You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/281104616

Do small differences in hydration status affect mood and mental


performance?

Article  in  Nutrition Reviews · September 2015


DOI: 10.1093/nutrit/nuv045

CITATIONS READS

45 628

2 authors:

David Benton Hayley Young


Swansea University Swansea University
326 PUBLICATIONS   10,488 CITATIONS    61 PUBLICATIONS   511 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Behavioural consequences of minor hypohydration View project

Interoception, eating behaviour and NSSI View project

All content following this page was uploaded by David Benton on 20 August 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Supplement Article

Do small differences in hydration status affect mood and


mental performance?
David Benton and Hayley A. Young

Although it has been suggested that many in the general population are
dehydrated to the extent that mood and cognition are disrupted, there has been lit-
tle research investigating mild levels of dehydration. When dehydration reduces
body mass by more than 2%, it has been consistently reported that mood is influ-
enced, fatigue is greater, and alertness is lower. In contrast, the effects on cognition
have been less consistent. Only a few studies have looked at females and these
studies made little attempt to consider hormones that influence kidney functioning.
In particular, there has been virtually no attempt to look at changes in hydration
status in the range that occurs in individuals with a sedentary lifestyle in a temper-
ate climate. There is a consequent need to study individuals who have lost up to
1% of body mass due to dehydration. While 4 intervention trials have found that
the cognition of children improved in response to water consumption, the effects of
water consumption on cognition in older adults, another high-risk group, have
been largely ignored.

INTRODUCTION disrupted by low intake of fluid? In healthy adults in the


general population, how consistent is the evidence that
Although water is an essential nutrient, the influence of mood and cognition are influenced by the variations in
small variations in its provision has been little studied hydration status that occur in the course of daily living?
in free-living members of the population. Researchers The point at which water loss affects performance
often assume that thirst and homeostatic mechanisms is not yet clearly established. However, given the funda-
have developed to the extent that the hydration status of mental importance of water for the body, dehydration
those living in temperate climates and whose lifestyle will inevitably disrupt functioning at some point. The
does not include prolonged activity is likely to be in the impact of dehydration on athletic performance has
normal range most of the time. Yet, a review of the lim- been widely studied, and a meta-analysis established
ited evidence concluded that “being dehydrated by just that dehydration amounting to 5% or more of body
2% impairs performance in tasks that require attention, mass impairs both VO2max and exercise performance.2
psychomotor, and immediate memory and working The evidence was, however, insufficient to conclude
memory tasks, as well as assessment of the subjective that smaller degrees of dehydration affected perfor-
state.”1 The following questions then arise. In everyday mance. It was noted that interpretation was often diffi-
life, to what degree is bodily fluid lost? Does a 2% loss cult, as the means of inducing dehydration introduced
of body mass occur often? What degree of dehydration potentially confounding variables, such as the effects of
disrupts the functioning of the brain? Can it be <2%? exercise, increased body temperature, changes in sub-
Are there high-risk groups, such as children and older strate availability, and the acid–base status. If there is
adults, whose psychological functioning is commonly this degree of uncertainty with the relatively large

Affiliation: D. Benton and H. Young are with the Department of Psychology, Swansea University, Swansea, Wales, UK.
Correspondence: D. Benton, Department of Psychology, Swansea University, Swansea SA2 8PP, Wales, UK. E-mail d.benton@swansea.ac.uk.
Phone: þþ44-1792-295607.
Key words: children, cognition, dehydration, hydration, mild dehydration, mood.
C The Author(s) 2015. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Life Sciences Institute. All rights reserved. For
V
Permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com.

doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuv045
Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96
R
83
changes in hydration associated with exercise, what evi- indicate would need 2 samples of 63 in a parallel design
dence is there that the degree of fluid loss experienced to have a power of 0.80 at the 0.05 level of significance.
during a normal day has the potential to be disruptive? Table 1 suggests that a difference in mood effected by
Although there are suggestions that a loss of as little as hydration status occurred repeatedly with much smaller
1%–2% of body mass may reduce psychological func- sample sizes, suggesting that a larger effect was being
tioning, is there evidence that changes of this magni- studied. It seems, however, that the effect of hydration
tude occur regularly? Alternatively, could a smaller loss on aspects of cognition is less robust; assuming there is
of body mass be established as disruptive when an ap- an effect, larger samples than used to date should be
propriate experimental design and sensitive measures considered.
are used? Table 13–32 briefly summarizes studies that If a typical sample size used in this area is put into
have varied hydration status and examined the effects context and compared, for example a sample of 10 who
on mood and cognition. Rather than discussing each were dehydrated compared with another 10 who were
study in turn in the present review, various generaliza- euhydrated, it would be possible, with 0.80 power and
tions are made and considered. 0.05 significance, to distinguish a difference amounting
to 1.25 standard deviations. Cohen defines a large effect
Changes in psychological functioning size as 0.8 of a standard deviation. A priori it might be
expected that minor changes in hydration status are not
One major impression created by examining the studies going to have such an obvious influence on cognitive
collectively is that it is easier to observe changes in function. Naturally, testing the same individuals under
mood than in cognition. All 21 studies that reported different conditions would decrease the sample size re-
ratings of mood found that it was adversely influenced quired, but such a repeated measures design often
by dehydration. Specifically, people were less alert and causes problems when psychological tests are consid-
more fatigued. In contrast, only 13 of the 25 cognition ered. The second time a test is taken, learning will have
studies reported at least 1 significant result. Another taken place, strategies may have been developed, and
impression is that the sample sizes used in many of the the motivation created by novelty may have passed. At
studies reflect those that are commonly used in exercise the very least, individuals should be tested so that the
studies rather than a considered intention to look for order of treatment is sometimes AB and at other times
changes in psychological functioning. There is a clear it is BA. This does not control for the order of testing,
need to use power statistics to indicate the necessary and this variable will need to be added to the analysis.
sample size. Human behavior is extremely variable, Unfortunately, it is not uncommon to find an interac-
both between individuals and within an individual. As tion between the order of testing and the treatment,
behavior reflects a multitude of factors, it is probable making the data difficult, if not impossible, to interpret.
that the manipulation of any single variable is going to Any expectation that minor levels of dehydration
make, at the most, a small difference. Clearly, when de- will cause only small cognitive differences raises various
hydration is extreme, the impact will be greater and will possible reactions. One is that although statistical signif-
be picked up in small-scale studies. However, if minor icance may be achieved with increased power, little of
changes in hydration status are sought, like those that practical importance may be gained. This response
occur in everyday life, only large samples will produce could easily become a reason for despair, as it is in the
the needed statistical power. An obvious hypothesis is nature of human behavior that a vast range of factors all
that the relatively frequent failure to find changes in have a small influence. It is unrealistic to expect that a
cognitive functioning may reflect a lack of statistical small change in a single variable is going to have a ma-
power. jor impact on everyday behavior. As such, any single in-
In considering future research, a first step is to ask tervention by itself may have little chance of success,
what effect size is likely to occur. There have been many although it may still be of use when it is part of a more
attempts to find a perceived improvement, using qual- widespread coordinated program of intervention.
ity-of-life measures to compute the minimum differ- Another response is that, in sport, where small margins
ence needed following a medical intervention. One make the difference, the philosophy of looking for
review that considered changes in quality of life con- many “marginal gains” can bring success. From this
cluded that such improvements have been found to be perspective, considering even small differences in hy-
consistently in the order of half of a standard deviation; dration status offers another opportunity for a marginal
this possibly reflects a limit in the ability to discriminate gain. A third reaction is the need to consider individual
that is about half of a standard deviation.33 Cohen34 de- differences, such that although, on average, the effects
fined a medium effect size as a difference of half of a may be small, for some individuals, they are larger and
standard deviation, a difference that power statistics more important.

84 Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


R
Table 1 Summary of studies investigating the effects of dehydration on mood and cognition
Reference Study design Participants Age (y)a Ambient Body mass Mood Cognition
temperature
Sharma et al.3 4 h at raised temperature followed M ¼ 25 20–35 25, 29.6, 32.2, Alertness ; Card sorting ;
by exercise 33.3 C with dry or Vigilance ;

R
humid heat Associative learning ;
for 4 h
Sharma et al.4 Heat þ exercise M¼8 21–24 45 C, 30% humidity 1%, 2%, 3% Symbol substitution n.s.
39 C, 60% humidity Working memory
1% n.s., 2%, 3% ;
Psychomotor

Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


1% n.s., 2%, 3% ;
Gopinathan et al.5 Water restriction and exercise in heat M ¼ 11 20  25 45 C 1%, 2%, 3%, Arithmetic ;
4% Memory ;
Visuomotor tracking ;
Cian et al.6 1.5 h and 3.5 h at 25 C with or without M¼8 27 25 C 2.8% Fatigue : Discrimination ;
fluid and exercise Psychomotor
memory ;
Cian et al.7 Heat þ exercise M¼7 25 45 C, 70% humidity 2.8% Fatigue : Working memory ;
50 C, 20% humidity Perceptual discrimination ;
Reaction times n.s.
Long-term memory n.s.
Tracking n.s.
Neave et al.8 No drinking from 2400 to 1100 6 300 mL M ¼ 12 20 Calmness ; Cognitive battery:
water F ¼ 12 Alertness ; all dimensions n.s.
Rogers et al.9 No drinking for 2 h prior to a M ¼ 30 26 Alertness : at 2 min When thirsty, drink increased
2-h test during which water was drunk F ¼ 30 but not 25 or sustained attention; when not
50 min thirsty, drink decreased
performance
Shirreffs et al.10 Fluid restriction for 13, 24, and 37 h M¼9 30 2.7% at Concentration ;
Comparison of serum at 24 h F¼6 37 h Alertness ;
290 mOsmol/kg with control of Tiredness :
281 mOsmol/kg
Patel et al.11 Comparison of fluid restriction þ M ¼ 24 22 2.5% Fatigue : Memory ;
exercise vs Concussive symptoms :
euhydration Reaction times n.s.
Comparison of urine Speed of processing n.s.
1.025 mOsmol/kg H2O with Working memory n.s.
1.010 mOsmol/kg H2O Postural stability n.s.
Szinnai et al.12 24 h fluid restriction M¼8 28 2.6% Tiredness : Addition n.s.
Comparison of serum 288 mOsmol/kg F¼8 25 Perceived effort : Reaction times n.s.
H2O with 295 mOsmol/kg H2O Alertness ; Tracking n.s.
Comparison of urine 567 mOsmol/kg H2O Stroop task n.s.
with 967
mOsmol/kg H2O
(continued)

