You are on page 1of 29

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/255712217

Barriers to the dissemination of Empirically Supported Treatments

Article · January 2009

CITATIONS READS
31 418

3 authors:

Bruce Wampold Zac E Imel


University of Wisconsin--Madison and Modum Bad, Norway University of Utah
300 PUBLICATIONS   22,147 CITATIONS    107 PUBLICATIONS   5,461 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Scott D. Miller
International Center for Clinical Excellence
138 PUBLICATIONS   6,301 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Practice-oriented research and research-guided practice at a large university counseling center View project

Common Factors and Mechanisms of Change in Psychotherapy (CAMP) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Scott D. Miller on 29 December 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


ISSN 0278-8403
ASSOCIATION FOR BEHAVIORAL
AND COGNITIVE THERAPIES VOLUME 32, NO. 7 • OCTOBER 2009

the Behavior Therapist


Contents President’s Message
President’s Message Sunk Costs: Backward-
Robert L. Leahy
Sunk Costs: Backward-Looking Decisions • 137 Looking Decisions
Series on Technology Robert L. Leahy, American Institute for
Michael D. Anestis and Joye C. Anestis Cognitive Therapy
Using Social Media Tools in Clinical Psychology:
e are all familiar
The Experience of Psychotherapy Brown Bag • 140

Research Forum
Bruce E. Wampold, Zac E. Imel, and Scott D. Miller
W with the following:
You pay good
money for a suit or dress, take
it home, look at it, and hang it
Barriers to the Dissemination of Empirically Supported in the closet. Years go by, you
Treatments: Matching Messages to the Evidence • 144 take it out and look at it and
say, “It’s not me—I’ll wear something else.” You
Book Review can’t seem to throw it out although you really
know you won’t wear it again. You say, “I can’t
Reviewed by Chad E. Drake throw it out. It’s hardly been used. I paid good
Roemer & Orsillo (2009), Mindfulness- and Acceptance-Based money for it.” Or, more significantly, you have
Behavioral Therapy in Practice • 156 been stuck in a dead-end relationship that has
dragged on for years. You know—“ratio-
Multimedia Journal nally”—it makes sense to get out, but you can’t.
Your friends urge you to look at the costs and
Maureen Whittal benefits of staying versus getting out. You know
Looking to the Future of Cognitive and Behavioral Practice • 157 rationally they are right, but you can’t pull the
plug. In both cases, you are “honoring” the sunk
Letter to the Editor costs of prior decisions. You can’t abandon the
sunk cost because you believe you have to justify
Patti Lou Watkins and George A. Clum why you have stayed in so long. You say, “If I left
Empirically Evaluated Self-Help Therapies • 157 it would mean I wasted all that time”; “I can’t
stand the feeling of loss”; “If I left it would prove
At ABCT I am a failure”; or “You don’t understand. There
really are good things there. I just have to wait
Kristene A. Doyle for things to turn around.” In fact, your reasons
To Lead or Not to Lead? • 159 for staying may continue to change—because
Virginia Rutter you are highly motivated to prove that you are
Three Recipients of the Neil S. Jacobson Research Award
not wrong in staying in the first place. Your deci-
sion is “backward-looking,” attempting to jus-
for Outstanding and Innovative Clinical Research • 160
tify what you have done in the past. Ironically,
CALL for AWARD NOMINATIONS • 161 the longer you stay in, the greater the sunk cost
and the greater the need to justify your decision
WELCOME, NEW MEMBERS • 162 to stay. You are making decisions looking back-

October • 2009 137


the Behavior Therapist
Published by the Association for
Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies
305 Seventh Avenue - 16th Floor
New York, NY 10001-6008
(212) 647-1890 / Fax: (212) 647-1865
www.abct.org

EDITOR · · · · · · · · · · · · Drew Anderson


Editorial Assistant . . . . . . . . Melissa Them
Behavior Assessment . . . Timothy R. Stickle
Book Reviews · · · · · · · · · · · C. Alix Timko
Clinical Forum · · · · · · · · · · · John P. Forsyth
Clinical Dialogues . . . . . . . Brian P. Marx
International Scene . . . . . . . . Rod Holland
Institutional
Settings. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . David Penn
Tamara Penix Sbraga
Lighter Side · · · · · · · · · · · · Elizabeth Moore
List Serve Editor . . . . . . . . Laura E. Dreer
News and Notes. . . . . . . . . David DiLillo
Laura E. Dreer
James W. Sturges
Public Health Issues. . . . Jennifer Lundgren
Research-Practice
Links · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · David J. Hansen
Research-Training
Links · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · · Gayle Y. Iwamasa
Science Forum · · · · · · · · · · · Jeffrey M. Lohr
Special Interest
Groups · · · · · · · · · · Andrea Seidner Burling
Technology Update. . . . . . James A. Carter INSTRUCTIONS Ñçê AUTHORS
ABCT President . . . . . . . Robert L. Leahy The Association for Behavioral and Submissions must be accompanied by
Executive Director · · · · · · Mary Jane Eimer Cognitive Therapies publishes the Behavior a Copyright Transfer Form (a form is
Director of Education & Therapist as a service to its membership. printed on p. 24 of the January 2008 issue
Meeting Services . . . . . . Mary Ellen Brown Eight issues are published annually. The of tBT, or contact the ABCT central of-
Director of Communications David Teisler purpose is to provide a vehicle for the rapid fice): submissions will not be reviewed without
dissemination of news, recent advances, a copyright transfer form. Prior to publication
Managing Editor . . . . . Stephanie Schwartz
and innovative applications in behavior authors will be asked to submit a final
therapy. electronic version of their manuscript.
Copyright © 2009 by the Association for Behavioral Authors submitting materials to tBT do so
and Cognitive Therapies. All rights reserved. No Feature articles that are approximately
part of this publication may be reproduced or trans- 16 double-spaced manuscript pages may with the understanding that the copyright
mitted in any form, or by any means, electronic or of the published materials shall be as-
mechanical, including photocopy, recording, or any be submitted.
information storage and retrieval system, without signed exclusively to ABCT. Submissions
Brief articles, approximately 6 to 12 via e-mail are preferred and should be sent
permission in writing from the copyright owner.
Subscription information: the Behavior Therapist is double-spaced manuscript pages, are to the editor at drewa@albany.edu.
published in 8 issues per year. It is provided free to preferred.
ABCT members. Nonmember subscriptions are Please include the phrase tBT submission
available at $40.00 per year (+$32.00 airmail Feature articles and brief articles in the subject line of your e-mail. Include
postage outside North America). should be accompanied by a 75- to the first author’s e-mail address on the
Change of address: 6 to 8 weeks are required for
address changes. Send both old and new addresses to 100 -word abstract. cover page of the manuscript attachment.
the ABCT office. Letters to the Editor may be used to By conventional mail, please send manu-
ABCT is committed to a policy of equal opportu-
nity in all of its activities, including employment. respond to articles published in the scripts to:
ABCT does not discriminate on the basis of race, Behavior Therapist or to voice a profes- Drew A. Anderson, Ph.D.
color, creed, religion, national or ethnic origin, sex, sional opinion. Letters should be lim-
sexual orientation, gender identity or expression, SUNY–Albany
age, disability, or veteran status. ited to approximately 3 double-spaced Dept. of Psychology/SS369
All items published in the Behavior Therapist, manuscript pages. 1400 Washington Ave.
including advertisements, are for the information of
our readers, and publication does not imply endorse- Albany, NY 12222
ment by the Association.

138
ward to past investments and not making make the trap more familiar (Leahy, 2000). References
decisions based on future utility. Third, you can divide (or bifurcate) the deci-
Arkes, H. R. (1996). The psychology of waste.
Rational decision-making models argue sion: “If you had never gotten into this be- Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 9, 213-
that we make choices based on future util- havior, would you make a decision to get 224.
ity, but evidence for sunk costs suggests that into it now?” Fourth, the patient can exam- Arkes, H. R., & Ayton, P. (1999). The sunk cost
we are often trapped by past commitments ine the justifications and challenges to these and Concorde effects: Are humans less ratio-
and investments. Indeed, the greater the rationalizations: “I have too much invested nal than lower animals? Psychological Bulletin,
sunk cost, the greater the escalation of com- 125, 591-600.
to walk away”; “I now have a responsibility
mitment. There are endless examples of Arkes, H. R., & Blumer, C. (1985). The psychol-
sunk costs. Along with your out-of-style to make it work out”; or “I’m not frivo-
lous—I don’t walk away from my commit- ogy of sunk cost. Organizational Behavior &
jacket or dress, there are sunk costs in rela- Human Decision Processes, 35, 124-140.
tionships, careers, purchases, and even in ments.” These assumptions may be
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive disso-
foreign policy. The Vietnam War is a much- examined utilizing cognitive therapy tech-
nance. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University
agreed-on sunk cost, but when the United niques: “What if you looked at your prior Press.
States was engaged in that war a significant investments as lost costs that you can never Festinger, L. (1961). The psychological effects of
majority of Americans supported the war. recover? How would putting more of your- insufficient rewards. American Psychologist,
An entire nation at times was committed to self into this help you achieve your ultimate 16, 1-11.
honoring sunk costs. President Johnson in goals?” Fifth, you can externalize the deci- Gilovich, T., & Medvec, V. H. (1994). The tem-
fact made the bold sunk-cost justification sion by asking, “What if your friends had to poral pattern to the experience of regret.
for staying, claiming that we couldn’t give make the decision for you? What would Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 67,
up because we had lost so many men. Sunk they decide?” This helps decouple the decider 357-365.
costs are common in behavioral finance from the decision. Sixth, you can identify the Gilovich, T., Medvec, V. H., & Chen, S. (1995).
where investors double-up on a losing in- Commission, omission, and dissonance re-
fear of “wasting,” which often underlies the duction: Coping with regret in the “Monty
vestment to “get their money back.” We
often “ride a loser.” fear that walking away from the sunk cost is Hall” problem. Personality & Social Psychology
Humans are the only animals who honor an admission of having wasted time and re- Bulletin, 21, 182-190.
sunk costs (Arkes & Ayton, 1999). sources. This fear can be addressed by rec- Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to
Laboratory rats may show a burst of activity ognizing that losses (or wasting) are always dispositions: The attribution process in per-
as they face extinction trials when reinforce- involved in decision making, but the self-in- son perception. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
ments have been eliminated, but they terest strategy would be not to throw good Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol.
2, pp. 219-266). New York: Academic Press.
quickly learn to look somewhere else for re- money after bad. Seventh, many people
wards. Why are rats “smarter” than hu- stay in sunk costs because of the fear of hu- Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect
theory: An analysis of decision under risk.
mans? Or are we too smart for our own miliation: “I would be telling everyone that Econometrica, 47, 263-291.
good? Unlike the “rational” rat, humans I was wrong and they were right.” This con- Kiesler, C. A., Nisbett, R. E., & Zanna, M. P.
appear condemned to continually reflect on cern can be addressed by recognizing that (1969). On inferring one’s beliefs from one’s
their past decisions, attempting to make most friends will be happy to have you behavior. Journal of Personality & Social
“sense” of them and to justify their future agree with them and happy that you are out Psychology, 11, 321-327.
decisions by reference to the past. Honoring
of your misery but, in the event that they Leahy, R. L. (2000). Sunk costs and resistance to
sunk costs can be explained by loss aversion change. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: an
use this as a reason for criticism, it may be a
(Wilson, Arvai, & Arkes, 2008), commit- International Quarterly, 14, 355-371.
ment theory (Kiesler, Nisbett, & Zanna, price worth paying to cut your losses.
Eighth, some people fear the flood of nega- Leahy, R. L. (2004). Decision making processes
1969), cognitive-dissonance theory and psychopathology. In R. L. Leahy (Ed.),
(Festinger, 1957, 1961), prospect theory tive feelings following abandoning a sunk
Contemporary cognitive therapy: Theory, research,
and loss frames (Kahneman & Tversky, cost. This can be addressed by an analogy of and practice (pp. 116-138). New York:
1979), fear of wasting (Arkes, 1996; Arkes pulling a splint from a toe. It hurts until it Guilford Press.
& Blumer, 1985), attribution processes (for stops hurting and then it is followed by re- Wilson, R. S., Arvai, J. L., & Arkes, H. R. (2008).
example, Jones & Davis, 1965), and inac- lief. Being stuck in a sunk cost is the ulti- My loss is your loss...sometimes: Loss aver-
tion inertia (Gilovich & Medvec, 1994; mate helplessness and is a guarantee for sion and the effect of motivational biases.
Gilovich, Medvec, & Chen, 1995). In each depression, anger, and hopelessness (Leahy, Risk Analysis, 28, 929-938.
case it is the absence of reward that makes 2000, 2004).
this puzzling until we recognize that it is the . . .
We often get trapped by our need to jus-
“interpretation” of change and the “need to tify the decisions that have continued our
explain” the past that keeps us trapped. misery—looking backward to the past for Correspondence to Robert L. Leahy, Ph.D.,
How can we liberate ourselves from the American Institute for Cognitive Therapy, 136
justification, rather than committing to ac-
sunk cost trap? First, standard cognitive E. 57th St., Suite 1101, New York, NY
therapy inquiries can be used, but may lead tions for future utility. Focusing on valued
10022; Leahy@CognitiveTherapyNYC.com
nowhere. For example, one can ask about goals rather than valuing our past can help
the costs and benefits of continuing in the liberate us from commitments whose pay-
course of action. Patients often say, “I know offs have turned into deficits. Our “rational-
it’s irrational, but I can’t get out.” Second, ity” may be less logical than we think and
educating the patient about sunk costs (the more determined by rationalizing the past
jacket in the closet) immediately helps to rather than pursuing a better future.

October • 2009 139


Series on Technology stand that our colleagues are extremely
busy and that our invitations could serve as a
bit of a burden, so we have had to develop
Using Social Media Tools in Clinical tactful, understanding approaches to mak-
ing such offers in order to entice contribu-
Psychology: The Experience of tors without becoming a bother. Our efforts
to develop a steady list of guest contributors
Psychotherapy Brown Bag are still ongoing and, as such, the workload
in these early days of the business is fairly
Michael D. Anestis and Joye C. Anestis, Psychotherapy Brown Bag high as we take on the vast majority of the
and Florida State University writing.
As with any new online business, one of
our initial priorities was to maximize traffic
E DITOR ’ S N OTE : This article marks ing tools (e.g., Typepad versus Blogger) and to the site and general knowledge of our
the first of a series of articles looking at po- the best way to obtain a domain name, and goals. We found ourselves somewhat un-
tential uses of new technologies for ABCT we spent a considerable amount of our early sure about how to develop a following for
members. If you have any ideas or sugges- PBB days brainstorming on the design of the site, as there did not seem to be an obvi-
the site—but these activities were actually ous method by which to alert a wide audi-
tions for articles, please send me an email.
quite enjoyable, as they marked the begin- ence to our project. Granted, a significant
—D.A. portion of the global population suffers with
ning of an exciting new business venture in
which we wholeheartedly believe and to or knows an individual suffering with a
s a pair of advanced graduate stu-

A dents trained in the clinical science


tradition, the struggles of dissemi-
nating empirically supported treatments
which we are fervently devoted. In our first 6
months, we have incurred some costs, as we
invested in professional services to design a
banner for the page and optimize our visi-
mental illness, but the general knowledge
regarding ESTs for mental illness and re-
search findings in clinical psychology is lim-
ited enough that a website with our goals
(ESTs) in a world full of misinformation has must seek out readers rather than simply
often been at the forefront of our minds. In bility in search engines (e.g., Google), but
we counter these costs through the use of scoop up an already existing audience. The
fact, we have at times been downright de- constraints of the graduate student wallet
moralized that all of this great research and advertisements and the creation of an online
store run in cooperation with Amazon prevented us from utilizing high-cost, more
these effective treatments exist, yet so few traditional advertising opportunities. As
people know about them. We knew we through which we recommend research-
based books, DVDs, and Kindle products such, we needed to find an alternative way
wanted to do something about it, but found to promote our services in a cost-effective
ourselves unsure how to make a difference. about psychology.
PBB is updated every weekday, exclud- manner that would enable us to reach as
After years of brainstorming ways in which broad an audience a possible, encompassing
we could effectively sing the praises of ESTs ing holidays and vacations, and we take
both professionals and nonprofessionals.
to a wide audience while we are on graduate turns supplying articles. One of the lucky
This is where social media tools became cru-
student wallets (which are slim) and sched- things about running a business with your
cial in our efforts.
ules (which are full), we decided to stick our spouse is that your business partner always
Our first attempt at publicity was simple
toes into the world of blogs (although we knows your work availability, so it is easy to
word of mouth. Through conversations and
tend to say “online magazine”) and distribute tasks between us! The average ar- e-mails, we alerted family, friends, and col-
launched Psychotherapy Brown Bag ticle takes us approximately 1 to 3 hours to leagues. This resulted in substantial encour-
(http://www.psychotherapybrownbag.com) write. For some this might seem like too agement, but only a small flurry of visits to
on March 1, 2009. The central aims of PBB much time outside of our academic respon- the site. Our next step was to create a pres-
are to disseminate research findings in clini- sibilities, but to us it is definitely worth it. ence on Facebook (www.facebook.com), a
cal psychology and information on ESTs for We also welcome articles from anyone who social media website that allows individuals
mental illness to the general public (both wants to write one, as long as the informa- to follow the lives of others as well as to re-
clinicians and consumers, thus ostensibly tion discussed is consonant with the goals of ceive news regarding causes, individuals,
helping to bridge the research-practice gap) PBB. On the first weekday of every month, and organizations of interest (e.g., political
and to help individuals find local clinics that we post a guest article from a distinguished figures, celebrities, support groups for par-
provide such services. member of the clinical psychology commu- ticular illnesses). We already had experience
The actual creation of PBB was far sim- nity—a professor, researcher, or clinician with Facebook, having maintained personal
pler than we ever anticipated. In fact, the who is willing to contribute. Past featured pages for several years and being involved
most difficult part of the process was com- contributors have included Robert Leahy, with several organizations via the website
ing up with a plan for continuously updat- the current president of ABCT, as well as (including ABCT). Creating a Facebook
ing the website and selecting a name. Once Craig Bryan of the United States Air Force, presence for Psychotherapy Brown Bag was
those decisions were made, the rest was easy. Jill Holm-Denoma of the University of the next logical step. We created two types of
We selected a blogging service, purchased a Denver, Sarah Fischer of the University of pages: a group (285 members as of
domain name, and PBB was born! In all, Georgia, and several other notable profes- 9/8/2009) and a fan page (150 fans as of
about 3 weeks lapsed between the day we sionals. Perhaps our greatest obstacle has 9/8/2009). Facebook has provided an av-
began researching this endeavor and the been our own anxiety about approaching enue through which individuals we might
launching of the website. We did have to others to write for the site. As graduate stu- not otherwise meet could discover PBB,
spend time researching the best blog host- dents in a demanding program, we under- glance at links to our articles, recommend

140 the Behavior Therapist


Behavior Therapy Associates, P.A.
Proudly Celebrates Its 30th Anniversary
1979–2009

Behavior Therapy Associates, P.A.


