You are on page 1of 19

How Easily Does a Social Difference

Become a Status Distinction? Gender


Matters
Cecilia L. Ridgeway Kristen Backor
Stanford University Stanford University

Yan E. Li Justine E. Tinkler


Stanford University Louisiana State University

Kristan G. Erickson
Stanford University

Are people quick to adopt status beliefs about a social difference that lead them to treat
others unequally? In a test of status construction theory, two experiments show that men
and women form equally strong status beliefs from only two encounters with others. Men
act powerfully on these new beliefs in their next encounters with others but women do
not, possibly because women face greater social risks for acting on ambiguous status
advantages. Women are just as likely as men, however, to treat someone unequally on the
basis of an established status distinction. This suggests that men are first movers in the
emergence of status distinctions, but women eventually adopt the distinctions as well.
Our results show that people readily transform social differences into status
distinctions—distinctions that act as formidable forces of inequality.

tatus distinctions based on social differ-


S ences such as gender, race, or ethnicity are
consequential for social inequality because they
between people becomes a status difference
when status beliefs develop that associate
greater social esteem and competence with peo-
affect how people are treated and how they treat ple in one category than with those from anoth-
others in turn. Status distinctions implicitly bias er and these beliefs become widely disseminated
the everyday processes through which people throughout a population (Berger and Webster
are evaluated, given access to rewards, and 2006). Status beliefs become consequential for
directed toward or away from positions of power inequality at the point when they begin to shape
and prestige in society (Berger et al. 1977; how people treat one another.
Berger and Webster 2006). The aggregate result Status distinctions likely develop as initial
of this unequal treatment is to transform cate- beliefs among some individuals, acquire a grow-
gorical differences between people into sys- ing appearance of social validity or certainty for
tematic axes of inequality. A mere difference those individuals so that they begin to act on the
beliefs, and then eventually spread until they are
widely held in the population (Ridgeway 1991,
2006). Status distinctions thus have dynamic
Direct correspondence to Cecilia Ridgeway, social histories. Their initial development may
Department of Sociology, 450 Serra Mall, Building
go smoothly or may be disrupted so that some
120, Stanford, CA 94305-2047 (ridgeway@
stanford.edu).This research was supported by social differences do not become widely shared
National Science Foundation grant SES-0417404 to status distinctions and axes of inequality
the senior author. We are grateful to Shelley J. Correll (Ridgeway and Correll 2006). Established sta-
for comments on an earlier draft of this article. tus distinctions can also fade if changing social

AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW, 2009, VOL. 74 (February:44–62)


HOW EASILY DOES DIFFERENCE BECOME STATUS?—–45

conditions undermine their validity so that peo- Status beliefs have the character of a social
ple become less likely to act on them. For exam- reputation because they are beliefs about which
ple, ethnic differences among Whites in the categories or groups are more respected. They
United States, such as being Irish or Italian, are beliefs about what “most people” think or,
have lost status significance in recent decades. in the case of newly forming status beliefs, what
To gain an analytic purchase on the dynamic most people would think if they were exposed
processes by which status distinctions develop, to the social difference. An assumption that sta-
we need to better understand the key juncture tus beliefs are widely shared, and thus socially
in this process: At what point do new status valid, gives status beliefs force in social relations
beliefs become powerful enough to affect the because it implies that others are likely to treat
way people treat one another? one in terms of those beliefs. Social validity is
How easily do people form status beliefs the collective dimension of legitimacy, a sense
about a social difference that are sufficiently that others will accept something in a situation,
firm as to cause them to treat others unequally whether or not the actor approves (Weber
on the basis of the difference? If people are 1968:31–33). If others are likely to treat actors
quick to leap to status assumptions that shape according to an implied status belief, then actors
their behavior toward socially different others, must take those beliefs into account in their
then status distinctions have the potential to own behavior. As we shall see, the apparent
develop readily in populations and to act as a social validity of status beliefs is an important
formidable force for social inequality. An answer determinant of people’s willingness to treat oth-
to this question is essential for understanding the ers according to those beliefs (Berger, Ridgeway,
power of the processes by which status inequal- and Zelditch 2002).
ities emerge in society.
This article addresses this question by draw- STATUS CONSTRUCTION THEORY
ing on status construction theory, a contempo- Status construction theory describes a set of
rary theory of how status beliefs develop and processes that are capable of creating status
spread widely throughout a population beliefs and spreading them widely in a popula-
(Ridgeway 1991, 2000, 2006; Troyer 2003; tion (Ridgeway 1991, 2006; Webster and Hysom
Webster and Hysom 1998). We also draw on evi- 1998). The theory focuses on the local contexts
dence suggesting that gender matters at the ini- in which people from different categories of a
tial point when people begin treating others social difference encounter one another. It
according to a newly emerging status distinction. argues that under certain conditions, these local
The literature on gender stereotyping suggests encounters create experiences that induce par-
that women will be more cautious about acting ticipants to form status beliefs about their social
on newly forming status beliefs. Using two difference. Furthermore, such local encounters
experiments, we evaluate how easily people spread status beliefs because participants carry
form status beliefs that cause them to treat oth- them into other encounters and “teach” them to
ers unequally. Each experiment allows us to new participants by treating them in accord
examine the process separately for men and for with the belief. The result is a diffusion process
women. that can spread status beliefs widely throughout
a population.
STATUS BELIEFS When do beliefs formed in local contexts
spread widely in a population? And which cat-
Status beliefs associate social worthiness and egory of a social difference will be cast as high-
competence at “the things that count” with rec- er status? To answer these questions, the theory
ognized social differences. Unlike the in-group turns to the social structural conditions that
favoritism that can arise from the simple per- frame the local contexts in which people from
ception of group difference, status beliefs are different categories encounter one another.
consensual. They are accepted or conceded as Particularly important is whether people from
a matter of social reality by those they disad- one category are structurally advantaged in
vantage as well as those they advantage (Brewer some factor, such as material resources or tech-
and Brown 1998; Tajfel and Turner 1986). nology, that provides a systematic advantage in
46—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

gaining influence over those from another cat- anticipate and manage the likely behavior and
egory. If this is the case, then status beliefs judgments of others. According to status con-
favoring the advantaged group are likely to struction theory, then, encounters between
overwhelm counter beliefs in the population socially different actors create status beliefs by
and spread to become widely held presenting their participants with apparently
(Ridgeway1991, 2006). valid local realities in which differences in influ-
According to the theory, local encounters ence and competence are associated with a
cause participants to form status beliefs by pre- social difference.
senting them with apparently valid associations Once actors form an initial status belief about
between a social difference and indicators of a social difference, what affects whether they act
esteem and competence, such as influence and on it and treat others in new situations accord-
deference (Ridgeway 2000, 2006). Specifically, ing to the belief? When status beliefs are shared,
when people from different categories of a rec- they act as common knowledge that allows
ognized social difference must regularly coop- actors to coordinate their behavior (Chwe 2001).
erate to achieve their goals, hierarchies of The sense that a status belief is socially valid is
influence and esteem are likely to develop in the sense that it will be accepted by most oth-
these intercategory encounters just as they do in ers and therefore can be relied on to help actors
virtually all cooperative, goal-oriented encoun- anticipate the behavior of others and effective-
ters. The formation of this hierarchy creates a ly coordinate their own behavior. While a valid
shared reality for the participants in which a per- status belief coordinates behavior, action on a
son from one category, say an A, is more active, status belief of uncertain validity risks a failure
influential, and apparently more competent, of coordination and a potentially confusing and
while the other person, a B, is cast into the role embarrassing social breach. Action on beliefs of
of reacting to the A. The hierarchy develops uncertain social validity, therefore, is poten-
implicitly through many small behaviors so its tially awkward and socially costly.
origins are obscure for the participants while the This suggests that for an actor to transfer a
social difference is salient. Through the unfold- fledgling new status belief to a different situa-
ing of events, it is simply revealed that the A is tion, and spontaneously treat another according
more proactive, influential, and competent, and to that belief, it must appear to be a convincingly
the B is more reactive and apparently less com- valid indicator of how people from different
petent. social categories are likely to behave. As a result,
Because both As and Bs participate in these initial status beliefs are likely to require some
events, the patterns that they enact appear con- degree of further validation before they become
sensual. As a result, the social validity of what a basis for behavior (Berger et al. 2002). Status
the pattern reveals about As and Bs becomes dif- construction theory assumes that the validity of
ficult for the participants to deny. If subsequent a new status belief is further strengthened by
encounters repeat the same pattern, either in subsequent encounters that support it and under-
the way the actor is treated or in behavior the mined by those that contradict it (Ridgeway
actor witnesses between others, the apparent 1991, 2006). Multiple, consistent, and clearly
consensual support for this pattern seems even valid local experiences are likely necessary to
broader. Eventually, even the Bs are forced to induce new status beliefs that are strong enough
concede, as a matter of social reality, that “most to affect actors’ treatment of others.
people” would rate As in general as higher sta-
tus and more competent than Bs. The repeated, DOES GENDER MATTER IN ACTING
consistent appearance of consensus in the local
ON INITIAL STATUS BELIEFS?
situations “bootstraps” the formation of status
beliefs. It pushes actors to presume that most Nothing in status construction theory’s argu-
others outside the local contexts would also ments implies that the process should differ by
accept such a belief as valid. If others will gender. As we shall see, tests of the theory have
accept a pattern that links the social difference found no gender differences in the initial for-
to esteem and competence, then the actor has mation of status beliefs about social differences
good reason to form a status belief about that (Ridgeway et al. 1998; Ridgeway and Correll
difference too, if for no other reason than to 2006; Ridgeway and Erickson 2000). Decades
HOW EASILY DOES DIFFERENCE BECOME STATUS?—–47

