You are on page 1of 240

The prevention of childhood anxiety in socioeconomically disadvantaged communities:

A universal school-based trial


Running head: PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

The prevention of childhood anxiety in socioeconomically disadvantaged communities:

A universal school-based trial

Jayne Stopa

University of Queensland

A dissertation submitted for the degree of Professional Doctorate in Clinical Psychology

School of Psychology - University of Queensland

Date: 17th June 2010

i
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Declaration of Originality

I declare that this dissertation is comprised of my own original work, and has not previously
been submitted for a degree or award at any other university. To the best of my knowledge,
this dissertation contains no material which has been previously published or written by
another person, except where due reference has been made within the dissertation itself.

Signed: __________________________

Date: ___________________________

ii
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Acknowledgements

I would like to express my eternal gratitude to my supervisor, Dr Paula Barrett, for


her supervision, guidance, and friendship for the duration of this research journey. Thank
you for supporting me, for sharing your knowledge, and for your enduring enthusiasm and
positive attitude towards my research.

I would also like to thank my husband, James, who has been by my side for the entire
five years of my postgraduate career. Thank you for loving me and believing in me, and for
your patience and understanding when things seemed impossible. Without your unwavering
love and support, I would never have achieved my goals.

Thank you to my parents for their unconditional love and support. You have always
worked hard to give me the best opportunity to succeed in every endeavour, and provided me
with support and encouragement to work hard and reach my goals. You have always
believed in me, and I could not have done this without you.

A big thank you to Dr Philippe Lacherez, who provided me with invaluable assistance
with the statistical aspects of my research. Thank you also to Dr Temesgen Kifle, who
helped me to solve some rather challenging problems with my data set.

I would also like to thank all those who were directly involved in various aspects of
this project, including Dr Kristy Pahl, Dr Leah Brennan, Alison Burnell, Meike Burow, and
all the staff members of Pathways Health and Research Centre. Thanks also to Dr Lara
Farrell, who provided me with a lot of support and guidance during the preliminary stages of
this research.

Special thanks must also go to Felisa Golingi, who has shared a large proportion of
this journey with me and provided me with invaluable support and assistance, and most
importantly, friendship.

Lastly, a special thank you to all the staff, students, and parents of Crestmead State
School, Burrowes State School, and Waterford West State School, who participated in this
research. It was a pleasure working with you all.

iii
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Abstract

Anxiety disorders are among the most prevalent psychiatric disorders in children and
adolescents, with up to 25% of children experiencing clinical anxiety at some point (Boyd,
Kostanski, Gullone, Ollendick, & Shek, 2000; Essau, Conradt, & Petermann, 2000; Neil &
Christensen, 2009; Tomb & Hunter, 2004). The impact of childhood anxiety is far-reaching,
resulting in compromised functioning across a range of psychosocial domains, and typically
persisting well into adolescence and adulthood (Pine, 1997). This issue is compounded by
the fact that the majority of children with anxiety disorders do not receive the treatment they
require (Esser, Schmidt, & Woemer, 1990; Hirschfeld et al., 1997; Olfson, Gameroff,
Marcus, & Waslick, 2003; Sawyer, Kosky, Graetz, Arney, Zubrick, & Baghurst, 2000).
Consequently, research efforts have prioritised the evaluation of universal interventions for
anxiety delivered within the school system, providing a more cost-effective model of
intervention. Research to date has been promising, indicating that universal school-based
prevention of anxiety is effective in reducing internalising symptoms in children and
adolescents (Neil & Christensen, 2009).

The issue of childhood anxiety is magnified in areas of socioeconomic disadvantage,


with children from these populations at an increased risk of developing anxiety disorders and
other psychiatric disorders (Kessler et al., 1994; Miech, Caspi, Moffitt, Entner Wright, &
Silva, 1999; Xue, Leventhal, Brooks-Gunn, & Earls, 2005). Children from disadvantaged
communities less likely to receive psychological assistance (Cunningham & Frieman, 1996;
Kazdin & Mazurick, 1994; Kazdin & Wassell, 1999; Misfud & Rapee, 2005), and face
significant barriers to accessing the treatment they require (Owens et al., 2002). Despite the
greater risk of anxiety faced by children living within socioeconomically disadvantaged
communities, there is a paucity of research investigating the prevention of childhood anxiety
disorders specifically within disadvantaged populations.

The primary objective of this thesis was to evaluate the effectiveness of a well-
validated cognitive-behavioural intervention for childhood anxiety, the FRIENDS for Life
program, when delivered as a universal school-based intervention within a socioeconomically
disadvantaged region. The study involved a cohort of 963 children from Grades 5, 6, and 7,
who were enrolled at one of three primary schools within this region. Children from all
schools participated in a teacher-led, manualised 12-session intervention for anxiety,

iv
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

delivered during regular classroom time within one regular school term. All participants
completed a package of self-report measures assessing levels of anxiety and depressive
symptomatology, as well as self-esteem, coping skills, and psychosocial difficulties, prior to
commencing the intervention. These measures were readministered at the completion of the
intervention, and again at 12 months post-intervention to participants in Grades 6 and 7
(children from Grades 5 and 6 during the intervention year) only.

Results indicated significant reductions in anxiety and depressive symptomatology


from pre to post-intervention, with these gains maintained at 12 months follow-up. Peer
problems and conduct problems were significantly lower both at post-intervention and again
at 12-months follow-up. Use of cognitive avoidance and behavioural avoidance strategies
also significantly decreased, whilst self-esteem and cognitive behavioural problem-solving
increased significantly over time. The findings further validate the FRIENDS for Life
program, and demonstrate it’s effectiveness as a universal school-based intervention for
children within socioeconomically disadvantaged populations. Clinical implications of these
results are discussed, along with limitations and directions for future research.

v
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Table of Contents

Declaration of originality …..……………………………………………………………ii


Acknowledgements ……………………………………………………………………….iii
Abstract ….………………………………………………………………………………..iv
Table of Contents …………………………………………………………………………vi
List of Tables …....………………………………………………………………………viii
List of Figures …..…………………………………………………………………………ix
List of Abbreviations ..…………………………………………………………………….x
CHAPTER ONE: CHILDHOOD ANXIETY DISORDERS …..………………………….1
Introduction …..……………………………………………………………………1
Anxiety in Children ……………………………………………………………………4
Epidemiology …………………………………………………………………..……5
Impact ……………………….……………………………………………………….9
CHAPTER TWO: RISK & PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR CHILDHOOD
ANXIETY ………………………………………………………………………………...14
Risk Factors …..………………………………………………………………………14
Biological Factors …...……………………………………………………………15
Psychological Factors ………………………………………………………………20
Environmental Factors ….…………………………………………………………28
Protective Factors ………….…………………………………………………………42
Child Protective Factors ..…………………………………………………………43
Family & Environmental Protective Factors ………………………………………47
CHAPTER THREE: TREATMENT OF CHILDHOOD ANXIETY DISORDERS …….51
Individual Cognitive Behaviour Therapy …………………………………………..51
Group Cognitive Behaviour Therapy ...…………………………………………….55
CHAPTER FOUR: PREVENTION OF CHILDHOOD ANXIETY DISORDERS ….…61
Levels of Prevention …………………………………………………………………..64
Indicated Prevention ………………………………………………………………69
Selective Prevention ………………………………………………………………71
Universal Prevention ………………………………………………………………73
CHAPTER FIVE: CHILDHOOD ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED
COMMUNITIES ………………………………………………………………………81
Prevalence of Childhood Psychopathology in Disadvantaged Communities .……82
Neighbourhood effects of socioeconomic disadvantage …..………………………84
Barriers to Mental Health Service Utilisation …….………………………………...87
Breaking down the barriers – School-based prevention …………………………..90
Prevention of childhood anxiety in disadvantaged communities …...……………91
The Current Study …………………………………………………………………..96
CHAPTER SIX ..…..……………………………………………………………………100
Method ……………………………………………………………………………….100

vi
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Participants ……….....……………………………………………………………100
Measures …………………………………………………………………………103
Procedure …………………………………………………………………………106
Intervention protocol ………………………..……………………………………110
CHAPTER SEVEN …..…………………………………………………………………116
Results ……...……..…………………………………………………………………116
CHAPTER EIGHT ……..………………………………………………………………130
Discussion …...………………………………………………………………………130
Clinical Implications ...……………...……………………………………………141
Limitations ……………………………...………………………………………..144
Future Directions ...……………………………………………………………….147
REFERENCES …………………………………………………………………………152
APPENDIX …...……...…………………………………………………………………212

vii
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

List of Tables

Table 1
Strengths and Weaknesses of Indicated, Selective and Universal approaches to
School-based Prevention ……………………………………………………………………67
Table 2
Measures of the SEIFA index of socioeconomic disadvantage by SLA ……………………102
Table 3
FRIENDS for Life components delivered per session …………………………………….114
Table 4
Sample size for all measures analysed, across time points ………………………...……….117
Table 5
Sample sizes for each school, by grade and gender ……………………………...……….118
Table 6
Mean and standard deviation for all clinical outcome measures at baseline ……………119
Table 7
Mean and standard deviation for all measures at pre-intervention, post-intervention
and follow-up …………………………………………………………………………123
Table 8
Means and standard deviations for difference between pre-intervention and
post-intervention scores ………………………………………………………………….125
Table 9
Pearson bivariate correlations between psychosocial predictors and difference
scores in outcome ……………………………………………………………………….128

viii
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

List of Figures

Figure 1
Variables used to calculate the index of relative socioeconomic disadvantage …………..101
Figure 2
Change in risk status over time ………………………………………..…………………112

ix
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

List of Abbreviations

ADHD Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder


CBT Cognitive Behavioural Therapy
CBT+FAM Cognitive Behavioural Therapy + Family Component
CBT+PAM Cognitive Behavioural Therapy + Parental Anxiety Management
CD Conduct Disorder
CDI Children’s Depression Inventory
CSCY Coping Scale for Children and Youth
DSM Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
DSM-III Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
DSM-III-R Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,
Edition 3, revised
DSM IV Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,
Edition 4
DSM IV-TR Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,
Edition 4, Revised
FCBT Family Cognitive Behaviour Therapy
FESA Family-based Education, Support, and Attention
FGCBT Family Group Cognitive Behaviour Therapy
GAD Generalised Anxiety Disorder
GCBT Group Cognitive Behaviour Therapy
ICBT Individual Cognitive Behaviour Therapy
OAD Overanxious Disorder
OCD Obsessive Compulsive Disorder
ODD Oppositional Defiant Disorder
PD Panic Disorder
PTSD Post Traumatic Stress Disorder
RCMAS Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale
SAD Separation Anxiety Disorder
SCAS Spence Child Anxiety Scale
SD Standard Deviation
SDQ Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire
SEI Self Esteem Inventory
SES Socioeconomic Status
SOP Social Phobia
SP Specific Phobia

x
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

CHAPTER ONE: CHILDHOOD ANXIETY DISORDERS

Childhood anxiety disorders are among the most prevalent forms of childhood

psychopathology, with serious immediate and prevailing implications for sufferers, their

families, and society at large (Donovan & Spence, 2000). Children with anxiety disorders are

more likely to have difficulties with social functioning, low academic achievement, and low

self-esteem, among other indicators of poor psychosocial adjustment (Pine, 1997). In light of

such factors, early research efforts have prioritised the development of interventions for

childhood anxiety, both at the individual and, more recently, the group level, with promising

results. Despite ongoing refinement and evaluations of such interventions, the fact remains

that most children with anxiety disorders do not receive the treatment they require (Esser,

Schmidt, & Woemer, 1990; Hirschfeld et al., 1997; Olfson, Gameroff, Marcus, & Waslick,

2003; Sawyer et al., 2000), whilst many of those who do either terminate prematurely

(Kazdin, 1996), or continue to experience ongoing difficulties (Barrett, Dadds, & Rapee,

1996; Donovan & Spence, 2000; Last, Perrin, Hersen, & Kazdin, 1996).

The poor utilisation and response to treatment mentioned above suggests that

addressing established disorders is not the most optimal intervention model for childhood

anxiety (Barrett & Turner, 2001). As such, research interests have shifted to prevention as a

more timely and effective method to reduce the burden of childhood anxiety on the wider

community. To address issues of accessibility, many researchers have focused on the

delivery of preventative interventions for anxiety through the school system, overcoming

many barriers which have typically prevented families from accessing individual treatment

(McLoone, Hudson, & Rapee, 2006; Owens et al., 2002). Empirical evaluation of such

interventions has yielded positive results, with significant growth in the literature validating

1
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

the use of universal school-based prevention for childhood anxiety across different ages and

developmental levels, both within Australia and internationally (Neil & Christensen, 2009).

Whilst preventative interventions for anxiety have been validated extensively in

private schools and public schools from more affluent catchments, little research has focused

exclusively on the prevention of childhood anxiety disorders in communities of recognised

low socioeconomic status (SES). This is despite the fact that mental illness is

overrepresented in socioeconomically disadvantaged communities, with both adults and

children from low SES communities significantly more likely to experience psychological

distress (Buckner & Bassuk, 1997; Drukker, Kaplan, Feron, & van Os, 2003), as well as other

health problems (Curtis, Dooley, Lipman, & Feeny, 2001). These communities face a unique

set of difficulties, including higher rates of poverty, unemployment, familial instability, and

low education, culminating in an increased risk for anxiety. Whilst research within low SES

populations understandably presents greater methodological challenges, it is unfortunate that

researchers have largely neglected this at-risk population, who arguably stand to benefit the

most from such interventions in the long term.

The objective of the present doctoral thesis is to expand upon the body of literature

examining the prevention and treatment of childhood anxiety, focusing specifically upon the

issue of childhood anxiety within socioeconomically disadvantaged communities. The

primary goal of this study is to investigate the effectiveness of a universal school-based

intervention for childhood anxiety, delivered to upper primary school-aged children from

three public schools located within a recognised low SES region. Specifically, it seeks to

examine both the immediate and long-term effects of the intervention, not only in terms of

the primary outcome measure of anxiety symptomatology, but additionally depressive

2
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

symptomatology, self-esteem, and coping skills. It is hoped that the findings of this research

advance the argument for standardised, curriculum-based anxiety intervention and prevention

programs in these populations, which have thus far been neglected in both research and

practice.

The aim of the current chapter is to provide an overview of anxiety in school-aged

children, through investigating prevalence rates, epidemiology and comorbidity of childhood

anxiety disorders. It also provides a synopsis of both the immediate and longitudinal impact

of childhood anxiety, in terms of psychological wellbeing and psychosocial functioning.

Chapter Two provides a review of recognised risk factors for childhood anxiety, followed by

an overview of protective factors noted within the literature. Chapter Three offers a review

of the literature on treatment for childhood anxiety disorders, focusing primarily on the

development of cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) as an individual therapy for anxiety, to

the progression towards group-based CBT for childhood anxiety. Chapter Four provides a

comprehensive review of the shift from treatment to prevention of childhood anxiety. It

examines three different levels of prevention, and reviews available prevention literature at

each of the three levels, focusing particularly on universal prevention delivered in the school

setting. Chapter Five is concerned specifically with the association between psychopathology

and socioeconomic disadvantage. It also reviews barriers to mental health services prevalent

in low SES populations, prior to providing an overview of the rationale and hypotheses of the

current study. The methodology for the current research is presented in Chapter Six, with the

results and discussion covered in Chapter Seven and Chapter Eight respectively.

3
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Anxiety in Children

Anxiety is a normal and evolutionarily adaptive emotional response to a real or

imagined threat, designed to aid in survival and the avoidance of danger. Anxiety responses

involve three primary components: physiological (e.g. heightened autonomic responses),

cognitive (e.g. threat perception), and behavioural (e.g. avoidance) (Essau & Peterman,

2001). All children experience anxiety as part of their normal social and emotional

development, and sources of anxiety in childhood and adolescence vary as a function of

developmental stage (Ollendick, King, & Yule, 1994). For example, anxiety in infants and

younger children may occur in response to separation from the attachment figure, whilst in

later childhood and adolescence, anxiety is more likely to be triggered by challenging

situations, social fears, and academic stressors (Warren & Sroufe, 2004). In essence,

childhood fears generally shift from concrete, external stimuli, to internalised and abstract

stressors across the lifespan (Koplewicz, 1996), which reflects the varying range of

experiences to which children are exposed over time. It is therefore important to be mindful

of normative anxiety contexts and developmentally appropriate fear in assessing anxiety in

children (Ollendick et al., 1994), and to acknowledge that the experience of anxiety does not

necessarily constitute disordered functioning.

Whilst anxiety responses are part of the normal childhood experience, these responses

become problematic when they persistently occur in response to an unreasonable perception

of threat, and at an intensity that is disproportionate to the objective threat (Essau &

Peterman, 2001). In this way, anxiety ceases to be adaptive, resulting in functional

impairment and interfering with aspects of everyday life. Diagnostically, the degree and

duration of interference in the context of the stressor may be indicative of clinical anxiety.

The hallmark feature of anxiety disorders in children is worry, which may manifest as overt

4
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

distress, or more implicitly through poor concentration and irritability. Behavioural

avoidance of anxiety-provoking stimuli is also common, in conjunction with a range of

somatic symptoms, including restlessness, headaches, fatigue, appetite loss, muscle tension,

nausea and vomiting, enuresis, and sleep disturbances (Essau & Peterman, 2001). Anxiety

disorders can therefore result in significant impairment in everyday functioning across social,

emotional, academic, and vocational domains.

It has been little over two decades since researchers first started to extensively foray

into the nebulous area of childhood anxiety; indeed prior to this, it was believed that children

lacked the cognitive capabilities to experience internalising disorders (Essau & Peterman,

2001). The current empirical understanding of childhood anxiety however places it alongside

adult anxiety in terms of basic phenomenology (Laurent & Ettelson, 2001; Turner & Barrett,

2003). Accordingly, the field is now afforded significant research attention, which more

accurately reflects the magnitude of the issue in child populations. The following section

provides an epidemiological overview of childhood anxiety disorders, and reviews the impact

of childhood anxiety upon other spheres of psychosocial functioning.

Epidemiology

Anxiety disorders are consistently cited as the most common type of disorders in

childhood (Beidel, 1991; Costello, Egger, & Angold, 2005; Essau, Conradt, & Petermann,

2000; Kashani & Orvaschel, 1988; Messer & Beidel, 1994). The most recently reported

prevalence rates range between 4% and 25% (Boyd, Kostanski, Gullone, Ollendick, & Shek,

2000; Essau et al., 2000; Neil & Christensen, 2009; Tomb & Hunter, 2004), with up to 28.8%

of children developing an anxiety disorder during their lifetime (Kessler, Berglund, Demler,

Jin, & Walters, 2005). Differences between reported prevalence rates are due to

5
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

methodological variations, including changes to diagnostic criteria over time, clinical cut-offs

used, diagnostic measures and sampling methods employed. Prevalence rates also vary

according to developmental stage; some disorders, such as separation anxiety disorder (SAD)

and specific phobia (SP), are more common in younger children, whilst others such as social

phobia (SOP) and panic disorder (PD), are more prevalent among adolescent samples.

Costello, Mustillo, Erkanli, Keeler, and Angold (2003) report that, collapsed across

diagnosis, prevalence rates tend to peak around the age of 9 to 10 years. Anxiety disorder

prevalence rates are typically twice as high for girls than for boys (Lewinsohn, Gotlib,

Lewinsohn, Seeley, & Allen, 1998). Rather than indicating a genuine predisposition for

anxiety in females, it has been argued that these prevalence rates may reflect the fact that

girls are more likely to report worries, fears, and symptoms of anxiety than boys (Bell-Dolan,

Last, & Strauss, 1990; Essau & Peterman, 2001; Muris, Meesters, Merckelbach, Sermon, &

Zwakhalen, 1998; Silverman & Treffers, 2001; Silverman, Greca, & Wasserstein, 1995).

The course of childhood anxiety disorders is typically chronic, and in many cases,

childhood anxiety is a precursor for long-term clinical presentations well into adolescence

and adulthood (Cartwright-Hatton, McNicol, & Doubleday, 2006). Anxiety disorders are

particularly robust if left untreated, and rarely remit without intervention (Costello, Egger, &

Angold, 2004). One study of a group of children aged from 6 to 19 years found that on

average, those with untreated anxiety disorders had experienced clinical difficulties for 4

years, with 30% of those who had recovered experiencing a recurrence of anxiety (Keller,

Lavori, Wunder, Beardslee, Schwartz, & Roth, 1992). Even where treatment is procured,

between 30% and 40% of children may continue to meet diagnostic criteria for an anxiety

disorder post-intervention (Barrett, Rapee, Dadds, & Ryan, 1996; Kendall, 1994). For those

who do successfully respond to treatment, recurrences later in life are common, with some

6
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

children going on to develop new anxiety disorders as well as other types of psychological

disorders (Last et al., 1996). Anxiety disorders in childhood are also predictive of

disturbances in later life such as substance abuse (Kessler et al., 1996), suicidality, and

personality psychopathology (Rudd, Joiner, & Rumzek, 2004), and confer a significant risk

for both concurrent and successive comorbidity with a range of other disorders, both in

community and clinical populations.

Childhood anxiety disorders have high rates of comorbidity with many internalising

and externalising disorders (Angold, Costello, & Erkanli, 1999; Brady & Kendall, 1992;

Essau & Peterman, 2001), with reported comorbidity rates for childhood anxiety disorders in

community samples of up to 39% (Anderson, Williams, MgGee, & Silva, 1987; Kashani &

Orvaschel, 1990). Anxiety disorders are commonly comorbid with each other during

childhood, and a high percentage of children with an anxiety disorder diagnosis also meet

criteria for a second, concurrent disorder. Comorbidity in generalised anxiety disorder

(GAD), the most frequently diagnosed childhood anxiety disorder, is very common. A study

involving a community sample found that up to 50% of children with overanxious disorder

(OAD; later subsumed under the new diagnosis of GAD) also met criteria for SAD (Kashani

& Orvaschel, 1990). Similarly, 46% of children diagnosed with SAD also fulfilled criteria

for SOP (Kashani & Orvaschel, 1990). The significant rates of comorbidity between other

pairs and clusters of childhood anxiety disorders have been discussed in detail elsewhere

(Curry, March, & Hervey, 2004). Clearly, comorbidity of anxiety disorders is a common

problem, resulting in a more complex constellation of difficulties for anxious children.

The link between childhood anxiety disorders and depressive disorders has also been

well established. Anxiety disorders are more frequently concurrently comorbid with

7
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

depressive disorders than with either attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),

conduct disorder (CD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), or substance abuse disorders

(Costello et al., 2003; Curry et al., 2004; Verduin & Kendall, 2003). A comprehensive

review of comorbidity between the two disorder clusters reported rates of comorbid

depression ranging between 5% and 69% in children and adolescents with anxiety disorders

(Angold et al., 1999). Children and adolescents with clinical anxiety were found to be 8.2

times more likely to meet criteria for a depressive disorder than children without clinical

anxiety (Angold et al., 1999; Costello et al., 2005). Similarly, rates of comorbid anxiety in

children and adolescents with depression have been reported to range up to 75% (Angold et

al., 1999), indicating the high degree of overlap between the two disorders.

The negative impact of comorbid anxiety and depression in childhood is typically

greater than that of either disorder alone, with higher levels of psychopathology, severity of

symptoms, and greater functional impairment all associated with comorbidity (Masi et al.,

2004; Seligman & Ollendick, 1998). Further to this, comorbid anxiety and depression may

constitute an increased risk of suicide in young people (Cole, Peeke, Martin, Truglio, &

Seroczynski, 1998), given that anxiety and depression are risk factors for suicide in their own

right. Additionally, research demonstrates that up to 84% of child and adolescent suicide

victims may suffer from comorbid mental disorders (Shafii, Steltz-Lenarsky, Derrick,

Beckner, & Whittinghill, 1988). With regard to psychosocial functioning, children with

comorbid anxiety and depression are at a distinct disadvantage. In research conducted by

(Masi, Favilla, Mucci, & Millepiedi, 2000) involving children aged between 8 and 18 years,

those with comorbid anxiety and depression had higher levels of functional impairment and

severity of anxiety of symptoms, than those with anxiety alone.

8
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Comorbidity of anxiety and depressive disorders is not always concurrent; a temporal

relationship between anxiety and depression has been found for both clinical and community

populations (Boyd & Gullone, 1997; Brady & Kendall, 1992; Cole et al., 1998; Orvaschel,

Lewinsohn, & Seely, 1995; Pine, Cohen, Gurley, Brook, & Ma, 1998; Seligman & Ollendick,

1998). There is a successive pattern of comorbidity between the two disorders, whereby

anxiety disorders in children may longitudinally predict the development of depressive

disorders in adolescence and at later stages of the life course (Angold, Costello, &

Worthman, 1998; Hankin et al., 1998; McGee, Feeham, Williams, & Anderson, 1992;

Silberg, Rutter, & Eaves, 2001; Velez, Johnson, & Cohen, 1989). A longitudinal study by

Kovacs, Gatsonis, Paulauskas and Richards (1989) found that in children with comorbid

depression and anxiety, the latter preceded depression in two-thirds of cases. This is

consistent with later longitudinal research by Orvaschel et al., (1995), who found that

approximately 64% of adolescents with diagnosable anxiety went on to develop a secondary

diagnosis of major depressive disorder. More support has been found through research

demonstrating anxiety symptomatology in children predicts higher levels of depressive

symptoms later in adolescence (Cole et al., 1998), and that children with comorbid anxiety

and depression tend to be older than children with anxiety alone (Strauss, Lease, Last, &

Francis, 1988).

Impact

The impact of childhood anxiety disorders on the individual can be both pervasive

and far-reaching, affecting many aspects of social functioning over time. In school-aged

children, anxiety has been shown to predict below average academic achievement (McLoone

et al., 2006), with longitudinal research demonstrating the link between anxiety and poor

school performance. Ialongo, Edelsohn, Werthamer-Larsson, Crockett, and Kellam (1995)

9
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

found that first grade students who suffered from high levels of anxiety symptomatology

were more likely to achieve lower scores for reading and mathematics by the time they

reached Grade 5, as well as experiencing ongoing difficulties with anxiety. More recently, a

longitudinal study of adolescents in New Zealand found that those with anxiety disorders

were more prone to educational underachievement in young adulthood (Woodward &

Fergusson, 2001). The tendency of anxious children to underachieve at school may be in part

due an increased risk of school avoidance, particularly for children with SOP or SAD (Beidel

& Turner, 1988). Accordingly, anxious children may also be more likely to leave school

prior to Grade 12, which may limit future vocational options (Donovan & Spence, 2000;

Rapee, Kennedy, Ingram, Edwards, & Sweeney, 2005), further indicating how the impact of

childhood anxiety may continue to resonate over time.

Another aspect of functioning which is compromised by childhood anxiety is that of

social competence, encompassing social skills and self-esteem. It is known that anxious

children are more likely to display a behaviourally inhibited temperament (see Chapter 2:

Risk Factors, for a more comprehensive discussion), typified by shyness, and reticence or

withdrawal in novel situations, which can have a detrimental effect on social development

(Ialongo, Edelsohn, Werthamer-Larsson, Crockett, & Kellam, 1994). Broadly speaking,

anxious children are more likely to have social skills deficits. Spence, Donovan, and

Brechman-Toussaint (1999) found that compared to a control group, children with diagnosed

SOP were rated by their parents as less socially skilled. These children typically interacted

less with peers, initiated fewer interactions, and were less likely to receive positive outcomes

from peers during school-based interactions. Socially anxious children are also less likely to

be accepted by their peers (Greco & Morris, 2005), which may increase the risk for more

entrenched internalising behaviour (Ollendick, Weist, Borden, & Greene, 1992; Strauss,

10
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Frame, & Forehand, 1987; Strauss, Lahey, Frick, Frame, & Hynd, 1988), and lead to further

interpersonal difficulties and social isolation (Beidel & Turner, 1988; King & Ollendick,

1989; Messer & Beidel, 1994; Pine, 1997). Not surprisingly, children with anxiety are also

more likely to have low self-esteem than their non-anxious peers (Ialongo, Edelsohn,

Werthamer-Larsson, Crockett, & Kellam, 1996; McLoone et al., 2006; Rapee et al., 2005;

Strauss et al., 1987). This may be a risk factor for withdrawal, potentially leading to the

reinforcement and exacerbation of existing anxiety.

In conjunction with the impact on social and academic functioning, the diagnosis of

childhood anxiety confers a significant risk for later psychopathology and poorer functional

outcomes. Both prospective and retrospective studies have demonstrated that childhood

anxiety often precedes anxiety disorders in adulthood (Aronson & Logue, 1987; Lipsitz et al.,

1994; Otto, Pollock, Rosenbaum, Sachs, & Asher, 1994; Pine et al., 1998), and adults with

anxiety disorders who had previously suffered from childhood anxiety may experience more

severe difficulties than anxious adults without a history of childhood anxiety (Otto et al.,

1994). Similarly, anxiety disorders in childhood are predictive of depression in later

adolescence and adulthood (Woodward & Fergusson, 2001). Childhood anxiety has also

been identified as a risk factor for substance abuse (Clark & Winters, 2002; Costello et al.,

2003; McLoone et al., 2006; Rapee et al., 2005; Woodward & Fergusson, 2001), with

research suggesting that some anxiety disorders are more associated with substance use

disorders than others. Clark and Neighbors (1996) note that SOP and PD during adolescence

are linked to substance use disorders, whilst Deykin and Buka (1997) found a lifetime

prevalence rate of 30% for post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), in a population of

adolescents with diagnosed drug or alcohol dependence. Other poorer functional outcomes

associated with childhood anxiety include conduct problems, personality psychopathology,

11
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

and increased use of medical and psychiatric services (Last, Hansen, & Franco, 1997; Rudd

et al., 2004; Weissman et al., 1999).

Summary

This chapter has provided a brief overview of the issue of childhood anxiety. A

review of the literature has revealed that anxiety disorders are the most common category of

childhood psychiatric disorders, with a chronic course that typically extends into adolescence

and adulthood if left untreated. Issues relating to comorbidity were also discussed,

highlighting the increased risk of more entrenched internalising psychopathology in children

with anxiety. The long-term effects of anxiety upon other psychosocial aspects of life have

been shown to be highly detrimental, and associated with such factors as significantly lower

self-esteem, poor social skills, and impaired interpersonal functioning. On a practical level,

anxiety has also been associated with negative outcomes such as educational

underachievement and vocational limitations.

It is irrefutable that anxiety is a significant mental health problem in childhood, both

in terms of direct morbidity, and the associated risks it presents to psychosocial functioning

across the life course. When considered in conjunction with the prevalence rates reported in

the literature, the magnitude of the impact of childhood anxiety at a macro level is especially

concerning, and highlights the real need for effective and timely intervention strategies.

The effectiveness of interventions for anxiety in children relies heavily on a thorough

understanding of the risk factors implicated in the development of childhood anxiety, and

how these may predispose some individuals to the development of the disorder. This

knowledge is complimented by an understanding of protective factors which may buffer the

12
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

child against risk factors for anxiety, and an understanding of how these can be built upon

and enhanced through strategic interventions. The following chapter reviews the literature

regarding risk and protective factors for childhood anxiety.

13
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

CHAPTER TWO: RISK & PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR CHILDHOOD ANXIETY

In considering the aetiology of anxiety, it is important to recognise the role that risk

and protective factors play in the likelihood of developing an anxiety disorder in childhood.

A better understanding of these factors more fully informs intervention strategies and

preventative programs. The following chapter reviews a range of both risk and protective

factors that are heavily implicated in childhood anxiety.

Risk Factors for Childhood Anxiety

Research into the aetiology of childhood anxiety disorders has yielded a number of

variables which are heavily associated with the presence of a disorder. The presence of these

variables, or risk factors, may predict the onset, severity, and duration of the disorder (Coie et

al., 1993). Risk factors can include variables specific to the individual, or those that operate

more broadly across a cultural level. Several categories of risk factors for anxiety have been

identified, including biological, psychological, and environmental (Donovan & Spence,

2000). Whilst strong correlations between anxiety and a range of risk factors have been

identified, these relationships are complex, and not necessarily causal. Risk factors are

dynamic, and their presence may fluctuate over time. In some cases, anxiety may precede the

presence of significant risk factors (Donovan & Spence, 2000). Furthermore, the effect of

risk factors may be developmentally sensitive, and their influence may vary depending on the

developmental stage of the individual (Coie et al., 1993; Mrazek & Haggerty, 1994). Whilst

the presence of risk factors does not always precipitate the development of anxiety, a higher

number of risk factors generally increases vulnerability, and may predict a more severe

illness course (Coie et al., 1993; Mrazek & Haggerty, 1994). This section discusses anxiety

risk factors across each of the three categories mentioned above. It will commence with a

14
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

discussion of biological risk factors, followed by psychological factors, and conclude with

environmental risk factors.

Biological Factors

Biological risk factors for anxiety are those which are thought to be innate from birth,

or inherited. One such factor is temperament, which is theorised to have a genetic component

(Donovan & Spence, 2000). Individuals with certain temperament characteristics are at an

increased risk for the development of anxiety disorders, and the temperament style that is

arguably most strongly associated with anxiety is behavioural inhibition (Biederman et al.,

1993; DiLalla, Kagan, & Reznick, 1994; Kagan, 1997; Kagan, Reznick, & Gibbons, 1989;

Kagan, Snidman, Zentner, & Peterson, 1999; Rosenbaum et al., 1993). The role of

behavioural inhibition as a risk factor for anxiety is discussed below.

Behavioural inhibition. Behavioural inhibition, which occurs in approximately 15%

of children (Asendorpf, 1990; Hirschfeld-Becker et al., 2008; Kagan, 1997) is manifested by

timidness, shyness, and avoidance behaviours in novel contexts, and increased physiological

arousal in response to these contexts (Biederman et al., 1993; Hirschfeld-Becker et al., 2008).

As behavioural inhibition is a coping mechanism which reduces fear and anxiety, the

behaviours of inhibited children become further entrenched through reinforcement, which can

lead to more severe anxiety difficulties and other psychopathology in the future (Biederman,

Hirschfeld-Becker et al., 2001; Gar, Hudson, & Rapee, 2005; Gladstone & Parker, 2006;

Gladstone, Parker, Mitchell, Wilhelm, & Mahli, 2005; Kagan et al., 1999; Rosenbaum et al.,

1991; Schofield, Coles, & Gibb, 2009).

15
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Pioneering research in the area of behavioural inhibition was conducted by Kagan and

colleagues (Garcia-Coll, Kagan, & Reznick, 1984; Kagan et al., 1989; Kagan, Reznick,

Clarke, Snidman, & Garcia-Coll, 1984; Kagan, Reznick, & Snidman, 1987; Kagan, Reznick,

Snidman, Gibbons, & Johnson, 1988), who assessed behavioural inhibition in two groups of

infants (aged 21 months and 31 months at the outset) over a 6-year period. The children were

exposed to novel stimuli, including unfamiliar situations, people, and objects, in a laboratory

setting, and their behaviours were observed by researchers. Those children who displayed a

behaviourally inhibited temperament were significantly more likely to be fearful of the novel

stimuli and to demonstrate avoidance behaviours. By comparison, children with an

uninhibited temperament were significantly less distressed by the novel stimuli, being more

likely to engage in social and spontaneous behaviours. Importantly though, behavioural

inhibition did not remain stable for all children over time. It was found that only infants who

were extremely behaviourally inhibited were more likely to remain so into childhood,

whereas those infants who were found to be extremely uninhibited were more likely to

remain uninhibited into childhood (Kagan et al., 1989). These findings indicate that

behavioural inhibition, particularly at this extreme end of the scale, may be a distinct

vulnerability for later difficulties with anxiety.

Later research by Biederman et al. (1990) revealed a link between behavioural

inhibition and childhood anxiety disorders. The study investigated behavioural inhibition and

psychopathology in children of parents with PD and agoraphobia, and children of parents

with other psychiatric disorders. It was found that the children of parents with PD and

agoraphobia were significantly more likely to be identified as behaviourally inhibited than

children of parents with other disorders. Furthermore, children in the behaviourally inhibited

group were also more likely to meet diagnostic criteria for an anxiety disorder, than children

16
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

in the uninhibited group. Biederman et al. (1990) also investigated rates of psychopathology

in the original sample examined by Garcia-Coll et al. (1984); whilst children who had been

identified as behaviourally inhibited infants were more likely to meet criteria for an anxiety

disorder, the difference in rates of disorders between the two dichotomised groups was not

significant. By comparison, results from a three-year follow-up of Biederman et al.'s (1990)

original sample demonstrated that children identified as behaviourally inhibited were

significantly more likely to meet diagnostic criteria for avoidant disorder (now known under

the diagnosis of SOP), SAD, and agoraphobia, than uninhibited children (Biederman et al.,

1993).

More recent research with the original sample has also supported the above findings.

Longitudinal research by Kagan et al. (1999) investigated the link between behaviours

associated with behavioural inhibition in 164 infants aged 4 months, and anxiety symptoms in

later childhood. It was found that infants identified as highly reactive were more likely to

display anxious symptomatology as 7-year-olds, based on parent-report measures. Despite

this finding, it is important to note that of those infants originally identified as highly

reactive, fewer than 10% went on to develop anxiety symptoms by age 7. Similarly, recent

longitudinal research by Hirschfeld-Becker et al. (2007) found that whilst behaviourally

inhibited temperament in preschool-aged children constituted a significant risk for social

anxiety five years later, most children with early behavioural inhibition did not go on to

develop significant anxiety difficulties in middle childhood. This finding indicates that the

relationship between behavioural inhibition and anxiety disorders is by no means causal, but

is likely mediated by several other factors.

17
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Whilst much of research in this field has been concerned with several key cohorts,

suggesting caution in interpretation of results, similar findings have resulted from research

with different data sets. One such example is a large scale longitudinal study of Australian

children, with data collected at eight time points from infancy to early adolescence (Prior,

Smart, Sanson, & Oberklaid, 2000). Prospectively, inhibited temperament during infancy

was associated with anxiety problems in later childhood. This relationship was strongest for

children rated as inhibited on at least six of the eight assessment occasions, with 42%

experiencing anxiety difficulties based on parent report measures. Retrospectively however,

47% of children with parent-reported anxiety difficulties in adolescence had either rarely or

never been identified as inhibited across the duration of the study. Again, this research

suggests that whilst behavioural inhibition is implicated in the development of anxiety

disorders in later childhood, the relationship between the two is likely influenced by a

complex interplay of other factors.

It is worth drawing attention here to methodological difference between studies

investigating the relationship between behavioural inhibition and anxiety disorders in

children. Whilst research reviewed to date has produced findings based on longitudinal data

collected from parent report measures (Hirschfeld-Becker et al., 2007; Kagan et al., 1989;

Kagan et al., 1988; Kagan et al., 1999; Prior et al., 2000), other researchers have established

this link using adult participants’ retrospective reports of their own functioning as children

(Gladstone et al., 2005; Reznick, Hegeman, Kaufman, Woods, & Jacobs, 1992; Schofield et

al., 2009). Gladstone et al. (2005) gathered data from both retrospective reports of childhood

inhibition and a measure of adult behavioural inhibition from 189 adults, aged between 17

and 68 years. Participants were also assessed for anxiety and depression using structured

diagnostic interviews. Participants were classified into either the “low”, “medium”, or “high”

18
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

inhibition group, based on both retrospective and current measures of behavioural inhibition.

Significant between-group differences were noted with regard to anxiety disorder diagnoses.

Specifically, participants in the high retrospective inhibition group were more likely to meet

criteria for SOP or SP, and were also more likely to have experienced multiple anxiety

disorders. This research therefore successfully used retrospective reports to further establish

behavioural inhibition as a risk factor for anxiety.

Most recently, another retrospective study by Schofield et al. (2009) examined links

between behavioural inhibition in early childhood, and symptoms of social anxiety,

depression and anxious arousal in young adults. Two hundred and forty-seven undergraduate

students completed self-report measures on retrospective behavioural inhibition, and anxious

and depressive symptomatology. Consistent with the earlier study (Gladstone et al., 2005),

retrospective reports of behavioural inhibition during early childhood were significantly

linked to social anxiety and anxious arousal symptoms in young adulthood. More

specifically, and as predicted, the social component of behavioural inhibition (typified by

reticence to interact with strangers) was more strongly associated with social anxiety than

with the non-social component of behavioural inhibition (characterised by reluctance to

explore unfamiliar surroundings, and fearful responses when exposed to potentially

threatening situations). Another interesting finding was that retrospective reports of

behavioural inhibition were also associated with depressive symptomatology, and the

relationship between these two was mediated by anxiety. These findings, consistent with

earlier research (Gladstone & Parker, 2006), suggest that not only are inhibited children at

greater risk of developing both anxiety and depressive disorders in later life, but that anxiety

is heavily implicated in the later development of depressive disorders in children identified as

behaviourally inhibited.

19
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

The research reviewed above provides considerable evidence for the predictive utility

of behavioural inhibition as a risk factor for anxiety not only in childhood, but well into

adolescence and adulthood as well. The relationship between behavioural inhibition and

anxiety has been established using both longitudinal data (Hirschfeld-Becker et al., 2007;

Kagan et al., 1989; Kagan et al., 1988; Kagan et al., 1999; Prior et al., 2000) and

retrospective reports of childhood inhibited behaviour (Gladstone et al., 2005; Reznick et al.,

1992; Schofield et al., 2009). The fact that this relationship has been demonstrated

successfully using two differing methodologies signifies that the relationship between the two

factors is robust, and may therefore be very useful in a clinical setting.

Psychological Factors

Research into correlates of anxiety disorders in children has revealed several

cognitive factors that are associated with childhood anxiety (Craig & Dobson, 1995; Muris &

Field, 2008; Prins, 2001). Such cognitive factors are linked to the tendency of anxious

children to disproportionately attend to threat, overestimate threat, underestimate their own

personal coping and social skills (Cartwright-Hatton, Tschernitz, & Gomersall, 2005) and

experience distorted and maladaptive cognitions (Craig & Dobson, 1995; Silverman &

Treffers, 2001). The link between cognition and anxiety has been explained by cognitive

theory, whereby schemas of danger result in prioritised processing of threatening information

and erroneous processing of ambiguous situations, leading to cognitive distortions and

apprehensive behaviours which reinforce anxiety (Kendall, 1985). The literature in this field

is extensive, and it is not viable to discuss all cognitive factors linked to anxiety here. As

such, this section will review evidence for two of the most empirically supported variables:

20
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

attention bias and interpretation bias, each of which will be discussed in the context of

childhood anxiety disorders below.