85
86
Table 1 Continued
Reference Study design Participants Age (y)a Ambient Body mass Mood Cognition
temperature
Lieberman et al.13 Monitoring of soldiers for 53 h during M ¼ 31 31 5.0% Vigor ; Vigilance ;
training in heat with little sleep Fatigue : Reaction times ;
Confusion: Memory ;
Depression : Reasoning ;
Grego et al.14 3 h cycling 6 M¼8 31 2.2% or Critical flicker fusion n.s.
fluid ingestion 4.1% Map recognition n.s.
Serwah et al.15 Exercise with 100%, 50%, or 0% of fluid M¼8 24 31 C 1.0% or Reaction times n.s.
lost consumed 1.7%
Petri et al.16 24 h without M ¼ 10 25 Energy ; Battery tests of perception,
fluid thinking, and memory took
longer when dehydrated
Tomporowski et al.17 Cycling for 120 min with or without fluid M ¼ 11 26 0.7 with fluid Reaction times n.s
replacement and 3.7% Memory n.s.
without Executive task n.s.
Edwards et al.18 Cycling þ soccer game 6 fluid M ¼ 11 24 0.7% with fluid Perceived Concentration n.s.
2.4% exertion :
no fluid
Backhouse et al.19 90 min run at M ¼ 15 21 1.4% Ratings of
70% VO2max þ fluid pleasure ;
6water 2.7%
fluid
Baker et al.20 Interval walking for 3 h M ¼ 11 21 40 C, 20% humidity 1% to 3% Lightheadedness : Vigilance ;
20% humidity Fatigue :
Adam et al.21 Soldiers studied without or without M¼6 24 2 or 20 C 3.0% Vigilance n.s.
being at 45 C for 8 hours F¼2 Simulated sentry duty n.s.
D’Anci et al.22 Comparison of athletes M ¼ 16 20 23 C, 45–55% M, 2.0% Anger : Vigilance ;
with F ¼ 15 humidity F, 1.65% Fatigue : Digit span :
and without Depression : Memory n.s.
fluid intake Tension : Spatial memory n.s.
Confusion :
Badelow et al.23 Comparison of soccer players before and M ¼ 20 20 34 C, 62–65% Up to Finger tapping n.s
after soccer match relative to humidity 2.5% Visual sensitivity n.s
fluid availability Working memory n.s
Ganio et al.24 Exercise-induced M ¼ 26 20 27.7 C, 42% 0.01% Fatigue : Vigilance ;
dehydration humidity 1.59% Anxiety : Working memory ;
Depression n.s.
Anger n.s.

R
Confusion n.s.
Armstrong et al.25 Exercise and diuretic-induced F ¼ 25 23 Exercised at 27.6 C 1.36% Anger : Vigilance n.s.
dehydration and 49.4% Fatigue : Reaction times n.s.
Comparison of plasma humidity Vigor ; Reasoning n.s.
3.5 mOsm/kg with 10.1 mOsm/kg Working memory n.s.
(continued)

Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


Table 1 Continued
Reference Study design Participants Age (y)a Ambient Body mass Mood Cognition

R
temperature
Streitbuerger et al.26 Water intake of 3–4 L over 10-h period, M¼3 25 þ1.2% Tower of London n.s.
then 150 mL per day for 2 days F¼3 2.5%
Edmond et al.27 Comparison of water intake vs no water M ¼ 12 29 Fear n.s. Reaction timec
intake first thing in the morning F ¼ 25 Confusionb: Associate learning n.s.
Sadnessb : Memory n.s.

Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


Anger n.s. Dimensional set shiftc
Energy n.s. Vigilancec
Tiredness n.s. Choice reaction time n.s.
Happiness n.s.
Tensionb :
Pross et al.28 No drink allowed F ¼ 20 25 Fatigue :
for 23–24 h Alertness ;
Comparison of plasma Confusion :
297 mOsm/kg with 302 Sleepiness :
mOsm/kg
Ely et al.29 Exercise þ heat M ¼ 32 22 50 C, 20% humidity 4.0% Fatigue : Psychomotor n.s.
exposure Confusion : Reaction times n.s.
Anger : Matching to sample n.s.
Depression : Reasoning n.s.
Vigor n.s.
Anxiety n.s.
Lindseth et al.30 Restricted fluid M ¼ 40 20 1 to 3% Working memory n.s.
intake for 2 weeks Spatial orientation ;
Flight performance ;
Shirreffs et al.31 Euhydrated or M¼9 30 Concentration ;
restricted intake for 37 h F¼6 Tiredness :
Alertness ;
Pross et al.32 Increased fluid consumption in tradition- M ¼ 11 25 With low intake:
ally low-level fluid consumers vs re- F ¼ 41 Fatigue ;
stricted fluid consumption in Confusion ;
traditionally high-level fluid consumers With high intake:
Content
Contentedness ;
Positive emotion ;
Vigor ;
a
Age expressed as range or mean
b
First test only
c
Interaction with order of testing
Abbreviations: ;, less than control; :, more than control; F, female; M, male; n.s., not significant,