of research on expectation states similarly shows Expectation states research on legitimacy


no systematic differences between men and shows that similar backlash reactions can occur
women in the likelihood that they will treat oth- when lower status actors attempt to assert them-
ers unequally on the basis of established status selves in relation to higher status actors (i.e., get
differences, with the occasional exception of the “above” their place) without a clearly estab-
status characteristic of gender itself (see Berger lished right to do so (Berger et al. 1998;
and Webster 2006; Foschi and Lapointe 2002; Ridgeway, Johnson, and Diekema 1994). The
Rashotte and Webster 2005). backlash reactions that women’s violations of
Status construction theory’s arguments do prescriptive communality elicit are in addition
imply, however, that acting on status beliefs of to those that status violations can invoke. They
uncertain validity is socially risky. There is evi- thus can occur even in situations where gender
dence from research on gender that the social status is not typically salient, such as in all
risk of asserting influence over another without female groups. Due to the distinctive cultural
the backing of a valid status advantage is greater requirements for communality to which women
for women than for men, other things being are held, women are particularly at risk of receiv-
equal (Eagly and Karau 2002; Rudman and ing negative reactions to their “unjustified”
Fairchild 2004; Rudman and Glick 2001). While assertions of superiority, even compared with
men and women do not appear to differ in their other lower status actors.
propensity to form status beliefs or act on well All else being equal, women may face a high-
established beliefs, they may differ in their ini- er risk of incurring negative sanctions or social
tial willingness to act on emerging beliefs costs than would men for acting on a newly
because of the different risk environments that formed status belief of uncertain social validi-
they face. ty. It therefore seems plausible that women
Cultural beliefs about gender (i.e., gender require more validation of their new status
stereotypes) carry not only descriptive infor- beliefs before acting on them. There are two
mation about how men and women are cultur- sides to this process. Women are likely to be cau-
tious about asserting a status advantage over
ally assumed to be, but prescriptive information
another on the basis of a new belief. To a less-
about how they should be (Fiske 1998). The
er extent, the same prescriptive gender expec-
principal prescriptive requirement for women is
tations are also likely to make women reluctant
that they appear communal in interpersonal
to grant another woman a status advantage over
relations (Cancian 1987; Eagly and Karau
them if that advantage is “unjustified” (i.e.,
2002). Women are expected to appear relative-
based on a belief of uncertain validity).
ly kind, helpful, concerned about others, and
People are likely to require multiple valid,
generally “nice.” The difficulty is that this
consistent experiences before forming new sta-
requirement to be communal is culturally per- tus beliefs that are sufficient to shape the way
ceived to conflict with the assertion of superi- they treat others, and women may require more
ority or dominance. As Rudman and Glick such experiences than men. But how many is
(2001) show, the assertion of superiority is per- “multiple”? Do people treat a social difference
ceived as “not being nice” and thus as a viola- as a status distinction after just a few experi-
tion of prescriptive gender expectations for ences, say two or three? Or do people require
women. As a result, unless a woman’s right to extensive confirmation before acting? What, if
do so is clearly legitimated in a particular situ- any, are the gender differences in this process?
ation, her assertion of influence over another can The answers affect the potential ease and speed
provoke a negative “backlash” reaction from with which status distinctions develop and
others of both sexes (Rudman and Glick 2001). spread in a population to become axes of social
A number of studies demonstrate these backlash inequality.
reactions to women’s assertions of superiority
(see Rudman and Fairchild 2004). Rudman and
EVIDENCE OF STATUS BELIEF
Fairchild (2004) show that actors generally rec-
FORMATION IN LOCAL CONTEXTS
ognize the sanctions they face for violating pre-
scriptive gender expectations and thus try to To address these questions empirically, it is use-
avoid them. ful to review what we already know about the
48—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

formation of status beliefs in encounters ly? Also, can we document gender differences
between people from different social categories. in this process? We conducted two experiments
Computer simulations confirm that a status to examine these questions, an initial test and
belief about a social difference could plausibly then an improved, more important second study.
diffuse widely in a population if local encoun-
ters create and spread status beliefs as status EXPERIMENT 1
construction theory argues (Ridgeway and
Balkwell 1997). Therefore, empirical investi- METHOD
gations have focused on whether local contexts OVERVIEW. The 2 ⫻ 2 design of the experiment
actually do induce their participants to form crossed the status of a subject’s own categori-
status beliefs as claimed. Experiments show cal group in the status beliefs to be induced
that men and women form similar, clearly (high or low) with the gender composition (all
defined status beliefs about a previously uneval- male or all female) of their encounters. We
uated categorical difference from only two con- excluded mixed-sex encounters from the design
sistent, repeated encounters with those of the to avoid confounding the status effects of gen-
other category (Ridgeway et al. 1998; Ridgeway der (which are salient in mixed-sex settings)
and Erickson 2000). Another experiment con- with the effects of newly formed status beliefs
firms that the formation of status beliefs in
(Wagner and Berger 1997). We randomly
these encounters turns on social validity, as the
assigned 98 undergraduates (46 males and 52
theory claims (Ridgeway and Correll 2006).
females) to conditions within sex, resulting in
This suggests that status beliefs not only form
23 males and 26 females each in the high and
as the theory predicts, but they do so rather
low status conditions. All subjects participated
quickly for both men and women.
in three dyadic encounters, each time with a dif-
Experiments support other important ele-
ferent same-sex partner, in which the pairs
ments of the theory, including the idea that bias-
worked on consecutive rounds of a cooperative
ing factors like material resources shape the
decision-making task. In each encounter, sub-
nature of the status beliefs that people form
jects’ partners differed from them on a newly
(Ridgeway et al. 1998). Experiments also con-
introduced social difference.
firm that people spread status beliefs by treat-
The first two rounds induced status beliefs
ing others according to the status belief, creating
about the new difference by replicating proce-
a diffusion process that disseminates status
dures from a key status construction experi-
beliefs in the population (Ridgeway and
ment (Ridgeway et al. 1998). This study
Erickson 2000).
demonstrated that encounters create status
The power of this diffusion process depends
beliefs favoring one categorical group over
on how quickly status beliefs become suffi-
another when a group possesses an advantage
ciently valid for people to act on them in a new
in material resources (in this case, pay) that is
encounter. The theory’s argument that people do
reflected in influence advantages in intercate-
eventually act on their new status beliefs in sub-
gory encounters (as pay differences typically
sequent encounters derives from expectation
are) (Stewart and Moore 1992). After two
states research that shows that people transfer
rounds following these procedures, subjects
modifications of existing status beliefs to new
completed a measure of their status beliefs about
situations (Markovsky et al. 1984). Existing
the new social difference. They then worked
status beliefs, however, already have general
on round 3 of the task with a new same-sex
validity in society (i.e., they are assumed to be
partner who also differed from them on the new
shared by most people), which facilitates their
distinction. In round 3, we examined whether
transfer to new situations. The transfer of a
subjects treated this partner according to their
newly formed status belief from one situation
new status beliefs by deferring to a partner
to the next is a more uncertain process.
believed to be high status and not deferring to
Our question now becomes, if people report
a partner believed to be low status.
clear status beliefs after only two consistent
encounters, are these beliefs already so strong
that people will spontaneously transfer them to PROCEDURES AND MANIPULATIONS. Following
the next encounter and treat others according- Ridgeway and colleagues (1998), subjects first
HOW EASILY DOES DIFFERENCE BECOME STATUS?—–49

