You are on page 1of 12

414145

et al.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin


PSPXXX10.1177/0146167211414145Becker

Article
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

Emotional Consequences of Collective 37(12) 1587­–1598


© 2011 by the Society for Personality
and Social Psychology, Inc
Action Participation: Differentiating Self- Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Directed and Outgroup-Directed Emotions


DOI: 10.1177/0146167211414145
http://pspb.sagepub.com

Julia C. Becker1, Nicole Tausch2, and Ulrich Wagner1

Abstract
The present research examines the emotional and behavioral consequences of collective action participation. It demonstrates
that “positive” and “negative” emotions can be experienced simultaneously as a result of collective action participation, yet
it is important to distinguish outgroup-directed from self-directed emotions. Results of two experiments (N = 71 and
N = 101) that manipulated participation in collective action illustrate that whereas collective action participants experience
more outgroup-directed anger and contempt, they feel more self-directed positive affect. Furthermore, collective action
participation predicted willingness to engage in moderate and radical collective actions in the future. These relations were
mediated by outgroup-directed, but not by self-directed, emotions, suggesting that outgroup-directed rather than self-
directed emotions play a crucial role in the maintenance of protest behavior. Theoretical and practical implications of these
findings are discussed.

Keywords
collective action participation, self-directed emotions, outgroup-directed emotions, intergroup emotions, moderate collective
action, radical collective action

Received February 4, 2010; revision accepted May 19, 2011

How do people feel after they have participated in collective psychological well-being (Boehnke & Wong, 2011; Klar &
protest? Does protest make them feel happy and satisfied or Kasser, 2009). In contrast, research on social identity pro-
does it inflame their anger and hate? Several models have cesses in collective action suggests that collective action par-
been developed to predict engagement in collective action ticipation heightens the salience of one’s social identity, the
(e.g., Stürmer & Simon, 2004; Van Zomeren, Postmes, & perception of group-based injustice, and corresponding emo-
Spears, 2008; Van Zomeren, Spears, Fischer, & Leach, 2004; tions (e.g., Drury & Reicher, 1999; Reicher, 1996; Reicher,
Wright, 2001), but outcomes of collective action participa- Spears, & Postmes, 1995; Stott & Reicher, 1998). Therefore,
tion have remained relatively underresearched (Louis, 2009; it can be argued that collective action increases “negative” or
Wright, 2009). For instance, researchers have examined the conflict-related emotions, such as feelings of anger and con-
role of emotions (such as anger) in predicting collective action tempt. In the present research, we integrate both perspec-
(e.g., Leonard, Moons, Mackie & Smith, 2011; Van Zomeren tives by distinguishing the targets of different emotions.
et al., 2004) but have neglected the role of emotions as a Specifically, we differentiate between self-directed and out-
result of collective action participation, at least in experimen- group-directed emotions. Thus, the purpose of the present
tal research (for qualitative approaches, see Drury, Cocking, research is to examine the strength and valence of self-
Beale, Hanson & Rapley, 2005; Drury & Reicher, 2005). We directed and outgroup-directed emotions as a result of collective
posit not only that emotions predict collective action partici- action participation. Moreover, it has been demonstrated that
pation but that collective action participation also feeds back activists who engaged in collective action in the past are more
into the strength and valence of emotions, which in turn
motivate engagement in future actions. 1
University of Marburg, Marburg, Germany
2
Based on previous research, different predictions concern- University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, UK
ing the impact of collective action participation on emotions
Corresponding Author:
are possible. On one hand, it can be argued that collective Julia C. Becker, Philipps-University Marburg, Department of Psychology,
action participation increases “positive” emotions, such as Social Psychology, Gutenbergstraße 18, 35032 Marburg, Germany
joy and happiness, because activism is positively related to Email: beckerj2@staff.uni-marburg.de
1588 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(12)

likely to engage in collective action in the future (e.g., Stürmer provide evidence for the causal order of the relevant variables.
& Simon, 2004). To extend this work further, the present To our knowledge, there is only one experimental study that
research also aims to investigate to what extent self-directed tried to demonstrate the causal effect of activist activity on
versus outgroup-directed emotions explain the relation individuals’ well-being (Klar & Kasser, 2009, Study 3). In
between past action and future participation intentions. this study participants were asked to report their well-being
either before or after completing a survey about the improve-
ment of a cafeteria. The survey contained arguments for
Emotional Consequences of improving ethical-political aspects of food in the cafeteria,
Collective Action Participation and participants were asked to rate the importance of the
We predict that collective action participation evokes arguments and were informed that their answers would be
“positive” as well as “negative” emotions simultaneously. sent to the college’s food services department. Contrary to
There are several theoretical approaches that predict an expectations, results did not indicate any difference in well-
increase of positive emotions after collective action partici- being between the two experimental conditions (Klar &
pation: In particular, researchers from sociology and politi- Kasser, 2009). We argue that this lack of effect likely resulted
cal science have argued that political activism can be as a consequence of the operationalization of activism: Rating
perceived as motivating “in and of itself” (Klar & Kasser, arguments is a weak operationalization of activism and a
2009, p. 756; Meier & Stutzer, 2008). Such intrinsically rather passive task (compared to, for instance, generating
motivated behavior can satisfy important psychological and publicly presenting one’s own arguments) that does not
needs (e.g., Ryan & Deci, 2001) and should therefore be really operationalize collective action on behalf of a group.
accompanied by positive emotional experiences. Indeed, In sum, based on the theoretical arguments and related
some recent research has demonstrated that indicators of research findings summarized above, we test the idea that
activism are positively related to psychological well-being collective action has positive consequences for the self and
(Boehnke & Wong, 2011; Klar & Kasser, 2009), suggesting evokes positive self-directed emotions. We further examine
that self-identified activists feel better than nonactivists. whether these positive emotions are likely to drive future
Moreover, based on sociometer theory, it can be argued that participation in collective action.
collective action participation satisfies the basic human need Contrasting the research on positive outcomes of activ-
to belong (e.g., Leary, 2006). Research has confirmed that ism, it has also been demonstrated that collective action par-
the quest for belonging and the formation and maintenance ticipation can intensify conflicts in a framework of struggle
of social bonds are important motivators of why individuals for justice and power (McLeod, 1999; Páez, Basabe, Ubillos,
join movements (e.g., Cable, 1992). By acting on behalf of & González-Castro, 2007). Many examples illustrate the
one’s own group, individuals can feel socially included and dynamic nature of collective protests that start with a peace-
might therefore experience positive emotions. Furthermore, ful demonstration and end in violent action (e.g., Drury &
Tropp and Brown (2004) demonstrated that “what benefits Reicher, 2005, 2009). The elaborated social identity model
the group can also benefit the individual” by showing that of crowd behavior (e.g., Drury & Reicher, 1999, 2000;
the motivation for individual enhancement was a significant Reicher, 1996; Stott & Reicher, 1998) explains this dynamic
predictor for women’s willingness to participate in collec- as an intergroup phenomenon by analyzing the emergence
tive action. In this research, individual enhancement referred and escalation of crowd conflict over time: If their social
to the anticipation of an increase in one’s self-esteem and identity and the identity of a relevant outgroup become
feelings of being important when getting involved in women’s salient, individuals direct their behavior according to
issues. Thus, individuals seem to anticipate that they can ingroup norms. Therefore, collective action participation
personally benefit from collective action participation, can be perceived as the royal road to express one’s social
which is likely to be connected with the experience of posi- identity and to fight collectively against the outgroup
tive emotions following past engagement in collective responsible for the injustice (Drury & Reicher, 2005, 2009).
action. Finally, research on social identity processes leads to If the outgroup is powerful (e.g., the police, the govern-
a similar prediction: When individuals categorize them- ment) and acts illegitimately in terms of ingroup under-
selves as group members, they evaluate those group mem- standings, this will unify members of the ingroup and evoke
bers (including themselves) particularly positive who more extreme behaviors among them. This can radicalize
represent the group values and interests best (e.g., Hogg, collective protests, sharpen the perception of injustice, and
Hardie, & Reynolds, 1995; Turner, Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & heat up “negative” emotions (Drury & Reicher, 1999;
Wetherell, 1987). Thus, it is likely that individuals evaluate Reicher, 1996; Reicher et al., 1995).
themselves more positively when they do something good
for their ingroup by participating in collective action to pro-
mote the group’s interests (Tajfel & Turner, 1979). The Present Research
Although the described findings are consistent with our The present research aims to provide a greater understanding
view that collective action participation might increase of the emotional consequences of collective action participa-
positive self-directed emotions, these studies are unable to tion using an experimental approach. Specifically, we bring
Becker et al. 1589

