You are on page 1of 9

Focus on Autism and Other

Schema-Based Strategy Instruction


Developmental Disabilities
26(2) 87­–95
© 2011 Hammill Institute on Disabilities

in Mathematics and the Word Reprints and permission:


sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1088357611405039
Problem-Solving Performance http://focus.sagepub.com

of a Student With Autism

Sarah B. Rockwell1, Cynthia C. Griffin1, and Hazel A. Jones1

Abstract
The purpose of this study was to provide preliminary results on the use of schema-based strategy instruction to teach addition
and subtraction word problem solving to a fourth grade student with autism.The student was taught to use schematic diagrams
to solve three types of addition and subtraction word problems. A multiple probes across behaviors single-case design was
used, with solving each of the three problem types treated as a separate behavior. Interpretation of results indicated that the
participant’s ability to solve all types of one-step addition and subtraction word problems improved following instruction.
Improvement also generalized to problems with unknowns in the initial and medial position and was maintained over time.

Keywords
autism, mathematics, problem solving, schema-based instruction

Federal legislation mandates that schools be increasingly autism in the school-age population (Centers for Disease
accountable for the academic progress of all students. The Control and Prevention, 2009) and to the impact of char-
No Child Left Behind Act (NCLB, 2001) requires that acteristic features of autism on mathematics performance.
schools demonstrate adequate yearly progress by increasing According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
student achievement, including the achievement of students Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-
with disabilities, on statewide assessments of reading and IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000), language
mathematics. Additionally, the Individuals With Disabili- impairment is a characteristic feature of autism. Researchers
ties Education Improvement Act (IDEA, 2004) requires that have found that children with autism perform significantly
students with disabilities have access to and make progress lower than typically developing children and language-
in the general education curriculum and participate in state ability-matched children on tasks measuring executive
and district assessments. Although both NCLB and IDEA functioning, including planning, organization, switching
recognize that some students will be unable to participate in cognitive set, and working memory (Hughes, Russell, &
these grade-level assessments, the majority of students with Robbins, 1994; Ozonoff, Pennington, & Rogers, 1991).
disabilities must be included. Researchers also have shown that both language impair-
Within the area of mathematics, the general education cur- ment and executive dysfunction have detrimental impacts
riculum and statewide assessments are increasingly focused on mathematics functioning (Donlan, 2007; Zentall, 2007).
on conceptual understanding and problem solving (Bottge, Language impairment may affect mathematics development
2001; National Council of Teachers of Mathematics in a variety of areas including number-word sequence, calcula-
[NCTM], 2002). However, helping students with disabilities tion skills, fact retrieval, and word problem solving (Donlan,
attain competence in mathematical problem solving has 2007). Language impairment likely has an effect on students’
proven especially challenging. According to Bottge (2001), ability to solve word problems because, unlike mathematical
students with disabilities advance at a slower rate than their
non-disabled peers, resulting in ever-increasing discrepan- 1
University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA
cies and limited proficiency on tests of mathematics
Corresponding Author:
competence. Sarah B Rockwell, University of Florida, P.O. Box 117050,
The mathematics progress of students with autism is of Gainesville, FL 32611-7050
particular concern due to the increasing prevalence of Email: srockwell@ufl.edu.
88 Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities 26(2)

