You are on page 1of 11

Eclipsing: When Celebrities

Overshadow the Brand


Jasmina Ilicic
The University of Adelaide

Cynthia M. Webster
Macquarie University

ABSTRACT

This paper applies Gestalt psychology and associative network theory to examine the effect of
eclipsing in celebrity endorsement on consumer attitude towards the endorsed brand. Eclipsing
occurs when the celebrity overshadows the endorsed brand by dominating in an advertisement and
diminishing the associative link between the celebrity and endorsed brand. Three studies take into
account match-up, celebrity attachment, brand familiarity, and the moderating role of eclipsing,
through manipulating two levels of eclipsing in advertising: (1) high eclipsing, when the celebrity is
the focus, and (2) low eclipsing, when both the celebrity and brand are emphasized. Consumers who
have a weak attachment to the celebrity endorser report a more positive brand attitude when they
see the celebrity and endorsed brand both emphasized in an advertisement (low eclipsing) than when
the celebrity overshadows and dominates the brand (high eclipsing), irrespective of whether
consumers perceive the celebrity and brand to match or mismatch. For consumers with strong
celebrity attachment, high eclipsing enhances brand attitude, regardless of whether they perceive
the celebrity and brand to either match or mismatch. The findings of these studies have significant
implications for advertisers and brand managers in the execution of their advertisements featuring
endorsements. © 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Using celebrity endorsers in advertisements attracts towards endorsed brands (Keel & Nataraajan, 2012).
consumer attention, increases brand awareness, and Most of the research in celebrity endorsement focuses
helps brands to be noticed in an increasingly cluttered on the effectiveness of celebrities as direct spokespeo-
media space (Friedman & Friedman, 1979; O’Mahony ple, who provide information about brands through
& Meenaghan, 1997/1998). More and more advertise- advocating and actively engaging with them in ad-
ments today spotlight the celebrities, emphasizing the vertisements. Celebrities eclipse the brand when they
celebrities and providing little emphasis on the en- feature as the focal point in the advertisement, with
dorsed brands. Here, the celebrity overshadows the more time or space dedicated to the celebrity than to
brand, termed “eclipsing” (Keel & Nataraajan, 2012). the brand itself. Match-up is an added critical element
Clear examples of eclipsing occur in recent advertise- with celebrity endorsers. Research shows a mismatch
ments for American Express (AMEX). The one-minute between the celebrity and brand results in negative con-
television commercial featuring Kate Winslet, for ex- sumer attitude towards the brand (Ilicic & Webster,
ample, takes 55 seconds going through her starring 2013; Kamins & Gupta, 1994; Lynch & Schuler, 1994)
film roles and leaves the last five seconds to AMEX. and dissuasion (Rossiter & Smidts, 2012). Consumer
The print advertisement is similar, with a glossy image attachment to the celebrity further complicates the en-
of Kate Winslet occupying half of the space and with dorsement situation, with strong celebrity attachment
much of the remaining devoted to information about resulting in brand attitude enhancement and weak at-
her memories, ambitions, and achievements. Only at tachment producing brand attitude dilution (Ilicic &
the end of the advertisement, on one line, does she state Webster, 2011).
that the card she uses is AMEX. These advertisements The aim of this paper is to extend research in the
include a clear endorsement for the brand, but mostly area of celebrity endorsements by examining the ef-
contain personal information about the celebrity, infor- fects of celebrity eclipsing on consumer evaluations of
mation relevant to the celebrity but not the brand. endorsed brands. Celebrity and endorsed brand match-
Although eclipsing is evident in recent advertising up, celebrity attachment and consumer attitude to-
campaigns featuring celebrities, no research to date wards the brand in high eclipsing versus low eclipsing
examines the effect of eclipsing on consumer attitude advertisements are examined in order to identify the

Psychology & Marketing, Vol. 31(11): 1040–1050 (November 2014)


View this article online at wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/mar
© 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. DOI: 10.1002/mar.20751
1040
conditions under which eclipsing may have negative celebrity’s attributes and, as a consequence, provides
ramifications for the brand. First, eclipsing and the key little information on the endorsed brand.
aspects of consumer perceptions of celebrity effective- As there is no empirical research specifically on
ness, including match-up and celebrity attachment, are eclipsing in celebrity endorsement, studies examin-
examined. Here, the roles of match-up and attachment ing visual and verbal perception may also provide
on brand evaluations are discussed. Based on these dis- some useful insights. Work in visual complexity in-
cussions, hypotheses relating to the effects of eclipsing dicates that sufficiently motivated people spend more
on consumer brand attitude are offered. Three stud- time looking at complex visual arrangements that are
ies test these hypotheses followed by discussions of the asymmetric, incongruous, and contain more elements
results with implications for researchers, advertisers, (Berlyne, 1966; Donderi, 2006). Consequently, moti-
and brand managers. vated individuals have a less-efficient search strategy
when a target stimulus is present yet obscured. Con-
spicuous, or highly visible, forms are much easier to
identify (Engel, 1970, 1977), however, as the number
LITERATURE REVIEW of similar elements increases, identification becomes
more difficult (Ho, Scialfa, Caird, & Graw, 2001). For
Eclipsing and Gestalt Psychology an eclipsing endorsement situation, which arguably is
a complex arrangement, these results suggest that con-
In endorsement situations, advertisers strategically sumers may be drawn to the advertisement and pro-
present popular, attractive celebrities together with cess the obvious celebrity communication, but may not
brands. Typically, celebrities feature as direct spokes- be interested enough to attend to the brand. In an au-
people, where they provide relevant information about dio study, Bigand, McAdams, and Foret (2000) examine
the brand they endorse throughout much of the ad- people’s ability to detect melodic errors while simulta-
vertisement, speaking extensively about the brand or neously listening to two different well-known nursery
actively engaging with it. For example, Katy Perry songs. In this complex auditory situation, they find non-
discusses the importance of Proactiv skin care in her musicians simplify the task by concentrating on only
life. Katy Perry’s fun, youthful, and eccentric image one of the songs, while experienced musicians are able
matches that of the Proactiv brand, providing con- to integrate the two songs creating one piece to focus
sumers with consistent information about the celebrity their attention. These results again show that when
and brand. In advertisements, viewers see her using the faced with composite situation, inexperienced individ-
product and hear her praising the brand. These adver- uals simply disregard unnecessary information, sug-
tisements spend most of the time explicitly highlighting gesting that brand familiarity may influence consumer
the benefits of the endorsed brand, Proactiv. perception in an eclipsing endorsement situation.
Based on Gestalt psychology (Koffka, 1935), these While informative, these studies in visual and ver-
standard endorsement practices are effective, in part, bal perception examine whether individuals accurately
as they incorporate accepted design principles of fo- identify stimuli and do not ask for their overall evalu-
cal point, figure-ground, balance, proximity, similar- ations of the arrangement. Rossiter and Percy’s (1980)
ity, and congruity (Lidwell, Holden, & Butler, 2010). study is one of the few in advertising to look at the
The endorsed brand figures as the focal point, gaining effects of visual and verbal imagery on consumer eval-
viewer attention, and the celebrity is featured in notice- uations. Although they do not consider an endorsement
able support for the brand, providing specific qualities situation and focus on only one brand at a time, they
that complement and do not confuse the brand message. find advertisements that emphasize the brand by using
Celebrities eclipse the endorsed brand when they cre- a relatively large picture and making explicit claims
ate an imbalance by being featured as the focal point in- result in strong consumer brand attitude, whereas
stead of the brand within an advertisement and placed small pictures and vague claims significantly weaken
as the dominant figure, while the brand recedes into the consumer brand attitude. In eclipsing situations, the
background. For example, in a recent 60-second adver- celebrity is elevated and the endorsed brand is dimin-
tisement for Pepsi, Beyonce features as the focal point, ished, which suggests that consumer attitude towards
dancing to her own music in front of mirrors relaying the endorsed brand weakens.
multiple images of herself (YouTube, 2013). Six of the
first 12 seconds are devoted to Beyonce opening and
drinking out of a can of Pepsi and the next 42 seconds Eclipsing and Associative Network Theory
are dedicated to Beyonce dancing and singing. Viewers
again are exposed to Pepsi in the 54th second. In this Associative network theory also underpins the mech-
advertisement, Beyonce features as the focal stimuli anisms by which celebrity endorsement is an effec-
with only 12 seconds of the 60-second advertisement tive advertising strategy (Till, 1998; Till & Busler,
devoted to the Pepsi brand. When celebrities dominate 2000). According to the theory, memory is struc-
advertisements, they engage the audience while also di- tured as an associative network containing nodes
verting attention away from the brand. The imbalance (brand and celebrity nodes) connected via related links
of time and space toward the celebrity accentuates the (Anderson, 1983; Collins & Loftus, 1975; Martindale,