together the literature on activism and psychological well- generally adhered to societal norms (Wright, Taylor, &
being with the predictions made by research on social iden- Moghaddam, 1990). However, these moderate actions can
tity processes in collective action by differentiating between be distinguished from radical collective actions, which are
self-directed emotions (e.g., feeling satisfied with or good often illegal and violate societal norms (e.g., arson attacks;
about oneself) and outgroup-directed emotions (e.g., feeling Wright et al., 1990). Tausch et al. (2011) demonstrated that
angry at an outgroup; e.g., Mackie, Devos, & Smith, 2000; whereas outgroup-directed anger predicted moderate (but
Smith, 1993; Smith, Seger, & Mackie, 2007). Distinguishing not radical) collective actions, feelings of contempt predicted
these two types of emotions enables a more precise predic- radical (but not moderate) collective actions. These effects
tion of why emotions of different valence (“positive” emotions can be explained in terms of the different social functions of
such as joy and “negative” emotions such as anger) can be anger and contempt (Fischer & Roseman, 2007). Anger is
experienced simultaneously as a result of collective action felt in relationships in which reconciliation is desired.
participation. Specifically, we predict that collective action Therefore, activists need to stick to social norms to articulate
participants experience more outgroup-directed anger, their concerns. In contrast, contempt is felt in relationships
though they feel increased self-directed positive affect. that lack reconciliatory intentions and entail a psychological
distancing from the object of contempt (Fischer & Roseman,
2007). Therefore, activists who feel contempt toward an
The Role of Emotions in opponent do not feel a need to follow social norms when
the Maintenance of Protest protesting against that target.
The second aim of the present research is to understand In Study 2, we included measures of radical collective
the role of emotions in the maintenance of protest behavior. action and feelings of contempt. We predicted that collective
Therefore, we also examine whether the emotions elicited action participation against tuition fees increases the willing-
by collective action participation do, in turn, predict the ness to engage in moderate collective action against the fees
willingness to engage in future actions. This research ques- in the future because respondents feel increased outgroup-
tion is important to understand how long-term political directed anger. Second, we predicted that participants who
activism works and why individuals often take part in col- engaged in actions against tuition fees would be more inter-
lective action for extended periods. We argue that emotional ested in engaging in radical actions against tuition fees in the
consequences of collective action participation are crucial in future because collective action participation can intensify
predicting the maintenance of political protest. In line with the conflict between students and the government. As a
this idea, Drury et al. (2005) demonstrated that a perception response to a heightened salience that the ingroup is treated
of “atmosphere and energy” during the protest was related unfairly, some students might respond not only with increased
to participants’ further involvement in collective action. outgroup-directed anger but also with feelings of contempt
Moreover, based on the finding that collective action par- against the government. Therefore, the relation between past
ticipation can be predicted by a motivation for self-enhancement participation and the intention for radical future action
(Tropp & Brown, 2004) and is associated with psychological against tuition fees should be mediated by outgroup-directed
well-being (Klar & Kasser, 2009) and happiness (Boehnke contempt.
& Wong, 2011), it is reasonable to assume that activists Finally, previous research has illustrated that beyond
continue to engage in actions because they anticipate posi- group-level emotions, social identification, particularly polit-
tive emotions. Similarly, it is possible that activists are icized identification (Simon & Klandermans, 2001), as well
motivated to continue their actions because of a heightened as group efficacy (Van Zomeren et al., 2004) predict collec-
salience of group-based disadvantage and corresponding tive action intentions. Thus, it is likely that these variables
negative outgroup-directed emotions. In the present research, also play a role in the relation between past and future
we examine the relative importance of self- versus outgroup- actions, such that participation leads to increased willingness
directed emotions as possible motivators for future action to participate in actions in the future because it increases
intentions. identification and perceived efficacy. We therefore exam-
In Study 1, we examined whether collective action par- ined the effects of self-directed and outgroup-directed emo-
ticipation increases the willingness to participate in future tions while controlling for these variables to illustrate their
collective action and investigated to what extent this is due effects over and above other established predictors of collec-
to increases in self-directed positive affect or increased tive action.
outgroup-directed anger (and therefore the intention to fur-
ther engage against group-based injustice). Moreover, cur-
rent research is increasingly interested in predictors of The Study Context
radical collective action. A recent theoretical extension of We tested our hypotheses in the context of the student
the collective action literature (Tausch et al., 2011) suggests protests against tuition fees in the federal state of Hessen,
that moderate and radical collective actions are predicted by Germany. These protests have been ongoing since 2006,
qualitatively different emotions. In Study 1, we specifically when the conservative state government of Hessen intro-
assessed collective actions that were fairly moderate and duced tuition fees. Although the fees were abolished in 2008
1590 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(12)

