You are on page 1of 13

The Management of Complex Tasks in

Organizations: Controlling the Systems


Development Process
Downloaded from informs.org by [131.94.16.10] on 28 August 2015, at 18:31 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

Laurie J. Kirsch
Katz Graduate School of Business, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania 15260

T his manuscript provides needed clarification o1 a number of key constructs in the area of
managerial control, a much discussed but inadequately studied phenomenon within the organi- zation
sci‹ nces. Further, the carefully designed field study provides clear evidence of the importance
of both b‹ havioral and self c‹›ntrol in the management of complex organizatic›nal tasks.

Robert Zmud

Abstract Introduction
Control theory attempts to explain how one person or group
Control in organizations has long been a topic of
in an organization can ensure that antither person or group
interest for researchers and practitioners alike, who
works toward and attains a set or organiz‹itii Anal goals. Prior
empirical work investigai ing control theory has shown that generally recognize that control mechanisms are
characteristics of the task and of the organizational environ- needed t‹a help ensure organizations achieve their goals.
ment predict the use or various types of ct introl. However, While control can be viewed from various perspectives,
this paper argues that w hen control theory is applied to a it iS viewed here in a hehauiorul sense, that is, as
complex, nonroutine task such as the management of infor- attempts to ensure that individuals working on organi-
mation systems development, the theory of control is incom- zational projects act according to an agreed-up‹in strat-
plete. In particular, it prt poses that knowletlge of the task is egy t‹i achieve desired objectives (Jaworsk i 1988,
a key determinant of typi of control. Merchant I 9h8). This definition implies that when a
t-tour modes ot contrc›l (behavior, outcome, clan, and self) controller exercises control over a controllee, she iS
arc identified from the ‹ organizational literature; each high-
taking some action in order to regulate or adjust the
lights different aspects ‹›i control in organizations. Building
hehaoior ref the controllee. The purpose of this study is
on prior empirical work, this paper integrales the dil‘ferent
the‹iretical perspectives a nd predicts the circumstances under to empirically test a model that predicts the circum-
w'hich each type of ctintrol will be implemented. Survey stances under which various modes of control will be
rcsp‹inses from 96 partic ipants of 32 systems development exercised in the context of information systems devel-
efft›rts suggest that the c tent to which behaviors arc moni- torccl opment (ISI3) Jaroject management. ISD is the process
interacts with the prtaject sponsor’s ieveI of systems of identi in k * tl siness objectives for a computerized
development knowledge io determine the amount of behav- information system; designing, building, and testing the
ior contra I; that t›utcomc control is rt functi‹›n of the extent system; and implementing and maintaining it. ISD pro-
tc› which behaviors are m ›nitored and outcorries are measur- ject management involves planning, organizing re-
able; and that self-conti ol is dependent on the extent to sources, :ind measuring progress against plans with an
which outcomes are mea* urable and the levy l ‹it the project overall gr›al oi achieving a set of project objectives
spc›nsor’s kntiwledge ab‹›ut systems development activities.
(Page-.fones 1985).
No relationship between clan control and t he independent
vari:ibles was found. Controlling complex organizational tasks is an elu-
(Monagrment I“ontrol. €."ontrol Theory; .Software Development) sive prcoci:ss. one that is not well understood (Flamholtz
et al. 198a, Merchant 1958, Snell 1992). Because the
management of ISD is nonroutine and diNicult (Slevin
and Pinto 1957, Stuckenbruck 1988), it provides a

1047-7039/ 96/0701 00tll / $i11.25


C“c›p›'right /*0 1996 lnstitu ie for Opt ati‹ins Research
and the Management Sciences O net AN I Z.UTI O s Si ir TCE /Vol. 7, No. 1, January —February 1996 1

Copyright O 2001 All Rights Reserved


LAURIE J. KIRSCH Management of Complex Tasks in Organizations

classic example of the issues associated with structuring integrate the separate knowledge bases and indepen-
control mechanisms for complex tasks. Much of this dently created products into a coherent whole (Walz
complexity stems from the communication and coordi- et al. 1993).
nation problems inherent in tasks that span organiza- This paper proceeds as follows. In the next section, a
tional departments. These problems are manifested in theoretical mode1 of control is developed and hypothe-
several ways. First, each stakeholder involved in ISD ses are generated. The research methodology is de-
has a set of goals, which may include a quality end- scribed in the following section. Next, the data analysis
product, timeliness of implementation, client satisfac-
Downloaded from informs.org by [131.94.16.10] on 28 August 2015, at 18:31 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

