You are on page 1of 12

This article was downloaded by: [Florida International University]

On: 27 August 2015, At: 14:42


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered
office: 5 Howick Place, London, SW1P 1WG

Theory Into Practice


Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/htip20

Conceptualizing and Assessing Higher-


Order Thinking in Reading
Peter Afflerbach, Byeong-Young Cho & Jong-Yun Kim
Published online: 09 Jul 2015.

Click for updates

To cite this article: Peter Afflerbach, Byeong-Young Cho & Jong-Yun Kim (2015) Conceptualizing
and Assessing Higher-Order Thinking in Reading, Theory Into Practice, 54:3, 203-212, DOI:
10.1080/00405841.2015.1044367

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00405841.2015.1044367

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,
our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to
the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions
and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,
and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content
should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources
of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,
proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or
howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising
out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &
Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-
and-conditions
Downloaded by [Florida International University] at 14:42 27 August 2015
Theory Into Practice, 54:203–212, 2015
Copyright q The College of Education and Human Ecology, The Ohio State University
ISSN: 0040-5841 print/1543-0421 online
DOI: 10.1080/00405841.2015.1044367

Peter Afflerbach
Byeong-Young Cho
Jong-Yun Kim

Conceptualizing and Assessing


Downloaded by [Florida International University] at 14:42 27 August 2015

Higher-Order Thinking in Reading

Students engage in higher-order thinking as they framework for understanding higher-order think-
read complex texts and perform complex reading- ing in reading, in relation to relevant theories and
related tasks. However, the most consequential research in reading, and standards and assessment
assessments, high-stakes tests, are currently limited initiatives. We conclude with the considerations in
in providing information about students’ higher- assessments of higher-order thinking in reading
order thinking. In this article, we describe higher- that can help teachers and students work toward
order thinking in relation to reading. We provide a attainment of the Common Core State Standards.

The Assessment of Higher-Order Thinking order thinking in reading must inform the creation
in Reading of assessments, comprised of texts, tasks, and

B
scenarios, that provide valid and reliable infor-
UILDING A detailed conceptualization of mation (Pellegrino, Chudowsky, & Glaser, 2001).
higher-order thinking in reading is a In this article, we describe higher-order thinking
challenging and necessary task (Kintsch, 1998; in reading. We argue for the importance of
Thorndike, 1917). An understanding of higher- distinguishing between higher-order thinking
and more basic thinking in reading, and propose
Peter Afflerbach is Professor of Reading in the College parameters for both. Next, we propose a
of Education at the University of Maryland; Byeong- conceptual framework that juxtaposes higher-
Young Cho is an Assistant Professor in the University order thinking in reading with major curricular
of Pittsburgh/LRDC; and Jong-Yun Kim is an (Common Core State Standards) and assessment
Instructor in the College of Education at Korea (National Assessment of Educational Progress;
University.
NAEP) initiatives. We conclude with character-
Correspondence should be addressed to Professor
Peter Afflerbach, College of Education, University of izations of assessment that can offer useful
Maryland, 2311 Benjamin Building, College Park, MD information about the development of students’
20742-1115. E-mail: afflo@umd.edu. higher-order thinking in reading.

203
Assessment of Complex Thinking

Attributes of Higher-Order Thinking in sequences and combinations of strategic processes


Reading (Dinsmore, Alexander, & Loughlin, 2008).
Our characterization of higher-order thinking
in reading derives from juxtaposing what is Basic and Higher-Order Thinking in Reading
known about expert, accomplished reading with Not all thinking in reading is of higher order,
established depictions of higher-order thinking and an important task is distinguishing between
(Pressley & Afflerbach, 1995). Based on this basic and higher-order thinking. Theory and
comparative process, we characterize higher- research that informs this distinction includes
order thinking in reading as (a) goal-directed, (b) processing models of text comprehension, text
responsive, and (c) self-regulated. complexity and readers’ thinking, and taxo-
Reading is goal-directed, and accomplished nomies of cognitive processes. First, models of
Downloaded by [Florida International University] at 14:42 27 August 2015