after the conservative government lost power to the center- the reintroduction of tuition fees. They were informed that the
left, their reintroduction is possible since the conservative aim was to collect as many arguments as possible against
party regained power in 2009. At the time of data collection tuition fees and to send them directly to the government.
(spring-summer 2010), the conservative party had just enforced Participants were asked to read and complete the following
a hiring freeze and other financial cuts for the university but protest note: “I, student at the University of Marburg, relent-
had not yet reintroduced the fees. lessly condemn a reintroduction of tuition fees by the gov-
In the present research, we heightened the salience of a ernment in Hessen and strongly oppose to the government.
possible reintroduction of tuition fees by the government and I protest against tuition fees, because . . .” At this point, stu-
provided participants with the opportunity to engage in col- dents were requested to list as many arguments as possible
lective action against the “pro-tuition fees” government. against tuition fees and reminded that all arguments would
Afterward, we measured participants’ self-directed and out- be sent to the government.
group-directed emotions. In Study 1, we compared self- Afterward, all participants completed the dependent mea-
directed and outgroup-directed emotions and intentions for sures. They completed a measure of self-directed and out-
future collective action among participants who engaged in group-directed emotions, followed by the control variables
collective action with those in a no-action control condition. identification with students, identification with the protest
In Study 2, we included four further control conditions to movement against the introduction of tuition fees (as a mea-
exclude potential alternative explanations and added a broader sure of politicized identity), and perceived group efficacy.
range of negative outgroup-directed emotions and action They then answered a measure about their future collective
tendencies. action intentions. Finally, they were thanked and debriefed.

Study 1 Measures
Method Unless otherwise indicated, all items were answered on a
7-point rating scale (0 = disagree strongly to 6 = agree
Participants. Participants in this web-based experiment strongly). The order of self-directed versus outgroup-directed
were students who participated for course credit (N = 71). emotion scales was randomly counterbalanced: Half of the
Participants’ ages ranged from 19 to 30 years (M = 21.30 years, participants completed the self-directed emotions measure
SD = 2.55). Most (92%) self-identified as German; 66% before the outgroup-directed emotions measure, whereas the
were female. Three participants were excluded because they other half completed the outgroup-directed emotions mea-
reported to be in favor of the reintroduction of tuition fees. sure before the self-directed emotions measure. Items were
Seventy-seven percent of those approached participated in given in a mixed but identical order for all participants.
the study. Outgroup-directed anger. To measure outgroup-directed
Design and procedure. The study had a 2 (collective action anger, participants were asked to indicate their current feel-
participation: yes, no) × 2 (order of emotion measurements) ings as a student toward the government in Hessen using
between-participants design. It was presented as a project of three items “As a student, I feel anger towards/I am mad at/
students who were allegedly writing their empirical thesis on I am in rage towards the government in Hessen” (α = .92).
the topic of tuition fees and wanted to connect their study Self-directed positive affect. To assess self-directed positive
with a protest against tuition fees. After completing a num- affect, individuals were instructed to describe their current
ber of demographic variables, participants read a text about feelings as an individual (“I feel good/happy/satisfied/glad/
the current hiring freeze at universities in Hessen enacted by proud/pleased/gratified”; α = .90). This measure was intro-
the government. Students were informed that the hiring duced as follows: “In the following, please concentrate on
freeze increases the likelihood of the reintroduction of tuition yourself as an individual and think about everything that
fees. This information served to heighten the salience of a makes you unique as a person and differentiates you from
potential reintroduction of fees by the government. Respon- other people. Please describe your current feelings as an
dents were asked whether they had heard of the hiring freeze, individual.”
whether they thought that the hiring freeze makes the rein- Collective action intentions. Respondents were asked to
troduction of tuition fees more likely, and about their general indicate how likely it would be that they take part in the fol-
attitudes toward tuition fees. Then, they were randomly lowing actions in case of a planned reintroduction of tuition
assigned to one of four experimental conditions: Half of the fees (0 = very unlikely, 6 = very likely): participate in discus-
participants were directly forwarded to the dependent mea- sion meetings, participate in plenary meetings, write flyers,
sures, and the other half participated in collective action sign the complaint against unconstitutionality of tuition fees,
against the government. Those in the collective action condi- take part in street theatre, and demonstrations. We averaged
tion were told that a large-scale campaign against the gov- these six items to yield a composite of likelihood of engag-
ernment in Hessen was currently being conducted to prevent ing in collective action (α = .83).
Becker et al. 1591

Table 1. Means (and Standard Deviations), Study 1 We did this while controlling for identification with stu-
dents, identification with the protest movement, and group
Collective action Control
condition condition efficacy. Collective action participation was dummy coded
(1 = collective action condition, 0 = control condition) and
Outgroup-directed anger 4.08 (1.32) 2.66 (1.44) predicted both outgroup-directed anger (B = 1.41, SE = .33,
Self-directed positive affect 3.64 (1.13) 3.01 (1.36) p < .001) and self-directed positive affect (B = .62, SE = .30,
Group efficacy 4.10 (1.15) 3.94 (1.13) p = .04). However, only outgroup-directed anger (B = .20,
Identification with students 4.55 (1.04) 3.66 (1.20)
SE = .08, p = .02) predicted the intention to engage in future
Identification with the protest 3.20 (1.62) 2.43 (1.21)
action, whereas self-directed positive affect did not (B = .03,
movement
Collective action intentions 3.53 (1.37) 2.89 (1.21)
SE = .09, p = .73). In line with full mediation, the total effect
of participation in collective action regarding the intention to
engage in collective action in the future (B = .64, SE = .31,
Control variables. Student identification was measured p = .04) was reduced to nonsignificance when the potential
using three items, e.g., “Being a student reflects an important mediators were included (B = –.23, SE = .24, p = .34). The
part of my personality” (α = .90). Identification with the pro- total indirect effect was significant (B = .86, SE = .27, p = .002).
test movement against the introduction of tuition fees (“polit- To examine the specific indirect effects, we tested a multiple
icized identity”) was measured using the same three items, mediation model using bootstrapping (as recommended by
addressing the protest movement (α = .92). Group efficacy Preacher & Hayes, 2008; Shrout & Bolger, 2002). The boot-
was measured by three items (e.g., “I think that students can strap estimates are based on 5,000 bootstrap samples, using
stop the introduction of tuition fees”; α = .92). the SPSS version of the Preacher and Hayes (2008) macro.
As expected, outgroup-directed anger significantly mediated
the relation between participation in collective action and
Results future collective action intentions, because the 95% confi-
Self-directed positive affect and outgroup-directed anger dence interval did not include zero, 95% CI [.05, .71].
were not significantly correlated (r = .15, p = .20). However, self-directed positive affect did not prove to be a
Effects on self-directed and outgroup-directed emotions. To significant mediator, 95% CI [–.12, .18]. Of the three control
evaluate whether collective action participation affected variables, only identification with the protest movement
self-directed positive affect and outgroup-directed anger, we proved to be a significant mediator, 95% CI [.10, .93].
conducted two 2 (collective action: yes, no) × 2(order of
emotions) ANOVAs. As expected, the first analysis with
self-directed positive affect as the dependent variable yielded Discussion
that participants who engaged in collective action felt more Study 1 provided support for our hypotheses that collective
self-directed positive affect, F(1, 71) = 6.05, p = .02, η2 = .08, action participation increases self-directed positive affect as
compared to the control condition (see Table 1). No other well as outgroup-directed negative affect. Consistent with
effects were significant, Fs < 1. The same analysis with out- previous research, this study also provided evidence that
group-directed anger as the dependent variable revealed that, individuals who have participated in collective action become
as predicted, participants who engaged in collective action more willing to engage in collective actions in the future.
felt more outgroup-directed anger, F(1, 71) = 18.20, p < .001, Extending previous research, we further demonstrated that
η2 = .21, compared to those assigned to the control condition this relation was mediated by outgroup-directed anger but
(see Table 1). No other effects were significant, Fs < 1. not by self-directed positive affect. Thus, we provided evi-
Effects on future collective action intentions and mediation by dence that outgroup-related emotions are more important in
emotions. Next, we tested our hypothesis that participation in the process of maintaining political activism compared to
collective action increases intentions to engage in collective self-directed emotions. Furthermore, identification with the
action in the future. The same ANOVA yielded a main effect protest movement proved to be a significant mediator. This
of collective action, F(1, 67) = 4.60, p = .04, η2 = .07, indicat- is in line with previous research, illustrating that particularly
ing that, in line with our prediction, participants who engaged the politicized identity (compared to identification with a
in collective action had a greater intention to participate in broader group) predicts collective action participation (Simon
collective action in the future, compared to those in the con- & Klandermans, 2001; Stürmer & Simon, 2004).
trol condition (see Table 1). No other effects were signifi- However, there are some limitations to this study and sev-
cant, Fs < 1. eral reasons to replicate and extend these findings. This pri-
Finally, we analyzed whether the relation between par- marily concerns the construct validity of our operationalization
ticipation in collective action and the intention to engage in of collective action participation. First, it is possible that our
future collective action was mediated by increased outgroup- results were not generated by engaging in collective action
directed anger and increased self-directed positive affect. but by dealing with the topic of tuition fees. More precisely,
1592 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(12)