Attention bias. Significant links have been identified between attention bias and

anxiety disorders, with evidence suggesting this factor may be fundamental in the

development and maintenance of clinical anxiety (Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-

Kranenburg, & van Yzendoorn, 2007; Muris & Field, 2008). A recent meta-analysis of

attention bias research demonstrated that attention is disproportionately allocated to

threatening stimuli by clinically anxious and highly anxious individuals (Bar-Haim et al.,

2007). This has been consistently found with adults, whereby anxious individuals show clear

attention biases to threatening stimuli, and are more likely to interpret threat in ambiguous

situations (Williams, Matthews, & MacLeod, 1996). This disproportional attention to threat

is generally not seen in non-anxiety disordered adults, and is significantly less common in

adults with sub-clinical levels of anxiety. Accordingly, attention bias is hypothesised to play

a significant role in the development and maintenance of anxiety disorders. Following from

the wealth of literature on attention biases in anxious adults, researchers have sought to

determine whether anxious children also demonstrate an attention bias towards threat. The

body of literature on attention bias to threat in children and adolescents is relatively small,

compared to that of other risk factors for anxiety, and results have been less conclusive than

those from research with adults. The following section will review the literature in this area

to date.

Early research into the link between attention bias and childhood anxiety disorders

was conducted by Vasey, Daleiden, Williams, and Brown (1995), who compared the

performance of 12 children diagnosed with an anxiety disorder and 12 non-disordered

21
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

children (aged 9 to 14 years) on a dot-probe detection task. The task was designed to

measure the direction of visual attention in response to emotionally threatening and neutral

words. Participants viewed word pairs on a screen; some pairs consisting of two neutral

words, whilst other pairs consisted of one emotionally threatening word (e.g. Accident) and

one neutral word (e.g. Apple). Following display of word pairs, a dot probe appeared on the

screen, and participants were required to press a button upon noticing the probe. It was found

that anxious children responded significantly faster to the appearance of probes following

threatening words than to probes following neutral words. By comparison, there was no

difference between the response-latencies of non-disordered children for probes following

either threatening or neutral words. These results were later replicated with a non-clinical

group of 40 children aged 12 to 14 years, who had been identified as having either high text

anxiety or low test anxiety (Vasey, El-Hag, & Daleiden, 1996). Consistent with the earlier

research (Vasey et al., 1995), the more anxious children displayed significantly faster

response-latencies to probes following threatening words. These findings were interpreted as

evidence of an attention bias towards threat in clinically and sub-clinically anxious children,

which is consistent with research conducted with adults (Bar-Haim et al., 2007; Williams et

al., 1996).

Later research also demonstrated evidence for attention bias to threat in both anxious

and non-clinically diagnosed children and adolescents. Taghavi, Dalgleish, Moradi, Neshat-

Doost, and Yule (2003) found that children and adolescents with GAD disproportionately

attended not only to threatening stimuli but also to depression-related stimuli. Similarly,

research with a non-clinical sample of youths aged between 9 and 17 years revealed that

more severe child-rated anxiety symptomatology was associated with attention bias to

threatening words and images (Watts & Weems, 2006). Later, Waters, Mogg, Bradley, and

22
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Pine (2008) investigated attention bias for emotional human faces in children aged 7 to 12

years. Children diagnosed with GAD (n=23) and a non-anxious control group (n=25)

completed a dot-probe task involving pairs of photographs of human faces on a computer,

with neutral-neutral, neutral-angry, or neutral-happy expression combinations. A relationship

between response latency and anxiety was found only for children with high clinical anxiety

from the GAD group, whereby these children were significantly quicker to respond to probes

presented after angry faces. Children with low clinical anxiety from the GAD group did not

differ in terms of their visual attention to angry, happy or neutral faces. The above-reviewed

literature suggests that attention bias to threat is significantly associated with more severe

anxiety in children from both clinical and community samples.

Most recently, innovative research by Legerstee et al. (2009) extended the

understanding of links between attention bias and childhood anxiety, via demonstrating that

attention bias to threat is also predictive of outcome in interventions for childhood anxiety.

The study involved 131 children aged 8 to 16 years with a diagnosable anxiety disorder.

Prior to commencing a CBT intervention for anxiety, all children participated in a pictorial

dot-probe task to assess selective attention. The task involved presentation of image pairs in

three different combinations: severely threatening/neutral, mildly threatening/neutral, and

neutral/neutral. Participants were asked to respond as quickly as possible to the presentation

of a dot probe immediately after the display of each image pair. Following the completion of

the intervention program, significant differences were noted between participants who

responded to treatment (i.e. were diagnosis free at post-treatment), and those who failed to

respond. Specifically, children who responded to treatment showed a selective attention

away from the severely threatening stimuli in the dot probe task, whereas children who failed

to respond typically displayed selective attention towards severe threat. This finding has

23
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

important ramifications for the field of anxiety intervention and prevention, indicating that

additional attention training components may be required for some children to make positive

gains from these types of interventions (Legerstee et al., 2009). Furthermore, it may also

indicate that children with attention bias towards threat are at greater risk of relapsing post-

treatment.

In contrast to the above reviewed literature, several researchers have failed to find

significant links between attention biases and anxiety in children. Kindt, Bierman, and

Brosschot (1997) sought to compare attention bias for spider words in children with self-

reported spider fears and control children. The study employed a Stroop task methodology,

and included four word sets: (1) incongruent colour words, (2) non-words, (3) spider words,

and (4) control words. It was found that colour-naming latencies were significantly greater

for spider words than for control words, in children both with spider and without identified

spider fears, indicating that all children demonstrated a stronger processing bias for spider

words. Other research employing the Stroop task methodology also failed to differentiate

between anxious and non-anxious children in terms of their attention bias to fearful medical

situations (Kindt, Brosschot, & Everaerd, 1997), and pictorial and linguistic spider stimuli

(Kindt & Brosschot, 1999). Similar results have been found in research employing the dot-

probe task methodology, with no significant differences in the response-latencies of clinically

anxious and non-disordered children (Waters, Lipp, & Spence, 2004).

One explanation for the discrepancy in the literature on the link between attention bias

and childhood anxiety disorders pertains to the age of the children examined. Research has

found that younger children aged between 8 and 12 years are more likely to demonstrate a

general bias for fear-related stimuli (Kindt, Bierman et al., 1997; Kindt, Brosschot et al.,

24
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

1997), whilst anxiety-specific biases are typically found in older children and adolescents,

aged 9 to 19 years (Moradi, Taghavi, Neshat-Doost, Yule, & Dalgleish, 1999; Taghavi et al.,

2003; Taghavi, Neshat-Doost, Moradi, Yule, & Dalgleish, 1999; Vasey et al., 1995; Vasey et

al., 1996; Vasey & MacLeod, 2001). In explanation, it has been argued that young children

do not possess the cognitive capacity to avoid processing fearful stimuli. With regards to

older children however, non-anxious individuals may inhibit selective attention to anxiety-

provoking stimuli, whereas anxious individuals do not possess this capability (Kindt,

Bierman et al., 1997). Regardless of variability in results, the available literature suggests

that attention bias does have a role to play in the development of anxiety in children, and that

this effect may be mediated or moderated by other factors.

Interpretation bias. Interpretation bias is used to describe the tendency of anxious

individuals to interpret threat in an ambiguous situation (Muris & Field, 2008). Situations

interpreted as threatening or dangerous can elicit typical anxiety responses such as

physiological arousal and avoidance behaviours, which serve to reinforce anxious cognitions

and maladaptive coping strategies (Muris & Field, 2008). Evidence of interpretation bias in

anxious children has been found by a number of researchers. Hadwin, Frost, French, and

Richards (1997) used a self-report measure of trait anxiety with 40 children aged 7 or 9 years,

who were then asked to listen to a set of homophones (words with two separate meanings)

played on a tape. Participants were then shown a card with two pictorial depictions of the

homophones, one neutral and one threatening (e.g. Bark – tree, and Bark - dog), and were

asked to point to the picture which represented the word they had heard. A significant

positive correlation was found between anxiety levels and threatening interpretations,

whereby children with higher levels of self-reported anxiety were more likely to interpret

homophones as threatening. In later research Taghavi, Moradi, Neshat-Doost, Yule, and

25
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Dalgleish (2000) found that anxious children were more likely than non-disordered control

children to select threatening homograph interpretations over neutral interpretations, in the

construction of sentences.

Whilst anxious children have been shown to interpret ambiguous stimuli such as

words and images as threatening, this effect has also been demonstrated in response to

potential real-life situations. Barrett, Rapee et al., (1996) compared interpretation biases in

anxiety-disordered children aged between 7 and 14 years, with children diagnosed with ODD,

and non-disordered controls. Participants were exposed to brief vignettes of ambiguous

situations and asked to interpret each. They were then presented with two potential neutral

outcomes and two potential threatening outcomes, and asked to select which was the most

likely outcome for each given situation. It was found that anxious and oppositional-defiant

children were significantly more likely to assign threat to ambiguous situations, and anxious

children were more likely to select an avoidant outcome to the situation, compared to the

other two groups. These results were also found in later research by Bögels and Zigterman

(2000), who investigated interpretation bias in three groups of children diagnosed with an

anxiety disorder (SOP, SAD, or GAD) compared to a normal control group. When

confronted with a range of scenarios, anxious children were significantly more likely to

report negative cognitions, judge the situations as dangerous, and to provide lower estimates

of their competency to cope with the dangers, compared to non-disordered children.

More recently, Dineen and Hadwin (2004) have investigated whether children with

anxious and depressive symptomatology differ in terms of interpretation bias. In this study,

29 children aged 7 to 9 years were exposed to a series of scenarios of children interacting in a

social setting. Scenarios were classified as positive (e.g., child holding a present for another

26
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

child at a party), negative (e.g., child pushing over another child to win a race), or ambiguous

(e.g., child knocking over another child’s bricks) in nature. Following this, participants were

asked to judge how the protagonist (other judgement) and how they themselves (self-

judgement) would interpret the intentions of the second child in each scenario. It was found

that whilst depressive symptomatology was associated with greater negative self-judgement

interpretations, anxiety was predictive of an increase in negative interpretations of others.

This finding reflects previous literature which indicating that anxious children demonstrate an

interpretation bias towards threat in ambiguous situations.

Another form of interpretation bias has been referred to as the reduced evidence for

danger (RED) bias, which refers to the speed with which anxious children interpret threat in

ambiguous situations (Muris, Rapee, Meesters, Schouten, & Geers, 2003). This bias is

generally demonstrated through the use of ambiguous vignettes; children are informed that

some will be scary, and some will be happy, and their task is to identify as quickly as possible

which vignettes fit each category. As the vignettes are presented sentence by sentence,

children have an opportunity to identify the vignette as scary or happy at the end of each

sentence. This methodology was used by Muris, Merckelbach, and Damama (2000) with a

sample of sub-clinical socially anxious children aged 8 to 13 years. It was found that

children with higher levels of social anxiety needed to hear fewer sentences before labelling a

story as scary. This finding was taken as evidence of a RED bias, whereby anxious children

require less information before interpreting threat in a given situation.

The literature reviewed above has established that both attention bias and

interpretation bias are significant risk factors for anxiety. With regard to the former, research

involving both clinically anxious children and sub-clinical populations has demonstrated that

27
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

anxiety symptomatology is strongly associated with attentional biases towards threatening

stimuli. The fact that this finding has been robust across the spectrum of anxiety severity

suggests that attention bias towards threat may be an important risk factor for the

development and maintenance of childhood anxiety. Similarly, the available literature on

interpretation bias quite clearly implicates interpretation of threat and danger in ambiguous

situations as a risk factor for anxiety. Persistent maladaptive interpretation may promote and

reinforce avoidance, and in turn, contribute to clinical anxiety in children. The understanding

of how these risk factors are related to anxiety may be used in the development of

intervention strategies which more effectively target these cognitive biases, thereby reducing

anxiety.

Environmental Factors

The environment within which a child functions has been shown to play a role in the

development of childhood anxiety. Environmental factors include those inside the home

environment, such as parental psychopathology, parenting behaviours, and the quality of

attachment between parent and child, as well as traumatic or stressful events and contexts that

the child may be exposed to in the course of everyday life. For the purpose of this section,

empirical evidence for a selection of environmental factors linked to childhood anxiety will

be reviewed, focusing primarily on parent variables and exposure to traumatic events.

Parental psychopathology. There is a strong link between childhood anxiety and

parental psychopathology (Last, Hersen, Kazdin, Greta, & Grubb, 1987; Mesman & Koot,

2000; Rosenbaum et al., 1988; Turner, Beidel, & Costello, 1987; Weissman, Leckman,

Merikangas, Gammon, & Prusoff, 1984). Research has demonstrated that children of anxious

parents are between two to seven times more likely to develop an anxiety disorder than

28
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

children of non-anxious parents (Capps, Sigman, Sena, Henker, & Whalen, 1996; Turner et

al., 1987) with children of depressed parents also at an increased risk of developing anxiety

disorders (Beidel & Turner, 1997; Kovacs et al., 1989). Both maternal psychopathology

(Leve, Kim, & Pears, 2005) and paternal psychopathology (Compas, Phares, Banez, &

Howell, 1991; Kane & Garber, 2009) significantly predict anxiety disorders in children.

Additionally, parents of children diagnosed with anxiety disorders are also significantly more

likely to report anxiety and depressive symptomatology than parents of non-disordered

children (Suveg, Zeman, Flannery-Schroeder, & Cassano, 2005).

Early research by Turner et al. (1987) investigated anxiety in children of anxious

parents, dysthymic parents, and parents without psychiatric diagnoses. Children of anxious

parents were twice as likely to be at risk for developing an anxiety disorder, compared to

children of dysthymic parents, and seven times more at risk than children of parents without

psychiatric diagnoses. In conjunction with this increased risk, children of anxious parents

were more likely to report that they had experienced difficulties at school, that they spent

more time worrying, and that they were more socially isolated than children of parents

without a diagnosis. A similar study investigated the prevalence of psychiatric disorders in

81 children of parents with a diagnosis of either anxiety, depression, or comorbid anxiety and

depression, versus children of parents free from psychiatric diagnoses (Beidel & Turner,

1997). Building on results from the earlier research, it was found that children of parents with

either diagnosable anxiety, depression, or comorbid anxiety and depression, were more likely

to fulfil criteria for a psychiatric disorder themselves, compared to children of parents who

were diagnosis free. For children of anxious parents, anxiety disorders were particularly

more common than any other type of disorder.

29
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

More recent research has focused on the longitudinal link between parental

psychopathology and anxiety in children. In a population-based study of 2230 children aged

10 to 11 years, lifetime parental psychopathology (depression, anxiety, substance

dependence, antisocial behaviour, and psychosis) was assessed through parent interview, and

child psychopathology was assessed via parental, child, and teacher self-report measures

(Ormel et al., 2005). At 2.5 years follow-up, lifetime parental internalising (anxiety and

depression) psychopathology significantly predicted internalising problems in children.

Another more recent longitudinal study investigated early childhood risk factors for anxiety

in preadolescence (Ashford, Smit, van Lier, Cuijpers, & Koot, 2008). Internalising problems

in 294 children were assessed by parent report at age 2-3 years, and by both parent and

teacher report at 4-5 years and 11 years. Risk indicators were also assessed, including

parental psychopathology, child health, single parenthood, life events, parenting stress, and

socioeconomic status. Parental psychopathology at age 2-3 years was found to be one of the

strongest predictors of internalising problems in children at the age of 11. Both these studies

indicate the longitudinal effects of parental psychopathology on the development of

childhood anxiety, with the later study demonstrating its predictive significance as a risk

factor in children as young as 2 to 3 years of age.

Following from the robust evidence that parental psychopathology and childhood

anxiety are linked, some researchers have investigated familial effects of specific anxiety

disorders. Biederman, Faraone et al. (2001) compared anxiety in children of parents with PD

on its own, and when comorbid with depression. Four groups of children and their parents

were investigated: (1) Children of parents with PD and comorbid depression (n = 179), (2)

children of parents with PD without comorbid depression (n = 29), (3) children of parents

with depression only (n = 59), and (4) children of parents with no psychiatric diagnosis (n =

30
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

113). As predicted, children of parents with PD, depression, or comorbid PD and depression

were significantly more likely to suffer from multiple anxiety disorder themselves, as

compared to children of parents without psychiatric diagnoses. Children of parents with PD,

regardless of comorbidity with depression, were significantly more likely to themselves meet

diagnostic criteria for PD and agoraphobia than the control group. By comparison, children

of parents with depression, regardless of comorbidity with PD, were more likely to meet

diagnostic criteria for SOP, SAD, and depression. The increased risk for PD in children of

parents who themselves suffer from PD suggest that vulnerabilities for specific anxiety

disorders may be transmitted intergenerationally.

Empirical research clearly demonstrates strong evidence that children of parents with

psychopathology are at an increased risk of childhood anxiety disorders. It is important to

note that the relationship between parental psychopathology and childhood anxiety is not

causal, but may be mediated through other genetic and environmental mechanisms (Donovan

& Spence, 2000). With regards to the latter, parenting behaviour is one risk factor that has

been posited as a variable in the relationship between parental psychopathology and

childhood anxiety. Parenting behaviour as a risk factor for childhood anxiety will be

discussed below.

Parenting behaviour. Empirical evidence suggests that some parenting behaviours

are associated with an increased risk of childhood anxiety disorders (Donovan & Spence,

2000). Parents of anxious children may encourage and maintain their child’s anxiety through

modelling their own anxious behaviours, attributional styles, and negative cognitions, and

through reinforcing anxious and avoidant behaviours (Bayer, Sanson, & Hemphill, 2006;

Fisak & Grills-Taquechel, 2007; Rapee & Szollos, 2002). Despite the strong correlation

31
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

between parental psychopathology and childhood anxiety discussed in the previous section,

the differential effects of biology and environment make the developmental pathway of

childhood anxiety a complex one. With regards to the latter however, research to date has

highlighted differences between the practices of parents of anxious and non-anxious children.

A selection of this research will be discussed below.

In examining the relationship between parenting practices and childhood anxiety,

Barrett, Rapee et al. (1996) investigated differences in threat interpretation bias in anxious,

oppositional, and non-disordered children by asking them to interpret and provide plans of

action for ambiguous scenarios (plans were coded as either pro-social, avoidant, or

aggressive). Following this, children participated in a 5 minute family discussion with their

parents to help them determine a final plan of action. It was found that anxious children and

their parents were more likely to interpret ambiguous situations as threatening. Anxious

children were also significantly more likely to choose an avoidant response prior to the

family discussion, compared to oppositional and non-disordered children. Perhaps the most

notable finding however was that significant numbers of anxious children changed from a

pro-social response to an avoidant response, following the family discussion. It was also

noted that children from the oppositional group were more likely to change to more

aggressive responses following family discussion. Barrett, Rapee et al. (1996) termed this the

FEAR effect - Family Enhancement of Avoidant and Aggressive Responses, demonstrating

the role that parenting behaviours play in the anxiety of children.

A later investigation of the FEAR effect was conducted by Shortt, Barrett, Dadds, and

Fox (2001). One hundred and forty-seven children (aged 6 to 14 years) and their parents

were exposed to seven ambiguous situations; both parents and children were asked how they

32
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

would interpret the situation, and parents were also asked how they thought their child would

behave if they were in the given situation. Following this, the child and their parents engaged

in a family discussion about what action the child would take if they were exposed to the

situation. It was found that anxious children were more likely to interpret threat in the

ambiguous situations than non-disordered children, and more likely to endorse more avoidant

response plans than children with externalising disorders and non-disordered children.

Consistent with earlier research (Barrett, Rapee et al., 1996), both anxious children and

children with externalising disorders were significantly more likely to change their pro-social

responses to avoidant and aggressive responses respectively. Interestingly, children within

the anxious group were equally as likely to change to an avoidant response whether they

presented their response with their parents, or alone in a separate room from their parents.

This finding suggesting the FEAR effect is the stronger than the tendency of anxious children

to be compliant and eager to please. Clearly, this research presents further evidence

supporting the notion that some parenting behaviours may encourage anxious and avoidant

behaviour in children.

More recent research has further examined how parents may unknowingly reinforce

anxiety through supporting or rewarding their child’s anxious and avoidant behaviours.

Barrett, Fox, and Farrell (2005) compared parent-child interactions in three different groups:

anxious children, similar-aged siblings of the anxious children, and a non-clinical control

group of children. For task one, each parent-child dyad was required to engage in a

discussion about how the child should interpret and respond to an ambiguous social situation.

For task two, parents initiated discussion with their child about a topic they had identified that

would cause their child to feel anxious. Observations of the interactions revealed that

mothers of anxious children showed more anxious parenting behaviours, such as focusing on

33
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

negatives, and doubting in the success of their child, than parents of children in the non-

clinical control group. Parents of anxious children exhibited the same parenting behaviours

during interactions with their non-anxious children, suggesting that their anxious parenting

behaviours were consistent across members of the family, rather than concentrated on the

anxious child.

In conjunction with the more direct influence of parenting behaviour and practices,

parents may also influence their child’s anxiety more indirectly via modelling of anxious

behaviour, particularly if the parents themselves suffer from clinical anxiety. The link

between parent modelling of anxious behaviour and childhood anxiety was aptly

demonstrated in research by Muris et al. (1996). Forty clinic-referred children (aged 9 to 12

years) and their parents completed self-report measures of state and trait anxiety, and fear.

For parents, an additional item was added to the fear measure, asking them to rate to what

degree they expressed fear in front of their children by selecting one of three options: 'almost

never', 'sometimes', or 'often’. A significant positive correlation was found between trait

anxiety in children, and trait anxiety in both mothers and fathers, a finding that further

demonstrates the link between parental and child anxiety. Additionally, self-reported

fearfulness in children was significantly correlated with self-reported fearfulness in mothers.

Of particular note, however, was a significant positive association between children’s self-

reported fearfulness, and mothers’ ratings of their own expressions of fear in front of their

children. More specifically, children of mothers who reported “always” expressing fears in

front of their children reported the highest fearfulness, whereas children of mothers who

reported “never” expressing fears in front of their children reported the lowest fearfulness.

This finding suggests that children of mothers who express fears more overtly are at greater

34
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

risk for experiencing anxiety and fear, through modelling the observed anxious behaviour

themselves.

The influence of parental modelling on childhood anxiety has been identified not only

via parental self-report of their anxious behaviours, but also from children’s perceptions of

their parents’ behaviours. Early research by Bruch, Heimburg, Berger, and Collins (1989)

analysed retrospective ratings of anxious behaviour in parents of individuals diagnosed with

SOP and agoraphobia. Those with SOP were more likely to report that their mothers were

avoidant, placed greater emphasis on the opinions of others, and isolated them as children, a

finding which has since been replicated (Bruch & Heimburg, 1994; Caster, Inderbitzen, &

Hope, 1999). Later, Muris and Merckelbach (1998) investigated links between child-reported

anxiety symptomatology and perceptions of parental rearing behaviours in a sample of non-

disordered children aged 8 to 12 years. Results indicated that anxious rearing behaviours

were positively correlated with child self-reported anxiety symptomatology, whereby

children who perceived their parents’ behaviours as anxious were themselves more likely to

report higher levels of anxiety symptoms. These findings were supported by later research

indicating that child perceptions of anxious behaviours in parents were associated with

greater self-reported worry in children (Muris, Meesters, Merckelbach, & Hulsenbeck, 2000).

Research to date supports the hypothesis of parental modelling as a contributing factor

to childhood anxiety disorders. It appears that some parents, particularly those with anxiety

disorders, may contribute to the anxiety of their offspring through expressing their own

anxious thoughts and demonstrating their own anxious and avoidant behaviours in front of

their children.

35
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Attachment style. The quality of the attachment between child and parent is a

significant factor in the development of childhood anxiety disorders (Erickson, Sroufe, &

Egeland, 1985; Main, 1996; Sroufe, Egeland, & Kreutzer, 1990), with poor parent-child

relationships a recognised risk factor for childhood anxiety (Barrett, Fox, et al., 2005).

Attachment relates to the child’s sense of security and safety. Children are motivated to seek

proximity to an attachment figure in times of uncertainty or stress; if the attachment figure is

unavailable or unresponsive, anxiety increases in response to the experience of uncertainty,

threat, or danger (Bowlby, 1988). A secure parent-child attachment is characterised by

warm, emotionally sensitive and responsive parenting behaviours, allowing the child to be

comforted and soothed, whilst contributing to the development of self through providing a

secure base from which children feels safe to explore their world (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters,

& Wall, 1978; Bowlby, 1988; Sroufe, 1996). By contrast, insecure attachment patterns such

as ambivalent attachment (characterised by heightened display of negative emotions when

seeking attention of the attachment figure) and avoidant attachment (characterised by a lack

of distress at the unavailability of the attachment figure) are typically related to unresponsive,

hostile, or rejecting parenting behaviours. This may lead to deficits in cognitive and

emotional coping strategies (Carlson & Sroufe, 1995; Svanberg, 1998; Sroufe, 1996), which

potentially increases the risk of later psychopathology.

There is evidence to indicate that insecure attachment is associated with many

unfavourable developmental outcomes, including clinical anxiety and other forms of

psychopathology (Main, 1996). Longitudinal research by Bosquet and Egeland (2006)

investigated factors implicated in the aetiology and course of anxiety in 155 children

followed from infancy through to adolescence. Quality of attachment between parent and

child was assessed when participants were 12 and 18 months of age. Children with insecure

36
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

attachment identified in infancy were significantly more likely to experience emotion

regulation difficulties by the time they reached preschool, and these difficulties were linked

to the development of anxiety symptoms in middle childhood. By the time the participants

reached preadolescence, those who experienced an insecure attachment in infancy were also

more likely to have difficulties with peer relationships, which was associated with increased

anxiety in adolescence. Further research by Roelofs, Meesters, Huurne, Bamelis, and Muris

(2006) investigated self-reported anxiety symptomatology in children aged 9 to 12 years.

Those with insecure attachments reported greater anxiety than those with secure attachments.

Whilst these studies did not differentiate between ambivalent and avoidant attachment styles,

they provide convincing evidence that insecure attachment in general may result in an

increased risk for anxiety in later childhood.

Given that the characteristics of avoidant and ambivalent attachment vary somewhat,

some researchers have argued that one pattern might be the more reliable pathway to

childhood anxiety than the other. The argument for the former is based on the fact that

parents of avoidant children may ignore or reject their children, and fail to comfort them

when in distress. In this way, avoidant children may learn to avoid displaying negative affect

to ensure their emotional needs are met (Bradley, 2000; Goldberg, MacKay-Soroka, &

Rochester, 1994), resulting in maladaptive emotional coping. Several studies support this

argument, with avoidantly attached children demonstrating elevated internalising symptoms

in comparison to ambivalently attached (Goldberg, Gotowiec, & Simmons, 1995) and

securely attached children (Lyons-Ruth, Easterbrooks, & Cibelli, 1997). Despite these

findings, it is important to note that anxiety symptoms were not specifically distinguished

from the broader internalising symptom variable for the purpose of analyses in the research

above, suggesting that results should be interpreted cautiously.

37
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

In contrast to the above argument, other researchers maintain that ambivalent

attachment is more strongly linked to the development of childhood anxiety disorders

(Cassidy & Berlin, 1994). This is thought to occur in response to worry brought about by the

inconsistent availability of the parent in times of distress (Shamir-Essakow, Ungerer, &

Rapee, 2005; Warren, Huston, & Egeland, 1997), leading to feelings of vulnerability and

resulting in persistent anxiety. Evidence for this argument was reported in a longitudinal

study by Warren et al. (1997), investigating the link between attachment style and the

development of anxiety disorders in 172 children. Participants and their mothers were

assessed when the infants were 12 months of age, with children screened for

psychopathology at 17.5 years of age. It was found that participants with an ambivalent

attachment pattern as infants were significantly more likely to meet criteria for an anxiety

disorder at 17, even after the effects of maternal anxiety and infant temperament were

controlled. Most recently, longitudinal research by Brumariu and Kerns (2008) identified the

link between attachment style and three aspects of social anxiety: fear of negative evaluation,

social avoidance and distress in new situations, and generalised social avoidance and distress.

Ambivalent attachment earlier in childhood predicted all three aspects of social anxiety, and

children with lower attachment security and higher ambivalent attachment were more likely

to self-report social anxiety.

Whilst there remains some discussion as to whether ambivalent or avoidant

attachment styles are more conducive to the development of anxiety disorders in children, the

above evidence clearly demonstrates that at the very least, children with insecure attachment

styles appear to be at significantly greater risk of developing anxiety symptomatology. It

38
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

therefore appears that attachment style constitutes a significant risk factor for childhood

anxiety.

Life events. Exposure to stressful experiences and traumatic events may contribute to

the development of anxiety in children, particularly in those who are predisposed to these

disorders (Boer et al., 2002; Grant, Compas, Thurm, & McMahon, 2004; Rapee & Szollos,

2002). A range of negative events may result in anxiety difficulties, ranging from acute

stressors such as accidents, natural disasters, the death of a loved one, and changing schools,

to more chronic psychosocial stressors such as dysfunctional parent-child relationships,

parental psychopathology, domestic violence, poverty, financial stress and low socio-

economic status, sustained physical or sexual abuse, chronic illness, and bullying (Bandelow

et al., 2004; Donovan & Spence, 2000). In some cases, anxiety following negative life events

is acute, and may resolve reasonably quickly. For other children however, anxiety may

persist and interfere with everyday functioning. Associations between childhood anxiety and

negative life events have been found both prospectively and retrospectively, with evidence

for both discussed below.

A small body of prospective research has demonstrated an increased incidence of

anxiety symptoms and diagnosable anxiety disorders in children in the wake of exposure to

traumatic or stressful life events and situations (Dollinger, O'Donnell, & Staley, 1984;

Goodyer, 1996; Rueter, Scaramella, Wallace, & Conger, 1999; Terr, 1981; Yule & Williams,

1990). Early research by Dollinger et al. (1984) found increased self-reported fear in children

who had witnessed a fatal lightning strike during a sports match, compared to a matched

control group. Symptoms noted in this cohort included avoidance behaviours, intrusive

thoughts, poor sleep, and somatic complaints (Dollinger, 1986; Dollinger et al., 1984). A

39
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

later study investigated the longitudinal effect of family conflict in 303 families with at least

one child aged 12 to 13 years. The children were assessed for anxiety and depressive

disorders at age 19 or 20. Participants with greater parent-adolescent discord at the age of 12

to 13 were at a significantly greater risk of anxiety in late adolescence/early adulthood,

demonstrating the effect of a chronic stressor on anxiety.

Most recently, McLaughlin and Hatzenbuehler (2009) attempted to demonstrate a

longitudinal link between stressful life events, anxiety sensitivity, and development of

anxiety symptomatology. Anxiety sensitivity refers to the fear of anxiety symptoms, based

upon beliefs that such symptoms may be socially, physiologically, or psychologically

harmful to the individual (Reiss, 1991). Anxiety sensitivity has been recognised as a risk

factor for the development of anxiety disorders in both children and adults. McLaughlin and

Hatzenbuehler (2009) hypothesised that exposure to stressful life events would increase

propensity for anxiety sensitivity, which would in turn contribute to changes in anxiety

symptoms over time. This large-scale longitudinal study assessed stressful life events,

anxiety sensitivity, and internalising symptoms in 1065 adolescents at three different time

points across a 7-month period. Results indicated that exposure to stressful life events,

particularly those related to health and family discord, was associated with longitudinal

increases in anxiety sensitivity. As predicted, anxiety sensitivity was found to mediate the

relationship between stressful life events and anxiety symptoms. This research has

implications for future intervention and prevention research, suggesting that intervention

components which address fearful cognitions relating to anxiety symptoms may in part buffer

the effects of stressful life events, and thereby assist in reducing anxiety symptoms in

children.

40
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

In contrast to the prospective research above, retrospective research into the link

between life events and anxiety disorders has been more prolific, with results demonstrating

that anxious children are significantly more likely to have experienced a greater number of

negative events, compared to non-anxious children (Benjamin, Costello, & Warren, 1990;

Boer et al., 2002; Duggal et al., 2000; Goodyer & Altham, 1991; Sandberg, McGuinness,

Hillary, & Rutter, 1998). Allen, Rapee, and Sandberg (2008) compared the degree of

exposure to severe life events in children with anxiety disorders and a non-disordered control

group. Mothers of children in both groups were interviewed regarding their child’s

retrospective experience of chronic adversities and severe life events. Results indicated that

anxious children were exposed to significantly more chronic adversities and severe life events

than control children prior to the onset of their disorder, a finding which has replicated that of

earlier research (Boer et al., 2002).

Some researchers have chosen to focus on links between stressful life events and

specific anxiety diagnoses. Bandelow et al. (2004) conducted retrospective interviews

tapping childhood traumatic experiences with social anxiety disorder (now referred to as

SOP) patients, and a healthy control group. Anxious individuals reported significantly more

traumatic childhood experiences than controls, including being separated from parents,

parental marital discord, sexual abuse, familial violence, and childhood illness. They were

also more likely to report greater dissatisfaction with their parents’ rearing style, and greater

rates of psychiatric disorders (including anxiety disorders) in their families. Other

researchers have focused on rates of negative life events and obsessive compulsive disorder

(OCD) in childhood. Gothelf, Aharonovsky, Horesh, Carty, and Apter (2004) found that

children with OCD and children with another (unspecified) anxiety disorder experienced

significantly more negative life events than healthy controls, in the year prior to disorder

41
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

onset. The two anxiety groups did not differ in terms of the mean number of life events

experienced, with ‘major illness or injury of a relative’ the only life event significantly more

common in the OCD group. Regardless of the diagnosis considered, it seems clear that

exposure to traumatic and stressful life events constitutes an overt risk for anxiety problems

in children.

The above section reviewed several well-established environmental risk factors for

anxiety. The literature on parental psychopathology suggests that having an anxious parent

may predispose children to developing anxiety themselves, with this association having been

found in both cross-sectional and longitudinal research. Certain types of parenting

behaviours have also been identified as a significant risk factor for childhood anxiety, with

children of parents who model or reinforce anxious behaviours at greater risk of negative

outcomes. The quality of attachment between child and parent has also been implicated in

the development of anxiety in children. Both avoidant and ambivalent attachment styles have

been linked to increased risk of anxiety disorders, highlighting the importance of a secure

early attachment to the primary caregiver to long term positive mental health. Lastly, both

prospective and retrospective research has demonstrated that children exposed to traumatic or

stressful life events are at greater risk of developing an anxiety disorder. The research

reviewed highlights the important influence that a child’s environment may have on their

mental health outcomes, both immediately and in the future.

Protective Factors

Not all children with risk factors for childhood anxiety progress to the stage of

diagnosable disorder. This key point has lead to the investigation of various protective

factors for anxiety, which may provide resilience against psychological dysfunction (Coie et

al., 1993; Donovan & Spence, 2000). Protective factors identified in the literature are

42
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

generally classified as either child intrinsic, familial, or environmental in nature (Coie et al.,

1993; Donovan & Spence, 2000; Masten & Powell, 2003; Rutter, 1985). The mechanism of

action for protective factors varies widely, and may include providing direct protection from

risk, mediating the relationship between risk factors and anxiety, and acting as a buffer

against risk factors (Cole, Michel, & Teti, 1994; Donovan & Spence, 2000; Wheaton, 1986).

Compared to the wealth of literature investigating risk factors for childhood anxiety, research

into protective factors has been considerably less prolific. As research into anxiety

prevention progresses however, there is growing interest in the understanding of protective

factors, and their role in preventative intervention. The following section reviews the

available literature on key identified protective factors for anxiety disorders in children.

Child Protective Factors

Research has identified several child-centred factors which may protect against the

development of anxiety problems in childhood, including coping skills, locus of control, and

emotional regulation (Masten & Powell, 2003). These will be discussed below.

Coping skills. Coping skills is one of the most empirically established child factors

which protects against anxiety and other childhood psychopathology (Muris, Schmidt,

Lambrichs, & Meesters, 2001). The term refers to the various techniques and processes used

to cope with a challenging or unpleasant experience or situation. The repertoire of coping

skills and strategies that a child possesses can shape the way they respond to difficult or

negative experiences, and thereby influence their affective and behavioural response

(Donovan & Spence, 2000). The use of appropriate coping skills can mediate the relationship

between such negative experiences, and psychological wellbeing (Folkman, Lazarus, Gruen,

& DeLongis, 1986). Effective coping skills include the use of positive strategies such as

43
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

thought challenging, positive self-talk, help-seeking, and problem-solving, to address the

problematic issue. By comparison, maladaptive coping skills tend to be emotion-focused,

resulting in cognitive and behavioural avoidance of the stressor, which ultimately reinforces

anxiety (Billings & Moos, 1981; Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, 1989; Endler & Parker, 1990;

Folkman & Lazarus, 1980, 1985).

Research with adolescents and adults has demonstrated that problem-based coping,

and cognitive appraisals based on an internal locus of control are associated with better

psychological outcomes. These outcomes include decreased risk of psychological

dysfunction, fewer internalising symptoms, improved academic achievement, social

competence, and improved psychological adjustment in general (Cauce, Stewart, Rodriguez,

Cochran, & Ginzler, 2003; Compass, Malcarne, & Fondaraco, 1988; Herman-Stahl &

Petersen, 1996; Lengua, Sandler, West, Wolchick, & Curran, 1999; Luthar, 1991; Luthar &

Zigler, 1992; Muris et al., 2001; Plancherel & Bolognini, 1995). By comparison, emotion-

focused coping strategies that facilitate avoidance are associated with poorer psychological

outcomes, including anxious symptomatology and anxiety disorders in adults (Carver et al.,

1989; Edwards & Trimble, 1992; Jeavons, Horne, & Greenwood, 2000; Whatley, Foreman,

& Richards, 1998; Windle & Windle, 1996; Zeidner & Ben-Zur, 1994). Similarly, emotion-

focused coping and avoidance is associated with increased risk of anxiety, depression, and

behavioural problems in adolescence (Compass et al., 1988; Ebata & Moos, 1991).

Overall, the evidence suggests that coping skills that encompass a problem-based

approach, adequate appraisal, and greater perceived control will enable children to approach

and manage difficult and negative situations in more adaptive ways. Children who lack these

skills are poorer-equipped to face challenging situations through lacking the ability to

44
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

appraise the situation more appropriately, which may moderate the impact of such

experiences upon mental wellbeing (Hudson, Flannery-Schroeder, & Kendall, 2004). This

highlights the importance of coping skills as a buffer against anxiety, suggesting that the

amelioration of maladaptive coping should form a significant part of preventative

interventions for childhood anxiety disorders.

Emotional regulation. Emotional regulation, or the ability to monitor, evaluate, and

modify the intensity and duration of emotional states to achieve social and biological

adaptation and individual goals (Eisenberg, Fabes et al., 1997; Eisenberg, Guthrie et al.,

1997), may also exert a protective influence against childhood anxiety. Emotional regulation

is the process of managing emotional arousal, and is required for the development of healthy

relationships and the ability to successfully negotiate the many challenges of life

(Hannesdottir & Ollendick, 2007). By comparison, emotional dysregulation, characterised by

an impaired ability to control emotional arousal, results in maladaptive coping mechanisms

and reinforces distorted cognitions, hindering both the ability to engage appropriately with

others, and to respond adaptively to various situations (Cole et al., 1994; Hannesdottir &

Ollendick, 2007). In the case of childhood anxiety disorders, emotional dysregulation may

manifest through emotional and physiological arousal stemming from distorted cognitions.

The inability to appropriately regulate this arousal typically results in the maladaptive coping

strategy of avoidance (Mash & Wolfe, 2002), which reinforces anxiety.

Emotional dysregulation has been found to be more common in anxious children.

Southam-Gerow and Kendall (2000) noted that anxious children lack knowledge about their

ability to mask or manipulate the outward expression of emotion for the purpose of achieving

interpersonal goals, compared to non-anxious children. It is also known that anxious children

45
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

attempt to avoid situations that elicit intense emotional arousal (Mash & Wolfe, 2002), which

is typical of emotional dysfunction. More recently, Suveg and Zeman (2004) demonstrated

that when anxious children do find themselves in such situations, they have limited skills to

handle their emotions. Specifically, anxious children are more likely to experience negative

emotions more intensely, to have dysregulated expressions, to demonstrate less adaptive

coping, and to report lower self-efficacy in their ability to alter their mood than non-

disordered children (Suveg & Zeman, 2004). This research highlights the pattern between

emotional regulation and anxiety, and the emotional and behavioural impact of the inability

to effectively regulate emotions.

Given that emotional dysregulation is related to impairment in several indicators of

successful functioning (Thompson, 1994), it therefore follows that healthy emotional

regulation should be associated with better mental health outcomes. Evidence to this nature

was provided by Buckner, Mezzacappa, and Beardslee (2003), who investigated the link

between emotional regulation and resilience in 155 children aged 8 to 17. Children who were

more adept at emotional regulation scored better on measures of emotional well-being and

mental health. Other researchers have found that factors linked to emotional regulation, such

as positive self-esteem, ego resilience and ego control, are associated with greater resilience

and improved mental well-being in children with multiple risk factors for psychopathology

(Cicchetti, Ackerman, & Izard, 1995; Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1997; Cicchetti, Rogosch, Lynch,

& Holt, 1993). Eisenberg, Fabes et al. (1997) found that high emotional regulation was

associated with higher-quality social functioning (encompassing constructs such as social

skills and pro-social behaviour), and lower levels of negative emotionality. Taken together

the evidence suggests that children with better control over their emotions are less vulnerable

to a range of negative psychosocial outcomes, and that emotional regulation may protect

46
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

against anxiety and other forms of psychopathology (Buckner et al., 2003; Werner & Smith,

1992). It follows then that interventions that teach children to identify and manage emotions

may provide a protective effect against the development of childhood anxiety.

Family and Environmental Protective Factors

Beyond child-centred protective factors for childhood anxiety, several environmental

factors have also been identified which may exert a protective influence against anxiety.

More specifically, these factors are those inherent to the familial and social systems within

which the child functions. Evidence for the protective nature of family and community

variables will be discussed below.

A supportive and nurturing family environment is conducive to normal child

development and mental wellbeing. A component of this that is postulated to protect against

anxiety is a secure attachment to a parent or caregiver (Greenburg, 1999; Masten, 2001). This

type of relationship, characterised by warmth, openness, and responsive parenting, may

protect against anxiety through the promotion of healthy childhood adjustment, including

social and emotional development, and academic achievement (Kim-Cohen, Moffit, Caspi, &

Taylor, 2004; Luthar & Latendresse, 2005; Masten, 2001; Radke-Yarrow & Brown, 1993;

Vanderbilt-Adriance & Shaw, 2008; Werner & Smith, 1992). The protective effect of a

secure attachment has been found regardless of risk status; children with a secure attachment

during infancy tend to display fewer symptoms of anxiety by the time they reach school-age,

in both high and low risk populations (Booth, Rose-Krasnor, McKinnon, & Rubin, 1994;

Dallaire & Weinraub, 2005; Goldberg, Gotowiec, & Simmons, 1995). Recent research by

Dallaire and Weinraub (2007) found that even when gender, family income, maternal

47
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

sensitivity, and past anxiety were controlled for, children with a more secure mother-child

attachment at age 15 months were less likely to display anxiety symptoms at 4.5 years of age.

By comparison, children who lack a secure attachment to a parent or caregiver face an

increased risk for negative psychosocial and emotional outcomes (Greenburg, 1999).

