You are on page 1of 5

Nocturnal Excretion of 6-Sulphatoxymelatonin in

Children and Adolescents with Autistic Disorder


Sylvie Tordjman, George M. Anderson, Nadège Pichard, Henriette Charbuy, and Yvan Touitou
Background: Many studies in autistic disorder report sleep problems and altered circadian rhythms, suggesting abnormalities in
melatonin physiology. Additionally, melatonin, a pineal gland hormone produced from serotonin, is of special interest in autistic
disorder given reported alterations in central and peripheral serotonin neurobiology.
Methods: Nocturnal urinary excretion of 6-sulphatoxymelatonin was measured by radioimmunoassay in groups of children and
adolescents with autistic disorder (n ⫽ 49) and normal control individuals (n ⫽ 88) matched on age, sex, and Tanner stage of
puberty.
Results: Nocturnal 6-sulphatoxymelatonin excretion rate was significantly and substantially lower in patients with autism than in
normal controls (mean ⫾ SEM, .75 ⫾ .11 vs. 1.80 ⫾ .17 ␮g/hr, p ⫽.0001), and was significantly negatively correlated with severity
of autistic impairments in verbal communication and play (p ⬍ .05).
Conclusions: These findings indicate clearly that nocturnal production of melatonin is reduced in autism. Further research is
warranted in order to understand the mechanisms underlying the lower melatonin production, to assess the impact of altered
melatonin on the pathophysiology and behavioral expression of autistic disorder, and to determine the utility of melatonin
administration in individuals with autism.

Key Words: 6-Sulphatoxymelatonin, melatonin, autistic disorder, an advanced or retarded sleep onset and morning awakening
autism severity, pineal, circadian sleep-wake rhythm (Dahlitz et al 1991). Blind individuals with free-running melato-
nin rhythms tend to take naps synchronized with their daytime
peaks in melatonin secretion (Lockley et al 1997). The stabiliza-

A
lterations in circadian measures of sleep-wake rhythm,
including reduced total sleep and longer sleep latency as tion of the sleep-wake rhythm that is observed around three
well as nocturnal and early morning awakenings, are months of age in the human infant corresponds to the establish-
often reported for individuals with autistic disorder (Honomichl ment of the circadian melatonin secretion rhythm (Zhdanova
et al 2002; Hoshino et al 1984; Johnson 1996; Patzold et al 1998; et al 1997).
Richdale and Prior 1995; Richdale 1999; Rutter 1968; Schreck and The possible therapeutic effects of melatonin in childhood
Mulick 2000). The accumulated clinical observations and sleep and adult sleep problems have been examined in a number of
studies strongly suggest that neuroendocrine functions involved studies (Jan et al 1999; Lewy et al 2001, 2002). Although the
in the circadian sleep-wake rhythm may be altered in autism. potential utility of melatonin in treating sleep problems in autism
The pineal gland hormone melatonin plays a key role in has been discussed (Lord 1998), the available data in this area are
regulating human circadian rhythms including the sleep-wake extremely limited (Hayashi 2000; Ishizaki et al 1999).