Towne Park Professional Center
35 Clyde Rd., Suite 101
Somerset, NJ 08873

Steven B. Gordon, Ph.D., ABPP Michael C. Selbst, Ph.D.


Director Associate Director
Board Certified in Cognitive Certified School Psychologist
and Behavioral Psychology NJ Lic. #3779, PA #9320
Clinical Psychologist
NJ Lic. #936

Michael J. Asher, Ph.D., ABPP Debra G. Salzman, Ph.D. Mark Cooperberg, Ph.D.
Board Certified in Cognitive Clinical Psychologist Clinical Psychologist
and Behavioral Psychology NJ Lic. #3160 NJ Lic. #4365, PA #15827
Clinical Psychologist
NJ Lic. #2792

Postdoctoral Fellows
Rory Panter, Psy.D.
Lisa Spano, Psy.D., BCBA

BTA provides clinical services, consultation, and training


using evidence based approaches within the context of
sensitivity and compassion, while recognizing the
uniqueness of each individual and setting.

PHONE: 732-873-1212 • FAX: 732-873-2584 • E-MAIL: behaviortherapy@aol.com


www.behaviortherapyassociates.com

October • 2009 141


the site to others, and communicate with the process of writing guest articles for our
Study Professional Psychology us. After we publish a new post on site on areas that are outside of our areas of
in the Pacific Northwest Psychotherapy Brown Bag, we alert indi- expertise but consistent with the overall
viduals following us through Facebook by goals of PBB. So just like Facebook, Twitter
PSY.D. in posting a link to our new article. Each has afforded us an opportunity to reach a
C l iniCal week, we email all Facebook users following
our group page and provide a list of the
global audience with little effort and no cost
on our part.
PS Y C h ologY week’s articles as well as any news pieces of Our third and final significant interac-
interest. With just a small amount of effort tion with social media tools has been with
on our part and at absolutely no cost, Reddit (http://www.reddit.com). Individu-
• APA accredited Facebook almost does the work for us, as als sign up for Reddit and post links either
Psy.D. degree fans of PBB alert their friends to our pages to the general site or to one of the subhead-
and services and those friends pass along in- ings. In our case, the psychology subheading
• Practitioner-scholar formation again, and so on. (http://www.reddit.com/r
model; contemporary From here, our next move was to create a /psychology) has proven to be the most use-
curriculum presence on a similar social media site, al- ful. As of September 8, 2009, the psychol-
beit one with which we had less personal ex- ogy subheading of Reddit had over 8,600
• School-operated perience: Twitter (http://www.twitter.com). subscribers. Subscribers click on links and
training clinic and Twitter allows users to post links and com- evaluate them by providing a score, either a
APA-accredited ments and to follow the comments of other plus or minus one. Links thus receive a total
internship program individuals. In doing so, individuals can also score based upon the summed opinions of
follow global conversations on particular readers. The higher the score, the further up
• Near Portland, Mt. topics (e.g., depression, a particular on the list of headlines it appears, thus in-
Hood, and the celebrity, a sporting event) or simply engage creasing the likelihood that it will be seen
Oregon coast in conversations with an individual or group and read by a greater number of individuals
of individuals on a smaller scale. Each mes- (although there are separate tabs for “dis-
Faculty interests include: sage is brief— limited to a total of 140 char- liked” and “controversial” links as well for
assessment, behavior therapy, child acters—so each post essentially serves as a individuals who wish to search for articles
headline. It was our belief that by entering not necessarily popular with other readers).
psychopathology, empirically sup-
these conversations and posting links to our Subscribers also have the option of posting
ported treatments, forensic psychol- articles, we could increase the degree to comments regarding the link, thus provid-
ogy, health psychology, integra- which individuals know about, visit, and in- ing an additional opportunity to interact
tive approaches, neuropsychology, teract with our site. This belief has thus far with readers, clarify any confusion, and as-
organizational behavior, bilingual proven to be true. We were recently named sess which topics might interest readers
psychotherapy with Latinos, psycho- as one of the top 100 science resources on more than others. Thus far, readers have
therapy with minorities, and single Twitter by OnlineCourses.org and, as of been directed to our site through Reddit
case research. September 8, 2009, we have more than from every continent except Antarctica,
1,800 individuals and organizations follow- with particularly heavy traffic in Europe. As
■■
ing our daily updates. That’s nearly 2,000 such, we have managed to develop new
CONTACT US AT: people who are receiving daily information connections with individuals we would oth-
Pacific University on ESTs—quite a success, in our opinion! erwise not have had any opportunity to
Office of Admissions Additionally, Twitter has developed a social meet while simultaneously accomplishing
HPC/Pacific University code by which individuals interact with one our goal of disseminating accurate, re-
another. When one individual finds another search-based information on psychopathol-
222 SE 8th Avenue, Ste 212 individual’s post interesting, he or she ogy and psychological treatments. Again,
Hillsboro, OR 97123 “retweets” the comment. Retweeting is a we reiterate this was low-effort and entirely
503-352-2218 slang term that refers to making the same free advertising.
800-933-9308 comment as another poster, crediting the Overall, our experience with social
admissions@pacificu.edu original poster for the comment (including media tools has been positive. Traffic on our
a link to that poster’s profile), and thus website continues to increase substantially,
making the comment visible to all people enabling us to reach hundreds of readers per
following his or her feed. As such, a single day. We remain optimistic that these tools
comment can be made visible to thousands will continue to help our company grown
of individuals in mere moments. This has and to reach a broader global audience. We
proven to be a highly valuable form of free have also begun experimenting with addi-
advertising. Additionally, because individu- tional social media tools such as Digg
als only follow the comments of people they (http://www.digg.com) and StumbleUpon
choose, the site provides an opportunity for (http://www.stumbleupon.com) and antici-
like-minded individuals to network and de- pate experimenting with others as we learn
velop professional connections. In this about them. It remains to be seen whether
sense, we have found several individuals these sites are as useful as traditional adver-
who have expressed an interest in or begun tising methods, but without question, they

142 the Behavior Therapist


are a quick and cost-effective means to interpret and links to alternative sources
through which to increase awareness of a of information that might explain the topic Study Evidence-Based Therapies
cause, issue, company, or service and to tar- in a manner more easily understood by the in the Pacific Northwest
get a particular audience likely to interact reader. In fact, we are not the only website
with whatever is being marketed. Perhaps out there promoting mental health care and Master of arts in
most importantly, utilizing social media
tools can help a website like ours or even a
we frequently link to such resources as we
learn about them. Given the sensitive na- C ou n s e l in g
therapist in private practice to keep up with ture of much of the material covered by PsyCholo g y
current trends in the field, issues that are clinical psychologists, whether they work as
gaining significant interest, and prominent therapists, researchers, or educators, such
names receiving attention on particular considerations are pivotal, otherwise read-
• Two-year program
topics. Although the goal of our site is to ers will be hurt and, in all likelihood, turn to with late afternoon
provide articles on a comprehensive variety other sources for information that might and evening classes
of psychology-related topics, we also strive rely less upon research and, as such, be more
to ensure that readers are able to find a mul- likely to encounter misinformation. A final • Emphasis on
titude of articles on topics directly related to consideration for professionals considering Evidence-Based
their interests. By interacting with readers incorporating social media tools into their Practices in Counseling
on social media websites, we have been able lives is the issue of personal privacy. We un- (Child and Adult)
to develop a greater sense of what topics derstand from discussions with colleagues and Elective Training
currently spark the most interest amongst (for example, subscribers to the ABCT list-
the widest array of individuals, thus helping serve) that many professionals are nervous in Organizational
to capture a larger audience likely to return about having an online presence because of Behavior and Latino
to the site and engage with other materials the potential for blurred therapist-client Mental Health
we publish. boundaries. This is a legitimate concern,
Despite all of the positive aspects of so- but one that can be avoided by being smart • Meets Oregon
cial media tools, we remain cautious with and careful in one’s use of these technolo- LPC educational
respect to how we present ourselves on gies. For example, on Facebook, we have requirements
these websites, as we believe there are sev- been able to maintain both professional and
eral potential risks that must be considered. personal identities by taking advantage of Faculty members are experienced
Perhaps the greatest of these risks is the po- the many privacy options that Facebook in both practice and research. Their
tential for individuals in crisis to misinter- provides. We have limited what the public interests include: behavioral and
pret our services and seek emergency can see on our personal sites, while keeping cognitive behavioral therapy, child
interventions through our website or our the Psychotherapy Brown Bag pages open and adolescent psychopathology,
profiles on various social media tools. As to the public (see the Facebook blog post,
anxiety and mood disorders, orga-
such, we are careful to clearly state that “10 Privacy Settings Every Facebook User
Psychotherapy Brown Bag is an educational Should Know” for details on how to do this: nizational behavior, program evalu-
tool, not a therapy provider, and to provide http://www.allfacebook.com/2009/02/face- ation, mindfulness-based therapies,
resources such as the National Suicide book-privacy/). Other social media sites can and multicultural counseling.
Prevention Lifeline (1-800-273-TALK) for also be navigated and protected in a similar ■■
individuals in crisis. This is an equally im- manner. CONTACT US AT:
portant consideration for therapists consid- In weighing the costs and benefits of uti-
ering utilizing social media tools, as it is lizing social media tools, there is one final Pacific University
entirely possible that individuals seeking point worthy of consideration: regardless of Office of Admissions
immediate help will stumble upon a web- whether or not a particular psychologist HPC/Pacific University
site and mistakenly interpret it as a source chooses to utilize these tools, other individu- 222 SE 8th Avenue, Ste 212
of help when, in fact, better alternatives als will make the choice to do so. Hillsboro, OR 97123
exist for their immediate situation. An addi- Unfortunately, many of those individuals
tional risk—one more unique to the ser- will offer ineffective therapeutic interven- 503-352-2218
vices provided by Psychotherapy Brown tions or attempt to perpetuate misinforma- 800-933-9308
Bag—is the potential that individuals will tion. These individuals will, in large part, admissions@pacificu.edu
misinterpret our postings in a manner that not do so out of malice, but rather igno-
would result in the proliferation of misinfor- rance. Whether this represents a lack of ed-
mation or, on a more personal level, hurt ucation regarding research methods and
feelings. Translating complex research find- data analysis, a philosophical devotion to a
ings published in fairly esoteric psychology particular therapeutic modality not sup-
journals into prose more consistent with ported by research, or a bad personal experi-
what one would read in a popular magazine ence with an otherwise effective treatment
can be difficult, as some topics require a sig- approach, the end result is the same: a
nificant amount of background information voice, widely accessible to the public, mak-
in order to be explained clearly. As such, we ing claims that contradict empirical facts.
take care to provide several different expla- Readers—consumers of psychological ser-
nations for findings that might be difficult vices and information—lack sure-fire

October • 2009 143


methods by which to determine who is pro- In this sense, regardless of whether or not a Correspondence to Michael D. Anestis or
viding accurate information and who is pro- clinical psychologist finds him- or herself in- Joye C. Anestis, Florida State University, 953,
viding false hope. As such, if there are more terested in social media tools, it could be ar- Parkview Dr., Tallahassee, FL 32311
individuals utilizing social media tools who gued that it is beholden upon them to mikeanestis@psychotherapybrownbag.com
are peddling misinformation and doing so engage in this new source of knowledge in joyeanestis@psychotherapybrownbag.com
on a large stage in a charismatic manner, an effort to provide a voice to research and
consumers will be defenseless and the pub- ESTs for mental illness. Psychotherapy
lic will remain in the dark regarding data- Brown Bag is lending its voice to the de-
driven conceptualizations of mental illness bate, and we hope others will join in the ef-
and the most effective means for treating it. fort.

Research Forum Psychotherapy Treatments


and a Possible Future
The history of psychotherapy is charac-
Barriers to the Dissemination of Empirically terized by efforts to promote particular the-
Supported Treatments: Matching Messages to oretical perspectives (Cushman, 1992;
Fancher, 1995; Wampold, in press). The
the Evidence claims of the superiority of one method over
another were endemic from the origins of
psychotherapy, as Freud and his disciples ve-
Bruce E. Wampold, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Zac E. Imel, VA Puget
hemently argued about theory and practice
Sound Healthcare System–Seattle Division and University of Washington, and (Makari, 2008). The disputes proliferated as
Scott D. Miller, International Center for Clinical Excellence the behaviorists criticized the psychoana-
lysts, the humanists took a different tack al-
together, cognitive constructs were
iven the complexity of the thera- treatments, have been limited by percep- accommodated by behavioral theories

G peutic endeavor, it is not surprising


that interpreting the evidence is
complex—if it were not, the debate sur-
tions that all psychotherapies are equally ef-
fective [the Dodo Bird verdict], and …
‘that common factors, therapist, and rela-
tionship variables account for the majority
(Fishman & Franks, 1992), a third wave of
“acceptance based” behavioral treatments
emerged (Hayes, 2004), and integrationists
attempted to reconcile deficiencies in uni-
rounding empirically supported treatments
(EST) would be inconspicuously absent. of the variance in therapy outcome studies’“ tary theories (Norcross & Goldfried, 2005).
Evidence is not simply observation of phe- (as quoted in Siev, Huppert, & Chambless, These efforts have resulted in hundreds and
nomena, regardless of whether the observa- 2009, p. 69). DiGiuseppe offered three al- hundreds of approaches to psychotherapy.
tions were derived in experimentally ternatives: “Either we rebut these conclu- Norcross and Newman (1992) said it aptly:
manipulated environments (e.g., random- sions, conduct new research to show they
ized controlled trials [RCTs]) or naturalistic are wrong, or we accept them and change Rivalry among theoretical orientations has a
settings. Rather, evidence involves the in- our message” (as quoted in Siev et al., p. long and undistinguished history in psy-
ferences that flow from observations. What 69). chotherapy, dating back to Freud. In the in-
constitutes evidence is ultimately decided Siev et al. (2009) have accepted the fancy of the field, therapy systems, like
by a confluence of two factors—the phe- premise that barriers to the dissemination of battling siblings, competed for attention
nomenon itself and people (Hacking, 1983; ESTs are due to the rhetorical talents of the and affection in a “dogma eat dogma” envi-
Latour, 1999). The phenomenon, under dodo birders and have chosen to rebut what ronment... Mutual antipathy and exchange
various environmental conditions, is ob- they believe are misguided conclusions of puerile insults between adherents of rival
served by people (i.e., the scientists), who about the nature of psychotherapy. orientations were much the order of the day.
then draw conclusions about the phenome- Although their article purports to “respond (p. 3)
non. The road from observation to conclu- to these contentions and to present an up-
sion is saturated with social influences on date on recent research bearing directly on A seemingly reasonable way to settle
the scientist. Ultimately, it is the scientific the Dodo Bird verdict” (p. 69), it mostly re- disputes between rival schools is to sift and
community that decides which conclusions capitulates old criticisms and ignores con- winnow the various approaches based on
are valid and disseminated. tradictory, and at times vast, evidence. their efficacy—treatments that produce
Accordingly, publications are, in some Barriers to the dissemination of ESTs are not demonstrable benefits should be preferred
sense, rhetorical devices, the purpose of the result of sophistry, but a rational reac- to others. Indeed, such logic gave birth to
which is to influence the scientific commu- tion to an interpretation of the evidence. and guided the EST movement (Chambless
nity about what is the “proper” evidence We take this opportunity to present evi- & Hollon, 1998; Task Force on Promotion
(Latour, 1999). With respect to the ongoing dence that has been omitted and address and Dissemination of Psychological
EST debate, Raymond DiGiuseppe (2007) several important questions raised by Siev Procedures, 1995). Unfortunately, agree-
recently lamented that “efforts to dissemi- et al. ment about both the type and meaning of
nate empirically supported treatments the evidence has proven to be more complex
(ESTs), and especially cognitive-behavioral than anticipated.