tasks that require students to make use of numerical informa- interpreted the results of the meta-analysis to conclude that
tion only, mathematical word problems require students to use instruction using visual representations resulted in large effect
both semantic and numerical information to problem solve. sizes, particularly when visual representations were paired
Researchers have shown that students with autism have diffi- with heuristics and direct instruction. Heuristics consist of
culty making use of semantic information (Frith & Snowling, generalized problem-solving strategies that provide students
1983). It is therefore likely that the additional semantic infor- with steps to follow when approaching word problems, such
mation in word problems may affect the ability of students with as shown in the early work of Pólya (1945, 1957).
autism to accurately solve these problems. Schema-based strategy instruction (SBI) is an intervention
Executive dysfunction also contributes to mathematics dif- that uses visual representations, heuristics, and direct instruc-
ficulties. According to Zentall (2007), “attention is a critical tion to teach students to solve word problems. A schema
factor in math performance and achievement” (p. 234). Poor consists of a mental problem-solution representation that
attention leads to alignment errors, procedural calculation allows an individual to efficiently solve a class of similar
errors, and difficulty learning math facts. Additionally, defi- problems (Gick & Holyoak, 1983). In one of the earliest
cits in sustained attention and working memory contribute to studies designed to explore schema development in mathe-
difficulties with mathematical problem solving and concept matical problem solving, Carpenter and Moser (1984) found
formation. Geary, Hoard, Byrd-Craven, Nugent, and Numtee that as children got older they progressed from concretely
(2007) suggest that working memory deficits also may con- modeling the actions indicated in word problems to using
tribute to the use of immature problem-solving strategies and more complex strategies based on an increased understand-
procedures. In addition, these deficits may lead to difficulties ing of the underlying schemas for change, combine, and
inhibiting irrelevant information, thus leading to procedural compare problem types. Change problems involve an initial
and problem-solving errors. For instance, students with work- quantity that is either increased or reduced by an action to
ing memory deficits may use finger-counting strategies rather result in a different ending quantity. Combine problems
than recalling math facts to solve computational problems. To involve two or more initial quantities that are combined to
solve word problems, these students may use concrete model- form an ending quantity. Compare problems involve two
ing rather than demonstrating an understanding of the connec- initial quantities that are compared to obtain a difference.
tion between semantic structures and operations. Researchers have documented the differentiated effective-
Given that students with autism exhibit language impair- ness of using SBI to improve the additive word problem-
ment and executive dysfunction, including deficits in atten- solving performance of students with learning disabilities in
tion control and working memory, these students will likely both resource room and inclusive settings (e.g., Jitendra
manifest difficulties with mathematical problem solving. et al., 2007; Jitendra et al., 1998) and have shown mainte-
Yet mathematical problem solving is a primary goal of nance of the skill across time (Jitendra & Hoff, 1996).
mathematics instruction, a necessary skill for proficiency on Despite the evident promise of SBI for improving mathe-
statewide assessments, and a critical process for successful matical word problem-solving performance, only one such
functioning in an increasingly technological and mathemati- study has included individuals with developmental disabili-
cally oriented society (NCTM, 2002). According to Browder ties. Neef, Nelles, Iwata, and Page (2003) evaluated the effec-
and Cooper-Duffy (2003), academic instruction for students tiveness of teaching the precurrent skills of identifying the
with significant disabilities, such as autism, should focus on initial value, change value, operation, and ending value on
developing skills that address academic standards while also the ability of two males, ages 19 years and 23 years, with
promoting successful functioning in the community. Therefore, developmental disabilities. The researchers concluded that
fostering successful problem-solving performance in mathe- teaching adults with developmental disabilities to identify
matics and its applications to everyday life for students with the component parts of change problems could effectively
autism is critical. improve their ability to solve novel problems of the same
Despite the need to identify scientifically based practices type. The results of this study are promising and can be
for teaching mathematical problem solving to students with used to suggest that students with developmental disabili-
autism, few researchers have undertaken the task. However, a ties, and perhaps students with autism, may benefit from a
substantial body of research has been used to evaluate the effi- behavioral approach to problem-solving instruction that
cacy of interventions aimed at improving math problem-solv- involves task analysis and discrete training in precurrent skills.
ing performance of students with math difficulties and learning Per requirements in IDEA and NCLB, teachers must use
difficulties. In their meta-analysis, Gersten et al. (2009) reviewed evidence-based practices to improve the mathematics per-
42 studies designed to improve students’ math performance. formance of students with disabilities. SBI includes several
Studies of word problem-solving interventions included components that might be helpful for teaching students with
instruction in general problem-solving heuristics, multiple- autism to solve additive word problems. The use of schematic
strategy instruction, peer-assisted instruction, direct instruc- diagrams to represent the semantic structure of word prob-
tion, and instruction using visual representations. The authors lems may reduce the language demands associated with
Rockwell et al. 89