and body temperature cycles. In addition, melatonin is produced Most of the studies that have examined melatonin production
in the pineal gland from serotonin and may be of special interest in developmental disorders have been conducted on children
in autistic disorder given reported serotonergic alterations in with disorders associated with mental retardation. Palm and
autism (Anderson 2002; Cook and Leventhal 1996). In humans, colleagues (1997) reported that the time of peak serum or urinary
melatonin is secreted almost entirely at night, with peak secretion melatonin levels was delayed in seven out of eight mentally
typically occurring at around 2 am. The production of melatonin retarded blind children and young adults with circadian sleep-
is powerfully suppressed by light acting through the retino- wake disturbances. Zhdanova et al (1997) found that 13 children
hypothalamic tract. Thus, melatonin secretion depends upon with Angelman syndrome tended to have lower peak melatonin
exogenous factors such as light and season, and may be also levels than those reported for normal children of a similar age
influenced by endogenous factors such as sex, age and pubertal range; four of the children exhibited a phase delay in nocturnal
stage (Brezinski 1997; Lindblom et al 2002; Sadeh 1997; Touitou melatonin secretion. Three studies have examined melatonin
2001; Wetterberg 1999). Many studies have reported that mela- secretion in autistic disorder, either by measuring blood levels
tonin influences the sleep-wake rhythm. For instance, an ad- (Kulman et al 2000; Nir et al 1995) or urinary excretion of
vanced or retarded peak in melatonin secretion is associated with melatonin (Ritvo et al 1993). The studies have been limited by
small sample sizes (10-14 subjects) and the results have not been
entirely consistent. Kulman et al (2000) and Nir et al (1995)
From the Center for Scientific Research, Unité de Recherche Mixte 7593,
reported decreased nighttime serum melatonin levels associated
Vurnérabilité, Adaptation et Psychopathologie, Hôpital Pitié-Salpétrière
(ST, NP), Centre Hospitalier Guillaume Régnier, Faculté de Médecine
with an abnormal or blunted circadian rhythm of melatonin
Université de Rennes 1; Department of Medical Biochemistry and Molec- secretion, while Ritvo et al (1993) reported increased daytime
ular Biology (HC, YT), Faculté de Médecine Pitié-Salpétrière, Paris, France; urinary melatonin levels and similar night time values compared
and the Child Study Center (GMA), Yale University School of Medicine, to normal controls.
New Haven, Connecticut. In order to better characterize the possible melatonin alter-
Address reprint requests to Sylvie Tordjman, M.D., Ph.D., Centre Hospitalier ation in autism, we studied nocturnal melatonin secretion in
Guillaume Régnier, 154 rue de Châtillon, 35200 Rennes, France; E-mail: large groups of individuals with autism and normal controls
lubart@idf.ext.jussieu.fr. matched on age, sex and stage of puberty, and examined also the
Received May 12, 2004; revised September 14, 2004; accepted November 1, relationship between melatonin and severity of the autistic
2004. disorder. Secretion of melatonin was assessed by measuring the