readers use strategies to identify, select, apply, reading comprehension describe how basic and
revise, and evaluate the means to achieving reading higher-order thinking contribute to successful
goals. Thus, higher-order thinking in reading text processing (Kintsch, 1998; McNamara &
includes strategies to determine and then pursue Magliano, 2009). Text comprehension requires
goals (Alexander, Graham, & Harris, 1998; van that readers build a textbase model and then
Dijk & Kintsch, 1983). Characterizing higher-order situation model. The textbase model represents
thinking in reading as goal-directed does not mean what exactly is written in the text, and the
that goals serve only as prompts to readers to building of this model involves largely the set of
execute particular algorithmic procedures. Rather, skills to understand written letters, words, and
goals may emerge and evolve in the midst of sentences: basic thinking that helps readers
complex reader-text-context interactions, requiring access text-explicit meanings. In contrast, the
readers’ effortful thinking (Afflerbach, Pearson, & situation model represents a coherent under-
Paris, 2008; Malmberg, Jarvenoja, & Jarvela, standing of text in relation to the goal, task,
2013). Readers’ higher-order thinking is marked by discourse context, and readers’ relevant knowl-
responsivity to both text and task. Responsive edge, and it is built through the use of complex
readers interpret, analyze, and evaluate different analytical, constructive, interpretive, and evalua-
aspects of text, including content, structure, and tive strategies and skills. This deep-level text
intended purposes, to determine the best pathways processing requires higher-order thinking.
Taxonomies of thinking (Bloom, 1956;
to goal attainment (Afflerbach & Cho, 2009;
Krathwohl, 2002) suggest that readers’ thinking
Pressley & Afflerbach, 1995). For example, in
operates at multiple levels, but not all reading-
Internet settings, responsive readers selectively
related thinking is complex. Basic thinking is
access and learn from multiple web sources, and
sufficient when readers identify written words
make informed-decisions about what to read and
and match them with word meanings from
how to read (Cho, 2014; Goldman, Braasch, memory, building an understanding of simple
Graesser, Wiley, & Brodowinska, 2012). Self- text. However, higher-order thinking is required
regulation marks many acts of higher-order when readers manage constructive and integra-
thinking (Zimmerman, 2008), and metacognition tive processes to make complex inferences using
plays an important role when goal-oriented readers text information and prior knowledge and parse a
read strategically (Bannert, Reiman, & Sonnen- text into the idea units to grasp what the text says.
berg, 2013; Magno, 2010). Hence, self-regulated Based upon the results of these reading processes,
reading involves higher-order thinking that con- readers may engage in subsequent higher-order
nects reading goal, task, situation, and discourse thinking, such as that required to question an
context to the reading strategies and skills in a author’s claim, applying what is learned from
conscious manner (Mischel, Cantor, & Feldman, reading in problem solving, or synthesizing
1996), and helps readers determine the best information from a group of texts.

204
Afflerbach, Cho, Kim Conceptualizing and Assessing Higher-Order Thinking

Task complexity also influences whether or thinking in reading. This is in contrast to


not higher-order thinking is used in reading. The decoding written language and accessing literal
National Assessment of Educational Progress meaning.
(NAEP) Framework defines reading as: A caveat concerning our conception of basic
and higher-order thinking is that the degree of
Reading is an active and complex process that effort and attention given by a reader to a reading
involves task is not a reliable metric of either basic or
higher-order thinking. Challenging reading does
. Understanding written text. not necessarily equate with higher-order thinking
. Developing and interpreting meaning. in reading. For example, a first grader who
. Using meaning as appropriate to type of text, exhibits strategic thinking and considerable effort
purpose, and situation. (National Assess- while decoding printed letters to sound is in the
Downloaded by [Florida International University] at 14:42 27 August 2015