completed a brief background information form ject was better paid (high in-group status con-
about education, prizes won, and employment ditions) and confidently and assertively when
history. To create a salient but unevaluated social the subject was lower paid (low in-group status
difference between subjects and their partners, conditions) (see Ridgeway et al. 1998). To track
subjects then completed a “personal response influence, confederate-partners announced a
style test” in which they chose between pairs of different initial choice than the subject on 8 of
paintings by Klee and Kandinsky, based on 10 trials in round 1, and 7 of 10 trials in round
which they preferred. The abstract, purely visu- 2. Subjects’ acceptance of influence is the per-
al nature of the test, borrowed from social iden- cent of these disagreement trials in which sub-
tity studies of “mere difference,” was designed jects changed their final responses to agree with
to minimize possible confounding associations their partners’ choices.
with established social or behavioral differences Clear influence hierarchies developed in both
(Tajfel and Turner 1986). We told subjects that rounds; subjects deferred to their partners in low
the test differentiates between two types of peo- status conditions (mean acceptance of influ-
ple, S2s and Q2s, whose distinct response styles ence, 80 percent) but resisted their partners’
are stable aspects of their selves. Supposedly, influence in high status conditions (mean influ-
there are roughly equal numbers of S2s and ence, 48 percent). Subjects clearly perceived
Q2s in the world. Next, we told subjects that these hierarchies. Differences between ratings
“based on the information the laboratory has” (on 9-point scales) of their own influence and
they had been assigned to a pay rate either $3 that of their partners favored them in high sta-
above (high in-group status conditions) or $3 tus conditions (mean difference = 1.99) and
below (low in-group status conditions) that of favored their partners in low status conditions
their round 1 partner. They then learned that they (mean difference = –2.67). Neither actual nor
were either an S2 or a Q2 and their partner was perceived influence differed by gender. Rounds
the opposite (a Q2 or an S2). Participants signed 1 and 2 created two encounters for subjects in
a pay sheet suggesting that other S2s and Q2s which the partners who differed from them on
similarly differed somewhat in pay. Whether the S2/Q2 distinction also consistently differed
subjects were S2s or Q2s was counterbalanced from them in pay and influence.
within conditions.
Seated in a separate room, subjects then
MEASURES OF STATUS BELIEFS. At the end of
began work via an audio link with their round
these two encounters, subjects responded to 7-
1 partner on a 10-trial, computerized, coopera-
point semantic differential items. The items
tive decision task, completing a brief question-
asked how “most people” and, separately, how
naire at the end. Next, subjects learned that
the subject personally view the typical S2 and
their round 2 partner also differed from them on
the typical Q2 in terms of status (respected–not
the S2/Q2 distinction and in pay. They began
respected, high status–low status, leader–fol-
work with this partner over the verbal link on
lower, powerful–powerless), competence (com-
an additional 10 trials of the task.
petent–incompetent, knowledgeable–not
The “meaning insight” task required subject-
knowledgeable, capable–incapable), and social
partner teams to correctly associate English
considerateness (considerate–inconsiderate,
words with one of three words supposedly from
cooperative–uncooperative, likeable–unlike-
a reconstructed language. On each trial, subject
able, pleasant–unpleasant). All these scales were
and partner made an initial choice, verbally dis-
reliable, as indicated by Cronbach’s alphas rang-
cussed this choice, and then privately entered
ing from .77 to .95 (average = .87). The “most
their final choice. The theory argues that influ-
people” scales for status and competence con-
ence differences will induce status beliefs in
stitute the primary measure of status beliefs
encounters. Pay differences typically lead to
about the S2/Q2 distinction because they cap-
influence differences by creating differences in
ture the distinctive reputational nature of such
confidence and assertiveness (Stewart and
beliefs.
Moore 1992). To ensure that this happened so
that clear influence hierarchies arose, the round
1 and round 2 partners were confederates who ACTING ON STATUS BELIEFS. We began round
acted hesitantly and deferentially when the sub- 3 by telling subjects that they would now work
50—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

on the task with a third same-sex partner, also (Q2s) were compared to Q2s (S2s) at most tasks
from the opposite S2/Q2 group. We gave no in general. This provides a measure of whether
information about this partner’s pay level. This subjects continued to report status beliefs about
time, subject and partner worked via computer the S2/Q2 distinction after round 3.
without an audio link and exchanged initial
choices without discussion. The round 3 part-
RESULTS
ner was actually a programmed other who pre-
sented a constant stimulus to which subjects FORMING STATUS BELIEFS. The first task is to
could potentially react differently based on their ascertain whether male and female subjects
newly formed status beliefs. The programmed formed clear status beliefs about the S2/Q2 dis-
partner’s initial choices disagreed with those tinction after round 2. The top half of Table 1
of the subject on 9 of the 12 task trials. The
displays the mean ratings (on 7-point scales) that
extent to which subjects deferred to their round
subjects in each condition thought “most peo-
3 partners by changing their answers to agree
with the partner provides our primary measure ple” would assign to their in-group and to the
of whether subjects transferred their new status other group on scales evaluating status, com-
beliefs to these partners and treated them petence, and considerateness. We analyzed these
unequally on the basis of those beliefs. A short rating measures with mixed model ANOVAs
post-round questionnaire, using a 7-point scale, in which status condition (i.e., in-group
asked subjects how capable they thought S2s pay/influence) and sex were between subject

Table 1. Experiment 1: Mean Perceived Evaluations of In-Group and Other Group (with Standard
Deviations)

Condition
Low In-Group Pay/Influence High In-Group Pay/Influence
Female Male Female Male
Mean of “Most People’s” Evaluations
—Status
———In-Group 3.96 (1.05) 4.37 (.89) 5.58 (.96) 5.62 (.77)
———Other Group 5.52 (1.01) 5.20 (.95) 3.84 (.91) 3.62 (.80)
———Difference –1.56 (1.49) –.83 (1.45) 1.74 (1.49) 2.00 (1.36)
—Competence
———In-Group 4.68 (1.20) 4.99 (.93) 5.67 (.85) 5.61 (.65)
———Other Group 5.58 (.87) 5.28 (.95) 4.36 (1.07) 4.32 (.87)
———Difference –.90 (1.44) –.29 (1.42) 1.31 (1.55) 1.29 (1.23)
—Considerate
———In-Group 5.63 (1.22) 5.48 (.96) 5.09 (.93) 4.22 (1.20)
———Other Group 3.92 (1.10) 4.04 (1.40) 6.21 (.73) 5.80 (.63)
———Difference 1.70 (1.40) 1.43 (1.76) –1.13 (1.06) –1.59 (1.52)

