You are on page 1of 16

Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Cognition
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/COGNIT

Anatomy of an error: A bidirectional state model of task


engagement/disengagement and attention-related errors
J. Allan Cheyne, Grayden J.F. Solman, Jonathan S.A. Carriere, Daniel Smilek *
Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, 200 University Ave. West, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada N2L 3G1

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: We present arguments and evidence for a three-state attentional model of task engage-
Received 15 February 2008 ment/disengagement. The model postulates three states of mind-wandering: occurrent
Revised 30 December 2008 task inattention, generic task inattention, and response disengagement. We hypothesize
Accepted 31 December 2008
that all three states are both causes and consequences of task performance outcomes
and apply across a variety of experimental and real-world tasks. We apply this model to
the analysis of a widely used GO/NOGO task, the Sustained Attention to Response Task
Keywords:
(SART). We identify three performance characteristics of the SART that map onto the three
Mind-wandering
Attention
states of the model: RT variability, anticipations, and omissions. Predictions based on the
Attentional engagement model are tested, and largely corroborated, via regression and lag-sequential analyses of
Attentional disengagement both successful and unsuccessful withholding on NOGO trials as well as self-reported
SART mind-wandering and everyday cognitive errors. The results revealed theoretically consis-
Inattention tent temporal associations among the state indicators and between these and SART errors
Attentional error as well as with self-report measures. Lag analysis was consistent with the hypotheses that
Action slips temporal transitions among states are often extremely abrupt and that the association
Sustained attention
between mind-wandering and performance is bidirectional. The bidirectional effects sug-
gest that errors constitute important occasions for reactive mind-wandering. The model
also enables concrete phenomenological, behavioral, and physiological predictions for
future research.
Ó 2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Smallwood, O’Connor, Sudberry, Haskell, & Ballantine,


2004; Smallwood, Obonsawin, & Heim, 2003; Smallwood,
The type of error in question is an attention-related ac- Obonsawin, & Reid, 2003; Smallwood et al., 2004), tune
tion or cognitive error caused by a loss of attentional outs and zone outs (Schooler, 2002; Schooler, Reichle, &
engagement in an ongoing primary task (Reason, 1990; Halpern, 2005; Smallwood, McSpadden, & Schooler,
Reason & Mycielska, 1982). Task disengagement and its 2007), and mind pops (Kvavilashvili & Mandler, 2004).
sources have been studied under a number of rubrics Whatever label is used, current approaches to attentional
including absent-mindedness (Cheyne, Carriere, & Smilek, task disengagement agree that the core phenomenon con-
2006; Reason & Lucas, 1984), mind-wandering (Kane sists of a state of reduced allocation of attentional re-
et al., 2007; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006; Wegner, 1997), sources to environmental task-related stimuli (Manly,
stimulus-independent thought (Antrobus, 1968; Teasdale, Robertson, Galloway, & Hawkins, 1999; Robertson, Manly,
Lloyd, Proctor, & Baddeley, 1993; Teasdale, Segal, & Andrade, Baddeley, & Yiend, 1997; Smallwood & Schooler,
Williams, 1995), task-unrelated/related thought (Giambra, 2006; Smallwood et al., 2007). Conditions promoting such
1995; Smallwood, Baracaia, Lowe, & Obonsawin, 2003; task disengagement arising from mind-wandering include
protracted, unvarying, familiar, repetitive, and undemand-
ing tasks and task environments (see Smallwood and
* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: dsmilek@uwaterloo.ca (D. Smilek). Schooler (2006) for a review). Although distinctions

0010-0277/$ - see front matter Ó 2009 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.12.009
J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113 99

between mind-wandering with and without awareness analysis, as well as the subsequent model of task disen-
(Hester, Foxe, Molholm, Shpaner, & Garavan, 2005; gagement, assumes adequate knowledge, skill, and motiva-
Schooler, 2002; Schooler et al., 2005; Smallwood et al., tion on the part of the performer. As noted at the outset,
2007) imply different modes or states of attentional disen- the error in question occurs despite optimal training, com-
gagement or decoupling (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006), petence, and motivation of the performer. The present
these have not been conceptually analyzed in terms of analysis is focused on the withdrawal of attentional re-
underlying cognitive processing failures. We propose a sources from an external task rather than on important is-
model of attentional engagement/disengagement consist- sue of the nature of the allocation of resources to internal
ing of three distinct states, describe their cognitive, processing (see Smallwood, Baracaia, Lowe, and Obonsa-
phenomenological, and performance characteristics, and win (2003), Smallwood, Heim, Rigby, and Davies (2006),
suggest real-world examples of each. We propose and as- and Smallwood et al. (2004, 2007)).
sess behavioral markers for each state using an established
sustained attention task and relate these markers to 2.2. Task disengagement: states of inattention
behavioral performance and to self-reported everyday
mind-wandering and cognitive failure. State 1: The model proposes three discrete states of
attentional disengagement. Based on the foregoing task
2. States of attentional disengagement: a three-state analysis, it follows that the attentionally most demanding
model dynamic level will be most vulnerable to mind-wandering
leading to occurrent task inattention. Occurrent task inat-
2.1. Task engagement: to what do we attend when we attend tention (State 1) consists of an often brief and/or partial
to a task? waning of detailed processing of moment-to-moment
stimulus meaning. The essential characteristic of State 1
The present model of mind-wandering and task disen- is a transient disengagement of attention from the dy-
gagement assumes that successful task performance in namic features of the task (e.g., the content of the present
everyday tasks such as reading a book, listening to a speak- reading passage or lecture or the current movements and
er, or driving a vehicle, has the following attentional signals of the vehicle ahead). Individuals are aware of such
requirements. First, at the most demanding level, atten- inattention some of the time during State 1 (Hester et al.,
tional resources must be allocated to occurrent, dynamic, 2005; Schooler, 2002; Schooler et al., 2005; Smallwood
and focal features of a task that change from moment to et al., 2007) and likely experience dual consciousness or di-
moment. In the language of intentionality theories, occur- vided attention (Smallwood et al., 2007) in which they are
rent attentional concerns correspond to intentions-in-ac- aware of the transient intrusions of trains of interfering
tion (Searle, 1983), immediate intentions (Brand, 1984), thought, which they may attempt to suppress. The phe-
present-direct intentions (Bratman, 1987), and proximal nomenology of this state is nicely captured in the meta-
(Mele, 1992) or P-intentions (Pacherie, 2008). Dynamic phor of ‘‘tuning out” (Schooler, 2002; Schooler et al.,
features are cues that guide moment-to-moment task per- 2005; Smallwood et al., 2007). State 1 inattention is brief
formance including feedback regarding the adequacy of and unstable, and likely to be affected by incidental events,
ongoing performance. These are often, though not exclu- errors, and near-misses. Near-misses will be common at
sively, semantic features such as specific word/phrase/sen- this early state compared to later states and attentional re-
tence meanings during a reading/listening task, or critical sources will often be sufficient to detect incipient slips be-
meaningful changes in a driving environment such as traf- fore they become overt errors. Nonetheless, because of its
fic signals, object visibility, surface features, turns, and the ubiquity in human performance, transient focal task inat-
behavior of other vehicles. tention may well be responsible for the majority of errors
Second, additional attentional resources must be dedi- despite its comparatively brief and mild character.
cated to monitoring the stable background task environ- Conflicted division of attention can occur at any time,
ment and goals (e.g., reading an instruction manual, even very early in a novel task, although it is more com-
listening to a lecture, and driving to work). As Pacherie mon after extended periods on routine tasks and in famil-
(2008) notes, P-intentions, or what we have called occur- iar settings. Yet, although attention lapses tend to increase
rent attention, must integrate overarching prior intentions as a function of task features such as the elapsed time, rep-
(i.e., distal or D-intentions) concerning task goals and per- etition, or familiarity of the ongoing task (Mackworth,
formance with the current task conditions. That is, in 1948; Manly, Lewis, Robertson, Watson, & Datta, 2002;
maintaining task set some attentional resources must be Manly et al., 1999; Robertson et al., 1997; Smallwood
given over to general task parameters and goals. et al., 2003, 2004), mind-wandering can occur even at the
Third, successful task engagement typically has implicit beginning of a novel task as the individual considers
and explicit performative aspects. Attentional resources thoughts of the upcoming task and potential quality of per-
must therefore also be allocated to the on-line recruitment, sonal performance. A novice driver will, for example, fre-
guidance, and maintenance of task responses (e.g., mainte- quently be quite explicitly preoccupied with recent
nance of saccades down a page; taking notes during a lec- errors, or aspects of the task of driving that are not relevant
ture, manipulation of the control surfaces of a vehicle). to the immediate task context (e.g., thinking, while practic-
Clearly anything that interferes with, or places chal- ing braking, about how to avoid grinding the gears next
lenges on, any of these three aspects of task-related atten- time gear shifting is required). Nonetheless, novel and
tion will increase task failures. Note that the foregoing challenging tasks, requiring frequent monitoring of chang-
100 J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113