0006-3223/05/$30.00 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2005;57:134 –138


doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.003 © 2005 Society of Biological Psychiatry
S. Tordjman et al BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2005;57:134 –138 135

Table 1. Demographic Characteristics of Study Groups autistic disorder were cognitively impaired (mean full scale IQ ⫾
SD: 42.2 ⫾ 3.2, with a range of 40-58; mean verbal IQ ⫾ SD: 45.5
Group
⫾ 2.2, with a range of 45-57; mean performance IQ ⫾ SD: 45.6
Individuals with Normal Control ⫾ 4.1, with a range of 45-80).
Autistic Disorder Subjects Based on direct clinical observation of the patient by two
Variable (n ⫽ 50) (n ⫽ 88) Statistic t/␹2 P independent child psychiatrists, a diagnosis of autistic disorder
was made according to the criteria of DSM-IV (American Psychi-
Sex M/F 33/17 49/39 1.41 .24
atric Association 1994), ICD-10 and CFTMEA (Classification
Pubertal Statusa 22/20/8 43/33/12 .33 .85
Française des Troubles Mentaux de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent;
Pre/Pub/Post
Age (years) 11.5 ⫾ 4.5 11.0 ⫾ 4.4 .59 .56
Mises and Quemada 1993), and was confirmed by the ADI-R
Mean ⫾ SD (Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised) and ADOS-G (Autism
Weight (kg) 39.4 ⫾ 19.8 40.6 ⫾ 17.8 .42 .67 Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic) scales (Lord 1997).
Mean ⫾ SD The ADOS-G (Module 1), an extensive observational scale, was
Height (cm) 141.4 ⫾ 24.0 145.1 ⫾ 23.1 .89 .37 administered by one trained child psychiatrist. The severity of
Mean ⫾ SD impairments in the behavioral domains of autism was scored
using the subset of items included in the ADOS (module 1)
The numbers of subjects for Tanner stages 1, 2, 3, 4 and 5 in the group
with austistic disorder were 22, 7, 3, 10 and 8, respectively; numbers of algorithm, following a procedure previously described (Tordj-
subjects in the comparison group were 43, 13, 7, 13 and 12, respectively. M, man et al 2001). Because the ADOS items are scored on an
male; F, female. ordinal scale (from 1 to 3 according to autism severity; the “0”
a
Prepubertal ⫽ Tanner stage 1; Pubertal ⫽ Tanner 2, 3 and 4; Postpuber- coding means that the autistic behavior was not present), we
tal ⫽ Tanner 5. took the median value of all items belonging to the same domain
of autistic impairment according to the ADOS (module 1)
overnight urinary output of the predominant melatonin metabo-
algorithm. This gave a score of central tendency for each of the
lite, 6-sulphatoxymelatonin (6-SM). It is well-established that
four main domains: Reciprocal Social Interaction Total (7 items),
urinary excretion of 6-SM gives an accurate reflection of pineal
Communication Total (5 items), Stereotyped Behaviors and
melatonin secretion (Deacon and Arendt 1994; Markey et al 1985;
Restricted Interests Total (3 items), and Play Total (2 items).
Matthews et al 1991).

