You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Molecular Neuroscience

https://doi.org/10.1007/s12031-019-01276-1

Iron Deficiency, Cognitive Functions, and Neurobehavioral


Disorders in Children
Lyudmila Pivina 1,2 & Yuliya Semenova 1,2 & Monica Daniela Doşa 3 & Marzhan Dauletyarova 4 & Geir Bjørklund 5

Received: 29 November 2018 / Accepted: 7 February 2019


# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019

Abstract
More than 25% of the world’s population is affected by anemia, of which more than 50% suffers from iron deficiency anemia
(IDA). Children below 7 years of age are the population group that is most vulnerable to iron deficiency. Iron is an essential
element in brain metabolism. Iron deficiency can cause changes in neurotransmitter homeostasis, decrease myelin production,
impair synaptogenesis, and decline the function of the basal ganglia. Therefore, IDA adversely affects cognitive functions and
psychomotor development. Research has shown that iron deficiency is a frequent comorbidity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder. Iron deficiency may also induce or exacerbate deficiency of other essential
nutrients, which may have a negative impact on the developing brain and other organs in infants. Many nations of the world have
programs to control IDA based on the use of iron supplementation, intake of fortified food and drinks, improved food safety, and
monitoring of dietary diversity. Based on the current recommendations of the World Health Organization on cost-effectiveness
(WHO-CHOICE), iron fortification and iron supplementation programs can be considered cost-effective or even highly cost-
effective in most countries of the world to averting cognitive impairment.

Keywords Anemia . Iron deficiency . Children . Cognitive function . Children . ADHD . Autism . Psychomotor development

Introduction and Ani 2001). For soon 20 years ago, the International
Nutritional Anemia Consultative Group (INACG) tentatively
Despite global progress achieved in medicine and science de- estimated that five billion people, or as much as 80% of the
velopment, anemia continues to be one of the most prevalent world’s population, may be iron-deficient (INACG2000).
disorders. More than 25% of the world’s population has signs Children under 2 years of age are particularly sensitive to iron
of anemia, and more than 50% of them, in particular women deficiency due to rapid growth. Kassebaum et al. (2014) pre-
and children below 7 years of age, suffer from iron deficiency sented an estimation of the varying severity of anemia for the
anemia (IDA). Iron supplementation may improve mental, period from 1990 to 2010 in 187 countries in 20 age groups
psychomotor, and physical development in children, although using data from the Global Burden of Diseases, Injuries and
evidence of such effects is rather scarce (Grantham-McGregor Risk Factors (GBD) 2010 study. In 2010, the world preva-
lence of anemia was 32.91% which corresponded to 68.36
million years of life affected by disability (8.8% the total num-
ber of all cases of disability). The highest prevalence rates
* Geir Bjørklund
bjorklund@conem.org were found in women of all age groups living in the countries
of South Asia and sub-Saharan Africa, while in the East Asia
1
Semey Medical University, Semey, Kazakhstan countries this rate tended to decrease over the study period.
2
CONEM Kazakhstan Environmental Health and Safety Research IDA prevailed in the structure of anemia. Malaria, schistoso-
Group, Semey Medical University, Semey, Kazakhstan miasis, and chronic kidney disease were the common causes
3
Department of Pharmacology, Faculty of Medicine, Ovidius of this pathology. The most challenging situation is observed
University, Constanta, Romania in the children under 5 years, where the prevalence of anemia
4
Astana Medical University, Astana, Kazakhstan has increased (Kassebaum 2016). In another study that cov-
5 ered data from 23 countries for children under 7 years old and
Council for Nutritional and Environmental Medicine (CONEM),
Toften 24, 8610 Mo i Rana, Norway women of reproductive age, IDA was observed in 25% of the
J Mol Neurosci

children (95% CI: 18.0, 32.0) and in 37.0% of the women p < 0.001) and MCV (OR, 2.08; 95% CI, 1.36–3.17; p =
(95% CI: 28.0, 46.0) (Petry et al. 2016). The prevalence of 0.001) were the most reliable indicators (Gera et al. 2009).
IDA in Iranian children under 18 years of age was 13.9%, The Denver II Developmental Screening Test (DDST-II) is
including 7.9% in the males and 8.5% in the females; iron an internationally recognized tool used to assess the psycho-
deficiency was found in 26.9% of all children (95% CI: motor development in children. Pala et al. (2010) applied it to
19.7–34.1) (Akbari et al. 2017). The prevalence of iron defi- evaluate the psychomotor development in children with iron
ciency in India is estimated to range from 70 to 90% (Sachdev deficiency and IDA in comparison with healthy controls,
and Gera 2013). The significant proportions of children with based on the following criteria: the test result was considered
iron deficiency were in the Eastern Mediterranean (46.6%), to be a Bdelay^ if a child scored over 90th percentile, and it
European (44.2%), and African (40.4%) regions (Muthayya was considered to be a Bcaution^ if a child scored between the
et al. 2013). 75th and 90th percentiles. Children with IDA had abnormal
At present, programs for control of IDA envisage the use of test results in nearly 68% of the cases, and iron deficiency was
iron treatment on a continuous or intermittent basis, intake of manifested by impaired psychomotor development in 22% of
iron sprinkles or fortified food and drinks, improved food the children. Meanwhile, 15% of healthy children with normal
safety, and monitoring of dietary diversity. The risk of anemia hematological parameters had abnormal DDST-II results.
development is strongly associated with a high prevalence of When anemic children were compared with non-anemic chil-
infectious and inflammatory diseases, in particular, malaria. dren, the difference in the proportion of abnormal scores was
Based on the concept of the World Health Organization significant (p < 0.01). However, this was not true for children
(WHO), careful epidemiological evaluation and control over with iron deficiency (p = 0.203) (Pala et al. 2010).
causative agents, design, and implementation of the most ef- Long-term follow-up for the development of children pre-
fective interventions capable of satisfying the needs of the sented with IDA in infancy showed that despite the treatment
population alone with security measures are the milestones provided, children had lower academic performance and de-
for global preventive strategies targeted on anemia reduction creased fine motor skills even 10 years’ posttreatment. Also,
especially in low income countries (Pasricha et al. 2013a, b). declined auditory cortex-evoked responses along with heart
Many countries in the world have developed programs to rate variability during the sleep-wake cycle were noted which
control IDA based on the use of iron supplementation, intake are considered to be associated with a decrease in myelin
of fortified food and drinks, improved food safety, and mon- synthesis due to iron deficiency (Walter 2003).
itoring of dietary diversity. The aim of this review is to clarify An observational study comprising 106 African American
the association between iron deficiency, cognitive functions, children with identified iron deficiency helped to establish an
and neurobehavioral disorders in children. association between body iron status and such indicators of
psychomotor development as Peabody Developmental Motor
Scale (PDMS), the motor quality factor of the Bayley
Iron Deficiency or Anemia in Children Behavioral Rating Scale, and sequential or bimanual coordi-
and Impaired Psychomotor Development nation in toy retrieval. The general linear model analysis was
carried out, which helped to identify the linear effects of iron
Iron takes part in a diversity of metabolic processes in the cen- parameters on developmental landmarks, PDMS (as a sugges-
tral nervous system. The role of iron in the synthesis of neural tive trend), the Infant Neurological International Battery
transmitters makes it very important for brain development both (INFANIB) standing item, overall motor quality, and toy-
antenatally and postnatally. The hematoencephalic barrier con- retrieval task. Developmental landmarks relied on iron defi-
trols the iron concentration in the cerebral structures. Iron defi- ciency with or without anemia as the threshold for effects,
ciency may result in decreased production of myelin, impaired while assessment of toy retrieval was based on INFANIB
synaptogenesis, and decline in the function of the basal ganglia, standing item, motor quality, and presence of IDA. Children
which adversely affect the psychomotor development and men- with iron deficiency showed signs of reduced motor function
tal capacity (Dommergues et al. 1989; Vallée 2017). even when anemia was absent (Shafir et al. 2008). These
Numerous studies have linked iron deficiency or anemia in findings were supported by another study, which established
children with impaired psychomotor development. In a meta- a reduction in mean mental developmental index (MDI), psy-
analysis that combined 17 studies (2416 children with febrile chomotor developmental index (PDI), and mean speech de-
convulsions and 2387 healthy children as control), a signifi- velopment quotients in children with IDA at the time of ex-
cant association of convulsions with IDA was found (OR, amination or those who suffered from IDA in the past as
1.98; 95% CI, 1.26–3.13; p = 0.003). The study was based compared with healthy controls (Hokama et al. 2005).
on an assessment of diagnostic criteria such as serum iron, A survey conducted in South Africa on children below
plasma ferritin, and mean corpuscular volume (MCV). In the 6 months of age investigated the association between the prev-
study, plasma ferritin (OR, 3.78; 95% CI, 1.80–7.94; alence of anemia, growth retardation, and psychomotor
J Mol Neurosci