word problem solving. In addition, Capizzi (2007) suggested


that the formation of problem schema may ease the working Part Part
memory demands associated with word problem solving. All
Although Neef et al. (2003) included participants with
developmental disabilities, they did not investigate all three
Figure 1. Schematic Diagram for Group Problems
addition and subtraction problem types, include school-aged
children, or include individuals with autism. The current
study extends prior research by examining the efficacy of general education classroom, but Samantha did not have her
using SBI with a fourth-grade student with autism. Research own aide. She also received occupational therapy for 30 minutes
questions addressed in this study included the following: each week to improve her handwriting. Samantha participated
(a) Will SBI improve the ability of a student with autism to in grade-level statewide and school district assessments.
solve one-step addition and subtraction word problems with According to her parents and teachers, Samantha was a hard
the unknown in the final position? (b) Will improvement in worker who was eager to please and who responded well to
problem-solving performance generalize to problems with verbal praise.
unknowns in other locations? (c) Will improvement in prob-
lem-solving performance be maintained over time?
Setting
This study was conducted over an 8-week period during the
Method summer between Samantha’s third and fourth grade years.
Participant The first author conducted all instruction during tutoring
sessions that occurred four times per week for a total of
The study participant, who was given the pseudonym Samantha, 540 minutes of instructional time. Tutoring sessions were
was a 10-year-, 3-month-old female. Samantha was not taking conducted at 9:00 a.m. on Monday through Thursday morn-
any medications, was not under any dietary constraints, and was ings in the first author’s home in a quiet room designated as
not participating in any private therapy at the time of this study. a home office. Tutoring sessions lasted 2 hours, the first 45
A clinical psychologist diagnosed Samantha with autistic dis- minutes of which were devoted to SBI. During the remain-
order in 2006 based on clinical observations and the results of der of tutoring sessions, Samantha received instruction in
the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord, reading, grammar, vocabulary, and expository writing.
Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, 1999). Samantha had nonverbal intel-
lectual abilities in the low average range (SS = 79) as measured
by the Universal Nonverbal Intelligence Test (UNIT; Bracken Independent Variable
& McCallum, 1998). Her language abilities, as measured by the Lesson checklists and teaching scripts were used to ensure
Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Fourth Edition that important instructional components were addressed
(Semel, Wiig, & Secord, 2003), were below average (SS = 45 during each session. Lesson scripts were modified based on
to 58). The evaluation report, which was consistent with teach- those used in a previous SBI study (Griffin & Jitendra, 2009).
ers’ reports, indicated that Samantha had difficulty following A sample script and implementation checklist is available
multistep directions, forming complete grammatically correct from the first author. Treatment was conducted in three
sentences, and using and understanding age-appropriate vocab- phases with each phase addressing one of the three problem
ulary. However, Samantha could follow single-step directions, types identified by Carpenter and Moser (1984), based on
use simple sentences and phrases to communicate her wants and the sequence established by Christou and Philippou (1999):
needs, and comprehend information conveyed using simple first group, then change, and finally compare problem types.
sentences and phrases. Her mathematics performance, as mea- Problem-type definitions. Group problems consist of two
sured by the KeyMath Diagnostic Assessment, Third Edition smaller groups, called parts, which are combined to form a
(Connolly, 2007), was shown to be in the very low range (SS = larger group, or all (refer to the group schematic diagram in
63). Her performance on the Applications cluster, which mea- Figure 1). Consider the story situation: Jen bought 12 apples
sures problem solving, was particularly low (SS = 55). and 15 oranges. Jen bought 27 pieces of fruit. In this prob-
Samantha’s problem-solving performance was inhibited lem the two parts are 12 apples and 15 oranges while the all
because she had difficulty determining which operation to use is 27 pieces of fruit.
when presented with simple addition and subtraction word Change problems consist of a beginning amount, change
problems. According to Samantha’s Individualized Education amount indicated by an action, and an ending amount.
Program, she spent the majority of the day in a general education Figure 2 is a schematic diagram for the change problem.
classroom but received mathematics instruction 4 hours per The change amount in the problem can indicate addition or
week from a special education teacher in a resource class- subtraction. The term “get more” was applied to additive
room. During reading, an aide was assigned to Samantha’s changes, while the label “get less” was applied to subtractive
90 Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities 26(2)

Larger Amount

Smaller Amount Difference

Figure 2. Schematic Diagram for Change Problems Figure 3. Schematic Diagram for Compare Problems