Methods and Materials Urine Collection


Subjects Nocturnal urine was collected at home by parents during a
Children and adolescents with autistic disorder (n ⫽ 50) were 12-hour period (from 8:00 PM to 8:00 AM). Subjects were in-
matched with normal control individuals (n ⫽ 88) on age, sex structed to empty their bladder between 7:45 PM and 8:00 PM.
and Tanner stage of puberty assessed by a pediatrician (Tanner Because of possible effects of season on the secretion of
1962). Demographic data are presented in Table 1 and indicate melatonin, urine was collected for all the patients with autistic
that the two groups did not differ significantly with respect to disorder and their comparison subjects in the spring (March-
age, sex, pubertal status, weight or height. May). Given that melatonin secretion can be suppressed by
All patients with autistic disorder were recruited from French exposure to light, parents were asked to report for the night of
child psychiatry day hospitals. The control individuals were re- urine collection, as well as for the month before the melatonin
cruited over a three-month period from a preventive medical center procedure, if their child was sleeping with the light on and if he
where they went for a regular check-up. All individuals in both or she got up during this night and turned on the light. On the
subject groups were sleeping in their parents’ house and were night of urine collection, all the individuals with autistic disorder
attending school or day hospitals on a daily basis from about 9:00 AM and the comparison subjects went to bed with lights out between
to 4:00 PM. All subjects were Caucasian, nonobese, had no history of 9:00 PM and 10:00 PM, and none of them was exposed to light
encephalopathy or neuroendocrinological disease, and were deter- until their morning wake-up between 7:00 AM and 7:45 AM. No
mined to be physically healthy based on an examination by a bedwetting or accidental loss of urine was reported. One prepu-
pediatrician. Control individuals were unmedicated and did not bertal boy with autistic disorder was excluded from the study due
show any particular sleep disturbance based on a parental sleep to an outlying urine collection volume of only 4 ml. Collected
questionnaire. Twenty of the 50 patients with autistic disorder were urines were stored in a refrigerator until delivered to the labora-
medicated. Nine patients were receiving neuroleptics and seven tory within 24 hours of the urine collection. The volume of the
were taking benzodiazepines, without dose adjustments, for at least urine collection was measured and a portion frozen until ana-
six months before the urine collection. Nineteen patients had a lyzed for creatinine and 6-SM.
history of idiopathic epilepsy, which had been diagnosed by a
neuropediatrician; twelve were receiving anticonvulsive medication
at the time of the study and for at least one year before the urine Determination of Urinary 6-SM
collection. The protocol was approved by the ethics committee of Blinded analysis of urine 6-sulphatoxymelatonin (6-SM) levels
Bicêtre hospital and written informed consent was obtained from was performed by radioimmunoassay using an assay kit from
parents. Stockgrand Ltd (Guildford, United Kingdom). The urine samples
were diluted prior to assay (1/250). The intra–assay coefficient of
Cognitive and Behavioral Assessments variation was 6% (n ⫽ 10) for a .030␮g/ml control sample value.
Cognitive functioning of patients with autistic disorder was Excretion of 6-SM was expressed as ␮g excreted per hour
assessed by two psychologists using the age-appropriate (␮g/hr) over the collection period: the urine 6-SM concentration
Weschler intelligence scales and the Kaufman-Assessment Bat- (␮g/ml) was multiplied by the collection volume in ml and
tery for Children (K-ABC) (Anastasi 1988). All patients with divided by the 12 hours of collection.