it is possible that just thinking about tuition fees already protest, individuals do something positive for their ingroup and
caused increased feelings of outgroup-directed anger. represent group interests (e.g., Drury et al., 2005; Hogg et al.,
Therefore, in Study 2 we used a control group that did not 1995; Klar & Kasser, 2009; Leary, 2006; Turner et al., 1987).
engage in collective action but only dealt with the topic of Finally, we included measures of contempt and willingness
tuition fees. Second, it is likely that engagement in any type to participate in radical collective action in the future. We
of collective action is associated with a boost in self-directed tested our prediction that whereas the relation between past
positive affect because individuals do something good for participation and the intention for moderate future action
their ingroup. Outgroup-directed negative affect should, should be mediated by outgroup-directed anger, the relation
however, be target specific such that negative emotions between past participation and the intention for radical future
toward the government only increase after collective action action should be mediated by outgroup-directed contempt
directed at the government. Therefore, in Study 2 we added (Tausch et al., 2011).
a control group to test whether engagement in any type of
collective action leads to changes in emotions. Finally,
although generating arguments that would be allegedly sent Method
to the government in the name of students represents collec- Participants. Participants were students at the University
tive action participation, it is not particularly confrontational. of Marburg (N = 101). Participation was voluntary. Partici-
Therefore, in Study 2 we used a different type of collective pants’ ages ranged from 19 to 31 years (M = 22.94 years,
action that would allow participants to get more involved SD = 1.31). Most (92%) self-identified as German; 69%
and to act in a more confrontational manner. were female. Students studied diverse subjects (42% social
science, 12% medicine, 5% economics, 5% education, 18%
were studying for a degree in teaching). Approximately every
Study 2 second person approached (55%) agreed to participate.
Study 2 aimed to corroborate the findings of Study 1 and to Procedure. We used the same cover story as in Study 1.
exclude potential alternative explanations. First, we selected Research assistants approached students in public areas and
a type of collective action that offered participants the asked whether they could spend 10 min participating in their
opportunity to get more involved. Specifically, participants research project. If participants agreed, the experimenter
were asked to read an unfriendly protest note against the explained that the study is about attitudes and protest: One
government as loudly as possible and to blow into a whistle part would be about attitudes toward nature protection
as long and loudly as possible while being recorded. This because there would be a waste transfer station in a neigh-
audio-message would allegedly be sent to the government. boring city, polluting the local river. A second part would be
Four control conditions were added to this experimental about the social cuts and hiring freeze enacted by the govern-
condition. Students in the first control condition read about ment in Hessen and the participants’ attitudes toward tuition
protests in the past against the government. We hypothesized fees. First, participants were asked to indicate their overall
that simply reading about protest would not affect outgroup- attitudes concerning nature protection and tuition fees to
directed emotions to the same extent as collective action par- exclude proponents of tuition fees before the experimental
ticipation. Students in the second control condition engaged manipulation. Only opponents of tuition fees were allowed
in collective action directed at another target group. We to participate in the experiment. Thus, we avoided selective
hypothesized that engagement in protest against another dropouts of proponents of tuition fees in the condition in
target group would not increase participants’ intentions to which they were asked to protest against the government.
engage in future action against the government. Similarly, Three proponents were directly thanked and debriefed. Next,
outgroup-directed emotions that target the government the experimenter randomly assigned participants to one of
should not be affected by participating in this alternative five conditions by blindly picking one of five texts.
protest, whereas self-directed emotions would be equally In the first condition (collective action against the gov-
affected in both collective action conditions as a conse- ernment), participants were informed that they could partici-
quence of doing something good for the ingroup. Students in pate in a protest against the government in Hessen. The
the third control condition read about past protests against an experimenter explained that they would collect 1,000 voices
alternative target group. Finally, a fourth baseline control and whistles of Marburg’s students to send to the govern-
group was added. As in Study 1, students assigned to this ment of Hessen as a cumulative audio-message in the name
baseline control group completed the dependent measures of students. They were handed a brief text and were asked to
directly, without any experimental manipulation. read this text loudly into a recorder and then to blow into a
In sum, we predicted that, compared to the four control whistle as loudly and for as long as possible so that “the ears
groups, only collective action against the government heats up of the members of the government would fly off.” Before
government-directed emotions. Second, we hypothesized that, they started they were asked if they were willing to partici-
irrespective of the target group, collective action participation pate, and afterward they could select one of the whistles.1
increases self-directed positive affect, because by engaging in The text said:
Becker et al. 1593