ment Governing Board, 2013, p. iv) midst of challenging work. The developing
reader uses declarative and procedural knowl-
“Understanding written text” utilizes basic think- edge to determine letter-sound correspondences,
ing to establish a surface-level text understanding, blend individual letter sounds, and then recognize
and higher-order thinking may be used in the word. The coordination and execution of
“developing and interpreting meaning.” Notably, these mental operations represents important
“using meaning” (the third bullet in the cited reading development and contributes to success-
NAEP definition) demands complex strategies ful reading. Yet, the student’s work does not
and mindsets as readers apply the information, include higher-order thinking because the atten-
knowledge, and understandings gained from tion and effort was not invested beyond the
reading in the tasks of evaluating different texts, establishment of a literal understanding of text.
solving problems, and achieving reading goals.
Thus, the final section of the NAEP reading
A Conceptual Frame for Assessing
definition invokes higher-order thinking. Further,
Higher-Order Thinking in Reading
the Common Core State Standards’ (Council of
Chief State School Officers & National Governors Assessment requires that one reason about the
Association Center for Best Practices, 2010) nature of higher-order thinking, and the assess-
reading benchmarks require higher-order thinking ment texts and tasks that can evoke it (Pellegrino
situated in critical-analytical reading tasks. The et al., 2001). Accordingly, we propose a
tasks range from a close reading of text segments, conceptual framework that describes the types
to the interpretation and synthesis of multiple and hierarchies of higher-order thinking in reading
texts, to the questioning and challenging of and the relevant assessment tasks (Table 1). The
authors’ arguments, and to citing specific textual framework modifies Krathwohl’s (2002) taxon-
evidence to support readers’ responses to the texts. omy of cognitive processes to suggest increasing
Reading research and theory support a fairly complexity of the reading strategies and skills
firm boundary between basic and higher-order along the hierarchy of multiple types of thinking:
thinking in reading. Basic thinking is regularly remember, understand, analyze, apply, evaluate,
used in the course of reading and contributes to create, and reflect. For example, basic thinking
locating, understanding, and recalling text- skills and strategies are largely involved when
explicit information. However, complex and readers use what they remember about letters,
higher-order thinking is called for when text, words, and language to understand a literal
related reading task, or both, become increasingly meaning of text. However, reading to analyze
demanding. For example, reading a text that the information, contents, and meanings implied
requires frequent, complex inferencing, evaluat- in the text demands more complex inferential and
ing the contents of text, or using the meaning integrative strategies for text comprehension.
derived from text clearly requires higher-order Higher-order thinking is called for when readers

205
Downloaded by [Florida International University] at 14:42 27 August 2015

Table 1

206
A Conceptual Frame for Assessing Basic to Complex Thinking in Reading1
Revised National Assessment CCSS Anchor Standards for
Taxonomy: Examples of Reading Skills of Educational Reading in Relation to the Revised Representative Assessment Types
Cognitive and Strategies Relevant to the Progress (NAEP) Framework: Taxonomy and the NAEP and Tasks Suitable for Particular
Dimension2 Revised Taxonomy Definition of Reading Framework3 Types of Thinking

Remember Determining word meanings Understanding written text Standard R1. Read closely to Maze test to assess vocabulary
Recognizing by accessing semantic memory determine what the text knowledge
Recalling Recall information that says explicitly Multiple choice items following
Assessment of Complex Thinking

is explicitly stated in the text text reading that focus on recall


of word meanings or
text-explicit information
Understand Identifying and building causal Developing and interpreting Standard R1. Read closely to Story map that describes a
Interpreting relations among a series of meaning make logical inferences from it reader’s anticipation and
Exemplifying previous events and anticipate Standard R2. Summarize key understanding of a story plot
Classifying subsequent events supporting details and ideas and and character development
Summarizing Identifying and summarizing the determine central ideas or Written summary that includes
Inferring main ideas by discerning themes of a text main ideas and supporting
Comparing important and unimportant Standard R4. Interpret words and details that the author presents
Explaining information in the text phrases as they are used in a in the text
text, including determining,
technical, connotative, and
figurative meanings
Analyze Parsing out the text into the idea Standard R3. Analyze how and Graphic organizer or visual
Differentiating segments and putting the ideas why individuals, events, or representation of complex
Organizing together, cuing signal words ideas develop and interact over relationships among the
Attributing and phrases, to grasp the overall the course of a text concepts and components
organization of text
information

(continued)
Downloaded by [Florida International University] at 14:42 27 August 2015

Table 1 – (Continued)