INFLUENCE OF ECLIPSING ON BRAND ATTITUDE 1041


Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
1991). Through consumption experiences and media ster, 2013; Kamins, 1990; Kamins & Gupta, 1994; Till
exposure, consumers gain information about brands & Busler, 1998, 2000; Till, Stanley, & Priluck, 2008).
and celebrities. Over time, consumers become highly Greater match-up also results in increased purchase
familiar with certain brands and celebrities. Research intentions (Lee & Thorson, 2008), advertisement view-
shows brand familiarity leads to much richer and more ing time, brand interest, and word-of-mouth communi-
developed knowledge structures, providing consumers cation (Törn, 2012).
with deeper confidence in their knowledge of the brand, When a celebrity is perceived to match the brand,
brand attributes, and benefits than those unfamiliar consumers are easily able to form a celebrity–brand
(Bettman & Sujan, 1987; Swaminathan, Fox, & Reddy, connection, as both the brand and celebrity encom-
2001). For unfamiliar or new brands, consumers report pass consistent attributes that facilitate ease of cogni-
either unformed or weak associations tied to the brand, tive processing (Kahneman, 2011). Consumers use this
resulting in weaker attitude strength and accessibility match-up as a simple heuristic to judge the endorse-
(Fazio, 1986, 1989). Initially, the brand and celebrity ment. When there is little fit, or a perceived mismatch
nodes are unconnected in consumer minds and it is between the celebrity and brand, the celebrity–brand
through the endorsement process that the link join- connection is absent or difficult to determine. With a
ing the brand and celebrity is formed. The endorse- mismatch, consumers are left to process disjointed in-
ment process presents the brand and celebrity nodes formation, which requires more analytical effort to un-
together, activating memory retrieval for both nodes derstand the endorsement (Lynch, 1981). Results show
simultaneously. that when a celebrity mismatches the brand, consumers
Advertisements featuring celebrities eclipsing the report negative brand evaluations (Ilicic & Webster,
brand provide consumers with much more information 2013; Kamins & Gupta, 1994).
about the celebrity than the brand, making it difficult When assessing match-up, consumers consider at-
for consumers to activate brand information. Focus- tributes from both the celebrity and the endorsed
ing predominantly on the celebrity distracts consumers brand. With eclipsing, the information is much more
from establishing a strong associative link between the one-sided, emphasizing the celebrity. As a result, pro-
celebrity and brand, and weakens the effectiveness of cessing brand-related information requires greater ef-
the celebrity endorser (Till, 1998). Ostensibly, a weak fort. When an eclipsing celebrity matches the brand,
celebrity–brand connection results in a more negative consumers simply evaluate the brand based on the
consumer brand attitude. As such, it is expected that celebrity status. Since consumers use match-up as a
when a celebrity is the focus of an advertisement (high shortcut to make quick judgments about their attitude
eclipsing endorsement), consumer attitude towards the towards the endorsed brand, it is expected that eclips-
brand weakens, compared to when both the celebrity ing exerts the strongest influence on those who perceive
and endorsed brand are strong elements (low eclips- the celebrity and brand to mismatch. When a celebrity
ing endorsement). Thus, it is expected that celebrity mismatches the brand, consumers use piecemeal, an-
eclipsing will have a negative effect on consumer brand alytical processing to draw conclusions for the pairing
assessments and it is proposed: of the celebrity and brand. A low eclipsing endorse-
ment should allow consumers to establish some sort
H1: Consumers report a more positive attitude to- of celebrity–brand link, resulting in a more positive
wards the endorsed brand when exposed to a attitude towards the brand. A high eclipsing endorse-
low eclipsing endorsement than a high eclipsing ment weakens the brand position, as consumers are
endorsement. distracted from establishing a celebrity–brand connec-
tion, resulting in a more negative attitude toward the
Eclipsing and Match-up brand. This suggests the following:
H2: When consumers perceive a mismatch, low
Celebrity–brand match-up, or congruence, is exten-
sively researched in the extant celebrity endorsement eclipsing leads to a more positive attitude to-
literature (Batra & Homer, 2004; Fleck, Korchia, & Le wards the brand whereas high eclipsing leads to
Roy, 2012; Till & Busler, 1998, 2000; Till, Stanley, & a more negative attitude. When consumers per-
Priluck, 2008). Advertisers attempt to tactically place ceive celebrity–brand match-up, both low and
a celebrity with a brand, whereby the image of the high eclipsing advertisements result in equally
celebrity is seen to match with that of the endorsed positive attitude toward the brand.
brand, providing consumers with consistent informa-
tion about the brand (Kamins, 1990: Kamins & Gupta,
1994). This match-up is said to occur when “highly rele- Eclipsing and Attachment
vant characteristics of the spokesperson are consistent
with highly relevant attributes of the brand” (Misra & Many consumers today seek meaningful relationships
Beatty, 1990, p. 160). Research in celebrity endorse- with celebrities and research shows consumers re-
ment shows that match-up has a positive effect on con- port possessing varying levels of attachment toward
sumer attitude towards the endorsed brands and the celebrities from mild curious interest to celebrity wor-
advertisements in which they feature (Ilicic & Web- ship (McCutcheon, Ashe, Houran, & Maltby, 2003;