Mean of “Personal” Evaluations


—Status
———In-Group 3.95 (1.18) 4.07 (1.14) 5.35 (1.02) 5.33 (.92)
———Other Group 5.31 (.86) 4.95 (.92) 3.94 (.91) 3.71 (1.04)
———Difference –1.36 (1.56) –.88 (1.57) 1.40 (1.64) 1.62 (1.73)
—Competence
———In-Group 5.32 (1.09) 5.33 (1.13) 4.77 (1.24) 5.46 (.85)
———Other Group 5.35 (.95) 4.77 (1.15) 4.47 (1.00) 4.46 (.96)
———Difference –.03 (1.72) .57 (1.63) .29 (1.66) 1.00 (1.28)
—Considerate
———In-Group 5.84 (1.34) 5.78 (1.00) 5.63 (1.02) 5.07 (1.06)
———Other Group 3.59 (1.37) 3.89 (1.51) 6.41 (.71) 5.98 (.70)
———Difference 2.25 (1.92) 1.89 (1.99) –.79 (.90) –.91 (1.22)
HOW EASILY DOES DIFFERENCE BECOME STATUS?—–51

factors and reported ratings of in-group versus conditions agreed that their in-group was high-
other group were a within subject factor. er status than the other group, just as those in
The top left quadrant of Table 1 shows the sta- low pay/influence conditions conceded that their
tus and competence ratings reported by sub- in-group was lower status than the other group
jects in conditions in which their in-group was (F = 64.01, p < .001). At a personal level, how-
lower paid and less influential (low in-group sta- ever, subjects resisted the idea that there were
tus conditions). Both males and females in these competence differences between the S2/Q2
conditions reported that most people would rate groups (F = 1.38, n.s.).
the typical member of their own S2/Q2 group Also as in previous studies, the ratings for the
as lower status and less competent than the typ- in-group and other group of social consider-
ical member of the other group. Males and ateness, for both “most people” and the subject
females who were consistently more influential personally (Table 1, rows 7 to 9 and 16 to 18),
and better paid than members of the other group show a compensatory pattern. Members of
(high in-group status conditions) thought most groups perceived as higher status (i.e., high
would rate their in-group as higher status and pay/influence groups) were seen as less social-
more competent than the other group (top right ly considerate than members of groups rated as
quadrant of Table 1). lower status (for “most people’s” ratings, F =
The easiest way to grasp this pattern is to look 99.72, p < .001; for personal ratings, F = 83.74,
at the mean differences between ratings of the p < .001). Research suggests that this compen-
in-group and the other group in terms of status satory pattern is typical of cultural beliefs about
(Table 1, row 3) and competence (Table 1, row status differences between social groups in
6). For males and females in low in-group North America (Conway, Pizzamiglio, and
pay/influence conditions, these difference scores Mount 1996).
are negative, indicating that the in-group was As expected, after two consistent encoun-
rated lower than the out-group on both scales. ters, subjects did form status beliefs about the
For males and females in high in-group S2/Q2 distinction that favored the group whose
pay/influence conditions, the difference scores members had a pay and influence advantage.
are positive, showing that the in-group was rated Subjects formed these beliefs even when they
more highly than the other group. These ratings cast their own S2/Q2 group as lower status.
of status and competence show the classic struc- Importantly, there were no gender differences in
ture of status beliefs in which individuals in the formation of status beliefs.
both the status advantaged group and the status
disadvantaged group agree that the advantaged ACTING ON STATUS BELIEFS. The question,
are higher status. In the ANOVAs, this pattern then, is did subjects transfer these new status
of results is reflected in powerful interaction beliefs to the round 3 partner and treat that part-
effects between in-group pay/influence condi- ner unequally on the basis of them? As Figure
tion and the perceived rating of the in-group ver- 1 shows, male subjects clearly did but female
sus the other group. For “most people’s” ratings subjects did not. Males in in-group low status
of both status and competence, this interaction conditions deferred to their partner’s influence
is highly significant (for status, F = 108.57, p in 54 percent of the round 3 disagreement tri-
< .001; for competence, F = 43.33, p < .001) and als. Those in the in-group high status conditions
there are no differences by gender in these rat- accepted their partner’s influence in only 40
ings (p > .30 for all tests). percent of the trials, a significant difference in
While ratings of “most people’s” views best an ANOVA (simple main effect of in-group sta-
reflect the reputational nature of status beliefs, tus for males, F = 6.27, p = .014). Females, in
it is also interesting to examine subjects’ per- contrast, accepted their round 3 partner’s influ-
sonal ratings of S2s and Q2s. The bottom half ence at an intermediate rate (43 and 46 per-
of Table 1 presents these results, shown as mean cent) that shows no effects (F < 1, n.s.) of the
ratings of a subject’s in-group and the other clear status beliefs they reported at the end of
group. As in previous studies, personal ratings round 2.
were very similar to estimates of most people’s If females failed to act on their status beliefs
views (Ridgeway et al. 1998; Ridgeway and in round 3, it was not because they gave up
Correll 2006). Subjects in high pay/influence those beliefs. At the end of round 3, subjects in
52—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Figure 1. Experiment 1: Round 3 Acceptance of Influence by In-Group Status and Gender

low status conditions still rated their in-group males treating their round 3 partners more dif-
as relatively less capable at most tasks than the ferentially based on status condition than do
other group (mean = 3.85), compared with how other groups, we added a 3-way interaction
those in high status conditions rated their in- effect (gender, status, and race) to the regression
group (mean = 4.77, F = 20.59, p < .001). There model, but this, too, is not significant (p > .20).
were no gender differences in this tendency (for These analyses are limited by the fact that our
both main and interaction effects p > .20). group of non-White subjects, while reasonably
Were women perhaps more reluctant than large (39 non-Whites, 50 Whites, and nine miss-
men to treat others according to new status ing values on race), was a heterogeneous com-
beliefs because women themselves are a low sta- bination of Asians, Latinos, and African
tus group? Yet, an examination of another low Americans who vary in racial status. We thus
status group, racial minorities, shows that minor- cannot rule out the possibility of an effect of
ity subjects do not differ substantially from belonging to a low status group, in addition to
Whites in their tendency to treat their round 3 the distinctive gender effect. Only gender, how-
partner differentially.1 In a regression of round ever, significantly predicts reluctance to act on
3 acceptance of influence with gender, status new status beliefs, perhaps because cultural
condition, and their interaction in the model, the expectations for communality are more strong-
main effect of race (White versus Other) is not ly associated with women than with other low
significant (p > .20). Because an inspection of status groups.
the data showed a slight trend toward White
SUMMARY

1
The results of the first experiment are clear.
Because teams were same sex and subjects did
After two consistent encounters that presented
not know the race of their partners, it is unlikely that
participants with apparently valid associations
the status implications of either gender or race were
immediately salient for subjects. Instead, this analy- between a social difference and influence and
sis and a similar analysis for Experiment 2 ask esteem, male subjects formed status beliefs that
whether the cumulative experience of the status dis- they were willing to transfer to their next
advantages of being female or non-White in the encounter with a person from the other catego-
United States creates a similar, learned hesitance to ry and to act on them by treating that person
act on new status beliefs. unequally. Women also formed status beliefs
HOW EASILY DOES DIFFERENCE BECOME STATUS?—–53

that were indistinguishable from men’s, but they according to the status beliefs. The experiment’s
did not act on these beliefs in their next 2 ⫻ 2 design again crossed the status position
encounter. The greater risk for women of treat- of the participants’ own categorical in-group in
ing another unequally on the basis of uncertain the induced beliefs (high or low) with the gen-
beliefs may have made them more cautious der composition of the encounters (all male or
about acting on their new beliefs without addi- all female). We again excluded mixed-sex
tional validation (and more resistant to other encounters to avoid the confounding effects of
women who did seem to act on them). gender status. There were 25 to 26 participants
Although Experiment 1 offers a useful first in each of the four conditions for a total of 103
test of our arguments, it has distinctive limita- subjects (52 males, 51 females).
tions. The structure of the experiment as three We added a set of contrast conditions to this
consecutive encounters within a single session main experiment. An additional 33 subjects
raises two concerns. The length of this three- worked on only the Phase 2 task with a partner
round session caused considerable fatigue described as either higher (eight males and nine
among subjects, which may have introduced females) or lower (eight males and eight
noise into the results and created an unreliable females) than them in an established status dis-
appearance of gender differences in behavior. tinction (educational attainment). Expectation
Also, because round 3 took place in the same states researchers consistently find that both
setting that created the status beliefs, we cannot men and women treat educational attainment as
be certain that even male subjects would have a socially valid status distinction by deferring
taken their new status beliefs with them after to those with more education and resisting def-
they left the setting. erence to those with less education (see Berger
Finally, the first experiment used procedures and Webster 2006).
to induce status beliefs that associate pay and Adding these contrast conditions serves two
influence differences with the difference dis-
purposes. First, by measuring treatment on the
tinction. Research confirms status construction
basis of both newly formed status beliefs and
theory’s argument, however, that the association
established status on the same task with the
between the difference distinction and appar-
same subject population, we can be certain that
ently valid influence differences alone is suffi-
any gender differences detected are limited to
cient to produce status beliefs (Ridgeway and
the tendency to act on newly formed status
Erickson 2000). But are beliefs formed without
beliefs, as theorized, rather than associated with
the backing of pay or resource differences also
sufficiently certain after just two encounters so status behavior more broadly. Second, these
that people will transfer them to a new encounter conditions offer a benchmark measurement of
and act on them? The answer to this question the effects of an established status difference on
affects the ease and speed with which status the Phase 2 task against which we can gauge the
beliefs can spread in a population to create axes extent to which newly formed status beliefs
of inequality. We designed a second experiment affect subjects’ unequal treatment of their part-
to address these concerns and otherwise improve ners.
on the first experiment.
MAIN EXPERIMENT PROCEDURES AND MEAS-
EXPERIMENT 2 URES. Phase 1 of the main experiment began
with the creation of a salient categorical dis-
METHOD tinction among participants using the S2/Q2
OVERVIEW. The principal part of Experiment 2 “personal response style” test described earli-
had two phases. The first phase induced status er. Subjects then participated in two same-sex,
beliefs about the S2/Q2 distinction by drawing task-oriented encounters, first as a member of
on procedures from Ridgeway and Erickson a two-person decision-making team and then as
(2000) that associate the distinction with influ- a member of a three-person team. In the first
ence differences without supporting pay dif- team, the partner differed from the subject on
ferences. The second phase took place one to the S2/Q2 distinction. In the second team, one
three days later and examined whether partici- teammate was of the same S2/Q2 category as
pants would spontaneously treat a new partner the subject; the other teammate was from the
54—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