Revised National Assessment CCSS Anchor Standards for


Taxonomy: Examples of Reading Skills of Educational Reading in Relation to the Revised Representative Assessment Types
Cognitive and Strategies Relevant to the Progress (NAEP) Framework: Taxonomy and the NAEP and Tasks Suitable for Particular
Dimension2 Revised Taxonomy Definition of Reading Framework3 Types of Thinking

Identifying and building the Standard R4. Analyze how Feature analysis of a set of
Afflerbach, Cho, Kim

semantic linkages between and specific word choices shape refutation texts that represents a
across different sources to build meaning or tone reader’s understanding of how
a global understanding of the Standard R5. Analyze the each text says about a certain
text set structure of texts, including how topic and the intertextual
specific sentences, paragraphs, relationships (e.g., mutually
and larger portions of the text to supportive, contradicting,
related each other and the whole affirmative)

Apply Applying the understanding of Using meaning as appropriate *Several standards in Writing and Annotated bibliographies that
Executing complex ecological concepts to type of text, purpose, Speaking & Listening in the include different perspectives,
Implementing and mechanisms learned from and situation CCSS reflect the importance of claims, and evidence related to
science text in the applying what students learned a controversy that a reader gains
investigations of a local from text into complex literacy from readings
ecosystem and content learning tasks Reflective essay that compares
Using the knowledge of and contrasts what a reader
fundamental principles of already knew ad what is newly
Newton Mechanics gained learned from readings, and that
from reading into the identifies inconsistencies
confrontation and revision of between them
the reader’s own
misconceptions about
relevant scientific
phenomena

(continued)
Conceptualizing and Assessing Higher-Order Thinking

207
Downloaded by [Florida International University] at 14:42 27 August 2015

Table 1 – (Continued)

208
Revised National Assessment CCSS Anchor Standards for
Taxonomy: Examples of Reading Skills of Educational Reading in Relation to the Revised Representative Assessment Types
Cognitive and Strategies Relevant to the Progress (NAEP) Framework: Taxonomy and the NAEP and Tasks Suitable for Particular
Dimension2 Revised Taxonomy Definition of Reading Framework3 Types of Thinking

Evaluate Assessing the trustworthiness of a Standard R8. Delineate and Critical rubrics to verify and
Checking text by examining its author, the evaluate the argument and evaluate source texts used in
Critiquing context in which it was written specific claims in a text, multiple text reading of
and published, and how it is including the validity of the a historical event
Assessment of Complex Thinking

associated with other texts reasoning as well as the Peer-led discussion about the
Judging the goal-relevance of relevance and sufficiency of the arguments and
texts and the validity of their evidence counterarguments learned from
information different texts

Create Identifying problem spaces and Standard R1. Cite specific textual Inquiry questions that reflect
Generating generating inquiry questions evidence when writing or understandings of the texts read
Planning while accessing, examining, speaking to support and that guides archival
Producing and selecting texts believed to conclusions drawn from the text research to collect and
be useful to the task goal Standard R9. Analyze how two or synthesize relevant sources
Constructing a new text by more texts address similar Professional presentation of a
reconstructing the information themes or topics in order to comprehensive plan, based
gained from multiple texts in build knowledge or compare upon learning from multiple
order to build the knowledge the approaches the authors take texts, which suggests legitimate
base for writing and alternative solutions to the
problems

Reflect: Detecting a comprehension *The NAEP assesses the *None of the standards directly Reader-generated think-aloud
Metacognition problem in one or multiple texts outcomes of the strategies and designates a particular reports that reflect strategic
Monitoring and attempt to solve the skills use, which requires metacognitive thinking in thinking and comprehension
Controlling detected problem by readers’ metacognitive reading monitoring processes
Revising searching for information from thinking
other available texts

(continued)
Downloaded by [Florida International University] at 14:42 27 August 2015

Table 1 – (Continued)

Revised National Assessment CCSS Anchor Standards for


Taxonomy: Examples of Reading Skills of Educational Reading in Relation to the Revised Representative Assessment Types
Cognitive and Strategies Relevant to the Progress (NAEP) Framework: Taxonomy and the NAEP and Tasks Suitable for Particular
Dimension2 Revised Taxonomy Definition of Reading Framework3 Types of Thinking
Afflerbach, Cho, Kim