1042 ILICIC AND WEBSTER


Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
Reeves, Baker, & Truluck, 2012; Thomson, 2006). Ap- sumers expected to report the lowest brand attitude
plying attachment theory (Bowlby, 1979, 1980), Thom- when exposed to a high eclipsing than a low eclipsing
son (2006) demonstrates that the strength of an attach- endorsement, due to the difficulty with which to judge
ment is evident in heightened feelings of separation dis- the brand. A mismatch/weak attachment/low eclipsing
tress when threatened with real or imagined separation situation allows consumers to construct some link be-
from the celebrity. Research also finds that consumer– tween the celebrity and brand, which should increase
celebrity attachment results in a more positive attitude consumer brand attitude. As such, the following is
towards the advertisement and brand that the celebrity proposed:
endorses (Ilicic & Webster, 2011).
When consumers have a strong attachment to a H4: When consumers have weak attachment to a
celebrity, they use their positive emotional connection celebrity that mismatches the brand, low eclips-
to the celebrity as a heuristic to form positive judgments ing results in a more positive attitude towards
about the endorsed brand. Previous research finds con- the brand whereas high eclipsing results in a
sumers form positive attitudes toward multiple en- lower attitude towards the brand. When con-
dorsed brands when their attachment to a celebrity is sumers have strong attachment to a celebrity
strong (Ilicic and Webster, 2011). According to associa- that matches the brand, eclipsing has no effect
tive network theory, when consumers have a weak at- on attitude towards the brand.
tachment to a celebrity, the celebrity–brand connection
is more difficult to establish. Consumers with a weak
attachment to the celebrity are expected to use ana- STUDY 1
lytical, detailed processing in order to establish and
understand the associative link, or pairing, between When consumers are unable to form a celebrity–brand
the celebrity and brand (Lynch, 1981). It is suggested connection, their attitude towards the brand is weak
that eclipsing may increase the effects of those with (Till, 1998). Study 1 aims to demonstrate that celebrity
weak celebrity attachment, with consumers reporting eclipsing, when the celebrity dominates the advertise-
increased attitude towards the brand when eclipsing ment, results in negative attitude towards the endorsed
is low than when eclipsing is high. Again, low eclips- brand.
ing endorsements should enable consumers to form a Study 1 uses an actual television advertisement to
celebrity–brand association, resulting in a more pos- examine an endorsement between a corporate brand
itive attitude towards the brand. It is not expected and a celebrity. The corporate brand is a leading re-
that eclipsing influences those with strong celebrity gional financial institution in Australia, the Greater
attachment, as simple peripheral processing (Petty, Building Society, and the celebrity is Jerry Seinfeld,
Cacioppo, & Schumann, 1983) is used to form quick an internationally known comedian and actor. A re-
positive judgments about the brand. Consumers with search company was used to recruit subjects from re-
strong celebrity attachment focus their attention on the gional areas within Australia to evaluate their attitude
celebrity in forming their judgments, regardless of the towards the Greater Building Society. A total of 220
level of eclipsing in the advertisement. As such, the subjects were assigned to one of two experimental con-
following is suggested: ditions: (1) low eclipsing advertisement, featuring both
celebrity and brand information (110 subjects) and (2)
H3: When celebrity attachment is weak, low eclips-
a high eclipsing advertisement, predominantly featur-
ing leads to a more positive attitude towards the
ing the celebrity (110 subjects). To control for exposure
brand whereas high eclipsing leads to a more time effects, both advertisements ran for 20 seconds.
negative attitude. When celebrity attachment Overall, 51.8% of subjects were female and 48.2% were
is strong, both low and high eclipsing result in male. Subjects were mainly between 35–49 and 50–64
equally positive attitude towards the brand. years of age (24.1% and 28.6%, respectively).

Eclipsing, Match-up, and Attachment


Procedure and Measures
A three-way interaction between eclipsing, match-up,
and attachment is expected. In a match-up/strong at- Subjects were directed first to view one of two ad-
tachment situation, consumers should be able to estab- vertisements. In the high eclipsing condition, subjects
lish a celebrity–brand connection, as match-up and at- viewed Jerry Seinfeld in an advertisement for the
tachment are used as heuristics in developing positive Greater Building Society. The advertisement featured
judgments about the brand, regardless of whether they the image of Jerry crawling along a sandy desert with
are exposed to a high or low eclipsing endorsement. As ripped clothing and then answering a phone stating
such, it is expected that those who perceive a match-up “Hello . . . I’m busy now.” The advertisement also con-
and have a strong attachment to the celebrity will re- tained the voiceover of a customer stating that since
port positive attitude towards the brand. Alternatively, escaping the big banks her savings were growing and
in a mismatch/weak attachment situation, eclipsing is showed the Greater Building Society’s brand logo at the
expected to exert the strongest influence, with con- end. In the low eclipsing condition, subjects viewed an