ing task conditions will tend to keep mind-wandering lim- by an insistent honking behind us or the crunching of me-
ited to State 1. tal. Ultimately, the internal train of thought may initiate a
State 2: In State 1 the individual maintains awareness of new stream of action. In some cases, the individual may
the general task environment. That is, attention to task set even wander physically, getting up from a work station,
remains active but with loss of sensitivity to moment-to- for example, to engage a colleague in a discussion concern-
moment task variations. In State 2, attention to the generic ing the intruded thought. Thus, conditions permitting,
task-relevant aspects of the environment decreases though mind-wandering can lead to a change of the primary task.
the individual continues to show well-practiced automatic Relations among states: In addition to specifying three
responding. In everyday terms we characterize this as discrete states of attentional engagement/disengagement,
‘‘going through the motions”. For simple repetitive tasks the three-state model also postulates specific relations
this minimal engagement may suffice for adequate perfor- among states and the relations of the states to specific
mance for brief, and sometimes surprisingly extended, errors. First, the model holds that the three states of
periods. Mind-wandering in this state is less likely to be mind-wandering are often sequential in that an individual
conscious (cf. O’Connell et al., 2007; Smallwood et al., will move from state to state. Second, because the states,
2007) and has been succinctly characterized as ‘‘zoning though discrete, are functionally and sequentially related,
out” (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). The individual may the three-state model holds that individuals with a pro-
be alerted by blatant errors but will be less sensitive to pensity of being in one state of disengagement should also
near-misses. State 2 can normally be maintained for more show a bias to enter other states of disengagement. None-
than a few seconds only in a relatively quiet, uneventful theless, the hypothesis of qualitative differences among
environment and/or during very tedious and undemanding the different states the model leads to the prediction that
tasks. Such tasks and environments are, however, not each level of attentional disengagement will potentially
uncommon. Reading, for example, is generally performed contribute independently to cognitive and action failures.
in the absence of spontaneously changing conditions. We
therefore sometimes find ourselves well beyond that last 3. Testing the three-state model
point of recall of what we have been reading, but clearly
have continued to scan down the page some way before We next describe the Sustained Attention to Response
recovering task awareness. Another such experience is Task (SART) that constitutes a controlled context to assess
waiting in a car at traffic lights for a green light. When traf- the model and provide distinct indicators for each of the
fic beside us begins to move and we may move with the three states. Although we claim considerable generality
traffic in response to peripheral background features of for the state model of task disengagement, the specific
the driving environment rather than appropriate cues behavioral indicators of each of the three states of mind-
(i.e., traffic signals) for correct task performance. Thus, if wandering will vary according to the nature of the task
the light change signaled a left turn for vehicles in the lane and hence critical features of each task must be analyzed
beside us, we might find that we have anticipated the rel- to determine such indicators for that task. Thus, we next
evant light change to find ourselves in the middle of the propose indicators relevant to the SART for each of the
intersection moving against a red light – at which point three states and test a set of novel predictions that emerge
we are likely to quickly recover full lucidity. Note, how- from the three-state model.
ever, that attentional resources at such points are required
to process the error itself. Such error-induced task-relevant 3.1. The SART
processing can, ironically, further compromise task perfor-
mance, and hence lead to further errors (see bidirectional- The Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART: Rob-
ity thesis below). This is one way in which errors beget ertson et al., 1997) is a measure of sustained attention
errors. and mind-wandering. Failures of sustained attention as
State 3: Discussions of mind-wandering have tradition- measured by the SART have been related to an increasing
ally stressed the ‘‘decoupling” of conscious processing from number of real-world problems and psychological vari-
on-line environmental sensory information as attention is ables, including self-reported everyday attention failures
directed inward to thoughts and feelings (Schooler et al., (Cheyne et al., 2006), general cognitive failures (Manly
2005; Singer, 1966; Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). This et al., 1999; Robertson et al., 1997), as well as specifically
has led to an important focus on changes in the individual’s attention-related cognitive errors (Cheyne et al., 2006),
access to representations of environmental stimuli during conditions such as ADHD (Bellgrove, Hawi, Gill, & Robert-
mind-wandering (Smallwood & Schooler, 2006). Attention son, 2006; Bellgrove, Hawi, Kirley, Gill, & Robertson,
can, however, also be drawn away from motor behavior as 2005; Johnson, Kelly, et al., 2007; Johnson, Robertson,
evidenced in action slips (Reason, 1979; Reason & Myciels- et al., 2007; Manly et al., 2001; Mullins, Bellgrove, Gill, &
ka, 1982). Hence, in State 3 there are also gross behavioral Robertson, 2005), TBI (Dockree et al., 2004; Manly et al.,
indicators of mind-wandering. The individual may be 2003; O’Keeffe, Dockree, & Robertson, 2004; Robertson
unresponsive to all but the most intrusive aspects of the et al., 1997 but see Whyte, Grieb-Neff, Gantz, & Polansky,
task environment. In real-world situations, such as 2007), boredom (Cheyne et al., 2006), and depression
reading, we cease even moving down the page and stare (Carriere, Cheyne, & Smilek, 2008; Smallwood et al.,
– blank, unmoving, and unseeing – at the page; or we fail 2007), as well as the neurophysiology of attention (Bell-
to drive on at all, even after the light has turned green grove, Hester, & Garavan, 2004; Dockree, Kelly, Foxe, Reilly,
and the traffic around us has moved on, until we are roused & Robertson, 2007; Dockree, Kelly, Robertson, Reilly, &
J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113 101

Foxe, 2005; Dockree et al., 2004; Fassbender et al., 2006;


1000
Garavan, Hester, Murphy, Fassbender, & Kelley, 2006;
800
Hester, Fassbender, & Garavan, 2004; Hester et al., 2005;

RTs (ms)
Kaufman, Ross, Stein, & Garavan, 2003; O’Connell et al., 600

2007). 400

200
3.2. SART Indices of attentional disengagement
0
0 50 100 150 200 250
The SART is a GO/NOGO continuous performance task in
which the NOGO stimulus appears infrequently. SART 1000

NOGO errors are typically interpreted as analogs to real- 800

RTs (ms)
world action slips caused by failures of sustained attention 600
rather than failures of inhibition (Manly et al., 1999; 400
Robertson et al., 1997). SART performance also provides
200
several behavioral indices of off-task mind-wandering that
0
could potentially lead to NOGO errors. 0 50 100 150 200 250
The first and most critical of these indices is the speed-
ing of response times (RTs), especially on GO trials immedi- 1000

ately preceding NOGO errors. The suggestion that fast RTs 800
index off-task mind-wandering is supported by (1) obser-

RTs (ms)
600
vations of negative correlations between overall RTs and
total NOGO errors across subjects (Cheyne et al., 2006; 400

Manly et al., 1999; Robertson et al., 1997), (2) evidence 200


of shorter mean sRTs in the trials immediately prior to
0
NOGO trials leading to SART errors (Manly et al., 1999; 0 50 100 150 200 250
Robertson et al., 1997) and shorter RTs prior to error NOGO Trials
trials than prior to successful NOGO trials (Cheyne et al.,
Fig. 1. Three sample cases of reaction times for individual subjects over
2006), (3) reports of an association between task-unrelated
225 SART trials including anticipations (RTs below 100 ms).
thought and speeded RTs during SART performance
(Smallwood et al., 2004), (4) the finding that as target
probability decreases GO RTs decrease and errors increase ual to have processed the immediately relevant stimulus
(Manly et al., 1999) presumably because infrequent NOGO and hence are consistent with the notion of responding
trials provide opportunities for increased mind-wandering, without the moment-to-moment task-relevant stimulus
and (5) reports of shorter RTs and more errors with ab- monitoring (‘‘going through the motions”). This interpreta-
sence of awareness during probe-caught mind-wandering tion is also consistent with the view that anticipations are
(Smallwood et al., 2007). fundamental form of action slips and spoonerisms (cf. pro-
Second, we draw attention to another potential index of lepsis; Douse, 1900; see also Norman, 1981) indicating
mind-wandering in the SART, namely, responses on GO tri- mind-wandering or absent-mindedness (Reason, 1979,
als that are too fast to be responses to the GO stimuli, but 1984; Reason & Mycielska, 1982). Finally, the distinct gap
rather appear to be anticipations of the presentation of GO in the data points evident in Fig. 1 is clearly consistent with
stimuli. In reexamining our own SART data, we noted a abrupt qualitative state changes.
striking pattern of RTs across trials (see Fig. 1). It is evident A third potential index of mind-wandering in the SART
in Fig. 1, which provides RT data across 225 trials for each task is the failure to respond to GO stimuli (i.e., omissions).
of three subjects, that the records reveal many impossibly Errors of omission are among the most common form of
fast RTs (Mean = 55.85; SD = 19.52). Anticipations are qual- human error (Norman, 1981; Rasmussen, 1980; Reason,
itatively different from normal RTs evidenced by a clear 1990, 1998). Errors of omission have been observed in
band of latencies essentially free of data points below the the SART with both fixed and random inter-trial intervals
majority of RTs and above the extremely short RTs. Extre- and have been interpreted as a break from task engage-
mely fast RTs (less than 100 ms) have been noted on the ment and thus reflective of lapsing attention (Johnson,
SART before (e.g., Johnson, Robertson, et al., 2007) and Robertson, et al., 2007; Manly et al., 1999). This interpreta-
these anomalous values have been replaced with RTs from tion of omissions is supported by the finding that omis-
adjacent trials. It occurred to us, however, that these sions tend to increase along with NOGO errors in the
events might provide distinctive information about task second half of the SART task when attentional engagement
disengagement. Anticipations may reflect speed–accuracy is thought to be low (Johnson, Robertson, et al., 2007). In
trade-offs, possibly encouraged by the fixed inter-trial addition, relative to controls, more omissions and NOGO
interval generally used in the SART. Some recent versions errors have been observed for individuals with ADHD
of the SART have employed variable ITIs to discourage (Johnson, Robertson, et al., 2007; O’Connell, Bellgrove,
speed–accuracy trade-offs. This apparent refinement may, Dockree, & Robertson, 2004) and patients with traumatic
however, conceal an important phenomenon. Anticipa- brain injury (Manly et al., 2003). Again, it is evident from
tions would seem to fit well our description of State 2 Fig. 1 that there is a substantial gap in the upper portion
mind-wandering. Anticipations are too fast for the individ- of each record, even for the slowest responder, between
102 J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113