development. The prevalence of anemia and growth retarda- randomized controlled trials conducted on children over
tion composed 36.4% and 28.5%, respectively, while multiple 6 years of age, adolescents, and women, showed that iron
regression analysis established associations between birth supplementation improves attention and concentration regard-
weight and combined psychomotor scores (β = − 3.427 (− less of the initial iron level (SMD 0.59, 95% CI 0.29; 0.90).
4.603, 1.891), p < 0.001) (Rothman et al. 2018). Those chil- Although in anemic children a 2.5 point (95% CI 1.24; 3.76)
dren who had lower hemoglobin level than 10.5 g/dL and had increase in intelligence quotient (IQ) was observed, this was
anemia in more than 3 months also had significantly lower not true for non-anemic individuals as well as for effects of
motor scores than the healthy controls (p < 0.05). Also, these iron intake on memory, psychomotor, or learning skills
children experienced reduced language capabilities and de- (Falkingham et al. 2010).
creased body balance-coordination skills as compared to con- The effects of iron deficiency on schooling were over the
trols. If iron deficiency progressed to established anemia, the past decades of years extensively discussed in the internation-
negative effects lasted for not less than 3 months, even when al literature, and most of the studies come from developing
iron treatment was provided (Walter et al. 1989). countries. In a randomized controlled trial, Soemantri et al.
The findings of a systematic review with meta-analysis (1985) investigated the effect of iron supplementation on ed-
suggest that short-term iron treatment within 5–11 days pro- ucational achievement and concentration in Indonesian prima-
vided to children with IDA below 3 years of age fails to dem- ry school children. Independently of their hematological pa-
onstrate conclusive evidence of positive effects in relation to rameters, those children who received iron supplementation
psychomotor development (Logan et al. 2001). Nevertheless, had significantly higher scores than children who received
the findings of other studies prove that in case of timely and placebo in terms of school achievements and concentration
adequate anemia management, the difference in psychomotor tests (Soemantri et al. 1985). Baumgartner et al. (2012)
development is diminished, which justifies the necessity of assessed cognitive functions in 6–11-year-old children who
early investigation and appropriate treatment enabling social received iron and omega-3 fatty acid supplementation alone
and intellectual maturation of children (Matiashvili et al. and in combination. A group of children with IDA who re-
2012). In a study by Soewondo (1995), it was noted that ceived iron supplementation had increased number of words
developmental delay completely disappeared after 4 months recalled, improved long-term memory, and retrieval as com-
of treatment with iron supplementation (Soewondo 1995). pared with non-anemic iron-deficient children (Baumgartner
Moreover, the prevention of psychomotor retardation in chil- et al. 2012). A study by Low et al. (2013) showed that daily
dren has to be started at the time of pregnancy and be based on iron intake in children aged 5–12 years improved global cog-
the mother ’s iron status, nutrition, and bad habits nitive assessment (SMD 0.50, CI 95% 0.11; 0.90, p = 0.01),
(Chmielewska et al. 2015; Iglesias et al. 2018). IQ for children with anemia (SMD 4.55, 95% CI 0.16; 8.94,
p = 0.04), and the level of concentration and attention. Iron
supplementation also improved age-adjusted height and
Iron Deficiency and Cognitive Functions weight in children with anemia. Dietary iron intake reduced
in Children the risk to develop anemia by 50% and the risk of iron defi-
ciency by 79%. Data about the safeness of iron supplementa-
Some observational studies suggest an association between tion was limited (Low et al. 2013).
anemia and impaired cognitive functions such as Teenage girls at the beginning of menstruations frequently
concentration, intellectual status, memory, and learning develop IDA. Based on the fact that this period often corre-
skills. A study by Hurtado et al. (1999) showed that children sponds with intensive schooling, girls’ academic performance
younger than 10 years who at an early age had IDA had may be jeopardized. The effects of iron treatment on memory
increased risk of moderate mental retardation. This relation- and attention of non-anemic adolescent girls with iron defi-
ship did not depend on birth weight, socioeconomic status, ciency were evaluated in a double-blind, placebo-controlled
age, and education of the mother, gender, or nationality clinical trial conducted by Bruner et al. (1996). Post-
(Hurtado et al. 1999). Children of primary school age, who intervention measurements of verbal learning and memory
had IDA in infancy with hemoglobin levels below 100 g/L, showed that girls who received iron supplementation per-
scored lower on tests for intellectual development rates formed significantly better (p < 0.02) on the tests than girls
(Lozoff et al. 1991). These results indicate how important it in the placebo group (Bruner et al. 1996). The study that
is to monitor anemia in infancy (Lozoff et al. 1991). was carried out could complement these findings by Nelson
Improved cognitive development due to long-term prophy- (1996), who suggested that the learning ability of British ad-
lactic oral administration of low-dose iron proves a relation- olescent girls may be affected by iron deficiency (Nelson
ship between iron deficiency and child development 1996). Both researchers advocated for the use of iron supple-
(Grantham-McGregor and Fernald 1997; Buys et al. 2005; mentation in this subgroup of school-age children.
Iannotti et al. 2006). A systematic review including 14
J Mol Neurosci