changes (Willis & Fuson, 1988). Consider the story situa- instruction in how to solve change problems consisted of four
tion: Jen had 12 oranges. She ate 3 of them. Now she has lessons. As in previous SBI studies, instruction on each type of
9 oranges. This is a “get less” situation with the beginning word problem was conducted in two parts. The first part used
amount of 12 oranges. The change amount is -3 oranges and story situations in which all components of the problem con-
the ending amount is 9 oranges. Now, consider the story sisted of known quantities in order to facilitate schema induc-
situation: Jen had 12 oranges. She picked 14 more. Now she tion. The second part included one unknown quantity for which
has 29 oranges. This is a “get more” situation with the begin- the participant solved. This unknown quantity was the all for
ning amount of 12 oranges. The change amount is +14 oranges group problems, the ending for change problems, and the dif-
and the ending amount is 29 oranges. ference for compare problems. These unknown positions were
Compare problems consist of a larger amount, a smaller chosen based on research indicating that it is easier for stu-
amount, and a difference, which results from comparing the dents to solve problems with unknowns in the final position of
smaller and larger amounts (see Figure 3 for the compare the diagram than in the initial or medial positions (Willis &
schematic diagram). Compare problems can be written in two Fuson, 1988).
forms. Consider the story situation: Jen has 22 apples. She has As in previous SBI studies, direct instruction (Gersten,
15 oranges. Jen has 7 more apples than oranges. This story Woodward, & Darch, 1986) consisting of teacher modeling,
situation also can be written: Jen has 22 apples. She has 15 guided practice, independent practice, and continuous teacher
oranges. Jen has 7 fewer oranges than apples. In both cases, feedback was used throughout all phases of instruction to
22 apples is the larger amount, 15 oranges is the smaller amount, teach Samantha to use schematic diagrams to complete the
and 7 is the difference. In one situation this differences refers RUNS steps. Direct instruction was used to teach Samantha
to fewer oranges, while in the other it refers to more apples. to recognize the salient features of the three problem types.
Instructional sequence. The SBI intervention procedures Throughout all phases of instruction Samantha was presented
used in this study were based on those used in previous with problems from all three problem types in order to facili-
SBI studies (e.g., Jitendra et al., 2007). As in previous studies, tate discrimination of when to use the diagrams taught.
Samantha was taught a four-step heuristic for problem solving Problem sorting activity. Because Samantha experienced
using the mnemonic RUNS. The steps in RUNS were based unanticipated difficulty discriminating between the different
on the FOPS mnemonic that included the following: (1) Find problem types, a brief problem sorting activity was added
the problem type, (2) organize the information in the problem during the second instructional session on group problems.
using the diagram, (3) plan to solve the problem, and (4) solve This sorting activity was included once during story situa-
the problem. The steps in FOPS were shortened to include tion instruction for each problem type. During this activity,
simple phrases easier for Samantha to understand and execute. Samantha was presented with story situations for all three
The steps in RUNS included: (1) Read the problem, (2) use a problem types. She was then asked to sort these problems as
diagram, (3) number sentence, and (4) state the answer. These belonging, or not belonging, to the type being taught. Once
steps were intended to cue Samantha to read the whole prob- sorted, she was asked to explain the reasons for her deci-
lem, choose the appropriate diagram to use, draw the chosen sions by describing the presence or absence of the critical
diagram, complete the diagram using numbers from the given features of the problem type being taught.
problem, use the diagram to create a number sentence that Generalization. Following instruction on all problem types,
results in problem solution, and write the answer with its label. one lesson was conducted that was focused on generalizing
Unlike in previous SBI studies, instruction in using schematic to problem solving with unknowns in other locations. This
diagrams to solve each type of word problem was conducted lesson began with a review of the algebraic reasoning
separately. This decision was made so that Samantha could required to solve for an unknown in the initial or medial
practice using each schematic diagram to mastery before being position of a number sentence. Instruction in algebraic rea-
introduced to additional diagrams. Instruction in how to solve soning included a review of the relationship between the
group and compare problems consisted of three lessons, while addition and subtraction operations as well as a discussion
Rockwell et al. 91