the longest RTs and the end of each trial (at 1150 ms). 1997; Sarason, Sarason, Keefe, Hates, & Shearin, 1986;
Thus, transition from normal RTs to omissions appears to Smallwood, Davies, et al., 2004; Smallwood, O’Connor,
be as abrupt as that for anticipations, consistent with a et al., 2004). Recent evidence suggests, however, that
qualitative change in state. reflection on task performance accounts for substantial
It is important to acknowledge that whether a given off-task processing, sometimes referred to as Task-Related
response latency is an anticipation or an omission is Interference (Smallwood, Davies, et al., 2004; Smallwood
determined by the temporal parameters of the task. The et al., 2003). Smallwood and colleagues specifically report
argument for different states, however, hinges on whether high levels of Task-Related Interference following errors
there is a discontinuity between acceptable variations in (Smallwood, Davies, et al., 2004). Thus, although it may
response times for the task. In the model, an important be the case that errors redirect attention back to the task,
executive function is to keep responding within task the redirection may often be in the form of off-line internal
parameters. Hence, in State 1, variation is kept within analysis, including evaluation of performance, itself a form
acceptable ranges. Moreover, when performance approaches of reactive mind-wandering, that may interfere with
the limits of acceptable response speed or precision the ongoing task performance. In the extreme, the bidirectional
data suggest that they are either brought back from that relation between mind-wandering and attention-related
limit or they ‘‘pop out” of that range into extremely fast errors in performance can snowball with disastrous conse-
(automaticity) or very slow (in the limit – non-response). quences in which off-line analysis leads to complete
We therefore suggest that the three SART indices of neglect of the task at hand (e.g. flying an aircraft or moni-
mind-wandering described above (i.e., RT changes, antici- toring a nuclear facility, cf. Reason, 1979; Vincente, 2004).
pations, and omissions) are potentially valid indicators of
the three states of attentional disengagement as described 3.4. Predictions
by the model.
Applying the model in the context of the SART, speeding Following are general sets of predictions that emerge
RTs indicate State 1. This pattern of alternating speeding from a three-state bidirectional model of attentional disen-
and slowing would generate increased RT variability gagement when applied to SART performance.
(Johnson, Kelly, et al., 2007). We therefore suggest that Relations among SART indicators and general measures of
an appropriate index of State 1 is the coefficient of variabil- mind-wandering: In general, the model predicts that RT CV,
ity (CV = SD/Mean), a measure of variability independent of anticipations, and omissions will all be correlated with
mean differences. We further propose that State 2 of atten- NOGO errors and, because they represent distinct states
tional disengagement in the SART is manifest in anticipa- of mind-wandering, each of these measures will contribute
tions of the SART stimuli. Anticipations occur when the independent variance to the prediction of NOGO errors.
focal semantic features of the stimulus (i.e., the value of Thus, the first set of specific predictions concerns associa-
the stimulus) are not fully processed and behavior (i.e., tions at the level of the individual differences between
the key press) occurs automatically according to the the proposed SART indicators of the states of disengage-
rhythm of stimulus presentation. Finally, we suggest that ment (i.e., RT CV, anticipations, and omissions) and self-re-
omissions index State 3; namely, a cessation of responding ported indices of everyday mind-wandering and
within the temporal bounds of adequate task performance. attentional failure. The model predicts positive correla-
tions among RT CV, anticipations, and omissions on GO tri-
3.3. Task disengagement and bidirectionality als as well as between each of these and SART errors, self-
reported mind-wandering measured by the Mindful
By task disengagement we mean the shifting of Awareness and Attention Scale – Lapses Only (MAAS-LO),
resources from on-line processing of task cues to off-line and self-reported attentional failures measured by the
processing of materials not relevant to immediate task Attention-Related Cognitive Errors (ARCES) scale. These
demands. Importantly, processing during task disengage- predictions fundamentally assume stable individual differ-
ment need not be entirely task irrelevant (Smallwood ences in propensities for mind-wandering and the external
et al., 2004); merely irrelevant to occurrent task demands. validity of the SART measures.
Mind-wandering is often more than idle wool-gathering We note that some of these predictions are counter-
and may involve serious off-line problem solving (Kane intuitive from an inhibition perspective, given that antici-
et al., 2007; Singer, 1966, 1978). Indeed, because of the pation would seem to index strong disinhibition whereas
assumption of sufficient background competence and omissions would appear to reflect complete inhibition.
motivation, our model is compatible with the likelihood According to the model, however, anticipations and omis-
that much off-line processing is relevant to task perfor- sions represent successive states in attentional disengage-
mance and is, indeed, often precipitated by variations in task ment related to task performance/nonperformance and
performance. Thus, even under optimal task conditions of hence should be positively mutually associated and both
competence and motivation, both successful and especially should be positively associated with commission errors
unsuccessful performance events, will induce off-line on NOGO trials. In our earlier analyses (Cheyne et al.,
analysis of performance. This leads to the prediction that 2006), we found, consistent with previous and subsequent
the performance – mind-wandering association is research, that overall RTs were significantly and robustly
bidirectional. negatively associated with NOGO errors (r = .64,
It has been commonly noted that errors redirect atten- p < .001). Previously, however, RTs included the anticipa-
tion back to the primary task at hand (Robertson et al., tions discussed above and, as argued above. For the present
J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113 103

study, we therefore calculated RT and RT CV only for trials a mean age of 32.23 (SD = 11.23). Blank or incomplete sub-
with RTs above 200 ms and counted as anticipations all RTs missions were removed from analysis and any instances of
less than 100 ms. The very few ambiguous RTs (i.e., be- multiple submissions from the same participant were
tween 100 and 200 ms) were ignored. removed. Similarly, results from participants who failed to
In our earlier research, we reported that a self-report complete all 225 trials of the SART, or responded in opposite
measure of mind-wandering propensity, the Mindful manner to that in which they were instructed (i.e., key press
Awareness of Attention Scale (MAAS) was significantly provided only when the target appeared), were removed
associated with SART GO RTs and accounted for the associ- from the data set. Of an original 717 participants, total of
ation between the self-report scale of Attention-Related 213 participant responses were removed in this manner.
Cognitive Errors (ARCES) and RTs (Cheyne et al., 2006).
On the other hand, the ARCES contributed unique variance 4.2. Measures
to the prediction of SART NOGO Errors. This pattern of re-
sults was consistent with the hypothesis that the MAAS RT CVs were calculated for all response latencies over
and SART RT speeding directly reflect mind-wandering, 200 ms. RTs less than 100 ms were coded as anticipations.
whereas ARCES and SART NOGO errors are cognitive per- If no response was made during a GO trial this was re-
formance consequences of mind-wandering. We therefore corded as an omission. Responses to the NOGO stimulus
predicted that a slightly revised scale, focusing on lapses (‘‘3”) were coded as NOGO errors and successful withhold-
only, the MAAS-LO (Carriere et al., 2008) and the ARCES ing of responses as NOGO successes. The self-report mea-
would be significantly correlated with RT CV, as well as sures included the 12-item ARCES and the 13-item
with anticipations and omissions but that the MAAS-LO revised Mindful Awareness of Attention with Lapses Only
would account for the association of the ARCES with each (MAAS-LO; see Carriere et al., 2008).
of these measures. The SART procedure, as described by Robertson et al.
Temporal relations and transitions between states: A crit- (1997), involves 225 single digits, 25 each from 1 to 9, pre-
ical aspect of the three-state model of attentional disen- sented for 250 ms and followed by an encircled  mask ()
gagement is the hypothesis that the three states are for 900 ms, for a total digit-to-digit duration of 1150 ms.
sequentially related. Hence, speeding of RT should occur Participants were instructed to respond via a key press to
on trials immediately prior to anticipations and omission each digit, unless that digit is a 3. The digits are randomly
as well as following anticipations. Second, the probability distributed, as are the font sizes of all 225 trials, with equal
of anticipations and omissions should increase on trials representation of 48, 72, 94, 100, and 120 point size Sym-
just before and after omissions and anticipations. bol font in white against a black background. The SART was
Bidirectionality of mind-wandering and NOGO errors: The recreated in Macromedia Flash MX 2004 according to these
model states that mind-wandering and the resulting inat- specifications, in order to allow on-line presentation of the
tention to the task leads to errors and to reflection on those SART with the rest of the questionnaires. Actual stimulus
errors, consequently directing attentional resources away displays were dependent on the computer equipment used
from current task conditions. Thus, the bidirectional aspect by each participant and viewing distance was not con-
of the model predicts that all performance tests (NOGO tri- trolled. On a Compaq Presario R3140CA series notebook,
als in the SART), successful as well as unsuccessful, will with 15.4 in WXGA (1280  800) display, used for scale
lead to reflection on task performance and hence provide development, stimulus height varied from 8.74 mm
occasions for mind-wandering. To assess these predictions, (34 px) to 21.33 mm (83 px) and the circular mask diame-
we examined RT changes, anticipations, and omissions fol- ter was 21.07 mm (82 px).
lowing as well as preceding both correct and incorrect
NOGO trials. We predict that, in the context of the SART, 4.3. Procedures
all indicators of mind-wandering (RT variability, anticipa-
tions, and omissions) will serve as both precursors and Participants received an informational e-mail regarding
consequences of both successful and unsuccessful task the study, including a link to the study website and a pre-
performance. assigned personal identification number. Upon visiting the
The bidirectional model leads to one further prediction. study website, participants received further instructions
If performance tests (i.e., NOGO trials) lead to increased regarding the purposes of the study and were given the
mind-wandering then closely spaced NOGO trials should opportunity to participate by completing, in random and
also lead to increased probability of errors, especially fol- undisclosed order, the ARCES, MFS, and MAAS. Participants
lowing a recent NOGO error. completed the three measures in a single session, followed
by the SART, at the end of which they were given a feed-
4. Method back page thanking them for their participation and pro-
viding further information about the purposes of the
4.1. Participants study and links to a website with additional information
on sleep paralysis.
Subjects were selected from among prior respondents
to a WWW survey on sleep paralysis (see Cheyne et al., 4.4. Lag analyses
2006). The final selection of participants who completed
the full study included 349 females, 155 males (N = 504). Lag RTs were calculated as follows. For each NOGO trial,
Participants ranged from adolescence to senescence, with separately for correct and incorrect responses, the RTs for
104 J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113

the four preceding trials and the four trials following the As can be seen in Table 1, the three SART indicators (RT
NOGO trial were tabulated. RTs were then averaged within CV, anticipations, and omissions) were significantly and
each lag. The same procedure was repeated for four trials strongly correlated with one another. Also as predicted,
preceding and four trials following anticipations and omis- RT CV, anticipations, and omissions were each robustly cor-
sions. Lag probabilities for anticipations and omissions related with NOGO Errors. These correlations corroborate
were calculated as follows. For each NOGO trial the four the interpretation of, and the model predictions for, RT
preceding and the four following trials were analyzed sep- CV, anticipations and omissions as indices of mind-wan-
arately for correct and incorrect responses. Probabilities for dering in the SART task. Also consistent with predictions,
a given trial type were generated by taking the number of RT CV, anticipations, and omissions were significantly cor-
occurrences of that type at a given lag, divided by the total related with self-reported mind-wandering as measured
number of trials counted at that lag (in most cases this was by the MAAS-LO and with self-reported action slips as-
equal to the number of errors considered, except when an sessed by the ARCES. Moreover, extending our previous
error occurred within four trials of the beginning or end of findings (Cheyne et al., 2006) that the association of the
the experiment). These analyses were performed using a MAAS-LO with the RTs completely accounted for associa-
command line tool written in C. tion of the ARCES and RT, a series of regression analyses,
To ensure that our measures of RTs, anticipations and replicated this result for RT CV, anticipations, and omis-
omissions before and after NOGO errors did not overlap, sions. The ARCES contributed no additional significant pre-
it was necessary to exclude some trials and participants dictive power for any of these variables beyond that of the
from the analyses. Hence, sample sizes vary slightly from MAAS-LO, though, also consistent with previous analyses,
measure to measure and between comparisons within it did for NOGO errors, b = .13, t = 2.02, p < .04. This finding
measures and are reflected in the changing degrees of free- is consistent with the conclusion that anticipations and
dom of the F-tests. For RT, for example, we recorded as omissions, like SART RTs, are best interpreted as direct
missing data trials preceding or following NOGO errors indices of mind-wandering.
that included omissions, anticipations, or other NOGO A further prediction arising from the three-state model
trials. Missing data also arose in cases in which RT was is that different states of mind-wandering make indepen-
not available because the end or beginning of the session dent contributions to cognitive errors and action slips.
occurred within four trials of the target NOGO trial. For Consistent with this prediction, a multiple regression anal-
anticipations and omissions, conditional probabilities for ysis with NOGO errors as the dependent variable, yielded
mutual transitions could be calculated only for individuals significant coefficients for RT CV, anticipations, and omis-
committing anticipations or omissions. sions (see Table 2). Anticipations made slightly stronger
Given the statistical power of the large sample size and the contributions than the other two variables and omissions
multiple repeated measures the lower-bound degrees-of- made slightly weaker contributions to the prediction of
freedom were used for all repeated-measures analyses, lead- SART errors. The latter finding may reflect a peculiarity of
ing to further variations in degrees of freedom across tests. omissions in the SART task. Because the critical feature of
successful task performance requires the withholding of a
5. Results and discussion response, omissions will occasionally lead to spuriously
successful NOGO trials, attenuating the general positive
5.1. Associations among indicators and consequences of association of omissions with NOGO errors. It is predicted
mind-wandering that omission will provide even better prediction for a var-
iant of the SART that requires an alternative positive re-
We began by testing our prediction that the three target sponse (rather than none).
variables (RT CVs, anticipations, and omissions on GO tri- Temporal associations among the SART indicators of mind-
als) as measures of states of mind-wandering generalize wandering RTs and anticipation: RTs for the four trials (lags)
beyond the SART. Hence, by hypothesis, each of the SART immediately preceding and following anticipations were
indices will be correlated with one another, with NOGO Er- analyzed (Fig. 2). The data were analyzed in a 2 (pre-antic-
rors, and with self-reported mind-wandering measured by ipations vs. post-anticipations) by 4 (lag) repeated-mea-
the MAAS-LO and self-reported attentional failures mea- sures ANOVA for individuals making anticipations. This
sured by the ARCES. The correlations among each of these analysis yielded significant temporal order and lag effects,
measures are shown in Table 1. which were superseded by a significant temporal order by