strategy and results are presented, followed by a dis-


tion, and reasonable project costs (Page-Jones 1985, cussion of the findings. Finally, limitations are noted
Frame 1987, Pinto and Slevin 1988). However, the and conclusions are drawn.
specific goals may not be clearly articulated; they may
be in conflict, resulting in trade-offs among them
(Wilemon and Baker 1988, Swanson and Beath 1990, Development of a Theoretical Model
Banker and Kemerer 1992); and they may shift over and Hypotheses
the course of the project (Frame 1987, Slevin and Pinto There are two broad categories of control discussed in
1987). Thus, part of the dynamics of project manage- the literature: formal and informal (Jaworski 1958,
ment involves managing multiple goals and multiple Ouchi 1979). Formal control is viewed by organiza-
stakeholders. Second, communication between users tional researchers as a performance evaluation strat-
and information systems (IS) developers can be quite egy, where either behaviors or outcomes are measured,
problematic. Users may find it difficult to translate evaluated, and rewarded (Eisenhardt 1955). This sug-
imprecise business needs into precise system specifica- gests that there are two types of formal control: behav-
tions (Naumann et al. 1950), and need a prototype to ior-based and outcome-based. This view of control stems
help articulate requirements (Zmud 1980, Alavi 1984). from the work of Thompson (1967) and Ouchi (1979).
IS developers may have trouble fully understanding In Ouchi’s conceptualization, the choice of a control
client needs and explaining information technology ca- strategy depends on “knowledge of the transformation
pabilities and ISD procedures (Naumann et al. 1980, process” and “ability to measure outputs.” His model,
Frame 1987, Newman and Noble 1990). The lack of shown in Figure 1, suggests that if knowledge of the
unambiguous requirements and clarity in IS-client transformation process is perfect, that is, if organiza-
communications contributes to the complexity of pro- tions know the precise behaviors and processes that
ject management. Finally, there are factors associated will transform inputs into outputs, they can use behav-
with the project itself that increase coordination and ioral control, evaluating the activities of the controllee
communication tasks. For example, team members of- and basing rewards on whether he followed the correct
ten work on separate ISD tasks (e.g., analysts identify behaviors. If it is possible to measure the organization’s
requirements and programmers write code), especially desired result, outcome control can be used: outcomes
for large development efforts (Zmud 1980, Ahituv, are evaluated and rewards are based on whether the
et al. 1984). Further, there may be multiple people controllee produced the desired output, regardless of
working on one task. Since these individuals bring the process or behaviors followed.
different knowledge and perspective to the task (Curtis Like organizational theorists, agency theorists
et al. 1985), the project leader must find a way to (Jensen and Meckling 1976, Mitnick 1982, Arrow
1985) note that behavior-oriented contracts reward
agents (controllees) on the basis of their behaviors, and
out- come-oriented contracts reward agents according
Figure 1 Organizational Literature Concept of Control to the consequences of their behaviors. Agency theory
Source: Ouchi 1979, p. 843.
recognizes that outcomes are a function of the agent’s
Knowledge of the Transformation Process
effort level as well as uncertainties in the task environ-
ment. Moreover, where Ouchi proposes that behavior-
based control can be used only when knowledge of the
Behavior or Output
Output transformation process is perfect, agency theory argues
Measurement
Ability to that behavioral contracts can be structured when
knowledge is imperfect i/ principles (controllers) invest
Behavior Rituai and Ceremony, in information systems in order to observe or monitor
Measurement ’Clan" Control
the actions of agents. Thus, agency theory adds to

2 O RG AN1 ZATI O N c“l EN € u/Vol. 7, No. 1, January— February


1996
LAURIE 1. KIRSCH Miinagement oJ’ €’omplex Tasks in Org‹inization.s

Ouchi’s conceptualization by positing that the choice of’ into a group, self-control stems from individual objec-
a control strategy is also dependent on “behavior ob- tives and standards (Jaworski 1988). The difference is
scrvability”: the extent to which the controller has subtle. With self-control, an individual monitors his
ac:cess to informati‹›n systems that reveal the con- behavior .ind rewards car sanctions himself accordingly.
trollee’s actions. These information systems, among With clan control, monitoring, rewarding and sanction-
‹other mechanisms, include boards of directors, ac- ing .me a functit›n of the group. Consider the following
counting systems, personal observation, and evaluative example ui' self-control. An IS programmer, because he
Downloaded from informs.org by [131.94.16.10] on 28 August 2015, at 18:31 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

meetings. believes that extensive documentation is critical, sets a


Informal controls differ from formal controls in personal goal of documenting the computer code he is
that they are base‹l on social or people strategies writing; he monitors how well he is progressing over
I F.isenhardt 1985, la vorski 1988). One type of informal time; a nd intrinsically rewards himself for completing
control is known as lan control (Ouchi 1979): the use the jol›, cnfirefy in‹lependent of formal organizational
‹ii selection processes and social mech‹inisms to control control mechanisms or clan norms. Tasks involving a
the behavior of individuals. Ouchi (1950) describes a great dcal of autonomy, creativity, or intellectual activ-
clan as any group ‹ ›f individuals wh‹› have common ity are goo‹l candidates for self-control since it is
goals and who are clependent on one another; exam- difficult f‹ir controllers to identify appropriate behav-
ples may include a profession or a labor union. To ir›rs (hlanz and Sims 1980, Greenberger and Strasser
implement clan conti of, the organizational group culti- 198a). Self-managed individuals are intrinsically moti-
vates common value s, philosophy, and approaches to vated I o ‹ichieve their objectives (Manz ct al. 1987),
problem sr›lving within the clan. Careful selection of dci’ine ihcir own goals and processes appropriate for
members and socialization help propiigate these com- the task, ‹ind need feedback to inform them about their
ion values and beli‹:ls, resulting in individuals with a perf‹›rmance (Kerr and Slocum 1981, Ashford and
strong sense of iden tity with and commitment to the Cummings 1983). Controllers can encourage self-regu-
group. Rituals and 'eremonies serve to identify and lation lay ‹illc wing individuals to select tasks and ways
reinforce acceptable behaviors among members of the of w ork ing, hissing ofgitflizational rewards, in part, on
chin, and individuals are rewarded for acting in accor- how well individuals manage their own work
‹tance with the group’s values. Ouchi ( 1979) p‹isits that (Von C linow 1983. l98'i). The characteristics of behav-
chin control will be implemented when knowledge of ior. ‹but corrie, clan, and self control are summarized in
the transformation process is imperfect and outcomes "Table I .
are difficult to measure (see Figure 1 ); in fact, he
‹irgues that the pro) her behaviors are often unknown Development of a Research Model
and desired outcome s evolve over the course ‹if time. Research on control has been dominated by conceptual
t3uchi provides the following example. In a research work rather than empirical work (Flamholtz et al. 1985,
lal›, it is difficult to know the precise behaviors that, if Merchant 1988), and there is a lack of agreement on
followed, will lead to scientific breakth roughs; it is also the me‹ining r›f key constructs and their relationships
hard to identify desired outcomes in a way that enables to e.ich other (Merchant and Simons 1986, Merchant
the measurement of ,cientists’ r›utputs in a meaningful 1988). One relationship that has often been con-
‹›r timely way. As an alternative to l›ehavior or out- t’ounded in the literature is the relationship between
come control, clan control can be implemented by monitoring alul the exer‹’ise oJ’ control (i.e., the regula-
rarefully selecting inilividuals with appropriate profes- tion of contrt›llee behaviors): monitoring is sometimes
sional training, socializing them to the goals and values viewed as part of control (e.g., Simons 1987, Snell
‹›f the clan, and rev arding them for participating in clan- 1992) and other times viewed as an antecedent to the
approved rituals, such as attending conferences and exer‹:ist of control (e.g., Koontz and Bradspies 1972).
publishing article s. Unlike much prior work, Eisenhardt (1985) is clear
A second type of informal control is self-regulation abt›ut this rel‹itionship, noting that monitoring serves
c›r .self-control: that is, the study c›f how and when as an inlormiition system and makes behaviors observ-
individuals in organizations control their own actions. able. whereas evaluation and reward constitute the way
T“his concept is consistent with the definition of self- in which actions are regulated. This perspective implies
miinagement (Manz c t al. 1957, Erez and Kanfer 1953): that the process oJ obtaining information on a con-
an individual sets his own goals, m‹initors his own trolled’!› behaviors is nt›t the same as acting on that
work. and rewards i ›r sanctions himself accordingly. inftirm‹tti‹›n in order to change the person’s behavior,
Whereas clan control is a function of being s‹icialized that is, it does not assume that the controller necessar-