We, students of the University of Marburg resist the Collective action intentions. Respondents were asked to
planned social cuts of the government and demand an indicate how likely it would be that they participate in 11
immediate stop of the cost-cutting program!! Please actions in case of a reintroduction of tuition fees (1 = very
prick up your ears: In the name of the students of the unlikely, 7 = very likely). Principal axis factor analysis
University of Marburg, I will now show you what yielded a two-factor solution, demonstrating that moderate
I think about your social cuts. actions (all actions already used in Study1) were clearly
separable from radical actions (throwing stones or bottles,
Then, they were asked to blow into the whistle. In the sec- arson attacks on university buildings, arson attacks on pri-
ond condition (reading about past collective action against vate property of the people held responsible, attacks on
the government), students read a short text to themselves police, attacks on persons held responsible; eigenvalues = 3.50,
that informed them about an alleged campaign against the 2.44, 1.13, .92, . . .). Therefore, we averaged the items mea-
government that took place some time ago. This was suring moderate collective action (α = .76) and those mea-
exactly the same campaign as described in the first condi- suring radical collective action (α = .81).
tion. Participants indicated whether they had heard about
this campaign. The third condition (collective action
against pollution) was identical to the first condition and Results
included reading and blowing a whistle; however, the col- Outgroup-directed anger and contempt were correlated (r = .52,
lective action was directed at the city council of a neighbor- p < .001), but neither correlated with self-directed positive
ing city that manages a waste transfer station. Before affect (r = .12, p = .25 and r = .11, p = .30, respectively).
blowing the whistle, participants read the following text Effects on self-directed positive affect. To test the hypothesis
into the recorder: that self-directed positive affect increases as a consequence
of engagement in collective action, independent of the target
We, citizens of Marburg resist the pollution of the group, but that reading about collective action does not affect
river “Lahn” through the waste transfer station in your self-directed positive affect, we calculated two planned con-
city and demand an immediate stop of the pollution!! trasts. Specifically, we compared the two collective action
Please prick up your ears: In the name of Marburg’s conditions with the baseline control condition (1,0,1,0,–2)
citizens, I will now show you what I think about your and the two reading conditions with the baseline control con-
pollution. dition (0,1,0,1,–2). As expected, the first contrast was sig-
nificant, F(1, 96) = 4.64, p = .03, η2 = .05, supporting the
The fourth condition (reading about past collective action hypothesis that, independent of the target, people who par-
against pollution) was identical to the second condition ticipated in collective action would feel more self-directed
except that participants read about past collective action positive affect compared to those assigned to the baseline
against pollution as described in the third condition. In the control condition (see Table 2). The second contrast was not
fifth condition (baseline control), participants only completed significant, F(1, 96) = .002, p = .96, η2 = .00, indicating that
the dependent measures. those who just read about collective action did not differ
After the experimental manipulation, all participants com- from the control condition in self-directed positive affect
pleted the same dependent measures as used in Study 1, as (see Table 2). Two further exploratory post hoc tests (Tukey)
well as a measure of contempt toward the government and a tested whether the two collective action conditions and the
measure of radical collective action tendencies. Finally, they two reading conditions differed in terms of self-directed
were thanked and debriefed. Their voices were immediately affect. Both tests were not significant, ps = 1.
deleted from the recorder. Effects on outgroup-directed anger and contempt. To test the
hypothesis that outgroup-directed anger and contempt
against the government increased as a consequence of par-
Measures ticipation in collective action directed against the govern-
Because we did not find any order effects of outgroup- ment, we contrasted the experimental group collective
directed versus self-directed emotions in Study 1, we action against tuition fees with all four control conditions
decided to ask all participants for their outgroup-directed (–4,1,1,1,1). Six further post hoc tests (Tukey) examined the
emotions first. We used the same measures of outgroup- hypothesis that respondents in the four control conditions did
directed anger (α = .79), self-directed positive affect (α = .82), not differ from each other in terms of outgroup-directed
identification with students (α = .72), identification with the emotions. As expected, participants in the experimental con-
protest movement (α = .85), and group efficacy (α = .88) as dition collective action against tuition fees experienced more
in Study 1. Following Mackie et al. (2000), outgroup- outgroup-directed anger, F(1, 96) = 16.63, p < .001, η2 = .15,
directed contempt was assessed using three items “As a and more outgroup-directed contempt, F(1, 96) = 27.21,
student, I feel contempt/disgust towards/I hate the govern- p < .001, η2 = .22, compared to respondents in the four con-
ment of Hessen” (α = .89). trol conditions (see Table 2). Furthermore, also in line with
1594 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(12)

Table 2. Means (and Standard Deviations), Study 2

Collective action conditions Reading conditions  


Baseline
  Tuition fees Pollution Tuition fees Pollution control
Outgroup-directed anger 5.63 (0.67) 4.42 (1.06) 4.41 (1.32) 4.70 (0.82) 4.68 (1.12)
Outgroup-directed contempt 4.27 (1.11) 2.15 (1.67) 2.39 (1.68) 2.41 (1.79) 1.75 (1.87)
Self-directed positive affect 3.86 (0.92) 3.94 (0.81) 3.33 (1.03) 3.35 (1.13) 3.35 (0.74)
Group efficacy 4.84 (1.18) 4.40 (1.32) 4.02 (1.26) 4.65 (1.15) 4.40 (1.01)
Identification with students 3.94 (1.36) 4.32 (1.38) 3.71 (1.22) 3.96 (1.39) 3.68 (1.34)
Identification with the protest movement 4.68 (1.01) 3.47 (1.51) 2.86 (1.26) 3.15 (1.35) 3.05 (1.29)
Moderate collective action intentions 4.87 (0.74) 4.32 (1.03) 4.33 (0.96) 4.22 (1.25) 4.33 (1.26)
Radical collective action intentions 0.68 (1.05) 0.18 (0.36) 0.21 (0.43) 0.32 (0.71) 0.12 (0.32)