Research with institutionalised children has shown that the psychological and behavioural

impacts of emotional deprivation, and lack of secure caregiver attachment, can be long

lasting and severe, and permeate other aspects of life well into adolescence and adulthood

(Beckett et al., 2006; MacLean, 2003). It has also been found that children classified as

insecurely attached at 15 months of age are significantly more anxious than children with a

secure parent-child attachment, regardless of the impact of negative life events. Other

research has highlighted that even an affluent upbringing fails to ameliorate the negative

effects of a deficient caregiver-child attachment (Luthar & Latendresse, 2005). Additional

literature regarding the negative effects of insecure attachment has been reviewed elsewhere

(see section on Risk Factors).

One protective factor which may be promoted both via the family and the community

is that of social support, which may buffer against the effects of several risk factors for

childhood anxiety. For example, social support has been shown to mediate the effect of

negative, traumatic, or stressful life events, leading to reduced risk of psychological disorder

(Cowen, Pedro-Carroll, & Alpert-Gillis, 1990; Murray, 1992; Spaccarelli & Fuchs, 1997;

White, Bruce, Farrell, & Kliewer, 1998). Social support within the family has been

highlighted as particularly beneficial in protecting children from anxiety. Longitudinal

research by White et al. (1998) found that children exposed to community violence who

received a high degree of familial social support were significantly less likely to be anxious.

48
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

A similar study investigating the effect of community violence on primary school children

found that both trait and state anxiety were lower in children with higher levels of social

support (Hill, Levermore, Twaite, & Jones, 1996). Social support from sources outside of the

home is also associated with positive mental health outcomes. Social support from

schoolmates and friends (Cowen et al., 1990; Hill et al., 1996; Varni, Setoguchi, Rappaport,

& Talbot, 1992), teachers (Danielsen, Samdal, Hetland, & Wold, 2009; Davidson &

Demaray, 2007; Huebner, Suldo, Smith, & McKnight, 2004) and sports coaches (Smith,

Smoll, & Barnett, 1995) have all been associated with improved indices of psychosocial

functioning.

Environmental factors within both the family unit and the wider community can exert

a crucially important protective effect against childhood anxiety. With regards to the family,

a stable and secure attachment to the parent or caregiver has been shown to protect against

the negative effects of stressful or traumatic events and situations, for children in both high

and low risk populations. Additionally, the positive benefits of social support, both within

the family and in the community in general, are associated with lower rates of anxiety in

children.

Summary

This chapter reviewed a selection of factors associated with increased risk of

childhood anxiety disorders. It is clear from this review that the aetiology of anxiety

disorders is complex, with a range of biological, psychological, and environmental risk

factors implicated in the development of clinical anxiety. Whilst individually they constitute

an increased risk for disordered functioning, the interaction and interconnectedness between

these factors makes it difficult to disentangle the effects of any single variable, and to infer a

49
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

causal relationship with anxiety. Despite this, risk factors for childhood anxiety may be used

to identify those individuals who are most vulnerable to poorer outcomes in general, and

provide targets for intervention and prevention of anxiety.

The current chapter also focused on protective factors for childhood anxiety,

including child-intrinsic, family-intrinsic, and environmental factors. This review highlighted

the important role of such factors in buffering against the effects of risk factors, and reducing

the likelihood of developing clinical anxiety.

Perhaps the most poignant conclusion that can be reached from the above literature

review is that the current understanding of protective factors for resilience against childhood

anxiety pales in significance compared to the wealth of literature on risk factors for childhood

anxiety disorders. Whilst knowledge of risk factors is undeniably crucial in identifying

populations in need of intervention, a better understanding of protective factors is required to

inform the content of such interventions, in the interest of maximising effectiveness.

Literature on both risk and protective factors has been heavily drawn upon in research of

treatment for childhood anxiety disorders, with interventions traditionally targeting

individuals and groups with multiple risk factors or an established disorder. Concurrently,

growing understanding of protective factors has enabled tailoring of these interventions to

enhance resilience and mental wellbeing in children with anxiety disorders, with promising

results.

The following chapter reviews literature on interventions for established anxiety

disorders in childhood. Early treatments based on the individual therapy model are reviewed,

followed by an exploration of studies evaluating group-based interventions.

50
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

CHAPTER THREE: TREATMENT OF CHILDHOOD ANXIETY DISORDERS

In response to greater awareness of the prevalence of childhood anxiety disorders,

extensive research has been conducted over the last few decades into treatment of childhood

anxiety disorders. Early treatment approaches for children suffering from clinically

diagnosed anxiety were typically downward extensions of empirically validated treatments

for adult populations, made appropriate for children (Cartwright-Hatton & Murray, 2008).

Of these, cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) is typically cited as the gold standard

treatment for childhood anxiety (Compton et al., 2004; Ollendick & King, 1998). CBT

addresses the cognitive, behavioural, and physiological elements of anxiety, through

treatment components including relaxation training, cognitive restructuring, exposure and

response prevention, and problem-solving. The efficacy of CBT as a treatment for anxiety in

children has been extensively validated. In a systematic review, Cartwright-Hatton, Roberts,

Chitsabesan, Fothergill, and Harrington (2004) report that trials evaluating CBT for

childhood anxiety have reported success rates approximating 60%. Furthermore, CBT for

childhood anxiety has been found to be equally as effective when delivered as an individual

or group intervention (Barrett, 1998; Shortt, Barrett, & Fox, 2001; Silverman et al., 1999).

The following section briefly reviews the progression of research into CBT as a treatment for

childhood anxiety, from its origins as an individual treatment modality, to its development

into a successful group intervention.

Individual CBT

Early research into CBT for the treatment of childhood anxiety disorders focused on

intervention at an individual level. The first randomised clinical trial of individual CBT for

childhood anxiety was conducted by Kendall (1994), in an evaluation of Coping Cat - a 16-

51
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

session manualised CBT treatment program for anxious children. The program was

comprised of components such as affective recognition, relaxation, cognitive restructuring,

and problem-solving. This study involved 47 children aged 9 to 13 years, each with a

diagnosis of either OAD, avoidant disorder (now referred to as SOP), or SAD. All children

were assessed via clinical interview to confirm diagnostic status. In conjunction with the

child-focussed treatment, parents of the children also participated in two additional sessions,

allowing the therapist to update them on their child’s progress.

The above intervention proved to be a success. Within the active treatment condition,

64% of the participants no longer qualified for a diagnosis at the completion of the program,

compared to 5% of the participants in the wait-list control group (Kendall, 1994). Significant

positive gains were noted from pre to post-treatment on parent-report, child-report, and

behavioural measures, with these gains maintained at 12 months follow-up. The efficacy of

the intervention was cemented by a later replication study involving a larger sample of

children (n=97) aged from 9 to 13 years (Kendall et al., 1997). By the completion of

treatment, 53% of children who received CBT were diagnosis-free, compared to only 6% of

children in the wait-list condition. For those children in the active condition who still met

criteria for an anxiety disorder post-treatment, measures of clinical severity for their disorder

had significantly decreased. Further support was provided by long term follow-up of the

original Kendall (1994) sample. The positive gains achieved in this first study were not only

maintained, but also enhanced, at both three years (Kendall & Southam-Gerow, 1996b) and

7.5 years (Kendall, Safford, Flannery-Schroeder, & Webb, 2004) post-treatment. This early

work demonstrated that childhood anxiety could be successfully treated using CBT on an

individual basis, forming the foundation for further research into treatment of childhood

anxiety disorders.

52
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Following pioneering work by Kendall (1994), other researchers expanded on the

individual CBT treatment model by including a family component, which acknowledged the

role that parents play in maintaining childhood anxiety (Hudson, 2005). Barrett, Rapee et al.

(1996) conducted the first study on family-based CBT intervention for anxious children. The

study included a sample of 79 children aged 7 to 14 years, each with a diagnosis of GAD,

SOP, or SAD. Children were randomly allocated to one of three conditions: individual CBT,

individual CBT plus a family component (CBT + FAM), or a wait-list condition. The family

component taught parents skills designed to assist them in managing their child’s anxiety

(including contingency management, communication skills, and problem solving), whilst

educating them about how to manage their own anxiety. At post-treatment, CBT + FAM was

shown to be more efficacious; 87% of children in this condition no longer met diagnostic

criteria for an anxiety disorder, compared to 57% of children in the CBT only condition, and

26% of children in the wait-list condition. Whilst no significant differences were noted

between the treatment groups at 6 months follow-up, by 12 months follow-up the significant

positive gains resurfaced, with 95% of children who received CBT + FAM diagnosis free,

compared to 70% of children who received CBT only. This research demonstrates that whilst

child-focused and family-focused CBT are significantly more effective treatments for

childhood anxiety, additional clinical benefits may be afforded by the inclusion of parents in

the intervention.

Since the above study was published, other researchers have further investigated

individual CBT for childhood anxiety when packaged with a family component. Recently,

Wood, Piacentini, Southam-Gerow, Chu, and Sigman (2006) compared a child-focused

individual CBT intervention for anxiety with a family-focused CBT intervention, the

53
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Building Confidence program. This program was comprised of traditional child-focused CBT

strategies plus an added parent training component, focusing specifically on changing the

communication patterns of parents which were theorised to maintain child anxiety.

Participants included 40 children aged 6 to 13 years with diagnosed SAD, SOP, or GAD.

Consistent with the earlier research, significant improvements were noted in both the child-

focused and family-focused CBT groups post-treatment. Greater improvements, however,

were noted for the latter group, in terms of both the diagnostician’s ratings of anxiety

severity, and parental report of anxiety symptomatology. The same pattern of results was

found at long term follow-up; positive gains were maintained 12-months post-treatment, with

diagnostician and parent ratings indicating greater reductions of anxiety for children in the

family CBT condition (Wood, McLeod, Piacentini, & Sigman, 2009). These findings

compliment those of Barrett, Rapee et al. (1996), and further support the inclusion of parents

in individual interventions for childhood anxiety.

A similar comparison of the relative efficacy of individual child-based and family-

based CBT interventions for childhood anxiety was conducted by Kendall, Hudson, Gosch,

and Flannery-Schroeder (2008), who compared these two treatment modalities against a third

condition: family-based education, support, and attention (FESA). The study involved 161

children aged between 7 and 14 years with a diagnosable anxiety disorder. At post-treatment,

children in both CBT conditions were significantly more likely to be diagnosis free, relative

to children in the FESA condition. There were no significant differences in rates of diagnosis

between the two CBT treatments at post-treatment, though teacher reports of anxiety

decreased significantly for children in the child-based CBT condition following treatment.

Importantly, the gains of children in both CBT groups were maintained at 12 months follow-

up. It was also noted that, for children of parents who both met criteria for an anxiety

54
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

disorder themselves, the family-based CBT condition was associated with greater

improvement in child outcomes. In contrast to earlier research (Barrett, Dadds et al., 1996;

Wood et al., 2006), this study found that child-based and family-based CBT for childhood

anxiety may be equally as efficacious, though the latter treatment model is likely to be more

effective for children of anxiety-disordered parents.

As discussed above, the positive effects of individual CBT for childhood anxiety have

been well established. Beyond immediate treatment effects, longitudinal research

demonstrating maintenance of treatment gains up to seven years post-treatment (Barrett,

Duffy, Dadds, & Rapee, 2001; Kendall & Southam-Gerow, 1996a; Kendall et al., 2004) has

significantly bolstered the continued use of CBT interventions for this type of individual

therapy with children. Research comparing child-focused interventions to family-focused

interventions suggests that, whilst both modalities are efficacious, the inclusion of a parental

component in traditional individual CBT interventions for children may afford additional

treatment benefits (Barrett, Dadds et al., 1996) (Wood et al., 2006), and that these benefits are

similarly robust over time (Wood et al., 2009).

Group CBT

Given that the efficacy of individual CBT has been well established (Hudson, 2005;

King, Heyne, & Ollendick, 2005), more recent research has shifted towards the evaluation of

group CBT for childhood anxiety disorders. Group interventions constitute a more efficient

treatment model than individual therapy treatments, in that the intervention ‘dosage’ can be

administered to larger numbers of anxious children simultaneously (Hudson, 2005).

Furthermore, group processes add another dimension to the standard individual CBT

intervention, with greater opportunities for encouragement and reinforcement, peer support,

normalisation of experiences, modelling, and sharing of resources among participants

55
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

(Albano, Marten, Holt, Heimburg, & Barlow, 1995; Heimburg et al., 1990; Kazdin, 1994). In

addition to these benefits, group CBT may be significantly more cost-effective, with a single

therapist able to deliver an intervention to a group of children in the same time that would be

devoted to an individual therapy program (Hudson, 2005; King et al., 2005). For this reason,

group intervention is also a time-effective alternative to individual therapy with anxious

children.

The first study investigating group CBT for anxiety disorders in children was

conducted by Barrett (1998). A sample of 60 children aged 7 to 14 years with a clinical

diagnosis of OAD, SAD, or SOP were randomly allocated into one of three conditions:

GROUP-CBT (child only condition), GROUP-FAM (child + family management training) or

wait-list control. Participants in both treatment conditions completed the Coping Koala

intervention program, a 12-week group CBT program for anxiety, and an Australian

adaptation of the Coping Cat program (Kendall, 1990). Post treatment, 56% and 71% of

children from the GROUP-CBT and GROUP-FAM conditions respectively were diagnosis

free, as compared to 25% of children from the wait-list control. These gains were maintained

over time, with 65% of children in the GROUP-CBT condition and 85% of children in the

GROUP-FAM condition diagnosis-free 12-months post-intervention. In comparison to

Barrett, Dadds et al.'s (1996) earlier study, group CBT was found to be equally as efficacious

as individual CBT in the treatment of childhood anxiety, both immediately post-treatment

and at 12 months follow-up, indicating group CBT to be a successful and efficient option for

treatment of childhood anxiety.

Early findings by Barrett (1998) have been bolstered by further clinic-based research

into group CBT for anxiety disorders in childhood. Silverman et al. (1999) investigated the

56
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

efficacy of a group CBT intervention for anxiety (incorporating concurrent parent sessions),

relative to a wait-list control group. Children in the active condition were significantly more

likely to be diagnosis free post-treatment, and showed significant improvements based on

both child and parent report measures of symptomatology. The positive effects of treatment

were still evident at 12 months follow-up. Later, a randomised clinical trial conducted by

Shortt, Barrett, and Fox (2001) compared a 10-session group family CBT intervention for

anxious children, the FRIENDS Program for Children (Barrett, Lowry-Webster, & Turner,

2000), against a wait-list control group. Participants included 71 children (aged 6 to 10

years) with a diagnosis of SAD, GAD, or SOP, who were randomly allocated to either the

treatment or wait-list condition. At post-treatment, 69% of children who received the group

intervention were diagnosis free, compared to 6% of children in the wait-list condition.

These treatment gains were maintained at 12 months follow-up, with 68% of children in the

treatment condition remaining diagnosis-free. Most recently, Hudson et al. (2009)

established that a group-based CBT intervention for childhood anxiety was superior to a non-

specific therapy intervention (designed to provide support and build relationships between

and within families) in reducing anxiety, with 68.8% of children in the CBT condition

diagnosis-free at 6 months follow-up, compared to only 45.5% of children in the active

control group. This research clearly demonstrates that group CBT is associated with greater

decreases in anxiety than both wait-list conditions and non-specific therapy formats.

Whilst time and cost-effectiveness clearly make group CBT a more attractive option

than individual CBT for the treatment of childhood anxiety, more importantly, the

effectiveness of the intervention is not compromised by group delivery. Flannery-Schroeder

and Kendall (2000) compared the treatment efficacy of individual CBT and group CBT for

anxiety disordered children. The sample consisted of 37 children (aged from 8 to 14 years)

57
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

with a diagnosis of GAD, SOP, or SAD. As predicted, children from both individual and

group CBT conditions were significantly less likely to meet diagnostic criteria for an anxiety

disorder post-intervention than children from a wait-list control condition. With regards to

outcome differences between the two active conditions, 73% of children in the individual

CBT condition were diagnosis free post-treatment, compared to 50% of children in the group

condition. This difference was not statistically significant, indicating that group CBT for

childhood anxiety can be considered to be an efficacious and efficient alternative to

individual treatment for this population.

Similar comparisons between individual and group CBT for childhood anxiety have

subsequently been published. Manassis et al. (2002) used a larger sample of 78 children

(aged 8 to 12 years) with diagnosable anxiety disorders (including GAD, SAD, SOP, PD, and

SP) to compare treatment efficacy of individual and group CBT. Both conditions included a

parent training component, which taught parents how to assist their children in coping with

anxious situations. Consistent with earlier research (Flannery-Schroeder & Kendall, 2000),

there was no significant difference in terms of program efficacy, and both individual and

group therapy were associated with reduced rates of diagnosable disorders post-intervention.

Most recently, a similar comparison was conducted by Liber et al. (2008), who evaluated

individual and group CBT for anxiety using the FRIENDS program intervention protocol.

Again, group CBT was found to be equally as effective as individual CBT in treating

diagnosable childhood anxiety disorders.

Following from research comparing individual CBT interventions to those with an

added parent component (Barrett, Dadds et al., 1996; Wood et al., 2006), Cobham, Dadds,

and Spence (1998) investigated what effect an added parental anxiety management

58
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

component (CBT + PAM) would have to n group CBT intervention for childhood anxiety.

This research was conducted with children of anxious parents, and children of parents

without an anxiety diagnosis. For the latter group, no significant difference in rates of

diagnosable anxiety was evident at post-treatment between the CBT and CBT + PAM groups.

A striking difference, however, was observed for children with anxious parents: 77% of these

children in the CBT + PAM condition were diagnosis-free, compared to only 39% for the

CBT only group. Clearly then, having an anxious parent appears to significantly compromise

the effectiveness of child-focused CBT for child anxiety, and CBT interventions with an

added parental component may be more effective for this group of children.

Later, Mendlowitz et al. (1999) expanded on the above research by comparing three

different intervention conditions for anxious children aged 7 to 12 years: parent and child

CBT, child only CBT, and parent only CBT. Whilst both anxious and depressive

symptomatology decreased significantly from pre to post-treatment in each of the three

conditions, greater improvements were noted for children in the combined parent and child

condition in terms of emotional well-being, as reported by parents. Children in the combined

group were also more likely to use more active coping strategies post-treatment, than those in

the other treatment groups. This research further indicates that the inclusion of parents in

group CBT programs for childhood anxiety may be associated with increased benefits to the

child, especially for those children with anxious parents.

Summary

This chapter has reviewed literature evaluating CBT as an intervention for children

with established anxiety disorders. The research in this domain supports the use of group

CBT as an efficacious, cost-effective, and time-effective treatment modality for childhood

59
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

anxiety, with demonstrated benefits over time (Barrett, 1998; Hudson et al., 2009; Shortt,

Barrett, & Fox, 2001; Silverman et al., 1999). Group CBT has been demonstrated to be as

effective as individual CBT in terms of treatment effects (Flannery-Schroeder & Kendall,

2000; Liber et al., 2008; Manassis et al., 2002), and treatment benefits may be improved

where the intervention is packaged with a parent component (Cobham et al., 1998;

Mendlowitz et al., 1999). Group-based CBT appears to be the treatment of choice for

children and adolescents with established anxiety disorders, however with greater

understanding of the growing issue of childhood anxiety in today’s society, researchers have

shifted focus from the treatment model to prevention. The issue of prevention of childhood

anxiety will be examined in the following chapter, with a particular focus on research

developments in this area.

60
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

CHAPTER FOUR: PREVENTION OF CHILDHOOD ANXIETY DISORDERS

Despite ongoing research into treatments for childhood mental illness, and continued

refinement and evaluation of intervention protocols, the majority of children with a

psychological disorder do not receive the treatment they require, whilst those who do often

terminate prematurely or do not improve following treatment (Donovan & Spence, 2000;

Essau, 2005; Farmer, Burns, Phillips, Angold, & Costello, 2003; Farrell & Barrett, 2007;

Hirschfeld et al., 1997; Olfson et al., 2003; Sawyer et al., 2000). Epidemiological research

has demonstrated that untreated anxiety disorders in childhood typically become entrenched

during adolescence and well into adulthood (Burke, Burke, Reiger, & Rae, 1990; Costello et

al., 2004; Dadds et al., 1999; Orvaschel et al., 1995). Further to this, the developmental

pathway of anxiety is well documented, with longitudinal research demonstrating a clear link

between childhood anxiety and subsequent development of depression, suicidality, substance

abuse, personality psychopathology, and other psychiatric disorders (Angold et al., 1999;

Cole et al., 1998; Last et al., 1996; Lewinsohn, Zinbard, Seeley, Lewinsohn, & Sack, 1997;

Rudd et al., 2004; Woodward & Fergusson, 2001). Untreated anxiety can compromise social,

emotional, and academic functioning (Donovan & Spence, 2000), resulting in poor social and

coping skills, low self-esteem, social isolation, and academic underachievement (McLoone et

al., 2006; Rapee et al., 2005). Across the life-course, these factors may limit vocational

opportunities, and contribute towards the development of depression and substance abuse in

adulthood (Donovan & Spence, 2000; Neil & Christensen, 2009; Rapee et al., 2005).

Whilst increased research into childhood anxiety has highlighted the importance for

timely intervention, there is no guarantee that children with anxiety will receive appropriate

psychological treatment. Many barriers exist to both access to, and engagement with, mental

61
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

health services for children. Owens et al. (2002) posit that there are three categories of

barriers to accessing child mental health services. The first category is structural barriers,

whereby access to services is limited by a lack of available services and long waiting lists,

due to increased demand on community mental health services (Donovan & Spence, 2000).

An additional structural barrier includes the inability to pay for private services, which are

beyond the financial means of many families (Snell-Johns, Mendez, & Smith, 2004). The

second category is barriers related to perceptions about mental illness, including the

proficiency of parents, teachers, and health care providers in identifying children requiring

assistance, denial of the severity of a child’s mental health problem, and beliefs that the

mental health problem does not require treatment (Esser et al., 1990; Halfon, Inkelas, &

Wood, 1995; Owens et al., 2002; Zubrick, Silburn, Burton, & Blair, 2000). The third

category is barriers related to perceptions about mental health services themselves, including

lack of trust, previous negative experience with mental health services, and stigma attached to

being treated for a mental illness (Flisher et al., 1997; Hoagwood et al., 2000; Stiffman et al.,

2000). These barriers may also be a factor in early termination of treatment, for those

children who progress to treatment phase (Kazdin, 1996). Barriers to mental health services

are discussed in more detail in Chapter 5.

The above barriers to accessing mental health services are heavily implicated in the

under-treatment of childhood anxiety disorders, which has dire consequences on a macro

level. The global public health burden of mental illness is already substantial, with

considerable negative impacts to the global economy. A report in the United States

investigated the financial burden of mental health services for children aged 1 to 17; it

reported a total yearly treatment expenditure estimate of $11.75 billion dollars, with inpatient

treatment, outpatient treatment, and medication costs accounting for 57%, 33%, and 9% of

62
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

the total expenditure respectively (Ringel & Sturm, 2001). With specific regard to anxiety,

Greenberg et al. (1999) estimated that in the decade of the 1990s, anxiety disorders cost the

USA alone in excess of $42 billion dollars, with the financial mental health burden projected

to rise even higher in the future. These projections, taken with the above research, suggest

that not only does the need for mental health service far outstrip the availability, but that

individual treatment of established disorders is an ineffective model of treatment (Barrett &

Turner, 2001), indicating a need for intervention on a much larger scale.

As populations continue to grapple with the forecasted impact of mental illness on the

economy, it is hardly surprising that interest in the prevention of mental illness has grown

markedly. In light of epidemiological research demonstrating the chronic course of anxiety

disorders which originate in childhood, preventative intervention in the formative years is

now seen as a model approach for proactively reducing the mental illness disease burden

(Bienvenu & Ginsburg, 2007; Neil & Christensen, 2009). Prevention not only assists

children with existing disorders to more adaptively manage and overcome their difficulties,

but also ‘inoculates’ non-disordered individuals against anxiety, by providing resiliency and

life-skills to more appropriately cope with future difficulties. Aside from the obvious

benefits to individuals, the timely delivery of anxiety interventions may reduce the economic

burden of these disorders through decreasing the need for costly clinical treatment once

disorders are established (Neil & Christensen, 2009; Spence & Dadds, 1996). This in turn

may relieve the pressure of high demand currently placed on already stretched mental health

services (Donovan & Spence, 2000). Further economic benefits of prevention programs for

anxiety delivered in childhood may include reduced rates of unemployment, and decreases in

lost productivity as a result of employee absences due to anxiety (Donovan & Spence, 2000).

63
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Levels of Prevention

At a preliminary level, how prevention is approached and structured depends largely

on both the population in question, and the mental health issue to be addressed. These

questions must be answered in deciding which strategy to implement, with individual

advantages and disadvantages inherent to each strategy. The current framework of choice for

prevention was described in a report by the Institute of Medicine (Mrazek & Haggerty, 1994),

and has been widely used as a multi-level model of prevention for mental illness. This three-

tiered framework posits that mental illness occurs on a spectrum of symptom severity, and

that the point of intervention may occur anywhere along this spectrum. With specific regard

to prevention, it acknowledges the developmental pathways of mental disorders as a function

of risk factors, and emphasises that the reduction of risk factors prior to the onset of a

diagnosable disorder constitutes the most optimal model of intervention.

The above framework draws distinction between three different levels of prevention:

universal, selective, and indicated. Universal prevention involves the treatment of an entire

population of individuals, regardless of differences in diagnostic status or risk. The goal is to

reduce risk factors for mental illness through targeting symptoms and building resiliency,

thereby reducing both the incidence and prevalence of diagnosable disorders. An example of

universal prevention is an intervention for anxiety at a school-level, with all students

participating. By comparison, selective prevention focuses on a subset of the population who,

whilst not yet displaying symptoms of a diagnosable disorder, have existing identifiable risk

factors for mental illness. For example, given that exposure to traumatic events is a

recognised risk factor for anxiety disorders, a selective intervention for anxiety may involve

running an anxiety program for a group of children who lived through a natural disaster, such

as a bushfire. Lastly, indicated prevention is targeted at individuals already experiencing

64
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

clear signs or symptoms of mental illness, who are therefore at greatest risk of developing a

diagnosable disorder. As an example, an indicated intervention for SOP in youths might

focus on those children already demonstrating elevated anxiety in social situations and

demonstrating avoidance behaviours.

Each level of prevention has both strengths and weaknesses in terms of utility,

methodology, and overall outcome. A comparison of these strengths and weaknesses for

each level of prevention is presented in Table 1 below. Given that they both involve only a

subset of a whole population, indicated and selective prevention strategies share many

characteristics. From a research perspective, these intervention strategies are associated with

the greatest effect size, given that they focus exclusively on reducing rates of

psychopathology in individuals who are most at risk of disorder or who already fulfil criteria

for mental disorder (Barrett & Farrell, 2007). For this reason, indicated and selective

prevention programs may be more likely to attract funding for research. Furthermore, given

that indicated and selective prevention strategies are applied only to the percentage of the

population who stand to benefit the most, the time, costs, and resources required to

implement such an intervention are significantly less than those required for universal

prevention strategies (Donovan & Spence, 2000). Another benefit is the smaller facilitator-

to-participant ratio typical of small group interventions, which may afford participants a more

concentrated dosage of the intervention, and more individual attention. The small-group

nature of these levels of intervention also means there will likely be less participant attrition.

Whilst these strengths make indicated and selective prevention attractive options for

school-based intervention, there are limitations to these approaches. Perhaps most notably,

the recruitment of individuals requires screening to identify individuals who fulfil participant

65
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

criteria. The identification of children as being at risk may result in these children being

stigmatised within the school environment, which may reinforce existing emotional

difficulties. Another complication of school-based indicated and selective prevention is

timetabling, as these interventions are typically run during normal school hours.

Accordingly, there is the possibility of learning being disrupted due to classroom absences to

attend intervention groups, particularly if groups are regularly held at the same time each

week. Furthermore, unless the schools involved have sufficient additional support staff (such

as teacher aides or guidance counsellors) who can be trained to deliver the interventions,

group facilitators may be required from outside of the school community, given that regular

classroom teachers will be occupied in class with the majority of students.

Universal prevention is distinct from indicated and selective prevention, in that it is

designed to be applied to all members of a given population. Prevention of childhood anxiety

delivered at the universal level affords all children the opportunity to learn and develop skills

designed to reduce the prevalence of anxiety disorders in childhood on the widest scale

possible. Whilst children with anxiety disorders obviously benefit from the intervention, the

advantage of universal prevention is that it includes children who would not otherwise be

eligible for, or have access to, mental health services. Therefore, universal prevention

overcomes a significant barrier for many families. Furthermore, given that universal

interventions are typically conducted in the school, the inclusion of all children also avoids

the possibility of stigmatisation. Universal prevention for internalising disorders conducted

in the school setting also serves to normalise the experience of worry and sadness in children,

and provides a non-threatening and familiar milieu in to explore and practice strategies for

dealing with these emotions (Evans, 1999; Kubiszyn, 1999).

66
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Despite the obvious benefits of universal prevention, interventions on a population

level can be very difficult to implement and research, due largely to the sheer number of

children involved. Universal prevention in schools requires a high degree of co-operation

from all stakeholders on a regular basis. The time and financial costs are greater than for

other levels of prevention, given the large number of children that must participate in pre,

post, and often, follow-up screening, to demonstrate program effectiveness (Barrett & Farrell,

2007). Another limitation is that universal prevention typically produces a lower effect size,

given that the group of children with clinically significant anxiety constitutes only a small

percentage of the total participant sample. Therefore, researchers may find it more difficult

to attract funding for universal prevention. Additionally, larger sample sizes generally

require a greater number of facilitators (in a school setting, these are most often teachers),

each of whom must be trained in the delivery of the intervention (Donovan & Spence, 2000).

A greater number of participants also increase the likelihood of attrition, in particular at long

term follow-up intervals. Despite these hardships, the body of research on universal

prevention in schools continues to grow, with promising findings encouraging continued

research in the field.

Table 1

Strengths and Weaknesses of Indicated, Selective and Universal approaches to School-based

Prevention

Strengths Weaknesses
Indicated School-  Larger effect sizes  Screening process involved
based Prevention  Cheaper to implement –  Risk of stigmatisation and
less materials required labelling of participants
&  Smaller numbers of group  Potential for children to
facilitators to train miss classes
Selective School-  More likely to attract  External group facilitators
based Prevention funding may be required

67
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

 Smaller participant groups


– higher intervention
“dosage”
 Lower ratio of children to
group leader
 More personalised –
children receive more
individual attention
 Lower attrition rates

Universal School-  All children have the  More training required for
based Prevention opportunity to develop greater number of
positive coping skills, facilitators
regardless of risk or  More expensive to deliver –
diagnostic status more materials required
 Overcomes barriers to  Smaller effect sizes due to
accessing mental health higher numbers
services for all families  Larger groups – higher ratio
 Avoids the possibility of of children to group leader
stigmatisation
 Normalises anxiety
 Can implement as part of
the curriculum

With the expansion of interest in the field of prevention, there have been several

investigations of preventative interventions for childhood anxiety at each of the three levels

discussed above. Much of this research has been conducted in the school setting, which

affords the perfect milieu for treatment of anxiety in children. Delivering mental health

services via schools overcomes many barriers to traditional community-based treatment,

including time, location and transportation difficulties, excessive cost, and stigmatisation

(Barrett & Pahl, 2006; Neil & Christensen, 2009), thereby engaging children who may not

otherwise receive treatment (Chatterji, Caffray, Crowe, Freeman, & Jensen, 2004). School-

based programs may be delivered at the indicated, selective, or universal level, with a recent

review of school-based interventions for anxiety concluding that over three-quarters of the

trials investigated effectively reduced childhood anxiety in the treatment population (Neil &

68
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Christensen, 2009). The following sections will review the body of literature pertaining to

school-based programs for childhood anxiety at each intervention level, with specific focus

on the prevention on a universal level.

Indicated Prevention

A small body of published research exists for the evaluation of indicated prevention

of childhood anxiety in schools. The first study was conducted by Kiselica, Baker, Thomas,

and Reedy (1994) who evaluated a preventative stress inoculation program with a sample of

48 students in Grade 9. Participants receiving the intervention were selected for inclusion

based on elevated anxiety scores on self-report measures of trait anxiety and symptoms of

stress. The eight-session intervention, which was delivered in a classroom setting, consisted

of three components: progressive muscle relaxation, cognitive restructuring, and

assertiveness training. Compared to a matched-sample control group, participants in the

intervention group scored significantly lower on both self-report measures following the

completion of the intervention. At four weeks post-intervention, participants in the

intervention group continued to report significantly less anxiety and symptoms of stress than

control participants. This early research provided preliminary support for school-based

indicated prevention of childhood anxiety.

Later research by Dadds, Spence, Holland, Barrett, and Laurens (1997) examined the

effectiveness of a 10-session CBT intervention for the prevention of anxiety in children and

adolescents. Participants were aged between 7 and 14 years, and were selected on the basis

of sub-clinical or mild clinical levels of anxiety, as determined by child self-report, teacher

nominations, and parental interviews. The intervention protocol evaluated was the

aforementioned Coping Koala program (Barrett, Dadds et al., 1996), a CBT intervention for

69
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

childhood anxiety with an added parental component designed to educate parents about

childhood anxiety, and provide information about concepts and strategies taught in the

program. In conjunction with the 10 weekly school-based child sessions, parents of

participants were given the opportunity to attend three parent education sessions during the

intervention phase. Significant decreases in anxiety were noted at the completion of the

program in both the intervention and the control group, however a significant preventive

effect on the diagnosis rate, child adjustment, and family adjustment, was evident at both 6

and 24-month follow-ups for the intervention group only (Dadds et al., 1999). This research

provides more evidence for the utility of indicated prevention of anxiety disorders in schools.

The above research has been strengthened by more recent replications demonstrating

the positive effects of school-based indicated interventions for anxiety. Research conducted

in the Netherlands involved the screening of 425 children aged 8 to 13 years for clinical

anxiety (Muris & Mayer, 2000, cited in Barrett & Turner, 2004). Of this sample, 42 children

with elevated anxiety scores were selected to participate in the Coping Koala program

(Barrett, Dadds et al., 1996). Whilst 86% of the students in the sample met full criteria for a

major anxiety disorder prior to the intervention, 75% of participants were diagnosis-free 6

months following the completion of the program.

Most recently, Bernstein, Layne, Egan, and Tennison (2005) compared the

effectiveness of group-based CBT, the FRIENDS for Life program (Barrett, Webster, &

Turner, 2000), when delivered alone, or with a parent training component. Children were

aged 7 to 11 years, and were identified as having elevated levels of anxiety according to

questionnaire self-report, teacher nomination, and parent and child diagnostic interviews. To

qualify for inclusion, all participants had to meet diagnostic criteria for SAD, GAD, and/or

70
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

SOP, or demonstrate one or more diagnostic criteria of at least one of these disorders.

Significant reductions in anxiety symptoms and diagnoses were noted post-intervention in

both intervention groups, relative to a control group. In terms of change in diagnostic status,

CBT alone was shown to be superior to CBT plus parent training. However, according to

parent report of child anxiety and clinician rated severity, CBT plus parent training was

shown to be superior to CBT alone, with no significant difference between the latter and the

control group.

The above reviewed research demonstrates that school-based group CBT for

childhood anxiety is successful when delivered at the indicated level of prevention.

Regardless of whether the intervention is structured as child-only, or includes a parent

training component, school-based indicated prevention of childhood anxiety is more effective

than no treatment in reducing anxiety symptoms in children.

Selective Prevention

Several studies have investigated prevention of childhood anxiety at the selective

level. Individuals and groups have typically been recruited for participation in selective

prevention studies based on possessing one or more risk factor for anxiety. Cultural change

and migration have been recognised as risk factors for the development of anxiety (Barrett,

Turner, & Sonderegger, 2000), hence several research groups have evaluated a school-based

CBT intervention for anxiety, the FRIENDS for Life program (Barrett, Lowry-Webster, &

Holmes, 1999), in culturally diverse populations. These populations have included children

and adolescents of former-Yugoslavian, Chinese, and mixed ethnic backgrounds (Barrett,

Moore, & Sonderegger, 2000; Barrett, Sonderegger, & Sonderegger, 2001; Barrett,

Sonderegger, & Xenos, 2003). In each study, participants who received the intervention

71
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

demonstrated significantly reduced scores on anxiety measures, relative to participants in

wait-list control groups. Significant improvements were also noted on self-esteem measures.

Promisingly, gains made by the intervention groups were maintained 6 months post-treatment

(Barrett et al., 2003). This research targeted minority groups with known risk factors for

childhood anxiety, and successfully demonstrated significant reductions in anxiety

symptomatology. This indicates that clinically significant improvements may be procured

via intervening prior to the onset of a diagnosable disorder in at-risk populations.

Exposure to community violence is another established risk factor for the

development of anxiety disorders in youth (Cooley-Quille, Boyd, Frantz, & Walsh, 2001).

Research conducted by Cooley, Boyd, and Gradas (2004) investigated the effectiveness of the

FRIENDS for Life program (Barrett et al., 1999) with a small sample of inner-city African-

American children aged 10 to 11 years. Participants were drawn from an elementary school

within a recognised low SES district, where crime rates were high. Children selected for the

intervention displayed moderate symptoms of anxiety, identified by a combination of both

child self-report and teacher nomination measures. The intervention was delivered in 11 bi-

weekly sessions during normal class time. A significant reduction in anxiety

symptomatology was noted from pre to post-intervention. The results suggest the program

was effective in reducing anxiety in this at-risk population, however long-term follow-up data

is required to determine the longitudinal effects of the intervention in populations with high

exposure to community violence.

Another significant risk factor for childhood anxiety is socioeconomic disadvantage

(Buka, Monuteaux, & Felton, 2002), which is of central importance to the current research.

In an evaluation of selective intervention using a CBT-based program for childhood anxiety

72
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

delivered in nine socioeconomically disadvantaged schools, Misfud and Rapee (2005)

reported significant decreases in anxiety symptoms post-treatment and at 4 months follow-up.

This study is unique, in that it is one of only two evaluations of childhood anxiety

intervention programs conducted specifically within a disadvantaged community. Further

details of this study, and a more thorough discussion of the relationship between

socioeconomic disadvantage and childhood psychopathology will be discussed in Chapter 5.

Universal Prevention

There has been a notable surge in research into school-based universal prevention of

anxiety over the last decade, with growing recognition of schools as the ideal platform for the

simultaneous delivery of interventions to large numbers of children (Masia-Warner, Nangle,

& Hansen, 2006; Neil & Christensen, 2009). The results of anxiety intervention and

prevention programs delivered in schools at the universal level have been largely positive,

with 69% of universal trials evaluated in a recent review reporting significant reductions in

anxiety for children in the intervention condition (Neil & Christensen, 2009). Whilst a

number of different intervention protocols have been researched, with several reporting

significant positive effects (Berger, Pat-Horenczyk, & Gelkopf, 2007; Garaigordobil, 2004;

Hains, 1992; Hains & Ellmann, 1994; Hains & Szyjakowski, 1990), by and large the most

extensively researched intervention program for childhood anxiety delivered universally is

the FRIENDS program. Accordingly, and given that the current research is concerned with

further evaluation of this program, the following section reviews empirical evidence detailing

the universal implementation of the FRIENDS program in schools.

The first published research in this area was conducted by Barrett and Turner, (2001),

who evaluated the effectiveness of the FRIENDS for Children program (Barrett et al., 1999)

73
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

when delivered as a universal prevention program to 489 Grade 6 children (aged 10 to 12

years) across 10 primary schools in Brisbane, Australia. Importantly, this research also

evaluated a “train-the-trainer” model of intervention, with the schools allocated to one of

three conditions: (a) psychologist led intervention (n = 188), (b) teacher led intervention (n =

263), and (c) standard curriculum with monitoring (n = 137). All teachers and psychologists

facilitated the intervention groups received intensive training in the program delivery, and

parents of children in both intervention groups were invited to attend four parent evenings

incorporating psychoeducation and parenting strategies. Screening for internalising

symptomatology included three self-report questionnaires completed within normal class

time. Evaluation of these measures post-intervention showed that children in both

intervention groups reported significantly fewer anxiety symptoms, relative to the monitoring

condition. These promising results provided early support for the effectiveness of the

FRIENDS program when delivered as a school-based universal prevention for anxiety,

regardless of whether the program was delivered by teachers or psychologists. More recent

support for teacher-led anxiety interventions has been reported by Neil and Christensen

(2009), who found that a higher percentage of trials with teachers as program leaders resulted

in significant reductions in anxiety, when compared to programs led by mental health

professionals, researchers, and graduate students.

A subsequent study also investigated the effectiveness of FRIENDS for Children

(Barrett, Webster et al., 2000) as a universal intervention with a slightly older population of

school-aged children (Lowry-Webster, Barrett, & Dadds, 2001). A total of 594 children in

Grades 5 to 7 (aged 10 to 13 years) from seven private Catholic schools in Brisbane,

Australia, were assigned to either the intervention or monitoring-only condition, with all

children screened using self-report measures of anxiety and depressive symptomatology. The

74
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

intervention was facilitated by teachers trained to deliver the program, and was implemented

in 10 weekly sessions during regular classroom time. Following the conclusion of the

program both the intervention and control group demonstrated significant reductions in self-

reported anxiety symptoms, however these reductions were significantly greater for the

intervention group. Only the intervention group showed a significant reduction in depressive

symptoms post-intervention.

The above study also investigated changes in risk status for anxiety. Children were

identified as being at high risk for anxiety if their pre-intervention scores fell above the

clinical cut-off on the self-report anxiety measure. It was found that 75.3% of high-risk

children in the intervention condition were no longer at risk post-intervention, compared to

54.8% of high-risk children in the control condition (Lowry-Webster et al., 2001).

Longitudinal effects of the intervention were later investigated in a 12-month follow-up study

(Lowry-Webster, Barrett, & Lock, 2003). The positive gains made by children in the

intervention condition were maintained, with this group demonstrating significantly lower

anxiety scores than children in the control condition. Further investigation with children

previously identified as high-risk was conducted through diagnostic interviewing, revealing

that 85% of high-risk children from the intervention group were diagnosis free 12 months

following the intervention, compared to 31% of high-risk children from the control group.

This research further demonstrates the effectiveness of the FRIENDS program in preventing

anxiety symptoms when delivered as part of the school curriculum, and points to its

continued effectiveness over time.

Another longitudinal study of a universal school-based application of the FRIENDS

program was conducted by Lock and Barrett (2003), to determine whether the program was

75
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

more effective when implemented in late primary, or early secondary school. Participants

included Grade 6 (n = 336; aged 9 to 10 years) and Grade 9 (n = 401; aged 14 to 16 years)

students from seven schools in Brisbane, Australia, of ranging socioeconomic diversity.