www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsych
136 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2005;57:134 –138 S. Tordjman et al

Figure 1. Distribution of nocturnal 6-SM excretion rates (␮g/hr) observed in individuals with autistic disorder and normal control subjects.

Statistical Analysis epines, or all medications combined) when compared to un-


Correlations between melatonin excretion rate and age, medicated patients.
weight or height were calculated by Pearson correlation analy- A three-way ANOVA including group (autistic disorder, nor-
ses. Group and subgroup comparisons of urine 6-SM levels were mal control group), puberty (prepubertal, pubertal and postpu-
performed using analyses of variance (ANOVA) and two-tailed bertal) and sex factors showed a significant effect of group (F ⫽
Student’s t tests. Relationships between melatonin levels and 10.58, df ⫽ 1.136, p ⫽ .001) and group interacting with puberty
autism severity within the major behavioral domains of impair- (F ⫽ 3.33, df ⫽ 1.136, p ⫽ .04). There were no significant effects
ment were studied using Spearman rank-order correlation anal- of puberty or sex alone, or of group interacting with sex. Patients
yses. In order to balance type I and type II errors in the statistical with autistic disorder had significantly and substantially lower
analysis of behavioral domains, a hierarchical strategy was used (42% of control) 6-SM excretion rates than normal control
(Cohen and Cohen 1983). First, the total behavioral domains comparison subjects (.75 ⫾ .11 vs. 1.80 ⫾ .17 ␮g/hr, t ⫽ 5.22,
were examined. If there was a significant result for an overall df ⫽ 134.4, p ⫽ .0001). This result was confirmed when
domain, then a further level of analysis occurred on the sub- comparing drug-free individuals with autistic disorder (.77 ⫾ .12
scores included in the domain. Group means are reported as ␮g/hr) to normal control subjects (t ⫽ 5.17, df ⫽ 134.6, p ⫽
⫾SEM, unless otherwise indicated; alpha was set at .05 for all .0001). The distribution of 6-SM excretion rates in individuals
analyses. with autistic disorder and normal controls is shown in Figure 1.
A large proportion of the individuals with autistic disorder
(31/49, 63%) had 6-SM excretion values which were less than half
Results
of the mean excretion rate observed in the control group.
Mean (⫾SEM) urine collection volumes were similar in the As seen in Figure 2, no significant puberty-related differences
autistic disorder and control groups (325 ⫾ 77 and 303 ⫾ 15 ml, in 6-SM excretion were found in autistic disorder. However, in
respectively). Urinary excretion of creatinine also did not differ the control group, rates of 6-SM excretion were significantly
between the autistic disorder and control groups (295 ⫾ 77 and higher in prepubertal subjects (2.30 ⫾ .31 ␮g/hr) compared to
294 ⫾ 18 mg/collection, respectively). There were no significant pubertal (1.44 ⫾ .19 ␮g/hr, t ⫽ 2.37, df ⫽ 63.7, p ⫽ .02) or
correlations between hourly 6-SM excretion and age, height or postpubertal subjects (1.28 ⫾ .18 ␮g/hr, t ⫽ 2.87, df ⫽ 53.9, p ⫽
weight in either group or in the combined sample. Among .006).
patients with autistic disorder, there were no differences seen in The lower 6-SM excretion observed in patients with autistic
hourly 6-SM excretion between patients with and without life- disorder compared to controls was most marked in prepubertal
time epilepsy (.62 ⫾ .13 vs. .93 ⫾ .18 ␮g/hr, t ⫽ 1.40, df ⫽ 43.1, subjects (.56 ⫾ .15 vs. 2.30 ⫾ .31 ␮g/hr, respectively, t ⫽ 5.05,
p ⫽ .17). In those patients with a lifetime history of epilepsy, no df ⫽ 54.8, p ⬍ .0001) (see Figure 2) and was more marked in
significant effect of anticonvulsants was seen when medicated males (.66 ⫾ .12 vs. 2.20 ⫾ .29 ␮g/hr, respectively, t ⫽ 4.82, df
and unmedicated groups were compared (.50 ⫾ .18 vs. .93 ⫾ ⫽ 50.7, p ⬍ .0001) than in females (.98 ⫾ .23 vs. 1.48 ⫾ .17
.18 ␮g/hr, respectively, t ⫽ 1.56, df ⫽ 24.9, p ⫽ .13). More ␮g/hr, respectively, t ⫽ 1.51, df ⫽ 63, p ⫽ .14).
generally, medication appeared to have little effect on 6-SM Finally, sleep behavior data obtained on the patients are being
excretion, as rates were not significantly different in subsets of analyzed and will be reported separately; preliminary results do
medicated patients (anticonvulsants, neuroleptics, benzodiaz- not indicate that melatonin sulfate excretion was closely associ-