Table 1
Means, SDs, and Pearson product-moment correlations among SART measures (GO RTs, anticipations, omissions, and NOGO errors) and self-report measures of
mind-wandering (MAAS-LO) and Attention-Related Cognitive Errors (ARCES). Means are in italics on diagonals. SDs are in parentheses, for all correlations,
p < .01, N = 504.

RT CV Anticipations Omissions NOGO errors MAAS-LO ARCES


RT CV 0.28 0.70 0.62 0.50 0.21 0.16
Anticipations (0.12) 3.13 0.53 0.52 0.17 0.12
Omissions (6.76) 3.40 0.42 0.19 0.13
NOGO errors (5.79) 10.89 0.31 0.28
MAAS-LO (5.83) 54.76 0.73
ARCES (12.05) 49.40
J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113 105

Table 2 0.16 Anticipation->Omission p(O|A)


Multiple regression predicting NOGO errors with RT CV, anticipations, and Omission->Anticipation p(A|O)
omissions. 0.14
Anticipation Baseline p(A)
Dependent variable: NOGO errors b t p 0.12
Omission Baseline p(O)

p(A|O) or p(O|A)
RT CV 0.21 3.71 .001 0.10
Anticipations 0.30 5.72 .001
Omissions 0.13 2.77 .006 0.08
R2 = .32, F(3, 500) = 77.74, p < .001
0.06

0.04

400 0.02

0.00
350
t-4 t-3 t-2 t-1 t0 t+1 t+2 t+3 t+4

300 Pre A or O A or O Post A or O

250 Fig. 3. Mean conditional probabilities of anticipations/omissions (with


SEs) for four trials preceding and four trials following omissions/
RT (ms)

PrePost Anticipation anticipations.


200
Baseline
150
tions and omissions showed the expected near mirror-
100
image pattern, with virtually identical results, including
linear, F(1, 208) = 62.90, p < .001, g2 = .23, quadratic,
50 F(1, 172) = 27.56, p < .001, g2 = .12, and cubic, F(1, 208) =
9.49, p < .001, g2 = .04, effects. Trials immediately preced-
0 ing (t  1) and following (t + 1) anticipations and omis-
t-4 t-3 t-2 t-1 t0 t+1 t+2 t+3 t+4
sions were, significantly above the overall baseline
Pre-Anticipation Ant. Post-Anticipation probabilities.
RTs and omissions: To assess the temporal association of
Fig. 2. Mean SART RTs (with SEs) for four trials preceding and four trials
following anticipations. RTs and omissions, the RTs for the four trials immediately
preceding and following omissions were analyzed in a 2
(pre vs. post) by 4 (lag) repeated-measures ANOVA for
lag interaction, F(1, 162) = 23.42, p < .001, g2 = .31. This individuals making anticipations (Fig. 4). This analysis
interaction was disambiguated by analyses of the lag ef- yielded significant order and lag effects, which were super-
fects for pre- and post-anticipation RTs. RTs preceding seded by a significant order by lag interaction,
anticipations showed a robust significant linear decrease, F(1, 205) = 22.31, p < .001, g2 = .25. This interaction was
F(1, 162) = 110.17, p < .001, g2 = .39, and a smaller qua- disambiguated by analyses of the sequential lag effects
dratic component, F(1, 162) = 47.88, p < .001, g2 = .22. RTs for pre- and post-omission RTs. Preceding RTs showed a
following anticipations appeared to show a near mirror- robust significant increase leading up to t0, well above
image linear decline, although RT speed was significantly the mean baseline, yielding significant linear F(1, 220) =
faster immediately prior to, than following, anticipations. 62.36, p < .001, g2 = .23, and quadratic, F(1, 210) = 31.69,
t(197) = 9.44, p < .001 (see Fig. 2). p < .001, effects. Conversely, there was a significant de-
Anticipations and omissions: To assess the sequential crease following t0, yielding significant linear, F(1, 208) =
associations of anticipations and omissions, the condi- 62.90, p < .001, g2 = .23, and quadratic, F(1, 208) = 27.56,
tional probabilities of anticipations for the four trials p < .001, g2 = .12, effects. The deviation from linearity in
immediately preceding and following omissions and the both cases is clearly evident at t  1 and t + 1. There was
conditional probabilities of omissions preceding and fol- substantial slowing of RTs on the trial immediately before
lowing anticipations were analyzed in a 2 (anticipation an omission. Hence, extremely slow RTs are harbingers of
vs. omission) by 2 (pre vs. post) by 4 (lag) repeated-mea- omissions. Had our inter-stimulus interval been shorter,
sures ANOVA for individuals making anticipations (see the pre-omission RTS responses would have themselves
Fig. 3). This analysis yielded significant temporal order been omissions. Ultimately, omissions are simply very long
and lag effects, which were superseded by a significant response times that exceed the available time for the task.
temporal order by lag interaction, F(1, 207) = 49.68, Nonetheless, it clear that there is an abrupt transition from
p < .001, g2 = .19 (see Fig. 3). The interaction was disambig- moderately slower RTs in the normal range to very long
uated by analyses of the lag effects for pre- and post-antic- RTs. This result and the post-omission speeding of RTs
ipations and omissions combined. Preceding conditional are consistent with abrupt transitions between states.
probabilities showed a robust significant linear increase
with a marked inflection between t  2 and t  1, well 5.2. Bidirectionality of mind-wandering and attentional
above the mean baseline, yielding significant linear errors
F(1, 210) = 62.36, p < .001, g2 = .23, quadratic, F(1, 210) =
32.69, p < .001, g2 = .13, and cubic, F(1, 210) = 10.48, The final set of predictions that we tested concerned the
p < .001, g2 = .05, effects. Probabilities following anticipa- bidirectionality of mind-wandering and task challenges
106 J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113

550 420 Correct


PrePost Omission RTs
Baseline Error
500 400 Baseline

450 380

RT (ms)
360
RT (ms)

400

350 340

320
300

300
250
t-4 t-3 t-2 t-1 t0 t+1 t+2 t+3 t+4

Pre-NOGO NOGO Post-NOGO


200
t-4 t-3 t-2 t-1 t0 t+1 t+2 t+3 t+4
Fig. 5. Mean SART RTs (with SEs) for four trials preceding and four trials
Pre-Omission O Post-Omission following NOGO incorrect and correct trials.

Fig. 4. Mean SART RTs (with SEs) for four trials preceding and four trials
following omissions.
likelihood of an anticipation following an error was greater
than for the other three conditions, p < .001. The finding
such as attentional errors. Specifically, we predicted that that anticipations were not significantly more likely pre-
each of the SART indicators of mind-wandering should ceding errors than correct NOGO trials was somewhat
both precede and follow NOGO trials, particularly those counter-intuitive, especially given that anticipations dur-
involving errors. To evaluate these bidirectional relations, ing a NOGO trial create an automatic error. Nonetheless,
we assessed RTs, anticipations, and omissions on four trials the increased probability of anticipations following errors
(lags) before and four trials (lags) after correct and incor- is consistent with the bidirectional model.
rect NOGO trials. Omissions preceding and following NOGO trials: 2 (correct
RTs preceding and following NOGO trials: The data were vs. incorrect) by 2 (temporal order: pre-NOGO trial vs.
analyzed in a 2 (correct vs. incorrect) by 2 (pre- vs. post- post-NOGO trial) by 4 (lag) repeated-measures ANOVA
NOGO trial) by 4 (lag) repeated-measures ANOVA. All ef- for omissions yielded a significant three-way interaction,
fects were highly significant, including the three-way F(1, 494) = 21.51, p < .001, g2 = .04 (see Fig. 7). A series of
interaction, F(1, 481) = 36.18, p < .001, g2 = .07 (see Fig. 5). lag analyses for pre-error trials, but not pre-success trials
This interaction was disambiguated by a series of analyses yielded a marginal cubic effect, F(1, 481) = 7.70, p < .006,
of the lag effects for correct and incorrect responses, pre- g2 = .02. There was a significant increase in omissions in
and post-NOGO trials. RTs preceding successful NOGO tri- the trial immediately preceding an error. Omissions fol-
als, were all significantly above baseline and characterized lowing a success decreased and then returned to near
by increasing RTs, with significant linear, F(1, 494) = baseline, producing a significant quadratic effect,
111.70, p < .001, g2 = .18, and quadratic components, F(1, 481) = 8.33, p < .001, g2 = .02. Omissions following an
F(1, 481) = 16.12, p < .001, g2 = .03. Conversely, RTs preced- error increased then declined, yielding significant linear,
ing error NOGO trials were all below baseline RTs and de- F(1, 481) = 56.07, p < .001, g2 = .10, and quadratic compo-
creased prior to a NOGO error, with significant linear, nents, F(1, 481) = 19.44, p < .001, g2 = .04.
F(1, 492) = 6.68, p < .01, g2 = .01, and cubic components,
F(1, 492) = 12.12, p < .001, g2 = .02. RTs following successful
NOGO trials, dropped markedly below baseline and then
0.04 Anticipations
rapidly recovered, yielding significant linear, F(1, 498) = Error p(A|E)
126.42, p < .001, g2 = .20, quadratic, F(1, 481) = 75.74, Correct p(A|C)
Baseline p(A)
p < .001, g2 = .13, and cubic components, F(1, 481) = 13.54, 0.03
p < .001, g2 = .03. In contrast, RTs following error NOGO tri-
p(A|E) or p(A|C)

als increased to baseline levels and then resumed signifi-


0.02
cantly below baseline and not different from preceding
RTs yielding significant quadratic, F(1, 497) = 23.08,
p < .001, g2 = .04, and cubic components, F(1, 497) = 7.55, 0.01
p < .001, g2 = .02. The mean RT for NOGO errors is also
shown in Fig. 5. The mean NOGO error RT is significantly
0.00
faster than mean RTs for any lag.
t-4 t-3 t-2 t-1 t0 t+1 t+2 t+3 t+4
Anticipations preceding and following NOGO trials: A 2
(correct vs. incorrect) by 2 (pre- vs. post-NOGO trial) by Pre-NOGO NOGO Post-NOGO