Participants were assigned to either the intervention or control condition (standard

curriculum), based on the school they attended, and all students completed self-report

measures on anxiety and coping. As with previous research (Lowry-Webster et al., 2001;

Lowry-Webster et al., 2003) risk group status was determined by scores on self-report

measures of anxiety. Consistent with the existing literature (Lowry-Webster et al., 2001;

Lowry-Webster et al., 2003), participants in the intervention conditions showed greater

reductions in anxiety symptomatology both post-treatment and at 12 months follow-up. With

regards to age differences, the Grade 6 group reported higher levels of anxiety both pre and

post-intervention than the Grade 9 group, however the younger group reported significantly

less anxiety symptoms at 12 months follow-up, suggesting that late primary school may be

the optimal time for anxiety prevention programs to be implemented in school. Similarly, a

subsequent longitudinal study investigating developmental differences in universal school-

based prevention of anxiety also concluded that intervention with children in younger grades

may be associated with more significant reductions in anxiety (Barrett, Lock, & Farrell,

2005).

In recent years, more extensive longitudinal research has been conducted with Lock

and Barrett's (2003) original sample, to determine the long-term effectiveness of the

FRIENDS program when delivered as a universal, school-based intervention (Barrett, Farrell,

Ollendick, & Dadds, 2006). The follow-up research investigated outcomes at 24 months

(Grade 8 and Grade 11) and 36 months (Grade 9 and Grade 12) post-intervention. Based on

self-report measures of anxiety, the original intervention gains were maintained for the

76
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

younger intervention group, with significantly lower ratings of anxiety at long-term follow-

up. By comparison, there were no significant differences in anxiety ratings for the older

intervention group across all follow-up time points. As with the earlier research (Lock &

Barrett, 2003), these findings appear to indicate that universal school-based anxiety

prevention may be most effective in reducing anxiety when delivered during late primary

school. With regards to risk group status at long-term follow-up, whilst there was no

significant difference between the intervention condition and control condition at the 24

month time-point, by 36 months, participants in the intervention groups were significantly

less likely to fall within the at-risk range (12%) relative to those in the control group (31%).

Overall, this research provides further promising support for the implementation of school-

based universal prevention programs for anxiety, particularly if delivered in late primary

school, and demonstrates that positive intervention effects are robust up to 3 years post-

intervention.

Whilst FRIENDS has been well validated as a universal intervention for anxiety

within Australian schools, the most recent evaluation of the program delivered as a universal

school-based intervention was conducted in the United Kingdom (Stallard, Simpson,

Anderson, Hibbert, & Osborn, 2007). Participants were 106 children (aged from 9 to 10

years) from three primary schools, including one school from a socially deprived region. All

participants completed self-report questionnaires to determine anxiety symptomatology and

levels of self-esteem 6 months prior to commencing the program, immediately before the

intervention, and 3 months post-intervention. School nurses were trained to deliver the

program, which was delivered over 10 weekly sessions during normal class time. Parents

were also invited to attend a psychoeducation session at the start of the program, detailing the

contents of the intervention. Results demonstrated significant increases in self-esteem and

significant reductions in self-reported anxiety symptoms from 6 months prior to intervention

77
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

to 3 months post-intervention. Stallard et al. (2007) also investigated treatment effects

specifically within the high-risk group. In this group, children reported significantly less

anxiety at 3 months post-treatment, than at both 6 months prior to treatment, and immediately

before treatment.

The above research was furthered more recently in a 12-month follow-up study

(Stallard, Simpson, Anderson, & Goddard, 2008). Results demonstrated that the positive

effects of the intervention at 3 months follow-up were maintained 12 months post-treatment

(Stallard et al., 2007), with no difference between 3 month and 12 month follow-up scores on

self-reported anxiety and self-esteem. Similarly, there was no difference in anxiety scores for

children in the high risk group, from 3 months to 12 months follow-up, demonstrating the

effects of the intervention were robust for the most highly anxious participants. Further to

this, 67% of children who were identified as high risk at baseline had moved into the low risk

group at 12 months post-intervention. It is worth noting the smaller sample size in this

research in comparison to the aforementioned studies, and that this research was affected by

attrition of 59% of the original sample size by 12-months follow-up. This research also

deviated from the previous literature in that it did not include a control group for comparison.

These issues not-withstanding, the results of this research demonstrate further evidence for

the effectiveness of the FRIENDS program as a universal school-based intervention for

anxiety, and compliment the existing literature in this area.

Summary

The body of literature reviewed above provides a strong evidence base for the

effectiveness of universal interventions for anxiety when delivered in a school setting. Most

studies to date have evaluated the FRIENDS program, extending on the existing body of

empirical research validating this program for use with groups in both a clinic and a school

78
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

setting (Barrett, Dadds et al., 1996; Barrett, Moore et al., 2000; Barrett, Sonderegger et al.,

2001; Barrett et al., 2003; Bernstein et al., 2005; Cooley et al., 2004; Dadds et al., 1999;

Dadds et al., 1997). The research clearly demonstrates the effectiveness of the program in

reducing rates of anxiety at post-intervention (Barrett & Turner, 2001; Lock & Barrett, 2003;

Lowry-Webster et al., 2001; Stallard et al., 2007), 12 month (Lock & Barrett, 2003; Lowry-

Webster et al., 2003; Stallard et al., 2008), 24 month and 36 month follow-up assessments

(Barrett et al., 2006). These impressive results, along with findings that the program is just as

effective when delivered by trained school staff as when delivered by psychologists (Barrett

& Turner, 2001), and it’s flexibility to be implemented as part of the standard school

curriculum, suggest this program is an ideal choice for school-based anxiety prevention.

It is worth noting that the research reviewed above has been conducted almost

exclusively within private schools, or public schools within middle to upper class

socioeconomic populations. It is acknowledged that a handful of researchers have attempted

to involved schools along the spectrum of socio-economic advantage (Barrett & Turner,

2001; Stallard et al., 2007; Stallard et al., 2008). Unfortunately however, these studies did

not explicitly report outcomes on a school-by-school basis, missing the opportunity to

specifically investigate program efficacy within disadvantaged schools. To date, only two

studies investigating the efficacy of school-based anxiety prevention in disadvantaged

communities have been published (Misfud & Rapee, 2005; Roberts et al., 2010). The latter

study, which used an alternative protocol to the FRIENDS for Life program, is notable in that

it is the only published evaluation of a universal school-based intervention program for

childhood anxiety carried out within socioeconomically disadvantaged schools. This study

will be discussed in more detail in the following chapter.

79
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

By and large, schools in socioeconomically deprived areas have been ignored as

potential research sites, resulting in a significant gap in the literature on school-based

prevention of anxiety. The current research will attempt to fill this gap in the literature, by

evaluating the delivery of a universal intervention for childhood anxiety in three schools

within a region recognised as socioeconomically disadvantaged. The issue of childhood

anxiety in low SES communities will now be discussed.

80
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

CHAPTER FIVE: CHILDHOOD ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

A link between socioeconomic status (SES) and mental illness has long been

recognised. Ever since an early study demonstrated the significant inverse relationship

between rates of schizophrenia and SES (Faris & Dunham, 1939), research has established

that adults and children from socioeconomically disadvantaged communities are at an

increased risk for psychopathology and a range of other negative health and psychosocial

outcomes (Buckner & Bassuk, 1997; Buka et al., 2002; Curtis et al., 2001; McLeod &

Shanahan, 1993). Psychiatric disorders and psychological distress are more prevalent in

adults with low income (Bruce, Takeuchi, & Leaf, 1991; Kessler, 1982), whilst children and

adolescents from poor families are significantly more likely to be in the clinical or sub-

clinical range for a range of emotional and behavioural problems (Sawyer et al., 2001).

Longitudinal research with children has found that low income predicted internalising

problems, as well as externalising difficulties, and social, attentional and thought problems,

as early as five years of age (Bor et al., 1997), whilst female adolescents whose families lived

in poverty for the first five years of their lives have been found to report greater symptoms of

anxiety and depression (Spence, Najman, Bor, O'Callaghan, & Williams, 2002). The link

between low SES and poor psychosocial outcomes is evident at the community level, and the

significant effects of community factors upon child outcomes have been demonstrated even

after controlling for personal and family characteristics (Brooks-Gunn, Duncan, & Aber,

1997; Buck, 2001; Coleman, 1988; Drukker et al., 2003) (Caspi, Taylor, Moffitt, & Plomin,

2000).

The following section reviews the link between socioeconomic disadvantage and

psychopathology, focusing specifically on research with children where available. It

81
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

discusses the magnitude of the problem of psychiatric disorders in low SES populations, and

examines barriers to mental health services commonly faced by families from disadvantaged

communities. The research reviewed supports the argument for universal intervention for

anxiety specifically within these high-risk communities, providing a rationale for the current

study.

Prevalence of Childhood Psychopathology in Disadvantaged Communities

There is a robust relationship between low SES and childhood psychopathology. In

the Great Smoky Mountains Study of Youth (Costello et al., 1996), a large-scale population-

based study investigating the need, utilisation, and development of mental health services for

children and adolescents, it was found that children of families below the poverty line were

three times more likely to meet criteria for any disorder, and every individual diagnosis

except tic disorder. More recent research, based on data collected from the Project on

Human Development in Chicago Neighborhoods, investigated levels of internalising

problems (depression, anxiety, withdrawal, and somatic problems) in 2800 children aged

between 5 and 11 years (Xue, Leventhal, Brooks-Gunn, & Earls, 2005). Significant

differences were found between neighbourhoods identified as being of low, medium, and

high SES, with 21.5%, 18.3%, and 11.5% of children ranking above clinical threshold levels

in each neighbourhood category respectively. These findings indicate the inverse relationship

between SES and internalising problems in children, demonstrating an increased risk of such

problems in conjunction with decreases in SES.

The link between SES and anxiety has also been investigated more specifically,

though research including younger populations has tended to focus on adolescents and young

adults, rather than children. Given the chronic course of anxiety however, and the tendency

82
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

of childhood anxiety to persist well into adolescence and adulthood, a review of this literature

is very relevant. Research by Kessler et al. (1994) investigated lifetime and 12-month

prevalence rates of DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association, 1987) psychiatric

disorders across several age ranges, including adolescents and early adults aged from 15 to 24

years. Anxiety disorders were significantly overrepresented in individuals from the lowest

income bracket, in terms of both 12 month and lifetime prevalence. Another interesting

finding was that low SES was more strongly related to anxiety disorders than to affective

disorders, suggesting that lower social status may exacerbate worries and stresses more so

than sadness (Kessler et al., 1994). More recently, Miech, Caspi, Moffitt, Entner Wright, and

Silva (1999) conducted a large-scale longitudinal study based on data from a Dunedin birth

cohort, which assessed the mental health of 1037 youths at ages 15 and 21. Consistent with

Kessler et al.'s (1994) research, it was found that anxiety disorders were disproportionately

represented in families of lower SES.

There remains some contention regarding the direction of the relationship between

SES and mental illness, with researchers generally adopting one of two contrasting

theoretical viewpoints (Buka et al., 2002). The selection perspective holds that individuals

with existing psychiatric disorders become disadvantaged as a result of downward mobility in

social class, resulting in the overrepresentation of mental disorders in lower socioeconomic

strata. By comparison, the causation perspective posits that low SES contributes to the

development of psychopathology through a range of mechanisms, such as stress,

occupational limitations, and poor social and psychological coping resources (Buka et al.,

2002; Miech et al., 1999). The differential effects of each process are difficult to disentangle,

though research to date points that SES is differentially related to mental illness according to

the type of disorder (Dohrenwend et al., 1992).

83
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

There have been concerted efforts by researchers to determine whether selection

theory or causation theory best explains the correlation between low SES and high anxiety.

Miech et al. (1999) attempted to unravel the differential effects of causation and selection

through investigating temporal differences in anxiety in relation to educational attainment, a

variable associated with SES, in adolescents and young adults. By age 21, it was found that

individuals with low educational attainment reported significantly higher levels of anxiety,

and that increases in anxiety between the ages of 15 and 21 were highest in participants with

low educational credentials (Miech et al., 1999). This was taken to support the utility of

causation theory - that low SES contributes to the development of psychopathology, in

explaining the link between SES and anxiety disorders.

Neighbourhood Effects of Socioeconomic Disadvantage on Childhood Anxiety

If indeed socioeconomic disadvantage precipitates anxiety and other

psychopathologies in children, it is useful to understand the factors which may contribute to

this situation. There is considerable evidence that a shared social environment imparts

significant effects on the psychological health and wellbeing of its residents, above and

beyond their individual risk profile (Coleman, 1988; Driessen, Gunther, & Van Os, 1998;

Kalff et al., 2001; Van Os, Driessen, Gunther, & Delespaul, 2000). Not surprisingly then, the

overrepresentation of psychopathology in disadvantaged communities has been explained as

a function of the characteristics of shared environments. Jencks and Mayer (1990) endorsed

4 models designed to explain how community factors may affect childhood wellbeing in

disadvantaged communities. The contagion model highlights the role of peers in influencing

a child’s behavioural outcome. The theory of collective socialisation, stipulates that child

outcomes are affected by the availability of positive adult role models, and their vigilance in

84
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

monitoring the child’s behaviour. The competition model holds that outcomes are influenced

by the degree of opportunities (such as employment and education) available to the

community. Lastly, the relative deprivation model refers to discontent experienced by

members of a community when they compare their own situation to that of others in the

community who are more fortunate. These models explore the elements of a physical

community that may be implicated in mental health outcomes at an individual level.

Later, Macintyre, MacIver, and Sooman, (1993) also discussed categories of shared

community environmental variables that are implicated in the effect of neighbourhood

socioeconomic status on population mental health. The five categories of variables identified

were (1) shared physical features (e.g. quality of air or water), (2) availability of healthy

work, school, home and play environments (e.g., decent housing, secure environments), (3)

public/private services available to support residents (e.g. education, transport, health and

welfare services), (4) socio-cultural factors (e.g. crime, community support networks) and (5)

neighbourhood reputation (how the region is perceived). These variables differ widely along

the socioeconomic spectrum (Macintyre, Ellaway, & Cummins, 2002). More recently, Ellen

and Turner (1997) also identified a set of neighbourhood factors focusing on social

connections within communities and characteristics of the community itself. These included:

(1) socialisation by adults, (2) local social networks, (3) peer influences, (4) quality of local

services, (5) exposure to crime and violence, and (6) isolation from economic opportunities.

These models share a common element: they use the interaction of socioeconomic factors and

infrastructural and social variables to explain the production of a social condition associated

with an overall lower quality of life, thereby impacting mental health at a community level.

Each of the models described above points to the social elements of a community

85
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

which may compromise the wellbeing of its citizens. These elements may be conceptualised

collectively under the concept of social capital, which in its simplest form refers to the quality

of connections within and between social networks (Coleman, 1988). Putnam (1993) later

expanded on this definition, to include community networks, civic engagement, sense of

belonging, cohesion of a community, reciprocity and cooperation, and community trust.

Social capital may be fostered and transferred through institutions that form social structures

designed to establish social bonds, such as families, schools, community groups, and

neighbourhoods in general (McKenzie, Whitley, & Weich, 2002). In regions of higher SES,

social capital manifests through more frequent and higher quality interaction between

members of the community, at a family, school, and neighbourhood level (Coleman, 1988),

contributing to a more supportive environment and leading to more positive mental health

outcomes.

By comparison, increased income inequality, a key indicator of differences in SES, is

thought to contribute to lower levels of social cohesion, trust, social control and trust, which

are key components of social capital (Aneshensel & Sucoff, 1996; Kawachi, Kennedy, &

Wilkinson, 1999; Sampson, Morenoff, & Earls, 1999; Whitley & McKenzie, 2005). Low

social capital is a neighbourhood factor that has been associated with an increased risk of

poor mental health outcomes (Coleman, 1988; Kawachi et al., 1999; Sampson et al., 1999), as

well as low neighbourhood socioeconomic status itself (Driessen et al., 1998; Kalff et al.,

2001). An inverse correlation exists between social capital and SES, such that lower levels of

social capital are predictive of higher levels of socioeconomic deprivation (Drukker et al.,

2003; van der Linden, Drukker, Gunther, Feron, & van Os, 2003; Warner & Rountree, 1997).

This interaction may provide the perfect conditions for the development of higher rates of

mental illness at a community level. With regards to internalising disorders, several studies

86
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

have demonstrated significant correlations between low social capital and social cohesion,

and anxiety and depressive symptomatology (Aneshensel & Sucoff, 1996; McCulloch, 2001;

Phongsaven, Chey, Bauman, Brooks, & Silove, 2006; Yen & Kaplan, 1999), though a recent

review concluded that this relationship is somewhat inconsistent (Whitley & McKenzie,

2005). Regardless of this inconsistency, the above evidence suggests that neighbourhood

effects on mental health are very real, and likely play a significant role in the development of

anxiety in children, particularly in socioeconomically disadvantaged communities.

In short, the research reviewed above indicates that there are clear negative effects at

the individual level of living within a disadvantaged neighbourhood, providing further

impetus for intervention specifically within these needy and high risk communities.

Barriers to Mental Health Service Utilisation in Disadvantaged Communities

Despite the fact that higher rates of mental illness are found in socioeconomically

disadvantaged communities, the utilisation of mental health services in these regions does not

correspond to this increased prevalence. Indeed, children with psychopathology from

disadvantaged communities are not only less likely than other children to receive treatment

(Cunningham & Frieman, 1996; Kazdin & Mazurick, 1994; Kazdin & Wassell, 1999; Misfud

& Rapee, 2005; Snell-Johns et al., 2004), but they are also more likely to disengage from

treatment before achieving positive treatment outcomes (Gonzales, 2005; Harrison, McKay,

& Bannon, 2004; Kazdin, Holland, & Crowley, 1997). An explanation for why children from

poor families do not receive the treatment they so desperately require necessitates the

examination of barriers to mental health service utilisation in these populations. In an

investigation of barriers hindering access to child mental health services in general, Owens et

al. (2002) identified three discreet categories of barriers: (1) structural barriers, (2) barriers

87
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

related to perceptions about mental health problems and (3) barriers related to perceptions

about mental health services. Each category will be discussed below, within the context of

socioeconomically disadvantaged populations.

Structural barriers may be described as any factors inherent to either the specific

family structure, or the mental health service structure, which may prevent an individual from

accessing an appropriate mental health service. In many cases, these barriers prevent families

from engaging with services when they have an express desire to do so. In an investigation

of mental health service usage in homeless youths and young people from low income

families, Buckner and Bassuk (1997) identified several family characteristics which may act

as structural barriers to accessing mental health services in disadvantaged communities. Lack

of social support, particularly in single-parent families, may act as a significant barrier via

placing practical and economic constraints on the parent’s time and resources (Harrison et al.,

2004). Buckner and Bassuk (1997) also identified the role of pragmatic constraints for low

income families, such as lacking transport and child care options, and competing financial

priorities such as food or accommodation, resulting in inability to pay for services. In terms

of service structure barriers, factors such as a lack of availability of providers and increased

wait times for treatment have been cited (Owens et al., 2002), due largely to the inability of

community mental health facilities to meet increasing demand for services (Donovan &

Spence, 2000). Unfortunately then, timely intervention may only be available via costly

treatment in a private setting, which is beyond the financial means of most families in

disadvantaged settings.

The perceptions of parents, teachers, and health care providers about mental illness in

children may also pose a significant barrier to accessing child-appropriate mental health

88
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

services. Parents and teachers in particular may have a poor understanding of the signs and

symptoms of mental illness in children, leaving them ill-equipped to identify the need for

assistance (Buckner & Bassuk, 1997; Owens et al., 2002). This is even more pertinent in the

case of childhood internalising disorders such as anxiety; internalising symptomatology may

be less identifiable in a classroom setting, where children exhibiting externalising behaviours

typically attract greater attention from teaching staff (Silverman & Treffers, 2001). Even

where an issue of concern is recognised, treatment may not be sought due to the belief that

difficulties will remit independently of intervention, leaving the child to suffer in silence.

Other perceptual barriers which may discourage service utilisation include the family being

daunted by the complexities of the referral and admission process, or perhaps more simply, a

lack of knowledge and understanding on how best to access and use the services and

resources available (Koroloff, Elliott, Koren, & Friesen, 1996).

Lastly, mental health service utilisation may be compromised by barriers related to

perceptions about these services. Parents and caregivers who do not trust mental health

services may be less likely to engage with these services for their child (Owens et al., 2002).

Where families do engage, this mistrust is likely to compromise not only the relationship

between the parent/caregiver and therapist, but also the parent/caregiver’s perceptions of

treatment relevance and effectiveness, which may result in early termination (Kazdin et al.,

1997). Another perceptual factor implicated in the underutilisation of child mental health

services is the stigma of mental illness. Despite acknowledging their child’s psychological

difficulties, parents may be apprehensive about seeking treatment both for fear of being

blamed, and concern that their child may be labelled and stigmatised (Gonzales, 2005).

Lastly, previous negative experiences with mental health services may inhibit future help-

seeking behaviours by disadvantaged families (Kerkorian, McKay, & Bannon, 2006; Owens

89
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

et al., 2002).

Breaking Down the Barriers: School-based Prevention of Anxiety

It is now clear that the community mental health model does not appear to be working

for families from socioeconomically disadvantaged communities. Despite the increased need

for services, the significant barriers to accessing services mean that these children either do

not receive the treatment they require, or may terminate prematurely. Hence the challenge

has been to develop a model of service delivery which overcomes as many of these barriers

as possible, whilst still providing a service which produces meaningful changes in mental

health outcomes for disadvantaged populations. Currently, that model is school-based

prevention. The school is an ideal access point to large numbers of children simultaneously,

whilst affording an excellent milieu for the population-based delivery of effective

interventions for several mental health issues (Misfud & Rapee, 2005). The school setting

has been used extensively and successfully in the past for brief interventions regarding

psychosocial issues pertinent to young people, such as substance use, unprotected sex,

violence, and depression (Gonzales, 2005; Hutchinson & Poole, 1998), as well as for health

promotion (Bond, Glover, Godfrey, Butler, & Patton, 2001) and the delivery of ongoing

mental health services (Weist, Sander, Axelrod-Lowie, & Christodulu, 2002).

School-based intervention, particularly when delivered in a universal format,

effectively neutralises many pragmatic and perceptual barriers to accessing traditional

community-based mental health services for children. Given that these barriers are typically

greater in socioeconomically disadvantaged communities, this intervention model is perhaps

especially suited to these populations. Firstly, given that the intervention is delivered as part

of the school curriculum during regular class time, barriers such as transportation and child

90
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

care difficulties are effectively eradicated (Neil & Christensen, 2009). Universal school-

based intervention also alleviates service structure barriers through a more efficient delivery

model, relieving the pressure on inundated community mental health services. Furthermore,

universal school-based intervention overcomes the barrier of anxious children not being

correctly identified as requiring treatment (Owens et al., 2002), given that all children,

regardless of risk profile, receive the intervention. School-based programs also reduces the

stigma attached to treatment for mental illness (Armbruster, 2002), which has been identified

as a significant barrier to traditional service utilisation (Gonzales, 2005). In this way,

universal school-based interventions normalise the experience of anxiety for children and

their families, by delivering services in a supportive, non-threatening, and familiar

environment.

School-Based Prevention of Childhood Anxiety in Low SES Communities

Universal school-based interventions for anxiety have been extensively researched

and evaluated for the past decade (see Chapter 4: Universal Prevention), with a wealth of

research establishing their effectiveness in significantly reducing anxiety in children and

adolescents (see Neil and Christensen, 2009, for a review). However, as addressed in the

previous chapter, research which exclusively evaluates school-based interventions for anxiety

in regions of socioeconomic disadvantage is almost non-existent. This gap in the literature is

alarming, given that these communities are at greater risk of anxiety psychopathology. To

the best of the author’s knowledge, only two studies have focused exclusively on the school-

based prevention of childhood anxiety in disadvantaged communities, at any level of

prevention. Both of these studies have been conducted in Australia, and will be discussed

below.

91
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

The first study, conducted by Misfud and Rapee (2005), evaluated a school-based

CBT intervention for anxiety delivered at the selective level of prevention. The study

involved nine schools in Western Sydney, Australia, which were selected due to their high

concentration of socioeconomically disadvantaged families (Misfud & Rapee, 2005). From

an initial large scale screening process, 91 children (8 to 11 years) were selected for inclusion

based on elevated anxiety symptoms, as determined by child self-report and teacher

nomination. Children from five schools were allocated to the treatment condition, and those

from the remaining four schools comprised the wait-list control group. The intervention

delivered was based on the school version of the Cool Kids program (Lyneham, Abbott,

Wignall, & Rapee, 2003; Rapee, Wignall, Hudson, & Schniering, 2000), a CBT program for

the prevention of childhood anxiety. Program components included psychoeducation about

anxiety, cognitive restructuring, exposure hierarchies for feared stimuli, social skills,

assertiveness training, and coping with teasing. The program was delivered in eight weekly

sessions during normal school time, which were supplemented by two parent information

sessions. At the conclusion of the program, participants in the intervention condition

demonstrated significant decreases in anxiety symptoms relative to the wait-list control

group, both on self-report and teacher report measures, with positive gains maintained at 4

months follow-up (Misfud & Rapee, 2005).

The second, more recent study was conducted by Roberts et al., (2010), and to date is

the only published study investigating universal school-based prevention of anxiety

exclusively in disadvantaged schools. This study involved 496 students in Grade 7 (aged 11

to 13 years) from 12 public primary schools in Perth, Western Australia, which were selected

based on the Census Index of Relative Socioeconomic Status, generated by the Australian

Bureau of Statistics. Half of the schools were randomly allocated to the intervention

92
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

condition and the other half to a control condition. Internalising symptoms were measured

using both child report and parent report questionnaires. The intervention delivered was the

Aussie Optimism Program. This program includes two components: Social Life Skills (SLS;

Roberts, Ballantyne, & van der Klift, 2003), designed to assist children with deficits in social

skills and social problem solving, low social support, and friendship difficulties, and

Optimistic Thinking Skills (OTS; Roberts et al., 2003) which focuses on reducing negative

cognitive elements such as pessimistic attribution style, negative self perceptions and future

expectations. The program was delivered by school teachers as a series of 60-minute lessons

over a 20-week period. Control group participants received 20 regular health education

lessons relating to self-management and interpersonal skills during this period.

The results of the above study were mixed. Based on child-report measures of

anxiety and depression, there were no significant differences in anxiety and depressive

symptomatology between the intervention and control groups post-intervention, or at the 6-

month or 18-month follow-up time points. Based on child-report measures, it appeared that

the intervention had no effect on levels of anxiety and depression. Some positive results were

found however in the comparison of parent-reported measures, which were inconsistent with

child-reported data. More specifically, parents of children in the intervention group reported

significant decreases in internalising symptoms post-treatment, relative to parents of children

in the control condition. Despite these positive findings, unfortunately the gains made by the

intervention group were not maintained over time; these between-group differences had

disappeared by 6 months follow-up, and there was no significant difference in parent-

reported internalising symptoms between the intervention and control groups at 18 months

post-intervention (Roberts et al., 2010). The results indicate that the intervention may have

93
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

resulted in some improvements in child anxiety symptoms in the short term, but that it was

largely ineffective in reducing childhood anxiety in the longer term.

The above two studies are significant in that they go some way towards addressing a

neglected area of research, through addressing anxiety prevention in low SES communities.

In this sense, these studies are innovative, and both have several strengths. In both studies,

the inclusion of a no-treatment comparison group (Misfud & Rapee, 2005), and an

alternative, non-active program (Roberts et al., 2010) allowed for more conclusive analysis of

potential significant effects of the intervention. The study by Misfud and Rapee (2005) also

indicated that a more brief CBT intervention (8 sessions, as compared to the FRIENDS for

Life 10 sessions) may significantly reduce anxiety in children with elevated symptoms. This

is useful information, given that some school terms are only brief, and may only be able to

accommodate eight weekly intervention sessions. An obvious strength in the study by

Roberts et al. (2010) lies in it’s sample size, with almost 500 students across 12 different

schools involved. Additionally, this study employed a more intensive follow-up schedule,

with data collected at both 6 months and 18 months post-intervention. This is quite

impressive, given that students from the original sample would have transitioned to high

school by the final follow-up time-point. An additional strength is that this study employs

not only child self-report questionnaires, but also parental report questionnaires, providing a

collateral measure of emotional functioning. Furthermore, the inclusion of a measure

assessing child-reported social skills enabled this study to examine any potential changes in

this aspect of functioning post-intervention.

Whilst these two studies are innovative, neither is without limitations. A key

limitation of the study by Misfud and Rapee (2005) is that it employs a selective prevention

model, rather than a universal prevention model. It is already well established that selective

94
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

group-based intervention is effective for those at risk for anxiety. What is not known is

whether a less concentrated ‘dosage’ of an intervention (such that would be delivered in a

universal intervention) is sufficient, given the larger proportion of mental illness in

disadvantaged populations. An additional limitation is that the question of potential long-

term gains associated with the chosen intervention remains unanswered. It would be

interesting to determine whether treatment gains were maintained beyond 4 months follow-

up, especially given that this sample was comprised of children with existing elevated anxiety

symptoms. It is also noteworthy that this research evaluated an alternative anxiety prevention

program to the FRIENDS for Life program. Whilst both programs are based upon a CBT

framework, which is identified as the gold standard for childhood anxiety treatment,

FRIENDS for Life has been more extensively researched. This broad and ever-growing

FRIENDS evidence base provides empirical support for the effectiveness of the program

across a range of different settings and populations, therefore affording greater opportunities

for comparison of outcomes with more current research.

The more recent study by Roberts et al. (2010) is somewhat more ambitious in it’s

scope, but the impact of this research is unfortunately compromised by inconsistent results.

Even so, despite the lack of significant self-reported reductions in anxiety and depression for

children receiving the intervention, the significant decrease in internalising symptoms

reported by parents does offer some promise for future research. Another limitation is that

this study only includes children from Grade 7, preventing the examination of any possible

effects of age or grade on intervention effectiveness. As with the research conducted by

Misfud and Rapee (2005), Roberts et al. (2010) evaluate the effectiveness of an alternative

intervention to the FRIENDS for Life program, the Aussie Optimism Program. A key point

here is the authors’ acknowledgement that the program evaluated lacks empirical evidence

95
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

demonstrating it’s efficacy as a treatment for anxiety and depression, both over the short term

and the long term (Roberts et al., 2010), irrespective of participants’ socioeconomic status.

Given that the authors stated that the Aussie Optimism Program is designed to target students

attending schools in low socioeconomic areas, the failure to achieve more significant results

suggests that perhaps this program is not best suited for implementation in these areas.

Therefore, it remains to be seen whether a more empirically validated program, such as

FRIENDS for Life, may deliver the more consistent, predicted changes that the above

research failed to demonstrate.

The above review of the literature on the prevention of childhood anxiety in

socioeconomically disadvantaged communities highlights several issues. Firstly, prevention

of childhood anxiety in disadvantaged schools is a significantly neglected field of research,

both within Australia and internationally. Secondly, the results yielded by the few studies

conducted in this field to date suggest that intervention within these populations may result in

clinically significant decreases in anxiety, highlighting the need for future research in the

area. Furthermore, given that research to date has been conducted using two less empirically

validated interventions for anxiety, research evaluating an alternative, more extensively

validated program for childhood anxiety is highly warranted. The current study is structured

to address this gap in the literature, and it will be discussed more extensively in the following

section.

The Current Study

A review of the available literature indicates that anxiety is a significant childhood

mental health problem, and despite the magnitude of this problem, the majority of children

will unfortunately not receive treatment (Hirschfeld et al., 1997; Sawyer et al., 2000). This

fact takes on new significance in the context regions of low SES, given that the risk for

96
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

anxiety disorders is magnified for residents of these regions (Sawyer et al., 2001). When

taken together with the knowledge that disadvantaged families face significant barriers to

accessing mental health services for children (Owens et al., 2002), universal school-based

prevention programs appear to be the most practical and logical option to provide timely

treatment to children who need it most. Whilst considerable evidence points to the

effectiveness of such school-based programs in reducing anxiety in children and adolescents

(Neil & Christensen, 2009), to date, research specifically with disadvantaged populations has

been slim. The available evidence suggests that such programs may be effective (Misfud &

Rapee, 2005; Roberts et al., 2010), however further research with more empirically validated

anxiety prevention programs is desperately required.

This study constitutes the first ever evaluation of a universal school-based prevention

program for childhood anxiety in disadvantaged schools using the FRIENDS for Life

program, an effective intervention for childhood anxiety that has been validated across all

three levels of prevention. This research encompassed students in Grades 5, 6, and 7, from

three public primary schools within Queensland, Australia. This age-group was selected

based on the finding that the upper primary school years provide an ideal window for the

prevention of internalising disorders in children (Barrett et al., 2006; Lock & Barrett, 2003;

Roth & Dadds, 1999). In the current study, the manualised FRIENDS for Life program was

delivered by classroom teachers who were trained to administer program, during normal class

time within a standard school term. Outcomes were recorded post-treatment, and at 12

months follow-up.

The primary objective of this research was to evaluate the effectiveness of the

FRIENDS for Life program when delivered as a universal, school-based program in three

97
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

schools from a socioeconomically disadvantaged region. Only two studies have attempted to

evaluate prevention of anxiety within disadvantaged schools exclusively. These studies,

neither of which employed the FRIENDS for Life protocol, yielded mixed results. The

ultimate goal was to provide support for the use of this intervention as a curriculum-based

program for anxiety in schools, in particular those within socioeconomically disadvantaged

regions.

FRIENDS for Life has been extensively validated as an effective intervention for

anxiety when delivered as a school-based program. In light of this, for the current study it

was predicted that anxiety symptoms would decrease from pre to post-intervention and

follow-up, based on child self-report measures. Similarly, as FRIENDS for Life has been

shown to effectively reduce depression in children, it was predicted that reductions in

depressive symptoms from pre to post-treatment and follow-up would be revealed, based on

child self-report measures.

The secondary focus of the current study was to examine the effects of the

intervention on both risk and protective factors and other indicators of psychosocial

functioning. It was predicted that use of positive coping skills, (assistance-seeking and

cognitive-behavioural problem-solving) would increase from pre to post-intervention and

follow-up. Accordingly, it was also predicted that the use of maladaptive coping skills,

(cognitive avoidance and behavioural avoidance), would decrease from pre to post-

intervention and follow-up. It was also hypothesised that self-esteem (social self-esteem and

school esteem) would increase from pre to post-intervention and follow-up. Lastly, it was

predicted that emotional problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity and inattention, and peer

98
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

relationship problems would decrease from pre to post-intervention and follow-up, and that

pro-social behaviour would increase from pre to post-intervention and follow-up.

Finally, the current study sought to examine predictors of outcome with regards to

internalising symptoms post-intervention. Predictors included pre-intervention anxiety

symptoms, as well as pre-intervention positive coping skills (assistance-seeking and

cognitive-behavioural problem-solving), maladaptive coping skills (cognitive avoidance and

behavioural avoidance), self-esteem (social self-esteem and school esteem), emotional

problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems, and pro-

social behaviour. It was hypothesised that children with higher levels of self-reported anxiety

symptomatology at pre-intervention would demonstrate the greatest reductions in anxiety and

depressive symptomatology at post-intervention. With regards to coping skills, it was

predicted that children who reported using more maladaptive coping skills (cognitive

avoidance and behavioural avoidance) at pre-intervention would demonstrate greater

reductions in internalising symptoms at post-intervention. Similarly, it was predicted that

children with lower social self-esteem and school esteem at pre-intervention would

demonstrate greater reductions in internalising symptoms at post-intervention. Finally, it was

predicted that children with a higher level of emotional problems, conduct problems,

hyperactivity/inattention, and peer problems at pre-intervention, would show greater

reductions in internalising symptoms post-intervention.

99
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

CHAPTER SIX

Method

Participants

The sample for the present study consisted of 963 children attending three public

primary schools located in Logan City, an urban local government region located South of

Brisbane, Australia. The sample consisted of 323 students in Grade 5, 340 students in Grade

6, and 300 students in Grade 7. The schools were matched for socio-economic level of

families attending the school. The sample consisted of 494 males and 469 females.

The three schools were selected based on their inclusion in a Community Renewal

program, an initiative of the Queensland Government Department of Housing. The

Community Renewal program involves collaboration between individual communities and

governments, focusing on addressing each community’s top priorities, such as community

safety, education, economic development, health and well-being, community infrastructure,

and accessibility of local services. Areas are selected based on a range of social and

economic indicators, and projects are jointly funded by the Queensland government, local

councils, businesses, and community organisations. The three schools included in this

research are located within the Marsden/Crestmead Community Renewal zone, a

geographical area within Logan City.

To officially quantify the level of socioeconomic disadvantage for the population

examined, data from the 2006 Census (ABS, 2008) was used to establish that the three

schools were located within a Statistical Local Area (SLA) associated with higher levels of

100
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

socioeconomic disadvantage. To establish this, data for the relevant SLAs were investigated

using the Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA), a four-index analysis providing

relative rankings of SLAs according to level of social and economic wellbeing. The four

indices are: Index of relative socio-economic disadvantage; Index of relative socio-economic

advantage and disadvantage; Index of economic resources; and Index of Education and

Occupation. For the purpose of this research, the Index of relative socioeconomic

disadvantage was selected as the primary index of disadvantage, based on the Australian

Bureau of Statistics’ recommendation of its utility in comparing disadvantage to lack of

disadvantage, and as a broad measure of disadvantage in general (ABS, 2006). This index

summarises 17 different measures, including low income, low educational attainment, high

unemployment, and unskilled occupations (ABS, 2006). A full list of the 17 measures is

available in Figure 1.

 % Occupied private dwellings with no internet connection


 % Employed people classified as Labourers
 % People aged 15 years and over with no post-school qualifications
 % People with stated annual household equivalised income between
$13,000 and $20,799 (approx. 2nd and 3rd deciles)
 % Households renting from Government or Community organisation
 % People (in the labour force) unemployed
 % One parent families with dependent offspring only
 % Households paying rent less than $120 per week (excluding $0 per
week)
 % People aged under 70 who have a long-term health condition or
disability and need assistance with core activities
 % Occupied private dwellings with no car
 % People who identified themselves as being of Aboriginal and/or Torres
Straight Islander origin
 % Occupied private dwellings requiring one or more extra bedrooms
(based on Canadian National Occupancy Standard)
 % People aged 15 years and over who are separated or divorced
 % Employed people classified as Machinery Operators and Drivers
 % People aged 15 years and over who did not go to school
 % Employed people classified as Low Skill Community and Personal
Service Workers
 % People who do not speak English well

Figure 1. Variables used to calculate the index of relative socioeconomic disadvantage

101
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

The SEIFA index of relative socioeconomic disadvantage yields several measures of

disadvantage for each SLA. The SEIFA rank is used to rank order all SLAs from lowest to

highest, based on relative socioeconomic disadvantage, where the lowest rank possible is 1.

The SEIFA decile divides areas into 10 equal groups; areas scoring in the lowest 10%

(indicating greatest socioeconomic disadvantage) are assigned decile number 1. The areas in

the highest 10% (indicating lowest socioeconomic disadvantage) are assigned decile number

10. Lastly, the SEIFA percentile places all regions on a percentile scale from 1 to 100, based

on relative socioeconomic disadvantage, where lower numbers indicate greatest

socioeconomic disadvantage. Ranks, deciles, and percentiles are available at both the

national and state level.

Two of the schools included in the current research were located within the SLA of

Marsden, whilst the third was located within the SLA of Waterford West. The SEIFA

measures of disadvantage (rank, decile, and percentile) for both these SLA regions, taken

from the 2006 Census data (ABS, 2008), are presented below in Table 2. These figures

demonstrate that both regions are areas of relative socioeconomic disadvantage, on both a

state and a national level.

Table 2

Measures of the SEIFA index of socioeconomic disadvantage by SLA

Marsden Waterford West


National SEIFA Rank 217 of 1394 SLAs 234 of 1394 SLAs
National SEIFA Decile 2 2
National SEIFA Percentile 16 17
Queensland SEIFA Rank 73 of 476 SLAs 82 of 476 SLAs
Queensland SEIFA Decile 2 2
Queensland SEIFA Percentile 16 18

Note. SEIFA = Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas; SLA = Statistical Local Area

102
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Measures

Strengths and difficulties questionnaire. The Strengths and Difficulties

Questionnaire (SDQ) (Goodman, 1997) is a 25 item self-report measure of psychological

adjustment for use with children aged 3 to 16 years. The items are divided between five

scales: emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship

problems, and pro-social behaviour. Participants are required to endorse either “not true” (0),

“somewhat true” (1), or “certainly true” (2) in response to each statement, with higher scores

indicative of greater problems for each subscale except for pro-social behaviour. The SDQ

has sound psychometric properties, including moderate to strong internal reliability for all

subscales, good test-retest reliability (Vostanis, 2006), concurrent validity, and the ability to

distinguish between community and clinical samples (Goodman, 2001; Goodman & Scott,

1999).

Self-esteem inventory. The Self-Esteem Inventory (SEI) (Coopersmith, 1967) is a

58-item self-report measure appropriate for use with children aged 8 to 15 years. The

measure consists of four subscales and a lie scale. The four subscales assess four separate

constructs of self-esteem: general self-esteem (e.g., “Things usually don’t bother me”); social

self-esteem (e.g., “I’m easy to like”); home esteem (e.g., “My parents understand me”); and

school esteem (e.g., “I’m doing the best work that I can”). Participants are required to

endorse either “like me” (1) or “unlike me” (0) in response to each statement, with higher

scores on each subscale indicative of higher self-esteem. The SEI has demonstrated sound

psychometric properties (Coopersmith, 1967, 1989), including good convergent validity and

an internal consistency of 0.86 (Robertson & Miller, 1986) (Kokenes, 1978). For the current

study, in the interest of condensing the size of the questionnaire battery, only items which

103
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

were used to calculate social self-esteem (eight items) and school esteem (eight items)

subscales were included.

Children’s depression inventory. The Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI)

(Kovacs, 1981) is a 27-item self-report measure of depressive symptomatology in children

and adolescents. Items are designed to tap cognitive, behavioural, and affective symptoms of

depression. Participants are required to select from one of three given statements for each

item, which best represents their current position in terms of cognitions, behaviours, or

emotions. Depending on level of severity endorsed by the participant, items are scored as

either “0”, “1”, or “2”. A total score is derived by summing together the responses for each

item, with a higher score indicating a higher likelihood of depression. For the current study,

a cut-off score of 16 was used to indicate the presence of moderate to severe levels of

depressive symptoms, which is supported by the literature (Kovacs, 1992; Roberts, Kane,

Thomson, Bishop, & Hart, 2003). The CDI has demonstrated sound psychometric

properties, with high internal consistency and moderate test-restest reliability (Saylor, Finch,

Spirito, & Bennett, 1984), and good convergent validity in discriminating between clinically

depressed and non-depressed children in inpatient and non-referred groups (Kovacs, 1992;

Lobotvits & Handal, 1985; Saylor et al., 1984). For the current study, and consistent with

earlier research (Hannon, Rapee, & Hudson, 2000; Shochet et al., 2001), one item regarding

suicide was omitted so as not to cause concern to participants, parents, or teaching staff.