87
Sex differences conclude that any disruption is due to a lack of fluid. A
raised body temperature, in particular, has been sug-
Most of the studies listed in Table 1 were conducted gested to influence both mood and cognitive function-
with males rather than females. Since it cannot be as- ing. In 1938, Kleitman et al.41 suggested for the first
sumed that the sexes respond in an identical manner, time that body temperature regulates performance,
more studies should be performed with both males and commenting that “there are two interpretations of the
females in order to make direct comparisons. It is sur- relationship between temperature and reaction time
prising that there have been few attempts to relate hy- possible: either a, mental processes represent chemical
dration status and fluid needs to the stages of the reactions in themselves, or b, the speed of thinking de-
menstrual cycle, as fluid retention is a frequently men- pends upon the level of metabolic activity of the cells of
tioned premenstrual symptom and estrogen, the level of the cerebral cortex, and by the raising of the latter
which is high in the follicular stage of the cycle, are through an increase in body temperature we indirectly
known to influence kidney function.35 speed up the thought process.” When body temperature
The American College of Sports Medicine has was raised to 38.80 C–39.05 C by immersion in wa-
noted that high levels of estrogen, such as occurs toward ter,42 the increase in core temperature resulted in an in-
the middle of the menstrual cycle, during pregnancy, crease in the speed of performance on a series of tests,
and when taking some types of oral contraceptives, are reduced alertness, and increased irritability. It has been
associated with water retention, leading to expansion of suggested that performance is better when body tem-
plasma volume. Throughout the menstrual cycle, perature is at its circadian peak and poorer at the low
changes in the level of antidiuretic hormone parallel point of the cycle. Body temperature rises from a nadir
those of estrogen and also rise when estrogen is given to in the early morning to a zenith in the early evening,
postmenopausal women. In a high-estrogen state, a with a variation of about 0.5 C. Wright et al.,43 in a
combination of increased fluid intake and greater fluid study that uncoupled temperature from other physio-
retention increases total body water. Prasha et al.36 logical parameters, found that working memory, alert-
found that urine production was lower and sodium re- ness, and attention were better at the stage of the cycle
lease greater in the postmenstrual phase; these changes when body temperature was at its highest. Another rea-
were not related to the rate of glomerular filtration. In son for taking circadian changes into account when
those taking an oral contraceptive, the excretion of wa- considering hydration is that urine production varies
ter and minerals and creatinine were lower. Female sex systematically over the 24-hour period, being lowest at
hormones cause renal vasodilation and decrease the night.44
proportion of fluid that, after reaching the kidneys, Such data suggest that those wishing to exclusively
passes into the renal tubules.37 A major symptom of consider the influence of dehydration need to distin-
premenstrual syndrome, a time when the levels of pro- guish any impact from changes in body temperature. In
gesterone are high, is water retention, although athletes addition, if body temperature is important, then the
find this to be less of a problem.38 There is also a com- time of day may prove to be critical. It would be ex-
mon belief that taking an oral contraceptive is associ- pected that any increase in temperature would prove to
ated with weight gain39 to the extent that a concern be more beneficial earlier in the day, before circadian
about gaining weight is a frequent reason for stopping changes reach maximum levels. Although the findings
the pill.40 Thus, in the context of dehydration, there is a primarily showed that dehydration was disruptive
need to compare males and females, and there is a need (Table 1) and the above studies found that a raised tem-
to consider the stage of the menstrual cycle and the use perature was beneficial, the possibility should be con-
of oral contraceptives. A minority of the studies listed sidered that a response to temperature changes is
in Table 13–32 considered both males and females. masking an adverse response to reduced fluid levels.

Body temperature Fluid replacement

To date, a majority of studies on dehydration have been Many of the studies listed in Table 13–32 simply reflect
orientated toward individuals engaging in sports and correlations such that causality cannot be demon-
exercise or those in the military, looking to see if judg- strated. Dehydration was induced and performance de-
ment is disrupted as hydration is reduced. Typically, de- clined, but why? A potentially better design is to induce
hydration was induced by either, or both, exercise and a dehydration and then rehydrate some individuals, ide-
high ambient temperature. Immediately, confounding ally using a dose–response paradigm. Cian et al.7 dehy-
variables such as physical fatigue and an increased body drated individuals by subjecting them to heat and
temperature are introduced, making it impossible to exercise (2.8% body mass) and then gave beverages

