006). Heart rate variability biofeedback:
back on psychosocial processes which may serve as an Effects of age on heart rate variability, baroreflex gain, and
index of sports performance. Thus, HRV BFB may serve as Asthma. Chest, 129, 278–284.
a powerful strategy in the area of sports psychophysiology Nicholls, A. R., Polman, R., & Levy, A. R. (2010). Coping self-efficacy,
for emotional and cognitive restructuring. pre-competitive anxiety, and subjective performance among
athletes. European journal of sport science, 10(2), 97–102.
Continued research is required in the emerging field of Raymond, J., Sajid, I., Parkinson, L. A., & Gruzelier, J. H. (2005).
HRV BFB as it may be incorporated with physical training Biofeedback and dance performance: A preliminary investiga-
to become an integral part of performance and rehabilita- tion. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 30, 65–73.
tion psychology in contemporary sports medicine. Spielberger, C. D. (1966). Theory and research on anxiety. In C.
S. Spielberger (Ed.), Anxiety and behaviour (pp. 3–20). New
York: Academic Press.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual
for the state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI). Palo Alto, CA:
References Consulting Psychologists Press.
Sutarto, A. P., Wahab, M. N. A., & Zin, N. M. (2010). Heart rate
Anyadubalu, C. C. (2010). Self-efficacy, anxiety, and performance in variability (HRV) biofeedback: A new training approach for
the English language among middle-school students in English operator’s performance enhancement. Journal of Industrial
language program in Satri Si Suriyothai School, Bangkok. Engineering and Management, 3(1), 176–198.

123
144 Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback (2012) 37:131–144

Strack, B. W. (2003). Effect of heart rate variability (HRV) biofeed- Wheat, A. L., & Larkin, K. T. (2010). Biofeedback of heart rate
back on batting performance in baseball. Dissertation Abstracts variability and related physiology: A critical review. Applied
International: Section B, the Sciences and Engineering, 64(3), Psychophysiology Biofeedback, 35, 229–242.
1540B. Wilmore, J., & Costill, D. (2004). Physiology of sport and exercise
Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North (5th ed.). USA: Human Kinetics Publishers.
American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. (1996). Heart Wilson, G., & Pritchard, M. (2005). Comparing sources of stress in
rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological inter- college student athletes and non-athletes. Athletic Insight: The
pretation, and clinical use. European Heart Journal, 17, 354–381. Online Journal of Sport Psychology, 7(1), 1–8.
Weinberg, R. S., Fowler, C., Jackson, A., Bagnall, J., & Bruya, L. Zupan, M. F., Arata, A. W., Wile, A., & Parker, R. (2006). Visual
(1991). Effect of goal difficulty on motor performance: A adaptations to sports vision enhancement training: A study of
replication across tasks and subjects. Journal of Sport & collegiate athletes at the US air force academy. In Optometry
Exercise Psychology, 13, 160–173. today/Optics today (Vol. 46, pp. 43–48).

123

You might also like