A systematic review of randomized clinical trials on the treatment with psychotropic drugs. These data indicate the
efficacy of oral administration of iron or iron-rich milk and need for screening of children with ADHD to detect iron de-
cereals in children younger than 27 months showed a moder- ficiency (Cortese et al. 2012a, b).
ate increase in mental development using the Bayley mental A meta-analysis that combined 17 trials that investigated
and psychomotor development indices (SMD 0.95 (95% CI - the associations between ADHD and iron status/iron deficien-
0.56 to 2.46, p = 0.22; p = 0.016 for heterogeneity). For IQ cy demonstrated that the levels of ferritin and peripheral serum
scores in the children older than 8 years, SMD was 0.41 ferritin were substantially lower in children with ADHD (chil-
(p < 0.001). There was not found a positive influence on mo- dren with ADHD = 1560, control group = 4691, p = 0.013).
tor development score (SMD 0.09, 95% CI −0.08; 0.26, p = However, there was no significant difference in serum iron
0.28) (Sachdev et al. 2005). and transferrin levels between the two groups. Also, children
There are till now inaccuracies in the assessment of the with iron deficiency had significantly more severe symptoms
safety of iron supplementation programs (Schümann et al. of ADHD as compared to healthy controls (OR = 1.636, p =
2007). A study by Pasricha et al. (2013a, b) showed a de- 0.031). Further longitudinal studies are needed to confirm
creased risk of anemia and iron deficiency in children aged these associations and to elucidate the potential mechanisms
4 months to 2 years who received iron supplements. However, (Tseng et al. 2018).
iron supplementation did not give a significant difference in
the mental or psychomotor development or the children’s Iron Deficiency and Autism Spectrum Disorder
height and weight gain, but it was associated with high risk
of vomiting (risk ratio 1.38, 95% CI 1.10; 1.73) and fever Iron deficiency is a frequent comorbidity in ASD due to food
(1.16, CI 1.02; 1.31). Further large randomized controlled selectivity provoked by stereotyped behavior—one of the dis-
trials with long-term follow-up are needed (Pasricha et al. order’s hallmarks (Herguner et al. 2012). On the other hand,
2013a, b). children with IDA tend to come from a less privileged socio-
economic background, which might deteriorate their ability to
seek healthcare services. Taking into account that iron is es-
Iron and Neurobehavioral Disorders sential for normal cognitive functioning, its deficiency may
prevent children from obtaining new skills and abilities and
Iron deficiency affects not only memory, attention, cognitive, make the manifestation of ASD even more severe (Schopler
and motor abilities but also emotional and psychological be- et al. 1980). In other words, the interrelation between iron
havior. The mechanisms are linked to long-term changes in deficiency and ASD appears to be bidirectional.
the metabolism of dopamine, GABA, hippocampal function Several studies attempted to describe the parameters of iron
and structure, and myelinization (Georgieff 2011; Kim and deficiency, i.e., hemoglobin concentration, ferritin, and
Wessling-Resnick 2014). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis- hematocrit, in children with ASD in comparison with
order (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are two healthy controls. Bener et al. (2017) established that ASD
of the most common neurobehavioral disorders in children. children had significantly lower (p = 0.003) mean serum iron
As reported below, recent studies have investigated the rela- level as compared with healthy controls (74.13 ± 21.61 μg/dL
tionship between these disorders and iron deficiency. and 87.59 ± 23.36 μg/dL, respectively), as well as mean he-
moglobin concentration (12.03 ± 2.13 g/dL versus 12.86 ±
Iron Deficiency and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity 2.02 g/dL, p < 0.001), ferritin level (36.57 ± 5.12 ng/mL ver-
Disorder sus 38.49 ± 5.73 ng/mL, p < 0.001), and hematocrit level
(36.32 ± 2.81 versus 39.07 ± 2.66, p < 0.001) (Bener et al.
The results of the studies about the association between iron 2017). Gunes et al. (2017) shared similar findings: all param-
deficiency and ADHD are inconsistent and contradictory eters, like hemoglobin concentration, hematocrit level, and
(Doom et al. 2015). One of the studies demonstrated that mean serum iron level were significantly (p < 0.05) lower in
ADHD is accompanied by a decrease in serum ferritin levels the children with ASD than in neurotypical controls.
(SMD = − 0.40, 95% CI − 0.66; − 0.14), although a correla- Moreover, the parameters of iron deficiency correlated with
tion between iron levels and ADHD has not been established autism severity. Children with intellectual disability had sig-
(SMD = − 0.026, 95% CI − 0.29; − 0.24) (Wang et al. 2017). nificantly lower levels (p < 0.05) of hemoglobin and hemato-
These results were supported by data from another study con- crit, while children with severe autism had significantly lower
ducted on children with ADHD. A significant association has hemoglobin concentration (Gunes et al. 2017). Although re-
been found between the severity of symptoms and serum fer- cent meta-analysis failed to confirm a significant difference in
ritin levels, as well as low levels of thalamic iron. Children serum ferritin level between ASD and neurotypical children
with sleep disorders have a higher risk to develop iron defi- (k = 4, Hedges g = 0.016, 95% CI − 0.482; 0.515, p = 0.949),
ciency. At the same time, anemia reduces the effectiveness of the prevalence of coexisting ASD and ID was significantly
J Mol Neurosci