of which operation would be used to find a larger quantity problem situations from each problem type for a total of six
and which operation would be used to find a smaller quan- problems. All problem situations involved unknowns in the
tity. The remainder of the lesson consisted of guided practice final position and information presented entirely in text,
in solving problems with unknowns in the initial or medial with the exception of generalization probes, which included
position. Generalization probes were conducted after this unknowns in other positions. On each probe, one change
lesson. A final set of probes with unknowns in the final loca- problem was a “get more” problem and one change problem
tion was administered 6 weeks following the completion was a “get less” problem. Also, one compare problem used
of instruction to assess maintenance of treatment effects the term “fewer” to compare quantities, while the other used
over time. Because positive teacher feedback and corrective the term “more” to compare quantities.
feedback included as part of the direct instruction procedures Samantha was given the following instructions prior to
were sufficient to reinforce correct responses and eliminate beginning each probe: “Here are six problems I want you to
incorrect responses for Samantha, an additional reinforce- solve by yourself. Remember to use what you have learned
ment schedule was not included as part of the intervention. to solve these problems. If you have trouble reading a prob-
Treatment Integrity. All treatment sessions were video- lem, let me know and I will help you. Do your best.”
taped. A graduate student trained in the instructional proce- Samantha was given up to 30 minutes to complete each
dures of the study viewed one-fourth (i.e., 27%) of the probe. Each of the six problems was worth a possible three
sessions that included a random sample of all three instruc- points. One point was awarded for writing the correct num-
tional phases. The student compared the lesson components ber sentence to solve the problem. Assuming this number
observed in the videotapes to those included in lesson scripts sentence was correct, additional points were earned for cor-
and checklists. Treatment integrity was calculated by divid- rectly computing the answer and correctly labeling the
ing the number of steps observed by the number of steps total. answer with a noun. These scoring procedures are derived
Treatment integrity was 100% for all lessons assessed. The from those used by Jitendra et al. (1996, 1998, 2007) and
graduate student commented that each component of instruc- Willis and Fuson (1988).
tion was easily distinguished in the videotaped lessons.
Interobserver Agreement
Dependent Variable The first author scored all of the probes. A graduate student
Problem-solving progress was assessed using experimenter- independently scored a sample of 35% of the probes and
developed assessment materials. Practice sheets were used 36% of the practice sheets. This sample included at least
as a formative assessment of student progress during train- one probe from each probe phase and at least one practice
ing phases. Problem-solving probes were used to evaluate sheet from each instructional phase. The graduate student
treatment effects. was blind to the phase from which each probe or practice
Practice sheets. During training on each problem type, sheet was drawn. Agreement was calculated on a point-by-
Samantha’s progress was assessed using practice problem point basis such that each probe could have a maximum of
sheets. Each practice sheet consisted of two problems of the 18 agreements and each practice sheet could have a maxi-
type being taught. During story-situation instruction, prac- mum of 6 agreements. The total number of agreements was
tice sheets included two story situations in which all three divided by the total number of agreements and disagree-
quantities were known. During problem-solving instruction, ments and then multiplied by 100% to obtain the percentage
practice sheets included problem situations in which the agreement. The average inter-rater agreement on probes
final quantity was unknown. Samantha’s scores on practice was 97% (range 94% to 100%). The average inter-rater
sheets administered at the end of each training session were agreement on practice sheets was 100%.
used for making decisions about the pace of instruction.
Probes. To evaluate the effectiveness of SBI, problem-
solving probes were administered to Samantha at baseline, Design and Analysis
following instruction on each problem type, following A single-case, multiple probes across behaviors design was
instruction on generalization to unknowns in other locations, used in this study. A multiple probes design was chosen
and 6 weeks later to assess maintenance. During probe because the use of probe sessions rather than continual
phases, no training sessions took place. Instead, one probe assessment of the dependent variable would minimize test
was administered during each regularly scheduled tutoring fatigue. In addition, the multiple probes design allows for
session. Problem-solving probes consisted of six-item the demonstration of experimental control following an
experimenter-developed tests. These probes were derived instructional intervention that cannot be easily removed for
from the problem-solving probes and tests used in the Jitendra a return to baseline (Kennedy, 2005). The ability to solve
et al. (1996, 1998, 2007) studies. Each probe presented two each type of problem was treated as a separate behavior for a
92 Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities 26(2)