144 the Behavior Therapist


The difficulty in using evidence from affect in the rational condition), the thera-
what many consider the “gold standard” of pist redirected the patient to their cogni-
sources—RCTs of psychological treat- tions about an event if the patient expressed
ments—is illustrated by a taking a hypo- emotion. Additionally, therapists were pro-
thetical visit to the future. scribed from using language that referenced
emotion. The patient was encouraged to ex-
A Hypothetical Future:
The Ascendance of Affect
plore their thoughts about events in their
lives and the therapist was instructed not to
POrtlAnd dBt
In 2020, due to advances in affective focus on particular themes, but rather allow PrOgrAM, PC
neuroscience (see Davidson, Sherer, & the patient to talk about what he or she 5200 SW Macadam Ave. Ste. 580
Goldsmith, 2003), Federico Perla devel- thought was important. Perla trained the Portland, Oregon 97239
oped a new psychotherapy, which he named four therapists used in Study 1 to conduct
RC as well as ACP and supervised all treat- DBT is a research-supported treatment
affect-centered psychotherapy (ACP). The that combines cognitive-behavioral
premise of ACP is that affect evolved to reg- ment. One therapist was removed prior to
the study because she could not adhere to theory and methods with Eastern
ulate social interactions in nonhuman and meditative principles and practices.
eventually human animals. The cognitions the requirement to ignore affect in the RC
condition. Patients were randomly assigned The Portland DBT Program provides
generated by individuals are post-hoc ex- treatment for adults, teens, couples,
planations of experience that interfere with to conditions and adherence measures
showed adequate fidelity. It was found that and families including:
the encoding and decoding of emotion, • Standard DBT
which in turn leads to dysfunction. ACP in- ACP was superior to RC on measures of
anxiety but was not definitively superior on • DBT for Adolescents
volves a systematic program to emphasize • DBT for Eating Disorders
secondary measures of depression and qual-
primary emotional responding and mini- • DBT for Substance Abuse
ity of life.
mize cognitive involvement. Similar to • Trauma Recovery Program
Perla, having shown that ACP was supe-
other therapies, ACP contains a number of • Medication Management Services
rior to a treatment that contained the com-
elements that are considered important and • DBT-informed Couples/Family Therapy
mon factors (i.e., a relationship with a
necessary but not constitutive of the treat- • DBT Training/Consultation Services
therapist), was convinced that the focus on
ment (Grünbaum, 1981), such as a rela-
affect was critical to the successful treat- For more information please visit our
tionship with a therapist, a cogent
ment of GAD, sought to show that it was website at www.PortlandDBT.com
explanation, therapeutic actions, and ex-
superior to a well-accepted treatment, or call 503-290-3291
pectations for change. Perla, a charismatic
namely cognitive-behavioral treatment
and persuasive scientist-practitioner, having
(CBT) of GAD, a well-established EST for Subsequently, several other groups com-
successfully treated many anxiety disorders
GAD (Chambless et al., 1998). However, to pared ACP to CBT and found divergent re-
with ACP, designed three successive RCTs
ensure that CBT did not work through any sults. In one study, CBT was superior to
to establish the efficacy and specificity of
mechanisms related to the encoding or de- ACP on GAD measures, in two studies
ACP.
coding of emotion, the therapists adminis- there were no significant differences, and on
Programmatic Research in ACP tering CBT, like those in the prior study
a fourth, ACP was superior to CBT on
using RC, were proscribed from engaging
The first study involved a comparison of GAD measures, although the size of the ef-
in any conversations about emotion, or
ACP to a waitlist condition. Using four of using any affective language or display of fect was smaller than in Perla’s study (viz., d
his therapists, Perla randomized 30 patients emotional responses, and, finally, instructed = .32 vs. d = 1.02, respectively). No differ-
with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) to to redirect any affect-laden material arising ences in studies were found on ancillary
one of the two conditions. Not surprisingly, in session to cognitions. The basic CBT pro- measures. Perla reasoned that in the studies
ACP was superior to no treatment on tar- tocol was otherwise left unchanged. A wait- in which CBT was equivalent to ACP, CBT
geted measures (i.e., measures related to list control group was included as well. worked because the therapists were not pro-
GAD) and on other measures (e.g., mea- Additionally, the same three therapists con- scribed from discussing affect and that the
sures of depression and quality of life). ducted both ACP and CBT. Because Perla affective components of CBT were responsi-
Encouraged by the results and con- received training in the United States by a ble for the benefits of CBT. Moreover, he
vinced that the focus on emotion was criti- prominent CBT therapist, he trained the claimed that when CBT was superior to
cal to the successful treatment of GAD, therapists in CBT and supervised both ther- ACP, ACP was not delivered in an adequate
Perla designed a trial that would control for apists in both modalities. Patients were ran- way. A meta-analysis of these studies
the common factors. The control condition domly assigned to both treatments. showed that the aggregate effect size for the
in this case was called rational counseling As in the prior studies, fidelity measures
five studies was significantly larger than
(RC), which involved an engaged therapist showed the treatments were delivered ac-
who probed the patient to express how they zero (d = .36) for GAD measures with no
cording to their respective protocols. On
thought about events in their lives; the thera- measures of GAD symptoms, ACP was su- differences on other variables of psychologi-
pist reinforced the verbalizations about cog- perior to CBT (d = 1.02); there were few cal functioning. Based on the totality of the
nitions, but did not convey any value about significant differences between ACP and results, it was concluded that (a) ACP
the rationality of the thoughts (i.e., did not CBT on ancillary measures, including de- should be designated as an EST, (b) ACP
discriminate between adaptive and mal- pression and quality of life. Both treatments works through the specific ingredient of at-
adaptive thoughts). To establish internal va- were superior to the no-treatment condition tending to affect, (c) ACP is preferred as a
lidity (i.e., control for potential focus on on all measures. treatment of GAD to other treatments.

October • 2009 145


Problematic Conclusions both treatments by the developer of ACP. clusion that a focus on affect is a specific in-
This failure of the double-blind is further gredient in the treatment of GAD.
The history and conclusions about ACP
exacerbated by the fact that the comparison Having briefly visited the past and fu-
closely resemble developments in modern
treatment, RC, is easily identified as a sham. ture of research on psychological treat-
clinical practice—and are equally problem-
For reasons of internal validity rather than ments, we now return to the present. In the
atic. Anyone familiar with research on psy-
quality of care, the therapists in the RC con- following sections, we apply the lessons
chotherapy will not find the results of the
dition were proscribed from actions that learned to the evidence reviewed by Siev et
first trial, where ACP proved superior to no
would be reasonable to most therapists al. (2009) and others (e.g., Crits-Christoph,
treatment, surprising. Every reasonable
(e.g., could not use language that referred 1997; Howard, Krause, Saunders, &
treatment administered to patients seeking
to affect). What consumer of mental health Kopta, 1997).
treatment has been found to be effective rel-
services would seek out a practitioner in any
ative to no treatment, including such treat- The Complete Evidence Base:
profession who believed the services being
ments as eye movement desensitization and The Dodo Bird Redux
offered were bogus? And yet, this is exactly
reprocessing and present-centered therapy
the case with the RC condition—therapists Siev et al.’s (2009) criticism of the dodo
for PTSD (e.g., McDonagh et al., 2005; F.
knew they were delivering a service that was bird conclusion that psychological treat-
Shapiro, 1989). More importantly, studies
not intended to be therapeutic. As such, the ments are equally effective is based largely
comparing a treatment to a no-treatment
finding that ACP was more effective is on two studies: (a) a meta-analysis pub-
control do not indicate whether the sup-
hardly surprising! lished almost 13 years ago (viz., Wampold,
posed “active” or specific ingredients of the
The third trial, in which it was found Mondin, Moody, Stich, et al., 1997) that
particular treatment are responsible for the
that ACP was superior to CBT, would rea- corroborated the dodo bird conjecture, and
change (Wampold, 2001). In the case of
sonably elicit the skepticism of CBT re- (b) a new meta-analysis (viz., Siev &
ACP, the benefits may be due to the fact
searchers. There are several consequential Chambless, 2007) that purportedly shows
that ACP delivers a treatment that has a co-
problems here. First, as was the case previ- the dodo bird conclusion to be false. Briefly,
gent rationale, creates positive expecta-
ously, the therapists had a clear allegiance to the study by Wampold, Mondin, Moody,
tions, involves a relationship with a skilled
ACP—they worked with Perla, were Stich, et al., utilized a test of effect size ho-
therapist who collaborates with the patient
trained by Perla, and were supervised by mogeneity to meta-analytically review all
to set treatment goals and involves reason-
Perla. Second, the CBT protocol was altered direct comparisons of psychotherapies and
able tasks related to those goals (i.e., the
to obviate any work on emotions, which, found that the results were consistent with
components that form the working al-
while justifiable as a research operation in- the dodo bird conclusion: There was not
liance)—all factors known to be related to
tended to tighten internal validity, an emo- sufficient evidence to reject the null hypoth-
successful psychotherapy (Anderson,
tionless CBT does not represent treatment esis that the effect size for the comparison of
Lunnen, & Ogles, in press; Imel &
as it would be delivered by a competent various treatments was zero. Said another
Wampold, 2008).
clinician. Moreover, the only significant dif- way, no evidence for differences in efficacy
The second clinical trial, which com-
ferences found were in the area of GAD between psychological treatments were
pared ACP to RC, is also problematic. Over
symptoms—both treatments were equally found. For many, particularly the advocates
the years, the RCT has become the “gold
effective in terms of addressing depression of ESTs and, for that matter, anyone who
standard” of research in psychotherapy and
and quality of life, which raises the issue of believes that the specific ingredients of par-
medicine. As most know, an essential ele-
whether ACP is truly superior to CBT or ticular treatments are responsible for the
ment of the RCT in medicine is the double
whether, as operationalized in this study, benefits of psychotherapy, this is a challeng-
blind; both the patient and the provider of
ACP is more focused on GAD symptoms, ing conclusion.
treatment are unaware of whether they are
raising the specter of reactivity of the mea- Although the conclusions of the
providing the real or sham treatment. To
sures (i.e., ACP was more focused on symp- Wampold, Mondin, Moody, Stich, et al.
point out the obvious, RCTs in psychother-
toms of GAD). (1997) meta-analysis were consistent with
apy are not double blinded (Seligman,
The superiority of ACP to CBT was previous ones that examined the dodo bird
1995; Wampold, 2001). Indeed, as
based on only five studies and a reanalysis of conjecture (Grissom, 1994; D. Shapiro &
Seligman has noted, “Whenever you hear
these trials shows that the effect size for Shapiro, 1982a, 1982b; Smith, Glass, &
someone demanding the double-blind
GAD symptoms was due primarily to the Miller, 1980; see Wampold, 2001, for a re-
study of psychotherapy, hold on to your
one study conducted by Perla comparing view), a number of important issues that
wallet” (p. 965). Unlike medicine, where a
ACP and CBT (i.e., the Perla result was an limited the generalizability of the results
pill that looks, tastes, and even mimics the
outlier). Indeed, when the Perla study was were raised. In their article, Siev et al.
side effects of the experimental drug makes
removed from the meta-analysis, the aggre- (2009) combined these earlier criticisms of
it difficult for providers to tell the difference
gate effect size was not significantly differ- the Wampold, Mondin, Moody, Stich, et al.
between the experimental and control
ent from zero. Thus, in terms of GAD study with findings from the more recent
treatments, control psychotherapies are
symptoms, it would be difficult to conclude meta-analysis by Siev and Chambless
easy to identify. In the ACP versus RC com-
that ACP was superior to CBT. The equiva- (2007), identifying four specific method-
parison, the therapists knew about both
lence of ACP and CBT for ancillary mea- ological flaws with the evidence in support
treatments they were delivering (i.e., the
sures adds strength to the conclusion that of the dodo verdict: (a) meta-analyses that
design was crossed). In addition, the thera-
there may be negligible differences between aggregate data across disorders masks dif-
pists were knowledgeable of the hypothesis
ACP and CBT. This meta-analysis provides ferences between specific treatments for
regarding the two therapies, had an alle-
little evidence to suggest that CBT is not in- specific disorders; (b) meta-analyses that
giance to one of those approaches (viz.,
dicated for GAD. In practical terms, there aggregate primary and secondary outcome
ACP), and were trained and supervised in
is insufficient evidence to support the con-

146 the Behavior Therapist


measures preclude the discovery of any spe- order (PTSD; Benish et al., 2008), depres- duced small to moderate effects in favor of
cific effects for targeted treatments; (c) the sion (Wampold, Minami, Baskin, & CBT (d = .38, .32, and .20) or small effect in
similarity between the treatments included Tierney, 2002), GAD (Siev & Chambless, favor of RT (d = -.20). Indeed, there was
in the supporting meta-analyses accounts 2007), and pediatric disorders (Miller, relatively large heterogeneity among the ef-
for the failure to find differences in out- Wampold, & Varhely, 2008, when alle- fects but the hypothesis of homogeneity
come; and (d) the way these same meta- giance was controlled, which is discussed in was not rejected because five studies yields a
analyses classify treatments as bona fide is a following section; Spielmans, Pasek, & drastically underpowered test of homo-
circular, eliminating treatments that are not McFall, 2007). geneity (Siev and Chambless calculated
efficacious. These issues are not new and The new meta-analysis cited by Siev et 37% of the variability in effects was due to
have indeed been helpful in guiding and al. (2009) as evidence that “broad judg- between study variability, although we cal-
structuring a program of systematic re- ments about the relative importance of culated 50%).
search conducted over the last decade. As technique … can be misleading” (p. 75) was But here is what is very important: The
will be discussed below, Siev et al. failed to conducted by Siev and Chambless (2007). advantage to CBT in the Siev and
cite this body of research, thereby present- Briefly, the study compared CBT and relax- Chambless (2007) meta-analysis was en-
ing an incomplete picture of the evidence. ation therapy (RT) for panic disorder with- tirely accounted for by the one study that
We now respond to each of the points raised out agoraphobia and GAD. No differences found a large effect for CBT (d = 1.02), a
by Siev et al., citing prior published re- were found between CBT and RT for GAD. study conducted by Clark et al. (1994)
sponses to earlier criticisms, and presenting However, for panic disorder, CBT was supe- more than 15 years ago. When that single
existing evidence. rior to RT for panic-related symptoms but study is eliminated, the aggregate effect size
not for other symptoms of anxiety or de- for CBT versus RT is not statistically differ-
Meta-Analyses Were Conducted Across pression. Based on these results, Siev and ent from zero and the between-study vari-
Disorders Chambless concluded, “The finding that ability goes from 37% (50% in our
Wampold, Mondin, Moody, Stich, et CT and RT do not differ in the treatment of calculation) to 0%. In other words, the
al.’s (1997) conclusion about the lack of GAD, but do for PD [panic disorder], is ev- Clark et al. result is, statistically speaking,
treatment differences was based on examin- idence for the specificity of treatment to dis- an outlier, without which there is no differ-
ing direct comparisons of treatments pub- order, even for 2 treatments within a CBT ence between CBT and RT for any class of
lished between 1970 and 1995 in six class, and 2 disorders within an anxiety measures. The basis of Siev et al.’s claim that the
journals that typically publish psychother- class” (p. 513). Importantly, they arrive at dodo bird conjecture is false and that there is sub-
apy RCTs, regardless of the disorders being such a conclusion despite the fact that (a) no stantial evidence for specificity rests solely on this
treated. Siev et al. (2009) raised what was, a priori prediction of such a pattern of re- one study that found that CBT was superior to
at the time the study appeared, an impor- sults was made, and (b) no description is RT.
tant point to consider (Crits-Christoph, given of the specific mechanism believed to Some would argue that, rather than
1997; DeRubeis, Brotman, & Gibbons, be present in CBT for panic that is responsi- being an anomaly, an outlier might reveal
2005), namely that ignoring disorder may ble for the superiority for panic symptoms an important phenomenon that is obscured
well mask differences between treatments only. Exactly what is the specific ingredient in other studies. With that possibility in
for particular disorders. DeRubeis et al. of CBT that makes it more effective in re- mind, it is worth taking a closer look at this
(2005) articulated this quite clearly: ducing panic-related symptoms in patients one study, as so much rests on its validity.
Ignoring disorder “is akin to asking with panic disorder but has no effect on pa- The one outlier in favor of CBT is a study
whether insulin or an antibiotic is better, tients with GAD and depression? Finally, it comparing CBT, RT, and imipramine for
without knowing the condition for which is ironic that specificity based on a the treatment of panic disorder (a no-treat-
these treatments are to be given…. GAD/panic disorder distinction is critical to ment condition was also included) con-
Alternatively, researchers should begin with promoting and disseminating CBT when a ducted by Clark and colleagues (1994).
a problem and ask how treatments compare perspicuous effort in CBT is to develop pro- There are several aspects of the Clark et al.
in their effectiveness for that problem” (p. tocols that are effective across the range of study that are important to note when con-
175). emotional disorders, based on a common sidering the results. The relaxation treat-
There are two responses to this argu- diathesis of such disorders (see, e.g., Moses ment in this study was Öst’s (1987) applied
ment, however. First, the trials examined by & Barlow, 2006). relaxation, which was modified in two ways
Wampold, Mondin, Moody, Stich, et al. Despite the relatively weak evidence for for the comparison with CBT. The first
(1997) were, in each case, two treatments specificity, it could be claimed that the supe- modification was changing the rationale
for a particular disorder—they were not simply riority of CBT to RT for the treatment of presented to the patients to exclude men-
two treatments selected at random or arbi- panic seems to be a clear counter-example tioning that the premise of Öst’s applied re-
trarily. Second, and more to the point, there to the dodo bird conjecture determined by laxation involved “a vicious circle in which
have been numerous subsequent meta- meta-analytic methods. Let’s examine this the physical symptoms of anxiety are aug-
analyses that addressed the criticism of dis- result on which so much rests. In the meta- mented by negative thoughts” (Clark et al.,
order heterogeneity that have found no analysis conducted by Siev and Chambless p. 761) and instead used a behavioral expla-
differences among treatments for particular (2007), the conclusion that CBT was supe- nation that excluded mentioning that
disorders, meta-analyses that Siev et al. rior to RT for panic-related measures and thoughts were involved. We would assume
(2009) did not mention. These meta-analy- not for generalized anxiety or depression this was done to increase internal validity
ses span a number of disorders, including al- was based on five studies. For the panic (thereby preventing excessive discussion of
cohol use disorders (Imel, Wampold, Miller, symptom measures, one study had an effect thoughts) and not to improve the quality of
& Fleming, 2008), posttraumatic stress dis- that was very large in favor of CBT (d = RT.
1.02) while the remaining four studies pro-