88 Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


R
with or without glucose and salt. After 2 hours, no ben- For 2 weeks during which individuals weighed
eficial effects on cognitive measures were found. themselves twice a day while consuming various bever-
Although Neave et al.8 did not report any indication of ages, weight declined by between 0.15% and 0.43%.47
the extent of dehydration, they found no positive re- Even in warmer countries, fluid is likely to be con-
sponse to water consumption after 12 hours without sumed regularly to reduce the extent of dehydration.
fluid. A study of military personnel induced dehydra- When body mass and water balance were monitored for
tion (3.0% body mass) and then either did or did not 6 days in Dar es Salaam, Tanzania, it was found that the
rehydrate.21 No cognitive measures differed in those coefficient of variation in body weight varied from
who were euhydrated compared with those who were 0.54% to 0.86%, with an average of 0.7%; the changes
hypohydrated. Such findings raise questions as they im- were accounted for by minor differences in the con-
ply that any changes are not related precisely to hydra- sumption and excretion of water.48 In 1947, Adolph49
tion status as such. Future research is needed to wrote about the physiology of individuals living in a de-
establish whether dehydration instigates changes that sert and suggested that water balance was closely regu-
are not rapidly reversed by fluid intake. lated and that body mass was typically kept within
61%; this generalization seems to have withstood the
Degree of dehydration passage of time.
In fact, little is known about the impact of differ-
The European Hydration Institute comments that “fluid ences in the habitual level of fluid intake on physiology.
losses of more than 1% of body weight can lead succes- Perrier et al.50 compared those with different habitual
sively to a reduction in exercise performance and in the levels of fluid consumption and related them to bio-
ability to control body temperature. With fluid deficits markers related to hydration status. Those who drank
of 4% and more, severe performance decrements may <1.2 L/day or between 2 and 4 L/day were contrasted.
be observed as well as difficulties in concentration, Although the specific gravity, osmolality, and color of
headaches, irritability and sleepiness, and increases in urine differed, plasma osmolality was similar, suggest-
body temperature and in respiratory rates. Dehydration ing the efficiency of mechanisms that preserve plasma
that causes a loss of 10% or more of body weight can be osmolality when fluid intake is low.
fatal.” If there is concern, not with athletes whose exer- It is clear from such data that for differences in hy-
tions will lead inevitably to the loss of fluid, but rather dration status to impact psychological functioning in
with normal variations in the general population who free-living individuals who have access to beverages,
do not exercise, is there reason to suggest that relatively one must consider changes that represent <1% of body
minor differences in hydration status influence psycho- mass. The studies listed in Table 13–32 present no rele-
logical functioning? vant evidence. Such findings put into context comments
There are marked individual differences in the rate such as “being dehydrated by just 2% impairs perfor-
at which dehydration develops when individuals are mance.”1 A 2% loss is unlikely to occur except when
subjected to the same environmental conditions. physically active for a prolonged period without con-
Benton et al.45 found that when individuals spent 4 h at suming fluid. Environmental conditions are rarely go-
30 C, although there was, on average, a 0.6% loss of ing to induce a 2% loss of body mass. The studies listed
body mass, there were substantial individual differ- in Table 13–32 give little support for adverse conse-
ences, with the losses varying from 0.24% to 2.39%. quences when there is, at most, a 1% loss of body mass.
When faced with such individual differences, the small In fact, very few studies have considered such small
sample sizes used in this area have the potential to hide changes. Although Sharma et al.4 found an adverse ef-
genuine effects, i.e., the variance added by individual fect of losing 3% of body mass, this was not found with
differences will prevent statistical significance in small- a 1% decline, although arguably the small sample size
scale studies. could not have demonstrated the significance of any
During daylight hours in the month of Ramadan, likely change. Is any suggestion that there is widespread
Muslims are not to consume food or liquid. Inevitably, underhydration in the general population simply
there will be a progressive loss of body water that wrong? Alternatively, can evidence be found of an influ-
amounts typically to about 1% of body mass by sunset.46 ence when dehydration is <2%?
In one study, when individuals did not drink for 37 h, a
loss of 1% of body mass was reported after 13 h, 1.7% Brain volume
body mass at 24 h, and 2.7% after 37 h.31 It is, however,
unlikely that many in temperate climates will experience The brain is enveloped by 3 layers of tissue called the
even this degree of dehydration, because the temperature meninges. The outer layer is the dura mater, the next is
will be lower and most will drink at some stage. the arachnoid mater, with the pia mater lying on the

Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


R
89
brain’s surface. One role of the meninges is to fix the

Brain: 0.55% after 16 h

Tower of London task


position of the brain and, therefore, reduce collisions

Ventricles increased 3.3% with Dehydration increased


Rehydration: þ0.72%

blood flow during


with the skull. In the subarachnoid space, the arachnoid
mater contains a pool of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) that
Cognition/mood

surrounds and cushions the brain. In the center of the


brain, there are fluid-filled cavities, known as the cere-
bral ventricles, which communicate with the subarach-
noid space. The choroid plexus, which is found in the
loss body mass and ventricle : ventricles, secretes about 450 mL of CSF per day.51 As
Ventricle: 6.73 mL dehydrated;

lated with decline in body


dehydration; effect corre-
the volume of the ventricles is about 25 mL,52 the CSF
in the ventricles is renewed about 18 times a day.