Research has shown that the removal of the suicide item does not significantly alter CDI

scores (Weiss et al., 1991).

Revised children’s manifest anxiety scale. The Revised Children’s Manifest

Anxiety Scale (Reynolds & Richmond, 1978) is a 37-item self-report measure of trait anxiety

104
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

in children. A total of 28 items pertain to trait anxiety, with a further nine items assessing

social desirability. Items are presented as a series of statements, which participants are

required to endorse as being either true or not true of them, by indicating either “yes” (1) or

“no” (0) respectively. A total score is derived by summing together the responses for each

item, with a higher score indicating greater levels of anxiety. The RCMAS has sound

psychometric properties, with good convergent validity (Reynolds, 1980), high internal

consistency, and moderate test-retest reliability (Reynolds & Richmond, 1985).

Spence children’s anxiety scale. The Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale (SCAS)

(Spence, 1997) is a 44-item self-report measure of child anxiety suitable for use in

community samples. The SCAS consists of 38 items assess anxiety symptoms, including

obsessions and compulsions, separation anxiety, social phobia, panic, agoraphobia,

generalised anxiety, and physical injury concerns, which correspond to DSM-IV (American

Psychiatric Association, 1994) anxiety disorder subtypes. The scale is also composed of an

additional six positive filler items designed to reduce negative response bias, which were

omitted in the current study. Children respond through rating the frequency with which they

experience each symptom on a 4-point scale, from “never” (0), to “always” (3). Scores from

the 38 anxiety items are summed to produce a total anxiety score, with higher scores

indicating more severe difficulties. The SCAS was found to have high internal consistency,

satisfactory test-retest reliability, and adequate convergent and divergent validity (Spence,

1998; Spence, Barrett, & Turner, 2003). For the present study, a cut-off score of 42 was used

to identify children at high risk for anxiety. This cut-off was recommended by the author of

the scale (Spence, 1997), and has been used in previous research (Barrett, Farrell et al., 2006;

Barrett & Turner, 2001; Lock & Barrett, 2003).

105
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Coping scale for children and youth. The Coping Scale for Children and Youth

(CSCY) (Brodzinsky et al., 1992) is a 29-item self-report measure with four factors related to

coping: assistance seeking (e.g., sharing one’s feelings with another person),

cognitive/behavioural problem-solving (e.g., developing and following a plan to solve a

problem), cognitive avoidance (e.g., pretending the problem does not exist), and behavioural

avoidance (e.g., physically avoiding a problem situation). Children are requested to identify

a current or recent life stressor, and indicate the frequency with which they use each given

coping strategy in relation to this stressor, from “not at all” (1) to “very often” (4). Items

corresponding to each factor are calculated to yield a mean score for each four subscales.

The CSCY has moderate to high internal reliabilities for each of the four factors (ranging

from 0.70 to 0.80), with test-retest reliabilities within each of the factors ranging from 0.70 to

0.83 (Brodzinsky et al., 1992).

Procedure

Phase one: teacher training. Prior to the commencement of the program, all regular

classroom teachers of Grades 5, 6, and 7 at each of the three schools participated in a one-day

intensive training workshop, which provided education on childhood anxiety and depression,

theory behind the FRIENDS for Life program, and instruction in delivery of the FRIENDS for

Life program. The training workshops were delivered by postgraduate (doctorate and PhD)

psychology students, who were also accredited FRIENDS for Life trainers. The teacher

training sessions also included an introduction to the screening measures that would be used

as part of the research, and instruction on how to administer the questionnaires in a group

format. All training sessions were held in each of the three schools, over a period of one

week.

106
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Phase two: pre-intervention screening. Parents of all children in Grades 5, 6, and 7

of each of the three schools were sent an information sheet informing them of the research to

be conducted within their schools, and describing the data collection process and the

intervention itself. It was made clear that the intervention would be delivered during regular

class time, and at no cost to parents. No parents refused to allow their child to participate in

the program. This high acceptance rate may be due to a higher level of familiarity with the

FRIENDS program, given that past pupils of the schools had been involved in previous

research projects incorporating the FRIENDS program, which had included parent

information evenings and training sessions for teachers.

Students participated in the initial screening process, which was conducted within a

one-week period at each of the schools involved. Screening of each grade varied according

to time-tabling, and in one school a composite class of Grade 6 and 7 students was screened

simultaneously. Screening was completed in either classrooms or multi-purpose rooms

within each of the schools, with each student sitting at their own desk or in their own space,

to ensure the privacy of responses.

Prior to the commencement of the screening, participants were informed that all

responses were confidential, and that responses would only be viewed by the research staff.

Participants were also informed that the screening process was not a school test, and that

there were no right or wrong answers on the questionnaires. Screening within each school

was facilitated jointly by teaching staff and postgraduate psychology students, with a

minimum of one teacher and one postgraduate student at every screening session. Every

question on each of the six questionnaires was read aloud, in chronological order, by either

teaching or research staff, to ensure all participants comprehended the question regardless of

107
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

differences in age and academic ability. Pauses were given after each question to allow

participants to record their responses, and participants were encouraged to raise their hand if

they did not understand a question, and a facilitator was able to assist them.

Phase three: intervention. The intervention phase commenced approximately one

week following the screening phase, with some variation in start dates both within and

between schools. The intervention was run on a class-by-class basis, with all program

sessions facilitated by the regular classroom teacher. Classroom teachers were instructed that

the program must be completed within one schooling term, with a recommended delivery

schedule of one session per week. Teachers were instructed that, provided all material within

each session was covered, and that each session was covered in chronological order, they did

not need to deliver each session as a whole block, but were able to split session content

between different time slots. The flexibility of the program delivery was necessary due to

individual differences in the curriculum between grades, timetabling differences between and

within schools, and the progress and composition of each individual class.

All children participating in the program received a copy of the FRIENDS for Life

workbook (Barrett, 2004), which they were able to keep at the completion of the program.

Classroom teachers were also supplied with a copy of the program manual (Barrett, 2004),

with detailed instruction on material to be covered in each session, and suggested activities

for each program component. Parents were encouraged to participate through attendance at

parent evening sessions held during the course of the intervention. These sessions, delivered

by a postgraduate psychology student and accredited FRIENDS for Life group facilitator,

were designed to provide education about childhood emotional development, anxiety and

depression in children, and instruction in various strategies that parents could use to assist

108
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

their child in reducing their anxiety. These sessions were held in lieu of parental attendance

in the final stages of each session, which was unfeasible given that the sessions were

delivered during normal class time. Parent sessions were held in the evenings. Given the

poor attendance rates at four parent sessions in a previous universal school-based evaluation

of the FRIENDS for Life program (Barrett & Turner, 2001), only two parent sessions per

school were held.

Phase four: post-intervention screening. The post-intervention screening process

was conducted within one week at each of the schools, approximately one week following

completion of the last intervention session. As with the pre-intervention screening process,

post-intervention screening was completed either in classrooms or multi-purpose rooms in

each school, with each participant sitting individually to ensure the privacy of responses.

Participants were reminded again that all responses on the questionnaires were confidential,

and would only be viewed by the research staff. The screening was co-facilitated by teaching

and research staff. Again, all questions were read aloud to all participants.

Phase five: 12 months follow-up screening. At 12 months following completion of

the intervention, a postgraduate student returned to each of the three schools to assist

classroom teachers in completing the 12 months follow-up screening assessment of

participants. As the intervention had been completed with Grade 7 students of the previous

year, the 12 months follow-up screening process was only able to be completed with Grade 6

and Grade 7 students of the current year (Grade 5 and Grade 6 students of the intervention

year). The follow-up screening procedure was identical to that used in both the pre screening

and post screening phases.

109
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Intervention Protocol

FRIENDS for Life is a brief, group-based CBT for clinical anxiety in children

(Barrett, 2004). The program teaches children skills and techniques to manage anxiety and

cope with difficult situations. The primary components of the program include relaxation,

cognitive restructuring, attention training, graded exposure to anxiety-provoking situations,

and problem-solving, all of which are facilitated by peer and family support (Barrett &

Turner, 2004). The program originated as the Coping Koala program (Barrett, Dadds et al.,

1996), an Australian adaptation of the USA-originated Coping Cat program (Kendall, 1990).

The development of the Coping Koala program into a group intervention program resulted in

the birth of the FRIENDS program (Barrett, 1998; Shortt, Barrett, & Fox, 2001). The current

program is available in two developmentally appropriate versions: FRIENDS for Life (for

children aged 7 to 11 years) (Barrett, 2004) and FRIENDS for Youth (for children aged 12 to

16 years) (Barrett, 2005). Most recently, a downward extension of FRIENDS for Life,

entitled Fun FRIENDS (Barrett, 2007), has been developed for use with preschool-aged

children. The current research evaluated FRIENDS for Life, which is suitable for upper

primary school-aged children.

FRIENDS for Life is a manualised group-based intervention comprised of 12 sessions

of approximately 1.5 hours’ duration. In a clinic setting, the program is designed to be run as

10 weekly sessions and two booster sessions, held one and three months respectively after the

completion of the intervention. The recommended group size in a clinical setting is 6 to 10

participants. Whilst the intervention can be run individually, the group intervention process

helps to encourage and nurture participants to explore their emotions in a safe, supportive,

and collaborative environment. The FRIENDS program may be delivered by a single

110
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

facilitator, but the addition of a co-facilitator is helpful to provide both individual and small-

group support to participants, as well as ensuring the sessions are run smoothly.

In a clinic setting, participants’ parents and siblings are encouraged to join the group

in the final 20 minutes of a session, allowing children to practice and reflect back what they

have learned in session by engaging in activities related to program skills with their parents.

This component of the sessions is useful in that it provides children with a sense of

accomplishment and reinforces learned material by allowing children to assume the role of

‘teacher’. It also demonstrates to parents the major themes and concepts covered in session,

so that they may better support their child in using FRIENDS strategies at home. This is also

facilitated by a number of ‘homework’ tasks, which encourage the practice of program skills

by all members of the family. In a school setting, this family component may be delivered as

a series of parent workshops, delivered at interval during the intervention.

The primary skills emphasised in the FRIENDS program are each denoted by a letter

in the FRIENDS anagram, each of which are discussed below. A breakdown of program

components covered by session is presented in Table 3.

Feelings. The feelings skill focuses on affective education, teaching children to

understand the range of emotions in themselves and in others, assisting in the development of

empathy. Children are taught to identify physiological signs (e.g. butterflies in the stomach,

muscle tension, racing heartbeat) and behavioural indicators (e.g. avoidance) of anxiety. It is

emphasised that these so-called “body clues” are normal, and provide the child with the

opportunity to explore what they are feeling. The family component of this skill encourages

family members to openly discuss and explore each other’s feelings, and practice focusing on

111
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

the physiological responses to anxiety, thereby further normalising the experience of anxiety

in response to stressors.

Remember to relax. Have quiet time. This component teaches children relaxation

and self regulation skills in response to physiological arousal due to anxiety. Body clues are

identified as a ‘sign’ that the body needs to relax. Children receive instruction in several

relaxation techniques, including diaphragmatic breathing, progressive muscle relaxation, and

visual imagery, and are given the chance to practice in session and reflect on their experience

of relaxation. Families are encouraged to support this component through regular practice of

relaxation strategies at home. They are also asked to prioritise quiet time by way of

preventing stress and anxiety over the longer term. Parents may assist their children in

brainstorming relaxation and quiet time activities, such as listening to music, drawing

pictures, or going for a walk in the park.

I can do it! I can try my best! This third step comprises a key component of the

cognitive element of the FRIENDS for Life program, and an introduction to the concepts of

self-talk and thought challenging. Children are taught how to access their inner thoughts

(self-talk), and learn to discriminate between unhelpful “red” thoughts, and helpful “green”

thoughts. Red thoughts are identified as those which can make people feel sad and scared,

whilst green thoughts help people to feel happy and brave. Children are exposed to several

generic vignettes that may trigger red unhelpful thoughts, and are encouraged to challenge

these with more positive green thoughts. Children are then assisted to generate a situation

which is personally relevant, and practice challenging their red thoughts with positive green

thoughts. A secondary aspect of this component involves teaching children attention training

strategies, enabling them to identify and focus on the positive aspects of different scenarios.

112
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Parents can nurture these skills at home by encouraging children to challenge negative

thinking, and to use green thoughts to improve mood and coping. Parents can assist both by

modelling this behaviour themselves, and through explicitly acknowledging and reinforcing

their child’s attempts at thought challenging.

Explore solutions and coping step plans. The fourth step of the FRIENDS for Life

program is comprised of two core learning components designed to help children cope in

challenging situations. The first is the coping step plan, a graded exposure hierarchy for a

feared or anxiety-provoking situation. Children are assisted in developing their own step plan

involving gradual exposure and response prevention, which will be followed for the

remainder of the program. The second component is instruction in problem solving,

presented to participants as the ‘Six Block Problem Solving Plan.’ This method includes the

following steps: (1) What is the problem - Define it!, (2) Brainstorm - list all possible

solutions, (3) List what might happen for each solution, (4) Select the best solution based on

the consequences, (5) Make a plan for putting this solution into practice and do it!, and finally

(6) Evaluate the outcome in terms of strengths and weaknesses, and if it did not work return

to step 2 and try again. The family element of this step involves teaching parents to create

their own coping step plans, and encouraging them to model the procedure at home. The

coping step plan may not necessarily be related to a feared situation, but may comprise a plan

for achieving a specific goal.

Now reward yourself! You’ve done your best! The rewards skill encourages

children to acknowledge their efforts and progress towards achieving their goals, and

recognise that trying one’s best, as well as succeeding, is deserving of a reward. This

component also introduces the notion of support networks, or ‘support teams’, which include

113
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

role models within the home, school, and wider community. Children are encouraged to

think of who might be the best member of their support team to assist them with a range of

different challenges. Parents can support children at this stage by attending to and rewarding

proactive behaviours, thereby reinforcing the use of coping skills learned in the program.

This improves the likelihood that such behaviours will be maintained.

Don’t forget to practice & Smile! Stay calm for life! The final two program

components are delivered together. The former is designed to remind children that their

coping skills will improve with regular practice. The final component reminds participants

that they can remain calm because they have been equipped with the necessary coping

strategies to manage anxiety. Families can assist the maintenance of these strategies by

encouraging their continued use, and assisting children to plan for upcoming challenges.

Family discussions of how FRIENDS for Life skills may be used in these situations can assist

children to prepare and feel more comfortable in their coping ability.

Table 3

FRIENDS for Life components delivered per session

Session Number Session Content and Learning Objectives

1 Rapport building and introduction of group participants


Establishing group guidelines
Normalisation of anxiety and individual differences in anxiety

2 Affective education and identification of various emotions


Introducing the relationship between thoughts and feelings

3 F: Feelings.
Identifying physiological symptoms of worry
R: Remember to relax. Have quiet time
Relaxation activities

4 I: I can do it! I can try my best!


Identifying self-talk, introducing helpful green thoughts and

114
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

unhelpful red thoughts

5 Attention training - looking for positive aspects in all situations


Challenging unhelpful red thoughts
E: Explore solutions and coping step plans
Coping step plans and setting goals

6 Problem-solving skills (6 stage problem-solving plan)


Coping Role models
Social support plans

7 N: Now reward yourself. You’ve done your best!

8 D: Don’t forget to practice


S: Smile. Stay calm for life!
Reflect on ways to cope in difficult situations

9 Generalising skills of FRIENDS to various difficult situations


Coaching others in how to use the FRIENDS coping skills

10 Skills for maintenance of the FRIENDS strategies


Preparing for minor set-backs that may occur

Booster 1 Review of FRIENDS strategies and preparing for future


challenges

Booster 2 Review of FRIENDS strategies and preparing for future


challenges

115
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

CHAPTER SEVEN

Results

Preliminary Analyses

Preliminary analysis of the data revealed significant amounts of missing data at all

time points, which became larger at later time periods. Of the original sample of 963

participants, there was fully completed data for 486 participants at both pre and post (see

Table 4). Missing value analysis further confirmed that data were not missing randomly. T-

tests were used to compare the sub-sample of participants who completed the questionnaire

measures at both pre and post-intervention, with the sub-sample who completed measures at

pre-intervention, but not at post-intervention, to determine whether there were any significant

differences in pre-intervention levels of internalising symptoms and other outcome measures.

There were no significant differences on any of the outcome measures at pre-intervention

between these two groups.

Due to the large amount of missing data, it was decided to analyse only complete data

points using Linear Mixed Effects models. This was used for the analysis of the overall

effect of treatment across the three time points, with participant identity as a random factor,

time as repeated measures, and an unstructured covariance metric. Linear mixed effects

models include all observations which are valid at each time point (Cnaan, Laird, Slassor,

1998). For subsequent analyses, ANOVAs, t-tests and regressions were performed on

difference scores formed from the difference between pre-intervention and post-intervention

scores on each measure, and therefore include only participants who completed both

assessments. Although there may still be issues of generalisability of findings derived from

116
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

these analyses, the analysis of complete data is less problematic for multivariate analyses than

is imputation of data when data are non-randomly and extensively missing, as these analyses

make less assumptions about the nature of the missing data and therefore produce less biased

estimates (Kalton & Kasprzyk, 1982).

All measures showed some degree of skew. Analyses were performed with both

untransformed and transformed data, using each of the square-root, logarithm, and power

transforms. The optimal transform was selected using the Box-Cox procedure, which

identifies the power transform which most closely resembles normality for any continuously

scaled variable (Box & Cox, 1964) for each variable. Where substantive differences were

observed, these are presented in the analyses. No significant outliers were observed after

transformation. Due to the large number of analyses calculated, only significant F-ratios will

be reported.

Table 4

Sample size for all measures analysed, across time points

Measure Pre Post Follow up


n n n
SCAS
Total 833 490 200
Panic Disorder 828 489 200
Separation Anxiety 832 490 200
Physical Injury 833 488 200
Social Phobia 832 490 200
OCD 829 487 200
GAD 833 488 200
RCMAS 610 488 199
CDI 830 512 195
SDQ
Emotional Problems 605 516 199
Conduct Problems 605 516 199
Hyperactivity/Inattention 605 516 199
Pro-social Behaviour 605 516 199
Peer Problems 605 516 199

117
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

SEI
Social Self-esteem 626 508 196
School Esteem 626 509 196
CSCY
Assistance Seeking 523 391 141
Problem Solving 523 391 141
Cognitive Avoidance 523 391 141
Behavioural Avoidance 523 392 141

Note. SCAS = Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale; RCMAS = Revised Children’s Manifest

Anxiety Scale; CDI = Children’s Depression Inventory; SDQ = Strengths and Difficulties

Questionnaire; SEI = Self Esteem Inventory; CSCY = Coping Scale for Children and Youth.

Differences in Baseline Measures (School, Gender, and Grade).

Preliminary examination of the data indicated that the gender and grade distributions

were not significantly different between the three schools (see Table 5). A series of one-way

ANOVAs comparing the three schools in terms of baseline anxiety and depression (see Table

6) showed that one of the three schools (Waterford West) had significantly lower depression

scores than the other two schools, F(2,573) = 4.56, p = .011. Another of the three schools

(Crestmead) had significantly lower scores on the SCAS separation anxiety subscale than did

the other two schools, F(2,573) = 4.18, p = .016.

Table 5

Sample sizes for each school, by grade and gender

School Grade 5 Grade 6 Grade 7


Male Female Male Female Male Female
Burrows 55 57 48 55 40 34
Waterford West 43 46 46 56 43 43
Crestmead 67 55 80 55 72 68

118
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Table 6

Mean and standard deviation for all clinical outcome measures at baseline

School
Burrowes Waterford West Crestmead
x̄ SD x̄ SD x̄ SD
SCAS
Total 27.87 20.46 27.40 21.95 27.26 19.42
Panic Disorder 3.83 5.09 4.14 5.17 3.78 4.51
Separation Anxiety 4.73 3.80 4.79 4.37 4.35 3.60
Fear of Physical Injury 3.35 3.10 3.36 3.24 3.20 3.15
Social Phobia 5.35 4.21 5.25 4.28 5.24 4.03
OCD 5.10 4.12 4.93 4.18 5.27 4.02
GAD 5.50 4.09 5.16 4.07 5.42 4.11
RCMAS 15.32 7.64 14.45 6.79 14.45 7.25
CDI 11.67 9.59 10.20 8.42 11.73 8.61

Note. SCAS = Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale; RCMAS = Revised Children’s Manifest

Anxiety Scale; CDI = Children’s Depression Inventory.

Two-way ANOVAs of each clinical measure indicated that overall, girls had

significantly higher anxiety scores than boys, as measured on the SCAS, F(1,827) = 35.93, p

< .001. A similar effect of gender was noted with regards to anxiety scores on the RCMAS,

whereby girls reported significantly higher anxiety than boys, F(1,604) = 11.91, p < .001.

With regards to differences in anxiety between the three grades, children in Grade 7 were

found to have lower anxiety scores on the SCAS than children in Grade 5, F(2,827) = 3.08, p

= 0.046. Children in Grade 6 did not significantly differ from the other two groups in terms

of overall anxiety scores on the SCAS. When investigating self-reported anxiety symptoms

on the RCMAS, children in Grade 7 exhibited less anxiety than children in either Grade 5 or

Grade 6, F(2,604) = 6.05, p = 0.002. Children in Grade 5 and Grade 6 did not differ

significantly in terms of self-reported anxiety on the RCMAS.

119
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Treatment Effects.

Anxiety. Table 7 shows the changes on all outcome measures over time. On the

SCAS total score, a significant main effect for time was found, F(2,120.08) = 33.05, p<.001,

indicating that anxiety scores did change over time. Anxiety scores decreased significantly

from pre to post intervention, though there was no significant difference between post-

intervention and follow-up scores. Similarly, for the RCMAS a significant main effect for

time was found, F(2,102.54) = 20.7, p<.001. Self-rated anxiety on this measure significantly

decreased from pre to post-intervention, though there was no significant difference between

post-intervention and follow-up RCMAS scores.

Significant main effects for time were also noted on all SCAS subscales. Contrasts

examining the main effect of time on the subscale for GAD, F(2,168) = 44.38, p<.001,

demonstrated that scores decreased significantly from pre to post-intervention, with

significant decreases also noted between post and follow-up scores. Contrasts examining the

main effect of time on the subscales for PD, F(2,201.29) = 14.33, p<.001, SAD, F(2,157.07)

= 34.33, p<.001, and OCD, F(2,168) = 44.38, p<.001, revealed that scores significantly

decreased from pre to post-intervention, with no significant differences noted between post-

intervention and follow-up scores. By comparison, contrasts examining the main effect of

time on the subscales for fear of physical injury F(2,101.29) = 4.87, p = 0.01, and SP,

F(2,133.68) = 16.64, p<.001, revealed that whilst significant decreases from pre to post were

evident, there were significant increases in scores on both scales from post to follow-up.

Depression. On the CDI, a significant main effect for time was found, F(2,112.7) =

14.77, p<.001, whereby self-rated depression significantly decreased from pre to post-

120
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

intervention. No significant difference was noted in depression scores from post-intervention

to follow-up.

Self-esteem, coping skills, and psychosocial difficulties. A significant main effect

of time was noted for the SDQ emotional problems subscale, F(2,147.29) = 14.68, p<.001,

whereby scores on this scale decreased from pre to post, with no significant difference

between post and follow-up scores. Significant main effects of time were also found for both

the CSCY cognitive avoidance subscale, F(2,277.95) = 11.52, p<.001, and the CSCY

behavioural avoidance subscale, F(2,243.32) = 11.19, p<.001, with contrasts indicating

significantly lower scores from pre to post-intervention, with further decreases noted between

post-intervention and follow-up scores. Significant main effects of time were also found for

the SDQ peer problems subscale F(2,195.19) = 7.1, p = 0.001, SEI social self-esteem

subscale, F(2,108.17) = 5.49, p = 0.005, CSCY problem solving subscale, F(2,228.82) =

11.56, p<.001, and for the transformed SDQ conduct problems subscale, F(2,179.77) = 5.61,

p = 0.004. Contrasts performed on each of these main effects revealed no significant

differences in scores for the above scales from pre to post-intervention, but demonstrated

significantly higher follow-up scores for the SEI social self-esteem scale, relative to those

both pre and post intervention, and significantly lower follow-up scores for the CSCY

problem solving subscale, SDQ peer problems subscale, and the transformed SDQ conduct

problems subscale, relative to those both pre and post intervention.

Change in risk status. Changes in risk status over time are displayed below in

Figure 2. Overall, 21.9% of children exhibited levels of anxiety at or above the clinical cut-

off on the total SCAS score at baseline, which reduced to 14.7% at post-test and 12% at

follow-up. On the CDI, 30.4% of children scored above the clinical cut-off for depression at

baseline, and this rate dropped to 23.4% at post-test, and 21% at follow-up.

121
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

35

30
Percentage of participants at risk

25

20
Pre
Post

15 Follow-up

10

0
SCAS CDI
Questionnaire Measure

Figure 2. Change in risk status over time.

122
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Table 7
Mean (and standard deviation) for all measures at pre-intervention, post-intervention and follow-up

Pre-test Post-test Follow-up Change over time


SCAS
Total 27.48 (20.44) 22.23 (17.06) 22.17 (18.23) F(2,120.08) = 33.05, p<.001***
Panic Disorder 3.89 (4.87) 3.01 (4.13) 3.07 (4.47) F(2,201.29) = 14.33, p<.001***
Separation Anxiety 4.59 (3.89) 3.59 (3.35) 3.37 (3.33) F(2,157.07) = 34.33, p<.001***
Fear of Physical Injury 3.29 (3.16) 2.96 (2.85) 3.06 (3.06) F(2,101.29) = 4.87, p = 0.01***
Social Phobia 5.28 (4.15) 4.42 (3.65) 4.71 (3.55) F(2,133.68) = 16.64, p<.001***
OCD 5.13 (4.09) 3.77 (3.45) 3.64 (3.52) F(2,168) = 44.38, p<.001***
GAD 5.37 (4.09) 4.55 (3.32) 4.34 (3.78) F(2,133.08) = 20.45, p<.001***
RCMAS 14.76 (7.25) 13.01 (6.72) 13.01 (6.58) F(2,102.54) = 20.7, p<.001***
CDI 11.26 (8.86) 9.4 (8.21) 8.72 (8.21) F(2,112.7) = 14.77, p<.001***
SDQ
Emotional Problems 3.69 (2.41) 3.2 (2.22) 3.06 (2.26) F(2,147.29) = 14.68, p<.001***
Conduct Problems 2.99 (2.1) 2.77 (1.97) 2.41 (1.92) F(2,179.77) = 5.61, p = 0.004***
Hyperactivity/Inattention 4.25 (2.31) 4.01 (2.27) 4.08 (2.5) F(2,99.11) = 1.73, p = 0.183
Peer Problems 2.5 (1.91) 2.43 (1.82) 2.03 (1.74) F(2,195.19) = 7.1, p = 0.001***
Pro-social Behaviour 7.26 (1.96) 7.28 (1.9) 7.56 (1.95) F(2,211.75) = 1.22, p = 0.298
SEI
Social Self-esteem 5.8 (1.96) 5.93 (1.94) 6.16 (1.73) F(2,108.17) = 5.49, p = 0.005***
School Esteem 5.54 (1.73) 5.57 (1.79) 5.7 (1.63) F(2,171.95) = 0.35, p = 0.705
CSCY
Assistance Seeking 8.83 (2.77) 8.75 (2.67) 8.32 (2.84) F(2,265.63) = 1.75, p = 0.176
Problem Solving 16.68 (5.69) 16.21 (5.54) 14.55 (5.46) F(2,228.82) = 11.56, p<.001***
Cognitive Avoidance 23.42 (7.47) 22.07 (7.13) 20.38 (7.46) F(2,277.95) = 11.52, p<.001***
Behavioural Avoidance 11.1 (3.87) 10.6 (3.53) 9.63 (3.69) F(2,243.32) = 11.19, p<.001***

Note. SCAS = Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale; RCMAS = Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale; CDI = Children’s Depression
Inventory; SDQ = Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; SEI = Self Esteem Inventory; CSCY = Coping Scale for Children and Youth.

123
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Predictors of improvement

Gender and grade. A series of two-way ANOVAs were conducted

examining whether improvement (difference between pre-intervention and post-

intervention scores) was related to the grade or gender of participants. For the SCAS

total score, there was an effect of gender, such that girls demonstrated a higher

improvement score than boys, F(1, 412) = 34.77, p < .001. However, it was found

that boys’ difference scores for the SCAS social phobia subscale were significantly

greater than girls’ scores, F(1,418) = 5.14, p = .024.

Two main effects were revealed with regards to difference scores on several

secondary outcome measures. There was a significant main effect of gender for the

SDQ Peer Problems subscale, such that boys’ scores on this measure decreased more

than girls’ scores, F(1,308) = 5.34, p = .022. With regards to self-esteem, there was a

significant main effect of gender for the SEI subscale for social self-esteem, such that

boys’ scores increased more than girls’ scores, F(1,326) = 9.96, p = .002. There was

also a significant two-way interaction of grade and gender for the SEI social self-

esteem subscale, such that scores increased more for boys than for girls in Grades 5

and 6, with no significant difference between boys and girls in Grade 7, F(2,326) =

3.62, p = .028.

124
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Table 8

Means and standard deviations for difference between pre-intervention and post-intervention scores

Male Female
Grade Grade
5 6 7 5 6 7
x̄ SD x̄ SD x̄ SD x̄ SD x̄ SD x̄ SD
SCAS
Total -7.16 19.91 -6.52 20.58 -5.91 12.61 -5.66 17.38 -4.85 17.07 -3.43 14.49
Panic Disorder -1.20 5.36 -1.53 5.91 -.48 3.90 -1.00 4.00 -1.14 5.21 -.32 3.67
Separation Anxiety -1.22 3.29 -1.30 3.82 -.87 2.20 -1.36 4.14 -1.00 3.44 -.60 3.19
Fear of Physical injury -.45 2.84 -.05 2.74 -.23 1.93 -.79 2.80 -.32 2.80 -.14 2.60
Social Phobia -1.66 4.11 -.87 3.71 -1.61 2.81 -.90 3.67 -.32 3.43 -.53 3.76
OCD -1.50 4.66 -1.87 4.65 -1.41 3.30 -.94 4.07 -1.32 3.89 -1.31 3.76
GAD -1.16 4.02 -.95 4.10 -1.32 3.15 -.67 3.50 -.75 4.06 -.52 3.47
RCMAS -3.57 6.19 -1.10 6.81 -.92 5.44 -1.59 6.53 -1.33 6.53 -1.33 5.99
CDI -.72 8.32 -1.71 9.82 -2.74 6.76 -.87 8.58 -.18 7.67 -2.68 6.19
SDQ
Emotional Problems -.76 2.10 -.61 1.99 -.16 2.36 -.42 1.99 -.65 2.50 -.38 2.38
Conduct Problems .05 2.36 -.50 1.96 .28 2.23 -.14 1.85 -.41 1.96 -.13 1.77
Hyperactivity/Inattention .10 1.99 .22 1.85 -.36 2.15 -.07 2.16 -.11 2.37 -.28 1.91
Peer Problems -.02 1.63 -.67 2.17 -.34 1.70 .28 1.86 .07 1.81 .01 1.52
Pro-social Behaviour .40 2.10 .11 2.17 .43 2.37 -.28 1.89 .31 2.03 -.25 1.93
SEI
Social Self Esteem .38 1.31 .16 1.71 -.16 1.84 .05 2.07 -.06 1.56 .16 1.49
School Esteem .81 1.77 .53 1.65 .03 1.83 -.41 1.63 -.14 1.91 .09 1.53
CSCY

125
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Assistance Seeking 1.00 3.99 -1.10 3.66 .74 3.36 -.34 3.25 .25 3.40 -.30 3.66
Problem Solving -1.35 8.83 -3.23 5.44 .05 7.90 -2.28 5.76 -.86 5.25 -.70 6.55
Cognitive Avoidance -3.45 11.72 -3.27 10.06 -2.17 9.19 -4.48 9.20 -2.41 9.11 -.15 9.20
Behavioural Avoidance -.60 5.20 -2.20 5.24 -.55 4.78 -1.31 5.04 -1.25 4.01 .06 3.68

Note. SCAS = Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale; RCMAS = Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale; CDI = Children’s Depression
Inventory; SDQ = Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; SEI = Self Esteem Inventory; CSCY = Coping Scale for Children and Youth.

126
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Baseline anxiety. Anxiety level at baseline was associated with the

effectiveness of the intervention, in terms of the difference between anxiety levels at

pre- and post-treatment. Improvement in SCAS-total score was correlated -.62 (p <

.001) with SCAS-total score at baseline, such that those with higher initial levels of

anxiety demonstrated a significantly greater decrease in anxiety symptoms post-

intervention.

Participants were stratified into low-risk and at-risk groups, based on their pre-

intervention total score on the SCAS. Participants were assigned to the low-risk

group based on scores below the clinical cut-off score of 42, whilst participants

scoring at or above this cut-off were allocated to the at-risk group. Improvement

scores were examined on the SCAS total score, RCMAS and CDI, as a function of

risk status, age, and grade, to establish whether there were differential patterns of

improvement. Of the 833 children who completed the SCAS at baseline, 183 (21.9%)

were allocated to the at-risk group based on their pre-intervention SCAS score.

Children in the at-risk group demonstrated significantly greater improvement than

children in the low-risk group on all primary outcome measures, F(1, 410) = 8.1, p =

.005 for CDI, F(1,305) = 12.28, p < .001 for RCMAS, and F(1,412) = 199.66, p <

.001 for SCAS-total. For the SCAS total score, there was a significant two-way

interaction of gender and clinical status F(1,412) = 23.15, p < .001; children in the at-

risk group experienced significantly greater reduction in self-reported anxiety than

those in the low-risk group, but this effect was stronger for boys than for girls.

Self-esteem, coping style, strengths and difficulties. Bivariate correlations

were examined to ascertain whether improvement on the clinical measures differed

according to psychosocial characteristics of participants at pre-intervention (see Table

127
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

9). Children with higher scores on the SDQ emotional problems subscale at pre-

intervention had significantly lower post-intervention scores on the SCAS, RCMAS,

and CDI, whilst children with higher scores for pro-social behaviour pre-intervention

had greater reductions on the CDI only. Children with lower scores on both SEI

subscales for social self-esteem and school esteem pre-intervention were more likely

to have lower scores on the CDI at post-intervention, whilst children with higher

scores on the CSCY behavioural avoidance subscale at pre-intervention had

significantly lower scores on both the SCAS total scale and the CDI at post-

intervention.

Table 9

Pearson bivariate correlations between psychosocial predictors and difference scores

in outcome (difference from pre to post on each clinical measure)

SCAS total RCMAS CDI


Correlation, p, N Correlation, p, N Correlation, p, N
SDQ
Emotional Problems -0.24 (0, 296) * -0.24 (0, 294) * -0.21 (0, 302) *
Conduct Problems -0.11 (0.05, 296) 0 (0.985, 294) -0.12 (0.037, 302)
Hyperactivity/Inattent -0.1 (0.09, 296) -0.05 (0.368, 294) -0.11 (0.061, 302)
Pro-social Behaviour -0.1 (0.094, 296) -0.07 (0.266, 294) -0.23 (0, 302) *
Peer Problems 0 (0.993, 296) -0.01 (0.842, 294) 0.13 (0.022, 302)
SEI
Social Self-esteem 0.08 (0.149, 318) 0.16 (0.005, 316) 0.29 (0, 317) *
School Esteem 0.07 (0.237, 318) 0.05 (0.372, 316) 0.24 (0, 317) *
CSCY
Assistance Seeking -0.07 (0.254, 276) -0.1 (0.105, 276) -0.01 (0.92, 280)
Problem Solving -0.19 (0.002, 276) -0.18 (0.002, 276) -0.01 (0.899, 280)
Cognitive Avoidance -0.18 (0.003, 276) -0.12 (0.054, 276) -0.11 (0.057, 280)
Behaviour Avoidance -0.27 (0, 276) * -0.19 (0.002, 276) -0.23 (0, 280) *
***significant at < .001 (bonferonni correction)

Note. SCAS = Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale; RCMAS = Revised Children’s

Manifest Anxiety Scale; CDI = Children’s Depression Inventory; SDQ = Strengths

128
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

and Difficulties Questionnaire; SEI = Self Esteem Inventory; CSCY = Coping Scale

for Children and Youth.

129
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

CHAPTER EIGHT

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to investigate the effectiveness of a universal

school-based intervention program for childhood anxiety (FRIENDS for Life) for

upper primary-school aged children (aged 9 to 13 years) within a socioeconomically

disadvantaged community. The primary objective was to examine changes in anxiety

and depressive symptomatology from pre to post-intervention, and at 12 months

follow-up. Consistent with predictions, participants reported significantly less anxiety

symptoms, as measured on both the SCAS and RCMAS, following the completion of

the intervention. There was no significant difference in total anxiety symptom scores

between post-intervention and 12-month follow-up on either anxiety measure,

indicating that the positive treatment gains were maintained over time. Furthermore,

as predicted, participants also reported significantly less depressive symptoms, as

measured by the CDI, following the intervention. As with anxiety symptom scores,

the improvements in depressive symptomatology were robust over time. With regards

to anxiety risk status, 75% of participants were no longer in the “at-risk” range for

anxiety at post-intervention, with this percentage increasing to 88% at 12-months

follow-up.

The findings noted above are consistent with earlier research which has

demonstrated immediate reductions in child-reported anxiety symptoms, following

implementation of a universal CBT program (Barrett & Turner, 2001; Lock & Barrett,

2003; Lowry-Webster, Barrett, & Dadds, 2001). The finding that the reductions in

130
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

anxiety symptoms were maintained at 12 months follow-up is also consistent with

previous research investigating the longitudinal effects of universal prevention for

anxiety (Barrett, Lock et al., 2005; Barrett, Farrell, Ollendick, & Dadds, 2006; Lock

& Barrett, 2003; Lowry-Webster, Barrett, & Lock, 2003; Stallard et al., 2008). From

the perspective of intervention within socioeconomically disadvantaged populations,

the findings of this research are consistent with those of Misfud and Rapee (2005),

who also found significant decreases in anxiety symptomatology from pre to post-

intervention in children who completed an indicated intervention for anxiety. By

comparison, the significant reduction in anxiety symptoms found in the current study

contrast the findings of Roberts, et al. (2010), who failed to demonstrate significant

differences in child-reported internalising symptoms from pre to post-intervention.

This discrepancy may be due to the different intervention protocol used by the current

study, as compared to that employed by Roberts et al. (2010).

Whilst the primary outcome measure of the current study was anxiety

symptomatology, the current research also revealed significant reductions in child-

reported depression symptoms over time. This finding is consistent with earlier

research which has also found reductions in depression following implementation of

the FRIENDS program (Barrett, et al., 2006; Lock & Barrett, 2003; Lowry-Webster,

et al., 2003). Taken together, this evidence suggests that FRIENDS for Life may be

effective in reducing internalising symptomatology in general, rather than anxiety

specifically. This theory is in keeping with research demonstrating the high rate of

comorbidity between anxiety and depression (Angold, Costello, & Erkanli, 1999;

Costello, Egger, & Angold, 2005), suggesting a possible common mechanism

underlying both symptom clusters which is responsive to CBT treatment. With

131
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

regards to the current study, it is notable that reductions in depressive

symptomatology were evident immediately at post-intervention. This is in contrast

with earlier research, where protective effects for depression emerged only at 12

months post-intervention (Lock & Barrett, 2003), or not at all (Barrett, Lock et al.,

2005; Roberts, et al., 2010). A possible explanation for the immediate response seen

in the current study may be related to the population sample, given that children from

disadvantaged populations are at greater risk for internalising disorders and

psychopathology (Costello, et al., 1996; Kessler, et al., 1994; Miech, Caspi, Moffitt,

Entner Wright, & Silva, 1999; Xue, Leventhal, Brooks-Gunn, & Earls, 2005).

As with several earlier studies of universal prevention in schools (Barrett,

Lock et al., 2005; Lock & Barrett, 2003; Stallard, et al., 2008), the current study was

affected by considerable amounts of missing data. In outcome studies of this nature,

there is always a question of whether the interpretability of significant results may be

compromised by differential rates of attrition according to symptom severity at pre-

intervention. In drawing conclusions from available data, it is important to

distinguish whether or not participants are more likely to drop out due to higher levels

of internalising symptoms pre-intervention. Such drop-out would invariably bias

results, given that post-intervention outcomes would be based primarily on the scores

of individuals with less severe difficulties to begin with. In the current study, no

significant differences in pre-intervention anxiety and depressive symptoms were

found between those participants who completed questionnaire measures at pre and

post-intervention, and those who completed measures at pre but not at post. This is an

important finding in the context of our results; it appears that significant reductions in

132
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

internalising symptoms post-intervention are not purely an artefact of missing data,

and as such the results presented above can be interpreted with greater confidence.

The present study also examined whether risk and protective factors for

childhood anxiety (principally, the use of maladaptive and positive coping strategies,

and self-esteem) would be modified following the completion of the intervention.

With regard to effects on the use of coping skills as measured by the CSCY

(assistance seeking, cognitive behavioural problem solving, cognitive avoidance, and

behavioural avoidance), the hypotheses were partially supported. Self-reported use of

cognitive avoidance and behavioural avoidance decreased significantly from pre to

post-intervention, and again from post-intervention to follow-up. This finding

suggests that participants were more likely to address and confront challenging and

anxiety-provoking situations following the intervention. Additionally, the significant

decrease from post to follow-up suggests that participants became more confident in

approaching challenges over time. This reduction of avoidance may facilitate the

decrease of anxiety symptoms over the long-term, given that reduced avoidance

allows for exposure to feared situations, giving children the opportunity to habituate

to such situations. The findings outlined above are similar to those reported in the

universal prevention study conducted by Lock and Barrett (2003), who found

significant decreases in both cognitive and behavioural avoidance as coping skills.

The findings of the current study are also consistent with research attesting to the role

of avoidant coping strategies in perpetuating anxiety in children Barrett, Rapee,

Dadds, & Ryan, 1996; Herman-Stahl & Petersen, 1996; Lengua, Sandler, West,

Wolchick, & Curran, 1999; Prior, Smart, Sanson, & Oberklaid, 2000).

133
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

By comparison, and contrary to prediction, use of cognitive behavioural

problem solving as a coping strategy significantly decreased 12 months post-

intervention, whilst there was no significant change in the use of assistance-seeking as

a coping strategy across time. These results indicate that participants were less likely

or no more likely to use these positive coping skills, following the intervention.