INFLUENCE OF ECLIPSING ON BRAND ATTITUDE 1043


Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
advertisement featuring an image of a Greater Building (high eclipsing vs. low eclipsing). Of the 185 subjects
Society customer discussing the multiple benefits of the recruited, 31.9% were male and 68.1% were female.
brand in terms of escaping the big banks and gaining a Subjects were mainly between the ages of 25–24 and
higher interest rate on savings accounts with no trans- 35–49 (28.1% and 24.3%, respectively). Ninety-six sub-
action fees. The advertisement then followed with the jects were assigned to the low eclipsing condition and
image of Jerry crawling along the desert and answer- 89 to the high eclipsing condition.
ing the phone, and ended with the Greater’s brand logo
shown on the screen (YouTube, 2011). Once subjects Procedure and Measures
viewed their allocated 20-second advertisement, they
were then asked to evaluate the Greater brand based Subjects were directed to view one of two advertise-
on the advertisement they just viewed. Subjects were ments. In the high eclipsing condition, subjects viewed
then asked to rate their attitude towards the brand us- Nicole Kidman in a print advertisement for the Omega
ing Mitchell and Olson’s (1981) 5-point semantic differ- brand, making no reference to the brand. In this adver-
ential scales that included four items good/bad, pleas- tisement, Nicole Kidman provided information about
ant/unpleasant, like very much/dislike very much, high herself including her name, inspiration, biggest chal-
quality/poor quality (Cronbach α = 0.932). lenge, childhood ambition, and truism. The ad featured
a half-page, informal portrait of the celebrity in black
and white on the left. The right-hand side of the page
Results and Discussion featured the same generated celebrity answers in hand-
written font to their inspiration (I am inspired by so
It is expected that subjects will report a more positive many things every day), biggest challenge (life), child-
attitude towards the brand when exposed to an ad- hood ambition (trumpet player), and truism (is about
vertisement that features a low eclipsing endorsement taking my own path). The advertisement did not men-
than when exposed to an advertisement that features tion the endorsed brand, instead highlighted informa-
a high eclipsing endorsement. To test this hypothesis, tion about the celebrity. In the low eclipsing condition,
an independent samples t-test was performed. The re- subjects viewed an advertisement similar to the high
sults find a significant difference (t = − 5.32, p = 0.000) eclipsing condition, but the advertisement also included
in brand attitude between the high eclipsing and low a clear endorsement for the brand in larger font (the ad-
eclipsing advertisements. As intended, subjects report vertisement stated “my watch, Omega”). The advertise-
a more positive attitude towards the brand when ex- ments for each condition featured the brand’s logo in the
posed to a low eclipsing advertisement (M = 3.92, SD = bottom right-hand corner of the same size. Once sub-
0.81) and a more negative attitude towards the brand jects viewed their allocated advertisement, they were
when exposed to a high eclipsing advertisement (M = then asked to indicate the degree to which they be-
3.64, SD = 0.99), providing support for Hypothesis 1. lieved the celebrity and brand match using three items
The results of Study 1 provide initial evidence drawn from Till and Busler (2000), including do not be-
of eclipsing effects in celebrity endorsement. When long together/do belong together, do not go together/do
celebrities overshadow the brand, when they are the go together, and do not fit/do fit (Cronbach α = 0.959).
focus within an advertisement, consumer attitude to- They then were asked their attitude towards the brand
wards the brand is weaker. using the same measures as Study 1, with internal con-
sistency confirmed (Cronbach α = 0.924). Subjects’ de-
gree of familiarity with the brand was also measured
STUDY 2 on a 7-point Likert scale to use as a covariate in the
analysis, as consumers report strong associations re-
Study 2 aims to demonstrate that celebrity eclips- sulting in stronger attitude for familiar brands (Fazio,
ing moderates the effect of perceived celebrity–brand 1986, 1989).
match-up on consumer attitude towards the endorsed
brand. It is expected that eclipsing reduces consumer Results and Discussion
brand attitude for those who perceive the celebrity and
brand to mismatch, with consumers reporting reduced Manipulation of eclipsing occurred as intended, with an
attitude towards the brand when eclipsing is high than independent samples t-test finding a significant differ-
when eclipsing is low. ence (t = 4.220, p = 0.000) in high versus low eclipsing
Study 2 examines another real celebrity endorse- on brand attitude. As intended, subjects report a more
ment between Nicole Kidman and Omega watches us- positive attitude towards the brand in the low eclipsing
ing a constructed advertisement, copying a similar condition (M = 4.06, SD = 0.69) and a weaker attitude
structure to those used in the 2007 AMEX print cam- toward the brand in the high eclipsing condition (M =
paign. A research company recruited subjects from 3.66, SD = 0.60).
metropolitan areas within Australia. A total of 185 com- The moderating role of eclipsing on brand atti-
plete questionnaires were obtained. Subjects were ex- tude is examined. Brand attitude is regressed sep-
posed to created print advertisements and assigned to arately on match-up (Model 1); on eclipsing (Model
one of the same two conditions as described in Study 1 2); on match-up and eclipsing (Model 3); and then on

1044 ILICIC AND WEBSTER


Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
Table 1. Study 2 Match-up and Eclipsing as Predictors in the Models of Brand Attitude.
Brand Attitude (N = 185)
Unstandardized Coefficient (β)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
(Match-up (Eclipsing (Match-up and (Match-up, Eclipsing, and
Only) Only) Eclipsing) Interaction)

Intercept 3.776∗∗∗ 4.753∗∗∗ 4.257∗∗∗ 4.095∗∗∗


Match-up 0.359∗∗∗ 0.329∗∗∗ 0.843∗∗∗
Eclipsing −0.356∗∗∗ −0.191∗∗∗ −0.138
Match-up × Eclipsing −.203∗∗
Brand Familiarity 0.201∗∗∗ 0.248∗∗∗ 0.198∗∗∗ 0.192∗∗∗
F-value 58.71 27.45 41.99 33.64
Adjusted R2 0.39 0.22 0.40 0.42