opposite category. Both teams were thus inter- gorically different other. In the second team,
category encounters. they witnessed a similar hierarchy arise among
Teams consisted of the subject, seated in a categorically different others. At the end of
separate room, and teammates linked by audio Phase 1, subjects completed the measures of sta-
and computer hook-ups. In the first team, sub- tus beliefs described in Experiment 1. These
ject and partner worked together verbally on status belief scales again proved reliable
10 trials of the “meaning insight” task used in (Cronbach’s alphas .71 to .94, averaging .86).
Experiment 1. In in-group low conditions, the Subjects returned to the lab one to three days
partner, who was a confederate, acted confi- later for Phase 2. To further validate the status
dently and nondeferentially toward the subject. beliefs that subjects formed in Phase 1, the
In in-group high conditions, the confederate experimenter greeted them by saying, “You’re
partner acted hesitantly and deferentially. As an S2 (Q2) right? Well, that makes sense. I lis-
expected, this again caused clear influence hier- tened to the tapes of your Phase 1 groups.”
archies to develop between subjects and their Subjects then worked via computer with a new
partners that favored the subjects in in-group same-sex partner from the other category (actu-
high conditions (mean partner’s influence: males ally a programmed other) on a new version of
= 33 percent, females = 37 percent) and favored the “meaning insight” task. This completely
the partners in in-group low conditions (mean computerized version was structured like the
influence: males = 90 percent, females = 79 standardized tasks that expectation states
percent). These influence hierarchies were less researchers use to document the effects of estab-
differentiated for females than for males (F = lished status distinctions such as race, gender,
4.45, p < .05), but there were no gender differ- education, and occupation (Berger et al. 1977;
ences in the perceived influence of self and Troyer 2002). It consisted of 25 trials in which
partner in these teams (F < 1, n.s.). Perceived the subject makes an initial choice of two task
influence differences favored the self in high options, learns the partner’s initial choice, and
conditions (mean difference = .86) and favored then makes a final choice. On 20 of the trials,
the partner in low conditions (mean difference the partner’s initial choice disagrees with the
= –3.31). subject’s choice. Subjects’ status-based treat-
In the second team, subjects worked on anoth- ment of their Phase 2 partners was indicated by
er 10 trials of the task with two different team- the proportion of these disagreement trials in
mates. In this team, the teammates were which subjects changed their responses to agree
assigned to discuss task choices on behalf of the with their partners.2 If subjects spontaneously
group while the subject listened as the evalua- treat their new partners according to the status
tor (see Ridgeway and Erickson 2000). The beliefs created in Phase 1, then this acceptance
teammates were actually taped interactions in of influence rate should be significantly high-
which the teammate who was categorically dif- er for those in in-group low conditions than
ferent from the subject asserted influence over those in in-group high conditions. At the end of
the teammate of the subject’s own category on Phase 2, subjects completed the status belief
100 percent of the seven disagreement trials in measures once again.
the in-group low conditions. In the in-group
high conditions, the categorically different team- CONTRAST CONDTIONS. Subjects in the con-
mate deferred (i.e., was influential on only 33 trast conditions completed only the Phase 2
percent of the disagreement trials) to the team- task. In low educational status conditions, the
mate from the subject’s in-group. Influence
rates were identical on male and female tapes.
The combination of two types of teams in 2 Rather than use the conventional expectation
Phase 1 was designed to increase the validity of
states measure of resistance to influence, p(s), we
the association that subjects experienced report our results in Experiment 2 as acceptance of
between the categorical difference and influence influence, which is 1–p(s), to maintain comparabil-
and apparent competence, while keeping session ity with the acceptance of influence measure in
length at a feasible two encounters. In the first Experiment 1. The Experiment 1 measure cannot be
team, subjects experienced an influence hier- converted to p(s) without error because there were
archy arising between themselves and a cate- three rather than two choice options on each trial.
HOW EASILY DOES DIFFERENCE BECOME STATUS?—–55

Table 2. Experiment 2: Mean Perceived Evaluations of In-Group and Other Group (with Standard
Deviations); Phase 1

Condition
Low In-Group Influence High In-Group Influence
Female Male Female Male
Mean of “Most People’s” Evaluations
—Status
———In-Group 3.73 (1.05) 3.75 (.83) 5.65 (.71) 5.45 (1.05)
———Other Group 5.25 (.80) 5.64 (.68) 3.63 (1.15) 3.57 (.88)
———Difference –1.52 (1.55) –1.89 (.96) 2.02 (1.59) 1.88 (1.73)
—Competence
———In-Group 4.59 (.91) 4.87 (.98) 5.64 (.76) 5.41 (.98)
———Other Group 5.31 (.82) 5.54 (.74) 4.24 (1.12) 4.28 (.86)
———Difference –.72 (1.11) –.67 (1.19) 1.40 (1.41) 1.13 (1.60)
—Considerate
———In-Group 5.75 (.92) 5.76 (.69) 4.63 (1.02) 4.07 (1.14)
———Other Group 3.43 (1.11) 3.57 (1.09) 5.96 (.68) 5.71 (.92)
———Difference 2.32 (1.67) 2.19 (1.53) –1.27 (1.62) –1.13 (1.52)

Mean of “Personal” Evaluations


—Status
———In-Group 3.63 (1.24) 4.18 (1.25) 5.24 (.89) 5.39 (.87)
———Other Group 5.13 (.93) 5.34 (.71) 3.76 (1.13) 3.53 (1.08)
———Difference –1.51 (1.80) –1.15 (1.47) 1.48 (1.55) 1.87 (1.79)
—Competence
———In-Group 4.99 (.95) 5.62 (.96) 5.35 (.99) 5.23 (.87)
———Other Group 4.73 (1.03) 4.99 (1.13) 4.64 (1.13) 4.59 (1.15)
———Difference .26 (1.32) .63 (1.43) .71 (1.35) .64 (1.54)
—Considerate
———In-Group 5.88 (1.21) 6.03 (.77) 4.69 (1.18) 4.75 (1.21)
———Other Group 3.12 (1.16) 3.21 (1.05) 6.06 (.68) 5.64 (1.03)
———Difference 2.77 (2.04) 2.82 (1.44) –1.37 (1.35) –.89 (1.70)

same-sex partner was presented to these under- thought most people would rate their own group
graduate subjects as a PhD candidate at their as higher status and more competent, but not as
university. In high educational status condi- considerate as the other group (top right quad-
tions, the partner was described as a freshman rant of Table 2). More importantly, subjects in
at the community junior college. low in-group influence conditions (top left quad-
rant of Table 2) thought that most people would
RESULTS evaluate their group as significantly lower sta-
tus and less competent than the other group,
FORMING STATUS BELIEFS. Both male and female albeit nicer. Mixed model ANOVAs reflect these
subjects in the main experiment formed clear results in substantial interaction effects between
status beliefs by the end of Phase 1. The top half in-group influence and the ratings of in-group
of Table 2 shows the results of our primary indi- versus other group (for status, F = 156.42, p <
cators of status beliefs: the comparisons between .001; for competence, F = 54.95, p < .001; for
how subjects thought “most people” would rate consideration, F = 143.98, p < .001). There are
their in-group and the other group in terms of no significant gender effects in the ANOVAs of
status and competence. As the difference scores status and competence ratings (Fs < 1.0, n.s.),
show, subjects formed clearly differentiated sta- indicating that men and women formed similar
tus beliefs favoring the group that was consis- status beliefs at the end of Phase 1.
tently high influence in their encounters. As in the first experiment, subjects’ person-
Subjects in high in-group influence conditions al evaluations of the differences between their
56—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Figure 2. Experiment 2: Phase 2 Acceptance of Influence by In-Group Status and Gender