loss of body mass and


0–25 Hounsfield units

Correlation between
14.29 mL hydrated

Dehydration decreases the volume of blood (hypo-


volemia) and may, therefore, reduce the amount of in-
Body mass, % Brain volume

Ventricle n.s.

tracerebral blood. Dehydration also increases plasma


Correlation

Ventricle :

ventricle :
Whole n.s.

osmolarity, which changes the volume of the brain53


Brain n.s.

mass
1.56

and leads to fluid being drawn from cells. Both of these


effects are compensated by changes in the volume of
CSF in the ventricles and the subarachnoid space.
When the capacity of the choroid plexus is insufficient
2.31

to replace the lost volume, CSF is drawn from the spinal


2.2

1.6

cord into the cranium54 until the choroid plexus is able


Table 2 Summary of studies investigating the influence of dehydration on the volume of cerebral ventricles

to replace any deficiency.


480 mOsm/kg and 499 mOsm/kg

Thus, although the associations between brain vol-


Plasma: 280 mmOsmol/L when

osmolarity was similar in the


hydrated; 290 mmOsmol/L

ume and the levels of intracranial blood and CSF are


19 C (61), 40% Urine: Prior to dehydration,
Serum: 294–340 nmol/kg
Urine: 223–512 nmol/kg

complex, there are good reasons to suggest that dehy-


dration influences the structure of the brain, with the
when dehydrated

possibility that its functioning will be compromised.


Dehydration can be expected to reduce brain volume, a
2 groups:
Osmolarity

consequence of increased serum osmolality that causes


Abbreviations: :, significantly positive correlation; F, female; M, male; n.s., not significant,

cells to shrink with an inevitable enlargement of the


ventricles.
This topic has been the subject of limited study, but
31 C (61), 40%

20 C (61), 45%

the few that have been performed are summarized in


temperature

humidity

humidity

humidity

Table 2.55–59 With dehydration, the expected increase in


(62%)

(65%)

(65%)
Participants Mean age (y) Ambient

the size of the ventricles has been found consistently.


Although Dickson et al.56 did not find an overall change
in volume, they did find that the loss in body mass cor-
related significantly with an increased size of the ventri-
cles. The procedures used induced changes of between
19.8

23.8

16.8

1.5% and 2.5 % of body mass. Kempton et al.59 similarly


27

reported a significant association between changes in


9 M 11 F

the volume of the lateral ventricle and body mass; those


5M5F

who lost about 2% of body mass had lateral ventricles


1M

6M

7M

that were 7% larger. In contrast, in those who lost about


for 90 min 6heavy

for 60 min 6heavy


þ 40 mg frusemide

1.3% of body mass, the volume of the ventricles did not


1 h before exercise

1 h before exercise
24 h dehydration

16 h dehydration

change.
Before/after 1 h
Study design

Streitburger et al.26 asked participants to drink


500 mL H2O

500 mL H2O
exercise

clothes

clothes

3–4 L of water over a 10-h period and found that the


size of the ventricles reduced by, on average, 3.5%. The
average difference in ventricular volume between indi-
Reveley55

viduals who were dehydrated and those who were


Reference

Kempton

Kempton
Mellanby

et al.56

et al.57

et al.58

et al.59
Dickson

hyperhydrated was 6.2%. Whether there is commonly a


Duning
and

minor degree of dehydration, such that many would


benefit from consuming more fluid, is uncertain.