www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsych
S. Tordjman et al BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2005;57:134 –138 137

level was expressed as a urine concentration (moles/liter). This


approach does not account for individual and group differences
in urine volumes.
This decrease of nocturnal 6-SM excretion was observed
especially in males with autistic disorder, which may be of
interest considering the high male prevalence reported in autistic
disorder (Bryson 1997). The effect of puberty on 6-SM excretion
observed in the control group is consistent with previous reports
suggesting that 6-SM excretion rate and melatonin secretion rate
are higher in prepubertal healthy children than in pubertal or
young adult individuals (Cavallo and Ritschel 1996; Silman et al
1979), but this effect is not found in all studies (Bojkowski and
Arendt 1990; Griefahn et al 2003). Our results raise the issue of
abnormally low melatonin excretion in prepubertal individuals
with autism. Indeed, the 6-SM excretion rate observed in the
prepubertal autistic group is seen to be greatly reduced even
when compared to the mean values observed in the older control
subjects (Figure 2). The markedly reduced nocturnal melatonin
production in younger autistic subjects may be of special interest
in terms of neurodevelopmental consequences.
Our finding of negative correlations between 6-SM excretion
and severity of autistic impairment in verbal communication and
play suggests that reduced melatonin secretion is associated with
greater severity of autistic disorder. The significant positive
correlation between verbal IQ scores and 6-SM levels found in
Figure 2. Nocturnal 6-SM excretion rates (mean ⫾ SEM) observed in indi- our patients with autistic disorder goes in the same direction,
viduals with autistic disorder and normal control subjects grouped by pu- although the range of IQ scores in the patients was too narrow to
bertal status. ** Prepubertal individuals with autistic disorder are signifi- thoroughly test the relationship between IQ scores and 6-SM
cantly different (p ⬍ .0001) from prepubertal normal control individuals; * levels. The observed correlations between severity of communi-
Prepubertal normal control individuals are significantly different (p ⬍ .01)
from pubertal normal control individuals or postpubertal normal control cation impairments and decreased 6-SM excretion might be
individuals. related to previous reports of an association between communi-
cation impairments and problems with sleep onset in low-
ated with degree of sleep disturbance. However, 6-SM levels functioning children (Quine et al 1991).
were significantly and negatively correlated with severity of The abnormally low group mean excretion of 6-SM found in
autistic impairment in total communication (Spearman rho ⫽ patients with autistic disorder could be related to a dysregulation
- .45, p ⫽ .02), verbal communication (Spearman rho ⫽ - .40, p in circadian rhythms. The hypothesis of dysregulation in neu-
⫽ .04) and play (Spearman rho ⫽ - .41, p ⫽ .04). In addition, roendocrine functions involved in circadian rhythms is also
6-SM levels were significantly and positively correlated with supported by Kulman and collaborators (2000) who report that
verbal IQ scores (Pearson r ⫽ .45, p ⫽ .0015), but not with total none of their patient with autistic disorder had a normal mela-
IQ or performance IQ. tonin circadian rhythm (10/14 showed no circadian variation and
4/14 showed an inverted circadian rhythm associated with more
pronounced sleep disturbance) and by reports of abnormalities
Discussion
in the circadian rhythm of cortisol secretion observed in individ-
Nocturnal 6-SM excretion rate was significantly and substan- uals with autistic disorder (for review, see Tordjman et al 1997).
tially lower in patients with autistic disorder than in normal It can be also speculated that the present findings might be
comparison subjects, and was significantly negatively correlated related to altered serotonin neurobiology. Melatonin is synthe-
with severity of autistic impairments in verbal communication sized directly from serotonin, and peripheral and central abnor-
and play. The finding of abnormally low nighttime excretion of malities in serotonin physiology have been described in autism
melatonin in autism is consistent with two smaller studies of (Anderson 2002; Cook and Leventhal 1996).
serum melatonin (Nir et al 1995; Kulman et al 2000). Nir and Our results, taken together with the prior studies, indicate
colleagues found a mean reduction in nocturnal serum melatonin clearly that nocturnal secretion of melatonin is reduced in autism,
of approximately 25% in autistic patients and Kulman et al especially in the more severely affected children. In the absence
reported that none of the 14 patients studied showed a physio- of 6-SM excretion data throughout a 24-hour cycle (due to the
logical increase of melatonin concentrations during the night. It inherent difficulties of obtaining complete urine collection in low
is noteworthy that urinary 6-SM, the main melatonin metabolite, functioning children with autistic disorder), the mechanisms
has been reported to be highly correlated with blood melatonin underlying this result remain to be ascertained. Is there a general
concentrations in humans (Arendt et al 1985; Markey et al 1985). decrease in melatonin secretion during the whole 24-hour cycle,
Our data and the results of the serum melatonin studies contra- or is the melatonin circadian rhythm altered, inverted or phase-
dict a study reporting no difference in nocturnal levels of urinary shifted in autistic disorder? Further research is necessary to
melatonin between patients with autistic disorder and normal determine whether the diurnal pattern of melatonin secretion is
controls (Ritvo et al 1993). However, the study of Ritvo and altered, whether the nocturnal reduction is influenced by the
colleagues was conducted with adults and the sample size was level of intellectual functioning, and whether this nocturnal
relatively small (n ⫽ 10). Perhaps more importantly, melatonin reduction is important in problems of sleep-wake rhythm often

www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsych
138 BIOL PSYCHIATRY 2005;57:134 –138 S. Tordjman et al