4 (lag) repeated-measures ANOVA of anticipations yielded


Fig. 6. Mean conditional probabilities of anticipations (with SEs) for four
a significant two-way accuracy by temporal order interac- trials preceding and four trials following NOGO incorrect and correct
tion, F(1, 494) = 14.84, p < .001, g2 = .03 (see Fig. 6). The trials.
J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113 107

0.06 compared to those of an unconstrained (saturated) model


Error p(O|E) Omissions
in which all parameters were free to vary. This comparison
0.05 Correct p(O|C)
resulted in a significantly poorer fitting model,
Baseline p(O)
v2(1) = 11.81, p < .001. Hence, the linear component of
p(O|C) or p(O|E)

0.04
the association of interval length and P(E|C) and P(E|E) dif-
0.03
fer significantly. The linear component was significantly
stronger for P(E|E) that P(E|C). In contrast, a parallel analy-
0.02 sis constraining the quadratic components to equality
yielded a well fitting model, v2(1) = 0.02, p = .90. Hence,
0.01 the quadratic component of the associations of interval
length and P(E|C) and P(E|E) did not differ significantly.
0.00
Summary: The present results are generally consistent
t-4 t-3 t-2 t-1 t0 t+1 t+2 t+3 t+4
with the hypothesis that the three indices of mind-wan-
Pre-NOGO NOGO Post-NOGO
dering are associated with errors on NOGO trials, and
Fig. 7. Mean conditional probabilities of omissions (with SEs) for four two of these indices (RT speeding and omissions) preceded
trials preceding and four trials following NOGO incorrect and correct errors at a rate significantly above baseline. Errors, in turn,
trials. lead to faster RTs and increased probabilities of both antic-
ipations and omissions. Successful trials are less likely than
errors to lead to mind-wandering as indicated by a brief,
The results for omissions following a NOGO error are one-trial speeding of RTs, a brief, one-trial increase in
perhaps the most striking of all, particularly at t + 1. Not anticipations, but no evidence of an increase in omissions.
only are RTs lengthened, and anticipations increased, but Errors lead, in contrast, to a sustained increase in anticipa-
omissions are markedly increased, with incomplete recov- tions and an especially dramatic increase in omissions. The
ery over the four post-error trials (see Fig. 7). results generally support the conclusion that the relation
Interval length and errors: The final prediction concerns between mind-wandering and attentional errors on NOGO
the effect of mind-wandering induced by NOGO trials. trials is bidirectional.
The previous results reveal increases in the indices of In addition, consistent with the model, many of the
mind-wandering. Therefore, error trials following soon transitions between indices appear to be rather abrupt.
after a NOGO trial should show increased error rates. This Thus, an abrupt increase in RT presaged the occurrence of
hypothesis was tested by assessing the association of an omission, but this occurred at t  1, as reflected in a sig-
length of interval between NOGO trials with probability nificant deviation from linearity in the trials preceding an
of an error at the end of the interval (i.e., the proportion omission. The transition between anticipations and omis-
of subjects making an error after varying interval lengths). sions was similarly qualitative.
The bidirectional hypothesis suggests, however, that the
effect should be especially strong for short intervals and 6. General discussion
hence this relation should show significant non-linear
components. Separate linear and quadratic regression We have described and tested a three-state bidirec-
analyses were conducted regressing probability of NOGO tional model of attentional engagement/disengagement.
errors following a correct prior NOGO trial, P(E|C), and According to this model, and consistent with the evidence
NOGO errors following an incorrect prior NOGO trial, presented, attentional disengagement during a sustained
P(E|E). Analysis of successful prior challenges reveals inde- attention task can be described as three distinct states of
pendent linear and quadratic effects, linear: b = .76, mind-wandering. Indices of these three states are posi-
t = 6.53, p < .001; quadratic: b = .33, t = 2.82, p < .010. The tively associated with one another, with NOGO errors,
overall model generated a robust effect of R2 = .72, and with self-reported measures of mind-wandering and
F(1, 22) = 26.45, p < .001. The R2 change produced by add- attention-related cognitive errors in everyday settings
ing the quadratic component was also significant, and tasks in theoretically consistent patterns. The relations
R2change ¼ :11, F(1, 21) = 7.97, p < .010. Analysis of unsuccess- between SART and self-report measures suggest that the
ful prior challenges also revealed independent linear and SART measures generalize beyond the SART task and pro-
quadratic effects, linear: b = .76, t = 7.08, p < .001; qua- vide potentially useful individual difference behavioral
dratic: b = .26, t = 2.28, p < .033. The overall model gener- measures of mind-wandering.
ated a substantial effect of R2 = .80, F(1, 22) = 26.45, We also found evidence that indices of these states pre-
p < .001. The R2 change produced by adding the quadratic cede and follow one another as well as NOGO errors at
component was also significant, R2change ¼ :07, F(1, 21) = above chance levels. Particularly striking is the fact that
5.18, p < .033. anticipations and omissions, which represent opposite ex-
The function for errors following shorter intervals ap- tremes of RT, are positively correlated with one another
pears somewhat steeper than that following correct with- across individuals, are sequentially associated in time
holds. To assess the effect the difference in the linear and, although also positively correlated with variation
component for correct and incorrect trials a SEM model within the normal range of RT (RT CV), and are associated
was constructed in which the linear components for Inter- with NOGO errors independently of such variation and
val length and both P(E|C) and P(E|E) were constrained to one another. One alternative possibility is that anticipa-
be equal. The results of the analysis of this model were tions reflect very late responses that carry over to the next
108 J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113

trial. This is consistent with the positive associations found


both across individuals and over time (trials). It is, how-
ever, not consistent with the finding of independent asso-
ciations of each with NOGO errors. Moreover, although the
carry-over interpretation is consistent with the strong
transition from omissions to anticipations (i.e., that an
anticipation is the much delayed response to the previous
trial, which would have been an omission), it is clearly
inconsistent with the significantly stronger likelihood of
transition from anticipation to omission (see Fig. 3).
The results for the lag analysis of RTs preceding NOGO
errors were consistent with prior findings and interpreta-
tions of the attentional implications of speeding of SART
GO RTs. This pattern is particularly consistent with the
interpretation of SART RT change directly reflecting atten-
Fig. 8. Quadratic regression of probability of NOGO error on length of
tional task disengagement. The present results also add the
interval following successful, P(E|C), and unsuccessful, P(E|E), NOGO
finding that longer RTs, within the normal range, prior to a trials.
NOGO trial are associated with an increasing probability of
a successful withholding, suggesting that changing RTs
within the normal range reflect both increases and de- slowing in RT signaled an upcoming omission, which, in
creases in attentional engagement in the task. In addition, the context of the present task, entailed a dramatic single
although mean NOGO error RTs were faster than RTs at any trial mean change of over 500 ms (Fig. 3). Such abrupt
lag they were still within normal RT range and appear to shifts provide corroboration for the claim of qualitatively
reflect a linear trend of increasing State 1 task disengage- different states. The pattern of findings is also consistent
ment prior to, and during, an error. Nonetheless, the most with the existence of an underlying process with a transi-
significant task events for the SART are errors on NOGO tri- tional period of quantitative changes (speeding and slow-
als. These appear to produce substantial task interference ing of task-relevant responses within task parameters)
as indexed by increased anticipations and omissions fol- and a reduction of on-line attentional processing followed
lowing errors. This interference appears to persist for sev- by an abrupt collapse of on-task processing. According to
eral trials (Figs. 7 and 8). Persisting interference is further the model, in the case of anticipations, the collapse repre-
indexed by the mutual and self-sequencing of anticipations sents a cessation of task analysis and, in the case of omis-
and omissions (Fig. 3) as well as the effects of NOGO trial sions, a failure of response preparation.
interval length. Moreover, although the immediate effect Bidirectionality: We found considerable evidence for
on RT was actually greater for successful NOGO trials, the bidirectionality between mind-wandering and attention-
effect on RT was more protracted following an error related errors. All three indicator variables occurred as
(Fig. 5). Thus, RT results are also consistent with the reactions to errors (Figs. 6–8). Anticipations and omissions
hypothesis that errors may lead to longer off-task rumina- were particularly sensitive to errors, suggesting that NOGO
tion than successful performance. trials and particularly errors on NOGO trials constitute
In the trial immediately following a NOGO error there important occasions for reactive mind-wandering. In all
was a significant slowing of RTs. This would appear to be cases, it seems possible that reactive mind-wandering
consistent with the hypothesis of increased caution and reflects attempts to reengage with the task, but which,
on-task attentional engagement (Manly et al., 1999; paradoxically, at least briefly adversely affect ongoing
Robertson et al., 1997). Alternatively, however, and consis- performance.
tent with the foregoing arguments, this one-trial effect Bidirectionality may be relevant to a recent distinction,
may reflect a brief disruptive reaction to the error, that similar that proposed in the present paper, between
is, a task-relevant distraction. Interestingly, in the trial sustained and transient aspects of control processing
immediately following a successful NOGO trial, there was (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001; Braver,
a significant and substantial speeding of RT. Indeed, this Barch, Gray, Molfese, & Snyder, 2001; Braver, Reynolds, &
was the only trial around a NOGO trial in which RTs asso- Donaldson, 2003; Burgund, Lugar, Miezin, & Peterson,
ciated with correct response were faster than those associ- 2003; Cohen, Botvinick, & Carter, 2000; Donaldson, Peter-
ated with an error. This may also reflect a brief distraction son, Ollinger, & Buckner, 2001; MacDonald, Cohen, Stenger,
from the task as subjects evaluate their performance. & Carter, 2000; Miller & Cohen, 2001; Mitchell et al., 2007;
Catastrophic mind-wandering: There were extremely Velanova et al., 2003; Visscher et al., 2003). A sustained or
abrupt transitions in response times indexing state transi- tonic mode of attention involves monitoring of task condi-
tions, anticipations and omissions, following lesser degrees tions and behavioral performance and has been associated
of acceleration and deceleration of RTs. That is, abrupt with prefrontal activity. The second, transient mode, is an
state transitions to anticipations and omissions were pre- occurrent form of attention, reactively engaged by behav-
saged in the immediately preceding trials by smaller devi- ioral and cognitive (cf. Mitchell et al., 2007) conflicts and
ations in the same direction. Thus, the precipitous drop in has been associated neurologically with anterior cingulate
RTs (350 ms) that defined anticipations were signaled by cortex activity (Carter et al., 1998; Cohen et al., 2000). Sus-
smaller but significant speeding of RT (Fig. 2). Conversely, tained and transient control processes work in tandem.
J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113 109