higher than in neurotypical controls (Tseng et al. 2018). There et al. 2010; Lozoff 2011). In iron-deficient children, disorders
is a need to monitor the iron deficiency parameters in all chil- of dopamine metabolism in the mesolimbic area may lead to
dren with ASD as a part of their routine examination. alteration of behavioral, social, and emotional impairments
Sidrak et al. (2014) reported on the prevalence of iron de- (Lozoff 2011). In children with ASD, dopamine has been
ficiency in children with global developmental delay (GDD) considered a predictive biomarker (Hassan et al. 2018).
and/or ASD and established that children with GDD without Changes in the concentration of iron in the brain have ef-
ASD had lower rates of iron depletion (1.8%) and iron defi- fects on the homeostasis of γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA) and
ciency without anemia (5.5%) as compared with ASD chil- glutamate. These changes not only cause deficits in motor
dren with or without GDD, for whom the same parameters skills and learning/memory capacity but also lead to psycho-
comprised 3.0% and 7.5%, respectively. However, children logical and emotional problems (Kim and Wessling-Resnick
with GDD but no ASD had higher rates of IDA (5.5%) as 2014). An imbalance between the inhibitory neurotransmitter
compared to ASD children with or without GDD (3.0%) al- GABA and excitatory neurotransmitters leads to symptoms of
though this finding was not statistically significant (p = 0.211) ADHD (Edden et al. 2012; Gallo and Posner 2016). Research
(Sidrak et al. 2014). has shown that persons with ADHD have decreased levels of
Low iron intake during pregnancy and the period of GABA in the central nervous system and decreased iron levels
breastfeeding has a negative impact on the risk of ASD devel- in the basal ganglia (Hill 1985; Youdim et al. 1989). In the
opment. Mothers who gave birth to a child with ASD were thalamus of children with ADHD, the iron levels are signifi-
less prone to take iron-fortified foods or supplements during cantly reduced (Cortese et al. 2012a, b). Recent research
the specified period (OR = 0.63, 95% CI 0.44; 0.91) and gen- shows that raised plasma concentration of GABA can be used
erally had significantly lower daily iron intake (51.7 mg/day, as a predictor of ASD development (Al-Otaish et al. 2018).
SD 34.0) than mothers who gave birth to a child with no ASD
(57.1 mg/day, SD 36.6). As a further matter, the lowest quin- Cost-Effectiveness of Iron Supplementation and Iron
tile of iron intake during pregnancy and breastfeeding was Fortification Programs
associated with an increased risk of ASD development in
comparison with the highest quintile. Maternal iron supple- To adequately treat iron deficiency, a proper diagnosis has to
mentation may be used as a part of the preventive strategies be made, and the underlying causes need to be eliminated.
for ASD (Schmidt et al. 2014). Iron deficiency is primarily treated through nutritional im-
Iron supplementation in children with ASD may have a provement. Various food products are rich in iron, and the
beneficial effect on sleep quality. The results of an 8-week absorption of iron is highest from meat and meat products.
clinical trial with an intake of oral iron supplement in a dose However, since this category of products cannot constitute
of 6 mg/kg/day showed that the occurrence of restless sleep the entire diet, other food products rich in iron also need to
improved significantly (p = 0.04) in 29% of children with be considered. Such products include shellfish, legumes, eggs,
ASD. Nonetheless, the delayed sleep onset improved insignif- dry fruits, and some vegetables, especially spinach. Beside
icantly (35% of children as compared with 44% at baseline). this, it is important to consider the factor of iron absorption
As for the improvement of baseline irritability following treat- that might be affected by environmental factors, as well as
ment, only a trend toward positive change was observed other food products and the pH level in the duodenum
(Dosman et al. 2007). (Özdemir 2015).
The abundance of evidence exists relating to the dose and
The Effects of Iron Deficiency on Brain Biochemistry the period the iron replacement therapy is administrated. Quite
and Neurobehavioral Development often inappropriately, low iron doses are prescribed since high
doses of iron are associated with side effects which may lead
Iron is essential for the early development of the brain and is to interruption of the therapy. Whenever it is possible, oral
needed for the proper behavioral organization (Kim and iron preparations should be preferred because they are the
Wessling-Resnick 2014). Decreased level of iron impacts neu- cheapest and the oral route of administration is less invasive
rotransmitter homeostasis, and the conduction of impulses in and has fewer side effects than the parenteral route. Oral iron
cortical fibers. Iron deficiency also reduces the metabolism of treatment also is more economical than parenteral treatment.
monoamine and brain myelination (Beard 2003; Beard and The most commonly used dosage for oral iron supplementa-
Connor 2003; Kim and Wessling-Resnick 2014). tion is 3–6 mg/kg/day, which is recommended divided into
Iron is a cofactor of the enzyme tyrosine hydroxylase, one or two doses between meals. The goal is to obtain a rise
which reduces the rate of dopamine synthesis (Sachdev in the hemoglobin level by 1 g iron/day within 4 weeks
1993). A decrease in dopamine transfer accompanies iron de- (Kleinman and Greer 2011).
ficiency through frontal-striatal circuits and the mesocortical As iron deficiency has a profound effect on cognitive
pathway (Erikson et al. 2000; Beard et al. 2002; Lukowski health and psychomotor development of affected children,
J Mol Neurosci