October • 2009 147


The second modification, more conse- equivalent benefit. Or, alternatively, the specific to a disorder (e.g., diagnostic crite-
quential, was that in Öst’s development of consumer may well benefit from psychody- ria) may be due to the focus of a treatment
RT, exposure to anxiety-provoking stimuli namic therapy, a treatment recently shown on these particular symptoms.
was not introduced until after training in re- to be efficacious in the treatment of panic Of more clinical importance, the issue of
laxation was completed (8 to 10 sessions), disorder (Milrod et al., 2007). primary and secondary variables attenuates
whereas in Clark et al.’s version, exposure So, to a large extent, the claim that the the importance of psychological function-
was begun after four sessions—that is to dodo bird conjecture is false rests on the Siev ing of patients and emphasizes diagnostic
say, Clark et al. exposed patients to the feared and Chambless (2007) meta-analysis—a criteria instead. Siev et al. (2009) make a
stimuli before they had learned to relax. Thus, it study that found no differences between point to show that targeted measures in
is quite possible that Clark et al. may have treatments for GAD but did find differ- panic disorder are quite large compared to
actually conditioned panic symptoms (i.e., ences for the treatment of panic disorder, all other effects (see panel 3 of their Figure
patients were exposed to the feared stimu- but only on panic measures, but not anxiety 1). Their results, as displayed in Figure 1,
lus without skills to cope with fear) whereas and depression. More importantly, these re- show that CBT is superior to RT with re-
Öst’s protocol correctly desensitized the pa- sults are based entirely on a single study gard to panic symptoms, but not for other
tients because they would have the coping (viz., Clark et al., 1994), one which severely, quite important measures of psychological
skills to reduce anxiety in the presence of if not fatally, modified RT and for whom the well-being—generalized anxiety and de-
the feared stimuli. clinicians delivering the treatments had a pression. It is disappointing that psy-
Clark et al.’s (1994) modifications make distinct allegiance to the superior treat- chotherapy research has come to the point
it difficult to argue that this trial was a com- ment. If for some, the dodo bird conjecture is that demonstrating relative advantage on
parison of CBT and RT, as RT has been devel- to fall because CBT was superior to RT in targeted symptom measures but having lit-
oped, administered, and tested. Recall the the Clark et al. study, so be it. The similari- tle impact on important measures of the
discussion of hypothetical model of the fu- ties to Perla and the conclusion that ACP is quality of patients’ lives is viewed as com-
ture. Perla’s comparison of ACP and CBT superior to CBT, which was worrisome sci- pelling evidence from which to argue for
was flawed because Perla modified CBT to entifically, are unmistakable. The (hypo- specificity. Removing symptoms but not
increase the validity of the comparison with thetical) future is the present. benefitting patients generally is not a desir-
ACP, but in the process the CBT delivered able outcome to many—most importantly
in Perla’s comparison was not CBT as de- It Is Inappropriate to Aggregate Primary to patients.
signed and delivered by CBT therapists. and Secondary Measures To illustrate the imprudence of focusing
Indeed, it is not unusual that when a cogni- exclusively on the symptoms of a particular
Siev et al. (2009) emphasized that meta-
tive or behavioral treatment is found to be disorder, consider a trial comparing CBT
analyses should not aggregate across out-
inferior to another treatment (e.g., an in- and present-centered therapy for PTSD
come measures because certain patterns of
sight-oriented treatment), the claim is (McDonagh et al., 2005). Present-centered
results would fail to be revealed—an argu-
made that the CBT was not delivered faith- therapy is based on a problem-solving
ment that echoes criticisms made by Crits-
fully or skillfully, despite adequate fidelity model and similar to condition in the Perla
Christoph (1997), who raised a similar
ratings (see Jacobson, 1991). example; RC and present-centered therapy
argument when Wampold, Mondin,
Another consequential aspect of the were both purposefully developed so as not
Moody, Stich, et al. (1997) was initially
Clark et al. (1994) study was also foreshad- to contain any specific ingredients of the ex-
published. Siev et al. failed to reference that,
owed in the Perla story—namely, the thera- perimental treatment (e.g., exposure or
in response to Crits-Christoph, the trials
pists involved in the trials. The therapists in cognitive restructuring) believed to be re-
meta-analyzed by Wampold, Mondin,
the Clark et al. trial were Ann Hackmann medial to the disorder. In the McDonagh et
Moody, Stich, et al. were re-analyzed by
and Paul M. Salkovskis, widely regarded as al. study, the only significant difference in
Wampold and his students (Wampold,
accomplished CBT therapists, co-authors of outcomes between the CBT and PCT was
Mondin, Moody, & Ahn, 1997) and it was
the article, and closely allied with Clark. for the completers in terms of proportion of
found that the dodo bird conclusion applied to
Indeed, Salkovskis, along with Clark, devel- patients meeting diagnostic criteria at fol-
targeted as well as secondary variables. As well,
oped the CBT used in this trial and Ann low-up. Said another way, significantly
we have published meta-analyses that now
Hackmann has published many articles on fewer patients who completed CBT met di-
segregate analyses for primary measures,
CBT and has been involved in the develop- agnostic criteria for PTSD at the follow-up
and in these meta-analyses no differences
ment of various cognitive-behavioral treat- assessment—a difference was not signifi-
are found among treatments for primary
ments. Both therapists were supervised by cant in the intent-to-treat sample. There
variables for a number of disorders (e.g.,
David Clark. Despite Siev et al.’s (2009) were no differences between the two treat-
Benish et al., 2008; Imel et al., 2008).
contention that the trials in the meta-analy- ments in terms of anxiety, depression, disso-
Moreover, there is strong evidence that pri-
sis were fair because therapists “were ciative experiences, hostility, anger, and
mary and secondary measures are not con-
crossed with treatment condition,” the quality of life. What conclusion can one
ceptually or psychometrically distinct
therapists in the Clark et al. study were make about a treatment that impacts the
(Flückiger, Regli, Grawe, & Lutz, 2007;
clearly aligned with and had allegiance to criteria used to make the original diagnosis
Krueger, 1999; McGlinchey &
CBT. Perhaps there is a clinical implication and offers little other relative advantage?
Zimmerman, 2007; Meyer, Pilkonis, &
of this study: A consumer seeing either of Combined with the fact that over 40% of
Krupnick, 2002; Moses & Barlow, 2006;
these two therapists would rather receive patients in CBT dropped out versus less
Tanaka-Matsumi & Kameoka, 1986). This
CBT than RT, as administered in this trial. than 10% in present-centered therapy, one
suggests that making a distinction between
However, the same consumer seeing a ther- has to question the emphasis placed on
these two classes is not appropriate and
apist who has an allegiance to RT and who meeting criteria for a disorder and ignoring
raises the specter that changes in measures
followed Öst’s protocol may well attain an measures of psychological functioning and

148 the Behavior Therapist


well-being and the acceptance of the treat- comes for patients who undergo a treat- could lead to therapeutic exposure, then
ment (see, as well, Schnurr et al., 2003, ment that is fully intended to be therapeu- questions are raised about the necessity of
which found a similar result). Certainly, psy- tic” (p. 158). The intent of Wampold and prolonged and repeated exposure to the
chotherapy can do better. And certainly we colleagues’ meta-analysis was to examine trauma memory in the form of PE (e.g., lis-
have to be cautious about disseminating the relative efficacy of various psychothera- tening to tapes, etc.). Conversely, if merely
CBT prematurely, given the high dropout pies that were plausibly therapeutic and to talking about the trauma is not sufficient,
rate in this study. exclude treatments that were shams created why restrict the conversations? Supportive
for the purpose of controlling for common counseling in this instance is as credible as
Preponderance of CBT and Behavioral factors. Perla’s rational counseling. How are we to
Treatments in Meta-Analyses As was clear in the Perla scenario, some interpret the meaning of differences be-
comparison treatments, such as rational tween the two conditions? Are they the re-
With regard to the preponderance of
counseling, are designed to control for some sult of curative power of exposure, or
CBT and behavioral treatments, Siev et al.
common factors, but are not legitimate perhaps some other factor? We believe the
(2009) again noted earlier criticisms of the
treatments. Such comparison treatments, answer is ambiguous at best.
Wampold, Mondin, Moody, Stich, et al.
often called supportive counseling, com- To identify those treatment conditions
(1997) meta-analysis without considering
mon factor controls, or psychological place- that were not intended to be therapeutic,
published responses or recent studies that
bos, do not have cogent psychological Wampold and colleagues (Wampold,
addressed these criticisms. Crits-Christoph
rationales, proscribe the therapist from ac- Mondin, Moody, Stich, et al., 1997) used
(1997) first suggested that the similarity of
tions that most therapist would consider the following criteria to designate a treat-
treatments in the Wampold, Mondin,
fundamental and therapeutic, and often ment as bona fide:
Moody, Stich, et al. meta-analysis was re-
have no coherent actions intended to be
sponsible for lack of difference in outcome.
therapeutic. In the trials that use these First, the treatment must have involved a
Siev et al. did not mention that Wampold,
treatments, the therapists delivering them therapist with at least a master’s degree and
Mondin, Moody, Stich et al. showed that
know full well they are not intended to be a meeting with a patient in which the thera-
the dissimilarity of the treatments com-
therapeutic (as was clear in the Perla sce- pist developed a relationship with the pa-
pared was not correlated with the effect size
nario), which demonstrates the pernicious tient and tailored the treatment to the
(i.e., comparisons of more dissimilar treat-
effects of not being able to blind psy- patient. Thus, any study that used solely
ments did not produce larger effects).
chotherapy trials. tape-recorded instructions to patients or a
Neither did Siev et al. cite an extensive dis-
A good example of a treatment not in- protocol that was administered regardless
cussion of this criticism by Wampold and
tended to be therapeutic is the supportive of patient behavior (e.g., a progressive re-
colleagues (Wampold, Mondin, Moody, &
counseling condition designed by Foa as a laxation protocol that was not modified in
Ahn, 1997). As just one example, Crits-
control for a CBT/exposure treatment for any way for particular patients) was ex-
Christoph (1997) classified many treat-
PTSD with women who had experienced cluded. Second, the problem addressed by
ments as cognitive behavioral when clearly
childhood sexual abuse (Foa, Rothbaum, the treatment must have been one that
they were not (e.g., Greenberg’s emotion-
Riggs, & Murdock, 1991). To rule out cog- would reasonably be treated by psychother-
focused therapy). Finally, there are several
nitive components and exposure, the thera- apy, although it was not required that the
subsequent meta-analyses containing treat-
pists providing supportive counseling were sample treated be classified as clinically dys-
ments with diverse theoretical bases that
not allowed to explore or discuss patients’ functional. For example, treatments to in-
have found no differences in outcome. For
attributions about their abuse or their cur- crease time that a participant could keep a
example, the meta-analysis of PTSD that
rent problems and the therapists could not hand submerged in cold water would be ex-
showed no differences contained treatments
allow patients to discuss their abuse, as the cluded because cold-water stress would not
as variable as CBT, eye-movement desensi-
latter would involve covert exposure and reasonably be considered a problem for
tization and reprocessing, hypnotherapy,
desensitization. Rather, their patients were which one would present to a psychothera-
present-centered therapy, psychodynamic
taught a general problem-solving strategy. pist. However, any treatment for depression
therapy, and exposure (Benish et al., 2008).
Therapists were allowed to respond indi- was included whether the participants met
Similarly, with regard to the alcohol use dis-
rectly and with unconditional support. diagnostic criteria for any depressive disor-
orders, inclusion of treatments based on
Critically, however, whenever a patient in der or scored below standard cutoffs on de-
cognitive-behavioral principles, motiva-
the study tried to talk about the sexual as- pression scales. Finally, the treatment had to
tional interviewing, and 12-step principles
sault they had experienced, therapists in the satisfy two of the following four conditions:
did not reveal significant differences among
supportive control condition were limited (a) a citation was made to an established ap-
treatments (Imel et al., 2008).
to redirecting the discussion to everyday proach to psychotherapy (e.g., a reference
If It Works Then It’s Bona Fide problems. What therapist would provide to Rogers’s, 1951, client-centered therapy),
such a treatment, much less believe in the (b) a description of the therapy was con-
One of the features of Wampold, efficacy of such a treatment? Some might tained in the article and the description con-
Mondin, Moody, Stich, et al.’s (1997) meta- not believe in repeated and prolonged tained a reference to psychological processes
analysis was that the comparisons involved imaginal exposure to the trauma as the pri- (e.g., operant conditioning), (c) a manual
only treatments that were intended to be mary curative ingredient of trauma therapy, for the treatment existed and was used to
therapeutic, a hypothesis proposed by but we know of few therapists who would guide the administration of the psychother-
Lambert and Bergin (1994): “Research car- advocate prolonged, intentional, and sys- apy, and (d) the active ingredients of the
ried out with the intent of contrasting two tematic redirection away from discussion of treatment were identified and citations pro-
or more bona fide treatments show surpris- the very topic that brought the patient to vided for those ingredients. Accordingly,
ingly small differences between the out- therapy. If merely talking about the trauma any treatments designed to control for com-

October • 2009 149


mon or nonspecific factors, such as placebo different criticism than the issue of circular- viewed with the therapist. A full description
control groups, alternative therapies, or ity. For example, RT would not be excluded of PCT is available in the therapy manual.
nonspecific therapies, were excluded. (pp. as a bona fide treatment for depression be- (p. 518)
206-207) cause it is not currently an EST for depres-
sion or because clinical experience suggests Both supportive counseling and present-
The criteria for differentiating the bona- it does not work. It would be excluded be- centered therapy involved contact with a
fide treatments from shams are objective, cause the researchers that used it intended reasonably skilled therapist, both were de-
and can be applied to any treatment, re- for it to be a control condition not fully signed to exclude cognitive and exposure
gardless of how effective the treatment is meant to work (i.e., no credible explanation ingredients, and both involve aspects of
generally thought to be, based on clinical for its effectiveness was given) and the ther- problem solving as a coping mechanism.
experience or past research. Importantly, in apists in the trial were aware that it was not Present-centered therapy, however, had a
the meta-analyses conducted to date, coders intended to be therapeutic. It seems plausi- cogent rationale, incorporated and refer-
applied the criteria for determining whether ble that some dedicated researcher could enced the psychological bases of the treat-
a treatment was intended to be therapeutic develop a bona fide RT for depression or any ment, involved tasks reasonably related to
or not while remaining blind to the results of other disorder, but we are not aware of any the patient’s distress, contained aspects of
the individual trials being assessed. Coders such attempts. treatment related to the particular patient’s
were given only the description of the treat- To illustrate the nuances of determining problems, and contained homework that
ment provided in the method section of the whether a treatment is designed as a control was integrated into the treatments.
trial in which the treatments were de- for common factors (i.e., is not intended to Moreover, the therapists in the supportive
scribed. be therapeutic) and a treatment that is counseling condition were trained by one of
Despite blind ratings and objective cri- plausibly therapeutic, contrast the support- the two authors of the study, who had an al-
teria, Siev et al. (2009) object to the classifi- ive counseling condition used by Foa et al. legiance to the two CBT conditions and
cation scheme, contending that the criteria (1991) discussed earlier and present-cen- were supervised by Foa, the first author and
are circular: tered therapy, a treatment designated as developer of one the treatments. On the
“intended to be therapeutic” by Benish et other hand, the therapists in the present-
Wampold and colleagues (e.g., Ahn & al. (2008). Present-centered therapy was centered therapy condition were trained
Wampold, 2001; Messer & Wampold, described by McDonagh et al. (2005) in the and supervised by the developers of the pre-
2002) conclude that treatment outcome following way: sent-centered manual. The present-cen-
studies are futile because comparisons be- tered therapy met Wampold and
tween bona fide treatments yield clinically The first two sessions of PCT were spent es- colleagues’ (Wampold, Mondin, Moody,
insignificant differences and those between tablishing rapport, giving an overview of Stich, et al., 1997) criteria of a treatment in-
bona fide treatments and controls yield un- the treatment, presenting the psychoeduca- tended to be therapeutic, whereas supportive
interesting differences. This contention is tional material, and establishing a treat- counseling did not.
somewhat circular, however, because cate- ment plan based on the client’s choices of The goal of designing RCTs that com-
gorization as a bona fide treatment is both a problems to address. The framework used pare an EST to an alternative is not to de-
criterion for inclusion in, and an implication to assist in understanding the ways in which sign comparison treatments that might
of, the results of clinical experience and CSA trauma can impact the client’s current formally meet criteria for treatments in-
treatment outcome research (and meta life was that of traumagenic dynamics. tended to be therapeutic, but rather to find
analyses that synthesize multiple such stud- Traumagenic dynamics organizes the conse- an adequate comparison treatment that will
ies). (p. 72) quences of the experience of CSA into four provide evidence about which treatment is
categories—a sense of betrayal, powerless- most efficacious or evidence about the
To summarize Siev et al.’s (2009) argu- ness, stigmatization, and traumatic sexual- mechanisms of change. Poorly designed al-
ment, they believe that a bona fide treat- ization (Finkelhor, 1987). Clients were ternative treatments do neither. When
ment is synonymous with an effective guided in noticing these dynamics in cur- comparison treatments are better designed,
treatment. They suggest that if it became rent life difficulties and factoring them in as it appears that they are as effective as the ev-
known that treatments that exclude cogni- information in the problem-solving model. idence-based treatments to which they are
tive or behavioral components are less effec- The problem-solving model was a modifi- compared (Baskin, Tierney, Minami, &
tive than cognitive/behavior treatments, it cation of systematic problem solving, which Wampold, 2003).
would be impossible to design a therapy has demonstrated efficacy in the treatment As an aside, it is important to note just
without these components that would not of depression (DeShazer et al., 1986; how well supportive counseling type and
be considered inferior. How they arrived at D’Zurilla & Goldfried, 1971; Nezu et al., other stripped-down interventions can
such a conclusion is unclear and a misinter- 1989). Therapists were encouraged to pro- be—especially given the deficiencies in
pretation of Wampold and colleagues’ vide empathy, unconditional regard, and these treatments vis-à-vis what therapists
method. Stated succinctly, designating a genuineness in their sessions (Meador & would find to be reasonable treatments.
treatment as bona fide has nothing to do Rogers, 1973). The foci of subsequent ses- Cottraux and colleagues (2008) compared
with either the efficacy of the treatment or sions were determined by the participants’ CBT to Rogerian therapy for the treatment
whether it contains ingredients that are be- choice of current issues to address with the of PTSD in an RCT. Rogerian therapy was
lieved to work. The equivalency of bona fide problem-solving skills. Homework was de- more focused than many alternative treat-
treatments is an implication, not an a priori signed to assist clients in consolidating the ments but the description remained quite
requirement of Wampold and colleagues’ information conveyed in sessions, writing regimented, naïve, and obsolete compared
research. To be sure, quibbles can be made about their problem solving efforts and, to what modern experiential or humanistic
about the specific criteria, but that is a very tracking those efforts in a journal to be re- therapists would employ currently (cf.