predictions, both outgroup-directed emotions did not differ to engage in moderate collective action in the future (B = .59,
between the four control conditions, all ps > .71 (see Table 2). SE = .25, p = .02) was reduced to nonsignificance when the
Effects on future collective action intentions and mediation by potential mediators were included (B = –.15, SE = .27, p = .59).
emotions. Next, we tested whether participation in collective The total indirect effect was significant (B = .74, SE = .20,
action increased the intention to engage in moderate and p < .001), suggesting an overall mediation effect. As in Study 1,
radical collective action against tuition fees in the future. The to examine the specific indirect effects, we tested a multiple
same contrasts as above were used to test our specific mediation model using bootstrapping. Replicating findings
hypothesis that only collective action against tuition fees from Study 1, outgroup-directed anger significantly medi-
should increase participants’ intentions to engage in future ated the relation between participation in collective action
action against tuition fees. In line with predictions, partici- against tuition fees and the intention to engage in moder-
pants in the experimental condition collective action against ate actions, 95% CI [.03, .48]. Neither outgroup-directed
tuition fees reported stronger intentions to engage in moder- contempt nor self-directed positive affect was a significant
ate collective action, F(1, 96) = 4.94, p = .03, η2 = .05, as mediator, 95% CI [–.27, .28] and [–.07, .05], respectively.
well as in radical collective action, F(1, 96) = 9.21, p = .003, Regarding the three control variables, only identification
η2 = .09, compared to those assigned to the control condi- with the protest movement proved to be a significant media-
tions (see Table 2). In addition, as expected, post hoc tests tor, 95% CI [.14, .72].
revealed that there were no differences between the four con- The second regression predicted the intention to engage
trol conditions in terms of participant’s intentions to engage in radical collective action in the future. Outgroup-directed
in moderate or radical collective action in the future, all ps >.86 contempt predicted the intention to engage in radical future
(see Table 2). action (B = .14, SE = .04, p < .001), whereas outgroup-
Finally, we examined whether the impact of participation directed anger and self-directed positive affect did not (B = –.03,
in collective action against tuition fees on intention to engage SE = .06, p = .70 and B = .01, SE = .06, p = .94, respectively).
in moderate and radical collective action in the future was The total effect of collective action participation against the
mediated by increased outgroup-directed anger and con- government on the intention to participate in radical actions
tempt, and increased self-directed positive affect. As in (B = .47, SE = .16, p = .01) was also reduced to nonsignifi-
Study 1, this analysis controlled for student and politicized cance when the potential mediators were included (B = .11,
identification and group efficacy. The experimental factor SE = .18, p = .53). Moreover, the total indirect effect was
was recoded into a dummy variable (1 = collective action significant (B = .36, SE = .12, p = .003). The bootstrapping
against tuition fees, 0 = all other conditions) and predicted procedure revealed that only outgroup-directed contempt
outgroup-directed anger (B = 1.01, SE = .24, p < .001) and mediated the relation between participation in collective
outgroup-directed contempt (B = 2.07, SE = .40, p < .001) action against tuition fees and radical future collective action
but did not predict self-directed positive affect (B = .35, intentions, 95% CI [.14, .49]. All other indirect effects were
SE = .23, p = .14). not significant, 95% CI [–.19, .09] and 95% CI [–.05, .08] for
The first regression predicted the intention to engage in outgroup-directed anger and self-directed positive affect,
moderate collective action in the future. Outgroup-directed respectively.
anger predicted the intention to engage in moderate future
action (B = .22, SE = .10, p = .03), whereas outgroup-directed
contempt and self-directed positive affect did not (B = .003, Discussion
SE = .06, p = .96 and B = –.05, SE = .10, p = .59, respec- Study 2 corroborated findings of Study 1 and excluded a
tively). In line with predictions, the total effect of collective number of alternative explanations. Importantly, outgroup-
action participation against the government on the intention directed anger and contempt increased only as a consequence
Becker et al. 1595

of participation in collective action against the government, (e.g., Klar & Kasser, 2009; Meier & Stutzer, 2008) with the
underlining the target specificity of outgroup-directed nega- predictions made by work on social identity processes in
tive emotions resulting from collective action participation. collective action (e.g., Drury & Reicher, 1999, 2005, 2009;
Neither reading about past protests opposing the government Reicher, 1996; Reicher et al., 1995; Tajfel & Turner, 1979)
nor engaging in an alternative type of protest (against pollu- and distinguished between self-directed and outgroup-
tion) affected outgroup-directed emotions in relation to the directed emotions as consequences of collective action
government. Second, as predicted, collective action partici- participation.
pation in general lead to increased self-directed positive To our knowledge, emotional consequences of collective
affect. Thus, irrespective of the target of protest, individuals action participation directed at different targets have not
felt more satisfied when they acted on behalf of their been examined in previous experimental research. Thus, the
ingroup, be it “students” or “citizens of Marburg.” current work extends past research on the relation between
Third, we replicated the finding that collective action par- emotions and collective action participation by distinguish-
ticipation against the government increased the intention to ing different types of emotions and by examining the impact
engage in moderate protest in the future and that this effect of these emotions on future collective action. Referring to
was due to increased outgroup-directed anger. More intrigu- self-directed emotions, we demonstrated that collective
ing, however, is the finding that collective action participa- action participation increased positive affect such as joy and
tion can also increase the intention to engage in radical satisfaction. Thus, our results are consistent with research
collective action and that this is mediated by increased feel- illustrating that activism is positively correlated with indi-
ings of contempt toward the government. These differential vidual’s well-being (Klar & Kasser, 2009), that activists
effects of outgroup-directed anger and contempt on willing- anticipate personal benefits from collective action participa-
ness to engage in either moderate or radical collective action tion (Tropp & Brown, 2004), and that collective action
are consistent with recent findings by Tausch et al. (2011). participation can evoke a positive emotional climate by
Finally, replicating results of Study 1, self-directed emotions emphasizing positive feelings (Páez et al., 2007; Rimé,
did not mediate the relation between collective action par- 2007). This is also in line with research in the context of
ticipation and willingness to engage in future action. confronting discrimination that demonstrated that confront-
ers experience personal satisfaction from defending their
group (Shelton, Richeson, Salvatore, & Hill, 2006).
General Discussion However, we also demonstrate that collective action par-
This research extends the literature on the relation between ticipation against the government inflamed student’s outgroup-
emotions and collective action in several important ways. directed anger and contempt. This finding supports predictions
First, it provides two separate empirical tests illustrating that based on a social identity theory approach to collective
collective action participation affects the emotional experi- action by showing that engaging in collective action height-
ence of activists. To our knowledge, this is the first direct ens not only participants’ politicized identity but also corre-
experimental evidence demonstrating the emotional conse- sponding emotions by increasing “negative” emotions directed
quences of collective action. Second, this research advances at the outgroup. Thus, this research contributes to our under-
theory on emotions as outcomes of collective action by dis- standing of the relation between collective action partici-
tinguishing outgroup-directed from self-directed emotions pation, social identity processes, and the experience of
and thereby providing an answer why “positive” and “negative” self-directed versus outgroup-directed emotions.
emotions can be experienced simultaneously as a result of
collective action participation. Finally, by experimentally
examining the predictive power of the emotions evoked by The Dynamic Relation Between Collective
collective action participation regarding the willingness to Action Participation and Emotions
engage in moderate and radical collective action in the The present research further illustrates the dynamic relation
future, this research contributes to the understanding of the between emotions and collective action and provides a first
dynamic relation between collective action participation and answer regarding the relative importance of outgroup-directed
emotions. Moreover, it addresses a crucial question regard- versus self-directed emotions in this process: Although both
ing the relative importance of different types of emotion for outgroup-directed and self-directed emotions were affected
the maintenance of political protest behavior. by collective action participation, only outgroup-directed
emotions predicted future action intentions. Specifically,
results of Study 2 showed that increased outgroup-directed
Emotional Consequences anger, which was evoked by collective action participation,
of Collective Action Participation enhanced participant’s willingness to engage in moderate
To provide a greater understanding of the emotional conse- but not radical collective action in the future. Feelings of
quences of collective action participation, we brought together outgroup-directed contempt, on the other hand, which were
the literature on activism and psychological well-being evoked by collective action participation, enhanced participants’
1596 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(12)