total of three demonstrations of experimental control. Data instruction on solving compare problems, Samantha’s per-
from probes and practice sheets were graphed following each formance improved to ceiling and was maintained at this
training or probe session to facilitate decision making. When level on subsequent probes. When solving these problems,
Samantha earned all possible points in three out of four or Samantha was observed using the correct diagram to facili-
three consecutive sessions during training on one type of tate problem solving.
problem story, instructional intervention progressed to the next
type of problem, and probe data were collected for follow-up
of the previously mastered problem type. Generalization Generalization
phases were begun when stable trend and level were estab- Samantha’s performance on generalization probes was at
lished using visual analysis on probes. This required a mini- ceiling for group and compare problems, and averaged 5 out
mum of three probe sessions. Maintenance probes were of 6 for change problems. Samantha’s failure to correctly
administered 6 weeks after termination of the intervention. solve one of the change problems appeared to be due to
incorrect algebraic reasoning as she had correctly com-
pleted the appropriate diagram for this problem.
Results
Group Problems
Maintenance
The results of the intervention are documented in Figure 4. On maintenance probes, Samantha performed at ceiling
Prior to receiving SBI on group problems, Samantha’s per- on group and change problems, and averaged 5.67 out of
formance on group problems averaged 3.75 points out of a 6 on compare problems. Samantha’s failure to reach ceil-
possible 6 points per probe. Although Samantha was able to ing for compare problems on the second maintenance
compute a correct numerical answer for these problems, she probe was due to a computational error involving regroup-
appeared to do so by chance. She was observed to apply the ing for subtraction. On all maintenance probes, Samantha
same solution strategy for all problems (i.e., adding the two correctly completed the appropriate diagram for each
given quantities together without taking time to read the problem.
problems). Following SBI, Samantha’s performance improved
to an average of 5.75 points out of a possible 6 and was
maintained at ceiling on subsequent probes. More impor- Discussion
tantly, Samantha was observed reading the problems and The results of this study can be interpreted to suggest that
using the appropriate diagram to arrive at a solution. SBI may be an effective way to teach a child with autism
to solve addition and subtraction word problems. The
participant in this study improved in her ability to solve
Change Problems single-step addition and subtraction word problems with
Prior to receiving SBI on group problems, Samantha’s per- unknowns in the final location after just 3 weeks of
formance on change problems averaged 2 out of a possible instruction. For all problem types Samantha was observed
6 points. These two points resulted from Samantha’s ability using the appropriate schematic diagram to solve word
to compute a correct numerical answer for the “get more” problems on post-training, maintenance, and generaliza-
problem through application of an indiscriminate solution tion probes.
strategy. Following instruction on group problems (i.e., first When Samantha made errors on post-training or mainte-
problem type targeted), Samantha’s performance improved nance probes, these errors were computational in nature.
to an average of 3 out of a possible 6 points due to her Because Samantha made only one error on generalization
increased use of labeling answers in accordance with the probes, the nature of this error cannot be determined with
RUNS steps. Samantha’s performance improved to ceiling certainty. However, considering Samantha’s performance
following instruction on solving change problems and was during guided practice, it is possible that she had difficulty
maintained on subsequent probes. Samantha was observed determining the correct operation to use when solving change
using the correct diagrams for change problems and correctly problems with unknowns in the initial or medial position.
discriminating “get more” and “get less” problems following During guided practice, Samantha seemed to rely on the
instruction. knowledge that addition should be used to find an unknown
larger quantity while subtraction should be used to find an
unknown smaller quantity. When solving change problems,
Compare Problems Samantha may have been less able to make use of this
Following instruction on group and change problems, but knowledge. This error analysis also indicates that following
prior to instruction on solving compare problems, Samantha’s SBI, Samantha was able to consistently and accurately use
performance was at floor on compare problems. Following schematic diagrams to facilitate problem solving.
Rockwell et al. 93

Figure 4. Points Earned on Probes and Practice Sheets for Each Problem Type

Samantha’s continued use of schematic diagrams at use of schematic diagrams when completing addition and
maintenance (i.e., 6 weeks following instruction) suggests subtraction word problems as part of homework assign-
that a child with autism who receives SBI can maintain gains ments. This evidence of maintenance and generalization is
in problem-solving performance over time. Furthermore, particularly promising given the challenges of programming
Samantha’s ability to solve problems with unknowns in the for skill generalization and maintenance among individuals
initial or medial position following one day of instruction with developmental disabilities (e.g., Koegel & Rincover,
suggests that a child with autism who receives SBI may be 1977; Stokes & Baer, 1977).
able to generalize performance to problems requiring The results of this study hold promise for improving
algebraic reasoning with minimal additional instruction. the addition and subtraction word problem-solving perfor-
Furthermore, Samantha’s classroom teachers provided anec- mance of children with autism. Findings can be used to
dotal evidence of maintenance and generalization, reporting identify ways in which SBI can be modified to meet the
that schematic diagram use and improved performance on needs of children with autism. It appears that although
addition and subtraction word problems generalized to the Samantha benefited from instruction focused on one type of
school setting. Samantha’s father also observed Samantha’s word problem, she also benefited from opportunities to
94 Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities 26(2)