150 the Behavior Therapist


Elliott & Greenberg, 2007; Ellison & be able to make valid inferences about the come across treatments, including psy-
Greenberg, 2007; Greenberg, 2008; efficacy of treatments. Allegiance is best chopharmacology (Horvath & Bedi, 2002;
Pascual-Leone & Greenberg, 2007). The controlled when therapists are nested Martin, Garske, & Davis, 2000; Wampold,
therapists in this study were CBT thera- within treatment (each therapist gives only 2001). Moreover, there is research that indi-
pists, Cottraux is an advocate of CBT, and one treatment) such that the treatment is cates that the alliance is not a result of early
the trial was conducted in centers known delivered by therapists who have an alle- symptom change (Baldwin, Wampold, &
for delivering CBT (the patients may well giance to the treatment they provide and Imel, 2007; Klein et al., 2003), although
have desired CBT). Nevertheless, there are trained and supervised by experts in the the evidence is not entirely conclusive.
were no differences in outcomes for the respective treatments (see Wampold, Without a doubt, alliance is difficult to
completers (and few in the intent-to-treat 2001). For example, in a trial of process-ex- study because levels of the alliance cannot
samples), although CBT retained more pa- periential therapy versus CBT for depres- be experimentally manipulated, but that
tients in treatment. Certainly, such findings sion, Watson, Gordon, Stermac, Kaloger- does not preclude the possibility that the al-
as this (see also McDonagh et al., 2005; akos, and Steckley (2003) ensured that their liance is causal to outcomes in psychother-
Schnurr et al., 2003) must pique the inter- allegiance to process-experiential therapy apy.
est of those who claim specificity of treat- was controlled by having experts in the two Interestingly, medicine is increasingly
ment: How can Rogerian therapy for the respective therapies train, supervise, and interested in relationship factors. For exam-
treatment of PTSD be as, or even nearly as, conduct the respective therapies and CBT ple, relationship factors between physician
effective as CBT? was not altered in any way. RCTs and meta- and patient have been found to reduce the
analyses need to control for the allegiance duration of the common cold (Rakel et al.,
Allegiance Effects effects if one is ever to sort out the nature of 2009). No therapist, whether psychody-
A confound alluded to in this critique of treatment differences. namic, CBT, or humanistic, would suggest
RCTs and Siev et al.’s (2009) conclusion is that their relationship with the patient and
researcher allegiance. Perla and Clark, in Common Factors: their collaboration around the goals and
our hypothetical and real-world examples, Alliance and Therapist Effects tasks of therapy, the elements of the thera-
have an allegiance to a particular treatment. peutic alliance, is unimportant. Clinical
Having criticized the conclusion that
Allegiance is not simply a hypothetical con- psychology will not be well served by mini-
there are negligible treatment differences,
jecture—the effects are demonstrable (see mizing the importance of relationship and
Siev et al. (2009) go on to argue that the ev-
Luborsky et al., 1999; Wampold, 2001). collaboration. Let us agree to differ on
idence related to the common factors is
Apparent differences among treatments are whether the alliance is the primary cause of
flawed as well. Two common factors, al-
often explained by differences in allegiance change or is necessary for the delivery of
liance and the therapist, were called into
(e.g., Imel et al., 2008; Robinson, Berman, & specific ingredients, as that is a question
question. Although Siev et al. highlight po-
Neimeyer, 1990)—the researcher’s alle- worthy of research effort.
tentially important limitations to common
giance to a treatment increases the likeli- Siev et al. (2009) raised an interesting
factor research, we will show that their crit-
hood that this treatment will be shown to point when they suggest that when thera-
icisms fail to acknowledge extant research
be superior to alternatives. pists address the alliance, the “very focus on
evidence and minimize the evidence that
Although the presence of researcher alle- alliance becomes a treatment technique” (p.
supports the alliance and therapists.
giance effects is apparent, its causes have 74). This raises a critical issue in under-
Alliance. Siev et al. (2009) echo a variety
not been well determined (Leykin & standing common factors and specific in-
of critiques of the alliance literature (see
DeRubeis, 2009; Luborsky et al., 1999; gredients. Common factors models as early
particularly DeRubeis et al., 2005), cor-
Wampold, 2001). On the one hand, alle- as Jerome Frank’s discussion of healing
rectly noting that various meta-analyses
giance may be manifest by the design of (Frank, 1961) have emphasized that a co-
have found that the alliance, measured early
comparison groups (e.g., by creating com- gent rationale and therapeutic actions are
in therapy, is correlated with final outcome,
parison treatments that have no cogent ra- essential ingredients of any healing practice
with an estimate in the neighborhood of
tionale, a lack of focus on the patient’s (Wampold, 2007). Indeed, there are treat-
.22, which indicates about 5% of the vari-
problems, and proscribe therapists from ac- ments for which work on the relationship
ability in final outcome is associated with
tions that most therapists would think ther- with the therapist is the focus of treatment
the alliance. What Siev et al. fail to mention
apeutic) or by therapist beliefs that one of (e.g., Safran, Muran, Samstag, & Stevens,
is that Wampold determined that differ-
the treatments is preferred and more effec- 2002). Common factor models posit that a
ences among treatments account for at most
tive. On the other hand, researcher alle- cogent rationale and therapeutic actions of
1% (Wampold, 2001), so the 5% figure is
giance may result in better outcomes for a some type are necessary and Siev et al. seem
quite impressive. Indeed, there is noth-
specific treatment because the therapists to agree: Focusing on the alliance will be
ing—absolutely no other variable, except
have particular expertise; for example, therapeutic if that is the focus of the treat-
initial severity—that can be measured so
Perla’s therapists may be extraordinarily ment, in the same way that CBT is thera-
early in therapy that correlates so highly
skilled at providing ACP and the extraordi- peutic by focusing on cognitions, ACP is
with outcome. Siev et al. (2009) then go on
nary results (vis-à-vis either RC or CBT) therapeutic by focusing on affect, and dy-
to cite a few studies that demonstrate an ef-
demonstrate the efficacy of ACP when de- namic therapies are therapeutic by focusing
fect that supports their argument and deni-
livered by therapists who have allegiance on the unconscious and attachment histo-
grate meta-analyses as means to understand
and competence to deliver ACP (Leykin & ries. We agree with Siev et al. that therapeu-
the evidence of a corpus of studies on the al-
DeRubeis, 2009). Whatever the explana- tic techniques are needed, when they are
liance.
tion, the impact of allegiance on outcome endemic to a cogently constructed treat-
The evidence is clear: The alliance has
demands that RCTs control for it in order to ment that the therapist believes will be
been shown to be robustly predictive of out-
therapeutic. This is exactly why we have so

October • 2009 151


much difficulty with the poorly constructed which is the basis of attempts to dissemi- be safe to say that we agree that psychologi-
“supportive counseling” type control nate ESTs, then it is necessary for the RCTs cal treatments are effective. There is also
groups—it seems the goal of these therapies to provide evidence that is generalizable to agreement that to be effective, a treatment
is for the therapist to do their very best to do such settings. There is a reasonable debate must have a cogent rationale, actions con-
nothing (or not do something) while still to be had about whether clinical trials pro- sistent with the rationale, and collaborative
providing some modicum of benefit to the duce evidence that is generalizable (Stirman work on the goals and tasks of therapy —
patient. They do not control for important & DeRubeis, 2006; Westen, 2006), but treatment is not simply listening to a pa-
ingredients common to psychotherapy and what is clear is that the operations of the tient. In Siev et al.’s (1990) terminology,
they cannot be faithfully executed by thera- study must be sufficient to make general- techniques are necessary. There is also an
pists. izations—and this has unambiguous conse- agreement that it is imperative to improve
Therapist effects. Therapist effects, which quences for designing RCTs and estimating the quality of services in routine practice.
are well established as noted by Siev et al. therapist effects. To be generalizable to Such efforts can certainly involve continued
(2009), is an area where all psychothera- therapists in routine care, therapists must work to develop, refine, and test treat-
pists, researchers, and patients covertly be considered a random effect and should be ments. Not surprisingly, there is also agree-
agree—the person of the therapist makes a representative of therapists to whom one ment that measuring outcomes in practice
difference. Rarely, if ever, are therapists ran- wishes to generalize (Crits-Christoph & and using that information to improve the
domly selected to deliver treatments in Mintz, 1991; Serlin & Lapsley, 1985; Serlin quality of services is a strategy than spans
RCTs. Perla used effective therapists to de- et al., 2003; Wampold & Serlin, 2000). multiple perspectives on psychotherapy
liver ACP, and the same is true of Clark et al. Using a select group of therapists, as Perla (Duncan, Miller, Hubble, & Wampold, in
(1994). The important issue, and the one on and Clark have done in their trials, limits press; Lambert, Harmon, Slade, Whipple,
which Siev et al. (2009) focuses, is deter- dramatically the inferences that can be & Hawkins, 2005; Miller, Duncan, &
mining the characteristics and actions of ef- made to how therapists in routine care Hubble, 2005).
fective therapists. Siev et al. speculate about might deliver the treatment (i.e., the notion So, where is the divergence? Siev et al.
therapist differences: “Some therapists are of dissemination), particularly because in (2009) and other EST advocates make some
likely more adept than others at using some clinical trials the training, supervision, and claims that not only appear to be contrary
techniques, formulating treatment plans, monitoring of therapists would be extraor- to the evidence, but also diverge sharply
encouraging their patients to do difficult dinary in routine care (clearly dissemination from the experience of therapists to whom
exposures, etc., even within CBT” (p. 74). involves training, but not nearly at the level they wish to disseminate the ESTs. First, the
In some ways, the evidence does not sup- provided in clinical trials). Moreover, to esti- claim that treatments are specific rests on
port this contention, as adherence to and mate therapist effects, there needs to be suf- criticisms of previous meta-analyses that
competence in a particular treatment have ficient numbers of therapists—they are have been addressed, and on one meta-
not been particularly strong predictors of considered to be drawn from a population analysis that showed one treatment to be
outcome (Schnurr et al., 2003; Shaw et al., of therapists. Just as the case for number of superior to another treatment for symptom
1999; Wampold, 2001). However, Siev et patients in a trial, there must be a sufficient measures only. And the one result that is of-
al.’s suggestion that effective therapists are number of therapists (at least 10) to make a fered to support specificity rests on a single
skilled in persuading patients to follow the reasonable estimation of the variability in and flawed study—not a reassuring bit of
treatment protocol is well taken—this is ex- outcomes. The partition of variance for flotsum in a sea of evidence.
actly part of the alliance involved in collabo- To add to the evidence, when control
Clark et al. (2006) in Siev et al.’s (2009) pie
rative agreement about the tasks and goals treatments without active ingredients are
chart is problematic because the Clark trial
of therapy. That is, effective therapists are well designed with a rationale and thera-
contained only 4 therapists—nobody
able to form alliances across a range of pa- peutic actions, they often are as, or nearly
would make conclusions about a treatment
tients. This is remarkably consistent with as, effective as “first-line” ESTs (Baskin et
with only four patients. The generalizability
Baldwin, Wampold, and Imel (2007), who al., 2003; Cottraux et al., 2001; McDonagh
of any trial that uses an extraordinarily
found that it was the therapist’s contribu- et al., 2005). Moreover, dismantling studies
small number of therapists, particularly if
tions to the alliance that predicted out- rarely if ever identify a particular ingredient
the therapists were selected in a way that
comes and that the therapist’s ability to that is necessary for the efficacy of a treat-
form an alliance (i.e., form a bond and col- makes them unusual (e.g., they developed
ment (Ahn & Wampold, 2001). Cognitive
laborate on the tasks and goals of therapy) ac- the treatment), received extraordinary therapy for depression does not need the
counted for the therapist differences in training, supervision, and monitoring, is se- cognitive components (Jacobson et al.,
outcomes. A recent study has shown that verely limited. Testing therapist effects in a 1996), cognitive processing therapy for
interpersonal skill of the therapist also ac- study with less than 10 therapists is unlikely PTSD does not need the cognitive process-
counts for therapist differences (Anderson, to be illuminating; ignoring therapist ef- ing or the writing components (Resick et
Ogles, Patterson, Lambert, & Vermeersch, fects (i.e., not estimating therapist effects) al., 2008), and CBT for PTSD does not
in press). in such studies does not improve the situa- need cognitive restructuring (Foa et al.,
If one is serious about studying therapist tion (actually, it makes it worse; see R. C. 2005).
effects, then attention must be paid to the Serlin et al., 2003; Wampold & Serlin, Establishing specificity requires the de-
proper design of RCTs to provide valid esti- 2000). tailed display of system-specific demonstra-
mates of these effects (Serlin, Wampold, & tions of mechanisms (Wampold, 2007) as
Conclusions
Levin, 2003; Wampold, 2001; Wampold & well as a simple superiority of one treatment
Serlin, 2000). If, as Siev et al. (2009) sug- For a moment, let us talk about the over another. Interestingly, there is evidence
gest, the goal is to use evidence from RCTs points the various sides in the common ver- that CBT provides patients with coping
to improve the quality of routine care, sus specific factors debate agree on. It might skills that are vital to lasting benefits. In a

152 the Behavior Therapist


study of 35 clients who responded to CBT competent therapists who believe in the Cottraux, J., Note, I., Yao, S. N., de Mey-
for depression, it was found that those treatment, to patients seeking treatment, Guillard, C., Bonasse, F. o., Djamoussian, D.,
clients who acquired cognitive coping skills are equally effective, or (b) the therapeutic et al. (2008). Randomized controlled com-
and displayed evidence that they were using alliance and therapists are potent therapeu- parison of cognitive behavior therapy with
Rogerian supportive therapy in chronic post-
CBT principles had lower relapse rates dur- tic ingredients. Therefore, consideration
traumatic stress disorder: A 2-year follow-
ing the year following treatment (Strunk, should be given to the DiGiuseppe’s third up. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 77,
DeRubeis, Chiu, & Alvarez, 2007). This option, in light of the evidence. 101-110.
type of research is needed to clearly under- Cottraux, J., Note, I., Yao, S. N., Lafont, S.,
stand mechanisms of change in an EST. References
Note, B., Mollard, E., et al. (2001). A ran-
Minimizing the importance of the al- Ahn, H., & Wampold, B. E. (2001). A meta- domized controlled trial of cognitive therapy
liance and of therapist effects, despite the analysis of component studies: Where is the versus intensive behavior therapy in obses-
evidence that exists, seems unnecessary. evidence for the specificity of psychotherapy? sive compulsive disorder. Psychotherapy and
One can accept the importance of the al- Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48, 251-257. Psychosomatics, 70, 288-297.
liance and therapists and remain committed Anderson, T., Lunnen, K. M., & Ogles, B. M. (in Crits-Christoph, P. (1997). Limitations of the
to developing and improving treatments. press). Putting models and techniques in dodo bird verdict and the role of clinical trials
To say that “if the goal of psychotherapy re- context. In S. D. Miller, B. L. Duncan, M. A. in psychotherapy research: Comment on
search is to determine the best ways to re- Hubble, & B. E. Wampold (Eds.), The heart Wampold et al. (1997). Psychological Bulletin,
lieve suffering for the most people, and soul of change (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: 122, 216-220.
researchers need to continue to focus on the American Psychological Association. Crits-Christoph, P., & Mintz, J. (1991).
areas that are most manipulable, such as Anderson, T., Ogles, B. M., Patterson, C. L., Implications of therapist effects for the de-
technique” (Siev et al., 2009, p. 74) is to Lambert, M. J., & Vermeersch, D. A. (in sign and analysis of comparative studies of
deny that there are ways to improve out- press). Therapist effects: Facilitative interper- psychotherapies. Journal of Consulting and
sonal skills as a predictor of therapist success. Clinical Psychology, 59, 20-26.
comes by focusing on areas other than tech-
nique. Indeed, Siev et al. go on to say that Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 755-768. Cushman, P. (1992). Psychotherapy to 1992: A
Baldwin, S. A., Wampold, B. E., & Imel, Z. E. history situated interpretation. In D. K.
using efforts that “integrate … the impor-
(2007). Untangling the alliance-outcome Freedheim (Ed.), History of psychotherapy: A
tance of the technique, alliance, and thera- century of change (pp. 21-64). Washington,
correlation: Exploring the relative impor-
pist factors” are needed (p. 74)—and we DC: American Psychological Association.
tance of therapist and patient variability in
agree. It would be informative to know how the alliance. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Davidson, R. J., Sherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H.
therapists use feedback to improve perfor- Psychology, 75, 842-852. H. (Eds.). (2003). Handbook of affective sciences.
mance: Do they alter techniques, address Baskin, T. W., Tierney, S. C., Minami, T., & New York: Oxford University Press.
ruptures in the alliance, or engage patients Wampold, B. E. (2003). Establishing speci- DeRubeis, R. J., Brotman, M. A., & Gibbons, C.
in an examination of progress? ficity in psychotherapy: A meta-analysis of J. (2005). A conceptual and methodological
Instead of insisting that therapists learn structural equivalence of placebo controls. analysis of the nonspecifics argument.
and deliver ESTs, perhaps we should insist Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 12,
that therapists attain a reasonable bench- 71, 973-979. 174-183.
mark regardless of the treatment they de- Benish, S., Imel, Z. E., & Wampold, B. E. Duncan, B., Miller, S. D., Hubble, M., &
liver (Minami et al., 2008). Therapists who (2008). The relative efficacy of bona fide psy- Wampold, B. E. (Eds.). (in press). The heart
are achieving outcomes comparable to or in chotherapies of post-traumatic stress disor- and soul of change: Delivering what works (2nd
excess of those achieved in clinical trials of der: A meta-analysis of direct comparisons. ed.). Washington, DC: American Psycholo-
ESTs might understandably be resentful of Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 746-758. gical Association.
efforts to mandate the type of treatment Chambless, D. L., Baker, M. J., Baucom, D. H., Elliott, R., & Greenberg, L. S. (2007). The
they deliver. Beutler, L. E., Calhoun, K. S., Daiuto, A., et essence of process-experiential/emotion-fo-
For us, the issue—the driving force be- al. (1998). Update on empirically validated cused therapy. American Journal of
hind our work—is proper attention to the therapies, II. The Clinical Psychologist, 51, 3- Psychotherapy, 61, 241-254.
16. Ellison, J. A., & Greenberg, L. S. (2007).
entire body of evidence. To be cast in the
role of prevailing “in the broad court of pro- Chambless, D. L., & Hollon, S. D. (1998). Emotion-focused experiential therapy. New York:
Defining empirically supported therapies. Springer Science + Business Media.
fessional opinion” on the basis of presenting
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Fancher, R. T. (1995). Cultures of healing:
a case “more aggressively to wide spread au- 66, 7-18.
diences” (Siev et al., 2009, p. 75), rather Correcting the image of American mental health
Clark, D. M., Ehlers, A., Hackmann, A., care. New York: W. H. Freeman.
than on the basis of solid research evidence, McManus, F., Fennell, M., Grey, N., et al.
is not a compliment many would want. Fishman, D. B., & Franks, C. M. (1992).
(2006). Cognitive therapy versus exposure Evolution and differentiation within behav-
DiGiuseppe offered three alternatives: and applied relaxation in social phobia: A ior therapy: A theoretical and epistemologi-
“Either we rebut these conclusions, conduct randomized controlled trial. Journal of cal review. In D. K. Freedheim (Ed.), History
new research to show they are wrong, or we Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 568- of psychotherapy: A century of change (pp. 159-
accept them and change our message” (as 578. 196). Washington, DC: American
quoted in Seiv et al., p. 71). Siev et al. have Clark, D. M., Salkovskis, P. M., Hackmann, A., Psychological Association.
neither rebutted nor presented sufficient Middleton, H., Anastasiades, P., & Gelder, Flückiger, C., Regli, D., Grawe, K., & Lutz, W.
new research to reject the conjectures that M. (1994). A comparison of cognitive ther- (2007). Similarities and differences between
(a) all treatments with cogent psychological apy, applied relaxation, and imipramine in retrospective and pre-post measurements of
bases, therapeutic actions consistent with the treatment of panic disorder. British outcome. Psychotherapy Research, 17, 371-
the rationale of the treatment (i.e., tech- Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 759-769. 377.
niques in Siev et al.’s language) delivered by