own and the other group corresponded fairly nificant in an ANOVA (simple main effect of in-
closely to their estimates of “most people’s” group status for males, F = 10.09, p = .002),
views (bottom half of Table 2). Again, the dif- shows that men did treat the categorical differ-
ference is that in personal evaluations, subjects ence between themselves and their partners as
in low in-group influence conditions resisted the a status distinction, even though they acquired
idea that their in-group was less competent than these status beliefs through just two encounters
the other group even though they conceded that with categorically different others.
their in-group was significantly lower status. As Figure 2 also reveals, women in low in-
Reflecting these patterns, ANOVAs show inter- group status conditions treated a new partner
action effects between in-group influence and just as deferentially as did men, accepting her
in-group/other group ratings for status (F = influence 50 percent of the time. Unlike men,
84.28, p < .001) and consideration (F = 144.42, however, women in the high in-group status
p < .001) but not competence (F < 1, n.s.). condition did not act on their status advantage
These patterns of personal evaluations do not with a new partner. Instead, these women also
vary by gender (Fs < 1, n.s.). accepted their new partner’s influence 50 per-
cent of the time.4
ACTING ON STATUS BELIEFS. Both men and
women reported clear status beliefs at the end ure in Experiment 1, only relative differences between
of Phase 1, but did they act on these beliefs conditions, not absolute scores, can be compared
days later when encountering a new partner in between experiments.
Phase 2? As Figure 2 shows, once again, men 4 Our design did not include equal status condi-

clearly did but women mostly did not. Men in tions, in which subjects simply completed the Phase
low in-group status conditions accepted their 2 task with a partner who differed from them on the
Phase 2 partner’s influence 49 percent of the S2/Q2 distinction, because the introduction of the
time, but in-group high status men deferred to social difference alone would create in-group
their new partner’s choices only 34 percent of favoritism effects that would confound the baseline
the time.3 This difference, which is clearly sig- measurement of equal status influence (Brewer and
Brown 1998). Status studies that use influence meas-
ures similar to ours show that men’s equal status
influence rates are not systematically different than
3 Because the metric of the Phase 2 acceptance of women’s and are below those that men show in high
influence measure differs from the equivalent meas- status situations (see Wagner and Berger 1997). This
HOW EASILY DOES DIFFERENCE BECOME STATUS?—–57

The full meaning of these results for men disagreeing partner in Phase 2, both men and
and women in the main conditions becomes women retained basic status beliefs about the
clearer when we compare them with our find- S2/Q2 distinction (as shown by the difference
ings for similar subjects in the contrast condi- scores for “most people’s” evaluations of status,
tions. Both men and women in our contrast competence, and consideration [see the top half
conditions treated their partners unequally on the of Table 3]). This characteristic pattern of results
basis of educational status. Those who believed remains significant in mixed model ANOVAs
they had lower educational status than their (interaction effects between status condition
partner deferred to their partner’s choices 48 and ratings of in-group/other group, for status
percent of the time (SD .17). Those who thought F = 55.07, p < .001; for competence, F = 19.36,
they had higher educational status accepted p < .001; for consideration F = 56.25, p < .001).
their partner’s influence 35 percent of the time The results do not vary by gender (all Fs < 1,
(SD .18), a significant difference in an ANOVA n.s.). Even personal beliefs (bottom half of
(F = 4.32, p < .05). Importantly, there are no Table 3), although weaker, kept much of the
gender differences in the unequal treatment of shape of status beliefs at the end of Phase 2
partners on the basis of educational status (main (interaction effects between status condition
effect of gender, F = 2.50, p = .13; gender by and ratings of in-group/other group, for status
status interaction, F < 1, n.s.). F = 35.32, p < .001; for competence, F < 1, n.s.;
Strikingly, the extent to which men treated for consideration F = 55.06, p < .001). They do
their partners unequally on the basis of the not show systematic gender differences (3-way
S2/Q2 distinction in the main conditions (49 interaction of status condition, ratings of in-
versus 34 percent deference) is almost identical group/other group, and gender, for status, F =
to the inequality of treatment (48 versus 35 per- 3.44, p = .10; for competence and consideration,
cent deference) that all subjects in the contrast Fs < 1.09, n. s.).
conditions displayed toward partners based on Recall that in the first team of Phase 1, the
an established status distinction, education. influence hierarchies that women in the main
These findings suggest that for men, at least, the conditions experienced were less differentiated
power of encounters to transform social differ- than those of men. Did this have an effect on
ences into status distinctions is formidable. their willingness to treat their partners unequal-
For women, the contrast conditions confirm ly in Phase 2? The differences between men’s
that women from the same population as those and women’s hierarchies in Phase 1, however,
in the main conditions were no more reluctant occurred primarily in low in-group status con-
than men to treat others unequally on the basis ditions. Men and women in these conditions
of an established status distinction. Rather, did not differ in their treatment of their Phase
women’s hesitance to assert a status advantage 2 partners. Along with the fact that there are no
was limited to the context of newly formed sta- gender differences in perceived influence hier-
tus beliefs. Women’s reluctance to act on their archies in Phase 1, this suggests that this is not
new status advantage in the main conditions the best explanation for the gender differences
did not occur because these women were less in behavior that we observed in Phase 2.
likely than similar men to accept that people like Because racial minorities did not differ from
them were higher status than people like their Whites in their tendency to act on their status
partners. Not only were women’s status beliefs beliefs in Phase 2, it also seems unlikely that
the same as men’s at the end of Phase 1, they women’s reluctance to act on their status advan-
were also no different from men’s when meas- tage was primarily a result of the fact that
ured again at the end of Phase 2 (see Table 3). women are themselves a lower status group. A
Even after their encounters with the constantly regression of Phase 2 acceptance of influence
with gender, status condition, and the interac-
tion of the two in the model shows no main
suggests that the gender difference we observe is effect of race (p > .30). Unlike the first exper-
not because men are generally more resistant to influ- iment, inspection revealed no race by sex trends
ence than women, except in low status situations, but in the data, as we confirmed by adding a 3-way
instead is more likely due to women’s reluctance to interaction term to the model (gender, status, and
assert a status advantage of uncertain validity. race) that proved nonsignificant ( p > .25).
58—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Table 3. Experiment 2: Mean Perceived Evaluations of In-Group and Other Group (with Standard
Deviations); Phase 2

Condition
Low In-Group Status High In-Group Status
Female Male Female Male
Mean of “Most People’s” Evaluations
—Status
———In-Group 4.04 (.92) 4.08 (.86) 5.07 (.90) 5.09 (1.02)
———Other Group 5.13 (.87) 5.34 (.88) 3.92 (1.09) 3.81 (1.06)
———Difference –1.10 (1.50) –1.26 (1.27) 1.15 (1.76) 1.28 (1.94)
—Competence
———In-Group 4.67 (1.01) 4.83 (1.02) 5.13 (.70) 5.13 (.76)
———Other Group 5.04 (.80) 5.13 (.81) 4.40 (1.04) 4.46 (.83)
———Difference –.37 (1.16) –.29 (1.19) .73 (1.24) .67 (1.19)
—Considerate
———In-Group 5.11 (1.11) 5.34 (.93) 4.30 (1.09) 4.04 (.98)
———Other Group 3.83 (1.12) 4.05 (1.05) 5.57 (.85) 5.17 (1.01)
———Difference 1.28 (1.99) 1.29 (1.57) –1.27 (1.62) –1.13 (1.52)