p < 0.05, ∗∗ p < 0.01, ∗∗∗ p < 0.001

match-up, eclipsing, and the interaction terms (Model


4), with brand familiarity as the covariate. The data
for the independent variable of match-up and covari-
ate of brand familiarity are mean centered. Eclipsing
is dummy coded 0 (low eclipsing) and 1 (high eclips-
ing). Table 1 indicates that match-up and eclipsing
are both similarly significant predictors of brand at-
titude, with the brand familiarity covariate also signifi-
cant. Match-up is a stronger predictor of brand attitude
than eclipsing, when both are included in the model
(Model 3; unstandardized betamatch-up = 0.329; unstan-
dardized betaeclipsing = −0.191). As expected, the model
with match-up, eclipsing, and the two-way interaction
(Model 4) offers a better goodness of fit and increased
variance explained, compared with the models in which
Figure 1. Study 2—moderating role of eclipsing on brand
match-up or eclipsing serve as single predictors. More
attitude.
specifically, Table 1 shows that the adjusted R2 values
significantly increase from 0.39 to 0.42 (Models 1–4:
F (180) = 5.456, p < 0.05) when match-up, eclipsing, estingly, eclipsing has a moderating effect on match-up,
and the two-way interaction appear as predictors in with low eclipsing resulting in a more positive brand at-
the model than when match-up features alone. titude than high eclipsing, providing partial support for
Model 4 shows that a significant, negative inter- Hypothesis 2.
action effect occurs. Aiken and West’s (1991) plotting The results of Study 2 reinforce the importance
technique, plus simple slopes analysis are used to ex- of eclipsing in influencing consumer brand attitude.
amine the significant two-way interaction on brand Eclipsing results in negative brand attitude irrespec-
attitude. Simple slopes analysis tests whether the ef- tive of perceived match-up. Eclipsing has a significant
fects of the independent variable significantly differ at moderating role on those who perceive the celebrity
low and high levels of the moderating variable (Aiken and brand to match and mismatch, with consumers
and West, 1991). The significant interaction effect for reporting a more positive brand attitude when they
match-up and eclipsing on brand attitude is plotted in see the celebrity and brand both in the advertisement
Figure 1, with high match-up as 1 SD above the mean (low eclipsing) than when the celebrity overshadows the
and low match-up as 1 SD below the mean. Simple brand (high eclipsing). Interestingly, the effect of eclips-
slopes analysis shows that when consumers perceive ing is greater when consumers perceive the celebrity
both mismatch and match, eclipsing has an effect on and brand to match, suggesting that low eclipsing may
brand attitude (unstandardized betalow eclipsing = 0.96, enhance perceived match-up between the celebrity and
t = 3.599, p < 0.001; unstandardized betahigh eclipsing = brand.
0.73, t = 4.003, p < 0.001).
As expected, consumers who perceive celebrity–
brand mismatch report the most positive attitude STUDY 3
towards the brand when they are exposed to an adver-
tisement that features low eclipsing, and a more nega- Study 3 aims to demonstrate that high celebrity eclips-
tive attitude towards the brand when they are exposed ing moderates the effect of weak consumer–celebrity at-
to an advertisement that features high eclipsing. Inter- tachment on consumer attitude towards the endorsed

INFLUENCE OF ECLIPSING ON BRAND ATTITUDE 1045


Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
Table 2. Study 3 Attachment and Eclipsing as Predictors in the Models of Brand Attitude.
Brand Attitude (N = 208)
Unstandardized Coefficient (β)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
(Attachment (Eclipsing (Attachment (Attachment, Eclipsing,
Only) Only) and Eclipsing) and Interaction)

Intercept 3.403∗∗∗ 3.628∗∗∗ 3.558∗∗∗ 3.581∗∗∗


Attachment 0.335∗∗∗ 0.323∗∗∗ 0.220∗∗∗
Eclipsing −0.408∗∗ −0.308∗ −0.317∗
Attachment × Eclipsing 0.227∗
Brand Familiarity 0.203∗∗ 0.200∗∗ 0.198∗∗ 0.218∗∗∗
F-value 32.882 8.534 24.339 20.346
Adjusted R2 0.24 0.07 0.26 0.28

p < 0.05, ∗∗ p < 0.01, ∗∗∗ p < 0.001.

brand. It is expected that eclipsing will reduce the the same size. Once subjects viewed their allocated
effects of weak attachment on brand attitude, with advertisement, they were then asked to indicate the
consumers reporting decreased attitude towards the degree to which they were attached to the celebrity,
brand when eclipsing is high than when eclipsing is using three items drawn from Thomson (2006) includ-
low. Study 3 also examines the three-way interaction ing: “Losing (celebrity) forever would be distressing to
between match-up/attachment/eclipsing on consumer me,” “I miss (celebrity) when (he/she) is not around,”
brand attitude. It is expected that when consumers and “If (celebrity) was permanently gone I would be up-
perceive celebrity–brand mismatch and have a weak set” (Cronbach α = 0.925). They then were asked their
attachment to the celebrity, low eclipsing will result in attitude towards the brand and perceived match-up be-
a more positive brand attitude than high eclipsing. tween the celebrity and brand using the same measures
Study 3 examines two other real celebrity endorse- as Study 2, with internal consistency confirmed (Cron-
ments between Toni Collette (actress) and Common- bach αattitude towards the brand = 0.948; Cronbach αmatch-up =
wealth Bank, and Keith Urban (musician) and Sam- 0.981). Subjects’ degree of familiarity with the brand
sung. A research company recruited subjects from was also measured on a 7-point Likert scale to use as a
metropolitan areas within Australia. A total of 208 com- covariate in the analysis.
pleted questionnaires were obtained. Subjects were ex-
posed to the same created print advertisement format
as Study 2 and assigned to one of the same two con- Results and Discussion
ditions as described in Study 2. Of the 208 subjects
recruited, 38.5% were male and 61.5% were female. Manipulation of eclipsing occurred as intended, with an
Subjects were mainly between the ages of 35–49 and independent samples t-test finding a significant differ-
50–64 (24.5% and 27.4%, respectively). ence (t = 2.784, p < 0.01) in high versus low eclipsing
on brand attitude. As intended, subjects report a more
positive attitude towards the brand in a low eclipsing
Procedure and Measures condition (M = 3.61, SD = 1.01) and a weaker attitude
toward the brand in the high eclipsing condition (M =
Subjects were directed to view one of two advertise- 3.21, SD = 1.03).
ments. In the high eclipsing condition, subjects viewed The moderating role of eclipsing on brand attitude
either Toni Collette or Keith Urban in a print adver- is again examined. Brand attitude is regressed sepa-
tisement for the assigned brand (Commonwealth Bank rately on attachment (Model 1); on eclipsing (Model 2);
and Samsung, respectively), making no reference to the on attachment and eclipsing (Model 3); and then on at-
brand. In this advertisement, the celebrity provided the tachment, eclipsing, and the interaction terms (Model
same information about herself or himself as in Study 4), with brand familiarity as the covariate. The data
2. The advertisement again featured a half-page, in- are mean centered for the independent variable of at-
formal portrait of the celebrity in black and white on tachment and covariate of brand familiarity. Eclipsing
the left. The right-hand side of the page featured the is dummy coded 0 (low eclipsing) and 1 (high eclips-
same generated celebrity answers in hand-written font ing). Table 2 indicates that attachment and eclips-
as in Study 2. In the low eclipsing condition, subjects ing both are significant predictors of brand attitude,
viewed an advertisement similar to the high eclips- with the brand familiarity covariate also significant.
ing condition, which also included a clear endorsement Attachment is a slightly stronger predictor of brand
for the brand in larger font (the advertisement stated attitude than eclipsing, when both are added to the
“my bank, Commonwealth Bank” or “my phone, Sam- model (Model 3; unstandardized betaattachment = 0.323;
sung”). The advertisements for each condition featured unstandardized betaeclipsing = −0.308). As expected, the
the brand’s logo in the bottom right-hand corner of model with attachment, eclipsing, and the two-way