October • 2009 153


Foa, E. B., Hembree, E. A., Cahill, S. P., Rauch, S. Klein, D. N., Schwartz, J. E., Santiago, N. J., Handbook of psychotherapy integration (2nd ed.).
A. M., Riggs, D. S., Feeny, N. C., et al. Vivian, D., Vocisano, C., Castonguay, L. G., (pp. 84-102). New York: Oxford University
(2005). Randomized trial of prolonged expo- et al. (2003). Therapeutic alliance in depres- Press.
sure for posttraumatic stress disorder with sion treatment: Controlling for prior change Miller, S. D., Wampold, B. E., & Varhely, K.
and without cognitive restructuring: and patient characteristics. Journal of (2008). Direct comparisons of treatment
Outcome at academic and community clin- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 71, 997- modalities for youth disorders: A meta-
ics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical 1006. analysis. Psychotherapy Research, 18, 5-14.
Psychology, 73, 953-964. Krueger, R. F. (1999). The structure of common Milrod, B., Leon, A. C., Busch, F., Rudden, M.,
Foa, E. B., Rothbaum, B. O., Riggs, D. S., & mental disorders. Archives of General Schwalberg, M., Clarkin, J., et al. (2007). A
Murdock, T. B. (1991). Treatment of post- Psychiatry, 56, 921-926. randomized controlled clinical trial of psy-
traumatic stress disorder in rape victims: A Lambert, M. J., & Bergin, A. E. (1994). The ef- choanalytic psychotherapy for panic disorder.
comparison between cognitive-behavioral fectiveness of psychotherapy. In A. E. Bergin American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 265-272.
procedures and counseling. Journal of & S. L. Garfield (Eds.), Handbook of psychother- Minami, T., Wampold, B. E., Serlin, R. C.,
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 715- apy and behavior change (4th ed., pp. 143- Hamilton, E., Brown, G. S., & Kircher, J.
723. 189). New York: Wiley. (2008). Benchmarking the effectiveness of
Frank, J. D. (1961). Persuasion and healing: A Lambert, M. J., Harmon, C., Slade, K., psychotherapy treatment for adult depres-
comparative study of psychotherapy. Whipple, J. L., & Hawkins, E. J. (2005). sion in a managed care environment: A pre-
Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Providing feedback to psychotherapists on liminary study. Journal of Consulting and
Greenberg, L. (2008). Emotion and cognition in their patients’ progress: Clinical results and Clinical Psychology, 76, 116-124.
psychotherapy: The transforming power of practice suggestions. Journal of Clinical Moses, E. B., & Barlow, D. H. (2006). A new
affect. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadi- Psychology, 61, 165-174. unified treatment approach for emotional
enne, 49, 49-59. Latour, B. (1999). Pandora’s hope: Essays on the re- disorders based on emotion science. Current
Grissom, R. J. (1994). Probability of the superior ality of science studies. Cambridge, MA: Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 146-150.
outcome of one treatment over another. Harvard University Press. Norcross, J. C., & Goldfried, M. R. (2005).
Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 314-316. Leykin, Y., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2009). Allegiance Handbook of psychotherapy integration (2nd ed.).
Grünbaum, A. (1981). The placebo concept. in psychotherapy outcome research: New York: Oxford University Press.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 19, 157-167. Separating association from bias. Clinical Norcross, J. C., & Newman, C. F. (1992).
Hacking, I. (1983). Representing and interviewing: Psychology: Science and Practice, 16, 54-65. Psychotherapy integration: Setting the con-
Introductory topics in the natural philosophy of sci- Luborsky, L., Diguer, L., Seligman, D. A., text. In J. C. Norcross & M. R. Goldfried
ence. Cambridge UK: Cambridge University Rosenthal, R., Krause, E. D., Johnson, S., et (Eds.), Handbook of psychotherapy integration
Press. al. (1999). The researcher’s own therapy alle- (pp. 3-45). New York: Basic Books.
Hayes, S. C. (2004). Acceptance and commit- giances: A “wild card” in comparisons of Öst, L. (1987). Applied relaxation: Description
ment therapy, relational frame theory, and treatment efficacy. Clinical Psychology: Science of a coping technique and review of con-
the third wave of behavioral and cognitive and Practice, 6, 95-106. trolled studies. Behaviour Research and
therapies. Behavior Therapy, 35, 639-665. Makari, G. (2008). Revolution in mind: The cre- Therapy, 25, 397-410.
Horvath, A. O., & Bedi, R. P. (2002). The al- ation of psychoanalysis. New York: Harper- Pascual-Leone, A., & Greenberg, L. S. (2007).
liance. In J. C. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy Collins. Emotional processing in experiential ther-
relationships that work: Therapist contributions Martin, D. J., Garske, J. P., & Davis, M. K. apy: Why “the only way out is through.”
and responsiveness to patients (pp. 37-70). New (2000). Relation of the therapeutic alliance Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
York: Oxford University. with outcome and other variables: A meta- 75, 875-887.
Howard, K. I., Krause, M. S., Saunders, S. M., & analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Rakel, D. P., Hoeft, T. J., Barrett, B. P.,
Kopta, S. M. (1997). Trials and tribulations Clinical Psychology, 68, 438-450. Chewning, B. A., Craig, B. M., & Niu, M.
in the meta-analysis of treatment differences: McDonagh, A., Friedman, M., McHugo, G., (2009). Practitioner empathy and the dura-
Comment on Wampold et al. (1997). Ford, J., Sengupta, A., Mueser, K., et al. tion of the common cold. Family Medicine,
Psychological Bulletin, 122, 221-225. (2005). Randomized trial of cognitive-be- 41, 494-501.
Imel, Z. E., & Wampold, B. E. (2008). The com- havioral therapy for chronic posttraumatic Resick, P. A., Galovski, T. E., Uhlmansiek, M.
mon factors of psychotherapy. In S. D. stress disorder in adult female survivors of O., Scher, C. D., Clum, G. A., & Young-Xu, Y.
Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of coun- childhood sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting (2008). A randomized clinical trial to dis-
seling psychology (4th ed., pp. 249-266). New and Clinical Psychology, 73, 515-524. mantle components of cognitive processing
York: Wiley. McGlinchey, J. B., & Zimmerman, M. (2007). therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder in
Imel, Z. E., Wampold, B. E., Miller, S. D., & Examining a dimensional representation of female victims of interpersonal violence.
Fleming. (2008). Distinctions without a dif- depression and anxiety disorders’ comorbid- Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
ference: Direct comparisons of psychothera- ity in psychiatric outpatients with item re- 76, 243-258.
pies for alcohol use disorders. Journal of sponse modeling. Journal of Abnormal Robinson, L. A., Berman, J. S., & Neimeyer, R.
Addictive Behaviors, 533-543. Psychology, 116, 464-474. A. (1990). Psychotherapy for the treatment
Jacobson, N. S. (1991). Behavioral versus in- Meyer, B., Pilkonis, P. A., & Krupnick, J. L. of depression: A comprehensive review of
sight-oriented marital therapy: Labels can be (2002). Treatment expectancies, patient al- controlled outcome research. Psychological
misleading. Journal of Consulting and Clinical liance and outcome: Further analyses from Bulletin, 108, 30-49.
Psychology, 59, 142-145. the National Institute of Mental Health Safran, J. D., Muran, J. C., Samstag, L. W., &
Treatment of Depression Collaborative Stevens, C. (2002). Repairing alliance rup-
Jacobson, N. S., Dobson, K. S., Truax, P. A.,
Research Program. Journal of Consulting and tures. In J. C. Norcross (Ed.), Psychotherapy re-
Addis, M. E., Koerner, K., Gollan, J. K., et
Clinical Psychology, 70, 1051-1055. lationships that work: Therapist contributions and
al. (1996). A component analysis of cogni-
tive-behavioral treatment for depression. Miller, S. D., Duncan, B. L., & Hubble, M. A. responsiveness to patients (pp. 235-254). New
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, (2005). Outcome-informed clinical work. In York: Oxford University Press.
64, 295-304. J. C. Norcross & M. R. Goldfried (Eds.),

154 the Behavior Therapist


Schnurr, P. P., Friedman, M. J., Foy, D. W., Hsieh, Smith, M. L., Glass, G. V., & Miller, T. I. (1980). therapies” for depression. Journal of Affective
F. Y., Glynn, S. M., Bernardy, N. C., et al. The benefits of psychotherapy. Baltimore: The Disorders, 68, 159-165.
(2003). Randomized trial of trauma-focused Johns Hopkins University Press. Wampold, B. E., Mondin, G. W., Moody, M., &
group therapy for posttraumatic stress disor- Spielmans, G. I., Pasek, L. F., & McFall, J. P. Ahn, H. (1997). The flat earth as a metaphor
der: Results from a Department of Veterans (2007). What are the active ingredients in for the evidence for uniform efficacy of bina
Affairs cooperative study. Archives of General cognitive and behavioral psychotherapy for fide psychotherapies: Reply to Crits-
Psychiatry, 60, 481-489. anxious and depressed children? A meta-ana- Christoph (1997) and Howard et al. (1997).
Seligman, M. E. P. (1995). The effectiveness of lytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, Psychological Bulletin, 122, 226-230.
psychotherapy: The Consumer Reports 642–654. Wampold, B. E., Mondin, G. W., Moody, M.,
study. American Psychologist, 50, 965-974. Stirman, S. W., & DeRubeis, R. J. (2006). Stich, F., Benson, K., & Ahn, H. (1997). A
Research patients and clinical trials are fre- meta-analysis of outcome studies comparing
Serlin, R. C., & Lapsley, D. K. (1985). bona fide psychotherapies: Empirically, “All
quently representative of clinical practice. In J.
Rationality in psychological research: The must have prizes.” Psychological Bulletin, 122,
C. Norcross, L. E. Beutler, & R. F. Levant
good-enough principle. American Psychologist, 203-215.
(Eds.), Evidence-based practices in mental health:
40, 73-83. Debate and dialogue on fundamental questions Wampold, B. E., & Serlin, R. C. (2000). The con-
Serlin, R. C., Wampold, B. E., & Levin, J. R. (pp. 191-179). Washington, DC: American sequences of ignoring a nested factor on mea-
(2003). Should providers of treatment be re- Psychological Association. sures of effect size in analysis of variance.
garded as a random factor? If it ain’t broke, Strunk, D. R., DeRubeis, R. J., Chiu, A. W., & Psychological Methods, 5, 425-433.
don’t “Fix” it: A comment on Siemer and Alvarez, J. (2007). Patients’ competence in Watson, J. C., Gordon, L. B., Stermac, L.,
Joorman (2003). Psychological Methods, 8, and performance of cognitive therapy skills: Kalogerakos, F., & Steckley, P. (2003).
524-534. Relation to the reduction of relapse risk fol- Comparing the effectiveness of process-expe-
Shapiro, D. A., & Shapiro, D. (1982a). Meta- lowing treatment for depression. Journal of riential with cognitive-behavioral psy-
analysis of comparative therapy outcome re- Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 523- chotherapy in the treatment of depression.
search: A critical appraisal. Behavioural 530. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Psychotherapy, 10, 4-25. Tanaka-Matsumi, J., & Kameoka, V. A. (1986). 71, 773-781.
Shapiro, D. A., & Shapiro, D. (1982b). Meta- Reliabilities and concurrent validities of pop- Westen, D. I. (2006). Patients and treatments in
analysis of comparative therapy outcome ular self-report measures of depression, anxi- clinical trials are not adequately representa-
ety, and social desirability. Journal of tive of clinical practice. In J. C. Norcross, L.
studies: A replication and refinement.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54, 328- E. Beutler, & R. F. Levant (Eds.), Evidence-
Psychological Bulletin, 92, 581-604.
333. based practices in mental health (pp. 161-170).
Shapiro, F. (1989). Efficacy of eye movement de- Washington, DC: American Psychological
Task Force on Promotion and Dissemination of
sensitization procedure in the treatment of Association.
Psychological Procedures. (1995). Training
traumatic memories. Journal of Traumatic in and dissemination of empirically-validated
Stress, 2, 199-203. psychological treatment: Report and recom- . . .
Shaw, B. F., Elkin, I., Yamaguchi, J., Olmsted, mendations. The Clinical Psychologist, 48, 2-
M., Vallis, T. M., Dobson, K. S., et al. (1999). 23. Correspondence to Bruce E. Wampold,
Therapist competence ratings in relation to Wampold, B. E. (2001). The great psychotherapy Ph.D., University of Wisconsin–Madison,
clinical outcome in cognitive therapy of de- debate: Model, methods, and findings. Mahwah, Department of Counseling Psychology, 244
pression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Rust-Schreiner Halls, Madison, WI 53715-
Psychology, 67, 837-846. Wampold, B. E. (2007). Psychotherapy: The hu- 1150; wampold@education.wisc.edu
Siev, J., & Chambless, D. L. (2007). Specificity of manistic (and effective) treatment. American
treatment effects: Cognitive therapy and re- Psychologist, 62, 857-873.
laxation for generalized anxiety and panic Wampold, B. E. (in press). Psychotherapy theory:
disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical The historical, cultural, and empirical context.
Psychology, 75, 513-522. Washington, DC: American Psychological
Siev, J., Huppert, J., & Chambless, D. L. (2009). Association.
The dodo bird, treatment technique, and dis- Wampold, B. E., Minami, T., Baskin, T. W., &
seminating empirically supported treat- Tierney, S. C. (2002). A meta-(re)analysis of
ments. the Behavior Therapist, 32, 69-75. the effects of cognitive therapy versus “other

CLASSIFIED
Q
T FA POSTDOCTORAL FELLOWSHIP IN
C What are the registration hours
AB ALCOHOL RESEARCH AT THE
at the ABCT Annual Meeting in NYC? UNIVERSITY OF WASHINGTON.
The fellowship will provide training for
individuals who wish to pursue a career in
• Thursday preregistration pick-up: 11:00 A.M. – 8:00 P.M.
alcohol research, with an emphasis on the
• On-site registration: 3:00 P.M. – 8:00 P.M. etiology and prevention of problem drink-
• Friday: 7:30 A.M. – 3:00 P.M. ing and alcohol dependence. For more
• Saturday: 8:00 A.M. – 3:00 P.M.
information please see our website:
http://depts.washington.edu/cshrb/
• Sunday: 8:00 A.M. – 11:30 P.M. newweb/postdoc.html

October • 2009 155


Book Review cover the gamut of concerns for a clinician.
Chapters 1 through 3 discuss case concep-
tualization, assessment, and treatment
Roemer, L., & Orsillo, S. M. (2009). Mindfulness- and planning. Chapters 4 and 5 focus on the re-
lationship between therapist and client and
Acceptance-Based Behavioral Therapy in Practice. the development of a therapeutic contract.
New York: The Guilford Press. Subsequent chapters expand upon the im-
plementation of acceptance strategies
(Chapter 6), values clarification (Chapter 7),
Reviewed by Chad E. Drake, University of South Carolina-Aiken the promotion of mindful and values-con-
sistent behavioral activation (Chapter 8),
Relapse Prevention, among others. The au- and the monitoring of progress and termi-
oemer and Orsillo have generated a