The first process engages in active, ongoing monitoring of exogenous task and non-task events (2) endogenous rhyth-
task conditions and engages a reactive process when out- mic attentional oscillations (see below), and (3) explicit
comes conflict with task goals (i.e., errors). Reactive pro- strategic modifications of attention based on expectations.
cessing then interferes with analysis of occurrent task Thus, it is possible that the high rate of errors found for clo-
demands (State 2) or produces failure of task performance sely spaced NOGO trials might reflect a lowered expecta-
(State 3). tion of another NOGO trial immediately following
The present findings also shed light on a recently re- preceding NOGO trials. Perhaps, subjects strategically relax
ported a principled components analysis of SART RTs in vigilance following NOGO trials and only gradually in-
blocks between critical events (NOGO trials or thought crease on-task attention as the expectation of a NOGO trial
probes) (Smallwood, McSpadden, Luus, & Schooler, 2008). grows over succeeding trials. Studies underway attempt to
The first of three components extracted appeared to be a assess this possibility by manipulating the probability of
conventional ‘‘size” factor and, as such, likely reflects con- NOGO trials at different trial intervals. Preliminary results
sistency in individual differences in mean RT across trials, suggest that the function presented in Fig. 8 is independent
possibly reflecting individual differences in Stage 1 pro- of such manipulations.
pensities. There appear to have been differences in this
component between correct and incorrect trials as well 6.1. Future studies: further predictions from the three-state
as between mind-wandering with and without awareness model
(Smallwood et al., 2008, Fig. 1). The second and third com-
ponents appeared to be more bipolar and contrasted trials In what follows we consider further potential corre-
following one critical event and, particularly the second lates, causes, and effects of the three states of attentional
component, those preceding the next critical event. That disengagement at behavioral, phenomenological, physio-
is, early RTs appeared to be mutually positively correlated logical, cognitive, and neural levels.
with one another, as are those of late trials, whereas early The influence of endogenous attentional rhythms: The lin-
and late trials are negatively correlated. Thus, individuals ear trends found in almost all analyses likely reflect
who show reactive mind-wandering appear to have a dif- increasing inattention across trials preceding NOGO errors.
ferent pattern of RTs prior to the next critical event from In a number of cases for all attentional indices, however,
those of less reactive individuals. If this interpretation is there were also frequent quadratic and cubic trends. These
correct, the bidirectional aspect of the present model pre- latter results may reflect unstable, fluctuating attentional
dicts that this component should be stronger in blocks ter- engagement throughout the task, suggesting that task
minated by an error. Consistent with this, error blocks engagement is an unstable and constantly fluctuating set
produced higher weightings on the second component of states. A further possibility that should be acknowledged
than did baseline blocks. Moreover, correct blocks appear is that the state transitions may be affected by an underly-
to generate intermediate weights on Factor 2, also consis- ing periodicity of attentional states or arousal, or underly-
tent with present model. ing neural oscillator driving manual responses, or
A paradox observed, explained, and generalized: The in- indirectly affecting attention (Gilden & Hancock, 2007;
creased probability of errors following very short intervals Johnson, Kelly, et al., 2007; Smallwood et al., 2008).
following prior NOGO trials, especially unsuccessful NOGO Smallwood and colleagues, for example, using a principal
trials, may seem at odds with prior research indicating that components analysis, report systematic fluctuations in
tasks with long intervals or slow presentation rates RTs leading up to SART errors or probe-detected mind-
increase error rates (Antrobus, 1968; Smallwood et al., wandering.
2003, 2004; Teasdale et al., 1993; but see Smallwood The increasing RTs prior to successful NOGO trials may
et al., 2008). There is an important difference however, be- suggest the possibility of an explicit strategy of allocating
tween tasks with long intervals providing many mind- attentional resources to the task as time passes and the
wandering opportunities, and episodes of repeated chal- expectation of a NOGO trial increases. Speeding following
lenges interspersed among long periods of inactivity. Thus, errors, conversely, might reflect a decreased expectation
combining the present results with previous findings, we of an imminent NOGO trial, though this would not explain
hypothesize that situations most conducive to mind- the differential speeding following correct and incorrect
wandering are those with long periods of inactivity (ran- trials. The present results do not provide direct evidence
dom or periodic mind-wandering) interspersed with bouts for anti-correlated endogenous oscillations such as have
of distracting critical events (reactive mind-wandering). been suggested for the ‘‘default network” (Sonuga-Barke
Such situations are not uncommon in reading and driving & Castellanos, 2007). Sonuga-Barke and Castellanos do
tasks, both of which may have long ‘‘fallow” periods inter- not explicitly consider the effects of task errors on the
spersed with critical events (passages critical for future power of endogenous rhythms. It is possible, however, that
understanding of text, encountering construction zones the current pattern of results might arise because task
or accident scenes). In addition, real-world catastrophes active (exteroceptive) cycles of the default network are
often entail series of closely spaced cumulative errors, suppressed and/or task negative (introspective) cycles are
which would likely compound the reactive effects sug- actually induced by the occurrence of errors, leading to
gested by our bidirectional model (Three Mile Island, the patterns observed in the present study (see also
Bhopal, Chernobyl: see Reason, 1990, Appendix). Smallwood et al., 2008). It is, however, impossible to eval-
The patterns of RTs, anticipations, and omissions could uate these alternative interpretations in the absence of
arise through a combination of at least three factors: (1) neurological data. Given the temporal parameters of the
110 J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113

task, imaging techniques such as MEG will likely be re- detection (but not necessarily conscious awareness) of er-
quired for future studies. ror, whereas late error positivity (Pe) appears to be related
Near-misses: During SART performance subjects some- to the (conscious) evaluation of response strategies in re-
time report perceived incipient movements as muscle ten- sponse to error awareness (Falkenstein, Hohnsbein,
sion in the arm or they may detect small hand or finger Hoorman, & Blanke, 1990; O’Connell et al., 2007).
movements too small to be conventionally recorded as Smallwood and colleagues (2008) also report a reduction
overt responses (i.e., key presses). These are potentially in P300 prior to errors and off-task reports. We hypothe-
informative behavioral cues that could be recorded elec- sized above that the post-error changes in RTs and in-
tromyographically (EMG) and potentially related to creases in the probability of anticipations and omissions
changes in the lateralized readiness potential, such as likely also reflect a cognitive response to error in the form
those reported for nontarget trials (Smallwood et al., of off-line processing (i.e., task-relevant mind-wandering).
2008), and possibly indexing incipient attention-related Hence, we predict that, whereas ERN will be associated
errors. According to the model, State 1 of mind-wandering with errors generally, only late error positivity (Pe) will
consists of brief and partial processing of stimulus mean- be specifically associated with RT change, anticipations,
ing of the sort likely to lead to near-misses. Thus, being and omissions that follow an error on NOGO trials.
presented with a NOGO trial while in State 1 of mind-wan- Memory effects: The available evidence suggests that
dering should lead to muscle activity in the arm as if the mind-wandering also has detrimental effects on recall of
subject were about to respond, but with sometimes suc- environmental- and task-relevant events (Smallwood
cessful inhibition of the overt response (a near miss). In et al., 2006). More specifically, mind-wandering is
State 2 of mind-wandering task monitoring is no longer associated with a decrease in recall (explicit memory) rel-
effective. Accordingly, we predict that being presented ative to familiarity (implicit memory) (Smallwood et al.,
with a NOGO trial while in State 2 will lead to a larger 2003, 2004). Recall should decline as mind-wandering
EMG response relative to State 1 and that this will more of- moves from State 1 to 3. We also predict an increasing shift
ten lead to an overt commission error. Finally, because from explicit to implicit remembering. It is an interesting
State 3 of mind-wandering is characterized by a complete possibility that both explicit and implicit recall will be
failure to engage to task response system, we predict that reduced during state 3.
presenting a NOGO trial during State 3 of mind-wandering Special populations: Attentional deficits are a common
will be associated with no EMG response. In addition, feature of debilitating psychiatric and neurological pathol-
when detected by subjects in State 1, near miss cues ogy (Sonuga-Barke & Castellanos, 2007). One of the central
should also lead to off-line ruminations about performance characteristics of the three-state model is that it can be
in the same way as overt errors. Such ruminations could be used to describe qualitative differences in attentional dis-
assessed by probes (e.g., Smallwood et al., 2004) designed engagement across individuals. That is, the model makes
to detect awareness of near-misses following speeding, specific predictions regarding how attentional disengage-
anticipations, and omissions. Smallwood and colleagues ment is affected by various attention-related disorders
(2007) also suggest the possibility of using probes sepa- such as traumatic brain injury (TBI), attention deficit
rated by different intervals to assess the timing and dura- hyperactive disorder (ADHD), and anxiety disorders such
tion of state transitions as well as the comparison of self- as obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD). ADHD individuals
caught and probe-caught mind-wandering as potential often have much more variable RTs (Klein, Wendling,
indices of early and late (e.g., States 1 and 2) mind- Huettner, Ruder, & Peper, 2006) and have been reported
wandering. to have, occasionally, extremely long RTs (Hervey et al.,
GSR: The galvanic skin response (GSR) has been fairly 2006). The interpretation of such findings has been
consistently associated with SART performance (O’Connell hampered by the absence of a coherent theory to integrate
et al., 2007; Smallwood et al., 2004). Also, for example, and such findings (Castellanos et al., 2005). The present model
consistent with our finding of behavioral responsiveness to provides a principled explanation for these findings in
both successful and unsuccessful NOGO trials, O’Connell terms of different states of mind-wandering related to
and colleagues reported GSR to both errors and successful the degree of decoupling of attention from task. Future
withholds on NOGO trials (O’Connell et al., 2007). Also con- studies employing the indices proposed here may well
sistent with the model, GSR was slightly greater to errors contribute to a theoretically grounded understanding of
than to successful withholds. Finally, across subjects, SART the nature of ADHD attentional deficits.
error detection rates and GSR were significantly correlated. With regard to TBI, the available evidence suggests that
Thus, GSR appears to index task-related mind-wandering TBI patients perform very poorly on the SART and, in
following NOGO trials. This suggests that GSR will be dif- addition, show poor error detection (Hart, Giovannetti,
ferentially related to indices of the three mind-wandering Montgomery, & Schwartz, 1998; McAvinue, O’Keeffe,
states following both success and error, but especially fol- McMackin, & Robertson, 2005; Stemmer, Segalowitz, Witz-
lowing error, on NOGO trials. ke, & Schonle, 2004). Moreover, GSR in response to NOGO
ERP: Several predictions also follow from the three-state errors is significantly reduced in TBI participants relative
model regarding brain activity (as measured by event-re- to controls even with awareness of errors (O’Keeffe et al.,
lated potentials – ERP). Error processing has long been 2004). Given that TBI patients seem less responsive to
thought to involve two separate mechanisms, one for error attention-related errors, we expect that they will have less
detection and another for error correction (Rabbitt, 1966). incentive for off-task rumination and should show weaker
Error negativity (ERN) appears to be related to implicit behavioral reactions to unsuccessful NOGO trials. That is,
J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113 111