Mean of “Personal” Evaluations


—Status
———In-Group 4.38 (.75) 4.44 (1.03) 4.71 (.80) 5.20 (1.04)
———Other Group 4.88 (.66) 5.13 (.92) 4.02 (.94) 3.62 (5.13)
———Difference –1.51 (1.80) –1.15 (1.47) 1.48 (1.55) 1.87 (1.79)
—Competence
———In-Group 4.68 (.93) 5.41 (1.06) 4.81 (1.01) 5.04 (1.14)
———Other Group 5.00 (.92) 4.62 (1.04) 4.80 (.91) 4.74 (1.14)
———Difference –.32 (1.27) .67 (1.53) .71 (1.35) .64 (1.54)
—Considerate
———In-Group 5.15 (1.01) 5.55 (1.20) 4.51 (1.08) 4.61 (.93)
———Other Group 3.76 (1.21) 3.79 (1.12) 5.20 (.77) 5.43 (.89)
———Difference 1.39 (1.84) 1.76 (1.90) –.69 (1.21) –.83 (1.31)

Again, these findings are limited by the fact that more stringent, time displaced test of subjects’
the non-White group, while substantial (44 ver- willingness to spontaneously take their beliefs
sus 59 Whites), was racially diverse. to a new context and act on them there. We
Women’s unwillingness to act on their new combined this more stringent test of action on
status beliefs in Phase 2, like in Experiment 1, beliefs with an improved measure of whether
appears to have something to do with gender subjects act on status beliefs in new situations.
itself. Furthermore, women’s hesitance to act on To these we also added countervailing proce-
their new status beliefs seems to pertain to gen- dures to modestly increase the validity of the
der in the context of newly forming status beliefs beliefs and, therefore, the likelihood that they
rather than to gender differences in status behav- would be acted on. Despite these differences, we
ior more generally. once again find that men and women formed
similar beliefs after two consistent encounters,
SUMMARY but men treated others unequally on the basis of
Despite procedural differences, the results of these beliefs in their next encounters, while
Experiment 2 are strikingly similar to those of women mostly did not. Yet we also find that sim-
Experiment 1. This increases our confidence ilar men and women did not differ in their ten-
about the reliability of the findings. Experiment dency to treat others unequally on the basis of
2 used different, less restrictive procedures for an established status distinction (i.e., educa-
inducing status beliefs in Phase 1 and posed a tion).
HOW EASILY DOES DIFFERENCE BECOME STATUS?—–59

These findings suggest that men form status ordered worlds of work and community.
beliefs that they are willing to act on rather Furthermore, because participants were will-
quickly, but women appear to require more con- ing to treat others unequally on the basis of
firming experiences that validate those beliefs beliefs formed from the relatively minimal and
before acting on them. Because women treat insignificant encounters of an experiment, it
others according to established status distinc- seems likely that the socially meaningful
tions just as men do, it seems likely that the encounters that make up people’s actual lives are
women, too, would eventually act on these new at least as powerful in their capacity to induce
status beliefs. The processes by which local status beliefs that quickly become a basis for
encounters with different others transform social unequal treatment. These results suggest that, for
differences into status distinctions seem to move men at least, encounters between socially dif-
more slowly for women than for men. ferent actors have a powerful capacity to trans-
form social differences into systematic axes of
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS inequality, as status construction theory sug-
gests.
Our findings make three distinct contributions. What does it mean that we were able to doc-
Primarily, they provide the first demonstration ument clear gender differences at the key junc-
of just how easily people, or at least men, trans- ture at which social differences become a basis
form a social difference into a status distinction. for unequal treatment? Although women in both
In addition, they document gender differences studies formed new status beliefs as readily as
in the process that have implications not only for men, they were not willing to act on these beliefs
gender dynamics but for the dynamics by which in their next encounters. Because college life is
status distinctions emerge. Finally, they docu- less gender differentiated than most contexts,
ment a key link in status construction theory’s this gender difference is unlikely to be an arti-
account of how encounters between people from fact of the undergraduate population. The first
different social categories can lead to widely problem is to explain the gender difference we
held status beliefs about social differences. found. The second is to consider its implications
Repeated encounters between people from for the dynamic processes by which widely
different social categories, when they present shared status beliefs emerge.
valid, consistent associations between a cate- Status construction theory argues that initial
gorical difference and influence and compe- status beliefs require validation before affecting
tence, have a powerful capacity to induce status behavior because action on beliefs of uncertain
beliefs with implications for unequal treatment. validity can be socially costly (Ridgeway 2006).
After only two such encounters, men were will- Gender research further shows that the risk of
ing to treat a new partner unequally based on a incurring such costs, all else being equal, is
difference distinction that they had just learned greater for women than for men (Rudman and
about. In the second study, the extent of differ- Fairchild 2004). This, we argue, makes women
ential treatment they displayed toward this new more cautious about acting on newly forming
partner was comparable to that which both men status beliefs.
and women showed toward a partner who dif- We do not, however, have direct evidence to
fered from them in an established status dis- confirm that this differential risk environment
tinction, education. Essentially, the men had produced the gender differences we observed.
transformed the new difference into a full- Nevertheless, we do have evidence that under-
fledged status characteristic. cuts some alternative explanations. Because the
What are the implications of the fact that differences we observed were between men and
these results are based on undergraduate par- women in same-sex rather than mixed-sex
ticipants? Because of the powerful effects of the encounters, the status of the gender distinction
student peer role, college is a time of unusual- itself is unlikely to have been salient for the
ly egalitarian relations among fellow students. participants. It is therefore unlikely to account
It therefore seems improbable that student sub- for the differences we observed (see Ridgeway
jects would be more likely to form status dis- and Smith-Lovin 1999). Also, other effects of
tinctions, and treat other students accordingly, belonging to a lower status group in society are
than would participants from more status- unlikely to fully account for women’s behavior.
60—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Members of another low status group, racial Taken together, these results suggest that peo-
minorities, did not significantly differ from oth- ple in everyday encounters readily transform the
ers in their tendency to act on new status beliefs salient social differences that divide them into
(but our ability to test for race effects had some status distinctions and act on them in ways that
limits). Finally, in our second study, women spread these distinctions throughout a popula-
from the same subject population as those who tion. Status distinctions are thus likely to emerge
were reluctant to act on new status beliefs were readily in populations and act as powerful forces
no less likely than men to treat others unequal- for social inequality. As people enter newly
ly based on a fully established status distinction. forming organizations, for instance, we should
Putting this evidence together suggests that not be surprised when they quickly form local
the cause of the differences we observed pertains status distinctions that continue to order organi-
specifically to newly forming, not yet socially- zational life for years to come (Troyer 2003).
established status beliefs and to processes more Furthermore, group differences that are highly
strongly associated with gender than status. salient in an interdependent population are
This contextual evidence substantially increas- unlikely to remain unordered differences for
es the plausibility of our argument that women long. Structural conditions will typically deter-
are more cautious than men about asserting sta- mine which group becomes higher status. If, for
tus over another on the basis of new beliefs example, changing political circumstances or
because of the risks of violating prescriptive increasing immigration raise the salience of an
gender expectations that they treat others “nice- ethnic distinction in a population (e.g., the Arab,
ly” (i.e., communally). To a lesser extent, these non-Arab distinction in the recent United States)
same prescriptive expectations for communal- when there is already a material inequality
ity make women reluctant to defer to other between the groups, that distinction will likely
women (who should also be “nice”) on the basis be transformed into a systematic status charac-
teristic that disadvantages even well-off mem-
of uncertain status beliefs. Interestingly, in the
bers of the newly low status group.
second experiment, which had stronger proce-
Status distinctions have dynamic histories
dures for creating valid beliefs, women did defer
and can fade with changing structural condi-
similarly to men in low status conditions but still
tions. Also, the emergence of new status beliefs
did not assert status in high status conditions.
may sometimes be disrupted so that a difference
Having documented these gender differences in
does not acquire broad significance as a status
the initial emergence of status beliefs, an impor-
distinction. Evidence indicates, for instance,
tant task for future research is to directly test the
that acts of resistance that challenge the link
differential risk environment hypothesis. between a social difference and status in initial
What are the implications of women’s com- local encounters can interrupt the formation of
parative reluctance to act on newly acquired status beliefs by undermining their apparent
status beliefs? If status beliefs are partly spread validity (Ridgeway and Correll 2006). Some
by people acting on them, then women’s greater status beliefs will crumble while others take
hesitation may slow the diffusion of emerging hold. This is an important direction for future
status beliefs in society. It is unlikely, however, research. Yet the ease with which new status dis-
to actually stop the diffusion process because tinctions emerge suggests that some form of
men and women interact frequently in our soci- status distinction will remain a powerful force
ety (Ridgeway and Smith-Lovin 1999). of social inequality that is not easy to suppress.
Although women might require more validating
experiences before acting on new status beliefs, Cecilia L. Ridgeway is the Lucie Stern Professor of
they are likely to gain those experiences from Social Sciences in the Department of Sociology at
men who treat them according to new beliefs Stanford University. She has published extensively on
and whom they witness treating others accord- status processes, groups, gender, and social psy-
chology. Her current research addresses the role that
ing to the belief (Ridgeway and Erickson 2000).
social hierarchies in everyday interaction play in
Men who act more quickly on status beliefs stratification and social inequality, especially in
will, through their actions, confirm the beliefs regard to gender. She is past editor of Social
for women so that women will act on them as Psychology Quarterly and recipient of the 2005
well. Cooley-Mead Award for career contribution to social
HOW EASILY DOES DIFFERENCE BECOME STATUS?—–61