R thoughtful overview of acceptance-


based behavioral therapies (abbrevi-
ated as “ABBTs” throughout the book) that
thors maintain a firm foothold in behavioral
practice, consistently relying on a func-
tional analysis of psychopathology. No par-
ticular diagnosis is given favor—the book
nation of treatment (Chapter 9). Chapter
10 weaves in the relevance of knowledge
and techniques from cognitive and behav-
ioral traditions while Chapter 11 discusses
the reader may find difficult to classify. It is
provides a broad, comprehensive view of the advantages of cultural sensitivity when
not a textbook, though it provides a syn-
treatment that is readily adaptable to a vari- conducting ABBTs with diverse clients. The
thetic view of theory and frequently cites re-
ety of clinical presentations. An emphasis book also contains an Appendix of recom-
search findings when relevant. It is not a
on theoretically driven processes and empir- mended resources.
treatment protocol, though it presents a
ically supported treatment components is Overall, the book is readable, compre-
comprehensive guide for treatment activi-
balanced with acknowledgment of limita- hensive, and aptly titled. Trainees and prac-
ties and includes numerous case examples
tions in the available data on these matters. titioners who have an interest would be
and transcripts. Rather, the book reads like a
Their synthetic approach makes for a hard-pressed to find a better resource than
guided meditation on the authors’ com-
book with broad appeal, especially for those Mindfulness- and Acceptance-Based Behavioral
bined knowledge and experience in practic-
who are still in training or wishing to ex- Therapies in Practice.
ing behavior therapy with mindfulness as a
central treatment component. pand upon their training experience.
Cognitive and behavioral practitioners will . . .
The book is transtheoretical, drawing
from and at times using terminology and receive a thorough introduction to mindful-
ness techniques and interventions, while en- Correspondence to Chad E. Drake, Ph.D.,
techniques from Acceptance and
thusiasts of mindfulness who lack a strong University of South Carolina, Aiken, 471
Commitment Therapy, Dialectical
background in evidence-based treatment University Parkway, Aiken, SC 29801
Behavior Therapy, Mindfulness-Based
will benefit from solid behavioral theory ChadD@usca.edu
Cognitive Therapy, Integrative Behavioral
Couple Therapy, and Mindfulness-Based and case conceptualization. Chapter topics

j
ABCT Annual Convention

Intensive Learning SATURDAY


8:15 a.m. – 10:15 a.m.
Opportunities 5. Mindfulness for Two: An ACT Approach
to Mindfulness in Psychotherapy
(Kelly G. Wilson)
2 CE Hours Master Clinician Seminars
10:30 a.m. – 12:30 p.m.
FRIDAY 6. Introduction to Parent-Child Interaction
8:15 a.m. – 10:15 a.m. Therapy (Cheryl McNeil)
1. DBT Skills Training With Adolescents and Families:
12:45 p.m. – 2:45 p.m.
Teaching Points Dos and Dont’s (Alec Miller & Jill Rathus)
7. The Reality of Conducting VR Exposure
10:30 a.m. – 12:30 p.m. Therapy: Expectations, Techniques,
2. Treating GAD: Evidence-Based Strategies (William Sanderson) and Limitations
12:45 p.m. – 2:45 p.m. (Mitchell Schare & Allen Grove)
3. Cognitive Therapy for Paranoia (Neil Rector)
3:00 p.m. – 5:00 p.m.
3:00 p.m. – 5:00 p.m. 8. “Why Would I Ever Want to Feel That?”
4. Ending on a Positive Note: Mastery of the Ethics and Practice Overcoming Emotional Avoidance
of Termination (Denise Davis) in Cognitive Therapy (Stephen Holland)

156 the Behavior Therapist


Multimedia Journal Letter to the Editor

ISSN 1077-7229
Looking to the Future of Empirically
Maureen Whittal
Cognitive and
Editor

Behavioral Cognitive and Behavioral Evaluated Self-Help


Practice
Practice Therapies
Maureen L. Whittal, Editor, Cognitive and Behavioral Patti Lou Watkins, Oregon State
University, and George A. Clum,
Practice, and University of British Columbia
Virginia Tech

to manuscripts, the video components will he June 2009 issue of the Behavior

C
ommunicating the complexity of
treatment strategies can be difficult
using the written word. However,
technology today offers us the opportunity
also undergo review. This review will be
handled by the action editor upon accep-
tance of your manuscript.
T Therapist announced the advent of a
“Seal of Merit” system to be applied
to self-help books “that are consistent with
to enhance our learning experiences To introduce the use of this technology, a CBT principles and that incorporate scien-
through the use of multimedia platforms video component is available and accompa- tifically tested strategies for overcoming
such as video. The addition of video pro- nies the electronic version of this manu- these difficulties” (p. 110). In line with the
vides the advantage of an alternate avenue script at www.sciencedirect.com. If you are overarching theme of the June 2009 issue,
to learn, similar to attending a training interested in submitting a manuscript that this system seems designed to promote im-
workshop without leaving your office or includes video clips, look for detailed in- proved dissemination of empirically vali-
home. Imagine being able to access video structions in the “Guide for Authors” sec- dated self-help programs to practitioners
segments that illustrate how to roll with re- tion on the C&BP web pages at Elsevier, and the general public alike. As researchers
sistance, setting up and debriefing behav- www.elsevier.com/locate/cabp. I believe in this field, we laud ABCT’s move to edu-
ioral experiments, or how to respond to that C&BP is the first psychology journal to cate consumers and promote the use of self-
ruptures in the therapeutic alliance. Given make use of multimedia. It is my hope that help materials with demonstrated efficacy,
the potential advantages, I am pleased to it will enhance the visibility and utility of especially given the plethora of self-help
introduce the opportunity to augment this unique journal. products of dubious quality (Rosen,
C&BP manuscripts with video components. In closing, I would like to thank you for Barrera, & Glasgow, 2008; Watkins,
The first two issues of Volume 17 in your continued interest in the journal. I en- 2008). We would also like to bring atten-
2010 will contain invited contributions to courage you to continue submitting manu- tion to another resource that aims to spur
illustrate the various ways in which this new scripts (with or without videos) and reading critical thinking about available self-help
application can be utilized. Similar to mak- the work of your colleagues. If you have approaches and facilitate the dissemination
ing reference to Tables and Figures in the feedback regarding the journal, be it posi- of approaches that have scientifically
body of a manuscript, authors will now tive or negative, we are always happy to demonstrated effectiveness: Handbook of
have the opportunity to embed video clips hear it. Or ultimate goal remains the same: Self-Help Therapies (Watkins & Clum,
at key points in the paper. In the first issue to be an enduring resource for scientist- 2008).
of 2010, the C&BP editorial team, includ- practitioners interested in empirically sup- As stated in the first chapter, which pro-
ing myself, Associate Editors Steve Safren ported approaches. With the imminent vides an overview of definitions, history, ad-
and Joaquin Borrego Jr., and Book Editor availability of multimedia manuscripts, we vantages, and limitations of self-help, “The
Sabine Wilhelm, along with our colleagues hope to stay on the cutting edge, for at least lack of empirical evaluation of self-help ma-
will be presenting manuscripts that include a little while! terials is, in fact, the impetus for this text”
the use of video. In the second issue, outgo- (Watkins, 2008, p. 15). Following initial
ing Publications Coordinator Phil Kendall . . . chapters describing various considerations
and Jack Rachman and their colleagues will in using self-help therapies as well as the
be presenting their recent work, aug- Correspondence to Maureen Whittal, theoretical underpinnings of this modality,
mented with video components. Ph.D., Vancouver CBT Centre, 708-777 W. the Handbook of Self-Help Therapies contains
The purpose of these video components Broadway, Vancouver BC V5Z 4J7 Canada 11 chapters, each detailing the empirical
is to concretize a concept or to illustrate the whittal@interchange.ubc.ca. evidence for self-help methods to treat psy-
specifics of a treatment strategy. The focus chiatric disorders such as depression and
of the video should be the clinician and not sexual dysfunctions as well as behavioral
the patient. Secondary to confidentiality, we health problems such as cigarette smoking
ask that real patients not be used. As you and weight management. In fact, over half
will see in the invited manuscripts, the of the inaugural titles that have received
videos are shot with actors or manuscript ABCT’s “Self-Help Seal of Merit” are dis-
collaborators. The number of clips will be cussed and referenced in these chapters.
up to the author, but the length of each The text also contains a chapter specifically
video should not exceed 7 minutes. Similar addressing the integration of self-help ther-

October • 2009 157


apies in primary care settings. For a critical havior therapy was based, but seem to have Ollendick, T. H. (November 2006). The evolution
review of the Handbook of Self-Help Therapies, given way to the former in recent years of cognitive and behavioral treatment and assess-
please see Wolleat (2008). (Ollendick, 2006). These recommendations ment: Past presidents’ panel. Paper presented at
Indeed, lack of knowledge regarding are consistent with Becker, Nakamura, the 49th annual meeting of the Association
which self-help interventions are effective Young, and Chorpita’s (2009) call in the for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies,
and appropriate for a given problem is a June issue of the Behavior Therapist for prac- Chicago, IL.
barrier that has kept many clinicians from tice-based evaluation of interventions. Rosen, G. M., Barrera, M. Jr., & Glasgow, R. E.
implementing this form of therapy. More Lastly, our hope with the Handbook of Self- (2008). Good intentions are not enough:
alarmingly, many practitioners prescribe Help Therapies is “to achieve praxis, provid- Reflections on past and future efforts to ad-
self-help books to their clients that advocate ing a resource that is both helpful to vance self-help. In P. L. Watkins, & G. A.
treatment techniques with no empirical practitioners working directly with clients Clum (Eds.), Handbook of self-help therapies
basis (Adams & Pitre, 2000). Rosen et al. and inspiring to researchers seeking to ex- (pp. 25-39). New York: Routledge.
(2008), however, remind us that treatment tend the data base in this area” (Watkins & Watkins, P. L. (2008). Self-help: Past and pre-
techniques with empirically demonstrated Clum, 2008, p. xii). As such, and again in sent. In P. L. Watkins & G. A. Clum (Eds.),
efficacy, when implemented by a therapist line with Becker et al.’s recommendations, Handbook of self-help therapies (pp. 1-24). New
in individual or group modalities, may not we envision this text as a potential resource York: Routledge.
be similarly efficacious when translated into in graduate training so that scientist-practi- Watkins, P. L., & Clum, G. A. (Eds.). (2008).
self-help formats. Thus, ongoing research of tioners might become as well-versed on self- Handbook of self-help therapies. New York:
self-help programs, even those based on sci- help interventions as they are in individual Routledge.
entifically tested treatment strategies in and group treatment modalities. Wilson, N. (2003). Commercializing mental
other venues, is necessary. Wilson (2003) health issues: Entertainment, advertising,
underscores the need for continued re- References and psychological advice. In S. O. Lilienfeld, S.
search, voicing a concern that the self-help J. Lynn, & J. M. Lohr (Eds.), Science and pseu-
industry, which is laden with nonvalidated Adams, S. J., & Pitre, N. L. (2000). Who uses
bibliotherapy and why? A survey from an doscience in clinical psychology (pp. 425-459),
products, may cast aspersions on the vali- New York: Guilford Press.
underserviced area. Canadian Journal of
dated products that do exist. She likens this Psychiatry, 45, 645-650. Wolleat, P. L. (2008). Evidence of efficacy of self-
industry to a tumor, warning that it has help interventions. [Review of the book
Becker, K. D., Nakamura, B. J., Young, J., &
“used the mental health profession as a host, Handbook of Self-Help Therapies]. Psyc-
Chorpita, B. F. (2009). What better place
but it has metastasized in ways that than here? What better time than now? CRITIQUES—Contemporary Psychology: APA
threaten to displace the profession itself ” (p. ABCT’s burgeoning role in the dissemina- Review of Books, 53 (Release 43), Article 8.
425). tion and implementation of evidence-based
In the final chapter of the Handbook of practices. the Behavior Therapist, 32, 89-95. . . .
Self-Help Therapies (Clum & Watkins, 2008), Clum, G. A., & Watkins, P. L. (2008). Self-help
we provide recommendations for future re- therapies: Retrospect and prospect. In P. L.
Correspondence to Patti Lou Watkins,
search that include both large-scale experi- Watkins, & G. A. Clum (Eds.), Handbook of Ph.D., Women Studies Program, 200 Gilkey
mental designs as well as a return to self-help therapies (pp. 419-436). New York: Hall, Oregon State University, Corvallis, OR
single-case designs on which the field of be- Routledge. 97331; pwatkins@oregonstate.edu

On Our Website

Call Meet ABCT’s Featured


for WORKSHOP SUBMISSIONS Clinicians of the Month
44th Annual Convention | November 18–21, 2009 Who are their mentors?
San Francisco How do they avoid burnout?
What are they reading?
Do they have any other talents?
Please send a 250-word abstract and a CV for How do they stay current?
each presenter to:
Most important, what do their
Carolyn M. Pepper, Ph.D. waiting rooms look like?
University of Wyoming
Dept. of Psychology, Dept. 3415 Don’t miss these in-depth interviews on
16th and Gibbon ABCT’s home page. A different thera-
Laramie, WY 82071
pist is highlighted each month.
or email: cpepper@uwyo.edu

http://www.abct.org
DEADLINE for submissions: February 1, 2010 

158 the Behavior Therapist


At ABCT at this year’s convention and drop your
nominations in the CALL FOR NOMINATIONS
box. You can also mail in your form to
To Lead or Not to Lead? That Is the Question ABCT’s Central Office, or fax it to (212)
647-1865. We can’t accept email nomina-
tions as original signatures are a require-
Kristene A. Doyle, Albert Ellis Institute, Chair, Leadership & Elections Committee
ment. All full members in good standing
are eligible to be nominated. There is no
Representative-at-Large position in the limit to the number of members you may
elieve it or not, nominating yourself

B or a colleague for office in ABCT is


easier than getting to attend all that
you want to at the Annual Convention!
2010 election serves as the liaison to
Academic and Professional Issues. The indi-
viduals elected serve a year as “elect” to
allow for all of the important particulars to
nominate.
You’ll find the call for nominations form
below, and in both the Winter and January
issues of tBT. A more thorough description
These are exciting times for ABCT. Edwin
be learned by the incoming officer. Once of each of the position appears in ABCT’s
H. Friedman once said, “Leadership can be
every 3 years a strategic planning meeting is bylaws at www.abct.org.
thought of as a capacity to define oneself to
held to assure that all elected members par- The individual elected as President-Elect
others in a way that clarifies and expands a
ticipate in at least one planning session dur- (2010-2011) will serve as ABCT’s President
vision of the future.” Make this the year you
ing their term of office. The next scheduled from 2011 to 2012. The Representative-at-
take steps to guide your professional home
strategic planning retreat will be held in Large candidate will serve November 2010
and make a worthy contribution. If you ask
2011 to accommodate ABCT’s cosponsor- through November 2013.
members who have previously served in of-
ship of the 2010 World Congress with The annual meeting of the Board takes
fice, you’ll find that many share similar rea-
Boston University’s Center for Anxiety place the Thursday of the convention, with
sons for doing so—they wanted to make a
Disorders and School of Social Work. monthly conference calls the remaining 10
difference, and they did. So could you or
Last year marked the first time ABCT months of the year. (There is no conferece
someone you know. In addition to the in-
members could vote electronically. Feed- call in August.) The President, Immediate
herent satisfaction achieved from contribut-
back indicated that, overall, the process was Past-President, President-Elect, and the
ing to ABCT, you have the opportunity to
convenient and effortless. Whether or not Secretary-Treasurer comprise the Executive
develop new friendships while reconnecting
you run for office, we encourage you and Committee. Conference calls are scheduled
with old ones.
your colleagues to vote for the individuals on an as-needed basis to ensure the contin-
Those members who receive the most
you believe will do the best job as soon as ued efficiency of ABCT. It is expected that
nominations will appear on the ballot. In
you receive your ballot. candidates have knowledge of ABCT’s mis-
April, full and new professional members in
How to get nominated. If you or some- sion, its bylaws, strategic plan, and existing
good standing vote on the candidates of
one you know possesses the skills, vision, priorities. If you want this year to be the
their choice to serve for 3 years. The candi-
and dedication to ABCT and its mission, year you (or a colleague) give it a shot,
date elected as President serves as elect, sit-
then stop by the membership sign-up booth please nominate yourself or a colleague.
ting, and past. The candidate who wins the

NOMINATE the Next Candidates for ABCT Office 2010 Call for Nominations
I nominate the following individuals Every nomination counts! Encourage colleagues to run
for office or consider running yourself. Nominate as many
for the positions indicated: full members as you like for each office. The results will be
tallied and the names of those individuals who receive the
P R E S I D E N T- E L E C T ( 2 0 1 0 – 2 0 1 1 )
most nominations will appear on the election ballot next
April. Only those nomination forms bearing a signature
and postmark on or before February 1, 2010, will be
counted.
R E P R E S E N TAT I V E - AT- L A R G E ( 2 0 1 0 – 2 0 1 3 )
Nomination acknowledges an individual's leadership
abilities and dedication to behavior therapy and/or cogni-
 tive therapy, empirically supported science, and to ABCT.
When completing the nomination form, please take into
consideration that these individuals will be entrusted to
represent the interests of ABCT members in important pol-
NAME (printed) icy decisions in the coming years. Contact the Leadership
and Elections Chair for more information about serving
ABCT or to get more information on the positions.
Please complete, sign, and send this nomination form
S I G N AT U R E ( r e q u i r e d ) to Kristene Doyle, Ph.D., Leadership & Elections Chair,
ABCT, 305 Seventh Ave., New York, NY 10001.