Refocusing on the goal for Self-assessment checklist that


reading when detecting guides higher-order thinking in
information overloads in reading
a complex hypertext reading,
and revising the strategies for
reading

Note. 1The framework assumes the basic and higher thinking processes involved in reading comprehension, and it excludes precursors to constructing meaning and
understanding (e.g., grapho-phonemic knowledge, word recognition fluency). The CCSS categorize these precursors as Foundational Skills.
2
We switch the order of Apply and Analyze because analysis is a thinking working toward sophisticated understanding of “text content” and “implied and hidden meanings” in
the text whereas application is one that is related to what students gained from reading (information, knowledge, concepts, perspectives, insights, etc.) into certain complex problem-
solving tasks. This helps align application with the NAEP category of Using meaning. Also, we add Reflect to the taxonomy, as a reflection of the importance of metacognition as
higher-order thinking in reading. Metacognition is an underlying executive function that affords choices of particular cognitive processes for particular reading situations. Thinking
processes at both basic and higher-order levels are monitored, assessed, and modified by a function of metacognition, and as reading becomes more complex, it often requires more
metacognitive reflections and self-regulatory processes.
3
CCSS: Common Core State Standards. The CCSS Anchor Standard for Reading R10 is not included in this table because it is related to a text factor. Also, each of the Standard
R 1 and 4 is segmented into smaller parts because these standards represent multiple levels of thinking.
Conceptualizing and Assessing Higher-Order Thinking

209
Assessment of Complex Thinking

evaluate the internal and external features of texts as they relate to particular types of thinking in
and apply what they learned from the texts into reading. For example, basic thinking can be
certain problem-solving tasks. We note that assessed with multiple-choice items that focus on
metacognition is included in our framework, literal comprehension. However, more analytical
requiring higher-order thinking as readers manage and interpretive thinking should be measured
complex reading text and task combinations. through performance-based assessment tasks
In addition, our framework provides a means where readers generate constructive responses
to compare particular types of thinking and (e.g., constructed response items, student-gener-
reading with relevant standards and benchmarks. ated visual representations of text content).
For example, basic thinking, including strategies In addition, to describe critical and creative
and skills, help students achieve part of the thinking in reading, assessment tasks can be
anchor standard “Read closely to determine
Downloaded by [Florida International University] at 14:42 27 August 2015

designed with which readers appraise and critique


what the text says explicitly” in the Common text perspectives (e.g., analytical essays), create
Core (CCSSO & NGA Center for Best Practices, informative resources (e.g., create video trailers
2010, p. 10). Strategy and skill also operate, as based on books), and share constructed meanings
readers understand written texts in the NAEP to address relevant problems and issues (e.g.,
framework. In contrast, higher-order thinking, white papers, professional presentations).
used to analyze and interpret texts, assists Further, metacognitive thinking and the related
readers in meeting numerous Common Core strategies and skills can be observed as readers
State Standards (e.g., analysis of various aspects
think out loud about their reading strategies.
of text, including information organization,
As noted earlier, successful reading may
development of themes and arguments, and
require both basic and higher-order thinking.
word choices and subtle meaning differences
Assessments ably describe both types of readers’
contextualized in the text). These higher-order
thinking. In classrooms, this means that the
strategies contribute to readers’ developing and
assessment of higher-order thinking in reading
interpreting text meanings as the NAEP frame-
must be accompanied by assessment that
work describes. Higher-order thinking is used
certifies that students are skilled in the
further in complex literacy tasks, such as citing
textual evidence, and evaluating different mechanics of reading, and capable of construct-
arguments represented in multiple texts. ing literal understanding of most texts. Assess-
To sum, the framework highlights connections ment should provide assurance that basic
among particular thinking types, relevant reading comprehension has occurred prior to engaging
strategies and skills, and the currently practiced students in higher-order thinking tasks. To help
standards and benchmarks. The framework can illustrate this need, we focus on a Common Core
inform the design of reading assessment for higher- State Standard for English Language Arts. Here
order thinking based on a designation of the target is the standard:
strategies and skills, tailored assessment tasks and Integration of Knowledge and Ideas: CCSS.
settings, and pertaining procedures and materials. ELA-LITERACY.RI.5.9