1046 ILICIC AND WEBSTER


Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
attachment to the celebrity, providing support for
Hypothesis 3.
The results of Study 3 again support the role of
eclipsing on consumer brand attitude. Eclipsing is a
significant moderator of consumer brand attitude when
consumers have a weak attachment to the celebrity,
with consumers reporting a more positive attitude
when they see the celebrity and brand both empha-
sized in an advertisement (low eclipsing), than when
the celebrity overshadows and dominates the brand in
an endorsement (high eclipsing).
Next, the three-way interaction between match-up,
attachment, and eclipsing is examined. Models 1 and 4
in Table 3 indicate that match-up is a much stronger
predictor than attachment and eclipsing (Model 1; un-
Figure 2. Study 3—moderating role of eclipsing on brand standardized betamatch-up = 0.568, Model 4; unstandard-
attitude. ized betamatch-up = 0.421, unstandardized betaattachment
= 0.119, unstandardized betaeclipsing = −0.222). Addi-
tionally, Model 4 shows a significant, negative three-
interaction (Model 4) offers a better goodness of fit and way interaction effect. As expected, the model with the
increased variance explained, compared with the mod- match-up, attachment, eclipsing, and the three-way in-
els in which attachment or eclipsing serve as single teraction (Model 4) offers a better goodness of fit and
predictors. Table 2 shows that the adjusted R2 values increased variance explained, compared with the mod-
significantly increase from 0.24 to 0.28 (Models 1–4: els in which match-up, attachment, or eclipsing serve
F (207) = 6.159, p < 0.01) when attachment, eclipsing, as single predictors. Table 3 shows that the adjusted R2
and the two-way interaction appear as predictors in the values significantly increase from 0.28 to 0.37 (Models
model than when attachment features alone. 1–4: F (203) = 24.193, p < 0.001) when match-up, at-
Aiken and West’s (1991) plotting technique plus sim- tachment, eclipsing, and the three-way interaction ap-
ple slopes analysis are used to examine the significant pear as predictors in the model, than when attachment
two-way interaction on brand attitude. Figure 2 is plot- features alone.
ted at strong (1 SD above mean) and weak (1 SD be- Aiken and West’s (1991) plotting technique plus sim-
low mean) levels of attachment. Simple slopes analysis ple slopes analysis are used to examine the significant
shows that the slope for high eclipsing is significant, three-way interaction on brand attitude. The signifi-
while the slope for low eclipsing is not (unstandardized cant interaction effect for the three-way interaction on
betahigh eclipsing = 0.54, t = 5.791, p < 0.001; unstandard- brand attitude is plotted in Figure 3A and B, with high
ized betalow eclipsing = −0.10, t = − 0.577, p > 0.05). as 1 SD above the mean and low as 1 SD below the
As expected, consumers report the most negative at- mean. Simple slope analysis (Figure 3) shows that when
titude towards the brand when they have a weak at- consumers have weak attachment to the celebrity and
tachment to the celebrity and are exposed to an ad- perceive a mismatch between the celebrity and brand,
vertisement that features high eclipsing, and a more the slopes for both high and low eclipsing are significant
positive attitude when exposed to a low eclipsing ad- (unstandardized betahigh eclipsing = 0.45, t = 5.033, p <
vertisement. In addition, eclipsing does not impact con- 0.001; unstandardized betalow eclipsing = 0.52, t = 5.047,
sumer brand attitude for those who have a strong p < 0.000). When consumers have a weak attachment

Table 3. Study 3 Match-up, Attachment, and Eclipsing as Predictors in the Models of Brand Attitude.
Brand Attitude (N = 208)
Unstandardized Coefficient (β)
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
(Match-up Only) (Attachment Only) (Eclipsing Only) (Three-Way Interaction)

Intercept 3.403∗∗∗ 3.403∗∗∗ 3.628∗∗∗ 3.515∗∗∗


Match-up 0.568∗∗∗ 0.421∗∗∗
Attachment 0.335∗∗∗ 0.119∗
Eclipsing −0.408∗∗ −0.222
Match-up × Attachment × Eclipsing 0.069∗
Brand Familiarity 0.187∗∗ 0.203∗∗∗ 0.200∗∗ 0.198∗∗∗
F-value 40.907 32.822 8.534 24.193
Adjusted R2 0.28 0.24 0.07 0.37

p < 0.05, ∗∗ p < 0.01, ∗∗∗ p < 0.001.

INFLUENCE OF ECLIPSING ON BRAND ATTITUDE 1047


Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
up, attachment, eclipsing, and the three-way interac-
tion indicate that match-up, attachment, and eclipsing
remain as significant predictors of brand attitude. The
significant three-way interaction effect, however, sug-
gests that those who have weak celebrity attachment
report a more positive attitude when exposed to a low
eclipsing endorsement, when the celebrity is perceived
to either match or mismatch the brand. Those who have
strong celebrity attachment report stronger brand at-
titude when exposed to a high eclipsing endorsement,
when the celebrity is perceived to either match or mis-
match the brand.