90 Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


R
The consequences of increasing the volume of the brain patients and 3.5% in those with mild cognitive impair-
is unclear since, at some stage, there will be an adverse ment.61 In adults, the skull is of a fixed size, with a vol-
effect; normally, there is a concentration gradient such ume equivalent to the size of the brain and the volume
that an excess of water outside a cell draws sodium of CSF and blood. However, the volumes of these 3
from the cell into serum. Hyponatremia is associated compartments can change, with implications for the
with a lower-than-usual level of sodium in plasma so susceptibility to brain damage that results from head in-
that water enters the cells of the brain, causing them to jury. More specifically, studies of neurodegeneration in-
swell. In extreme cases, this causes hyponatremic en- creasingly use brain imaging to detect the progression of
cephalopathy, which is potentially damaging as the disease, with consequences for early diagnosis and the
brain is confined inside the rigid bones of the skull. evaluation of therapeutic interventions. Given that such
Whether there is a benefit from a minor increase in the techniques can pick up changes that amount to as little
level of hydration, bringing with it cognitive benefits, as 0.2% of brain volume,62 even mild dehydration, if it
could be considered. Although clearly the volumes of has consequences for the volume of the brain, will inevi-
water consumed in the study of Streitbuerger et al.26 are tably have an adverse influence on the accuracy of such
in excess of what is consumed regularly, whether bever- assessments. It has been found that in healthy adults
ages of different compositions are more or less able to there is an annual expansion of the ventricles of between
hydrate the brain, without compromising the electrolyte 1.5% and 3.0%.63 It is easy to suggest that with a disor-
levels, could be considered. der such as Alzheimer’s disease, a failure to remember
It would, nevertheless, be premature to conclude to drink regularly could result in brain shrinkage that, if
that there are no implications for brain functioning be- not controlled, could be confused with brain atrophy.
cause the ventricles were not enlarged when there was a As such, there is an obvious need to establish the impact,
decline in body mass of <1%. A measurement of if any, of even minor degrees of dehydration.
changes in the size of the ventricles is an extremely Thus, at the very least, hydration status has the abil-
crude index of the status of the brain. It is reasonable to ity to add error when using brain imaging to monitor
view any change in the size of the brain as a substantial the progression of disease. In addition, dehydration
insult, such that more minor changes could also prove may potentially exacerbate existing problems. In sum-
to be disruptive and more likely to be demonstrated us- mary, there is clear evidence that dehydration of the
ing functional rather than structural imaging. To date, level that occurs during some sports causes shrinkage of
only 1 study has used this approach to relate psychologi- the brain. As yet, whether more limited dehydration has
cal functioning to dehydration-induced changes in implications for the brain and, consequently, psycho-
brain functioning. Kempton et al.59 employed the logical functioning is unclear.
Tower of London task that monitors executive function-
ing, i.e., planning carried out by the frontal lobes of the Older adults
cerebral cortex. Dehydration did not change the ability
of individuals to perform the task; however, when dehy- The studies listed in Table 1 almost exclusively investi-
drated, functional magnetic resonance imaging showed gated individuals between the ages of 20 and 30 years,
greater blood flow to the frontal cortex. It appeared that such that little can be said about older or younger age
dehydration had placed demands on the brain such that groups (see Table 3). In fact, most studies have consid-
greater resources were required to achieve the same level ered fit young individuals, and it may be that the find-
of output. A related study used electroencephalography ings do not readily generalize to those who are older or
to monitor brain activity while various cognitive tasks to those without a history of regular exercise. For a vari-
were performed by participants who had been kept at ety of reasons, older adults are likely to be more suscep-
25 C or 35 C to increase core body temperature.60 tible to dehydration than younger adults; in fact, higher
Evoked potentials showed an increase in amplitude and mortality rates and readmission to the hospital have
a decrease in latency, findings that are consistent with a been associated with low fluid intake.64 In addition to
need for increased neural activity to obtain the same the physiological changes associated with aging, disabil-
level of performance when core temperature was raised. ity may reduce access to fluids, and the use of diuretics
It is an obvious suggestion that the use of functional im- and other drugs may decrease hydration status.
aging techniques may offer a method that is sensitive In older adults, a lower level of lean body mass re-
enough to demonstrate the consequences of a relatively sults in an increased ratio of extracellular to intracellular
small degree of dehydration. mass65; in fact, the bodies of older rather than younger
The magnitude of these changes is put in context by adults can contain from 10% to 15% less water. There is
the report that a 1 year follow-up found that ventricular also less renal mass with age, reflecting glomerular scle-
volume was increased by 5%–16% in Alzheimer’s rosis and glomerular loss,66,67 with a consequent reduced

Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


R
91
Table 3 Summary of studies investigating the influence of water consumption on cognitive test performance in children
Reference Study design No. of participants, Cognition/mood test Outcome
age, climatea
Bar-David et al.76 Comparison of urine N ¼ 58 10.112.4 y, Hidden figures Hydration status did not influence perfor-
osmolarity in the warm climate Number span mance in morning
morning vs the Making groups Number span poorer in afternoon if
afternoon Verbal analogies dehydrated
Addition Other tests n.s.
Benton and Comparison of water N ¼ 40, 8.7 y Memory Results of both tests better when water
Burgess77 intake vs no water Sustained attention had been drunk
intake
Edmonds and Comparison of children N ¼ 58, 79 y Memory Visual attention tasks (letter cancellation
Burford78 who did or did not Letter cancellation and spot the difference) better after
drink water Spot the difference drinking water
Tracking Memory and tracking n.s.
Edmonds and Comparison of children N ¼ 23, 7.3 y Visual attention Visual attention and visual search better
Jeffes79 who had and had not Visual search in those who had drunk water
drunk water Memory Rated themselves happier with water
Motor performance intake
Memory and motor performance n.s.
Benton80 Comparison of 6 testing N ¼ 22, 9 y Activity sampling used When water had been drunk more time
periods; 3 when water to establish the time was spent concentrating on school
had been drunk and 3 spent in school work
when it had not concentrating on
their work
Fadda et al.81 Comparison of children N ¼ 168, 9  11, y Selective attention Better hydration status correlated with
who did or did not warm climate Addition better working memory (number span)
drink water Number span Other cognitive measures n.s.
Verbal analogies Better hydration associated with higher
Spatial ability ratings of vigor but the rating of fatigue
Mood scale was n.s.