Assessment of Higher-Order Thinking in Integrate information from several texts on the


same topic in order to write or speak about the
Reading
subject knowledgeably. (Retrieved July 17,
Valid and reliable assessment is attainable 2014 from: http://www.corestandards.org/ELA-
when one consults current understandings of both Literacy/RI/5/)
assessment and higher-order thinking. The final
column of Table 1 (“Representative Assessment A task analysis of assessment demonstrates that
Types and Tasks Suitable for Particular Types students meeting this standard must do the
Thinking”) suggests assessment types and tasks following:

210
Afflerbach, Cho, Kim Conceptualizing and Assessing Higher-Order Thinking

. Apply basic reading strategies and skills to will be accompanied by an increased focus on
construct an accurate understanding of each formative assessment. These assessments serve to
text; regularly inform educators of students’ progress and
. Summarize each text and note relevant details development, and are used by teachers to adjust
within each text; instruction. Consider again the Common Core State
. Make evaluative judgments to determine what Standard described previously. There are, at a
information from each text is suitable for use minimum, five different aspects of higher-order
in a written or spoken presentation; thinking that a student must conduct to be successful
. Synthesize information within and across at the Standard. Each of these is an assumed result of
texts, noting unique and shared aspects of this classroom teaching and learning, and formative
information; assessment can provide current and detailed
. Develop a spoken or written presentation; and information on student progress with each.
Downloaded by [Florida International University] at 14:42 27 August 2015

. Use metacognitive strategies to manage the If assessment is to make major contribution to


entire enterprise. students’ attainment of higher-order thinking in
reading, it will be in the formative phase (Wiliam,
We noted earlier in this article that higher-order 2013). Effective formative assessment is at the
thinking in reading depends, in part, on readers’ center of effective teaching, the teaching that helps
proficiency with basic reading strategies and skills. students progress on the path toward complex
A higher-order thinking task that requires students performances. Summative assessment can tell one
to “Integrate information from several texts on the whether or not a student has met a standard, or is
same topic . . . ” requires that students understand capable of a performance that involves reading and
each of the texts. Thus, it is important that higher-order thinking. Formative assessment helps
assessment certifies a student’s ability to construct teachers and students determine what is needed to
literal meaning. NAEP Reading results help build toward this attainment. Finally, we note that
reiterate this point: There are hundreds of thousands the development of higher-order thinking in reading
of students who do not meet the basic reading level is not an exclusively cognitive enterprise and related
for NAEP 4th-, 8th-, and 12th-grade reading. These affective factors including students’ motivation and
students struggle to construct literal understanding self-efficacy are worthy of assessment attention
of relatively simple texts. Placing these students in (Afflerbach, Cho, Kim, Crassas, & Doyle, 2013).
complex reading and assessment contexts that
assume an ability to construct meaning prior to
attempting higher-order thinking will have dire Conclusions
consequences for students, and the utility of the
assessment. Effective assessment of higher-order thinking
The assessments associated with the Common in reading is possible when one attends to both
Core State Standards, specifically the performance the accurate conceptualization of higher-order
assessments created by Smarter Balance Assess- thinking in reading, and the development of
ment Consortium and Partnership for Assessment of assessment text-task combinations that evoke
Readiness for College and Careers will be used to such higher-order thinking. In regards to the first,
certify that students have met specific standards. we have considerable knowledge of the nature of
As summative assessments, they serve the import- reading, and the characteristics of both basic, and
ant purpose of measuring student achievement with higher-order thinking. We gathered information
increasingly complex text-task combinations, and in support of our conceptualization of higher-
higher-order thinking. However, summative assess- order thinking from diverse, but related fields.
ments do not inform classroom instruction that helps Assessment text-task combinations that require
students move toward attainment of standards that higher-order thinking will certify that students
demand higher-order thinking. This means that any meet today’s challenging educational standards.
progress in the assessment of higher-order thinking Higher-order thinking in reading is always