O eG AN I ZAT U3 I S C I ENCE / Vol. 7, No. 1, January—February 199fi 3


LAURIE J. KIRSCH Management of Complex Tasks in Organization.s

Table 1 Characteristics of Four Modes of Control


control, while pay plans that include commission con-
stitute outcome control. The results from a survey of 54
BEHAVIOR
retail stores suggest that when tasks are programmable,
Behaviors that transform inputs to outputs are known
Controller monitors and evaluates controllee’s behaviors
or behaviors are measurable (observable), behavior
Expllclt link exists between extrinsic rewards and followlng control is implemented; when the cost of outcome
behaviors measurement is low, or outcome uncertainty is low,
outcome control is used.
Downloaded from informs.org by [131.94.16.10] on 28 August 2015, at 18:31 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

OUTCOME Govinctarajan and Fisher (1990) extended this line of


Desired task outcomes are known and measurable
research into the strategy literature, further refining
Controller evaluates whether outcomes were met.
Explicit link exists between extrinsic rewards and producing Eisenhardt’s and Ouchi’s models. In particular, they
out- comes modified Ouchi’s original model to include the behav-
ior observability construct from agency theory, and
CLAN they retained the notion of task programmability from
Task-related behaviors and outcomes are not prespecified
Eisenhardt’s work. Govindarajan and Fisher’s model
Goals are determined by clan and evolve during the task perlod
Clan identifies and reinforces acceptable behaviors
suggests that behavior observability, outcome measura-
Rewards are based on acting in accordance with clan’s values bility, and task programmability are antecedents of
and attitudes type of control strategy.
Shared experiences, values, and beliefs among the clan Applying this line of research, and in particular the
mem- “task programmability” construct, to ISD project man-
bers agement presents certain difficulties. First, the project
Members exhibit strong commitment to the clan leader’s job encompasses a variety of human, financial,
SELF and technical ctimponents (Slevin and Pinto 1957).
Controllee sets own task goals and procedures While it may be possible to specify a set of activities or
Controllee is intrinsically motivated behaviors for ISD project leaders to follow, there is
Controllee engages in self monitoring and self evaluation still much uncertainty about a project’s outcomes that
Rewards are based partly on controllee's ability to self manage results from those behaviors, especially for the non-
technical components of the job. Because the link
between following a prescribed set of behaviors and
generating known outcomes is tenuous, “task pro-
ily understands the processes and behaviors the con- grammability” does not appear to be a particularly
trollee is following. useful construct for studying the control of ISD project
Eisenhardt’s insight comes from integrating Ouchi’s management.
theory of control with agency theory into one model Second, it is important to recognize that the con-
troller’s capabilities to observe the behaviors that con-
that predicts the choice of control strategy. From Ouchi,
trollees engage in does not necessarily mean that they
she notes that “outcome measurability” and “knowl-
edge of the transformation process” are predictors of will be able to evaluate, reward, or sanction the con-
control strategy; from agency theory, she includes “be- trollees based on what they observe. Since ISD efforts
havior observability” and “uncertainty” as predictors. are interdepartmental, both user and IS management have
Eisenhardt tested this integrated model in the retail reason to control the project teams; however, their
sales compensation context. In conducting the empiri- levels of understanding about the systems development
cal study, she equated Ouchi’s notion of “knowledge process can vary greatly, and there is no guarantee that
of the transformation process” with “task program- the large amount of managerial and technical informa-
mability,” which is defined as the degree to which ap- tion produced during the ISD process is meaningful to
propriate behaviors can be specified in advance. It is them. It can only be useful to those controllers who
important to note that by equating “knowledge of the truly understand the meaning and implications of the
transformation process” with “task programmability,” data. Thus, the degree to which controllers can observe
this dimension is considered a characteristic of the task’, behaviors and make sense of them can differ dramati-
in other words, it is a function of whether documented cally. This implies that “knowledge of the transformation
behaviors exist. Eisenhardt operationalized control process” is not simply a matter of articulating a set of
strategy in terms of monetary compensation, arguing behaviors, but it is a function of how well the controller
that salaries or hourly rates are a form of behavior understands the task.