October • 2009 159


Awards and Recognition PTSD-related impairment in autobio-
graphical memory specificity leads to short-
term changes in memory specificity, PTSD
Three Recipients of the Neil S. Jacobson and depression symptoms, and vulnerabil-
ity factors for the disorder. Results of this re-
Research Award for Outstanding and search may suggest novel treatments,
treatment components, or preventative in-
Innovative Clinical Research terventions for PTSD that involve improv-
ing specificity of retrieval.
Virginia Rutter, Framingham State College Eddie Selby is a Ph.D. candidate in clin-
ical psychology at Florida State University;
he received B.A. and B.S. degrees in psy-
hree talented young clinical re- cuses on how the brain changes in response chology and physiology/neuroscience at the

T searchers—Katherina Hauner, Sally


Moore, and Eddie Selby—will re-
ceive the Neil S. Jacobson Research Awards
to effective psychotherapy. For this disserta-
tion project (supervised by Sue Mineka at
Northwestern University), Ms. Hauner will
employ neuroimaging to examine the ex-
University of Wyoming in 2005. He is pri-
marily interested in emotion dysregulation
as it relates to borderline personality disor-
for Outstanding and Innovative Clinical der, suicidal behaviors, and eating disorders.
Research at 5:00 p.m. on Friday, November tinction of fear, by observing the brain His work on emotional cascades and behav-
20, 2009, during the awards and recogni- changes that are associated with successful ioral dysregulation has been published in
tion ceremony at the ABCT Convention in exposure therapy for spider phobia. During the Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Review of
New York City. The awards coincide with the study, participants who have lifetime di- General Psychology, and Behavior Research and
the 10th anniversary of Neil S. Jacobson’s agnoses of spider phobia will receive a sin- Therapy. Mr. Selby’s project is “A Real Time
death—and with his 60th birthday earlier gle, 2-hour session of exposure therapy; Evaluation of Emotional Cascades and
this year. before and after the therapy, participants’ Dysregulated Behaviors in Borderline
Neil Jacobson made original contribu- neural response to spider images will be ob- Personality Disorder” (supervised by
tions in three areas of clinical psychology: served via fMRI, and the differences in the Thomas Joiner at Florida State University).
couple therapy, depression treatment, and observed brain activity (pre- and post-ther- Mr. Selby, mindful that individuals with
intimate partner violence. Much of his pro- apy) will be compared. Ms. Hauner’s re-
borderline personality disorder (BPD) may
fessional and personal sense of accomplish- search has been supported by grants from
engage in many impulsive/dysregulated be-
ment, though, came from mentoring and Northwestern University and a Society for
haviors as a way of inhibiting emotional cas-
training outstanding graduate students. the Science of Clinical Psychology
cades and rumination can amplify negative
These awards honor his contributions by Dissertation Award.
emotion to an unbearable state in order to
Sally Moore is a postdoctoral research
recognizing and funding three innovative get some relief, devised a study that ex-
fellow at the Seattle VA Medical Center’s
young scholars in clinical psychology. plores the relationship between emotional
Mental Illness Research, Education, and
The award recipients, Katherina Hauner cascades and dysregulated behaviors in in-
Clinical Center. In 2008, she received her
(Northwestern University), Sally Moore dividuals with BPD. The study will use ex-
Ph.D. in clinical psychology from the
(Seattle Veterans Administration Medical perience sampling, in which participants
University of Washington, where she stud-
Center and University of Washington), and complete records of interpersonal experi-
ied memory difficulties associated with
Eddie Selby (Florida State University), were ences, emotions, thoughts, and behaviors
PTSD (under the mentorship of Lori
selected from 50 submissions. The Awards over a period of multiple days using Palm
Zoellner). Her graduate research was
Committee—consisting of Andrew Christ- Pilots. It is expected that intense fluctua-
funded by a National Research Service
ensen (UCLA), Sona Dimidjian (University Award from NIMH. Her primary research tions of rumination and negative affect will
of Colorado, Boulder), Steven Hollon interests are in memory processes and emo- arise from interpersonal stressors, and that
(Vanderbilt University), Bob Kohlenberg tion regulation difficulties associated with these fluctuations will significantly predict
(University of Washington), and Virginia PTSD, exposure-based therapies, and treat- dysregulated behaviors in those with BPD.
Rutter (Framingham State College)— ment development. Dr. Moore will be These recipients will be at the ABCT
sought to identify work that showed the working on “Specific Memory Retrieval meeting to receive their Neil S. Jacobson
kind of deep thinking about important Practice in Veterans With PTSD and Research Award for Outstanding and
problems and novel methodology that Depression” (Tracy Simpson at the Seattle Innovative Clinical Research. In addition,
characterized Neil’s early career. VA Medical Center is her sponsor for this Ms. Hauner will be presenting her work on
study). In this project, Dr. Moore addresses fear extinction in a symposium on Sunday,
About the Recipients November 22, at 11:30 a.m.; Dr. Moore
how, in addition to intrusive memories of
Katherina Hauner is a doctoral candi- trauma, individuals with PTSD have diffi- will be chairing and presenting at a sympo-
date in clinical psychology at Northwestern culties retrieving specific memories of non- sium on information processing in PTSD on
University, and is currently on clinical in- traumatic life events when they are Saturday, November 21, at 3:00 p.m.; and
ternship at the University of Illinois at intentionally trying to do so. This difficulty Mr. Selby will be presenting a symposium
Chicago. She received her B.S. from the appears to contribute to the maintenance of on self-injury in BPD on Sunday, November
University of Chicago in psychology, with a PTSD symptoms over time and may repre- 22, at 9:45 a.m. Please join us in honoring
specialization in biopsychology. Her pro- sent a vulnerability factor for the develop- these young scholars, and for remembering
posal, “Neuroanatomical Substrates of Fear ment of PTSD. Dr. Moore’s study will Neil S. Jacobson and his many contribu-
Extinction During Exposure Therapy,” fo- examine whether brief training to address tions.

160 the Behavior Therapist


Call
1 6 t h An n ual Award s & Re c o g n itio n

Aw a r d
for
Nominations
The ABCT Awards and Recognition Committee, chaired by Shelley Robbins of Holy Family
University, is pleased to announce the 2010 awards program. Nominations are requested in
all categories listed below. Please see the specific nomination instructions in each category.
Please note that award nominations may not be submitted by current members of
the ABCT Board of Directors.

Outstanding Contribution by an Individual of your submission to ABCT, Student Dissertation Awards, 305
for for Research Activities Seventh Ave., New York, NY 10001.
Eligible candidates for this award should be members of ABCT in
good standing who have provided significant contributions to the Distinguished Friend to Behavior Therapy
literature advancing our knowledge of behavior therapy. Past Eligible candidates for this award should NOT be members of
recipients of this award include Alan E. Kazdin in 1998, David H. ABCT, but are individuals who have promoted the mission of cog-
Barlow in 2001, Terence M. Keane in 2004, and Thomas nitive and/or behavioral work outside of our organization.
Borkovec in 2007. Please complete an on-line nomination form Applications should include a letter of nomination, three letters of
at www.abct.org. Then, e-mail the completed forms to srob- support, and a curriculum vitae of the nominee. Past recipients of
bins@holyfamily.edu. Also, mail a hard copy of your submission this award include Jon Kabat-Zinn, Nora Volkow, John Allen,
to ABCT, Outstanding Researcher, 305 Seventh Ave., New York, Anne Fletcher, Jack Gorman, Art Dykstra, and Michael Davis.
NY 10001. Please complete an on-line nomination form at www.abct.org.
Then, e-mail the completed forms to srobbins@holyfamily.edu.
Also, mail a hard copy of your submission to ABCT, Distinguished
Outstanding Mentor Friend to BT Award, 305 Seventh Ave., New York, NY 10001.
This year we are seeking eligible candidates for the Outstanding
Mentor award who are members of ABCT in good standing who Career/Lifetime Achievement
have encouraged the clinical and/or academic and professional Eligible candidates for this award should be members of ABCT in
excellence of psychology graduate students, interns, postdocs, good standing who have made significant contributions over a
and/or residents. Outstanding mentors are considered those who number of years to cognitive and/or behavior therapy.
have provided exceptional guidance to students through leader- Applications should include a letter of nomination, three letters of
ship, advisement, and activities aimed at providing opportunities support, and a curriculum vitae of the nominee. Past recipients of
for professional development, networking, and future growth. this award include Albert Ellis, Leonard Ullman, Leonard Krasner,
Appropriate nominators are current or past students of the men- Steve Hayes, and David H. Barlow. Please complete an on-line
tor. The first recipient of this award was Richard Heimberg in nomination form at www.abct.org. Then, e-mail the completed
2006, followed by G. Terence Wilson in 2008. Please complete forms to srobbins@holyfamily.edu. Also, mail a hard copy of your
an on-line nomination form at www.abct.org. Then, e-mail the submission to ABCT, Career/Lifetime Achievement Award, 305
completed forms to srobbins@holyfamily.edu. Also, mail a hard Seventh Ave., New York, NY 10001.
copy of your submission to ABCT, Outstanding Mentor, 305
Seventh Avenue, NY, NY 10001. NOMINATIONS FOR THE FOLLOWING AWARD ARE SOLICITED
FROM MEMBERS OF THE ABCT GOVERNANCE :

Student Dissertation Awards: Outstanding Service to ABCT


• The Virginia A. Roswell Student Dissertation Award Members of the governance, please complete an on-line nomina-
• The Leonard Krasner Student Dissertation Award tion by visiting www.abct.org. Then, e-mail the completed forms to
Each award will be given to one student based on his/her doc- srobbins@holyfamily.edu. Also, mail a hard copy of your submis-
toral dissertation proposal. The research should be relevant to sion to ABCT, Outstanding Service to ABCT Award, 305 Seventh
behavior therapy. Accompanying this honor will be a $1,000 Ave., New York, NY 10001.
award to be used in support of research (e.g., to pay participants,
to purchase testing equipment) and/or to facilitate travel to the
Questions? Contact: Shelley Robbins, Ph.D., Chair, ABCT Awards
ABCT convention. Eligible candidates for this award should be
& Recognition Committee; e-mail: srobbins@holyfamily.edu
student members who have already had their dissertation pro-
posal approved and are investigating an area of direct relevance
to behavior therapy, broadly defined. A student's dissertation Nominate on line: www.abct.org
mentor should complete the nomination. Please complete an on-
line nomination form at www.abct.org. Then, e-mail the com- Deadline for all nominations:
pleted forms to srobbins@holyfamily.edu. Also, mail a hard copy Monday, March 2, 2010
October • 2009 161
Welcome, New Members
Full Members Daniel Singley Jason W. Beaumier Kristin Elisabeth Naragon
Alice Abarbanel Denise E. Stack Emily Becker-Weidman Gainey
Robert T. Ammerman Jan Stampley Amanda Tennyson Berger Yuliana E. Gallegos Rodriguez
Claudia Arlo Stephan Stevens Joshua Berger Miguelina German
Daniel M. Bagner Nicole D. Swain Christopher Robert Berghoff Matthew E. Goldfine
Christopher Barrick Jeff Szymanski L. Cinnamon Bidwell Benjamin Gottesman
Madeleine Beaudry Yvette N. Tazeau Timothy Billings Renee Grinnell
Michelle Bell Dana S. Thordarson Jason Oliver Black Jessica Grossman
Silvia Bernardini Frances P. Thorndike Lora Black Kathleen M. Grubbs
John Ross Bradley Jill-Marie Tiedemann Sasha Collins Blackwell Nate G. Gruner
Pascale Brillon Tero Timonen Jennifer Bolden Jennifer Guadagno
Wally Bzdell Karen Cameron Wells Alexa Noelle Bonacquisti Liza Lin Guequierre
Anil Chacko Laura Farrell West Amanda Bordfeld Vito Guerra
Cheryl A. Chessick Lena A-K Wiklund Charmaine Borg Jessica Gundy
Maria Clara Cuevas Karen Williams Hayden C. Bottoms Rebekah L. Haas
Dara Delancy Carolyn Z. Wilson Evan Bronstein Courtney Haight
Renda Dionne Michael D. Young Jelena C. Bubanj Kate V. Hardy
Rachel Eddins Melanie Ariel Burns Rebecca Hashim
Michelle M. Ernst New Professionals Sabine Calle Christina Hauke
Gabrielle Faggella Gina Di Giulio Steven R. Caplan Adrienne J. Heinz
Anne M. Fatone Robert Ferguson Kristin V. Christodulu Jennifer L. Herring
Roberto Flachier Maxine E. Holmqvist Brooke E. Coccia Andrea L Hobkirk
Greta Francis Robert D. Latzman Shiri Cohen Jessica Hughes
Claire A. B. Freeland Amy B. Lerner Peter Colvin Alexis M. Inabinet
Shannon Ann Glenn Gosta Liljeholm Martha Anne Combs Matthew Thomas Jameson
Susan Golden Monica C. Mann-Wrobel Melisa Constantiner Alexandra Johnson
Lisa J. Hoffmeyer Perry L. Masoti Erin B. Cooper Grace W. Jones
Marcus J. H. Huibers Melisa Moore Tara K. Cossel Sara Jucaj
Soledad Iucci Cyndi R. I. Murrer Lisa M. Couperthwaite Kristine M. Kent
Cynthia Rheney Johnson Sarah K. Ravin Cerissa L. Creeden Lauren King
Elizabeth C. Katz Rachel E. Rubin Taya Cromley Laura A. Knight
Peter James Kelly Veronika I. Voyages Katherine Cunningham Ellen Kolomeyer
Bruria L Kleinman Loren Watt Emily D’Antonio Grace Kong
Kristin Knapp-Ines Kim R. Zlomke Maria Fernanda Dasilva Alla Kryss
Naomi B. Knoble Joshua L. Davis Sadie E. Larsen
Jaimelyn Kost Post-Baccalaureates Genevieve L. Davis Federica Latta
Jeffrey C. Lanfear Gina Marie Cossavella Kelly B. Decker Allison L. Lebowitz
Marlena Larson Kathryn DeYoung Ashleigh Nicole DeFries Jenna Lenhardt
Rachel E. Lauber Aubrey Edson John Parkinson Dehlin Jessica C. Levenson
Earl E. Ledford Jr. Jason Daniel Jones John E. Dencoff Sara Levenson
Ryan Robert Lindsay Mary Munroe Gohar Derhovanesian Michelle Levy
Duane A. Lundervold Ashley Marie Smith Dan DeSena Elana R. Light
Melissa Marcantuone Jonathan P. Stange Whitney A. Dicterow Victoria Josefina Limon
Melanie McConnell Debora Anna D’Iuso Sara E. Little
Elizabeth Valarie Michael Students Halina J. Dour Thailyn Lopez
Nandita Mishra Idan Moshe Aderka Jordan H. Drackett Muhammad Hassan Majeed
Rachael Cheri Murrihy Sara Afienko Chris Drescher Jonathan W. Martin
Darcy Clothier Norling Gillian Marta Alcolado Claudia Drossel Lisa Matthews
Richard L. Osburn Kimberly M. Alexander Maria Drvoshanov Melissa Maxwell
Wendy A. Ossman Jennifer Alosso Maha Alex Eidi Tina Mayes
Kanako Otsui Carrie E. Ambrose Natasha Elkovitch Jessica McCarthy
Amy Marie Pacos Lena S. Andersen Kristen Ellison Salena McCaslin
Peter Pramataris Kristin Anderson Kendra L Ellway Megan Leigh McCormick
Cynthia S. Randolph Maria R. Anthony Laura J. Ely Christine Catherine McDunn
James E. Rosser Kristin E. Austin Jonathan Feiner Metta McGarvey
Gail A. Rothman-Marshall Megan Lee Avery Bill R. Ferguson Andrew Miller
Kristen Leigh Schmidt Effie Avgoustis Karla C. Fettich Jennifer Minarcik
Kathleen Shay Sharon Michelle Batista Kathi M. Fine Dominic C. Moceri
Ian David Shulman Emily Rebecca Beamon Kelci Cornelia Flowers Oswaldo Moreno

162 the Behavior Therapist


New Student Members, continued
Arezou Mortazavi Elise Resnick Randi Melissa Schuster Alison A. Tebbett
Beth L. Mugno Kimberly Lynn Rinehart Nisha Sethi Jasmine H. Teleki
Hillary E. Nammack Dixie Robinson Cara Anne Settipani Ryan Trim
Aaron Martin Norr Ashley Ross Melina Sevlever Samantha P. VanHorn
Kathleen A. O’Malley Jennifer A. Roters Alison M Shanholtzer Jennifer Celene Veilleux
Adjoa T Osei Jennifer Nicole Rough Christina B. Shook Lana M. Wald
Natalie Pastelak Julia Rovinsky Mark Matthew Silvestri Caitlin Elizabeth Walsh
Jessica C. Payne-Murphy Ashley Brett Rudnick Lauren Sippel Lindsay Washington
Alexander M. Penney Laura Rusch Gina Sita
Tiffany West
Andrew Philip Sheila C. Russell Kathryn E. Smith
Melody K. Sorenson Michelle Woidneck
Emily Pichler Minette Russell-Irace
Dorothy Porter Arthur R. Sandt Paige Spencer Matthew Worley
Chelsea Price Dana A. Satir Adrianne L. Stevens Andrea Woznica
Steven L. Proctor Lindsay Adele Sauers Suzanne Stone Jodi B. Yarnell
Nicky Elizabeth Pugh Rachel Schafer sherin talebian Andrea Yee
Katarina Radisavljevic Chris Scheller Rachel L. Talero David Yood
Yevgeniya Ratnovsky Sara W. Schonwetter Ryan Eric Talley Yiling Zhang
Bonney Reed-Knight Luke T. Schultz Annie Yuh-Jiun Tang Marian Rose Zimmerman

Invited Panel
NEW ABCT • 43rd Annual Convention • Nov. 19–22, 2009 • Marriott Marquis
YORK register today! www.abct.org
CITY

Overcoming the Glass Ceiling–Lessons Learned and Lessons to Give:


A Conversation With the Trailblazers
MODERATORS: Lata K. McGinn, Yeshiva University, and Michelle Newman, Pennsylvania State University
PANELISTS: Dianne Chambless, Edna Foa, Robin Jarrett, Marsha Linehan, Barbara McCrady,
Susan Mineka, Rosemery Nelson, Patricia Resick, Antonette Zeiss

Friday, November 20 | 2:45 P.M. | Broadway North

This unique panel will address the following topics:


• The impact of being a woman, including decisions made and challenges or adversities faced during
participants’ career building years;
• Ways in which participants prevented or overcame any adversities or challenges faced;
• Potential challenges faced by participants and women professionals in general today and how they
may differ from early challenges;
• Lessons that emerging female professionals can learn from experiences faced by participants;
• Discussion of strategies to further break the glass ceiling: (e.g., mentorship of younger female pro-
fessionals, development and maintenance of informal networks (an old girl’s club), raising aware-
ness of subtle biases faced by women today).

October • 2009 163


the Behavior Therapist PRSRT STD
Association for Behavioral
and Cognitive Therapies U.S. POSTAGE
305 Seventh Avenue, 16th floor PAID
New York, NY 10001-6008 Hanover, PA
212-647-1890 | www.abct.org Permit No. 4
ADDRESS SERVICE REQUESTED

Professional Development
NEW ABCT • 43rd Annual Convention • Nov. 19–22, 2009 • Marriott Marquis
YORK register today! www.abct.org
CITY

INTERNSHIP PANEL
Internship Training Site Overview | Jeanette Hsu & Justin Nash
Friday, Nov. 20, Manhattan Ballroom, 8:45 a.m.

POSTGRADUATE PANEL
Postdoctoral Paths for Professional Development | Richard Seime & Antonette Zeisss
Friday, Nov. 20, Marquis B & C, 10:30 a.m.

MEMBERSHIP PANEL DISCUSSION


What Every Graduate Student, Postdoc, and Early Career Professional Needs to
Know About the NIHM Loan Repayment Programs | Borrego et al.
Friday, Nov. 20, Cantor/Jolson, 1:00 p.m.

View publication stats

You might also like