TBI patients should be less likely to engage in post-error only to find oneself automatically following a habitual,
ruminations, which should decrease their post-error RT highly familiar route. In State 3, the driver will become
speeding, anticipations, and omissions relative to normal unresponsive to the driving environment. If briefly falling
controls. Thus, compared to normal controls, the bidirec- into State 3 while driving, an individual might completely
tional relations between mind-wandering and attention- fail to brake in response to external cues (e.g., inadvertently
related errors should be reduced for TBI patients. running a red light). Moreover, following from the bidirec-
In contrast, individuals with anxiety disorders, depres- tionality predictions of the model, when the driver commits
sion or OCD might experience exaggerated bidirectional an error (e.g., begins to drive off of the road); awareness of
relations between mind-wandering and attention-related the error might lead to more mind-wandering in the form of
errors. Anxious individuals will likely be more attentive rumination about inattention and driving performance
to their errors and will tend to ruminate on them following potentially generating another bout of mind-wandering.
errors. Farrin and colleagues report that depressed individ-
uals may respond ‘‘catastrophically” to errors producing an 7. Concluding comments
enhanced sense of failure (Farrin, Hull, Unwin, Wykes, &
David, 2003). It is also possible that this is because such Our major goals have been to present a model of atten-
exaggerated reactions to error produce greater rumination tional engagement/disengagement consistent with exist-
on performance (cf. task-related interference; e.g., Small- ing data on mind-wandering and sustained attention, to
wood, Davies, et al., 2004). This would be especially disaf- present new data to assess directly a number of predictions
fecting in that individuals would have a subjective sense of from the model, and to suggest novel predictions to guide
increased task effort associated with increasingly poorer future research. As noted in our introductory remarks indi-
performance. This should result in greater post-error RT ces employed in the present study were selected based on
speeding, anticipations, and omissions in anxious individu- our task analysis of the SART and that detailed task analy-
als compared to normal controls. The associated task-re- ses will be required for the application of the model to dif-
lated mind-wandering then leads to more attention- ferent tasks and contexts. At an applied level, the three-
related errors, which will lead to more task-related rumi- state model of attentional disengagement also points to
nations, ultimately leading to a snowball effect. Similar possible means to prevent, or at least limit, the progression
predictions can be made for individuals suffering from of attentional disengagement. Given signatures for each
sub-clinical levels of performance anxiety. state, for example, we could use these as triggers to pro-
Real-world tasks: Although we have empirically as- vide extrinsic cues to alert subjects to the onset of mind-
sessed the three-state model in the context of the SART, wandering during attentionally demanding tasks. Previous
our reference to real-world mind-wandering situations in research has reported that providing even non-contingent,
the exposition of the model reflects our conviction that non-predictive, content-free ‘‘cues-to-attend” (Fassbender
the model applies beyond the SART task to many everyday et al., 2006) reduces SART errors in both healthy subjects
continuous performance tasks such as reading and driving. and stroke patients (Manly et al., 2004). Based on the pres-
An important aspect of our model is its ability to make con- ent model, similar extrinsic cues provided contingently on
crete predictions about attentional disengagement in these RT changes, anticipations, and omissions should be even
real-world tasks. For example, in the context of reading, more effective at improving performance of subjects, and
the model makes specific predictions about readers’ eye especially individuals with marked deficits in sustained
movements and their phenomenology as they progress attention (e.g., ADD and TBI). Indeed, it is possible that cue-
through states of task disengagement. In particular, we ing based on either decreasing or increasing RTs, anticipa-
predict that entering State 1 disengagement during reading tions, and omissions might selectively reduce inattentional
will be accompanied by sporadic increases in the speed of states as well as errors. Use of fading techniques in which
eye movements and reduced memory for content. There cues become more subtle and even subliminal might effec-
may also be more regression saccades (re-reading) as read- tively ‘‘wean” people from the cues as they become sensi-
ers catch themselves mind-wandering and attempt to get tized to the onset of mind-wandering states, and be less
back on task. In State 2, readers’ eyes will be moving from likely to engage in reactive mind-wandering. It would also
word to word in a very stereotyped fashion reflecting a lack be of interest to consider potentially more general effects
of processing of textual meaning and, hence, if stopped and of such training on an individual’s sense of self efficacy
tested at such times, readers will have very little to no and self control, as well as possible changes in affect. Fu-
memory of the content. Finally, in State 3, readers will stop ture investigations of these possibilities should contribute
moving their eyes altogether or look away and stare com- to the further development of the current model of atten-
pletely engaged in internal processing. tion disengagement with the ultimate goal of developing
Similar predictions from the three-state model can be techniques to reduce the debilitating effects and conse-
made with regard to driving performance. In State 1 of quences of failures of sustained attention and attention-re-
mind-wandering, a driver will be intermittently less lated errors in the real world during attentionally
responsive to the focal moment-to-moment demands of demanding tasks.
the immediate driving situation but be periodically aware
that at times he/she is fully engaged in driving. In State 2,
References
drivers’ responses will be more automatic and triggered
only by generic cues present in the environment. In this Antrobus, J. S. (1968). Information theory and stimulus-independent
state, one might, for example, intend to take a new route thought. British Journal of Psychology, 59, 423–430.
112 J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113