4 O R G ANI ZATI O N S oI ENC E / Vol. 7, No. 1, January—February 1996


Coovrioht 2001 All Riohts Reserved
To extend this line of research into l he ISD manage- efforts ‹if the project leader, the more likely they will
ment c‹›ntcxt, then, t he task programmability construct implement behavior-based control. Further, Ouchi’s
is replaced with “cr›utroller’s k nowle‹ige of the trans- theory suggests that as knowledge of the transforma-
formation prr›cess,” which retcrs to the controller’s tion pr‹:›ccss increases, the use of behavior control will
knowledgC ilbout lSI J. This suggests that type of con- increase; the argument made here is that it is the
t r‹›l is dependent ‹›i behavior observability, outcome ‹ on f/ olli•r’s knowledge of the transformation process
measurability, hand t he controller’s knowledge of the that is important: the more knowledgeable the con-
i i.tnsformation proce ss. The research m‹›del, shown in
Downloaded from informs.org by [131.94.16.10] on 28 August 2015, at 18:31 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

trollers are ‹ibout ISD, the more likely they will im-
I igui c 2, is a retincincnt ‹a1 Govindar: jan and Fisher’s plerrient behavior-based control. This ‹argument is
( I “J9ti) model. It is ink portant to note that the depiction consists:nt with Snell’s ( 1992) finding that, among other
‹›t ‹i certain mocle c›f ct›ntrol in cach cell is nr›t meant things, the use of behavior control is dependent on
It i imply that mocJes of control ‹occur independently. “hat ing complete cause-effect knowledge”(p. 317), and
ltathcr. the intent i• tr› predict the rcl‹itive extent to suggests that behavior control will be implemented in
w hich the various m‹odes of control arc present. While cells 1 antl 3.
t he model :ippears t‹ be descriptive, C ovindarajan and t“untrol theories also suggest that the controllers in
I is he r ( l9'i0) point ‹but that il is normative ‹is well, the situations described by cells 1 and 5 have two
implying I h‹it fr›r el lective pcrli›rmance, an organiza- options. l3eciiuse outcome measurability is high, one
i it›n should use the I ‹arm of control specified” (p. 261) opti‹›n is to implement outcome-based control. While
in cach cell. The rai ionale for the hvpr›theses gener-
this approach is feasible, agency theory suggests that it
‹ited from Figure 2 i explained next.
place:s risk ‹›n a risk-‹iverse controllee. In the ISD
Co nsider first cells 1 and 3. Agencv theory suggests
c‹intext, thcrc would also seem t‹i be a bias against the
th‹tt when 1 ehavior ‹ observability is high, behavior use •f ‹outcome contra 1: with outcome control, errors
con- trol will he used: t lie more contrt›llcrs monitor the
may not he detected until the system is completed;
howeve r, the costs of correcting the errors are much
greater at this point than during the early phases of
ISU (Pressm:in 1992, Walz et al. 1993). According to
agency thcorv, a second option in cells 1 and 5 is to
Figure 2 Research Model purchase inf‹›rmation about the controllee’s behavior
Predictions in cells 1, 4, 5, 7, and 8 differ from and structure a behavioral contract. This makes sense
Govindarajan and Fisher's (19901 model, where the i{ the i-ontroller understands the transformation pro-
respective t›redictions are Outcome or Behavior; cess we11 enough to recognize which behaviors are
Clan; Behavior Behavior; and Clan appropriate (cell 1). For example, to ensure that the
Controller's Knowledge of the Transformation Process
users’ require ments are met, the controller can monitor
the ‹:oritr‹i1lee in a number of ways, including partici-
pating i n walk-throughs to review project progress, and
nt›ting the way in which the project leader incorporates
Behavior Ou‹ come a devel‹ipment method‹ilogy to structure the ISD task.
observability
lf t he t ‹introller is knowledgeable about ISD, she can
observe these types of activities, evaluate whether they
measurability
are .ipJ›ropriate, and reward or sanction the project
leader accor‹1ingly. However, if the controller is not
w B v ha yGOF
observability knt›wle‹âgc able, it is unlikely that she can make sense
o1 and ev‹iluiite what she observes; thus, it is unlikely
Cell 4 Cell 7 she will clepcnd on behavior control mechanisms to
High behavior
regulate the behavior ot the project leader. Thus, when
Behavior Clan
observability behavior ribservability is high, that is, when information
IAWOUtOOm
about the ISD task is available, the controller will
Cell 4 implement behavior control only if she is knowledge-
able about the systems develc›pment process (cells 1
Self
anti 3). Thus. Figure 2 suggests that behavior observ-
‹ bservab›Iiry
ability antl controller’s knowledge of the tr‹insforma-

b3 R€k S Id \ 11 O^' S C’I ENt‘i /Vol. 7, Nc›. 1, Jan iiary—February 1996 5

Copyright O 2001 All Rights


LAURIE 3. KIRSCH Management of Complex Tasks in Organizations

tion process interact to determine the level of behavior heavy reliance on controller—controllee interaction that
control: leads to a sense of trust and commitment between the
HYPOTHESIs 1. Behavior observability and controller’s parties. Thus, Figure 2 predicts that the use of clan
knowledge of the transformation process haue a positive control increases as behavior observability increases, as
interaCtiue effect on the leuel of’ behauior control. outcome measurability decreases, and as controller’s
knowledge decreases. The model suggests that these
Since the controller in cell 5 is not knowledgeable three independent variables are not only related to
Downloaded from informs.org by [131.94.16.10] on 28 August 2015, at 18:31 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