which may last till later life, it remains an important public included cognitive improvements as potential future benefits
health problem in many developing countries as well as a (Horton and Ross 2003).
barrier to the income growth of the poor. Since children from Research has shown that the effectiveness of iron prepara-
lower socio-economic classes are vulnerable to the irrevers- tions increases due to synergistic interactions with other
ible effects of iron deficiency on cognitive functions and psy- micronutrients and vitamins (Bjørklund et al. 2017). For pre-
chomotor development, the cost-effectiveness of iron supple- vention and treatment of childhood anemia at a population
mentation to tackle this problem has to be scrutinized level in India, the policy thrust can only be done according
(Hurtado et al. 1999). Since each 1.0 g/dL decrease in hemo- to a program of iron implementation, where only 3.8–4.7% of
globin concentration results in long-lasting loss of intelligence the preschoolers receive iron-folate supplements
quotient equal to 1.73 points (Stoltzfus et al. 2004), many (Swaminathan et al. 2013). There is an urgent need for effec-
countries introduced iron supplementation and iron fortifica- tive governance and implementation of iron supplementation.
tion at a national level. Both clinical and economic impacts of A cluster-randomized controlled trial was conducted in China
these programs require careful consideration (Gould 2017). to evaluate the effects of a nutritional powder, containing iron
In a cost–benefit analysis that included 5000 simulated and other micronutrients, in the prevention/treatment of ane-
Pakistani infants at 6–12 months, Sharieff et al. (2008) mia and developmental delay in rural children aged 6–
projected the effect of home fortification with iron sprinkles 11 months. Six months after the MMP administration, the
on IQ scores and lifetime earnings. The study also estimated hemoglobin concentrations of the children had increased
the effect of zinc fortification on morbidity and mortality due (marginal effect 1.77 g/L, 95% CI 0.017–3.520, p value =
to diarrhea. After random assignment to treatment and placebo 0.048) and improved cognitive development was observed
groups for 4 months, all individuals were followed for 55 years (marginal effect 2.23 points, 95% CI 0.061–4.399, p value =
in a Markov process of time cycles, which allowed establish- 0.044). However, there was not found a significantly im-
ing that decreased rate of diarrhea and increased hemoglobin proved motor development, and 48% (721) of the children
concentrations were associated with reduced child mortality remained cognitively delayed at the end line (Luo et al.
related to diarrheal illnesses, higher IQ scores, and generally 2017). In children under 3 years of age with established
higher earnings. These findings enabled the conclusion that in IDA, a systematic review by Wang et al. (2013) showed that
a setting of the developing countries, home fortification with iron given in combination with vitamin C for 30 days did not
sprinkles can be considered a cost-effective strategy with significantly improve the psychomotor development or the
long-lasting benefits far outweighing its costs (Sharieff et al. cognitive functions of the children. Only one of these studies
2008). proved moderate evidence for cognitive improvements after
Baltussen et al. (2004) estimated the cost-effectiveness 4 months with intervention (the Bayley Mental Development
of iron supplementation and iron fortification programs in Index was 18.80, 95% CI 10.17; 27.43, p < 0.0001) (Wang
four subregions of the world (African, South American, et al. 2013). The use of multi-nutrient food supplements con-
European, and Southeast Asian) using the population taining iron or folic acid together with iron demonstrated
model designed to estimate the lifelong impact of these changes in intellectual development according to the
programs on individuals benefiting from them. Annually, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition
iron supplementation would avert 12,482 disability- (WISC-IV) (Li et al. 2015). The prophylactic administration
adjusted life years (DALYs) in the European subregion, of folic acid to pregnant women or provision of micronutrient
247,383 DALYs in South American subregion, and al- supplements within the entire period of the pregnancy resulted
most 2.5 million DALYs in the African and Southeast in 1.13 times (95% CI, 0.15–2.10) increase in the IQ scores of
Asian subregions, while iron fortification would avert their children (mean age 11.7 years). The offspring of women
8849 DALYs in the European subregion, 138,461 who received both iron and folic acid supplementation had
DALYs in South American subregion, and more than 2.03 times (95% CI, 0.61–3.45) higher scores on the verbal
1.0 million DALYs in the African and Southeast Asian comprehension index compared to the children of the untreat-
subregions (Baltussen et al. 2004). ed women. Also, the children of mothers who took supple-
In ten developing countries, the production losses due to ments with micronutrients during the pregnancy had a 2.6-fold
iron deficiency in terms of decreased physical productivity excess in their IQ scores (95% CI, 0.98–3.94) compared to
were estimated to be 0.57% of the gross domestic product children of mothers who only received supplements contain-
(GDP) (around USD 2.32 per capita), while the combined ing folic acid and iron (Zhu et al. 2018).
median physical and cognitive losses resulted in 4.05% of To estimate the lifetime health and cost consequences of
the GDP (or USD 16.78 per capita). The median benefit– IDA, Wieser et al. (2017) developed the health economic
cost ratio value for long-lasting iron fortification programs simulation model in 6- to 59-month-old Pakistani children
was assumed to be 6:1 for a cost of USD 1.33 per case of based on the data obtained from the 2012–2013 Pakistan
anemia prevented, and it raised to 36:1 when the model Demographic and Health Survey, which included a
J Mol Neurosci

representative sample of 14,000 households. The societal Concluding Remarks


costs of impaired cognitive development caused by IDA
amounted to 2148 million USD in future production losses The carried-out analysis of published papers bears witness to
(after the age of 5) and to 1392 million DALYs. The costs high global prevalence of anemia and iron deficiency among
were lower in wealthier households due to the lower preva- women of reproductive age, children, and infants, especially
lence and severity of iron deficiency and the lower number of in low-income countries. Screening for anemia has to be ini-
children (Wieser et al. 2017). tiated in 1-year-old children following the recommendations
Based on the current WHO recommendations on cost- of the American Academy of Pediatrics and the WHO (Wang
effectiveness (WHO-CHOICE), an intervention is regarded 2016). There are numerous evidences that IDA is associated
as highly cost-effective if the cost per DALY is less than one with cognitive impairment, psychomotor disorders, and be-
GDP per capita and it is viewed as cost-effective if the cost per havioral problems in children of different age. Disorders of
DALY is around one to three GDP per capita (WHO 2016), attention, emotion, intelligence, and sensory perception are
both iron fortification and iron supplementation programs the most common cognitive functions impaired by iron defi-
could be considered as cost-effective or even highly cost- ciency. These effects that can last a long period can have a
effective in most countries of the world in terms of averting negative impact on learning capability and acquisition of pro-
cognitive impairment. fessional skills (Radlowski and Johnson 2013; Jáuregui-
Figure 1 presents a schematic view of how iron deficiency Lobera 2014). The importance of iron deficiency and IDA
might affect lifetime earnings and the rationale to implement raises multiple discussions and determines the need for clini-
iron fortification and iron supplementation programs. In pop- cal and economic studies devoted to treatment and prevention
ulations with iron-deficient children, public health interven- with iron supplementation or increased intake of dietary iron
tions can improve work productivity in adulthood through the in pregnant women and children of early age (Larson et al.
iron-related neurocognitive effects. This judgment is support- 2017). Deficiency of other trace elements, like zinc, selenium,
ed by the following considerations: (i) each 10-g/L increment and copper, as well as folic acid and vitamins A and C in
in hemoglobin concentration results in a rise of IQ equal to addition to iron deficiency, plays a role in the development
1.73 points (Stoltzfus et al. 2004), (ii) this increment of IQ of cognitive impairment and psychomotor disorders. It is cru-
scores has a long-lasting effect (Campbell et al. 2001), and (iii) cial to consider the fact that iron deficiency will lead to en-
each one-point increase in IQ scores leads to a 1.1% wages hancing or suppressing absorption of other essential trace el-
growth in adult life (Alderman et al. 1996). ements. Therefore, the management of iron deficiency among

Improper Reduced IQ
cognitive Decreased
development fine motor
Impaired skills
psychomotor GDD and/or
development ASD
Growth Poor
retardation concentration
Neuro- Reduced
behavioral school
dysfunction achievements