willingness to engage in radical but not in moderate collec- with the disadvantaged group and for individuals with low
tive action in the future. These findings are in line with the self-esteem, because these people may engage in protest for
recent conceptualization of anger and contempt as serving different reasons than people high in identification and self-
different social functions (Fischer & Roseman, 2007; Tausch esteem. Compared to activists highly identified with the
et al., 2011). Anger is felt in relationships where reconcili- protest movement, less identified activists do not engage in
ation and an improvement of the relationship are desired. collective action because they feel outgroup-directed anger
Thus, anger tends to result in actions that conform to social but because of pragmatic and strategic reasons (e.g., Van
norms, because actions outside the norms of the system Zomeren et al., 2008). Therefore, the experience of outgroup-
would jeopardize the reconciliation process. In contrast, directed anger should be less important for activists low in
contempt is felt in relationships that lack reconciliation group identification. Instead, the experience of personal
intentions and often develops on top of anger (Fischer & benefits (i.e., self-directed positive affect) might energize
Roseman, 2007). Therefore, in line with our results, individu- participation in future actions. Furthermore, the experience
als are less compelled to adhere to social norms when acting of self-directed positive affect (instead of outgroup-directed
against the target toward which they feel contempt (see also anger) might be crucial for individuals with low self-esteem
Tausch et al., in press). as a motivator to engage in future action. These qualifica-
In sum, we have demonstrated that outgroup-directed, but tions would be consistent with research illustrating that
not self-directed, emotions evoked by collective action par- activists who are members of an organization have different
ticipation have implications for intentions to engage in future motives to engage in collective action than activists who are
action. Therefore, our research contributes to understanding not organized (Hornsey et al., 2006). Therefore, one interest-
why political protest is often persistent and provides insights ing avenue for future work would be an investigation of the
into the roles of emotional consequences of collective action moderating role of group identification and self-esteem:
participation in motivating future protest participation. By Both variables might have differential implications for the
showing that outgroup-directed anger and contempt are more predictive power of outgroup-directed and self-directed
important than self-directed positive affect in predicting emotions on future action intentions. To test these hypothe-
future action, the current work supports the argument that ses, future research needs to measure these variables before
people engage in collective action to improve the conditions the experimental manipulation.
of their group (e.g., Wright et al., 1990). That is, collective Furthermore, it is important to note that the nature of
action participation heightens the perception that the ingroup emotions depends on the outcome of collective action. In our
is treated unfairly, which produces corresponding “negative” research, participants did not know whether their protest
outgroup-directed emotions and increases the intention to would be successful. However, it has been shown that suc-
engage in actions against this type of injustice in the future. cess versus failure of collective action affects activists (e.g.,
Therefore, the current research qualifies recent approaches Drury & Reicher, 2005). For instance, activists who were
in which researchers focused on egoistic motivations for col- asked to describe empowering collective action experiences
lective action participation such as collective action partici- reported positive feelings, whereas they reported feelings of
pation as a means for reducing negative aversive tensions discouragement, fear, and disappointment when they were
(Stürmer & Simon, 2009). Although our work is consistent asked to describe disempowering collective action experi-
with the idea that activists anticipate individual enhancement ences (Drury et al., 2005). Similarly, Maitner, Mackie and
after collective action participation (Tropp & Brown, 2004) Smith (2006) demonstrated that outgroup-directed anger
and feel good after collective action participation, we dem- depends on the success of the protest: If the ingroup’s protest
onstrated that outgroup-directed emotions rather than indi- resulted in an apology by outgroup members, outgroup-
vidual enhancement (i.e., self-directed positive affect) are directed anger was reduced. However, if the ingroup’s protest
the driving force in long-term political protest. was not successful—that is, if it did not lead to an apology by
the outgroup—outgroup-directed anger increased. Thus, future
research is needed to examine changes in self-directed and
Limitations and Future Research outgroup-directed emotional reactions depending on success
Although our findings clearly demonstrate that outgroup- and failure of a protest. Moreover, it would be interesting to
directed emotions are more important than self-directed investigate the emotions experienced by individuals who
emotions in terms of explaining the maintenance of long- participated in radical collective action. Engagement in radi-
term protest, we believe that this effect might be moderated cal compared to moderate collective action should have a
by individual-difference variables such as group identifica- different psychological impact on activists. For instance, it
tion and self-esteem. Both variables may dictate how the has been demonstrated that activists who engaged in radical
individual experiences collective action participation and collective action on students’ behalf disidentified with stu-
how these experiences relate to future action. It is conceiv- dents when the mismatch of their actions with student norms
able that self-directed positive affect mediates the relation became apparent (Becker, Tausch, Spears, & Christ, 2011).
between past and future action for those who are less identified Engagement in moderate collective action, on the other hand,
Becker et al. 1597