distinguish the different problem types. During the guided participants to ensure that other children with autism respond
practice component of the instructional phase for group similarly to the intervention. Once the results have been veri-
problems, it became evident that Samantha had difficulty fied through replication, teachers should be trained to con-
determining which problems were not group problems. As a duct the SBI with small groups of students with autism to
result, a problem-sorting activity was added to the interven- ensure that teachers can successfully implement the inter-
tion wherein Samantha was required to sort word problems vention with fidelity in typical educational contexts.
as group or not group problems. This same activity was Additional studies also may be designed to teach children
repeated for change and compare problems during the with autism to solve multiplicative word problems, multi-
corresponding instructional phases. step word problems, and word problems that include charts
Samantha may have had some difficulty using algebraic and graphs beyond the text. Overall, SBI holds promise for
reasoning to solve change problems with the unknown in improving the mathematics learning of students with autism;
the initial or medial position but did not have similar diffi- further research may help to refine our understanding of
culties solving group or compare problems with the unknown effective mathematics problem-solving instruction for stu-
in the initial or medial position. It is possible that Samantha dents with autism.
was better able to solve the group and compare problems
because the schematic diagrams for these problems better Acknowledgments
represented the relationships among quantities in the prob- A version of this article was presented at the 2009 annual meeting
lems. The change diagram, on the other hand, is a more of the American Educational Research Association in San Diego,
abstract representation of the relationships among the com- California. The authors are grateful to Samantha and her parents.
ponents in the problem. It may be helpful to modify the Without their support, this study would not have been possible.
change diagram to better represent the relationships among
the parts of the problem, such as using separate diagrams Declaration of Conflicting Interests
for “get more” and “get less” problems, and teaching these The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
as two different problem types as was done in the Fuson and to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Willis (1989) study.
Although we found that SBI may be useful for teaching Funding
addition and subtraction word problem solving to a student The author(s) received no financial support for the research,
with autism, there are several limitations to this study. First, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
the inclusion of only one participant limits the generaliz-
ability of the findings. Only tentative conclusions can be References
drawn regarding generalization to participants with charac- American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical
teristics similar to Samantha’s, and no conclusions can be manual of mental disorders (4th ed., text revision). Washington,
drawn regarding generalization to participants with dissimi- DC: Author.
lar characteristics such as comorbid intellectual disability. Bottge, B. A. (2001). Reconceptualizing mathematics problem
In addition, although generalization across problem types solving for low-achieving students. Remedial and Special
was minimal, the use of a multiple probes across behaviors Education, 22, 102–112.
design may not be optimal, as solving the three types of Bracken, B. A., & McCallum, R. S. (1998). The Universal Nonverbal
word problems included in this study could be viewed as Intelligence Test. Chicago, IL: Riverside.
related behaviors. Furthermore, because only single-step Browder, D. M., & Cooper-Duffy, K. (2003). Evidence-based prac-
addition and subtraction word problems were included in tices for students with severe disabilities and the requirement
this study, only a subset of skills needed for successful for accountability in “No Child Left Behind.” The Journal of
mathematical word problem solving was addressed. Finally, Special Education, 37, 157–163.
SBI was conducted in a one-to-one setting by a researcher Capizzi, A. M. (2007). Mental efficiency as a measure of
familiar with the intervention. Therefore, it is unclear schema formation: An investigation in elementary math
whether teachers in typical educational contexts could suc- problem solving (Doctoral dissertation, Vanderbilt Univer-
cessfully implement SBI with students with autism without sity, 2006). Retrieved January 20, 2009, from ProQuest
some professional development. Dissertation & Thesis Full Text database (Publication No.
Future research designed to replicate and expand this AAT 3237020).
study is necessary to improve confidence in SBI in mathe- Carpenter, T. P., & Moser, J. M. (1984). The acquisition of addition
matics with children with autism. To enhance internal valid- and subtraction concepts in grades one through three. Journal
ity, future researchers may use a multiple probes across for Research in Mathematics Education, 15, 179–202.
participants design in order to control for transfer of skills Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2009). Prevalence
from one problem type to another. To contribute to external of autism spectrum disorders—Autism and developmen-
validity, this study needs to be replicated with more tal disabilities monitoring network, 2006. Morbidity and
Rockwell et al. 95