Anemia or Cognitive and Low wages


iron neurobehavioral GDP losses
deficiency disorders DALY losses
Fig. 1 How iron deficiency anemia in childhood may lead to loss of earnings in adult life
J Mol Neurosci

high-risk populations such as pregnant mothers and young Beard JL, Connor JR (2003) Iron status and neural functioning (2003).
Annu Rev Nutr 23:41–58
children should be considered carefully to prevent any subse-
Beard JL, Erikson KM, Jones BC (2002) Neurobehavioral analysis of
quent adverse effects on health. Even though many studies developmental iron deficiency in rats. Behav Brain Res 134:517–
included in this review have a methodological difference, 524
the evidence exists that micronutrient supplementation (in- Bener A, Khattab AO, Bhugra D, Hoffmann GF (2017) Iron and vitamin
D levels among autism spectrum disorders children. Ann Afr Med
cluding iron) improves cognitive functions in children.
16:186–191. https://doi.org/10.4103/aam.aam_17_17
There is a need to carry out more high-quality prospective Bjørklund G, Aaseth J, Skalny AV, Suliburska J, Skalnaya MG,
placebo-controlled trials to develop and successfully imple- Nikonorov AA, Tinkov AA (2017) Interactions of iron with man-
ment effective intervention programs to combat nutritional ganese, zinc, chromium, and selenium as related to prophylaxis and
treatment of iron deficiency. J Trace Elem Med Biol 41:41–53
and micronutrient deficiencies, as well as to address the prob-
Bruner AB, Joffe A, Duggan AK, Casella JF, Brandt J (1996)
lems faced by the children with deficiencies of iron and trace Randomised study of cognitive effects of iron supplementation in
elements. non-anaemic iron-deficient adolescent girls. Lancet 348:992–996
Buys MC, Guerra LN, Martín B, Miranda CE, Torrejón I, Garrot T (2005)
Author Contributions All authors confirmed they have contributed to the Prevalence of anemia and iron deficiency in 12 year old school
intellectual content of this paper and have met the following three require- children from Jujuy. Medicina (B Aires) 65:126–130
ments: (a) significant contributions to the conception and design, acqui- Campbell F, Pungello E, Miller-Johnson S, Burchinal M, Ramey CT
sition of data, or analysis and interpretation of data; (b) drafting or revis- (2001) The development of cognitive and academic abilities: growth
ing the article for intellectual content; and (c) final approval of the pub- curves from an early childhood educational experiment. Dev
lished article. Psychol 37:231–242
Chmielewska A, Chmielewski G, Domellöf M, Lewandowski Z,
Szajewska H (2015) Effect of iron supplementation on psychomotor
Compliance with Ethical Standards development of non-anaemic, exclusively or predominantly
breastfed infants: a randomised, controlled trial. BMJ Open 5:
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of e009441. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009441
interest. Cortese S, Angriman M, Lecendreux M, Konofal E (2012a) Iron and
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: What is the empirical evi-
dence so far? A systematic review of the literature. Expert Rev
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to juris- Neurother 12:1227–1240. https://doi.org/10.1586/ern.12.116
dictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. Cortese S, Azoulay R, Castellanos FX, Chalard F, Lecendreux M,
Chechin D (2012b) Brain iron levels in attention-deficit/hyperactiv-
ity disorder: a pilot MRI study. World J Biol Psychiatry 13:223–231
Dommergues JP, Archambeaud MP, Ducot B, Gerval Y, Hiard C,
Rossignol C, Tchernia G (1989) Iron deficiency and psychomotor
References development tests. Longitudinal study between 10 months and 4
years of age. Arch Fr Pediatr 46:487–490
Doom JR, Georgieff MK, Gunnar MR (2015) Institutional care and iron
Akbari M, Moosazadeh M, Tabrizi R, Khatibi SR, Khodadost M, Heydari
deficiency increase ADHD symptomology and lower IQ 2.5-5 years
ST, Tahami AN, Lankarani KB (2017) Estimation of iron deficiency
post-adoption. Dev Sci 18:484–494. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.
anemia in Iranian children and adolescents: a systematic review and
12223
meta-analysis. Hematology 22:231–239. https://doi.org/10.1080/
Dosman CF, Brian JA, Drmic IE, Senthilselvan A, Harford MM, Smith
10245332.2016.1240933
RW, Sharieff W, Zlotkin SH, Moldofsky H, Roberts SW (2007)
Alderman H, Behrman J, Ross D, Sabot R (1996) The returns to endog- Children with autism: effect of iron supplementation on sleep and
enous human capital in Pakistan’s rural wage labour market. Oxf ferritin. Pediatr Neurol 36:152–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Bull Econ Stat 58:29–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0084.1996. pediatrneurol.2006.11.004
mp58001003.x Edden RA, Crocetti D, Zhu H, Gilbert DL, Mostofsky SH (2012)
Al-Otaish H, Al-Ayadhi L, Bjørklund G, Chirumbolo S, Urbina MA, El- Reduced GABA concentration in attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis-
Ansary A (2018) Relationship between absolute and relative ratios order. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69:750–753. https://doi.org/10.1001/
of glutamate, glutamine and GABA and severity of autism spectrum archgenpsychiatry.2011.2280
disorder. Metab Brain Dis 33:843–854. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Erikson KM, Jones BC, Beard JL (2000) Iron deficiency alters dopamine
s11011-018-0186-6 transporter functioning in rat striatum. J Nutr 130:2831–2837
Baltussen R, Knai C, Sharan M (2004) Iron fortification and iron supple- Falkingham M, Abdelhamid A, Curtis P, Fairweather-Tait S, Dye L,
mentation are cost-effective interventions to reduce iron deficiency Hooper L (2010) The effects of oral iron supplementation on cog-
in four subregions of the world. J Nutr 134:2678–2684. https://doi. nition in older children and adults: a systematic review and meta-
org/10.1093/jn/134.10.2678 analysis. Nutr J 9:4. https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-2891-9-4
Baumgartner J, Smuts CM, Malan L, Arnold M, Yee BK, Bianco LE, Gallo EF, Posner J (2016) Moving towards causality in attention-deficit
Boekschoten MV, Müller M, Langhans W, Hurrell RF, hyperactivity disorder: overview of neural and genetic mechanisms.
Zimmermann MB (2012) Combined deficiency of iron and (n-3) Lancet Psychiatry 3:555–567. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-
fatty acids in male rats disrupts brain monoamine metabolism and 0366(16)00096-1
produces greater memory deficits than iron deficiency or (n-3) fatty Georgieff MK (2011) Long-term brain and behavioral consequences of
acid deficiency alone. J Nutr 142:1463–1471. https://doi.org/10. early iron deficiency. Nutr Rev 69(Suppl 1):S43–S48. https://doi.
3945/jn.111.156281 org/10.1111/j.1753-4887.2011.00432.x
Beard J (2003) Iron deficiency alters brain development and functioning. Gera T, Sachdev HP, Nestel P (2009) Effect of combining multiple
J Nutr 133(5 Suppl 1):1468 micronutrients with iron supplementation on Hb response in
J Mol Neurosci