associated with autism. The significant relationship found in this Lewy AJ, Bauer VK, Hasler BP, Kendall AR, Pires ML, Sack RL (2001): Capturing
study between melatonin levels and severity of autistic impair- the circadian rhythms of free-running blind people with 0.5 mg melato-
ments indicates that abnormalities in melatonin physiology may nin. Brain Res 918:96 –100.
Lewy AJ, Emens R, Sack RL, Hasler BP, Bernert RA (2002): Low but not high
play a role in or be closely linked to the pathophysiology and doses of melatonin entrained a free-running blind people person with a
behavioral expression of autistic disorder. The biochemical and long circadian period. Chronobiol Int 19:649 – 658.
neuronal pathways governing pineal melatonin production are Lindblom N, Heiskala H, Kaski M, Leinonen L, Laakso ML (2002): Sleep frag-
well-characterized and offer promising avenues for investigation. mentation inmentally retarded people decreases with increasing day
Given the greatly diminished 6-SM excretion seen in the prepu- length in spring. Chronobiol Int 19:441– 459.
bertal individuals with autism, research may be most fruitfully Lockley SW, Skene DJ, Tabandeh H, Bird AC, Defrance R, Arendt J (1997): Relation-
ship between napping and melatonin in the blind. J Biol Rhythms 12:16–25.
focused on younger patients. Finally, there is the prospect that Lord C (1997): Diagnostic instruments in autism spectrum disorder. In: Co-
administration of melatonin could serve, at least in some autistic hen DJ, VolkmarFR (editors). Handbook of autism and pervasive devel-
individuals, to normalize physiological, developmental and be- opmental disorders, 2nd ed.New York: John Wiley and Sons Inc, pp 460 –
havioral processes that are influenced by this pineal hormone. 483.
Randomized clinical trials would be necessary to establish po- Lord C (1998): What is melatonin? Is it useful treatment for sleep problems in
tential therapeutic efficacy of melatonin in autistic disorder. autism? J Autism Dev Disord 28:345–346.
Markey SP, Higa S, Shih M, Danforth DN, Tamarkin L (1985): The correlation
between human plasma melatonin levels and urinary 6-hydroxymelato-
This research was supported by La Fondation de France (ST). nin excretion.Clin Chim Acta 150:221–225.
Matthews CD, Guerin MV, Wang X (1991): Human plasma melatonin and
American Psychiatric Association (1994): Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of urinary 6-sulphatoxymelatonin: Studies in natural annual photoperiod
Mental Disorders, 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. and in extended darkness. Clin Endocrinol 35:21–27.
Anastasi A (1988): Psychological Testing, 6th ed. New York: Macmillan. Mises R, Quemada N (1993): CIM-10 (ICD-10) et Classification Française des
Anderson GM (2002): Genetics of childhood disorders: XLV. Autism, Part 4: Troubles Mentaux de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent [CFTMEA, French Classifica-
Serotonin in autism. J Amer Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 41:1513–1516. tion of Child and Adolescent Mental Disorders], 3rd ed. Vanves: Center
Arendt J, Bojkowski C, Franey C, Wright J, Marks V (1985): Immunoassay of Technique National d‘Etudes et de Recherches sur les Handicaps et les
6-hydroxymelatonin sulfate in human plasma and urine: abolition of the Inadaptations (CTNERHI), Presses Universitaires de France.
urinary 24-hour rhythm with atenolol. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 60:1166 – Nir I, Meir D, Zilber N, Knobler H, Hadjez J, Lerner Y (1995): Brief report:
1172. circadian melatonin, thyroid-stimulating hormone, prolactin, and corti-
Bojkowski CJ, Arendt J (1990): Factors influencing urinary 6-sulpha- sol levels in serum of young adults with autism. J Autism Dev Disord
toxymelatonin, a major melatonin metabolite, in normal human sub- 25:641– 654.
jects. Clin Endocrinol 33:435– 444. Palm L, Blennow G, Wetterberg L (1997): Long-term melatonin treatment in
Bojkowski CJ, Arendt J, Shih MC, Markey SP (1987): Melatonin secretion in blind children and young adults with circadian sleep-wake disturbances.
humans assesses by measuring its metabolite, 6-sulphatoxymelatonin. Dev Med Child Neurol 39:319 –325.
Clin Chem 33:1343–1348. Patzold LM, Richdale AL, Tonge BJ (1998): An investigation into sleep char-
Brezinski A (1997): Melatonin in humans. New Engl J Med 336:186 –195. acteristics of children with autism and Asperger’s disorder. J Paed Child