These results may be partly reconciled with those reported by Lock and Barrett

(2003), who investigated changes in coping strategies (assessed using the CSCY) for

children in Grade 6 (late primary school) and Grade 9 (early high school). In this

study, whilst it was found that females and children from the Grade 9 group reported

increased cognitive behavioural problem solving and assistance-seeking post-

intervention, improvements had disappeared at 12 months follow-up. The study’s

authors concluded that whilst younger children were less likely to avoid anxiety-

provoking situations post-intervention, they were also less likely to use positive

coping strategies post-intervention than older children. This may be reflected again

by results of the current study, given that the sample was comprised of late primary

school-aged children. In reconciling these findings with the FRIENDS program

content, the significant decrease in the use of cognitive and behavioural avoidance as

coping strategies suggest that children of late primary school age respond well to the

FRIENDS component incorporating graded exposure. In this component, children are

taught to come up with step plans to actively address anxiety provoking situations.

By comparison, the failure to find hypothesised improvements in positive coping

strategies (assistance seeking and cognitive behavioural problem solving) suggests

that greater efforts are needed to improve the way that positive coping strategies are

taught to children in the FRIENDS program.

134
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Predicted improvements in self-reported self-esteem were also found by the

current study; specifically, self ratings on the social self-esteem subscale of the SEI

were significantly higher at 12 months follow-up, relative to scores at pre and post-

intervention. This finding suggests that participants were more likely to view

themselves positively, and as being more popular and likeable within their peer group,

one year after the intervention. By comparison, the expected change in self-reported

school esteem (on the school esteem subscale of the SEI) was not found, suggesting

that children’s self-esteem with regards to their school efforts and achievement did

not vary in response to the intervention. Though the predictions were only partially

supported, the findings above have positive implications for children with anxiety.

Given that low self-esteem has been associated with anxiety in children (Ialongo,

Edelsohn, Werthamer-Larsson, Crockett, & Kellam, 1996; McLoone, Hudson, &

Rapee, 2006; Rapee, Kennedy, Ingram, Edwards, & Sweeney, 2005; Strauss, Frame,

& Forehand, 1987), improvements in self-esteem may act as a protective factor

against anxiety difficulties in the future.

The above findings regarding self-esteem may be interpreted in the context of

the content of the FRIENDS program. In reconciling these findings with the program

content, it may be argued that the intervention is geared more towards the promotion

of general and social self-esteem, rather than self-esteem related to a specific aspect of

functioning such as that measured by the school esteem subscale of the SEI. This can

be seen through activities encouraging team work, social support, cognitive

challenging, and use of positive self-talk. Whilst each of these elements may

certainly be applied to functioning within the school domain, aspects which relate

specifically to school esteem, such as pride in one’s work, attitude towards school and

135
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

learning, and relationships with school teachers, are not explicitly dealt with by the

program. It would be interesting to investigate whether the inclusion of a ‘school’

component in the standard protocol would yield any significant improvements in

school esteem in future research.

The improvements in self-esteem outlined above are similar to those reported

by Stallard et al. (2008), who found significant improvements in total self-esteem

from pre-intervention to 3 months post-intervention and 12-months follow-up. The

results are also similar to those of Barrett, Sonderegger, and Xenos (2003), who noted

significant improvements in self-esteem for participants in the intervention condition,

both from pre to post-intervention, and from post-intervention to 6 months follow-up.

In comparison to the previously mentioned research however, improvements in self-

esteem did not become apparent in the current study until 12 months post-

intervention. The delayed improvement in self-esteem scores may be due to

participants requiring enough time to build up confidence to engage in a range of

different situations and activities that may have previously been avoided, which

would serve to increase self-esteem over the long-term.

With regards to other measures of psychosocial functioning, significant

improvements were noted in several areas. As predicted, self-reported emotional

problems decreased significantly from pre to post-intervention, with these gains

maintained at 12 months follow-up. These findings are consistent with the significant

decreases in anxiety and depressive symptomatology reported above, providing

further evidence for the effectiveness of the intervention. By comparison, self-

reported peer problems remained stable from pre to post-intervention, but improved

136
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

significantly at 12-months follow-up. This finding indicates that children reported

experiencing significantly fewer problems within their peer group 12 months

following the intervention. This compliments the earlier reported finding of

significant increases in social self-esteem, which suggested children felt they were

more liked by their peers. Similar to changes in self-esteem, significant reductions in

self-reported peer problems did not emerge until 12 months post-intervention,

suggesting children may require more time to practice their new skills before reaping

social benefits.

Predicted improvements in self-reported conduct problems were also noted.

Whilst these problems remained stable from pre to post-intervention, significant

decreases were evident at follow-up, indicating that children reported exhibiting fewer

disruptive behavioural problems 12 months following the intervention. This

reduction may also be in part related to decreases in peer problems, improvements in

self-esteem, and reduction in internalising difficulties in general, such that children

were less likely to act out behaviourally. By comparison, predicted improvements in

self-reported pro-social behaviour were not observed, which is interesting given the

improvements noted in the areas of self-esteem and peer problems. Lastly, there was

no significant reduction in self-reported hyperactivity and inattention, suggesting that

these issues may be better addressed by other interventions tailored more specifically

to problem behaviours.

The secondary focus of this research was to investigate predictors of

improvement (difference between pre-intervention and post-intervention scores) on

the primary outcome measures (anxiety and depressive symptomatology), and

137
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

secondary outcome measures (coping skills, self-esteem, and psychosocial factors).

In terms of demographic predictors, girls demonstrated significantly greater

improvement in anxiety symptoms post-intervention, a finding consistent with earlier

research (Barrett, et al., 2006; Lock & Barrett, 2003). However, given that some

researchers have failed to find significant gender differences in terms of symptom

improvement (Dadds, Spence, Holland, Barrett, & Laurens, 1997; Lowry-Webster, et

al., 2003; Roberts, et al., 2010; Shortt, Barrett, & Fox, 2001), whilst others have

reported greater improvement in boys (Dadds, et al., 1999), further research may be

required to conclusively identify gender differences in response to treatment. It was

also found that males were significantly more likely than females to demonstrate

decreases in SOP symptoms post-intervention, suggesting that boys may derive

greater benefits than girls from the FRIENDS program in terms of developing social

confidence. This finding has some important implications in terms of social

development in boys, who typically lag behind girls in terms of establishing

meaningful social relationships from a young age. It appears the FRIENDS program

may impart the necessary skills and strategies for boys to approach social situations

with greater confidence.

The current research failed to identify any between-grade differences in terms

of improvement in internalising difficulties. Several earlier studies investigating

developmental differences in universal anxiety prevention established that children

from younger grades tend to respond more positively to treatment (Barrett, Lock et

al., 2005; Lock & Barrett, 2003). Notably however, the mean age of the comparison

groups selected by these researchers (Grade 6 and Grade 9) differed by approximately

five to six years. By comparison, the participants of the current study were drawn

138
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

from three successive school grades (Grades 5, 6, and 7), resulting in a much smaller

age range across the total sample. It is likely that the lack of difference between

grades reflects the similarity between grades of children of upper primary school age,

relative to the greater heterogeneity between upper primary school-aged children and

early high school-aged children, as highlighted by the earlier research (Barrett, Lock

et al., 2005; Lock & Barrett, 2003).

In terms of secondary outcome measures, male gender predicted greater

reductions in peer problems, whereby boys were more likely to have fewer difficulties

with peers following the intervention. Furthermore, boys from Grades 5 and 6

demonstrated greater improvements in social self-esteem relative to girls, whilst there

was no significant difference in improvement in social self-esteem between boys and

girls in Grade 7. The findings suggest that boys from younger grades felt more liked

by their peers than girls and older boys, post-intervention. It would be useful to

conduct further longitudinal research to examine whether these gender and age

differences disappear over time, given that significant changes in self-esteem

collapsed across the total sample were not evident until 12 months follow-up.

Baseline anxiety was found to be a significant predictor of improvement in

internalising symptoms, such that children in the at-risk group at pre-intervention

demonstrated greater reductions in both anxiety and depressive symptomatology.

This finding is consistent with earlier research demonstrating that children with higher

levels of pre-intervention anxiety tend to report greater reductions in both anxiety and

depression post-intervention (Barrett, Lock et al., 2005). These findings have

important implications, particularly with regards to anxiety intervention within low

139
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

SES communities. Given that socioeconomic disadvantage is a risk factor for anxiety

and depression, the finding that children at risk for anxiety respond most positively to

the intervention suggests that curriculum-based anxiety prevention is ideal for these

populations. It was also found that, within the at-risk group, male gender predicted

greater reductions in internalising symptoms. This finding is important, given that

boys are less inclined to report anxiety difficulties than girls (Bell-Dolan, Last, &

Strauss, 1990; Essau & Peterman, 2001; Muris, Merckelbach, Meyer, & Meesters,

1998; Silverman & Treffers, 2001; Silverman, Greca, & Wasserstein, 1995).

Therefore, the current study indicates that participation in school-based anxiety

prevention programs may be very effective for anxious boys, whose difficulties may

otherwise not come to the attention of parents and teachers.

Significant relationships were also identified between several psychosocial

variables at pre-intervention, and anxiety and depressive symptomatology at post-

intervention. Children with higher self-reported emotional problems prior to the

intervention showed greater reductions in both anxiety and depressive

symptomatology post-intervention. This is consistent with earlier reported findings

that children with higher levels of pre-intervention anxiety experienced the greatest

improvements in internalising symptoms. Furthermore, lower social self-esteem and

school self-esteem at pre-intervention was predictive of significantly decreased

depression post-intervention, highlighting self-esteem as a possible mediator for

intervention effectiveness. Lastly, greater use of behavioural avoidance pre-

intervention predicted greater improvement in both anxiety and depressive

symptomatology post-intervention. This finding reflects an existing theorised

connection between avoidant coping and anxiety (Barrett, Rapee, et al., 1996;

140
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Herman-Stahl & Petersen, 1996; Lengua, et al., 1999; Prior, et al., 2000), and further

research should examine whether the modification of either positive or maladaptive

coping skills may mediate the intervention effect on internalising symptoms.

Clinical Implications

The current study is the first to examine the effectiveness of the universal

school-based delivery of the FRIENDS for Life program within a socioeconomically

disadvantaged population. In conducting research exclusively within a low SES

community, this study addresses the gap in literature on the prevention of childhood

anxiety. It follows only two earlier studies which have examined anxiety prevention

programs exclusively within disadvantaged communities (Misfud & Rapee, 2005;

Roberts, et al., 2010), both of which evaluated alternative intervention protocols to the

FRIENDS program. The findings indicate that the FRIENDS program was successful

in reducing anxiety and depression in children in this population, who are at greater

risk of psychopathology. This finding has important implications for the

implementation of childhood anxiety interventions in low SES communities.

Specifically, it appears that the treatment dosage afforded by the universal delivery of

FRIENDS for Life is sufficient to produce meaningful decreases in internalising

symptoms, despite the increased prevalence of anxiety symptomatology within these

populations.

The significance of the above findings is strengthened by results

demonstrating that the above treatment gains were maintained 12 months post-

intervention. The only other published evaluation of universal anxiety prevention in

disadvantaged schools failed to demonstrate any long-term treatment effects (Roberts,

141
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

et al., 2010). It therefore provides further evidence of the protective effect afforded

by the FRIENDS for Life program, and demonstrates for the first time that long-term

symptom reduction is possible when intervening with children from disadvantaged

communities at the universal level.

This study expanded on the standard prevention evaluation model by

examining predictors of outcome other than those solely related to anxiety and

depressive symptoms. Specifically, measures assessing the use of positive coping

skills (assistance seeking and cognitive behavioural problem solving), maladaptive

coping skills (cognitive avoidance and behavioural avoidance) self-esteem (social self

esteem and school esteem), and other psychosocial factors (emotional problems, peer

problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, and pro-social behaviour)

were employed to provide a broader picture of intervention effectiveness. This

improves on the work conducted by both of the previously published studies

evaluating childhood anxiety prevention in disadvantaged communities, affording a

more complete report of the benefits attributable to universal prevention.

Furthermore, an examination of associations between these secondary outcome

measures and improvement in internalising symptoms offers some insight into what

factors predict a more positive outcome. These results provide an impetus for

additional research, to determine what aspects of the program may be most useful in

improving emotional resilience in children from disadvantaged communities.

The current research employed a considerably larger sample size than either of

the two earlier studies conducted in low SES communities, consisting of children

from three different school grades, as compared to two grades (Misfud & Rapee,

142
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

2005) and one grade (Roberts, et al., 2010). In comparing this study with the only

other universal study in this field (Roberts, et al., 2010), the percentage of children in

the current sample identified as at-risk of anxiety and depression (based on scores on

the SCAS and CDI) was notably higher than that identified by the earlier study. This

is notable, given that Roberts, et al. (2010) employed the same measures, and used a

slightly more conservative cut-off score on the CDI. Arguably then, the constellation

of difficulties faced by children in the current sample may more accurately reflect

those of a population at risk, which gives further credence to the significant

intervention effects revealed in this study.

Previous research has investigated the application of a ‘train-the-trainer’

model with regards to the FRIENDS program, with studies demonstrating that

FRIENDS is effective when delivered by classroom teachers trained to facilitate the

program (Barrett & Turner, 2001; Lowry-Webster, et al., 2001). Indeed, there is

evidence to suggest that teacher-led interventions may be associated with greater

improvements in anxiety symptomatology than those interventions run by mental

health professionals (Neil & Christensen, 2009). Whilst this study did not compare

program effectiveness based on different types of facilitators, the positive results do

provide further support that teacher-led interventions are associated with significant

improvements in anxiety and depression in children. Given the increased prevalence

of anxiety symptomatology within disadvantaged populations, such a comparison may

be an interesting research endeavour for the future, to determine whether a program

delivered by mental health professionals would enhance treatment gains further in this

needy population.

143
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Limitations

A significant limitation of this study was the lack of a wait-list control

condition, which was denied ethical clearance for two reasons. Firstly, substantial

evidence for the effectiveness of the FRIENDS program when delivered at the

universal level has already been provided by previous research (Barrett, Lock et al.,

2005; Barrett & Turner, 2001; Barrett, et al., 2006; Lock & Barrett, 2003; Lowry-

Webster, et al., 2001; Lowry-Webster, et al., 2003; Stallard, et al., 2008; Stallard et

al., 2007). Secondly, the three schools involved in this research were selected on the

basis of their location within a region of socioeconomic disadvantage. Due to the

increased risk of psychopathology faced by children from disadvantaged

communities, it was deemed unethical to deny children the opportunity to participate

in an empirically validated anxiety prevention program.

The lack of a wait-list comparison condition in the present study limits the

interpretability of the results, given that it cannot be inferred that the intervention is

solely responsible for the significant improvement in levels of anxiety and depression.

The potential influence of both placebo and maturation effects cannot be conclusively

discounted as factors in the significant improvement in anxiety and depressive

symptoms over time. The use of a comparison group, whereby half of the participants

completed an alternative program to the FRIENDS protocol, would have provided

another element of comparison in lieu of a wait-list group. Unfortunately however,

the inclusion of an additional active condition would have proven too resource and

labour-intensive for the current research, and was not within the project scope

required for a doctoral thesis.

144
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

A second limitation of this study relates to the psychometric measures used in

the research, and the related implications for interpretation of results. Levels of

anxiety and depression were assessed using self-report measures, rather than by

clinical interview, therefore levels of anxiety and depression must be discussed in

terms of symptomatology rather than diagnosable disorders. Some earlier studies

have incorporated a diagnostic clinical interview, such as the ADIS-C (Silverman &

Albano, 1996) in the assessment package (Barrett, 1998; Dadds, et al., 1999; Lock &

Barrett, 2003; Lowry-Webster, et al., 2003), which allows for more conclusive

discussion of anxiety and depression based on diagnostic prevalence. Unfortunately,

due to financial constraints and known challenges inherent in obtaining parental

consent for interviewing children, conducting diagnostic interviews was not feasible

within the scope of this thesis. Accordingly, changes in diagnostic status cannot be

evaluated. Future research should endeavour to incorporate diagnostic interviewing

to more conclusively establish the effectiveness of this intervention within

disadvantaged populations.

A related limitation is that the results of this study are based solely on

children’s subjective self-reporting of symptoms. There may therefore be some

question as to the accuracy of results. Again, due to the financial and personnel

constraints associated with longitudinal universal prevention research, this study did

not incorporate any parent or teacher self-report measures of child functioning.

Undoubtedly, these measures would have provided useful collateral information to

further gauge the effectiveness of the intervention, and provided greater scope for

145
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

statistical evaluation. As such, future researchers in this area would do well to collect

data from multiple sources.

Perhaps the key limitation of the current study was the significant portion of

missing data across all three time-points, meaning that results must be interpreted

with caution. The degree of missing data rendered the use of data imputation

techniques (such as multiple imputation or expectation maximisation) inappropriate

for use with the current study, limiting the range of statistical analyses able to be

performed. Missing data in universal school-based intervention research is a

relatively common problem, and significant rates have been reported in several other

universal evaluations of the FRIENDS program (Barrett, Lock et al., 2005; Lock &

Barrett, 2003; Stallard, et al., 2008). It is probable that the proportion of missing data

was due largely to the characteristics of the population sample. Specifically, data

collection was likely complicated by issues such as increased rates of absenteeism due

to truancy or family difficulties, and the greater tendency of families in this region to

relocate and change schools, due in part to residential and employment instabilities.

Additionally, given the increased involvement of child safety organisations within this

region, it is also possible that missing data may be due to some parents being

suspicious of the purpose the questionnaires.

It is also important to note the issue of missing data within participants,

whereby some children completed some questionnaire measures and not others, at any

given time point. This may be related to the rate of learning difficulties and disruptive

behaviour disorders such as ADHD within the sample population. Efforts were made

to manage this by ensuring all assessment sessions were facilitated by two adults, with

146
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

all questions read aloud to avoid issues relating to poor reading ability. However, as

most assessment sessions were conducted in classroom groups of approximately 25 to

30 students, thorough supervision of all participants’ progress was impossible. It is

also likely that missing data within participants was an artefact of the assessment

process, which necessitated the completion of the questionnaire package over a week-

long period, rather than all at once. It is possible that the proportion of missing data

may have been lessened by conducting the complete questionnaire battery during one

session, though admittedly this would have been a time-intensive exercise, and

potentially tiring and frustrating for the children. Whilst endeavours were made to

maximise consistency of the assessment phase both within and between schools, the

individual structure, staff availability, and timetabling issues of each school presented

obvious barriers to ensuring a uniform process.

Future Directions

Research into the universal prevention of childhood anxiety is still in its

infancy; the very first published evaluation of intervention at a universal level is

scarcely 10 years old (Barrett & Turner, 2001), and there remains much work to be

done in this field. In particular, the dearth of studies investigating prevention of

childhood anxiety in socioeconomically disadvantaged communities must be

addressed. Individuals in these communities have been shown to be most at risk of a

range of adverse psychosocial factors, not least of which is clinical anxiety. In the

interest of minimising the disease burden of internalising disorders on a macro level,

priority must be directed to prevention, especially in high-risk populations. Further

evaluation of the FRIENDS program within disadvantaged schools may provide

impetus for this program to eventually be implemented as part of the curriculum

147
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

within such schools. This initiative would overcome the many barriers to mental

health services faced by those families who most require help.

Future evaluations of childhood anxiety prevention in socioeconomically

disadvantaged regions should strive to overcome what is arguably the most inherent

challenge in universal school-based prevention – missing data. Whilst issues such as

increased absenteeism and residential instability in these populations are not easily

overcome, greater data integrity may be achieved by ensuring that participants

complete the questionnaire battery within one session. This would necessarily require

more planning and personnel, and may be facilitated by selecting fewer, or more

succinct assessment measures. Additionally, reductions in missing data may be

achieved by providing incentives for children to complete the questionnaires, such

token rewards (for example, stickers), or enjoyable activities such as computer time,

free play, or sports games in lieu of classroom time. Such initiatives would require

greater collaboration between the research team and the schools involved, to

determine a mutually appropriate option that does not heavily compromise class time.

Another option may be to provide a significant prize, such as a family pass to a theme

park. Children may be more motivated to fully complete their questionnaires if they

are eligible to win such a prize.

The current study focused exclusively on intervention outcomes in three

disadvantaged schools within an urban geographical area. Whilst further studies in

urban regions are required to highlight the value of implementing programs such as

FRIENDS for Life into the school curriculum, future research should also focus on

evaluating potential similarities and differences in program effectiveness between

148
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

urban and rural disadvantaged schools. Such work would build on the existing small

body of research investigating the impact of internalising disorders in rural school

children (Lyneham & Rapee, 2007; Roberts, Kane, Thomson, Bishop, & Hart, 2003).

A related project may be an evaluation of the FRIENDS program within both urban

and rural indigenous school communities, which are typically disadvantaged on many

levels. A comparison of program outcomes between urban, rural, and indigenous

children may provide useful information to help optimise programs such as FRIENDS

for Life for special needs populations, by adopting a more tailored, and culturally

appropriate approach.

An additional suggestion for future research is to evaluate the longitudinal

effects of universal, school-based anxiety prevention for socioeconomically

disadvantaged populations beyond 12 months follow-up. Past research has

established that the positive gains associated with the FRIENDS program are retained

up to 3 years post-intervention for students commencing the intervention during late

primary school years (Barrett, et al., 2006). However, this research was not

conducted within disadvantaged communities. Given the increased hardships,

challenges and traumas typically faced by individuals from these communities, it

would be interesting to determine whether the resilience benefits afforded by the

intervention are enough to buffer against such experiences over the long term.

Since the commencement of this study, a number of positive measures of

psychosocial functioning have become available for use in prevention research.

These measures assess factors such as hopefulness, self-efficacy, life satisfaction,

social support and resilience. The ‘new generation’ of research into the FRIENDS

149
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

program is incorporating such measures into their questionnaire batteries, and

published results are eagerly anticipated. Future prevention research should

endeavour to incorporate not only traditional measures of symptomatology and

functioning deficits, but also these more positive ‘strengths-based’ measures, to

provide a more thorough picture of program effectiveness. Future studies should also

strive to gather data from additional sources, rather than just the study participants.

Such sources include classroom teachers and parents, who can provide collateral

information to provide a broader view of program effectiveness.

Whilst efforts were made to keep parents informed of the program

components of the current study, the active involvement of parents in the intervention

was not a major priority of this research. There is evidence demonstrating that

clinically significant reduction in childhood anxiety is possible, independent of

parental involvement in interventions (Kendall, 1994). However, given that later

research has pointed to the benefits of involving parents in the treatment component

(Barrett, Dadds, & Rapee, 1996), the goal of increasing parental involvement in

universal prevention may be a key aspect of future research. In terms of research with

disadvantaged communities, increasing parental engagement is likely to be a

challenging task. However, given the increased risk of psychopathology to both

adults and children in such communities, it would be interesting to determine whether

the involvement of parents may produce greater improvements in child functioning, as

well as additional benefits to parents.

150
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Summary

The present study was the first to examine the efficacy of the FRIENDS for

Life program delivered as a universal school-based intervention specifically within a

socioeconomically disadvantaged community. The results indicate that the

intervention was associated with robust, long-term reductions in internalising

symptoms, in conjunction with significant improvements in self-esteem, coping skills,

and other aspects of psychosocial functioning. This research suggests that

intervention at a universal level is sufficient to produce significant reductions in

anxiety and depression symptoms in children from disadvantaged communities,

despite the increased risk of internalising disorders faced by individuals in these

communities. Socioeconomically disadvantaged regions remain an ignored

population in universal childhood anxiety prevention research, and there is scope for

much additional work in this field. Recommendations for future research include

investigating the long-term effectiveness of anxiety prevention programs given the

increased risks in these communities, as well as comparisons between urban and rural

or indigenous disadvantaged populations. Future researchers should also strive to

incorporate measures of positive psychosocial functioning, and to obtain information

from multiple sources, to provide a more complete picture of program effectiveness

for children from socioeconomically disadvantaged communities.

151
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of

attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. New Jersey:

Erlbaum.

Albano, A., Marten, P. A., Holt, C. S., Heimburg, R. G., & Barlow, D. (1995).

Cognitive-behavioural group treatment for social phobia in adolescents: A

preliminary study. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183, 649-656. doi:

10.1097/00005053-199510000-00006

Allen, J. L., Rapee, R., & Sandberg, S. (2008). Severe life events and chronic

adversities as antecedents to anxiety in children: a matched control study.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1047-1056. doi: 10.1007/s10802-

008-9240-x

American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Anderson, J. C., Williams, S., MgGee, R., & Silva, P. A. (1987). DSM-III disorders in

preadolescent children: Prevalence in a large sample from the general

population. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 69-76. Retrieved from

http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/

Aneshensel, C. S., & Sucoff, C. A. (1996). The neighbourhood context of adolescent

mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 37(4), 293-310. doi:

10.2307/2137258

Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Erkanli, A. (1999). Comorbidity. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(1), 57-87. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00424

152
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Worthman, C. M. (1998). Puberty and depression: the

roles of age, pubertal status and pubertal timing. Psychological Medicine,

28(1), 51-61. doi: 10.1017/S003329179700593X

Armbruster, P. (2002). The administration of school based mental health services.

Child and adolescent Psychiatry Clinics of North America, 11, 23-41. doi:

10.1016/S1056-4993(03)00059-2

Aronson, T. A., & Logue, C. M. (1987). On the longitundinal course of panic

disorder: Developmental history and predictors of phobic complications.

Comprehensive Psychiatry, 28(4), 344-355. doi: 10.1016/0010-

440X(87)90071-X

Asendorpf, J. B. (1990). Development of inhibition during childhood: evidence for

situational specificity and a two-factor model. Developmental

Psychopathology, 26, 721-730. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.26.5.721

Ashford, J., Smit, F., van Lier, P. A., Cuijpers, P., & Koot, H. M. (2008). Early risk

indicators of internalising problems in late childhood: A 9-year longitudinal

study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 774-780. doi:

10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01889.x

Australian Bureau of Statistics (2008) Census of Population and Housing: Socio-

Economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA), Australia - Data only, 2006, 'Table 3:

Statistical Local Area (SLA) Index of relative socioeconomic disadvantage,

2006', data cube: Excel spreadsheet, Cat. no. 2033.0.55.001, viewed 19 May

2008,<http://www.abs.gov.au/AUSSTATS/abs@.nsf/DetailsPage/2033.0.55.0

012006?OpenDocument>

Australian Bureau of Statistics (2006) An Introduction to Socio-Economic Indexes for

Areas (SEIFA), Cat. no. 2039.0, viewed 19 May 2008,

153
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

<http://www.abs.gov.au/AUSSTATS/abs@.nsf/Lookup/2039.0Main%20Featu

res42006?opendocument&tabname=Summary&prodno=2039.0&issue=2006

&num=&view=2039.0 >

Bandelow, B., Torrente, A. C., Wedekind, D., Broocks, A., Hajak, G., & Rüther, E.

(2004). Early traumatic life events, parental rearing styles, family history of

mental disorders, and birth risk factors in patients with social anxiety disorder.

European Archives of Psychiatric and Clinical Neuroscience, 254(6), 397-

405. doi: 10.1007/s00406-004-0521-2

Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van

Yzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and non-

anxious individuals: a meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1-24.

doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1

Barrett, P. M. (1998). Evaluation of cognitive-behavioral group treatments for

childhood anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27(4), 459-

468. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9866083

Barrett, P. M. (2004). Friends for Life Program - Group Leader's Workbook for

Children (4th ed.). Brisbane: Australian Academic Press.

Barrett, P. (2004). Friends for Life Program - Participant Workbook for Children (4th

ed.). Brisbane: Australian Academic Press.

Barrett, P. M. (2005). Friends for Life Program - Group Leader's Workbook for Youth

(4th ed.). Brisbane: Australian Academic Press.

Barrett, P. M. (2007). Fun Friends. The Teaching and Training Manual for Group

Leaders. Brisbane: Fun Friends Publishing.

154
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Barrett, P. M., Dadds, M., & Rapee, R. M. (1996). Family treatment of childhood

anxiety: A controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64,

333-342. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.64.2.333

Barrett, P. M., Duffy, A., Dadds, M., & Rapee, R. (2001). Cognitive-behavioural

treatment of anxiety disorders in children: Long term (6 year) follow-up.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 135-141. doi:

10.1037/0022-006X.69.1.135

Barrett, P., & Farrell, L. (2007). Behavioural intervention for childhood anxiety. In J.

M. Briesmeister & C. E. Schaefer (Eds.), Handbook of parent training:

Helping parents prevent and solve problem behaviours (pp. 133-163). New

Jersey: Wiley.

Barrett, P. M., Farrell, L. J., Ollendick, T. H., & Dadds, M. (2006). Long-Term

Outcomes of an Australian Universal Prevention Trial of Anxiety and

Depression Symptoms in Children and Youth: An Evaluation of the Friends

Program. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35(3), 403-

411. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3503_5

Barrett, P. M., Fox, T. L., & Farrell, L. J. (2005). Parent-child interactions with

anxious children and their siblings: An observational study. Behaviour

Change, 22, 220-235. doi: 10.1375/bech.22.4.220

Barrett, P. M., Lock, S., & Farrell, L. J. (2005). Developmental differences in

universal preventive intervention for child anxiety. Clinical Child Psychology

and Psychiatry, 10(4), 539-555. doi: 10.1177/1359104505056317

Barrett, P. M., Lowry-Webster, H., & Holmes, J. (1999). FRIENDS for Children

group leader manual (2nd ed.). Queensland, Australia: Australian Academic

Press.

155
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Barrett, P., Lowry-Webster, H., & Turner, C. (2000). FRIENDS program for children:

Group leader's manual. Brisbane: Australian Academic Press.

Barrett, P. M., Moore, A. F., & Sonderegger, R. (2000). The FRIENDS program for

young former-Yugoslavian refugees in Australia: A pilot study. Behaviour

Change, 17(3), 124-133. doi: 10.1375/bech.17.3.124

Barrett, P., & Pahl, K. M. (2006). School-based intervention: Examining a universal

approach to anxiety management. Australian Journal of Guidance &

Counselling, 16(1), 55-75. doi: 10.1375/ajgc.16.1.55

Barrett, P., & Turner, C. (2001). Prevention of anxiety symptoms in primary school

children: Preliminary results from a universal school-based trial. British

Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40(4), 399-410. doi:

10.1348/014466501163887

Barrett, P. M., & Turner, C. M. (2004). Prevention of childhood anxiety and

depression. In P. M. Barrett & T. H. Ollendick (Eds.), Handbook of

interventions that work with children and adolescents: Prevention and

treatment (pp. 429-474). New York: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

Barrett, P. M., Turner, C. M., & Sonderegger, R. (2000). Childhood anxiety in ethnic

families: Current status and future directions. Behaviour Change. Special

Issue: Adolescent health, 17(3), 113-123. doi: 10.1375/bech.17.3.113

Barrett, P., Webster, H., & Turner, C. (2000). FRIENDS Prevention of Anxiety and

Depression for Children: Group Leader's Manual. Bowen Hills, Australia:

Australian Academic Press.

Barrett, P. M., Rapee, R. M., Dadds, M. R., & Ryan, S. M. (1996). Family

enhancement of cognitive style in anxious and aggressive children. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 24(2), 187-203. doi: 10.1007/BF01441484

156
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Barrett, P. M., Sonderegger, R., & Sonderegger, M. L. (2001). Evaluation of an

anxiety-prevention and positive-coping program (FRIENDS) for children and

adolescents of non-English-speaking background. Behaviour Change, 18(2),

78-91. doi: 10.1375/bech.18.2.78

Barrett, P. M., Sonderegger, R., & Xenos, S. (2003). Using FRIENDS to combat

anxiety and adjustment problems among young migrants to Australia: A

national trial. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 8(2), 241-260. doi:

10.1177/1359104503008002008

Bayer, J. K., Sanson, A. V., & Hemphill, S. A. (2006). Parent influences on early

childhood internalising difficulties. Journal of Applied Developmental

Psychology, 27, 542-559. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.08.002

Beckett, C., Maughan, B., Rutter, M., Castle, J., Colvert, E., & Groothues, C. (2006).

Do the effects of early severe deprivation on cognition persist into early

adolescence? Findings from the English and Romanian Adoptees Study.

Child Development, 77, 696-711. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00898.x

Beidel, D. C. (1991). Social phobia and overanxious disorder in school-age children.

Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 545-

552. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199107000-00003

Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (1988). Shy children, phobic adults: Nature and

treatment of social phobia. Washington, DC: American Psychological

Association.

Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (1997). At risk for anxiety: I. Psychopathology in the

offspring of anxious parents. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 918-924. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-

00013

157
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Bell-Dolan, D. J., Last, C. G., & Strauss, C. C. (1990). Symptoms of anxiety disorders

in normal children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 29(5), 759-765. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199009000-00014

Benjamin, R., Costello, E. J., & Warren, M. (1990). Anxiety disorders in a pediatric

sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 4(4), 293-316. doi: 10.1016/0887-

6185(90)90027-7

Berger, R., Pat-Horenczyk, R., & Gelkopf, M. (2007). School-based intervention for

prevention and treatment of elementary-students' terror-related distress in

Israel: A quasi-randomized controlled trial. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 20,

541-551. doi: 10.1002/jts.20225

Bernstein, G. A., Layne, A. E., Egan, E. A., & Tennison, D. M. (2005). School-based

interventions for anxious children. Journal of the American Academy of Child

& Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(11), 1118-1127. doi:

10.1097/01.chi.0000177323.40005.a1

Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Hirschfeld-Becker, D. R., Friedman, D., Robin, J. A.,

& Rosenbaum, J. F. (2001). Patterns of psychopathology and dysfunction in

righ-risk children of parents with panic disorder and major depression.

American Journal of Psychiatry, 158(49-57). doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.1.49

Biederman, J., Hirschfeld-Becker, D. R., Rosenbaum, J. F., Herot, C., Friedman, D.,

Snidman, N., Kagan, J., & Faraone, S.V. (2001). Further evidence of

association between behavioural inhibition and social anxiety in children.

American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 1673-1679. doi:

10.1176/appi.ajp.158.10.1673

Biederman, J., Rosenbaum, J. F., Bolduc-Murphey, E. A., Faraone, S. V., Chaloff, J.,

Hirschfeld, D. R., & Kagan, J. (1993). A 3-year follow-up of children with and

158
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

without behavioural inhibition. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(4), 814-821. Retrieved from

http://www.jaacap.com/

Biederman, J., Rosenbaum, J. F., Hirschfeld, D. R., Faraone, S. V., Bolduc, E. A.,

Gersten, M., Merninger, S.R., Kagan, J., Snidman, N., & Reznick, S. (1990).

Psychiatric correlates of behavioral inhibition in young children of parents

with and without psychiatric disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 47(1),

21-26. Retrieved from http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/

Bienvenu, J. O., & Ginsburg, G. S. (2007). Prevention of anxiety disorders.

International Review of Psychiatry, 19(6), 647-654. doi:

10.1080/09540260701797837

Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1981). The role of coping response and social

resources in attenuating the impact of stressful life events. Journal of

Behavioural Medicine, 4, 129-147. Retrieved from

http://www.springerlink.com/content/101595/

Boer, F., Markus, M. T., Maingay, R., Lindhout, I. E., Borst, S. R., & Hoogendijk, T.

H. G. (2002). Negative life events of anxiety disordered children: Bad fortune,

vulnerability, or reporter bias? Child Psychiatry & Human Development,

32(3), 187-199. doi: 10.1023/A:1017952605299

Bögels, S. M., & Zigterman, D. (2000). Dysfunctional cognitions in children with

social phobia, separation anxiety disorder, and generalised anxiety disorder.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(2), 205-211. doi:

10.1023/A:1005179032470

Bond, L., Glover, S., Godfrey, C., Butler, H., & Patton, G. C. (2001). Building

capacity for system level change in schools: lessons from the Gatehouse

159
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Project. Health Education Behaviour, 28, 368-383. doi:

10.1177/109019810102800310

Booth, C. L., Rose-Krasnor, L., McKinnon, J., & Rubin, K. H. (1994). Predicting

social adjustment in middle childhood: The role of preschool attachment

security and maternal style. Social Development, 3, 189-204. doi:

10.1111/j.1467-9507.1994.tb00040.x

Bor, W., Najman, J. M., Andersen, M. J., O'Callaghan, M., Williams, G. M., &

Behrens, B. C. (1997). The relationship between low family income and

psychological disturbance in young children: An Australian longitudinal

study. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 31, 664-675. doi:

10.3109/00048679709062679

Bosquet, M., & Egeland, B. (2006). The development and maintenance of anxiety

symptoms from infancy through adolescence in a longitudinal sample.

Development and Psychopathology, 18(2), 517-550. doi:

10.1017/S0954579406060275

Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human

development. New York: Basic Books.

Box, G. E. P., & Cox, D. R. (1964). An analysis of transformations. Journal of the

Royal Statistical Society, Series B, 26 (2), 211–252. Retrieved from

http://www.rss.org.uk/main.asp?page=1711

Boyd, C. P., & Gullone, E. (1997). An investigation of negative affectivity in

Australian adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26(2), 190-197.

Retrieved from http://www.jccap.net/

Boyd, C. P., Kostanski, M., Gullone, E., Ollendick, T. H., & Shek, D. T. L. (2000).

Prevalence of anxiety and depression in Australian adolescents: Comparisons

160
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

with worldwide data. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 161, 479-492.

Retrieved from

http://heldref.metapress.com/app/home/journal.asp?referrer=parent&backto=li

nkingpublicationresults,1:119924,1&linkin=634110373429061760

Bradley, S. (2000). Affect regulation and the development of psychopathology. New

York: Guilford.

Brady, E. U., & Kendall, P. C. (1992). Comorbidity of anxiety and depression in

children and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 244-255. doi:

10.1037/0033-2909.111.2.244

Brodzinsky, D. M., Elias, M. J., Steiger, C., Simon, J., Gill, M., & Clarke Hitt, J.

(1992). Coping scale for children and youth: Scale development and

validation. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 13, 195-214. doi:

10.1016/0193-3973(92)90029-H

Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G. J., & Aber, J. L. (1997). Neighborhood Poverty. New

York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Bruce, M. L., Takeuchi, D. T., & Leaf, P. J. (1991). Poverty and psychiatric status:

Longitudinal evidence from the New Haven Epidemiologic Catchment Area

Study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 48(5), 470-474. Retrieved from

http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/

Bruch, M. A., & Heimburg, R. G. (1994). Differences in perceptions of parental and

personal characteristics between generalised and nongeneralised social

phobics. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 8(2), 155-168. doi: 10.1016/0887-

6185(94)90013-2

Bruch, M. A., Heimburg, R. G., Berger, P., & Collins, T. M. (1989). Social phobia

and perceptions of early parental and personal characteristics. Anxiety

161
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Research, 2(1), 57-65. Retrieved from

http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/content~db=all~content=a778321326

Brumariu, L. E., & Kerns, K. A. (2008). Mother-child attachment and social anxiety

symptoms in middle childhood. Journal of Applied Developmental

Psychology, 29(5), 393-402. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.06.002

Buck, N. (2001). Identifying neighbourhood effects on social exclusion. Urban

Studies, 38(12), 2251-2275. Retrieved from http://usj.sagepub.com/

Buckner, J. C., & Bassuk, E. L. (1997). Mental disorders and service utilization

among youths from homeless and low income housed families. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 890-900. doi:

10.1097/00004583-199707000-00010

Buckner, J. C., Mezzacappa, E., & Beardslee, W. R. (2003). Characteristics of

resilient youths living in poverty: The role of self-regulatory processes.

Development and Psychopathology, 15(1), 139-162. doi:

10.1017/S0954579403000087

Buka, S. L., Monuteaux, M., & Felton, E. (2002). The epidemiology of child and

adolescent mental disorders. In M. T. Tsuang & M. Tohen (Eds.), Textbook in

Psychiatric Epidemiology (Second ed., pp. 657-704). New York: John Wiley

& Sons

Burke, C. B., Burke, J. C., Regier, D. A., & Rae, D. S. (1990). Age of onset of

selected mental disorders in five community populations. Archives of General

Psychiatry, 47, 511-518. Retrieved from http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/

Capps, L., Sigman, M., Sena, R., Henker, B., & Whalen, C. (1996). Fear, anxiety and

perceived control in children of agoraphobic parents. Journal of Child

162
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(4), 445-452. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-

7610.1996.tb01425.x

Carlson, E. A., & Sroufe, L. A. (1995). Contribution of attachment theory to

developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.),

Developmental processes and psychopathology: Volume 1. Theoretical

perspectives and methodological approaches (pp. 581-617). New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Cartwright-Hatton, S., McNicol, K., & Doubleday, E. (2006). Anxiety in a neglected

population: Prevalence of anxiety disorders in pre-adolescent children.

Clinical Psychology Review, 26(7), 817-833. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.12.002

Cartwright-Hatton, S., & Murray, J. (2008). Cognitive therapy with children and

families: treating internalizing disorders. Behavioural and Cognitive

Psychotherapy, 36(6), 749-756. doi: 10.1017/S1352465808004840

Cartwright-Hatton, S., Roberts, C., Chitsabesan, P., Fothergill, C., & Harrington, R.

(2004). Systematic review of the efficacy of cognitive behaviour therapies for

childhood and adolescent anxiety disorders. British Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 43(4), 421-436. doi: 10.1348/0144665042388928

Cartwright-Hatton, S., Tschernitz, N., & Gomersall, H. (2005). Social anxiety in

children: Social skills deficit or cognitive distortion? Behaviour Research and

Therapy, 43(1), 131-141. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.12.003

Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. (1989). Assessing coping strateiges:

A theoreticallybased approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

54, 267-283. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267

163
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Caspi, A., Taylor, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Plomin, R. (2000). Neighborhood deprivation

affects children's menatl health: Environmental risks itentified in a genetic

design. Psychological Science, 11, 338-342. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00267

Caster, J. B., Inderbitzen, H. M., & Hope, D. (1999). Relationship between youth and

parent perceptions of family environment and social anxiety. Journal of

Anxiety Disorders, 13(3), 237-251. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(99)00002-X

Cauce, A. M., Stewart, A., Rodriguez, M. D., Cochran, B., & Ginzler, J. (2003).

Overcoming the odds? Adolescent development in the context of urban

poverty. In S. S. Luthar (Ed.), Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation in the

context of childhood adversities (pp. 343-363). New York: Cambridge

University Press.