children: systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Public of three with iron deficiency anaemia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev
Health Nutr 12:75. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1368980008003145 2:CD001444
Gould JF (2017) Complementary feeding, micronutrients and develop- Low M, Farrell A, Biggs BA, Pasricha SR (2013) Effects of daily iron
mental outcomes of children. Nestle Nutr Inst Workshop Ser 87:13– supplementation in primary-school-aged children: systematic re-
28. https://doi.org/10.1159/000448934 view and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. CMAJ
Grantham-McGregor S, Ani C (2001) A review of studies on the effect of 185:E791–E802. https://doi.org/10.1503/cmaj.130628
iron deficiency on cognitive development in children. J Nutr Lozoff B (2011) Early iron deficiency has brain and behavior effects
131(2S-2):649S–666S. https://doi.org/10.1093/jn/131.2.649S consistent with dopaminergic dysfunction. J Nutr 141:740–746.
Grantham-McGregor SM, Fernald LC (1997) Nutritional deficiencies https://doi.org/10.3945/jn.110.131169
and subsequent effects on mental and behavioral development in Lozoff B, Jimenez E, Wolf AW (1991) Long-term developmental out-
children. Southeast Asian J Trop Med Public Health 28 Suppl 2: come of infants with iron deficiency. N Engl J Med 325:687–694
50–68 Lukowski AF, Koss M, Burden MJ, Jonides J, Nelson CA, Kaciroti N,
Gunes S, Ekinci O, Celik T (2017) Iron deficiency parameters in autism Jimenez E, Lozoff B (2010) Iron deficiency in infancy and
spectrum disorder: clinical correlates and associated factors. Ital J neurocognitive functioning at 19 years: evidence of long-term def-
Pediatr 43:86. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13052-017-0407-3 icits in executive function and recognition memory. Nutr Neurosci
Hassan WM, Al-Ayadhi L, Bjørklund G, Alabdali A, Chirumbolo S, El- 13:54–70. https://doi.org/10.1179/147683010X12611460763689
Ansary A (2018) The use of multi-parametric biomarker profiles Luo R, Yue A, Zhou H, Shi Y, Zhang L, Martorell R, Medina A, Rozelle
may increase the accuracy of ASD prediction. J Mol Neurosci 66: S, Sylvia S (2017) The effect of a micronutrient powder home for-
85–101. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12031-018-1136-9 tification program on anemia and cognitive outcomes among young
Herguner S, Keleşoğlu FM, Tanıdır C, Cöpür M (2012) Ferritin and iron children in rural China: a cluster randomized trial. BMC Public
levels in children with autistic disorder. Eur J Pediatr 171:143–146. Health 17(738). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-017-4755-0
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00431-011-1506-6 Matiashvili K, Manjavidze N, Ghonghadze T (2012) Influence of
Hill JM (1985) Iron concentration reduced in ventral pallidum, globus ferrotherapy on psychomotor development of children of yearly
pallidus, and substantia nigra by GABAtransaminase inhibitor, age with irondeficiency anemia. Georgian Med News 206:38–41
gamma-vinyl GABA. Brain Res 342:18–25 Muthayya S, Rah JH, Sugimoto JD, Roos FF, Kraemer K, Black RE
Hokama T, Gushi Ken M, Nosoko N (2005) Iron deficiency anaemia and (2013) The global hidden hunger indices and maps: an advocacy
child development. Asia Pac J Public Health 17:19–21 tool for action. PLoS One 8:e67860. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
Horton S, Ross J (2003) The economics of iron deficiency. Food Policy pone.0067860
28:51–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-9192(02)00070-2 Nelson M (1996) Anaemia in adolescent girls: Effects on cognitive func-
Hurtado EK, Claussen AH, Scott KG (1999) Early childhood anemia and tion and activity. Proc Nutr Soc 55(1B):359–367
mild or moderate mental retardation. Am J Clin Nutr 69:115–119. Özdemir N (2015) Iron deficiency anemia from diagnosis to treatment in
https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/69.1.115 children. Turk Pediatri Ars 50:11–19. https://doi.org/10.5152/tpa.
Iannotti LL, Tielsch JM, Black MM, Black RE (2006) Iron supplemen- 2015.2337
tation in early childhood: health benefits and risks. Am J Clin Nutr Pala E, Erguven M, Guven S, Erdogan M, Balta T (2010) Psychomotor
84:1261–1276 development in children with iron deficiency and iron-deficiency
Iglesias L, Canals J, Arija V (2018) Effects of prenatal iron status on child anemia. Food Nutr Bull 31:431–435
neurodevelopment and behavior: a systematic review. Crit Rev Food Pasricha SR, Drakesmith H, Black J, Hipgrave D, Biggs BA (2013a)
Sci Nutr 58:1604–1614. https://doi.org/10.1080/10408398.2016. Control of iron deficiency anemia in low- and middle-income coun-
1274285 tries. Blood 121:2607–2617. https://doi.org/10.1182/blood-2012-
INACG - International Nutritional Anemia Consultative Group (2000) 09-453522
INACG Symposium. 12 March 1999, Durban South Africa. ILSI Pasricha SR, Hayes E, Kalumba K, Biggs BA (2013b) Effect of daily iron
Research Foundation, Washington supplementation on health in children aged 4-23 months: a system-
Jáuregui-Lobera I (2014) Iron deficiency and cognitive functions. atic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials.
Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 10:2087–2095. https://doi.org/10.2147/ Lancet Glob Health 1:e77–e86. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2214-
NDT.S72491 109X(13)70046-9
Kassebaum NJ (2016) The global burden of anemia. Hematol Oncol Clin Petry N, Olofin I, Hurrell R, Boy E, Wirth J, Moursi M, Donahue Angel
North Am 30:247–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.hoc.2015.11.002 M, Rohner F (2016) The proportion of anemia associated with iron
Kassebaum NJ, Jasrasaria R, Naghavi M, Wulf SK, Johns N, Lozano R, deficiency in low, medium, and high human development index
Regan M, Weatherall D, Chou DP, Eisele TP, Flaxman SR, Pullan countries: a systematic analysis of national surveys. Nutrients
RL, Brooker SJ, Murray CJ (2014) A systematic analysis of global 8(11):693
anemia burden from 1990 to 2010. Blood 123:615–624. https://doi. Radlowski EC, Johnson RW (2013) Perinatal iron deficiency and
org/10.1182/blood-2013-06-508325 neurocognitive development. Front Hum Neurosci 7:585. https://
Kim J, Wessling-Resnick M (2014) Iron and mechanisms of emotional doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00585
behavior. J Nutr Biochem 25:1101–1107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Rothman M, Faber M, Covic N, Matsungo TM, Cockeran M, Kvalsvig
jnutbio.2014.07.003 JD, Smuts CM (2018) Infant development at the age of 6 months in
Kleinman RE, Greer FR (2011) Pediatric nutrition, 7th edn. American relation to feeding practices, iron status, and growth in a peri-urban
Academy of Pediatrics, Elk Grove Village community of South Africa. Nutrients 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/
Larson LM, Phiri KS, Pasricha SR (2017) Iron and cognitive develop- nu10010073
ment: what is the evidence? Ann Nutr Metab 71 Suppl 3:25–38. Sachdev P (1993) The neuropsychiatry of brain iron. J Neuropsychiatry
https://doi.org/10.1159/000480742 Clin Neurosci 5:18–29. https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.5.1.18
Li C, Zeng L, Wang D, Yang W, Dang S, Zhou J, Yan H (2015) Prenatal Sachdev H, Gera T (2013) Preventing childhood anemia in India: iron
micronutrient supplementation is not associated with intellectual supplementation and beyond. Eur J Clin Nutr 67:475–480. https://
development of young school-aged children. J Nutr 145:1844– doi.org/10.1038/ejcn.2012.212
1849. https://doi.org/10.3945/jn.114.207795 Sachdev H, Gera T, Nestel P (2005) Effect of iron supplementation on
Logan S, Martins S, Gilbert R (2001) Iron therapy for improving psycho- mental and motor development in children: systematic review of
motor development and cognitive function in children under the age randomised controlled trials. Public Health Nutr 8:117–132
J Mol Neurosci