Bellgrove, M. A., Hawi, Z., Gill, M., & Robertson, I. H. (2006). The cognitive Hart, T., Giovannetti, M. S., Montgomery, M. W., & Schwartz, M. F. C.
genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Sustained (1998). Awareness of errors in naturalistic action after traumatic
attention as a candidate phenotype. Cortex, 42, 838–845. brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma, 13, 16–28.
Bellgrove, M. A., Hawi, Z., Kirley, A., Gill, M., & Robertson, I. H. (2005). Hervey, A. S., Epstein, J. N., Curry, J. F., Tonev, S., Arnold, L. E., Conners, C.
Dissecting the attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) K., et al (2006). Reaction time distribution analysis of
phenotype: Sustained attention, response variability and spatial neuropsychological performance in an ADHD sample. Child
attentional asymmetries in relation to dopamine transporter (DAT1) Neuropsychiatry, 12, 125–140.
genotype. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1847–1982. Hester, R., Fassbender, C., & Garavan, H. (2004). Individual differences in
Bellgrove, M. A., Hester, R., & Garavan, H. (2004). The functional error processing: A review and reanalysis of three event-related fMRI
neuroanatomical correlates of response variability: Evidence from a studies using the GO/NOGO task. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 986–994.
response inhibition task. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1910–1916. Hester, R., Foxe, J. J., Molholm, S., Shpaner, M., & Garavan, H. (2005).
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. Neural mechanisms involved in error processing: A comparison of
(2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological errors made with and without awareness. NeuroImage, 27, 602–608.
Review, 108, 624–652. Johnson, K. A., Kelly, S. P., Bellgrove, M. A., Barry, E., Cox, M., Gill, M., et al
Brand, M. (1984). Intending and acting. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (2007). Response variability in attention deficit hyperactivity
Bratman, M. E. (1987). Intention, plans, and practical reason. Cambridge, disorder: Evidence for neuropsychological heterogeneity.
MA: Cambridge University Press. Neuropsychologia, 45, 630–638.
Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Gray, J. R., Molfese, D. L., & Snyder, A. (2001). Johnson, K. A., Robertson, I. H., Kelly, S. P., Silk, T. J., Barry, E., Dáibhis, A.,
Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: Effects of frequency, et al (2007). Dissociation of performance of children with ADHD and
inhibition, and errors. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 825–836. high-functioning autism on a task of sustained attention.
Braver, T. S., Reynolds, J. R., & Donaldson, D. I. (2003). Neural mechanisms Neuropsychologia, 45, 2234–2245.
of transient and sustained cognitive control during task switching. Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., &
Neuron, 39, 713–726. Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: An
Burgund, E. D., Lugar, H. M., Miezin, F. M., & Peterson, S. E. (2003). experience sampling study of working memory and executive control
Sustained and transient activity during an object-naming task: A in daily life. Psychological Science, 18, 614–621.
mixed blocked and event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 19, 29–41. Kaufman, J., Ross, T. J., Stein, E. J., & Garavan, H. (2003). Cingulate
Carriere, J. S. A., Cheyne, J. A., & Smilek, D. (2008). Everyday attention hypoactivity in cocaine users during a GO/NOGO task as revealed by
lapses and memory failures: The affective consequences of event-related fMRI. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 7839–7843.
mindlessness. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 835–847. Klein, C., Wendling, K., Huettner, P., Ruder, H., & Peper, M. (2006). Intra-
Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Noll, D., & Cohen, J. subject variability in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online Biological Psychiatry, 60, 1088–1097.
monitoring of performance. Science, 280, 747–749. Kvavilashvili, L., & Mandler, G. (2004). Out of one’s mind: A study of
Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Scheres, A., Di Martino, A., Hyde, involuntary semantic memories. Cognitive Psychology, 48, 47–94.
C., & Waters, J. R. (2005). Varieties of attention-deficit/hyperactivity MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2000).
disorder-related intra-individual variability. Biological Psychiatry, 57, Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior
1416–1423. cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science, 288, 1835–1838.
Cheyne, J. A., Carriere, J. S. A., & Smilek, D. (2006). Absent-mindedness: Mackworth, N. H. (1948). The breakdown of vigilance during
Lapses in conscious awareness and everyday cognitive failures. prolonged visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Research,
Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 578–592. 1, 6–21.
Cohen, J. D., Botvinick, M. M., & Carter, C. S. (2000). Anterior cingulate and Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., &
prefrontal cortex: Who’s in control? Nature Neuroscience, 3, 421–423. Robertson, I. H. (2001). The differential assessment of children’s
Dockree, P. M., Kelly, S. P., Foxe, J. J., Reilly, R. B., & Robertson, I. H. (2007). attention: The test of everyday attention for children TEA-CH)
Optimal sustained attention is linked to the spectral content of normative sample and ADHD attention. Journal of Child Psychology
background EEG activity: Greater ongoing tonic alpha (10 Hz) power and Psychiatry, 42, 1065–1081.
supports successful phasic goal activity. European Journal of Manly, T., Davidson, B., Gaynord, B., Greenfield, E., Heutniki, J., & Parr, A.
Neuroscience, 25, 900–907. (2004). An electronic knot in the handkerchief: ‘Content free cueing’
Dockree, P. M., Kelly, S. P., Robertson, I. H., Reilly, R. B., & Foxe, J. J. (2005). and the maintenance of attentive control. Neuropsychological
Neurophysiological markers of alert responding during goal-directed Rehabilitation, 14, 89–116.
behavior: A high-density electrical mapping study. NeuroImage, 27, Manly, T., Lewis, G. H., Robertson, I. H., Watson, P. C., & Datta, A. K. (2002).
587–601. Coffee in the cornflakes: Time-of-day as a modulator of executive
Dockree, P. M., Kelly, S. P., Roche, R. A., Hogan, M. J., Reilly, R. B., & response control. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1–6.
Robertson, I. H. (2004). Behavioural and physiological impairments of Manly, T., Owen, A. M., McAvinue, L., Datta, A., Lewis, G. H., Scott, S. K.,
sustained attention after traumatic brain injury. Brain Research et al (2003). Enhancing the sensitivity of a sustained attention task to
Cognitive Brain Research, 20, 403–414. frontal damage: Convergent clinical and functional imaging evidence.
Donaldson, D. I., Peterson, S. E., Ollinger, J. M., & Buckner, R. L. (2001). Neurocase, 9, 340–349.
Dissociating state and item components of recognition memory using Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Galloway, M., & Hawkins, K. (1999). The absent
fMRI. NeuroImage, 13, 129–142. mind: Further investigations of sustained attention to response.
Douse, T. L. (1900). On some minor psychological interferences: A study of Neuropsychologia, 37, 661–670.
misspellings and related mistakes. Mind: New Series, 9, 85–93. McAvinue, L., O’Keeffe, F., McMackin, D., & Robertson, I. H. (2005).
Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoorman, J., & Blanke, L. (1990). Effects of Impaired sustained attention and error awareness in traumatic brain
errors in choice reaction tasks on the ERP under focused and divided injury: Implications for insight. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 15,
attention. In C. H. M. Brunia, A. W. K. Gaillard, & A. Kok (Eds.), 569–587.
Psychophysiological brain research. Tilburg: Tilburg University Press. Mele, A. R. (1992). Springs of action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Farrin, L., Hull, L., Unwin, C., Wykes, T., & David, A. (2003). Effects of Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal
depressed mood on objective and subjective measures of attention. cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167–202.
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 15, 98–104. Mitchell, J. P., Heatherton, T. F., Kelley, W. M., Wyland, C. L., Wegner, D. M.,
Fassbender, C., Simoes-Franklin, C., Murphy, K., Hester, R., Meaney, J., & Macrae, C. N. (2007). Separating sustained from transient aspects of
Robertson, I. H., et al (2006). The role of a right fronto-parietal cognitive control during thought suppression. Psychological Science,
network in cognitive control: Common activations for ‘‘cues-to- 18, 292–297.
attend” and response inhibition. Journal of Psychophysiology, 20, Mullins, C., Bellgrove, M. A., Gill, M., & Robertson, I. H. (2005). Variability
286–296. in time reproduction: Difference in ADHD combined and inattentive
Garavan, H., Hester, R., Murphy, K., Fassbender, C., & Kelley, C. (2006). subtypes. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent
Individual differences in the functional neuroanatomy of inhibitory Psychiatry, 44, 169–176.
control. Brain Research, 1105, 130–142. Norman, D. A. (1981). Categorization of action slips. Psychological Review,
Giambra, L. M. (1995). A laboratory method for investigating influences 88, 1–15.
on switching attention to task-unrelated imagery and thought. O’Connell, R. G., Bellgrove, M. A., Dockree, P. M., & Robertson, I. H. (2004).
Consciousness and Cognition, 4, 1–21. Reduced electrodermal response to errors predicts poor sustained
Gilden, D. L., & Hancock, H. (2007). Response variability in attention attention performance in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
deficit disorders. Psychological Science, 18, 796–802. Neuroreport, 15, 2535–2538.
J. Allan Cheyne et al. / Cognition 111 (2009) 98–113 113

O’Connell, R. G., Dockree, P. H., Bellgrove, M. A., Kelly, S. P., Hester, R., Smallwood, J. M., Heim, D., Rigby, L., & Davies, J. B. (2006). Encoding
Garavan, H., et al (2007). The role of cingulate cortex in the detection during the attentional lapse: Accuracy of encoding during the
of errors with and without awareness: A high density electrical semantic SART. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 218–231.
mapping study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 2571–2579. Smallwood, J. M., McSpadden, M., Luus, B., & Schooler, J. W. (2008).
O’Keeffe, F. M., Dockree, P. M., & Robertson, I. H. (2004). Poor insight in Segmenting the stream of consciousness: The psychological
traumatic brain injury mediated by impaired error processing? correlates of temporal structures in the time series data of a
Evidence from electrodermal activity. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, continuous performance task. Brain and Cognition, 66, 50–56.
101–112. Smallwood, J. M., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2007). The lights are
Pacherie, E. (2008). The phenomenology of action: A conceptual on but no one’s home: Meta-awareness and the decoupling of
framework. Cognition, 107(1), 179–217. attention when the mind wanders. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review,
Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1966). Errors and error correction in choice–response 14, 527–533.
tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71, 264–272. Smallwood, J., Obonsawin, M. C., & Heim, S. D. (2003). Task-unrelated
Rasmussen, J. (1980). What can be learned from human error reports? In thought: The role of distributed processing. Consciousness and
K. Duncan, K. Gruneberg, & D. Wallis (Eds.), Changes in working life. Cognition, 12, 169–189.
London, UK: Wiley. Smallwood, J., Obonsawin, M. C., & Reid, H. (2003). The effects of block
Reason, J. T. (1979). Actions not as planned: The price of automatization. duration and task demands on the experience of task unrelated
In G. Underwood & R. Stevens (Eds.), Aspects of consciousness: Vol. 1. thought. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 22, 13–31.
Psychological issues. London, UK: Wiley. Smallwood, J. M., O’Connor, R. C., Sudberry, M. V., Haskell, C., & Ballantine,
Reason, J. T. (1984). Lapses of attention in everyday life. In R. Parasuraman C. (2004). The consequences of encoding information on the
& R. Davies (Eds.), Varieties of attention. New York: Academic Press. maintenance of internally generated images and thoughts: The role
Reason, J. T. (1990). Human error. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University of meaning complexes. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 789–820.
Press. Smallwood, J. M., O’Connor, R. C., Sudberry, M. V., & Obosawin, M. (2007).
Reason, J. T. (1998). How necessary steps in a task get omitted: Reviving Mind-wandering and dysphoria. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 816–842.
old ideas to combat a persistent problem. International Journal of Smallwood, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological
Cognitive Technology, 3, 24–32. Bulletin, 132, 946–958.
Reason, J., & Lucas, D. (1984). Absent-mindedness in shops: Its incidence, Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., & Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Spontaneous attentional
correlates and consequences. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23, fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: A
121–131. neurobiological hypothesis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,
Reason, J. T., & Mycielska, K. (1982). Absent-minded? The psychology of 31, 977–986.
mental lapses and everyday errors. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Stemmer, B., Segalowitz, S. J., Witzke, W., & Schonle, P. W. (2004). Error
Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., & Yiend, J. (1997). detection in patients with lesions to the medial prefrontal cortex: An
‘Oops!:’ Performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 42, 118–130.
traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 35(6), Teasdale, J. D., Lloyd, C. A., Proctor, L., & Baddeley, A. (1993). Working
747–758. memory and stimulus independent thought: Effects of memory load
Sarason, I. G., Sarason, B. R., Keefe, D. E., Hates, B. E., & Shearin, E. N. and presentation rate. European Journal of Psychology, 5, 417–
(1986). Cognitive interference: Situational determinants and trait-like 433.
characteristics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., & Williams, J. M. G. (1995). How does cognitive
215–226. therapy prevent depressive relapse and why should attentional
Schooler, J. W. (2002). Re-representing consciousness: Dissociations control (mindfulness) training help? An information processing
between experience and meta-consciousness. Trends in Cognitive analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 25–39.
Science, 6, 339–344. Velanova, K., Jacoby, L. L., Wheeler, M. E., McAvoy, M. P., Petersen, S. E., &
Schooler, J. W., Reichle, E. D., & Halpern, D. V. (2005). Zoning-out during Buckner, R. L. (2003). Functional–anatomic correlates of sustained
reading: Evidence for dissociations between experience and and transient processing components engaged during controlled
metacognition. In D. T. Levin (Ed.), Thinking and seeing: Visual retrieval. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 8460–8470.
metacognition in adults and children. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Vincente, K. (2004). The human factor. New York: Routledge.
Searle, J. (1983). Intentionality. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Visscher, K. M., Miezin, F. M., Kelly, J. E., Buckner, R. L., Donaldson, D. I.,
Singer, J. L. (1966). Daydreaming. New York: Random House. McAvoy, M. P., et al (2003). Mixed blocked/event-related designs
Singer, J. L. (1978). Experimental studies of daydreaming and the stream separate transient and sustained activity in fMRI. NeuroImage, 19,
of thought. In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.), The stream of 1694–1708.
consciousness. New York: Plenum. Wegner, D. M. (1997). Why the mind wanders. In J. D. Cohen & J. W.
Smallwood, J. M., Baracaia, S. F., Lowe, M., & Obonsawin, M. (2003). Task Schooler (Eds.), Scientific approaches to consciousness. Mahwah, NJ:
unrelated thought whilst encoding information. Consciousness and Erlbaum.
Cognition, 12, 452–484. Whyte, J., Grieb-Neff, P., Gantz, C., & Polansky, M. (2007). Measuring
Smallwood, J. M., Davies, J. B., Heim, D., Finnigan, F., Sudberry, M., sustained attention after brain injury: Differences in key findings
O’Connor, R., et al (2004). Subjective experience and the attentional from the sustained attention to response task (SART).
lapse: Task engagement and disengagement during sustained Neuropsychologia, 44, 2007–2014.
attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 657–690.

You might also like