GENERAL DISCUSSION AND


CONCLUSION

This research examines celebrities when they eclipse,


or overshadow, the brand in endorsements, where the
celebrity dominates the advertisement. Previous re-
search on the effectiveness of celebrity endorsers cen-
ters on the concept of match-up, or fit, of relevant
characteristics between the celebrity and brand (Fleck,
Korchia, & Le Roy, 2012; Kamins, 1990; Kamins &
Gupta, 1994; Till & Busler, 1998, 2000; Till, Stanley, &
Priluck, 2008), and the degree of consumer attachment
toward the celebrity (Ilicic & Webster, 2011; Thom-
son, 2006). Findings from three studies confirm that
celebrity eclipsing can negatively influence consumer
attitude towards endorsed brands.
Results show that although consumers may perceive
a match between the celebrity and endorsed brand, ad-
vertisements should not focus predominantly on the
celebrity themselves. By accentuating the celebrity, the
Figure 3. Study 3—moderating role of attachment and connection between the celebrity and brand is dimin-
eclipsing on brand attitude. (A) Weak attachment and (B) ished and, as such, consumer brand attitude weak-
strong attachment. ens. Advertisers should emphasize both the celebrity
and brand in advertisements in order to establish a
celebrity–brand association within consumer memory
and perceive celebrity–brand mismatch and match, low structures.
eclipsing increases their brand attitude. The results for Findings suggest that eclipsing strongly influences
strong attachment, mismatch, and match show that the consumer brand attitude when consumers have a weak
slopes for both high and low eclipsing are significant attachment to the celebrity. Those consumers with
(unstandardized betahigh eclipsing = 0.45, t = 5.033, p < weak celebrity attachment have significantly more pos-
0.001; unstandardized betalow eclipsing = 0.52, t = 5.047, p itive attitude towards the endorsed brand in low eclips-
< 0.000). Interestingly, when consumers have a strong ing endorsements, when both the celebrity and brand
attachment to the celebrity and perceive the celebrity are emphasized, than in high eclipsing endorsements.
to either match or mismatch, their brand attitude in- Low eclipsing enables consumers to form a celebrity–
creases when they are exposed to a high eclipsing en- brand connection, which results in a more positive
dorsement than a low eclipsing endorsement, providing brand attitude, whereas high eclipsing further discon-
only partial support for Hypothesis 4. nects the celebrity and brand, and results in a more
The results of Study 3 again demonstrate that eclips- negative brand attitude. Findings from this study also
ing may negatively influence consumer brand attitude. show a three-way interaction, with high eclipsing re-
Eclipsing has a significant moderating role on those sulting in a more negative brand attitude when con-
who have a weak attachment to the celebrity, with sumers have a weak attachment to the celebrity and
subjects reporting a more positive attitude when they when they perceive the celebrity and brand to either
see the celebrity and brand both in the advertisement match or mismatch. Interestingly, low eclipsing can re-
(low eclipsing) than when the celebrity overshadows the sult in a negative brand attitude when consumers have
brand (high eclipsing). The models that include match- a strong attachment to the celebrity and when they

1048 ILICIC AND WEBSTER


Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
perceive the celebrity and brand to either match or mis- 2007), research also should look at eclipsing in ad-
match. vertisements with other types of co-branding partner-
The findings of these studies have significant impli- ships, such as strategic alliances between corporate
cations for advertisers and brand managers in the ex- brands in related versus unrelated product categories.
ecution of their advertisements featuring celebrity en- For example, airlines, hotels, and car rentals such
dorsers. Match-up exerts stronger influence on brand as Qantas, Marriott, and Hertz are related product
attitude when consumers are exposed to low eclipsing categories, whereas media conglomerate Disney and
endorsements. Low eclipsing endorsements also result car manufacturer Mazda are unrelated. Findings from
in a more positive brand attitude when consumers have such research would identify additional conditions un-
weak celebrity attachment. Brand managers are un- der which co-branding partnerships can be detrimental
likely to select an endorser to whom all of their target or beneficial to consumer evaluations of co-branding
market is attached. As such, low eclipsing advertise- partners.
ments ensure that consumer brand attitude is not fur-
ther diluted. Alternatively, when the target market is
strongly attached to the celebrity, regardless of whether
the celebrity and brand are perceived to match or mis-
match, advertisements should focus on the celebrity REFERENCES
and create a high eclipsing endorsement situation in
order to increase brand attitude. Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression testing
Associative network theory (Anderson, 1983; Mar- and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
tindale, 1991) underpins the mechanisms under which Anderson, J. R. (1983). Language, memory, and thought. Hills-
eclipsing may affect consumer attitude towards en- dale, N J: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
dorsed brands. According to the theory, a brand should Bailey, A. A. (2007). Public information and consumer skep-
enable consumers to make a celebrity–brand connec- ticism effects on celebrity endorsements: Studies among
young consumers. Journal of Marketing Communications,
tion. The more difficult it is for consumers to make
13, 85–107.
a connection, the weaker their attitude towards the Batra, R., & Homer, P. M. (2004). The situational impact of
endorsed brand. Evidence is provided to suggest that brand image beliefs. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14,
eclipsing occurs across multiple media, in both televi- 318–330.
sion advertisements and also in print. Future research, Berlyne, D. E. (1966). Curiosity and exploration. Science, 153,
however, should examine whether eclipsing influences 25–33.
consumer message comprehension and elaborative pro- Bettman, J. R., & Sujan, M. (1987). Effects of framing on evalu-
cessing in television versus print. Length of exposure ation of comparable and noncomparable alternatives by ex-
to the celebrity may influence consumer involvement in pert and novice consumers. Journal of Consumer Research,
the advertisement and advertisement elaboration. One 14(September), 141–154.
Bigand, E., McAdams, S., & Foret, S. (2000). Divided Attention
way to test for the effects of eclipsing and consumer
in Music. International Journal of Psychology, 35, 270–278.
involvement is to conduct an eye-tracking experiment. Bowlby, John (1979). The making and breaking of affectional
With eye tracking, precise measurements of attention bonds. London: Tavistock.
to the celebrity versus brand are obtained. Also, eclips- Bowlby, John (1980). Loss: sadness and depression. New York:
ing should be investigated with established versus new Basic Books.
brands to examine whether this execution style dilutes Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). Theory of semantic pro-
brand attitude only for new brands, where consumers cessing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428.
have weakly formed brand associations. Donderi, D. C. (2006). Visual complexity: A review. Psycholog-
Future research also should explore the role of ical Bulletin, 132, 73–97.
eclipsing on celebrity attitude. A low eclipsing endorse- Engel, F. L. (1970). Visual conspicuity, directed attention and
retinal locus. Vision Research, 11, 563–576.
ment situation that places the celebrity in a weak-
Engel, F. L. (1977). Visual conspicuity, visual search and fixa-
ened position detracts from the celebrity and subse- tion tendencies of the eye. Vision Research, 17, 95–108.
quently, the brand then eclipses and overshadows the Fazio, R. H. (1986). How do attitudes guide behavior? In R. M.
celebrity. To date, research examines consequences for Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), The handbook of moti-
the endorsed brands; however, research should em- vation and cognition: Foundations for social behavior (pp.
pirically explore the reciprocal effect of endorsements 204–243). New York: Guilford Press.
on celebrities. Since consumers consider celebrities as Fazio, R. H. (1989). On the power and functionality of atti-
“selling out” by pairing with brands purely for mone- tudes: The role of attitude accessibility. In A. Pratkanis,
tary reasons (Bailey, 2007), celebrities are increasingly S. Breckler, & A. Greenwald (Eds.), Attitude structure and
concerned with protecting their own brand image and function (pp. 153–179). Hillsdale, N J: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
maintaining consumer perceptions of their authentic-
Fleck, N., Korchia, M., & Le Roy, I. (2012). Celebrities in ad-
ity. Research is yet to examine the conditions under vertising: Looking for congruence or likability? Psychology
which endorsements are mutually beneficial and when & Marketing, 29, 651–662.
they are detrimental to celebrity “human brand” status. Friedman, H. H., & Friedman, L. (1979). Endorser effective-
As celebrity endorsement is a form of co-branding ness by product type. Journal of Advertising Research,
partnership (Keel & Nataraajan, 2012; Seno & Lukas, 19(October/November), 63–71.