Schmidt RJ, Tancredi DJ, Krakowiak P, Hansen RL, Ozonoff S (2014) meta-analysis. Sci Rep 8:788. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-
Maternal intake of supplemental iron and risk of autism spectrum 19096-x
disorder. Am J Epidemiol 180:890–900. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/ Vallée L (2017) Iron and neurodevelopment. Arch Pediatr 5S:5S18–
kwu208 5S22. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0929-693X(17)24005-6
Schopler E, Reichler RJ, DeVellis RF (1980) Toward objective classifi- Walter T (2003) Effect of iron-deficiency anemia on cognitive skills and
cation of childhood autism: Childhood Autism Rating Scale neuromaturation in infancy and childhood. Food Nutr Bull 24(4
(CARS). J Autism Dev Disord 10:91–103. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Suppl):S104–S110
BF02408436 Walter T, De Andraca I, Chadud P, Perales CG (1989) Iron deficiency
Schümann K, Ettle T, Szegner B, Elsenhans B, Solomons NW (2007) On anemia: adverse effects on infant psychomotor development.
risks and benefits of iron supplementation recommendations for iron Pediatrics 84:7–17
intake revisited. J Trace Elem Med Biol 21:147–168 Wang M (2016) Iron deficiency and other types of anemia in infants and
Shafir T, Angulo-Barroso R, Jing Y, Angelilli ML, Jacobson SW, Lozoff children. Am Fam Physician 93:270–278
B (2008) Iron deficiency and infant motor development. Early Hum
Wang B, Zhan S, Gong T, Lee L (2013) Iron therapy for improving
Dev 84:479–485. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.12.009
psychomotor development and cognitive function in children under
Sharieff W, Zlotkin SH, Ungar WJ, Feldman B, Krahn MD, Tomlinson G
the age of three with iron deficiency anaemia. Cochrane Database
(2008) Economics of preventing premature mortality and impaired
Syst Rev 6:CD001444. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.
cognitive development in children through home-fortification: a
CD001444.pub2
health policy perspective. Int J Technol Assess Health Care 24:
303–311. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0266462308080409 Wang Y, Huang L, Zhang L, Qu Y, Mu D (2017) Iron status in attention-
Sidrak S, Yoong T, Woolfenden S (2014) Iron deficiency in children with deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analy-
global developmental delay and autism spectrum disorder. J Paediatr sis. PLoS One 12:e0169145. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.
Child Health 50:356–361. https://doi.org/10.1111/jpc.12483 0169145
Soemantri AG, Pollitt E, Kim I (1985) Iron deficiency anemia and edu- WHO - World Health Organization (2016) Using health technology as-
cational achievement. Am J Clin Nutr 42:1221–1228 sessment for universal health coverage and reimbursement systems.
Soewondo S (1995) The effect of iron deficiency and mental stimulation World Health Organization, Geneva
on Indonesian children's cognitive performance and development. Wieser S, Brunner B, Tzogiou C, Plessow R, Zimmermann MB,
Kobe J Med Sci 41:1–17 Farebrother J, Soofi S, Bhatti Z, Ahmed I, Bhutta ZA (2017)
Stoltzfus RJ, Mullany L, Black RE (2004) Iron deficiency anaemia. In: Societal costs of micronutrient deficiencies in 6- to 59-month-old
Ezzati M, Lopez AD, Rodgers A, Murray CJL (eds) Comparative children in Pakistan. Food Nutr Bull 38:485–500. https://doi.org/10.
quantification of health risks: global and regional burden of disease 1177/0379572117720012
attributable to selected major risk factors. World Health Youdim MB, Ben-Shachar D, Yehuda S (1989) Putative biological mech-
Organization, Geneva, pp 163–209 anisms of the effect of iron deficiency on brain biochemistry and
Swaminathan S, Edward B, Kurpad A (2013) Micronutrient deficiency behavior. Am J Clin Nutr 5:319
and cognitive and physical performance in Indian children. Eur J Zhu Z, Cheng Y, Zeng L, Elhoumed M, He G, Li W, Zhang M, Li W, Li
Clin Nutr 67:467–474. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejcn.2013.14 D, Tsegaye S, Chang S, Yan H, Wang EY, Wang D, Jaffar S, Dibley
Tseng PT, Cheng YS, Yen CF, Chen YW, Stubbs B, Whiteley P, Carvalho MJ (2018) Association of antenatal micronutrient supplementation
AF, Li DJ, Chen TY, Yang WC, Tang CH, Chu CS, Yang WC, Liang with adolescent intellectual development in rural Western China: 14-
HY, Wu CK, Lin PY (2018) Peripheral iron levels in children with year follow-up from a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatr 172:
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and 832–841. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.1401

You might also like