Chatterji, P., Caffray, C. M., Crowe, M., Freeman, L., & Jensen, P. (2004). Cost-

assessment of a school-based mental health screening and treatment program

in New York City. Mental Health Services Research, 6, 155-166. doi:

10.1023/B:MHSR.0000036489.50470.cb

Cicchetti, D., Ackerman, B. P., & Izard, C. E. (1995). Emotions and emotion

regulation in developmental psychopathology. Development and

Psychopathology, 7, 1-10. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400006301

Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (1997). The role of self-organization in the promotion

of resilience in maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology, 9(4),

797-815. doi: 10.1017/S0954579497001442

Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Lynch, M., & Holt, K. D. (1993). Resilience in

maltreated children: Processes leading to adaptive outcome. Development and

Psychopathology, 5(4), 629-647. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400004429

164
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Clark, D. B., & Neighbors, B. (1996). Adolescent substance abuse and internalizing

disorders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 5(1),

45-57. Retrieved from http://www.childpsych.theclinics.com/

Clark, D. B., & Winters, K. C. (2002). Measuring risks and outcomes in substance use

disorders prevention research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,

70(6), 1207-1223. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.70.6.1207

Cnaan, A., Laird, N. M., & Slasor, P. (1998). Using the general linear mixed model to

analyse unbalanced repeated measures and longitudinal data. Statistics in

Medicine, 16(20), 2349-2380. Retrieved from

http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/2988/home?CRETRY=1&SRET

RY=0

Cobham, V. E., Dadds, M. R., & Spence, S. H. (1998). The role of parental anxiety in

the treatment of childhood anxiety. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 66(6), 893-905. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.66.6.893

Coie, J. D., Watt, N. F., West, S. G., Hawkins, J. D., Asarnow, J. R., Markman, H. J.,

Ramey, S.L., Shure, M.B., & Long, B. (1993). The science of prevention: A

conceptual framework and some directions for a national research program.

American Psychologist, 48, 1013-1022. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.48.10.1013

Cole, D. A., Peeke, L. G., Martin, J. M., Truglio, R., & Seroczynski, A. D. (1998). A

longitudinal look at the relation between depression and anxiety in children

and adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66(3), 451-

460. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.66.3.451

Cole, P. M., Michel, M., & Teti, L. (1994). The development of emotion regulation

and dysregulation: A clinical perspective. Monographs of the Society for

Research in Child Development, 59, 73-100. doi: 10.2307/1166139

165
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Coleman, J. (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal

of Sociology, 94, 95-120. Retrieved from

http://www.journals.uchicago.edu/toc/ajs/current

Compas, B. E., Phares, V., Banez, G. A., & Howell, D. C. (1991). Correlates of

internalising and externalising behavior problems: Perceived competence,

causal attributions, and parental symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 19(2), 197-218. doi: 10.1007/BF00909978

Compas, B. E., Malcarne, V. L., & Fondacaro, K. M. (1988). Coping with stressful

events in older children and young adolescents. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 56, 405-411. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.3.405

Compton, S. N., March, J. S., Brent, D., Albano, A. M., Weersing, V. R., & Curry, J.

(2004). Cognitive-behavioural psychotherapy for anxiety and depressive

disorders in children and adolescents: An evidence-based medicine review.

Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(8),

930-959. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000127589.57468.bf

Cooley-Quille, M., Boyd, R. C., Frantz, E., & Walsh, J. (2001). Emotional and

behavioural impact of exposure to community violence in inner-city

adolescents. . Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30(2), 199-206. Retrieved

from http://www.jccap.net/

Cooley, M. R., Boyd, R. C., & Grades, J. J. (2004). Feasibility of an anxiety

prevention intervention for commmunity violence exposed African-American

children. The Journal of Primary Prevention, 25(1), 105-123. doi:

10.1023/B:JOPP.0000039941.85452.ea

Coopersmith, S. (1967). The antecedents of self-esteem. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.

166
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Coopersmith, S. (1989). Self-esteem Inventories. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting

Psychologists Press.

Costello, E. J., Angold, A., Burns, B. J., Stangl, D. K., Tweed, D. L., Erkanli, A., &

Worthman, C.M. (1996). The Great Smoky Mountains Study of youth: Goals,

design, methods, and the prevalence of DSM-III-R disorders. Archives of

General Psychiatry, 53(12), 1129-1136. Retrieved from http://archpsyc.ama-

assn.org/

Costello, E. J., Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2004). Developmental epidemiology of

anxiety disorders. In T. H. Ollendick & J. S. Marsh (Eds.), Phobic and Anxiety

Disorders in Children and Adolescents: A Clinician's Guide to Effective

Psychosocial and Pharmacological Interventions. New York: Oxford

University Press.

Costello, E. J., Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2005). The developmental epidemiology

of anxiety disorders: phenomenology, prevalence, and comorbidity. Child and

Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 14, 631-648. doi:

10.1016/j.chc.2005.06.003

Costello, E. J., Mustillo, S., Erkanli, A., Keeler, G., & Angold, A. (2003). Prevalence

and development of psychiatric disorders in children and adolescence.

Archives of General Psychiatry, 60(8), 837-844. doi:

10.1001/archpsyc.60.8.837

Cowen, E. L., Pedro-Carroll, J. L., & Alpert-Gillis, L. J. (1990). Relationships

between support and adjustment among children of divorce. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 31, 727-735. doi:

10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb00813.x

167
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Craig, K. D., & Dobson, K. S. (Eds.). (1995). Anxiety and depression in adults and

children. CA: Sage Publications.

Cunningham, P. J., & Frieman, M. P. (1996). Determinants of ambulatory mental

health service use for school-age children and adolescents. Health Services

Research, 31(4), 409-427. Retrieved from http://www.hsr.org/

Curry, J. F., March, J. S., & Hervey, A. S. (2004). Comorbidity of childhood and

adolescent anxiety disorders: Prevalence and Implications. In T. H. Ollendick

& J. S. March (Eds.), Phobic and Anxiety Disorders in Children and

Adolescents (pp. 116-140). New York: Oxford University Press.

Curtis, L. J., Dooley, M. D., Lipman, E. L., & Feeny, D. H. (2001). The role of

permanent income and family structure in the determination of child health in

Canada. Health Economics, 10, 287-302. Retrieved from

http://www3.interscience.wiley.com/journal/5749/home

Dadds, M. R., Holland, D. E., Laurens, K. R., Mullins, M., Barrett, P., & Spence, S.

H. (1999). Early intervention and prevention of anxiety disorders in children:

Results at 2-year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,

67(1), 145-150. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.67.1.145

Dadds, M. R., Spence, S. H., Holland, D. E., Barrett, P. M., & Laurens, K. R. (1997).

Prevention and early intervention for anxiety disorders: A controlled trial.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 65(4), 627-635. doi:

10.1037/0022-006X.65.4.627

Dallaire, D. H., & Weinraub, M. (2005). Predicting children's separation anxiety at

age 6: The contributions of infant-mother attachment security, maternal

sensitivity, and maternal separation anxiety. Attachment and Human

Development, 7, 393-408. doi: 10.1080/14616730500365894

168
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Dallaire, D. H., & Weinraub, M. (2007). Infant-mother attachment security and

children's anxiety and aggression at first grade. Journal of Applied

Developmental Psychology, 28, 477-492. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2007.06.005

Danielsen, A. G., Samdal, O., Hetland, J., & Wold, B. (2009). School-related social

support and students' perceived life satisfaction. The Journal of Education

Research, 102(4), 303-318. doi: 10.3200/JOER.102.4.303-320

Davidson, L. M., & Demaray, M. K. (2007). Social support as a moderator between

victimization and internalizing-externalizing distress from bullying. School

Psychology Review, 36(3), 383-405. Retrieved from

http://www.nasponline.org/publications/spr/sprmain.aspx

Deykin, E. Y., & Buka, S. L. (1997). Prevalence and risk factors for posttraumatic

stress disorder among chemically dependent adolescents. The American

Journal of Psychiatry, 154(6), 752-757. Retrieved from

http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/

DiLalla, L. F., Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1994). Genetic etiology of behavioral

inhibition among 2-year-old children. Infant Behavioral Development, 17,

405-412. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(94)90032-9

Dineen, K. A., & Hadwin, J. A. (2004). Anxious and depressive symptoms and

children's judgements of their own and others' interpretation of ambiguous

social scenarios. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 18(4), 499-513. doi:

10.1016/S0887-6185(03)00030-6

Dohrenwend, B. P., Levav, I., Shrout, P. E., Schwartz, S., Naveh, G., Link, B. G.,

Skodol, A.E., & Stueve, A. (1992). Socioeconomic status and psychiatric

disorders: The causation-selection issue. Science, 255, 946-952. doi:

10.1126/science.1546291

169
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Dollinger, S. J. (1986). The need for meaning following disaster: Attributions and

emotional upset. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12(3), 300-310.

doi: 10.1177/0146167286123006

Dollinger, S. J., O'Donnell, J. P., & Staley, A. A. (1984). Lightning-strike disaster:

Effects on children's fears and worries. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 52(6), 1028-1038. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.52.6.1028

Donovan, C. L., & Spence, S. H. (2000). Prevention of childhood anxiety disorders.

Clinical Psychology Review, 20(4), 509-531. doi: 10.1016/S0272-

7358(99)00040-9

Driessen, G., Gunther, N., & Van Os, J. (1998). Shared social environment and

psychiatric disorder: A multilevel analysis of individual and ecological effects.

Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33(12), 606-612. doi:

10.1007/s001270050100

Drukker, M., Kaplan, C., Feron, F., & van Os, J. (2003). Children's health-related

quality of life, neighbourhood socioeconomic deprivation, and social capital:

A contextual analysis. Social Science & Medicine, 57, 825-841. doi:

10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00453-7

Duggal, S., Malkoff-Schwartz, S., Birmaher, B., Anderson, B. P., Matty, M. K.,

Houck, P. R., Bailey-Orr, M., Williamson, D.E., & Ellen, F. (2000).

Assessment of life stress in adolescents: self-report versus interview methods.

Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 445-

452. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200004000-00013

Ebata, A. T., & Moos, R. H. (1991). Coping and adjustment in distressed and healthy

adolescents. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 12, 33-54. doi:

10.1016/0193-3973(91)90029-4

170
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Edwards, J. M., & Trimble, K. (1992). Anxiety, coping and academic performance.

Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 5, 337-350. doi: 10.1080/10615809208248370

Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Murphey, B. C., Guthrie, I. K., Jones, S.,

Friedman, J., Poulin, R., & Maszak, P. (1997). Contemporaneous and

longitudinal prediction of children’s social functioning from regulation and

emotionality. Child Development, 68, 642-664. doi: 10.2307/1132116

Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Fabes, R. A., Reiser, M., Murphey, B. C., Holgren, R.,

Maszk, P., & Losoya, S. (1997). The relations of regulation and emotionality

to resiliency and competent social functioning in elementary school children.

Child Development, 68, 295-311. doi: 10.2307/1131851

Ellen, I. G., & Turner, M. A. (1997). Does neighborhood matter? Assessing recent

evidence. Housing Policy Debate, 8(4), 833-866. Retrieved from

http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/rhpd

Endler, N. S., & Parker, J. D. A. (1990). State and trait anxiety, depression and coping

styles. Australian Journal of Psychology, 42, 207-220. doi:

10.1080/00049539008260119

Erickson, M. F., Sroufe, L. A., & Egeland, B. (1985). The relationship between

quality of attachment and behavior problems in preschool and a high risk

sample. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 50,

147-166. doi: 10.2307/3333831

Essau, C. A. (2005). Frequency and patterns of mental health services utilization

among adolescents with anxiety and depressive disorders. Depression and

Anxiety, 22(3), 130-137. doi: 10.1002/da.20115

171
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Essau, C. A., Conradt, J., & Petermann, F. (2000). Frequency, comorbidity, and

psychological impairment of anxiety disorders in German adolescents. Journal

of Anxiety Disorders, 14(3), 263-279. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(99)00039-0

Essau, C. A., & Peterman, F. (Eds.). (2001). Anxiety disorders in children and

adolescents: epidemiology, risk factors and treatment. East Sussex: Brunner-

Routledge.

Esser, G., Schmidt, M. H., & Woemer, W. (1990). Epidemiology and course of

psychiatric disorders in school-age children: Results of a longitudinal study

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31(2), 243-263. doi:

10.1111/j.1469-7610.1990.tb01565.x

Evans, S. W. (1999). Mental health services in schools: Utilization, effectiveness and

consent. Clinical Psychology Review, 19, 165-178. doi: 10.1016/S0272-

7358(98)00069-5

Faris, R. E. L., & Dunham, W. C. (1939). Mental Disorders in Urban Areas: An

Ecological Study of Schizophrenia and other Psychoses. Chicago: University

of Chicago Press.

Farmer, E. M. Z., Burns, B. J., Phillips, S. D., Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (2003).

Pathways into and through mental health services for children and adolescents.

Psychiatric Services, 54(1), 60-66. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.54.1.60

Farrell, L. J., & Barrett, P. M. (2007). Prevention of childhood emotional disorders:

Reducing the burden of suffering associated with anxiety and depression.

Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 12(2), 58-65. doi: 10.1111/j.1475-

3588.2006.00430.x

Fisak, B., & Grills-Taquechel, A. E. (2007). Parental modeling, reinforcement, and

information transfer: risk factors in the development of childhood anxiety?

172
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 10(3), 213-231. doi:

10.1007/s10567-007-0020-x

Flannery-Schroeder, E. C., & Kendall, P. C. (2000). Group and individual cognitive-

behavioural treatments for youth with anxiety disorders: A randomised clinical

trial. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 251-278. doi:

10.1023/A:1005500219286

Flisher, A. K., Kramer, R. A., Grosser, R. C., Alegria, M., Bird, H. R., Bourdon, K.

H., Goodman, S.H., Greenwald, S., Horwitz, S.M., Moore, R.E., Narrow,

W.E., & Hoven, C.W. (1997). Correlates of unmet needs for mental health

services by children and adolescents. Psychological Medicine, 27, 1145-1154.

doi: 10.1017/S0033291797005412

Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of coping in a middle-aged

community sample. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 21(3), 219-239.

doi: 10.2307/2136617

Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1985). If it changes it must be a process: Study of

emotion and coping during three stages of a college examination. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 48(1), 150-170. doi: 10.1037/0022-

3514.48.1.150

Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Gruen, R. J., & DeLongis, A. (1986). Apraisal, coping,

health status, and psychological symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 50(3), 571-579. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.3.571

Gar, N. S., Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2005). Family factors and the development

of anxiety disorders. In J. L. Hudson & R. M. Rapee (Eds.), Psychopathology

and the family (pp. 125-145). New York: Elsevier Science.

173
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Garaigordobil, M. (2004). Effects of a psychological intervention on factors of

emotional development during adolescence. European Journal of

Psychological Assessment, 20, 66-80. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.20.1.66

Garcia-Coll, C., Kagan, J., & Reznick, J. S. (1984). Behavioral inhibition in young

children. Child Development, 55(3), 1005-1019. doi: 10.2307/1130152

Gladstone, G. L., & Parker, G. B. (2006). Is behavioral inhibition a risk factor for

depression? Journal of Affective Disorders, 95, 85-94. doi:

10.1016/j.jad.2006.04.015

Gladstone, G. L., Parker, G. B., Mitchell, P. B., Wilhelm, K. A., & Mahli, G. S.

(2005). Relationship between self-reported childhood behavioral inhibition

and lifetime anxiety disorders in a clinical sample. Depression and Anxiety,

22(3), 103-133. doi: 10.1002/da.20082

Goldberg, S., Gotowiec, A., & Simmons, R. J. (1995). Infant-mother attachment and

behavior problems in healthy and chronically ill preschoolers. Development

and Psychopathology, 7, 267-282. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400006490

Goldberg, S., MacKay-Soroka, S., & Rochester, M. (1994). Affect, attachment, and

maternal responsiveness. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 335-340. doi:

10.1016/0163-6383(94)90013-2

Gonzales, M. J. (2005). Access to mental health services: The struggle of poverty

affected urban children of colour. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal,

22(3), 245-256. Retrieved from http://www.springerlink.com/content/104690/

Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581-586. doi:

10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x

174
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the Strengths and Difficulties

Questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 40, 1337-1345. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200111000-00015

Goodman, R., & Scott, S. (1999). Comparing the strengths and difficulties

questionnaire and the child behaviour checklist: is small beautiful? Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 17-24. doi: 10.1023/A:1022658222914

Goodyer, I. M. (1996). Recent undesirable life events: their influence on subsequent

psychopathology. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 5, 33-37. doi:

10.1007/BF00538541

Goodyer, I. M., & Altham, P. M. (1991). Lifetime exit events and recent social and

family adversities in anxious and depressed school-age children and

adolescents: I. Journal of Affective Disorders, 21(4), 219-228. doi:

10.1016/0165-0327(91)90001-9

Gothelf, D., Aharonovsky, O., Horesh, N., Carty, T., & Apter, A. (2004). Life Events

and Personality Factors in Children and Adolescents With Obsessive-

Compulsive Disorder and Other Anxiety Disorders. Comprehensive

Psychiatry, 45(3), 192-198. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2004.02.010

Grant, K. E., Compas, B. E., Thurm, A. E., & McMahon, S. D. (2004). Stressors and

child and adolescent psychopathology: Measurement issues and prospective

effects. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 412-425.

doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3302_23

Greco, L. A., & Morris, T. L. (2005). Factors influencing the link between social

anxiety and peer acceptance: Contributions of social skills and close

friendships during middle childhood. Behavior Therapy, 36(2), 197-205. doi:

10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80068-1

175
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Greenberg, M. T., Sisitsky, T., Kessler, R. C., Finkelstein, S., Berndt, E., Davidson,

J., Ballenger, J.C., & Fyer, A.J. (1999). The economic burden of anxiety

disorders in the 1990s. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60, 427-435. Retrieved

from http://www.psychiatrist.com/default2.asp

Greenburg, M. T. (1999). Attachment and psychopathology in childhood. In J.

Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research,

and clinical applications (pp. 469-496). New York: Guilford Press.

Hadwin, J., Frost, S., French, C. C., & Richards, A. (1997). Cognitive processing and

trait anxiety in typically developing children: Evidence for an interpretaiton

bias. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(3), 486-490. doi: 10.1037/0021-

843X.106.3.486

Hains, A. A. (1992). Comparison of cognitive-behavioural stress management

techniques with adolescent boys. Journal of Counseling and Development, 70,

600-605. Retrieved from http://www.accessmylibrary.com/archive/2344-

journal-of-counseling-and-development.html

Hains, A. A., & Ellmann, S. W. (1994). Stress innoculation training as a preventative

intervention for high school youths. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An

International Quarterly, 8, 219-232. Retrieved from

http://www.springerpub.com/product/08898391

Hains, A. A., & Szyjakowski, M. (1990). A cognitive stress-reduction intervention

program for adolescents. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37, 79-84. doi:

10.1037/0022-0167.37.1.79

Halfon, N., Inkelas, M., & Wood, D. (1995). Nonfinancial barriers to care for children

and youth. Annual Review of Public Health, 16, 447-472. Retrieved from

http://arjournals.annualreviews.org/loi/publhealth?cookieSet=1

176
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Hankin, B. L., Abramson, L. Y., Moffit, T. E., Silva, A., McGee, R., & Angell, K. E.

(1998). Development of depression from preadolescence to young adulthood:

Emerging gender differences in a 10-year longitudinal study. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 107, 128-140. doi: 10.1037/0021-

843X.107.1.128

Hannesdottir, D. K., & Ollendick, T. H. (2007). The role of emotion regulation in the

treatment of child anxiety disorders. Clinical Child and Family Psychology

Review, 10(3), 275-293. doi: 10.1007/s10567-007-0024-6

Hannan, A. P., Rapee, R. M., & Hudson, J. L. (2000). The prevention of depression in

children: A pilot study. Behaviour Change, 17, 78-83. doi:

10.1375/bech.17.2.78

Harrison, M. E., McKay, M., & Bannon, W. M. (2004). Inner-city child mental health

service use: The real question is why youth and families do not use services.

Community Mental Health Journal, 40(2), 119-131. doi:

10.1023/B:COMH.0000022732.80714.8b

Heimburg, R. G., Dodge, C. S., Hope, D. A., Kennedy, C. R., Zollo, L. J., & Becker,

R. E. (1990). Cognitive-behavioural group treatment for social phobia:

Comparison with a credible placebo control. Cognitive Therapy and Research,

14, 1-23. doi: 10.1007/BF01173521

Herman-Stahl, M., & Petersen, A. C. (1996). The protective role of coping and social

resources for depressive symptoms among young adolescents. Journal of

Youth and Adolescence, 25, 733-753. doi: 10.1007/BF01537451

Hill, H. M., Levermore, M., Twaite, J., & Jones, L. P. (1996). Exposure to community

violence and social support as predictors of anxiety and social and emotional

177
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

behaviour among African American children. Journal of Child and Family

Studies, 5, 399-414. doi: 10.1007/BF02233862

Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Biederman, J., Henin, A., Faraone, S. V., Davis, S.,

Harrington, K, & Rosenbaum, J.F. (2007). Behavioral inhibition in preschool

children at risk is a specific predictor of middle childhood social anxiety: A

five-year follow-up. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,

28(3), 225-233. doi: 10.1097/01.DBP.0000268559.34463.d0

Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Micco, J., Henin, A., Bloomfield, A., Biederman, J., &

Rosenbaum, J. F. (2008). Behavioral inhibition. Depression and Anxiety,

25(4), 357-367. doi: 10.1002/da.20490

Hirshfeld, R., Keller, M., Panico, S., Arons, B., Barlow, D., Davidoff, F., Endicott, J.,

Froom, J., Goldstein, M. Gorman, J.M., Marek, R.G., Maurer, T.A., Meyer,

R., Phillips, K., Ross, J., Schwenk, T.L., Sharfstein, S.S., Thase, M.E. &

Wyatt, R.J. (1997). The National Depressive and Manic-Depressive

Association consensus statement on the undertreatment of depression. Journal

of the American Medical Association, 277, 333-340. doi:

10.1001/jama.277.4.333

Hoagwood, K., Horwitz, S. M., Stiffman, A., Weisz, J., Bean, D., Rae, D. et al.

(2000). Concordance between parent reports of children's mental health

services and service records: The services assessment for children and

adolescents (SACA). Journal of Child and Family Studies, 9(3), 315-331. doi:

10.1023/A:1026492423273

Hudson, J. L. (2005). Efficacy of cognitive-behavioural therapy for children and

adolescents with anxiety disorders. Behaviour Change, 22(2), 55-70. doi:

10.1375/bech.2005.22.2.55

178
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Hudson, J. L., Flannery-Schroeder, E., & Kendall, P. C. (2004). Primary prevention of

anxiety disorders. In D. J. A. Dozois & K. S. Dobson (Eds.), The prevention of

anxiety and depression: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 101-130).

Washington DC: American Psychological Association.

Hudson, J. L., Rapee, R. M., Deveney, C., Schniering, C. A., Lyneham, H. J., &

Bovopoulos, N. (2009). Cognitive-behavioural treatment versus an active

control for children and adolescents with anxiety disorders: A randomized

trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,

48(5), 533- 544. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819c2401

Huebner, E. S., Suldo, S. M., Smith, L. C., & McKnight, C. G. (2004). Life

satisfaction in children and youth: Empirical foundations and implications for

school psychologists. Psychology in the Schools, 41(1), 81-93. doi:

10.1002/pits.10140

Hutchinson, M. R., & Poole, D. L. (1998). Adolescent health and school health: It's

time to meet the challenge. Health and Social Work, 23, 3-7. Retrieved from

http://findarticles.com/p/articles/mi_hb138/

Ialongo, N., Edelsohn, G., Werthamer-Larsson, L., Crockett, L., & Kellam, S. (1994).

The significance of self-reported anxious symptoms in first grade children.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 22, 441-455. doi:

10.1007/BF02168084

Ialongo, N., Edelsohn, G., Werthamer-Larsson, L., Crockett, L., & Kellam, S. (1995).

The significance of self-reported anxious symptoms in first grade children:

Prediction to anxious symptoms and adaptive functioning in fifth grade.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36(3), 427-437. doi:

10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01300.x

179
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Ialongo, N., Edelsohn, G., Werthamer-Larsson, L., Crockett, L., & Kellam, S. (1996).

Social and cognitive impairment in first-grade children with anxious and

depressive symptoms. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 25(1), 15-24.

Retrieved from http://www.jccap.net/

Jeavons, S., Horne, D. J. d. L., & Greenwood, K. M. (2000). Coping style and

psychological trauma after road accidents. Psychology, Health and Medicine,

5(2), 213-221. doi: 10.1080/713690183

Jencks, C., & Mayer, S. E. (1990). The social consequences of growing up in a poor

neighbourhood. In L. E. Lynn & M. G. H. McGeary (Eds.), Inner city poverty

in the United States. Washington DC: National Academy Press.

Kagan, J. (1997). Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity. Child

Development, 68, 139-143. doi: 10.2307/1131931

Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., Clarke, C., Snidman, N., & Garcia-Coll, C. (1984).

Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 55, 2212-2225.

doi: 10.2307/1129793

Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Gibbons, J. (1989). Inhibited and uninhibited types of

children. Child Development, 60(4), 838-845. doi: 10.2307/1131025

Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1987). The physiology and psychology of

behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, 58, 1459-1473. doi:

10.2307/1130685

Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., Snidman, N., Gibbons, J., & Johnson, M. O. (1988).

Childhood derivatives of inhibition and lack of inhibition to the unfamiliar.

Child Development, 59, 1580-1589. doi: 10.2307/1130672

180
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Kagan, J., Snidman, N., Zentner, M., & Peterson, E. (1999). Infant temperament and

anxious symptoms in school age children. Development and Psychopathology,

11, 209-224. doi: 10.1017/S0954579499002023

Kalff, A. C., Kroes, M., Vles, J. S., Hendriksen, J. G., Feron, F. J., Steyaert, J., van

Zeben, T.M.C.B., Jolles, J., & van Os, J. (2001). Neighbourhood level and

individual level SES effects on child problem behaviour: A multilevel

analysis. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 55(4), 246-250.

doi: 10.1136/jech.55.4.246

Kalton, G., & Kasprzyk (1982). Imputing for Missing Survey Responses In the

Proceedings of the Section on Survey Research Methods (pp. 22-31).

Alexandria, VA: American Statistical Association. doi: 10.1007/s10826-008-

9250-x

Kane, P., & Garber, J. (2009). Parental depression and child externalising and

internalising symptoms: Unique effects of fathers' symptoms and perceived

conflict as a mediator. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 18(4), 465-472.

Kashani, J. H., & Orvaschel, H. (1988). Anxiety disorders in mid adolescence: a

community sample. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 960-964. Retrieved

from http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/

Kashani, J. H., & Orvaschel, H. (1990). A community study of anxiety in children and

adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147(3), 313-318. Retrieved from

http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/

Kawachi, I., Kennedy, B. P., & Wilkinson, R. G. (1999). Crime: social

disorganisation and relative deprivation. Social Science & Medicine, 48, 719-

731. Retrieved from

181
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.cws_home/315/descripti

on#description

Kazdin, A. E. (1994). Behaviour modification in applied settings. Monterey:

Brooks/Cole.

Kazdin, A. E. (1996). Dropping out of child psychotherapy: Issues for research and

implications for practice. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1, 133-

156. doi: 10.1177/1359104596011012

Kazdin, A. E., Holland, L., & Crowley, M. (1997). Family experience of barriers to

treatment and premature termination from child therapy. Journal of Consulting

and Clinical Psychology, 65, 453-463. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.65.3.453

Kazdin, A. E., & Mazurick, J. L. (1994). Dropping out of child psychotherapy:

Distinguishing early and late dropouts over the course of treatment. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1069-1074. doi: 10.1037/0022-

006X.62.5.1069

Kazdin, A. E., & Wassell, G. (1999). Barriers to treatment participation and

therapeutic change among children referred for conduct disorder. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 28, 160-172. Retrieved from

http://apa.org/journals/ccp/

Keller, M. B., Lavori, P. W., Wunder, J., Beardslee, W. R., Schwartz, C. E., & Roth,

J. (1992). Chronic course of anxiety disorders in children and adolescents.

Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(4),

595-599.

Kendall, P. C. (1985). Toward a cognitive-behavioural model of child

psychopathology and a critique of related interventions. Journal of Abnormal

Child Psychology, 13, 357-372. doi: 10.1007/BF00912722

182
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Kendall, P. C. (1990). Coping Cat workbook. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University.

Kendall, P. C. (1994). Treating anxiety disorders in children: results of a randomised

clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 100-110. doi:

10.1037/0022-006X.62.1.100

Kendall, P. C., Flannery-Schroeder, E., Panichelli-Mindel, S. M., Southam-Gerow,

M., Henin, A., & Warman, M. (1997). Therapy for youth with anxiety

disorders: A second randomised clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 65, 366-380. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.65.3.366

Kendall, P. C., Hudson, J. L., Gosch, E., & Flannery-Schroeder, E. (2008). Cognitive-

behavioural therapy for anxiety disordered youth: A randomized clinical trial

evaluating child and family modalities. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 76(2), 282-297. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.2.282

Kendall, P. C., Safford, S., Flannery-Schroeder, E., & Webb, A. (2004). Child anxiety

treatment: Outcomes in adolescence and impact on substance use and

depression at 7.4 year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 72, 276-287. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.72.2.276

Kendall, P. C., & Southam-Gerow, M. A. (1996). Long-term follow-up of a cognitive-

behavioural therapy for anxiety-disordered youth. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 64, 724-730. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.64.4.724

Kerkorian, D., McKay, M., & Bannon, W. M. (2006). Seeking help a second time:

Parents'/caregivers' characterizations of previous experiences with mental

health services for thier children and perceptions of barriers to future use.

American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 76(2), 161-166. doi: 10.1037/0002-

9432.76.2.161

183
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Kessler, R. C. (1982). A disaggregation of the relationship between socioeconomic

status and psychological distress. American Sociological Review, 47(6), 752-

764. doi: 10.2307/2095211

Kessler, R.C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., & Walters, E. (2005). Lifetime

prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national

comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 593-602.

doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593

Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., Zhao, S., Nelson, C. B., Hughes, M., Eshleman, S.

et al. (1994). Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric

disorders in the United States: Results from the National Comorbidity Study.

Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 8-19. Retrieved from http://archpsyc.ama-

assn.org/

Kessler, R. C., Nelson, C., MgGonagle, K., Edlund, M., Frank, R., & Leaf, P. (1996).

The epidemiology of co-occuring addictive and mental disorders: Implications

for prevention and service utilization. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,

66, 17-31. doi: 10.1037/h0080151

Kim-Cohen, J., Moffit, T. E., Caspi, A., & Taylor, A. (2004). Genetic and

environmental processes in young children's resilience and vulnerability to

socioeconomic deprivation. Child Development, 75, 651-668. doi:

10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00699.x

Kindt, M., Bierman, D., & Brosschot, J. F. (1997). Cognitive bias in spider fear and

control children: Assessment of emotional interference by a card format and a

single-trial format of the Stroop task. Journal of Experimental Child

Psychology, 66(2), 163-179. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1997.2376

184
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Kindt, M., & Brosschot, J. F. (1999). Cognitive bias in spider-phobic children:

Comparison of a pictorial and a linguistic spider Stroop. Journal of

Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 21, 207-220. doi:

10.1023/A:1022873331067

Kindt, M., Brosschot, J. F., & Everaerd, W. (1997). Cognitive processing bias of

children in a real life stress situation and a neutral situation. Journal of

Experimental Child Psychology, 64(1), 79-97. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1996.2336

King, N., Heyne, D., & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). Cognitive-behavioural treatments for

anxiety and phobic disorders in children and adolescents: A review.

Behavioral Disorders, 30(3), 241-257. Retrieved from

http://www.ccbd.net/behavioraldisorders/

King, N. J., & Ollendick, T. H. (1989). Children's anxiety and phobic disorders in

school settings: Classification, assessment, and intervention issues. Review of

Educational Research, 59(4), 431-470. doi: 10.2307/1170206

Kiselica, M. S., Baker, S. B., Thomas, R. N., & Reedy, S. (1994). Effects of stress

innoculation training on anxiety, stress, and academic performance among

adolescents. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41(3), 335-342. doi:

10.1037/0022-0167.41.3.335

Kokenes, B. (1978). A factor analytic study of the Coopersmith Self-Esteem

Inventory. Adolescence, 13(49), 149-155. Retrieved from

http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.careers/622849/descripti

on

Koplewicz, H. (1996). Childhood phobias. In L. C.G. (Ed.), Handbook of the

treatment of anxiety disorders (2nd ed., pp. 175-187). Lanham: Jason

Aronson.

185
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Koroloff, N. M., Elliott, D. J., Koren, P. E., & Friesen, B. J. (1996). Linking low-

income families to children's mental health services: An outcome study.

Journal of Emotional and Behavoural Disorders, 4(1), 2-11. doi:

10.1177/106342669600400101

Kovacs, M. (1981). Rating scales to assess depression in school-aged children. Acta

Paediopsychiatry, 46, 305-315. Retrieved from

http://www.worldcat.org/title/acta-paedopsychiatrica/oclc/1460958

Kovacs, M. (1992). Children's Depression Inventory Manual New York: Multi-

Health Systems.

Kovacs, M., Gatsonis, C., Paulauskas, S. L., & Richards, C. (1989). Depressive

disorders in childhood: IV. A longitudinal study of comorbidity with and risk

for anxiety disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46(9), 776-782.

Retrieved from http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/

Kubiszyn, T. (1999). Integrating health and mental health services in schools:

Psychologists collaborating with primary care providers. Clinical Psychology

Review, 19(2), 179-198. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(98)00073-7

Last, C. G., Hansen, C., & Franco, N. (1997). Anxious children in adulthood: a

prospective study of adjustment. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(5), 645-652. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199705000-

00015

Last, C. G., Hersen, M., Kazdin, A. E., Greta, F., & Grubb, H. J. (1987). Psychiatric

illness in the mothers of anxious children. The American Journal of

Psychiatry, 144(12), 1580-1583. Retrieved from

http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/

186
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Last, C. G., Perrin, S., Hersen, M., & Kazdin, A. E. (1996). A prospective study of

childhood anxiety disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1502-1510. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199611000-

00019

Laurent, J., & Ettelson, R. (2001). An examination of the tripartite model of anxiety

and depression and its application to youth. Clinical Child and Family

Psychology Review, 4(3), 209-230. doi: 10.1023/A:1017547014504

Legerstee, J. S., Tulen, J. H. M., Kallen, V. L., Dieleman, G. C., Treffers, P. D. A.,

Verhulst, F. C., & Utens, E.M.W.J. (2009). Threat-related selective attention

predicts treatment success in childhood anxiety disorders. Journal of the

American Association of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 196-205. doi:

10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819176e4

Lengua, L., Sandler, I. N., West, S. G., Wolchick, S. A., & Curran, P. J. (1999).

Emotionality and self-regulation, threat appraisal, and coping in children of

divorce. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 15-37. doi:

10.1017/S0954579499001935

Leve, L. D., Kim, H. K., & Pears, K. C. (2005). Childhood temperament and family

environment as predictors of internalising and externalising trajectories from

ages 5 to 17. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33(5), 505-520. doi:

10.1007/s10802-005-6734-7

Lewinsohn, P. M., Gotlib, I. H., Lewinsohn, M., Seeley, J. R., & Allen, N. B. (1998).

Gender differences in anxiety disorders and anxiety symptoms in adolescents.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107(1), 109-117. doi: 10.1037/0021-

843X.107.1.109

187
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Lewinsohn, P. M., Zinbard, R., Seeley, J. R., Lewinsohn, M., & Sack, W. (1997).

Lifetime comorbidity among anxiety disorders and between anxiety disorders

and other mental disorders in adolescents. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 11,

377-394. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(97)00017-0

Liber, J. M., Van Widenfelt, B. M., Utens, E. M. W. J., Ferdinand, R. F., Van der

Leeden, A. J. M., Van Gastel, W. et al. (2008). No differences between group

versus individual treatment of childhood anxiety disorders in a randomised

clinical trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(8), 886-893. doi:

10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01877.x

Lipsitz, J. D., Martin, L. Y., Mannuzza, S., Chapman, T. F., Leibowitz, M. R., Klein,

D. F., & Fyer, A.J. (1994). Childhood separation anxiety disorder in patients

with adult anxiety disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 927-929.

Retrieved from http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/

Lobovits, D. A., & Handal, P. J. (1985). Childhood depression: Prevalence among

DSM-III criteria and validity of parent and child depression scales. Journal of

Pediatric Psychology, 10, 45-54. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/10.1.45

Lock, S., & Barrett, P. M. (2003). A longitudinal study of developmental differences

in universal preventive intervention for child anxiety. Behaviour Change,

20(4), 183-199. doi: 10.1375/bech.20.4.183.29383

Lowry-Webster, H. M., Barrett, P. M., & Dadds, M. R. (2001). A universal

prevention trial of anxiety and depressive symptomatology in childhood:

Preliminary data from an Australian study. Behaviour Change, 18(1), 36-50.

doi: 10.1375/bech.18.1.36

188
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Lowry-Webster, H. M., Barrett, P. M., & Lock, S. (2003). A universal prevention trial

of anxiety symptomology during childhood: Results at 1-year follow-up.

Behaviour Change, 20(1), 25-43. doi: 10.1375/bech.20.1.25.24843

Luthar, S. S. (1991). Vulnerability and resilience: A study of high-risk adolescents.

Child Development, 62(3), 600-616. doi: 10.2307/1131134

Luthar, S. S., & Latendresse (2005). Comparable “risks” at the socioeconomic status

extremes: Preadolescents’ perceptions of parenting. Development and

Psychopathology, 17, 207-230. doi: 10.1017/S095457940505011X

Luthar, S. S., & Zigler, E. (1992). Intelligence and social competence among high-

risk adolescents. Development and Psychopathology, 4, 287-299. doi:

10.1017/S0954579400000158

Lyneham, H. J., Abbott, M. J., Wignall, A., & Rapee, R. A. (2003). The Cool Kids

Anxiety Treatment Program Sydney: MUARU, Macquarie University.

Lyneham, H. J., & Rapee, R. M. (2007). Childhood anxiety in rural and urban areas:

Presentation, impact and help seeking. Australian Journal of Psychology,

59(2), 108-118. doi: 10.1080/00049530701317082

Lyons-Ruth, K., Easterbrooks, M. A., & Cibelli, C. D. (1997). Infant attachment

strategies, infant mental lag, and maternal depressive symptoms: Predictors of

internalizing and externalizing problems at age 7. Developmental

Psychopathology, 33, 681-692. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.681

Macintyre, S., Ellaway, A., & Cummins, S. (2002). Place effects on health: how can

we conceptualise, operationalise, and measure them? Social Science &

Medicine, 55, 125-139. Retrieved from

http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.cws_home/315/descripti

on#description

189
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Macintyre, S., MacIver, S., & Sooman, A. (1993). Area, class, and health: Should we

be focusing on places or people? Journal of Social Policy, 22, 213-234.

Retrieved from http://journals.cambridge.org/action/displayJournal?jid=JSP

MacLean, K. (2003). The impact of institutionalisation on child development.

Development and Psychopathology, 15, 853-884. doi:

10.1017/S0954579403000415

Main, M. (1996). Introduction to the special section on attachment and

psychopathology: overview of the field of attachment. Journal of Consulting

and Clinical Psychology, 64, 237-243. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.64.2.237

Manassis, K., Mendlowitz, S. L., Scapillato, D., Avery, D., Fiksenbaum, L., Freire,

M., Monga, S., & Owens, M. (2002). Group and individual cognitive-

behavioural therapy for childhood anxiety disorders. A randomised trial.

Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(12),

1423-1430. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200212000-00013

Mash, E. J., & Wolfe, D. A. (2002). Abnormal child psychology (2nd ed.). Belmont:

Wadsworth.

Masi, G., Favilla, L., Mucci, M., & Millepiedi, S. (2000). Depressive comorbidity in

children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Child Psychiatry

& Human Development, 30(3), 205-215. doi: 10.1023/A:1021351722364

Masi, G., Millepiedi, S., Mucci, M., Poli, P., Bertini, N., & Milantoni, L. (2004).

Generalized Anxiety Disorder in Referred Children and Adolescents. Journal

of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(6), 752-760.

doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000121065.29744.d3

Masia-Warner, C., Nangle, D. W., & Hansen, D. J. (2006). Bringing evidence-based

child mental health services to the schools: General issues and specific

190
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

populations. Education and Treatment of Children, 29(2), 165-172. Retrieved

from http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/education_and_treatment_of_children/

Masten, A. S. (2001). Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development.

American Psychologist, 56, 227-238. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.227

Masten, A. S., & Powell, J. L. (2003). A resilience framework for research, policy and

practice. In S. S. Luthar (Ed.), Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation in the

context of childhood adversities (pp. 1-25). New York: Cambridge University

Press.

McCulloch, A. (2001). Social environments and health: cross sectional national

survey. British Medical Journal, 323, 208-209. doi:

10.1136/bmj.323.7306.208

McGee, R., Feehan, M., Williams, S., & Anderson, J. C. (1992). DSM-II disorders

from age 11 to age 15 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 50-59. Retrieved from http://www.jaacap.com/

McKenzie, K., Whitley, R., & Weich, S. (2002). Social capital and mental health.

British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, 280-283. doi: 10.1192/bjp.181.4.280

McLaughlin, K. A., & Hatzenbuehler (2009). Stressful Life Events, Anxiety

Sensitivity, and Internalizing Symptoms in Adolescents. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 118(3), 659-669. doi: 10.1037/a0016499

McLeod, J. D., & Shanahan, M. J. (1993). Poverty, parenting and children's mental

health. American Sociological Review, 58(3), 351-366. doi: 10.2307/2095905

McLoone, J., Hudson, J. L., & Rapee, R. M. (2006). Treating anxiety disorders in a

school setting. Education and Treatment of Children, 29, 219-242. Retrieved

from http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/education_and_treatment_of_children/

191
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Mendlowitz, S. L., Manassis, K., Bradley, S., Scapillato, D., Miezitis, S., & Shaw, B.

F. (1999). Cognitive-behavioural group treatments in childhood anxiety

disorders: The role of parental involvement. Journal of the American Academy

of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1223-1229. doi: 10.1097/00004583-

199910000-00010

Mesman, J., & Koot, H. M. (2000). Common and specific correlates of preadolescent

internalising and externalising psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 109, 428-437. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.428

Messer, S. C., & Beidel, D. C. (1994). Psychosocial correlates of childhood anxiety

disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 33, 975-984. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199409000-00007

Miech, R. A., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Entner Wright, B. R., & Silva, P. A. (1999).

Low socioeconomic status and mental disorders: A longitudinal study of

selection and causation during young adulthood. The American Journal of

Sociology, 104(4), 1096-1131. doi: 10.1086/210137

Misfud, C., & Rapee, R. M. (2005). Early intervention for childhood anxiety in a

school setting: Outcomes for an economically disadvantaged population.

Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(10),

995-1004. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000173294.13441.87

Moradi, A. R., Taghavi, M. R., Neshat-Doost, H. T., Yule, W., & Dalgleish, T.

(1999). Performance of children and adolescents with PTSD on the Stroop

colour-naming task. Psychological Medicine, 29(2), 415-419. doi:

10.1017/S0033291798008009

192
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Mrazek, P. J., & Haggerty, R. J. (Eds.). (1994). Reducing risks for mental disorders:

Frontiers for preventative intervention research. Washington DC: National

Academy Press.

Muris, P., & Field, A. P. (2008). Distorted cognition and pathological anxiety in

children and adolescents. Cognition and Emotion, 22(3), 395-421. doi:

10.1080/02699930701843450

Muris, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., & Hulsenbeck, P. (2000). Worry in children

is related to perceived parental rearing and attachment. Behaviour Research

and Therapy, 38(5), 487-497. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00072-8

Muris, P., Meesters, C., Merckelbach, H., Sermon, A., & Zwakhalen, S. (1998).