psychology and the 2008 SWS Feminist Lecturer Brewer, Marilyn B. and Rupert J. Brown. 1998.
Award. “Intergroup Relations.” Pp. 554–94 in The
Handbook of Social Psychology, 4th ed., Vol. 2,
Kristen Backor is a PhD candidate in Sociology at
edited by D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, and G. Lindzey.
Stanford University and the head research assistant
at the Stanford Institute for the Quantitative Study of New York: McGraw-Hill.
Society (SIQSS). Her research focuses on the inter- Cancian, Francesca M. 1987. Love in America:
section of social psychology, science and technolo- Gender and Self-Development. New York:
gy, and emotions. Cambridge.
Chwe, Michael S-Y. 2001. Rational Ritual: Culture,
Yan E. Li is a PhD candidate in the department of Coordination, and Common Knowledge. Princeton,
Sociology at Stanford University. Her research com- NJ: Princeton University Press.
bines theoretical inquiries of micro-level founda- Conway, Michael, M. Teresa Pizzamiglio, and Lauren
tions of social inequality and empirical examinations Mount. 1996. “Status, Communality, and Agency:
of how immigrants are affected by race and gender Implications for Stereotypes of Gender and Other
inequalities both socioeconomically and ideologi- Groups.” Journal of Personality and Social
cally. Psychology 71:25–38.
Justine E. Tinkler is an Assistant Professor of Eagly, Alice H. and Stephen J. Karau. 2002. “Role
Sociology at Louisiana State University. Broadly, Congruity Theory of Prejudice towards Female
her research focuses on how social psychological Leaders.” Psychological Review 109:573–79.
processes perpetuate macro-level inequality. Her Fiske, Susan T. 1998. “Stereotyping, Prejudice, and
recent work examines how cultural beliefs, privi- Discrimination.” Pp. 357–411 in The Handbook of
leges, and norms of interaction slow the effectiveness Social Psychology, 4th ed., Vol. 2, edited by D. T.
of laws aimed at reducing race and gender inequal- Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, and G. Lindzey. Boston, MA:
ity. Recent publications on this topic appear in Social McGraw-Hill.
Psychology Quarterly (with Yan Li and Stefanie Foschi, Martha and Vanessa Lapointe. 2002. “On
Mollborn) and in Law and Social Inquiry. Current Conditional Hypotheses and Gender as a Status
projects also include an analysis of how gender influ- Characteristic.” Social Psychology Quarterly
ences the decision making of venture capitalists and 65:146–62.
an examination of implicit attitudes about transracial Markovsky, Barry, LeRoy F. Smith, and Joseph
families. Berger. 1984. “Do Status Interventions Persist?”
Kristan G. Erickson received her doctorate in soci- American Sociological Review 49:373–82.
ology from Stanford University in 1998. Her research Rashotte, Lisa Slattery and Murray Webster Jr. 2005.
interests include social psychology, status process- “Gender Status Beliefs.” Social Science Research
es, adolescent development, and family processes. 34:618–33.
Her published work includes papers in American Ridgeway, Cecilia L. 1991. “The Social Construction
Journal of Sociology, Child Development, Journal of of Status Value: Gender and other Nominal
Adolescent Research, Youth and Society, and Journal Characteristics.” Social Forces 70:367–86.
of Youth and Adolescence. ———. 2000. “The Formation of Status Beliefs:
Improving Status Construction Theory.” Pp.
77–102 in Advances in Group Processes, Vol. 17.
REFERENCES Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Berger, Joseph, M. Hamit Fisek, Robert Z. Norman, ———. 2006. “Status Construction Theory.” Pp.
and Mor ris Zelditch Jr. 1977. Status 301–23 in Contemporary Social Psychological
Characteristics and Social Interaction. New York: Theories, edited by P. J. Burke. Stanford, CA:
Elsevier. Stanford University Press.
Berger, Joseph, Cecilia L. Ridgeway, M. Hamit Fisek, Ridgeway, Cecilia L. and James W. Balkwell. 1997.
and Robert Z. Norman. 1998. “The Legitimation “Group Processes and the Diffusion of Status-
and Delegitimation of Power and Prestige Orders.” Value Beliefs.” Social Psychology Quarterly
American Sociological Review 63:379–405. 60:14–31.
Berger, Joseph, Cecilia L. Ridgeway, and Morris Ridgeway, Cecilia L., Elizabeth Heger Boyle, Kathy
Zelditch Jr. 2002. “Construction of Status and Kuipers, and Dawn T. Robinson. 1998. “How Do
Referential Structures.” Sociological Theory Status Beliefs Develop? The Role of Resources and
20:157–79. Interactional Experience.” American Sociological
Berger, Joseph and Mur ray Webster. 2006. Review 63:331–50.
“Expectations, Status, and Behavior.” Pp. 268–300 Ridgeway, Cecilia L. and Shelley J. Correll. 2006.
in Contemporary Social Psychological Theories, “Consensus and the Creation of Status Beliefs.”
edited by P. J. Burke. Stanford, CA: Stanford Social Forces 85:431–54.
University Press. Ridgeway, Cecilia L. and Kristan G. Erickson. 2000.
62—–AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

“Creating and Spreading Status Beliefs.” American ed by S. Worchel and W. C. Austin. Chicago, IL:
Journal of Sociology 106:579–615. Nelson-Hall.
Ridgeway, Cecilia L., Cathryn Johnson, and David Troyer, Lisa. 2002. “The Relation between
Diekema. 1994. “External Status, Legitimacy, and Experimental Standardization and Theoretical
Compliance in Male and Female Groups.” Social Development in Group Processes Research.” Pp.
Forces 72:1051–77. 131–47 in Theory, Simulation, and Experiment:
Ridgeway, Cecilia L. and Lynn Smith-Lovin. 1999. Theory Development, Testing, and Growth in
“The Gender System and Interaction.” Annual Group Processes Research, edited by J. Szmatka,
Review of Sociology 25:1991–1216. M. Lovaglia, and K. Wysienska. Westport, CT:
Rudman, Laurie A. and Kimberly Fairchild. 2004. Praeger.
“Reactions to Counterstereotypic Behavior: The
———. 2003. “The Role of Social Identity
Role of Backlash in Cultural Stereotype
Processes in Status Construction.” Advances in
Maintenance.” Journal of Personality and Social
Group Processes 20:149–72.
Psychology 87:157–76.
Rudman, Laurie A. and Peter Glick. 2001. Wagner, David G. and Joseph Berger. 1997. “Gender
“Prescriptive Gender Stereotypes and Backlash and Inter personal Task Behaviors: Status
toward Agentic Women.” Journal of Social Issues Expectation Accounts.” Sociological Perspectives
57:743–62. 40:1–32.
Stewart, Penni A. and James C. Moore Jr. 1992. Weber, Max. 1968. Economy and Society. Edited by
“Wage Disparities and Performance Expectations.” G. Roth and C. Wittich and translated by E.
Social Psychology Quarterly 55:70–85. Frischoff, et al. New York: Bedminster Press.
Tajfel, Henri and John C. Turner. 1986. “The Social Webster, Murray and Stuart J. Hysom. 1998.
Identity Theory of Intergroup Behavior.” Pp. 7–24 “Creating Status Characteristics.” American
in The Psychology of Intergroup Relations, edit- Sociological Review 63:351–79.

You might also like