Mortality Weekly Report, 58, 1–24. Retrieved January 27, Kennedy, C. H. (2005). Single-case designs for educational
2010, from http://www.cdc.gov/mmwr/ research. Boston: Pearson/Allyn and Bacon.
Christou, C., & Philippou, G. (1999). Role of schemas in one-step word Koegel, R., & Rincover, A. (1977). Research on the difference
problems. Educational Research and Evaluation, 5, 269–289. between generalization and maintenance in extra-therapy
Connolly, A. J. (2007). KeyMath Diagnostic Assessment (3rd ed.). responding. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 1–12.
Minneapolis, MN: Pearson Assessments. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism
Donlan, C. (2007). Mathematical development in children with Diagnostic Observation Schedule-WPS (ADOS-WPS). Los
specific language impairments. In D. B. Berch & M. M. M. Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
Mazzocco (Eds.), Why is math so hard for some children? The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics. (2002). Principles
nature and origins of mathematical learning difficulties and and standards for school mathematics. Reston, VA: Author.
disabilities (pp. 151–172). Baltimore: Brookes. Neef, A. N., Nelles, F. E., Iwata, B. A., & Page, T. J. (2003). Analy-
Frith, U., & Snowling, M. (1983). Reading for meaning and read- sis of precurrent skills in solving mathematics story problems.
ing for sound in autistic and dyslexic children. British Journal Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 21–33.
of Developmental Psychology, 1, 329–342. No Child Left Behind Act of 2001, 20 U.S.C. 70 § 6301 et seq. (2002).
Fuson, K. C., & Willis, G. B. (1989). Second graders’ use of sche- Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive
matic drawings in solving addition and subtraction word prob- function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Rela-
lems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 514–520. tionship to theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and
Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Byrd-Craven, J., Nugent, L., & Numtee, Psychiatry, 32, 1081–1105.
C. (2007). Cognitive mechanisms underlying achievement def- Pólya, G. (1945). How to solve it. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
icits in children with mathematical learning disability. Child University Press.
Development, 78, 1343–1359. Pólya, G. (1957). How to solve it. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
Gersten, R., Chard, D. J., Jayanthi, M., Baker, S. K., Morphy, P., Semel, E. M., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. (2003). Clinical Evalu-
& Flojo, J. (2009). Mathematics instruction for students with ation of Language Fundamentals, Fourth Edition (CELF-4).
learning disabilities: A meta-analysis of instructional compo- San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
nents. Review of Educational Research, 79, 1202–1242. Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technology of gen-
Gersten, R., Woodward, J., & Darch, C. (1986). Direct instruction: eralization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 349–367.
A research-based approach to curriculum design and teaching. Willis, G. B., & Fuson, K. C. (1988). Teaching children to use
Exceptional Children, 53, 17–31. schematic drawings to solve addition and subtraction word
Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and ana- problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 192–201.
logical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 1–38. Zentall, S. S. (2007). Math performance of students with ADHD:
Griffin, C. C., & Jitendra, A. K. (2009). Word problem solving Cognitive and behavioral contributors and interventions. In
instruction in inclusive third grade mathematics classrooms. D. B. Berch & M. M. M. Mazzocco (Eds.), Why is math so
Journal of Educational Research, 102, 187–202. hard for some children? The nature and origins of mathematical
Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for exec- learning difficulties and disabilities (pp. 219–244). Baltimore:
utive dysfunction in autism. Neurosychologia, 32, 477–492. Brookes.
Individuals With Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004,
20 U.S.C. § 1400 et seq. (2004) (reauthorization of the Indi- About the Authors
viduals with Disabilities Education Act of 1990). Sarah B. Rockwell, MEd, is a doctoral candidate in special
Jitendra, A. K., Griffin, C. C., Haria, P., Leh, J., Adams, A., & education at the University of Florida. Her current interests
Kaduvettoor, A. (2007). A comparison of single and multiple include mathematics and reading instruction for students with
strategy instruction on third grade students’ mathematical autism and teacher preparation.
problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99,
115–127. Cynthia C. Griffin, PhD, is a professor of special education at the
Jitendra, A. K., Griffin, C. C., McGoey, K., Gardill, M. C., Bhat, P., & University of Florida. Her current interests include teacher profes-
Riley, T. (1998). Effects of mathematic word problem solving sional development in mathematics and mathematics interven-
by students at risk or with mild disabilities. Journal of Educa- tions in inclusive elementary classrooms.
tional Research, 91, 345–355.
Jitendra, A. K., & Hoff, K. (1996). The effects of schema-based Hazel A. Jones, PhD, is an associate professor of special education
instruction of the mathematical word-problem-solving perfor- in the Early Childhood Studies Concentration at the University of
mance of students with learning disabilities. Journal of Learn- Florida. Her current interests include autism, language and literacy
ing Disabilities, 29, 422–431. development, and professional development of in-service teachers.

You might also like