Worry in normal children. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 33, 975-984. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199807000-

00009

Muris, P., & Merckelbach, H. (1998). Perceived parental rearing behaviour and

anxiety disorders symptoms in normal children. Personality and Individual

Differences, 25, 1199-1206. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00153-6

Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., & Damama, E. (2000). Threat perception bias in

nonreferred socially anxious children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,

29, 348-359. Retrieved from http://www.jccap.net/

Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Meyer, B., & Meesters, C. (1998). Common fears and

their relationship to anxiety disorders symptomatology in normal children.

Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 575-578. doi: 10.1016/S0191-

8869(97)00190-6

Muris, P., Rapee, R., Meesters, C., Schouten, E., & Geers, M. (2003). Threat

perception abnormailities in children: The role of anxiety disorder symptoms,

193
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

chronic anxiety, and state anxiety. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 17, 271-287.

doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(02)00199-8

Muris, P., Schmidt, H., Lambrichs, R., & Meesters, C. (2001). Protective and

vulnerability factors of depression in normal adolescents. Behaviour Research

and Therapy, 39(5), 555-565. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00026-7

Muris, P., Steernemen, P., Merckelbach, H., & Meesters, C. (1996). The role of

parental fearfulness and modeling in children's fear. Behaviour Research and

Therapy, 34(3), 265-268. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(95)00067-4

Murray, J. (1992). Prevention and identification of high risk groups. International

Review of Psychiatry, 4, 281-286. doi: 10.3109/09540269209066329

Neil, A. L., & Christensen, H. (2009). Efficacy and effectiveness of school-based

prevention and early intervention programs for anxiety. Clinical Psychology

Review, 29(3), 208-215. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.01.002

Olfson, M., Gameroff, M. J., Marcus, S. C., & Waslick, B. D. (2003). Outpatient

treatment of child and adolescent depression in the United States. Archives of

General Psychiatry, 60(12), 1236-1242. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.12.1236

Ollendick, T. H., King, N., & Yule, W. (Eds.). (1994). International handbook of

phobic and anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. New York: Plenum

Press.

Ollendick, T. H., & King, N. J. (1998). Empirically supported treatments for children

with phobic and anxiety disorders: Current status. Journal of Clinical Child

Psychology, 27(2), 156-167. Retrieved from http://www.jccap.net/

Ollendick, T. H., Weist, M. D., Borden, C. M., & Greene, R. W. (1992). Sociometric

status and academic, behavioral, and psychological adjustment: A five-year

194
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

longitudinal study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 80-87.

doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.60.1.80

Ormel, J., Oldehinkel, A. J., Ferdinand, R. F., Hartman, C. A., De Winter, A. F., &

Veenstra, R. (2005). Internalising and externalising problems in adolescence:

General and dimension specific effects of familial loadings and preadolescent

temperament traits. Psychological Medicine, 35, 1825-1835. doi:

10.1017/S0033291705005829

Orvaschel, H., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Seely, J. R. (1995). Continuity of

psychopathology in a community sample of adolescents. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 1525-1535. doi:

10.1097/00004583-199511000-00020

Otto, M., Pollock, M., Rosenbaum, J. F., Sachs, G. S., & Asher, R. H. (1994).

Childhood history of anxiety in adults with panic disorder: Association with

anxiety sensitivity and comorbidity. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 1, 288-

293. Retrieved from

http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~db=all~content=t713723043

Owens, P. L., Hoagwood, K., Horwitz, S. M., Leaf, P. J., Poduska, J. M., Kellam, S.

G., & Ialongo, N.S. (2002). Barriers to children's mental health services.

Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(6),

731-738. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200206000-00013

Phongsaven, P., Chey, T., Bauman, A., Brooks, R., & Silove, D. (2006). Social

capital, socio-economic status and psychological distress among Australian

adults. Social Science & Medicine, 63(10), 2546-2561. Retrieved from

http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/journaldescription.cws_home/315/descripti

on#description

195
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Pine, D. S. (1997). Childhood anxiety disorders. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 9(4),

329-338. Retrieved from http://journals.lww.com/co-

pediatrics/pages/default.aspx

Pine, D. S., Cohen, P., Gurley, D., Brook, J., & Ma, Y. (1998). The risk for early-

adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with anxiety and

depressive disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 56-64. doi:

10.1001/archpsyc.55.1.56

Plancherel, B., & Bolognini, M. (1995). Coping and mental health in early

adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 18(4), 459-474. doi:

10.1006/jado.1995.1033

Prins, P. J. M. (2001). Affective and cognitive processes and the development and

maintenance of anxiety and its disorders. In W. K. Silverman & P. D. A.

Treffers (Eds.), Anxiety disorders in children and adolescents: Research,

assessment and intervention. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Prior, M., Smart, D. M., Sanson, A., & Oberklaid, F. M. (2000). Does shy-inhibited

temperament in childhood lead to anxiety problems in adolescence? Journal of

the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 461-468. doi:

10.1097/00004583-200004000-00015

Putnam, R. (1993). Making democracy work: civic traditions in modern Italy.

Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Radke-Yarrow, M., & Brown, E. (1993). Resilience and vulnerability in children of

multiple-risk families. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 581-592.

Retrieved from http://journals.cambridge.org/action/displayJournal?jid=DPP

Rapee, R. M., Kennedy, S., Ingram, M., Edwards, S., & Sweeney, L. (2005).

Prevention and early intervention of anxiety disorders in inhibited preschool

196
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 488-497. doi:

10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.488

Rapee, R. M., & Szollos, A. A. (2002). Developmental antecedents of clinical anxiety

in childhood. Behaviour Change, 19(3), 146-157. doi: 10.1375/bech.19.3.146

Rapee, R. M., Wignall, A., Hudson, J. L., & Schniering, C. A. (2000). The treatment

of anxiety disorders in children and adolescents: An evidence-based

approach. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger.

Reiss, S. (1991). Expectancy model of fear, anxiety, and panic. Clinical Psychology

Review, 11, 141-153. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(91)90092-9

Reynolds, C. R. (1980). Concurrent validity of the What I think and Feel: the Revised

Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 48, 774-775. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.48.6.774

Reynolds, C. R., & Richmond, B. O. (1978). What I think and feel: A revised measure

of children's manifest anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 6, 271-

280. doi: 10.1023/A:1025751206600

Reynolds, C. R., & Richmond, B. O. (1985). The revised children's manifest anxiety

scale: Manual. New York: Western Psychological Services.

Reznick, J. S., Hegeman, I. M., Kaufman, E. R., Woods, S. W., & Jacobs, M. (1992).

Retrospective and concurrent self-report of behavioral inhibition and their

relation to adult mental health. Development and Psychopathology, 4(2), 301-

321. doi: 10.1017/S095457940000016X

Ringel, J. S., & Sturm, R. (2001). National estimates of mental health utilization and

expenditures for children in 1998. Journal of Behavioural Health Sciences and

Research, 28(3), 319-333. doi: 10.1007/BF02287247

197
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Roberts, C., Ballantyne, F., & van der Klift, P. (2003). Aussie Optimism: Social life

skills. Teacher resource. Perth: Curtin University of Technology.

Roberts, C., Kane, P., Bishop, B., Cross, D., Fenton, J., & Hart, B. (2010). The

prevention of anxiety and depression in children from disadvantaged schools.

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 68-73. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.09.002

Roberts, C., Kane, R., Thomson, H., Bishop, B., & Hart, B. (2003). The prevention of

depressive symptoms in rural school children: A randomised controlled trial.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(3), 622-628. doi:

10.1037/0022-006X.71.3.622

Roberts, R., Roberts, C., Cosgrove, S., Hounston, K., Ludlow, T., & Mar, D. (2003).

Aussie optimism. Optimistic thinking skills. Teacher resource. Perth: Curtin

University of Technology.

Robertson, T. G., & Miller, E. (1986). The Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory: A

factor analytic study. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 46(1),

269-273. doi: 10.1177/0013164486461033

Roelofs, J., Meesters, C., Huurne, M., Bamelis, L., & Muris, P. (2006). On the links

between attachment style, parental rearing behaviors, and internalising and

externalising problems in non-clinical children. Journal of Child and Family

Studies, 15, 331-344. doi: 10.1007/s10826-006-9025-1

Rosenbaum, J. F., Biederman, J., Bolduc-Murphey, E. A., Faraone, S. V., Chaloff, J.,

Hirschfeld, D. R., & Kagan, J. (1993). Behavioral inhibition in childhood: A

risk factor for anxiety disorders. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 1(1), 2-16.

doi: 10.3109/10673229309017052

Rosenbaum, J. F., Biederman, J., Gersten, M., Hirschfeld, D. R., Meminger, S. R.,

Herman, J. B., Kagan, J., Reznick, S., & Snidman, N. (1988). Behavioral

198
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

inhibition in children of parents with panic disorder and agoraphobia: A

controlled study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 45(5), 463-470. Retrieved

from http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/

Rosenbaum, J. F., Biederman, J., Hirschfeld, D. R., Bolduc, E. A., Faraone, S. V.,

Kagan, J., Snidman, N., & Reznick, J.S. (1991). Further evidence of an

association between behavioural inhibition and anxiety disorders: Results from

a family study of children from a non-clinical sample. Journal of Psychiatric

Research, 25, 49-65. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(91)90015-3

Roth, J. H., & Dadds, M. R. (1999). Prevention and early intervention strategies for

anxiety disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 12(2), 169-174. Retrieved

from http://journals.lww.com/co-psychiatry/pages/default.aspx

Rudd, M. D., Joiner, T. F., & Rumzek, H. (2004). Childhood diagnoses and later risk

for multiple suicide attempts. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behaviour, 34,

113-125. doi: 10.1521/suli.34.2.113.32784

Rueter, M. A., Scaramella, L., Wallace, L. E., & Conger, R. D. (1999). First onset of

depressive or anxiety disorders predicted by the longitudinal course of

internalising symptoms and parent adolescent disagreements. Archives of

General Psychiatry, 56, 726-732. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.8.726

Rutter, M. (1985). Resilience in the face of adversity: Protective factors and resistance

to psychiatric disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 598-611. doi:

10.1192/bjp.147.6.598

Sampson, R. J., Morenoff, J. D., & Earls, F. (1999). Beyond social capital: spatial

dynamics of collective efficacy for children. American Sociological Review,

64, 633-660. doi: 10.2307/2657367

199
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Sandberg, S., McGuinness, D., Hillary, C., & Rutter, M. (1998). Independence of

childhood life events and chronic adversities: a comparison of two patient

groups and controls. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 37, 728-735. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199807000-00012

Sawyer, M. G., Arney, F. M., Baghurst, P. A., Clark, J. J., Graetz, B. W., Kosky, R.

J., Nurcombe, B., Patton, G.C., Prior, M.R., Raphael, B., Rey, J.M., Whaites,

L.C., & Zubrick, S.R. (2001). The mental health of young people in Australia:

Key findings from the child and adolescent component of the national survey

of mental health and well-being. Australian and New Zealand Journal of

Psychiatry, 35(6), 806-814. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2001.00964.x

Sawyer, M. G., Kosky, R. J., Graetz, B. W., Arney, F., Zubrick, S. R., & Baghurst, P.

(2000). The national survey of mental health and wellbeing: The child and

adolescent component. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,

34(2), 214-220. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1614.2000.00729.x

Saylor, C. F., Finch, A. J., Spirito, A., & Bennett, B. (1984). The Children's

Depression Inventory: A systematic evaluation of psychometric properties.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 955-967. doi:

10.1037/0022-006X.52.6.955

Schofield, C. A., Coles, M. E., & Gibb, B. E. (2009). Retrospective reports of

behavioral inhibition and young adults' current symptoms of social anxiety,

depression, and anxious arousal. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(7), 884-890.

doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.05.003

Seligman, L. D., & Ollendick, T. H. (1998). Comorbidity of anxiety and depression in

children and adolescents: An integrative review. Clinical Child and Family

Psychology Review, 1(2), 125-144. doi: 10.1023/A:1021887712873

200
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Shafii, M., Steltz-Lenarsky, J., Derrick, A. M., Beckner, C., & Whittinghill, J. R.

(1988). Comorbidity of mental disorders in the post-mortem diagnosis of

completed suicide in children and adolescents. Journal of Affective Disorders,

15(3), 227-233. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(88)90020-1

Shamir-Essakow, G., Ungerer, J. A., & Rapee, R. M. (2005). Attachment, behavioral

inhibition, and anxiety in preschool children. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 33(2), 131-143. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-1822-2

Shochet, I. M., Dadds, M. D., Holland, D., Whitefield, K., Harnett, P., & Osgarby, S.

(2001). The efficacy of a universal school-based program to prevent

adolescent depression. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30, 303-315.

Retrieved from http://www.jccap.net/

Shortt, A. L., Barrett, P. M., Dadds, M. R., & Fox, T. L. (2001). The influence of

family and experimental context on cognitions in anxious children. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 29(6), 585-598. doi: 10.1023/A:1012289427845

Shortt, A. L., Barrett, P. M., & Fox, T. L. (2001). Evaluating the FRIENDS program:

A cognitive-behavioural group treatment for anxious children and their

parents. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 30(4), 525-535. Retrieved from

http://www.jccap.net/

Silberg, J., Rutter, M., & Eaves, L. (2001). Genetic and environmental influences on

the temporal association between earlier anxiety and later depression in girls.

Biological Psychiatry, 49, 1040-1049. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01161-1

Silverman, W. K., & Albano, A. M. (1996). Anxiety Disorder Interview Schedule for

DSM-V, Child and Parent versions. San Antonio, TX: Psychological

Corporation.

201
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Silverman, W. K., & Treffers, P.D.A. (Eds.). (2001). Anxiety disorders in children

and adolescents: research, assessment and intervention. Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press.

Silverman, W. K., Greca, A. M. L., & Wasserstein, S. (1995). What do children worry

about? Worries and their reation to anxiety. Child Development, 66, 671-686.

doi: 10.2307/1131942

Silverman, W. K., Kurtines, W. M., Ginsburg, G. S., Weems, C. F., Lumpkin, P. W.,

& Charmichael, D. H. (1999). Treating anxiety disorders in children with

group cognitive-behavioural therapy: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67(6), 995-1003. doi: 10.1037/0022-

006X.67.6.995

Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L., & Barnett, N. P. (1995). Reduction of children's sport

performance anxiety through social support and stress-reduction training for

coaches. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16, 125-142. doi:

10.1016/0193-3973(95)90020-9

Snell-Johns, J., Mendez, J. L., & Smith, B. H. (2004). Evidence-based solutions for

overcoming access barriers, decreasing attrition, and promoting change with

undeserved families. Journal of Family Psychology, 18(1), 19-35. doi:

10.1037/0893-3200.18.1.19

Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2000). A preliminary study of the emotion

understanding of youths referred for treatment of anxiety disorders. Journal of

Clinical Child Psychology, 29(3), 319-327. Retrieved from

http://www.jccap.net/

202
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Spaccarelli, S., & Fuchs, C. (1997). Variability in symptom expression among

sexually abused girls: Developing multivariate models. Journal of Clinical

Child Psychology, 26, 24-35. Retrieved from http://www.jccap.net/

Spence, S. H. (1997). Structure of anxiety symptoms among children: a confirmatory

factor-analytic study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 280-297. doi:

10.1037/0021-843X.106.2.280

Spence, S. H. (1998). A measure of anxiety symptoms among children. Behaviour

Research and Therapy, 36, 545-566. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00034-5

Spence, S. H., Barrett, P. M., & Turner, C. M. (2003). Psychometric properties of the

Spence Children's Anxiety Scale with young adolescents. Anxiety Disorders,

17, 605-625. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6185(02)00236-0

Spence, S. H., & Dadds, M. R. (1996). Preventing childhood anxiety disorders.

Behaviour Change, 13(4), 241-249. Retrieved from

http://www.australianacademicpress.com.au/Publications/Journals/Behaviour_

Change/behavechange.htm

Spence, S. H., Donovan, C. L., & Brechman-Toussaint, M. (1999). Social skills,

social outcomes, and cognitive features of childhood social phobia. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology, 108(2), 211-221. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.108.2.211

Spence, S. H., Najman, J. M., Bor, W., O'Callaghan, M., & Williams, G. M. (2002).

Maternal anxiety and depression, poverty and marital relationship factors

during early childhood as predictors of anxiety and depressive symptoms in

adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(4), 457-470.

doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00037

Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in

the early years. New York: Cambridge University Press.

203
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., & Kreutzer, T. (1990). The fate of early experience

following developmental change: Longitudinal approaches to individual

adaptation in childhood. Child Development, 61, 1363-1373. doi:

10.2307/1130748

Stallard, P., Simpson, N., Anderson, S., & Goddard, M. (2008). The FRIENDS

emotional health prevention programme: 12-month follow-up of a universal

UK school based trial. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 17(5), 283-

289. doi: 10.1007/s00787-007-0665-5

Stallard, P., Simpson, N., Anderson, S., Hibbert, S., & Osborn, C. (2007). The

FRIENDS emotional health programme: Initial findings from a school-based

project. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 12(1), 32-37. doi:

10.1111/j.1475-3588.2006.00421.x

Stiffman, A. R., Hadley-Ives, E., Dore, P., Polgar, M., Horvath, V. E., Striley, C. et al.

(2000). Youths' access to mental health services: the role of providers'

training, resource connectivity, and assessment of need. Mental Health

Services Research, 2, 141-154. doi: 10.1023/A:1010189710028

Strauss, C. C., Frame, C. L., & Forehand, R. (1987). Psychosocial impairment

associated with anxiety in children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,

16(3), 235-239. Retrieved from http://www.jccap.net/

Strauss, C. C., Lahey, B. B., Frick, P., Frame, C. L., & Hynd, G. W. (1988). Peer

social status of children with anxiety disorders. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 56, 137-141. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.56.1.137

Strauss, C. C., Lease, C. A., Last, C. G., & Francis, G. (1988). Overanxious disorder:

An examination of developmental differences. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 16, 433-443. doi: 10.1007/BF00914173

204
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Suveg, C., & Zeman, J. (2004). Emotion regulation in children with anxiety disorders.

Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33, 750-759. doi:

10.1207/s15374424jccp3304_10

Suveg, C., Zeman, J., Flannery-Schroeder, E., & Cassano, M. (2005). Emotion

Socialization in Families of Children With an Anxiety Disorder. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 33(2), 145-155. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-1823-1

Svanberg, P. O. G. (1998). Attachment, resilience and prevention. Journal of Mental

Health 7, 543-578. doi: 10.1080/09638239817716

Taghavi, M. R., Dalgleish, T., Moradi, A. R., Neshat-Doost, H., & Yule, W. (2003).

Selective processing of negative emotional information in children and

adolescents with Generalised Anxiety Disorder. British Journal of Clinical

Psychology, 42(3), 221-230. doi: 10.1348/01446650360703348

Taghavi, M. R., Moradi, A. R., Neshat-Doost, H. T., Yule, W., & Dalgleish, T.

(2000). Interpretation of ambiguous emotional information in clinically

anxious children and adolescents. Cognition and Emotion, 14(6), 809-822.

doi: 10.1080/02699930050156645

Taghavi, M. R., Neshat-Doost, H., Moradi, A. R., Yule, W., & Dalgleish, T. (1999).

Biases in visual attention in children and adolescents with clinical anxiety and

mixed anxiety-depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27(3), 215-

233. doi: 10.1023/A:1021952407074

Terr, L. (1981). Psychic trauma in children: Observations following the Chowchilla

school-bus kidnapping. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 138(1), 14-19.

Retrieved from http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/

205
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition.

Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 25-52. doi:

10.2307/1166137

Tomb, M., & Hunter, L. (2004). Prevention of anxiety in children and adolescents in a

school setting: The role of the school-based practitioners. Children & Schools,

26(2), 87-101. Retrieved from

http://www.naswpress.org/publications/journals/cs.html

Turner, C. M., & Barrett, P. M. (2003). Does age play a role in structure of anxiety

and depression in children and youths? An investigation of the tripartite model

in three age cohorts. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71(4),

826-833. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.71.4.826

Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., & Costello, A. (1987). Psychopathology in the offspring

of anxiety disorders patients. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,

55(2), 229-235. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.55.2.229

van der Linden, J., Drukker, M., Gunther, N., Feron, F., & van Os, J. (2003).

Children's mental health service use, neighbourhood socioeconomic

deprivation, and social capital. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric

Epidemiology, 38(9), 507-514. doi: 10.1007/s00127-003-0665-9

Van Os, J., Driessen, G., Gunther, N., & Delespaul, P. (2000). Neighbourhood

variation in incidence of schizophrenia: Evidence for person-environment

interaction. British Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 243-248. doi:

10.1192/bjp.176.3.243

Vanderbilt-Adriance, E., & Shaw, D. S. (2008). Protective factors and the

development of resilience in the context of neighbourhood disadvantage.

206
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 887-901. doi: 10.1007/s10802-

008-9220-1

Varni, J. W., Setoguchi, Y., Rappaport, L. R., & Talbot, D. (1992). Psychological

adjustment and perceived social support in children with congenital/aquired

limb deficiencies. Journal of Behavioural Medicine, 15, 31-44. doi:

10.1007/BF00848376

Vasey, M. W., Daleiden, E. L., Williams, L., & Brown, L. M. (1995). Biased attention

in childhood anxiety disorders: A preliminary study. Journal of Abnormal

Child Psychology, 23(2), 267-279. doi: 10.1007/BF01447092

Vasey, M. W., El-Hag, N., & Daleiden, E. L. (1996). Anxiety and the processing of

emotionally threatening stimuli: Distinctive patterns of selective attention

among high and low test anxious children. Child Development, 67(3), 1173-

1185. doi: 10.2307/1131886

Vasey, M. W., & MacLeod, C. (2001). Information processing factors in childhood

anxiety: A review and developmental perspective. In M. W. Vasey & M. R.

Dadds (Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. 253-277).

New York: Oxford University.

Velez, C. N., Johnson, K., & Cohen, P. (1989). A longitudinal analysis of selected

risk factors for childhood psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy

of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(6), 861-865. doi: 10.1097/00004583-

198911000-00009

Verduin, T. L., & Kendall, P. C. (2003). Differential occurrence of comorbidity

within childhood anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent

Psychology, 32(2), 290-295. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP3202_15

207
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Vostanis, P. (2006). Strengths and difficulties questionnaire: research and clinical

applications. Current Opinions in Psychiatry, 19, 367-372. doi:

10.1097/01.yco.0000228755.72366.05

Warner, B. D., & Rountree, P. W. (1997). Local social ties in a community and crime

model: questioning the systemic nature of informal social control. Social

Problems, 44, 520-536. Retrieved from

http://ucpressjournals.com/journal.asp?j=sp

Warren, S. L., Huston, L., Egeland, B., & Srouf, L.A. (1997). Child and adolescent

anxiety disorders and early attachment. Journal of the American Academy of

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 637-644. doi: 10.1097/00004583-

199705000-00014

Warren, S. L., & Sroufe, L. A. (2004). Developmental issues. In T. H. Ollendick & J.

S. March (Eds.), Phobic and anxiety disorders in children and adolescence: A

clinician's guide to effective psychosocial and pharmacological interventions

(pp. 92-115). New York: Oxford University Press.

Waters, A. M., Lipp, O. V., & Spence, S. H. (2004). Attentional bias towards fear-

related stimuli: an investigation with nonselected children and adults and

children with anxiety disorders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,

89, 320-337. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.06.003

Waters, A. M., Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Pine, D. S. (2008). Attentional bias for

emotional faces in children with generalised anxiety disorder. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(4), 435-442. doi:

10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181642992

208
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Watts, S. E., & Weems, C. F. (2006). Associations among selective attention, memory

bias, cognitive errors, and symptoms of anxiety in youth. Journal of Abnormal

Child Psychology, 34, 841-852. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9066-3

Weiss, B., Weisz, J., Politano, M., Carey, M., Nelson, W., & Finch, A. (1991).

Developmental differences in the factor structure of the Children's Depression

Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 3, 38-45. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.3.1.38

Weissman, M. M., Leckman, J. F., Merikangas, K. R., Gammon, G. D., & Prusoff, B.

A. (1984). Depression and anxiety disorders in parents and children: Results

from the Yale Family Study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 41, 845-852.

Retrieved from http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org/

Weissman, M. M., Wolk, S., Wickramaratne, P., Goldstein, R. B., Adams, P.,

Greenwald, S., Ryan, N.D., Dahl, R.E., & Steinberg, D. (1999). Children with

prepubertal-onset major depressive disorder and anxiety grown up. Archives of

General Psychiatry, 56(9), 794-801. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.9.794

Weist, M. D., Sander, M. A., Axelrod-Lowie, J., & Christodulu, K. V. (2002). The

expanded school mental health framework. Childhood Education, 75, 269-

273. Retrieved from http://198.66.129.179/cehp.htm

Werner, E. E., & Smith, R. S. (1992). Overcoming the odds: High risk children from

birth to adulthood. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

Whatley, S. L., Foreman, A. C., & Richards, S. (1998). The relationship of coping

style to dysphoria, anxiety, and anger. Psychological Reports, 83(3), 783-791.

doi: 10.2466/PR0.83.7.783-791

Wheaton, B. (1986). Models for the stress-buffering functions of coping resources.

Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 26, 352-365. doi: 10.2307/2136658

209
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

White, K. S., Bruce, S. E., Farrell, A. D., & Kliewer, W. (1998). Impact of exposure

to community violence on anxiety: A longitudinal study of family social

support as a protective factor for urban children. Journal of Child and Family

Studies, 7, 187-203. doi: 10.1023/A:1022943216319

Whitley, R., & McKenzie, K. (2005). Social capital and psychiatry: Review of the

literature. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 13(2), 71-84. doi:

0.1080/10673220590956474

Williams, J. M. G., Matthews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional Stroop task

and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3-24. doi: 10.1037/0033-

2909.120.1.3

Windle, M., & Windle, R. C. (1996). Coping strategies, drinking motives, and

stressful life events among middle adolescents: Associations with emotional

and behavioral problems and with academic functioning. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 105(4), 551-560. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.105.4.551

Wood, J. J., McLeod, B. D., Piacentini, J. C., & Sigman, M. (2009). One-year follow-

up of family verses child CBT for anxiety disorders: exploring the roles of

child age and parental intrusiveness. Child Psychiatry & Human Development,

40, 301-316. doi: 10.1007/s10578-009-0127-z

Wood, J. J., Piacentini, J. C., Southam-Gerow, M., Chu, B. C., & Sigman, M. (2006).

Family cognitive behavioral therapy for child anxiety disorders. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(3), 314-321. doi:

10.1097/01.chi.0000196425.88341.b0

Woodward, L. J., & Fergusson, D. M. (2001). Life course outcomes of young people

with anxiety disorders in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of

210
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1086-1093. doi: 10.1097/00004583-

200109000-00018

Xue, Y., Leventhal, T., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Earls, F. (2005). Neighborhood residence

and mental health problems of 5-to-11-year-olds. Archives of General

Psychiatry, 62(5), 554-563. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.554

Yen, I. H., & Kaplan, G. A. (1999). Poverty area residence and change in depression

and perceived health status. International Journal of Epidemiology, 28, 90-94.

Retrieved from " http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/"

Yule, W., & Williams, R. M. (1990). Post-traumatic stress reactions in children.

Journal of Traumatic Stress, 3(2), 279-295. doi: 10.1002/jts.2490030209

Zeidner, M., & Ben-Zur, H. (1994). Individual differences in anxiety, coping, and

post-traumatic stress in the aftermath of the Persian Gulf War. Personality

and Individual Differences, 16(3), 459-476. doi: 10.1016/0191-

8869(94)90072-8

Zubrick, S. R., Silburn, S. R., Burton, P., & Blair, E. (2000). Mental health disorders

in children and young people: Scope, cause and prevention. Australian and

New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 34(4), 570-578. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-

1614.2000.00703.x

211
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Appendix

This Appendix contains the questionnaire battery used in the current research. This
battery was administered at pre-intervention, post-intervention, and 12 months follow-up.
Questionnaire names are provided for the convenience of those reading this document,
however questionnaire names were omitted from the battery completed by participants.

INFORMATION ABOUT THE


QUESTIONNAIRES

These questionnaires form part of an ongoing project at the University of


Queensland. We are interested in understanding how kids think and feel,
and how kids cope with difficult situations in their lives. Most of the
questionnaires in the booklet ask you about yourself, some ask a little bit
about your family. Remember that everything you write on these
questionnaires is confidential! These questions are only going to be seen
by some researchers at the University of Queensland – not anyone in your
family, or anyone at school. This means that no one will ever know what
you write, so please be as honest as possible when answering these
questions.

This is not a test, so there are no right or wrong answers. It’s just about
what you think and feel. Please remember to tell us the truth. There is
no point in being silly because it wastes our time, and it wastes your time!

As we go through the questionnaires, please listen to, and read all


instructions very carefully. Some of the questions or instructions might
seem a little difficult, so please put up your hand and ask for help if you
need to. Remember these questions are about you, so it is important that
you don’t speak to other students when you are filling in answers.

Please remember to answer every question, and if you have any trouble,
put up your hand and we will come and help you.

Thank you very much for filling in these questionnaires!!!

212
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Your name: ______________________________

Your school: ______________________________

Your grade: ______________________________

Your age: ______________________________

What is your nationality: __________________________

What is your parents nationality: ______________________

What is your mother’s job: __________________________

What is your father’s job: ___________________________

Please tick who lives with you in your house?

 Mother  Stepmother

 Father  Stepfather

 Sister - how many?  Stepsister - how many?

 Brother - how many?  Stepbrother - how many?

 Grandmother

 Grandfather

 others (please tell us who)

213
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Strengths & Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)


For each item, please mark the box for Not True, Somewhat True, or Certainly True. It would
help us if you answered all items as best you can, even if you are not absolutely certain.
Please give your answers on the basis of how things have been for you over the last six
months.

Not Somewhat Certainly


True True True
1 I try to be nice to other people. I care about their feelings □ □ □
2 I am restless, I cannot stay still for long □ □ □
3 I get a lot of headaches, stomach-aches, or sickness □ □ □
4 I usually share with others, for example CDs, games, food □ □ □
5 I get very angry and often lose my temper □ □ □
6 I would rather be alone than with people of my age □ □ □
7 I usually do as I am told □ □ □
8 I worry a lot □ □ □
9 I am helpful if someone is hurt, upset or feeling ill □ □ □
10 I am constantly fidgeting or squirming □ □ □
11 I have one good friend or more □ □ □
12 I fight a lot. I can make other people do what I want □ □ □
13 I am often unhappy, depressed, or tearful □ □ □
14 Other people my age generally like me □ □ □
15 I am easily distracted, I find it difficult to concentrate □ □ □
16 I am nervous in new situations, I easily lose confidence □ □ □
17 I am kind to younger children □ □ □
18 I am often accused of lying or cheating □ □ □
19 Other children or young people pick on me or bully me □ □ □
20 I often volunteer to help others (parents, teachers, children) □ □ □
21 I think before I do things □ □ □
I take things that aren’t mine from home, school or
22
elsewhere □ □ □
23 I get along better with adults than with people my own age □ □ □
24 I have many fears, I am easily scared □ □ □
25 I finish the work I’m doing, my attention is good □ □ □

214
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Self Esteem Questionnaire (SEI)

If the sentence describes how you usually feel, circle “Like Me”.
If the sentence does not describe how you usually feel, circle “Unlike Me”.

1. I find it very hard to talk in front of the class Like Me Unlike Me

2. I’m a lot of fun to be with Like Me Unlike Me

3. I’m popular with kids my own age Like Me Unlike Me

4. Kids usually follow my ideas Like Me Unlike Me

5. I often feel upset in school Like Me Unlike Me

6. Most people are better liked than I am Like Me Unlike Me

7. I often get discouraged at school Like Me Unlike Me

8. I’m easy to like Like Me Unlike Me

9. I proud of my schoolwork Like Me Unlike Me

10. I’m doing the best work I can Like Me Unlike Me

11. I would rather play with children younger than I am Like Me Unlike Me

12. I like to be called on in class Like Me Unlike Me

13. I’m not doing as well in school as I’d like to Like Me Unlike Me

14. I don’t like to be with other people Like Me Unlike Me

15. Kids pick on me very often Like Me Unlike Me

16. My teachers make me feel I’m not good enough Like Me Unlike Me

215
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI).

Kids sometimes have different feelings or ideas. This form lists different
feelings and ideas in groups. From each group, pick one sentence that best
describes you in the past two weeks. Put a cross like this X next to your answer.

Let’s try the first one as an example. Put a cross next to the
answer that describes you best.

I read books all the time


I read books once in a while
I never read books

O.K. Let’s go and do the rest of them now.

1. I am sad once in a while


I am sad many times
I am sad all the time

2. Nothing will ever work out for me


I am not sure if things will work out for me
Things will work out for me O.K.

3. I do most things O.K.


I do many things wrong
I do everything wrong

4. I have fun in many things


I have fun in some things
Nothing is fun at all

216
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Remember to put a cross next to the answer


that describes you best!!!

5. I am bad all the time


I am bad many times
I am bad once in a while

6. I worry about bad things happening to me once in a while


I worry that bad things will happen to me
I am sure that terrible things will happen to me

7. I hate myself
I do not like myself
I like myself

8. All bad things are my fault


Many bad things are my fault
Bad things are not usually my fault

9. I feel like crying everyday


I feel like crying many days
I feel like crying once in a while

10. Things bother me all the time


Things bother me many times
Things bother me once in a while

217
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Remember to put a cross next to the answer


that describes you best!!!

11. I like being with people


I do not like being with people many times
I do not want to be with people at all

12. I cannot make up my mind about things


It is hard to make up my mind about things
I make up my mind about things easily

13. I look O.K.


There are some bad things about my looks
I look ugly

14. I have to push myself all the time to do my schoolwork


I have to push myself many times to do my schoolwork
Doing schoolwork is not a big problem

15. I have trouble sleeping every night


I have trouble sleeping many nights
I sleep pretty well

16. I am tired once in a while


I am tired many days
I am tired all the time

218
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Remember to put a cross next to the answer


that describes you best!!!

17. Most days I do not feel like eating


Many days I do not feel like eating
I eat pretty well

18. I do not worry about aches and pains


I worry about aches and pains many times
I worry about aches and pains all the time

19. I do not feel alone


I feel alone many times
I feel alone all the time

20. I never have fun at school


I have fun at school only once in a while
I have fun at school many times

21. I have plenty of friends


I have some friends but I wish I had more
I do not have any friends

22. My schoolwork is all right


My schoolwork is not as good as before
I do very badly in subjects I used to be good in

219
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Remember to put a cross next to the answer


that describes you best!!!

23. I can never be as good as other kids


I can be as good as other kids if I want to
I am just as good as other kids

24. Nobody really loves me


I am not sure if anybody loves me
I am sure that somebody loves me

25. I usually do what I am told


I do not do what I am told most times
I never do what I am told

26. I get along with people


I get into fights many times
I get into fights all the time

220
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Revised Children’s Manifest of Anxiety Scale (RCMAS)

Read each question carefully. Circle the word “YES” if you think it is
true about you. Put a circle around the word “NO” if you think it is
not true about you.

1. I have trouble making up my mind …………………………………………… Yes No


2. I get nervous when things do not go the right way for me … Yes No
3. Others seem to do things easier than I can …………………………… Yes No
4. I like everyone I know ………………………………………………………………… Yes No
5. Often I have trouble getting my breath ………………………………… Yes No
6. I worry a lot of the time …………………………………………………………… Yes No
7. I am afraid of a lot of things …………………………………………………… Yes No
8. I am always kind …………………………….…………………………………………… Yes No
9. I get mad easily …………………………………………………………………………… Yes No
10. I worry about what my parents will say to me ……………………… Yes No
11. I feel that others do not like the way I do things ……………… Yes No
12. I always have good manners ……………………………………………………… Yes No
13. It is hard for me to get to sleep at night ..………………………… Yes No
14. I worry about what other people think of me ..…………………… Yes No
15. I feel alone even when there are people with me .………………… Yes No
16. I am always good …………………………………………………….…………………… Yes No
17. Often I feel sick in my stomach …..………………………………………… Yes No
18. My feelings get hurt easily ………...…………………………………………… Yes No
19. My hands feel sweaty ……………………..……………………………………… Yes No
20. I am always nice to everyone ……………………….………………………… Yes No
21. I am tired a lot …………………………….…………………………………………… Yes No
22. I worry about what is going to happen ………………………………… Yes No
23. Other people are happier than I am ……………………………………… Yes No
24. I tell the truth every single time …………………………………………… Yes No
25. I have bad dreams ………………………………………………………….………… Yes No
26. My feelings get hurt easily when I am yelled at ………………… Yes No
27. I feel someone will tell me I do things the wrong way ……… Yes No
28. I never get angry ……………………….……………………………………………… Yes No
29. I wake up scared some of the time ……………………………………… Yes No
30. I worry when I go to bed at night …………….………………………… Yes No
31. It is hard for me to keep my mind on my schoolwork ………… Yes No
32. I never say things I shouldn’t ………………………………………………… Yes No

221
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Read each question carefully. Circle the word “YES” if you think it is
true about you. Put a circle around the word “NO” if you think it is
not true about you.

33. I wiggle in my seat a lot ……………………….………………………………… Yes No


34. I am nervous ………………………………………………………………………………… Yes No
35. A lot of people are against me ………………………………………………… Yes No
36. I never lie ……………………………………………………………….…………………… Yes No
37. I often worry about something bad happening to me ……… Yes No

222
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Spence Child Anxiety Scale (SCAS)

Please tick the box under the word that shows how often each of these things
happen to you. There are no right or wrong answers.

Never Sometimes Often Always

1. I worry about things

2. I am scared of the dark

3. When I have a problem, I get a funny


feeling in my stomach

4. I feel afraid

5. I would feel afraid being on my own at home

6. I feel scared when I have to take a test

7. I feel afraid if I have to use public toilets


or bathrooms

8. I worry about being away from my parents

9. I feel afraid that I will make a fool of


myself in front of people

10. I worry that I will do badly in my schoolwork

11. I worry that something awful will happen to


someone in my family

12. I suddenly feel as if I can’t breathe when


there is no reason for this

13. I have to keep checking that I have done


things right (like the switch is off, or the door is locked)

223
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Please tick the box under the word that shows how often each of
these things happen to you.

Never Sometimes Often Always

14. I feel scared if I have to sleep on my own

15. I have trouble going to school in the mornings


because I feel nervous or afraid

16. I am scared of dogs

17. I can’t seem to get bad or silly thoughts


out of my head

18. When I have a problem, my heart beats


really fast

19. I suddenly start to tremble or shake when


there is no reason for this

20. I worry that something bad will happen to me

21. I am scared of going to the doctor or dentist

22. When I have a problem, I feel shaky

23. I am scared of being in high places or lifts


(elevators)

24. I have to think of special thoughts (like


numbers or words) to stop bad things from happening

25. I feel scared if I have to travel in the car,


or on a bus or train

224
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Please tick the box under the word that shows how often each of
these things happen to you.

Never Sometimes Often Always

26. I worry what other people will think of me

27. I am afraid of being in crowded places


(like shopping centres, the movies, buses, busy playgrounds)

28. All of a sudden I feel really scared for no


reason at all

29. I am scared of insects or spiders

30. I suddenly become dizzy or faint when there


is no reason for this

31. I feel afraid if I have to talk in front of my


class

32. My heart suddenly starts to beat too quickly


for no reason

33. I worry that I will suddenly get a scared


feeling when there is nothing to be afraid of

34. I am afraid of being in small closed spaces,


like tunnels or small rooms

35. I have to do some things over and over again


(like washing my hands, cleaning, or putting things in a certain order)

36. I get bothered by bad or silly thoughts or


pictures in my mind

225
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Please tick the box under the word that shows how often each of
these things happen to you.

Never Sometimes Often Always


37. I have to do some things in just the right
way to stop bad things happening

38. I would feel scared if I had to stay away


from home overnight

226
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Coping Scale for Children and Youth (CSCY)

All children and have some problems they find hard to deal with and
that upset them or worry them. We are interested in finding out what
you do when you try to deal with a hard problem. Think about some
problem that has upset you or worried you in the past few months. It
could be a problem with someone in your family, a problem with a
friend, a school problem or anything else. Briefly describe what the
problem is in the space below:

Listed below are some ways that children and teenagers try to deal
with their problems. Please tell us how often each of these statements
has been true for you when you tried to deal with the problem you
described above. Please circle your response.

Never Sometimes Often Very


often
1. I asked someone in my family
for help with the problem. 1 2 3 4
2. I thought about the problem
and tried to figure out what I 1 2 3 4
could do about it.
3. I tried not thinking about the
problem. 1 2 3 4
4.I stayed away from things that
reminded me about the problem. 1 2 3 4
5. I got advice from someone
about what I should do. 1 2 3 4
6. I took a chance and tried a
new way to solve the problem. 1 2 3 4
7. I went on with things as if
nothing was wrong. 1 2 3 4

227
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Never Sometimes Often Very


often
8. I tried not to feel anything
inside me. I wanted to feel 1 2 3 4
numb.
9. I shared my feelings about
the problem with another 1 2 3 4
person.
10. I made a plan to solve the
problem 1 2 3 4
and then I followed the plan.
11. I pretended the problem
wasn’t very important to me. 1 2 3 4
12. I went to sleep so I wouldn’t
have to think about it. 1 2 3 4
13. I kept my feelings to
myself 1 2 3 4
14. I went over in my head some
of the things I could do about 1 2 3 4
the problem.
15. I knew I had lots of feelings
about the problem, but I just 1 2 3 4
didn’t pay attention to them.
16. When I was upset about the
problem, I was mean to someone 1 2 3 4
even though they didn’t deserve
it.
17. I realised there was nothing
I could do. I just waited for it to 1 2 3 4
be over.
18. I tried to get away from the
problem for a while by doing 1 2 3 4
other things.
19. I hoped that things would
somehow work out so I didn’t do 1 2 3 4
anything.

228
PREVENTION OF ANXIETY IN DISADVANTAGED COMMUNITIES

Never Sometimes Often Very


often
20. I learned a new way of 1 2 3 4
dealing with the problem
21. I pretended the problem had
nothing to do with me. 1 2 3 4
22. I decided to stay away from
people and be by myself. 1 2 3 4
23. I tried to figure out how I
felt about the problem. 1 2 3 4
24. I tried to pretend that the
problem didn’t happen. 1 2 3 4
25. I figured out what had to be
done and then I did it. 1 2 3 4
26. I tried not to be with anyone
who reminded me of the 1 2 3 4
problem.
27. I tried to pretend that my
problem wasn’t real. 1 2 3 4
28. I put the problem out of my
mind. 1 2 3 4

29. I thought about the problem


in a new way so that it didn’t 1 2 3 4
upset me as much.

229

You might also like