about ISD, behavior control is not an option. Control clan control, but interact to determine the level of clan
theories argue that when outcomes are measurable, control:
controllers can use outcome-based control, regardless HYPOTHESis 3. Behavior observability, outcome
of their knowledge of the transformation process. For mea- surability, and controller’s knowledge of the
example, a controller does not have to understand the transforma- tion proces.s haue an interactive effect on
intricacies of ISD to know whether the system was the leuel of clan control. In particular, high levels of
implemented on time. Thus, in cell 5, the controller is behavior obseruahil- ity, low levels of outcome
more likely to use her ability to measure outcomes and measurability, and low leuel.s of controller's knowledge
structure an outcome-based contract, passing on some of the transformation process lead to increased use of
risk to the project leader. Further, consistent with clan control.
control theory predictions and previous empirical work
(e.g., Snell 1992), the controllers in cells 2 and 6 will Finally, consider cells 4 and S. Prior work predicts
most likely use outcome control because outcomes are the use of clan controls; however, it seems more likely
measurable. Based on these arguments, Figure 2 sug- that the controller will induce the project leader to use
gests that outcome control will be used in those cells self-control. Since behaviors are not observable and
where outcomes are highly measurable, except where outcomes are not measurable, behavior and outcome
both behaviors are highly observable and controllers controls are ruled out. Further, the low level of behav-
are very knowledgeable, in which case behavior control ior observability suggests the controller is not closely
will be used and unnecessary risk will not be passed to supervising or interacting with the controllee, resulting
controllees (i.e., in cells 2, 5, and 6, but not in cell 1): in less opportunity for clan-type conditions to evolve. It
also suggests that the controllee has much more discre-
HYPOTHESIS 2. Behavior observability and controller’s tion or freedom in choosing and managing his work
knowledge of the transformation process haue a negative (Alavi et al. 1986, Manz and Angle 1986), implying the
interactive effect on the leuel of outcome Control. presence of self-control. The absence of observable
outcomes means that a set of precise project goals has
Next consider cell 7. Outcome control is no longer
not been articulated. This, in turn, suggests that the
an option since outcomes are not measurable. While
controllee may self-set goals, again implying the pres-
behaviors are observable, they are not very meaningful;
ence of self-control. Thus, in cells 4 and 8, the con-
thus, behavior control is not an option. However, be-
troller is likely to rely on the controllee’s ability to
cause behavior observability is high, it suggests that the
manage his own work. Specifically, the model in Figure
controller has invested in information systems such as
2 predicts that self-control is a function of behavior
reports, meetings, and personal observation to follow
observability and outcome measurability. The model
the progress of the project. Given that behavior and
also implies that outcome measurability and behavior
outcome controls are not possible, this high level of
observability have an interactive effect on self-control,
behavior observability provides the controller with an
suggesting, for example, that low behavior observability
opportunity to implement clan control: she can use her
results in an increase in self-control only when out-
time with the controllee to explain group values, norms,
come measurability is low:
and problem-solving approaches. For example, when a
user works with the project leader to identify system HYPOTHESIS 4. Behavior obseroubility and outcome
requirements, to lay out screen and report formats, and measurability haue a negative tnteractiue effect on the
when she attends project meetings, she can sensitize level of self-control.
the project leader to the goals of the user community
and build a sense of trust between the user and IS. Methodology'
This is consistent with Beath’s (1987) argument that Research Design
successful social contracting is exercised through a While there are a number of stakeholders involved in
ISD, in this research, two key relationships are the

6
O RG AN1ZATI O N S Cl KN Cu/ Vol. 7, No. 1, January—February 199f›

CoDvriaht O 2001 All Riohts


LAURIE J. KIRSCH Mr inugemrnt ‹aJ L ompl‹'x tasks in £J ryanizanon s

target of study. the relationship between the systems participants. Each participant was contacted by the
development manage r and the project leader, and be- rcse arc’her, who conducted (on average) a 15-minute
tween the user contact and the project leader. The user teleyiht›nc interview. The purpose of the phone inter-
cc›ntact is the key lia isr›n person in the client area, who view w‹is primarily to obtain the person’s commitment
,resists in arious phases of the development process. to the research. since the questionnaires were quite
for example, require ments specification, testing, and long: 12 pages for the controllee, and 9 for the con-
implementation. The development m anager and user
troller. At the same time, relevant background infor-
Downloaded from informs.org by [131.94.16.10] on 28 August 2015, at 18:31 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