INFLUENCE OF ECLIPSING ON BRAND ATTITUDE 1049


Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar
Ho, G., Scialfa, C. T., Caird, J. K., & Graw, T. (2001). Visual Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Schumann, D. (1983). Cen-
search for traffic signs: The effects of clutter, luminance, tral and peripheral routes to advertising effectiveness: The
and aging. Human Factors, 43, 208–216. moderating role of involvement. Journal of Consumer Re-
Ilicic, J., & Webster, C. M. (2011). Effects of multiple endorse- search, 10, 135–146.
ments and consumer-celebrity attachment on attitude and Reeves, R. A., Baker, G. A., & Truluck, C. S. (2012). Celebrity
purchase intention. Australasian Marketing Journal, 19, worship, materialism, compulsive buying, and the empty
230–237. self. Psychology & Marketing, 29, 674–679.
Ilicic, J., & Webster, C. M. (2013). Celebrity co-branding part- Rossiter, J. R., & Percy, L. (1980). Attitude Change through
ners as irrelevant brand information in advertisements. Visual Imagery in Advertising. Journal of Advertising, 9,
Journal of Business Research, 66, 941–947. 10–16.
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking fast and slow, London: Pen- Rossiter, J. R., & Smidts, A. (2012). Print advertising:
guin Books. Celebrity presenters. Journal of Business Research, 65,
Kamins, M. A. (1990). An investigation of the ‘match-up hy- 874–879.
pothesis’ in celebrity advertising: When beauty may be only Seno, D., & Lukas, B. A. (2007). The equity effect of product
skin deep. Journal of Advertising, 19, 4–13. endorsement by celebrities: A conceptual framework from
Kamins, M. A., & Gupta, K. (1994). Congruence between a co-branding perspective. European Journal of Marketing,
spokesperson and product type: A match-up hypothesis per- 41, 121–34.
spective. Psychology & Marketing, 11, 4–13. Swaminathan, V., Fox, R. J., & Reddy, S. K. (2001). The im-
Keel, A., & Nataraajan, R. (2012). Celebrity endorsements and pact of brand extension introduction on choice. Journal of
beyond: New avenues for celebrity branding. Psychology & Marketing, 65, 1–15.
Marketing, 29, 690–703. Thomson, M. (2006). Human brands: Investigating an-
Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York: tecedents to consumers’ strong attachments to celebrities.
Harcourt, Brace & World. Journal of Marketing, 70, 104–119.
Lidwell, W., Holden, K., & Butler, J. (2010). Universal prin- Till, B. D. (1998). Using celebrity endorsers effectively:
ciples of design: 125 ways to enhance usability, influ- Lessons from associative learning. Journal of Product and
ence perception, increase appeal, make better design de- Brand Management, 7, 400–409.
cisions, and teach through design. Singapore: Rockport Till, B. D., & Busler, M. (2000). The match-up hypothesis:
Publishers. Physical attractiveness, expertise, and the role of fit on
Lee, J. G., & Thorson, E. (2008). The impact of celebrity- brand attitude, purchase intent and brand beliefs. Journal
product incongruence on the effectiveness of product en- of Advertising, 14, 1–13.
dorsement. Journal of Advertising Research, 48, 433– Till, B. D., Stanley, S. M., & Priluck, R. (2008). Classical condi-
449. tioning and celebrity endorsers: An examination of belong-
Lynch, J. G. (1985). Uniqueness issues in the decompositional ingness and resistance to extinction. Psychology & Market-
modeling of multiattribute overall evaluations: An infor- ing, 25, 179–196.
mation integration perspective. Journal of Marketing Re- Törn, F. (2012). Revisiting the match-up hypothesis: Ef-
search, 22, 1–19. fects of brand-incongruent celebrity endorsements. Jour-
Lynch, J., & Schuler, D. (1994). The match-up effect of nal of Current Issues and Research in Advertising, 33,
spokesperson and product congruency: A schema the- 20–36.
ory interpretation. Psychology & Marketing, 11, 417– YouTube (2011). Greater-great escape—Jerry Seinfeld 1.
445. Retrieved October 2009, from http://www.youtube.com/
Martindale, C. (1991). Cognitive psychology: A neural-network watch?v=gQPHV8SI6g0&feature=related.
approach. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Youtube (2013). Pepsi Beyoncé “Mirrors”—Official 2013
McCutcheon, L. E., Ashe, D. D., Houran, J., & Maltby, J. video—#BeyHereNow. Retrieved September 2013, from
(2003). A cognitive profile of individuals who tend to wor- http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_2smYV10z.
ship celebrities. The Journal of Psychology, 137, 309–
322. The authors thank Stacey M. Baxter and Lawrence Ang for
Misra, S., & Beatty, S. E. (1990). Celebrity spokesperson and their helpful comments and suggestions, significantly improv-
brand congruence: An assessment of recall and affect. Jour- ing the quality of this paper. They also thank the University of
nal of Business Research, 2, 159–173. Adelaide and Macquarie University for sponsoring this study.
Mitchell, A. A., & Olson, J. C. (1981). Are product attribute
beliefs the only mediator of advertising effects on brand
attitude? Journal of Marketing Research, 18(August), 318– Correspondence regarding this article should be sent to:
332. Jasmina Ilicic, Lecturer in Marketing, Faculty of the Pro-
O’Mahony, S., & Meenaghan, T. (1997/1998). The impact of fessions, Business School, The University of Adelaide, 10
celebrity endorsements on consumers. Irish Marketing Re- Pulteney Street, Adelaide, South Australia, 5005, Australia
view, 10, 15–24. (jasmina.ilicic@adelaide.edu.au).

1050 ILICIC AND WEBSTER


Psychology & Marketing DOI: 10.1002/mar

You might also like