211
Assessment of Complex Thinking

accompanied by more basic thinking, and useful Goldman, S., Braasch, J., Wiley, J., Graesser, A., &
assessment programs will provide valid infor- Brodowinska, K. (2012). Comprehending and
mation on both types of thinking. A second, acute learning from internet sources: Processing patterns
need is formative and summative assessments of better and poorer learners. Reading Research
that work in tandem: the formative providing Quarterly, 47, 356– 381.
regular, diagnostic information that provides Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for
cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University
markers on the path to students’ complex,
Press.
higher-order thinking, and that informs instruc-
Krathwohl, D. (2002). A revision of Bloom’s Taxonomy:
tion as students move towards increasingly
An overview. Theory Into Practice, 41, 121–218.
complex and demanding performances. Magno, C. (2010). The role of metacognitive skills in
developing critical thinking. Metacognition Learn-
Downloaded by [Florida International University] at 14:42 27 August 2015

ing, 5, 137– 156.


References Malmberg, J., Jarvenoja, H., & Jarvela, S. (2013).
Patterns in elementary school students’ strategic
Afflerbach, P., & Cho, B.-Y. (2009). Identifying and actions in varying learning situations. Instructional
describing constructively responsive comprehen- Science, 41, 933– 954.
sion strategies in new and traditional forms of McNamara, D., & Magliano, J. (2009). Towards a
reading. In S. E. Israel & G. G. Duffy (Eds.), comprehensive model of comprehension. In
Handbook of research on reading comprehension B. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and
(pp. 69 – 90). New York, NY: Routledge. motivation (Vol. 51, pp. 297– 384). New York, NY:
Afflerbach, P., Cho, B.-Y., Kim, J.-Y., Crassas, M. E., Elsevier Science.
& Doyle, B. (2013). Reading: What else matters Mischel, W., Cantor, N., & Feldman, S. (1996).
besides strategies and skills? The Reading Teacher, Principles of self-regulation: The nature of willpower
66, 440– 448. and self-control. In E. T. Higgins & A. Kruglanski
Afflerbach, P., Pearson, D., & Paris, S. (2008). Clarifying (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic
differences between reading skills and reading
principles (pp. 329–360). New York, NY: Guilford.
strategies. The Reading Teacher, 61, 364–373.
National Assessment Governing Board. (2013). Read-
Alexander, P., Graham, S., & Harris, K. (1998). A
ing Framework for the 2013 National Assessment of
perspective on strategy research: Progress and
Educational Progress. Washington, DC: Author.
prospects. Educational Psychology Review, 10,
Pellegrino, J., Chudowsky, N., & Glaser, R. (2001).
129– 154.
Knowing what students know: The science and
Bannert, M., Reiman, P., & Sonnenberg, C. (2013).
design of educational assessment. Washington, DC:
Process mining techniques for analysing patterns
and strategies in students’ self-regulated learning. National Academy Press.
Metacognition and Learning, doi:10.1007/s11409- Pressley, M., & Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols
013- 9107-6 of reading: The nature of constructively responsive
Bloom, B. (1956). Taxonomy of educational objec- reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
tives: Cognitive and affective domains (1st ed.). Thorndike, E. (1917). Reading as reasoning: A study of
New York, NY: David McKay. mistakes in paragraph reading. Journal of Edu-
Cho, B.-Y. (2014). Competent adolescent readers’ use cational Psychology, 8, 323– 332.
of Internet reading strategies: A think-aloud study. van Dijk, T., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of
Cognition and Instruction, 32, 253– 289. discourse comprehension. New York, NY: Aca-
Council of Chief State School Officers & National demic Press.
Governors Association Center for Best Practices. Wiliam, D. (2013). Assessment: The bridge between
(2010). Retrieved from http://www.corestandards. teaching and learning. Voices from the Middle, 21,
org/ELA-Literacy/. 15 – 20.
Dinsmore, D., Alexander, P., & Loughlin, S. (2008). Zimmerman, B. (2008). Investigating self-regulation
Focusing the conceptual lens on metacognition, and motivation: Historical background, methodo-
self-regulation, and self-regulated learning. Edu- logical developments, and future prospects. Amer-
cational Psychology Review, 20, 391– 409. ican Educational Research Journal, 45, 166– 183.

212

You might also like