did not affect student’s identification with the broader ingroup. Boehnke, K., & Wong, B. (2011). Adolescent political activism
Thus, given that engagement in radical action is more risky and long-term happiness: A 21-year longitudinal study on the
than participation in moderate action, it is likely to also result development of micro- and macrosocial worries. Personality
in qualitatively different emotions, such as in increased lev- and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 435-447.
els of anxiety and fear, but also in intensive feelings of pride Cable, S. (1992). Women’s social movement involvement: The
and exhilaration. role of structural availability in recruitment and participation
Finally, although we instructed our participants to focus processes. Sociological Quarterly, 33, 35-50.
on themselves as individuals when measuring positive emo- Drury, J., Cocking, C., Beale, J., Hanson, C., & Rapley, F. (2005).
tions, it could be argued that we did not measure self-directed The phenomenology of empowerment in collective action. British
but rather ingroup-directed emotions in our studies. We are Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 309-328.
confident that the positive emotions measured in our studies Drury, J., & Reicher, S. (1999). The intergroup dynamics of col-
were not ingroup directed as they were not correlated with lective empowerment: Substantiating the social identity model
identification with the protest movement against tuition fees of crowd behaviour. Group Processes and Intergroup Rela-
(p > .15). Moreover, ingroup-directed emotions would be tions, 2, 1-22.
likely to mediate the relation between past and future action. Drury, J., & Reicher, S. D. (2005). Explaining enduring empower-
Yet, we did not find evidence that the positive emotions ment: A comparative study of collective action and psycho-
mediated this relation. Future research is necessary to exam- logical outcomes. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35,
ine the impact of collective action participation on ingroup- 35-38.
directed emotions. Moreover, recent research suggests that Drury, J., & Reicher, S. (2009). Collective psychological empow-
individuals can also feel action-directed emotions. For erment as a model of social change: Researching crowds and
example, it has been demonstrated that if ingroup aggression power. Journal of Social Issues, 65, 707-725.
directed at an outgroup elicited feelings of satisfaction (satis- Fischer, A. H., & Roseman, I. J. (2007).Beat them or ban them: The
fied, pleased, content), ingroup members are more likely to characteristics and social functions anger and contempt. Journal
support future ingroup aggression (Maitner, Mackie, & Smith, of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 103-115.
2007). Thus, future research could extend the present work Hogg, M. A., Hardie, E. A., & Reynolds, K. J. (1995). Prototypical
by examining effects of collective action participation not similarity, self-categorization, and depersonalized attraction: A
only on self-directed and outgroup-directed emotions but perspective on group cohesiveness. European Journal of Social
also on ingroup-directed and action-directed emotions. Psychology, 25, 159-177.
Hornsey, M. J., Blackwood, L., Louis, W., Fielding, K., Mavor, K.,
Authors’ Note Morton, T., & . . . White, K. M. (2006). Why do people engage in
The authors thank Stephanie Laux, Marcus Raser, Lena Rupp and collective action? Revisiting the role of perceived effectiveness.
Martin Vogel for their help with data collection. This research was Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36, 1701-1722.
conducted while Julia Becker was Associated Postdoctoral Fellow Klar, M., & Kasser, T. (2009). Some benefits of being an activist:
of the DFG Research Training Group “Group-Focused Enmity” Measuring activism and its role in psychological well-being.
(GRK 884) at the University of Marburg and Nicole Tausch was Political Psychology, 30, 755-777.
British Academy Postdoctoral Fellow at Cardiff University. Leary, M. R. (2006).Sociometer theory and the pursuit of relational
value: Getting to the root of self-esteem. European Review of
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Social Psychology, 16, 75-111.
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to Leonard, D. J., Moons, W. G., Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2011).
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. We’re mad as hell and we’re not going to take it anymore:
Anger self-stereotyping and collective action. Group Processes
Funding and Intergroup Relations, 14, 99-111.
The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author- Louis, W. R. (2009). Collective action—and then what? Journal of
ship, and/or publication of this article. Social Issues, 65, 727-748.
Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Inter-group emotions:
Note Explaining offensive action tendencies in an inter-group context.
1. Two participants in both collective action conditions refused to Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 602-616.
participate and were directly thanked and debriefed. Maitner, A. T., Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2006). Evidence
for the regulatory function of intergroup emotion: Implement-
References ing and impeding intergroup behavioral intentions. Journal of
Becker, J. C., Tausch, N., Spears, R. & Christ, O. (2011). Commit- Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 720-726.
ted dis(s)idents: Participation in radical collective action fos- Maitner, A. T., Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2007). Antecedents
ters disidentification with the broader in-group but enhances and consequences of satisfaction and guilt following ingroup
political identification. Personality and Social Psychology aggression. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 10,
Bulletin, 37, 1104-1116. 225-239.
1598 Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37(12)

McLeod, J. R. (1999). The sociodrama of presidential politics. Stott, C., & Reicher, S. (1998). Crowd action as intergroup process:
American Anthropologist, 101, 359-373. Introducing the police perspective. European Journal of Social
Meier, S., & Stutzer, A. (2008). Is volunteering rewarding in itself? Psychology, 26, 509-529.
Economica, 75, 39-59. Stürmer, S., & Simon, B. (2004). The role of collective identifi-
Páez, D., Basabe, N., Ubillos, S., & González-Castro, J. L. (2007). cation in social movement participation: A panel study in the
Social sharing, participation in demonstrations, emotional cli- context of the German gay movement. Personality and Social
mate, and coping with collective violence after the March 11th Psychology Bulletin, 30, 263-277.
Madrit bombings. Journal of Social Issues, 63, 323-337. Stürmer, S., & Simon, B. (2009). Pathways to collective protest:
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008).Asymptotic and resam- Calculation, identification or emotion? A critical analysis of
pling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects the role of group-based anger in social movement participation.
in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, Journal of Social Issues, 65, 681-705.
40, 879-891. Tausch, N., Becker, J. C., Spears, R., Christ, O., Saab, R., Singh, &
Reicher, S. (1996). “The Battle of Westminster”: Developing the Siddiqui, R. N. (2011). Explaining Radical Group Behaviour:
social identity model of crowd behaviour in order to explain Developing Emotion and Efficacy Routes to Normative and
the initiation and development of collective conflict. European Non-normative Collective Action. Journal of Personality and
Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 115-134. Social Psychology, 101, 129-148.
Reicher, S., Spears, R., & Postmes, T. (1995). A social identity Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup
model of deindividuation phenomena. European Review of conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychol-
Social Psychology, 6, 161-198. ogy of intergroup relations (pp. 33-48). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Rimé, B. (2007).The social sharing of emotion as an interface Tropp, L. R., & Brown, A. C. (2004). What benefits the group can
between individual and collective processes in the construction also benefit the individual: group-enhancing and individual-
of emotional climates. Journal of Social Issues, 63, 307-322. enhancing motives for collective action. Group Processes and
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human poten- Intergroup Relations, 7, 267-282.
tials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., &
Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141-166. Wetherell, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-
Shelton, J. N., Richeson, J. A., Salvatore, J., & Hill, D. M. (2006). categorization theory. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Silence is not golden: Intrapersonal consequences of not con- Van Zomeren, M., Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (2008). Toward an
fronting prejudice.In S. Levin & C. Van Laar (Eds.), Social integrative social identity model of collective action: A quan-
stigma and group inequality: Social psychological perspec- titative research synthesis of three socio-psychological perspec-
tives. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. tives. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 504-535.
Shrout, P. E., & Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and Van Zomeren, M., Spears, R., Fischer, A. H., & Leach, C. W.
nonexperimental studies: New procedures and recommenda- (2004). Put your money where your mouth is!: Explaining
tions. Psychological Methods, 7, 422-445. collective action tendencies through group-based anger and
Simon, B., & Klandermans, B. (2001). Politicized collective iden- group efficacy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
tity: A social psychological analysis. American Psychologist, 87, 649-664.
56, 319-331. Wright, S. C. (2001). Strategic collective action: Social psychol-
Smith, E. R. (1993). Social identity and social emotions: Toward ogy and social change. In R. Brown & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.),
new conceptualizations of prejudice. In D. M. Mackie & Intergroup processes: Blackwell handbook journal of social
D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 409-430). Oxford, England: Blackwell.
Interactive processes in group perception (pp.297-315). San Wright, S. C. (2009). The next generation of collective action
Diego, CA: Academic Press. researh. Journal of Social Issues, 65, 859-879.
Smith, E. R., Seger, C. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2007). Can emo- Wright, S. C., Taylor, D. M., & Moghaddam, F. M. (1990).
tions be truly group-level? Evidence regarding four concep- Responding to membership in a disadvantaged group: From
tual criteria. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, acceptance to collective protest. Journal of Personality and Social
93, 431-446. Psychology, 58, 994-1003.

You might also like