Bryson SE (1997): Epidemiology of autism: overview and issues outstanding. Health 34:528 –533.
In: Cohen DJ, Volkmar FR (editors). Handbook of autism and pervasive Quine L (1991): Sleep problems in children with mental handicap. J Ment
developmental disorders, 2nd ed. New York: John Wiley and Sons Inc, pp Defic Res 35:269 –290.
41– 46. Reiter RJ, Barlow-Walden L, Poeggeler B, Heiden SM, Clayton RJ (1996):
Cavallo A, Ritschel WA (1996): Pharmacokinetics of melatonin in human Twenty-four hour urinary excretion of 6-hydroxymelatonin sulfate in
sexual maturation. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 81(5):1882–1886. Down syndrome subjects. J Pineal Res 20:45–50.
Cohen J, Cohen P (1983): Applied Multiple/correlation Analysis for the Behav- Richdale AL (1999): Sleep problems in autism : prevalence, cause, and inter-
ioral Sciences, 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. vention. Dev Med Child Neurol 41:60 – 66.
Cook BH, Leventhal BL (1996): The serotonin system in autism. Curr Opin Richdale AL, Prior MR (1995): The sleep-wake rhythm in children with autism.
Pediatr 8:348 –354. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 4:175–186.
Dahlitz M, Alvarez B, Vignau J, English J, Arendt J, Parkes JD (1991): Delayed Ritvo ER, Ritvo R, Yuliwer A, Brothers A, Freeman BJ, Plotkin S (1993): Elevated
sleep phase syndrome. Lancet 337:1121–1124. daytime melatonin concentration in autism. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry
Deacon SJ, Arendt J (1994): Phase shifts in melatonin, 6-sulphatoxymelato- 2:75–78.
nin and alertness rhythms after treatment with moderately bright light Rutter M (1968): Concepts of autism-a review of research. J Child Psychol
at night. Clin Endocrinol 40:413– 420. Psychiatry 9:1–25.
Griefahn B, Brode P, Blaszkewicz M, Remer T (2003): Melatonin production Sadeh A (1997): Sleep and melatonin in infants : A preliminary study. Sleep
during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal study on the excre- 20:185–191.
tion of urinary 6-hydroxymelatonin sulfate. J Pineal Res 34:26 –31. Schreck KA, Mullick JA (2000): Parental reports of sleep problems in children
Hayashi E (2000): Effect of melatonin on sleep-wake rhythm: The sleep diary with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 30:127–135.
of an autistic male. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 54:383–384. Silman RE, Leone RM, Hooper RJL, Preece MA (1979): Melatonin, the pineal
Honomichl RD, Goodlin-Jones BL Burnham M, Gaylor E, Anders T (2002): gland and human puberty. Nature 282:301.
Sleep patterns of children with pervasive developmental disorders. J Tanner JH (1962): Growth of Adolescents. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publi-
Autism Dev Disord 32:553–561. cations.
Hoshino Y, Wanatabe H, Yashima Y, Kaneko M, Kumashiro H (1984): An Tordjman S, Anderson GM, McBride PA, Hertzig M, Snow M, Hall L, et al
investigation on the sleep disturbance of autistic children. Folia Psychiatr (1997): Plasma beta-Endorphin, Adrenocorticotropin Hormone and Cor-
Neurol Jpn 38:45–51. tisol in Autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 38(6):705–716.
Ishizaki A, Sugama M, Takeuchi N (1999): Usefulness of melatonin for developmen- Tordjman S, Gutknecht L, Carlier M, Spitz E, Antoine C, Slama F, et al (2001):
tal sleep and emotional/behaviour disorders—studies of melatonin trial on 50 Role of the serotonin transporter gene in the behavioral expression of
patients with developmental disorders. No To Hattatsu 31:428–437 autism. Mol Psychiatry 6:434 – 439.
Jan J, Freeman RD, Fast DK (1999): Melatonin treatment of sleep-wake cycle Touitou Y (2001): Human aging and melatonin. Clinical relevance. Exp Ger-
disorders in children and adolescents. Dev Med Child Neurol 41:491–500. ontol 36:1083–1100.
Johnson CR (1996): Sleep problems in children with mental retardation and Wetterberg L (1999): Melatonin and clinical application. Reprod Nutr Dev
autism. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Amer 5:673– 683. 39:367–382.
KulmanG,LissoniP,RovelliF,RoselliMG,BrivioF,SequeriP(2000):Evidenceofpineal Zhdanova I, Lynch H, Wurtman R (1997): Melatonin: a sleep-promoting
endocrine hypofunction in autistic children. Neuroendocrinol Lett 20:31–34. hormone. Sleep 20:899 –907.

www.elsevier.com/locate/biopsych

You might also like