cc›ntact exercise cram rol over the project leade r, who is


mation w as collected. After the interview, the notes
the tocal point ot’ the ISD cffo rt thFtn 8h managing the were transcribed and the appropriate questic›nnaire
diiy-i o-da›' process ‹ nd making decisions anal choices was m‹iilcd. Ol the l tJ5 people interviewe‹1, 101 re-
thdt are ultimately iiicc›rporated into the system. In this turned the questionnaire, for a response rate of 96Wo.
reseiirch, then, the IS manage r and user contact arc Ht›wever, because of missing data, three projects were
i hc• c’ontrollers, and i he prt›jCct leader is the
eventually di oppcd frum the analysis, resulting in 96
c‹introllee. usable responses, for a response rate of 91 %.
these relatic›nships (clevclopinent ir ‹in ^8e •- project The participating firms are all large, representing
leader, and user com tact—prc›jcct leafier) were chosen
vari‹›u›. industries, with annual revenues ranging from
because the cuntrtill‹'rs have trec|uerit and direct inter-
$1fi8 million to $49 billion, averaging a little over $9
action with the proj‹ ct leader rluring systems develop-
billion. The systems were developed by IS departments
irenl, and have mu:h to lose if their contr‹›l efforts
within these organizations; the number of IS employ-
tail; thus, both IS ni,tnagers and user contacts play key
ees ranged from I l to 5,0()(), with an average of 572.
.‘tales in c‹int rolling the man agemcn t c›f ISD tasks.
Twenty-seven of the 3ñ systems were designed to run
L urther, given thal “control lc r’s knowledge of the
on niainirames; the remainder were developed for
transformati‹in prcic‹ ss” is a key consl ruct, it was criti-
mid range and person‹il computers. Development ap-
‹Tal t‹i sample frtim a ptipulatic›n that e xhihits variabilitv
proiichcs incl uded the systems development life cycle,
.«'ross this construe . It seemed like ly that most IS
information engineering, prototyping, and in-house de-
development managt rs would he quit c knowledgeable
velopers nietlic›dologies. The size of the projects varied,
ahout ISD, whereas there would be n iuch more vari a-
both in terms of the number of person days to com-
tion acr‹ass user cc›ni acts. While IS m‹inagers and user
plete I he project (range: 42 to 45,000 person days;
cc›nt:icts may have v.irit›us reasons to exercise control.
average: 7,938), and the elapsed time needed to de-
1 he IS literature sugi;csts they have fuur g‹ als for ISD
velop i he system (range: three months to seven years;
pi’ojccts. ln particul: ir. cc›ntrollers want the ISD pro-
average: two years). At the time of data collection, 11
‹:c ss to result in systt me: that meet users’ require ments,
projects were in the linal stagcs of‘ implementation.
that arc delivered or time’, that adhere to lS .stundar‹ls:
Fourteen of the projects were completed in the six
.iricl whose development costs do not exceecl huñgets
month!› prior tt› data collection; six projects, in the
i Hallam and Scrivcii 1976, I’om t')87, Kumar and
previous 1 2 months: and one was completed lS months
13 orn-Andersen 199t1, Banker ‹ind Kemerer 1992).
prior to dicta Collection. While IS months was a longer
T‹› study the relat onships describeLl in thc research
time li ame than the tit her projects, the detrtils of this
hypotheses, question naires were desi,¿ncd to measure
particular project wcre very s‹ilient to the piirticipants
l›chavior observability, outcome measurability, con-
because it w‹is one t›f the last major efforts undertaken
ir‹allcr’s knowledge ‹›i‘ lSD. anal mode of control (with
before mt›st application work was outsourced. Further,
respect to the means with which IS managers and user
facts given by all three participants during the tele-
ccint:icts exercise en ntrol over ISD project Ie‹iders to
phone interviews were in complete agreement, so the
crisui’e the: delivery ot system s thal meet the four
decision was made to keep this project in the sample.
pi oject goals). The clue stionnaires were pretested with
Project teams varied in size from three tto approxi-
lS project leaders, sy stems development managers. and
mately 1(1(1 ‹ind included programmers, analysts, and
user contacts associn ed with four ISD projects at three
othe r technical personnel, according to the needs of
i Jl'gtlnizatiOflS.
the pr‹›ject. Fhe user c‹intacts varied in terms ef orga-
After the pretest, firms fr‹im the Midwest and South
nizational p‹isition and experience with IS projects.
were contacted and asked tra participate in this re-
Ninctecn r›f the 35 occupicd positions in which a major
search; 17 agreed. E.ich firm identifieil one to i ive ISD
responsibility is to act as a liaison between end-users
pi’ojects, as well as t he IS project 1eac)er, his manager,
and IS departments. "I“wenty of the 35 user contacts
arid user contact a›sr›ciated with cach project. This
were involved in relatively few (zero to five) informa-
resul ted in a total ‹ ›t 35 projects anal 105 individual

(3ftG.\ N IZA 1’I O N S I I k Nt 1 / ¥'ol. 7, N u. 1, Jan uary—February 1996


LAURIE J. KIRSCH Management of Complex rasks fn Orgonizntions

tion systems development efforts prior to the current or clan controls are operating: with formal control,
effort. goals are typically set by a supervisor or management;
with clan control, goals evolve in concert with the clan’s
Instruments value set. Additional constructs from the sclf-control
New scales were developed to measure behavior ob- conceptual literature include: controllee self-monitor-
servability, outcome measurability, and controller’s ing; controllee self-evaluation, intrinsic motivatic n of
knowledge of the transformation process. In addition, controllee; and degree to which the controller rewards
Downloaded from informs.org by [131.94.16.10] on 28 August 2015, at 18:31 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

a measure of how important the project goal is to the the controllee for exhibiting self-management. Thus,
controller (i.e., meeting user requirements; delivering measures for each of these four aspects, along with a
the system on-time; keeping costs within budget; ad- measure of the extent to which the controllee is the
hering to standards) was also developed. This construct source of the project goal, are combined ter indicate
was added after analyzing the pre-test data, since those how strongly self-control is operating.
preliminary results suggested that if the project goal Recall that what is of interest is the strength of
was unimportant to the controller, then she did not behavior, outcome, clan, and self-controls put in place
exert control at all. by each of the two controllers to ensure that the
Determining how to measure mode of control (be- project leader delivers a system that meets the four
havior, outcome, clan, and self) was especially trouble- project goals. Because the focus of the research is on
some since much of the prior research measures it in a multiple controller—controllee relationships and multi-
somewhat simplistic approach, often conceptualizing ple-project goals, the design of the questionnaires is
control as a unidimensional construct and measuring it complex. In particular, each construct is measured
with a single question. (See, for example, Ouchi multiple times by modifying the question for each goal
(1978) and Govindarajan and Fisher (1990).) In a and/or for each controller, as appropriate. To reduce
manner similar to Snell (1992), the approach taken threats from common source variance, the dependent
here was to recognize the multidimensional nature of variables were measured on the project leader (control-
control, con- structing composite measures. In lee) questionnaire, while the independent variables and
particular, for each mode of control, key conditions goal importance were measured on the controller (IS
were identified, the existence of which would strongly manager and user contact) questionnaire. Where possi-
suggest that a partic- ular approach was being used by ble, existing instruments were used; however, all items
the controller to regulate controllee behaviors. First, were modified to fit the ISD context. Specific items
since formal con- trols are written and management- and their source are in the Appendix.
initiated (Jaworski 1988), the measures of formal
behavior and outcome controls include an indication of Instrument Validation
the extent to which behaviors and outcomes are known
and prespecified by someone other than the controllee, Internal Consistency. Internal consistency was mea-
as well as an as- sessment of the degree to which sured by computing Cronbach’s alpha fer each scale.
organizational rewards are based on following The conventional cut-off for acceptable reliability is
prescribed behaviors or produc- ing desired outcomes 0.70 (Nunnally 1978). In Table 2, each scale is listed,
(i.e., the link between rewards and following behaviors along with the number of items measuring the con-
or producing outcomes). Sec- ond, when clan control struct, the sample size of the dataset used to compute
is operating, organizational goals are typically reliability, and the reliability. In addition, descriptive
unknown at the start of a project, but instead evolve statistics are included. Each item was measured on a
over time (Ouchi 1979). Further, the clan exhibits scale of one to seven.
shared values that are cooperative in nature (Birnberg Because self-monitoring (both feedback from the
and Snodgrass 1988). Given the simi- lar value set and task and feedback from agents) and self-evaluation
the desire to work together, an individual’s high level have reliabilities less than 0.70, these scales were
of commitment to the group is an indication that clan dropped. Conceptually, dropping these scales does not
controls are operating. Thus, the measure of clan mean that the essence of the self-control measure has
control combines measures for goal instability and been lost, as there was some overlap in the constructs
commitment to the clan. Third, when self-controls are being measured. In particular, if a person is highly
operating, the individual sets his own goals (Manz et intrinsically motivated (a scale that was retained), he is
al. 1987), which may or may not be formally rewarding or sanctioning himself. In order to reward or
documented. This notion of self-set goals con- trasts sanction himself, that person has to monitor and evalu-
sharply with the approaches taken when formal

O RGAN i ZATI O N S ci r xc u /Vol. 7, No.1, January—February 199a›


8

Copvri0ht O 2001 All Rights


LAURIE 3. KIRSCH M.anagement oJ L"omple:s Fas'ks tn £lryanizations

Table 2 Scales and Reliabilities

S‹:ale # items a h, mean std dev range

PI especifieo Behaviors 3 136 CI73 3 73 1 59 1 00—6 67


L ink fJetween Behaviors ñ. Rewards 2 260 3 53 1 E1 1 00—7 00
Prespecited Outcomes 3 133 4 27 0 96 2 00—7 00
Downloaded from informs.org by [131.94.16.10] on 28 August 2015, at 18:31 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

Link Between Outcomes . \ Rewards 264 4 38 1 72 1 00—7 00


Commitment to the Clan 9 08 4 99 1 11 2 56—7 00
Goal Instability 2 136 4 42 1 93 1 00-7 00
LJ ntrollee as Source of I .cal 2 136 3 54 1 99 1 00—7 00
S °lt Monitoring (Feedbac frcm Task) 2 136 4 98 1 56 1 00—7 00
Self Monitoring (Feedba‹: from Agenls/ 2 136 4 32 1 68 1 00—7 00
S‹*lf E valuation (Kncwlefl‹ e ot ResrJlts) 2 1.31 5 00 1 55 1 00—7 00
lntrinr›ic Motivatir›n 4 132 5 79 1 01 3 25—7 00
Reward for S°-If Manager› ,ent 1 n/a 4 17 1 91 1 00—7 00
Behavior Ob'›ervability 4 2?7 3 92 1 50 1 00—6 75
Outc‹ame Measurability 249 5 54 1 49 1 00—7 00
LJ.3ntroTIer’s Knowledge 268 4 45 2 06 1 00—7 00

’ ! he sample size depenr›s on how the variable was measured (e g once per pro ect, once per controller, once per goal) For some variables,
tne \›oss/D/e sample sizt is 35 projects x 4 goals x 2 controllers or 260 F‹›r ethers, it is 35 pro ents x 4 goals - 140, or 35 pro ects x 2
controllers 70 Actual s imole sizes differ slightly due t‹› trussing aata

il t his work as the ar i e t progresses. Thus, dropping


the self-monitoring and self-evaluation scales, which the m‹:asures of the dependent variables are formed by
averaging the scores for each of the scales of the
exhibit poor psychometric properties. should not have
composite measures. In particular, behavior control is
an adverse effect on how well self-c‹›ntrol is measured.
me‹isured by averaging the scores for pre-specified
behavi‹›rs and the link between rewards and following
C‹›nt›erge.nt arts f9is‹:riminant Validity. Multitrait-
bchavitirs; outcome control, by averaging the scores
multimethod matrix es (Campbell and Fiske l9ñ9,
for pre-spccil’ied outcomes and the link between
Bagozzi l9btl) were used to assess convergent and
rewards and pr‹iducing outcomes; clan control, by
discriminant validitics, where the multiple itcms used
averaging the scores i'or gt›al instability and
tc› measure each construct correspond to Campbell
commitment to the clan; and self control, by averaging
and Fiskc’s notion of multiple methods. lty examining
the scores for controllee as s‹ource ‹if the project goal,
the c‹irrclations o1’ an ilem measuring oiie construct
intrinsic motivation of the controllee, and reward for
both with other items measuring the same
self-management. For each project, then, there are
construct (‘ within-c‹anstruct” ‹'‹irrelations) and with
eight measures of each type of control: ‹one measure
items mea- siriig different c‹instructs (“cross-
per controller per project goal. Similarly. the
construct” cor- relations). the matt ices provide
independent variables are measured eight times per
some evidence f’or
project.
c‹inve•8C nt and disci iminant validities. lividence of the
ft›rmer comes lr‹illi the “sufficiently large” within-
c‹instruct correlations ranging from 0.29 to (I.8f›, all
significant at 0.tJ0l. The relatively few number of in- Analysis
stances where cross c‹instruct correlations are greater One way to approach the analysis is to regress each
than within-construe t correlatit›ns (25 of 83(1 compar- type oi’ control on goal importance, behavior observ-
isons) provides evid‹ rice of discriminant validity. ability, outc‹ime measurability, and controller’s knowl-
Overall, the results suggest that the instruments re- edge of the transformation process. However, because
tained are reliable ind valid. Based on this analysis. multiple data points are drawn from each project, the
responses may not be independent and the regression

bJ aG.\N1ZATI ON Sc’i eN‹ i / Vol. 7, No. 1, I anuary —February


1996 9
CoDvriaht O 2001 All Riohts Reserved
LAURIE 3. KIRSCH Management of Complex Tasks in Organizations

error terms may be correlated, violating an assumption allows for the possibility that other mechanisms may be
underlying regression analysis. In particular, for this implemented to control for other project goals. Note
data, some of an individual’s responses with respect to that the argument here is not that project responsibtlities
the four goals within a single project are highly corre- are split along different potential goals, only that con-
lated, while others are not. The ranges of correlations trollers may implement different control strategte.s for
are: behavior observability: 0.12-0.65; outcome mea- the different project goals.
surability: — 0.01—0.34; controller’s knowledge of the The second reason for not analyzing the data at the
transformation process: 0.68—0.88; behavior control:
Downloaded from informs.org by [131.94.16.10] on 28 August 2015, at 18:31 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

project level is that low power exists with a sample size


0.50-0.83; outcome control: — 0.08-0.41; clan control: of 32. When power is low, the phenomenon may actu-
0.12—0.63; and self-control: 0.66-0.84. ally exist, but may not be detected because of a weak
An alternate approach would be to conduct the statistical test (Baroudi and Orlikowski 1989). Follow-
analysis at the project level by averaging the scores for ing Cohen’s (1988) approach for determining sample
each construct (i.e., for each project, there would be size suggests that a sample of 35 is sufficient to detect
one score for behavior observability, one score for a phenomenon displaying a large effect; however,
behavior control, etc.). This approach is problematic Baroudi and Orlikowski (1989) argue that it is unrealis-
for two reasons. First, since organizations have multi- tic for MIS researchers to expect large effect sizes,
ple goals for ISD projects, it is conceivable that they suggesting that a sample of 32 would lead to inconclu-
implement different control strategies to ensure that sive results.
the different outcomes are met. The results of the To conduct the analyses, a third approach was se-
pretest provide some support for this position. For lected. The problem of nonindependent responses is
example, at one organization, the project leader and IS often due to the omission of key independent variables
manager explained that the requirements and time- from the statistical model (Neter et al. 1985, Berry and
frame were both prespecified and documented in a Feldman 1985). In order to capture the variance due to
Project Notebook. The IS manager implemented out- nonspecified project variables (e.g., size of the
come control to ensure the implementation date was project), a dummy variable is included for each
met by tying part of the project leader’s performance project. Using the dummy variables forces a within-
review and subsequent raise to meeting that date; project analysis. In effect, the analysis is done by
further, he implemented behavior control to ensure the estimating the regression coefficients for the eight data
required functionality was built into the system by points within each pro- ject, and then “averaging”
meeting regularly with the project leader to monitor those estimates across the sample. In contrast, a
the behaviors and processes she was following, to as- project-independent analysis would estimate the
sess the appropriateness of her efforts, and to redirect regression coefficients by examin- ing all 256 data
her work, as necessary. There is also some support for points. Thus, if the individual re- sponses within a
examining control at the project goal level in the litera- project are highly correlated, there is less chance of
ture. For example, Banker and Kemerer t1992) suggest finding significant results with the dummy variable
that organizations use four different performance approach. For example, if the responses to the
metrics for software development projects: budget, controller’s knowledge items are identical within each
schedule, effectiveness (e.g., user satisfaction), and project, but different across projects, then a project-
maintainability (e.g., standards). They examined two independent analysis might find this variable signifi-
organizations and found formal control mechanisms in cant, but a within-project analysis would not, because
place for budget and schedule, no mechanisms for there is no variation within each project. To represent
maintainability, and few mechanisms available for mea- the 32 projects, 31 dummy variables were added to the
suring and controlling project effectiveness. Similarly, regression equations. Where missing values were
Jones (1991) notes that there are wide variations in the found, means were substituted in order to avoid
use of software metrics by various industries, pointing computa- tional problems with the dummy variable
out that some firms measure system quality, user satis- approach. The sample size for each regression is 256
faction, budget, and schedule, while some firms do not. (32 projects x 2 controllers X 4 goals).
The implication of both Jones (1991) and Banker and Besides the three independent variables, goal impor-
Kemerer (1992) is that organizations at times measure tance was also entered into the regression equations as
and track some project goals. This, in turn, suggests a control variable. The measure of goal importance is
that formal control mechanisms are put in place to at the ordinal level. While the inclusion of rank order
ensure that a subset of project goals is met, and it data in regression analyses is not universally accepted,
a number of researchers assert that it is acceptable as
10
O RG ANI ZATI O N S CIEsCu/Vol. 7, No. 1, January—February 1996
Copyright O 2001 All Rights Reserved

You might also like