a
Ages expressed as ranges or means; climate included when specified

ability to control the balance of both sodium and water, associated with slower psychomotor processing speed,
resulting in an increased incidence of dehydration. An poorer attention, and poorer memory. However, in the
impaired thirst mechanism resulting from hormonal absence of evidence that any detriment in functioning
changes may also play a role in the reduction of fluid in- responded to rehydration, it cannot be concluded that
take.68,69 The above changes are only a few of many the lack of fluid intake was the causal mechanism. A
mechanisms that result in a reduced ability to maintain later study by the same group looked at postmenopausal
an adequate level of hydration with age. Water intake women, with a mean age of 60 years, who lived in the
from food and beverages has been found to be lower in community.72 Again, the ratio of total body water to
American men aged 70–79 years than those aged 40–49 weight was used as an index of hydration status and was
years. Similarly, older males produce a greater volume of found to relate to memory. However, when diastolic
urine.70 Individual differences in both the intake of wa- blood pressure was included in the analysis, the rela-
ter and urine production were found, and it was con- tionship between hydration status and cognition de-
cluded that water turnover is highly variable, with these clined. As hydration is known to influence blood
differences being little explained by anthropometric pa- pressure, it appears that it needs to be included in future
rameters. Although such problems are widely acknowl- considerations of hydration before it can be concluded
edged, there have been surprisingly few attempts to that a difference in fluid levels is the mechanism. In a
relate hydration status to mood and cognition in older study of older adults with an average age of 72 years,
individuals. 21.3% of a sample with chronic hyponatremia had been
Suhr et al.71 compared 2 groups that either had or admitted to hospital after falling; this figure compared
had not been asked to abstain from eating and drinking with only 5.3% of a control group without hyponatre-
after their evening meal. The following day, bioelectrical mia. Performance on a test of attention was poorer in
impedance was used to estimate body fluid; changes in those with hyponatremia.73 However, studies that con-
the ratio of total body water to body weight approached sider existing dehydration status and relate it to cogni-
statistical significance. In this sample of individuals be- tion produce no more than correlations that cannot
tween the ages of 50 and 82 years, poorer hydration was demonstrate whether a lack of fluid is necessarily a

92 Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


R
problem. The occurrence of dehydration may involve be kept in mind that those given a drink inevitably
more than a simple lack of fluid intake. There may also know that this is the case and a placebo effect cannot be
be, for example, a reduced food intake such that one excluded.
can never be certain that hydration is responsible for In 8-year-old children, the consumption of water
any differences found. in the afternoon resulted in better memory and sus-
The influence of hydration status on older adults is tained attention.77 Similarly in 7- to 9-year-olds, visual
clearly an underresearched topic. Given the many rea- attention and memory were better after water was con-
sons for believing that older adults are at a high risk of sumed, although a visuo-motor tracking task was not
dehydration, it is strange that the possibility that im- influenced.78 A third group of 7-year-olds rated them-
proving hydration status might improve cognitive func- selves as happier after drinking water, and visual atten-
tioning has not been systematically examined. Although tion and visual search performance were better,
dehydration can be induced in deprivation studies and although memory and psycho-motor performance were
related to aspects of cognition, there is a remaining unchanged.79 Finally, 9-year-olds who did and did not
need to demonstrate that the findings generalize to in- drink water were observed and those who drank water
dividuals living freely in the community. Having as- spent more time on their school work.80
sessed preexisting hydration status, evidence that Thus, 4 studies have reported that water consump-
rehydration was beneficial would be instructive and tion benefitted aspects of children’s cognition. As nei-
studies of free-living individuals are needed. That age ther physical exercise nor subjection to heat was a part
may be an important variable is illustrated by a study of these studies, the findings are consistent with these
that examined those who subjected themselves to 10 children being dehydrated even though they lived in a
days of strenuous hill walking. In an older group, but temperate climate. There was a degree of consistency in
not a younger group, an increase in urine osmolality the outcomes; attention improved on 4 occasions77–80
was related to a decline in the speed of reaction times.74 and memory was better on 2 of 3 occasions. In the fu-
ture, the nature of the cognitive problems that are com-
Children promised, the effects of different ambient temperatures,
the optimal fluid intake, and the pattern of consump-
Children are another group said to be at high risk of be- tion need to be established.
coming dehydrated as they are relatively active, have a
greater surface to mass area, and are often reliant on DISCUSSION
others to ask if they are thirsty and to provide a drink.
Yet again, there is limited literature on this topic. Attempts to relate minor changes in hydration status to
However, in this instance, there is evidence of a positive mood and cognition are characterized by a series of
response to the provision of fluid. A 2006 review75 of methodological problems that make it difficult to draw
the association between hydration status and the cogni- conclusions. For example, it is impossible to keep par-
tion of children could not find any intervention studies. ticipants blind as to whether they have or have not been
In fact, there was only 1 study and it took a correla- able to drink. Given the known major influence of
tional approach.76 Table 376–81 lists the studies per- placebo responses, any differences in performance asso-
formed to date. ciated with dehydration may, in part at least, reflect as-
In a school in the Israeli desert, where the outside sociated expectations or a response to symptoms such
temperature was 35 C, the scores of 5 tests of cognition, as thirst or a dry mouth. Thus, studies that find a posi-
which were taken in the morning, did not depend on tive response when water is consumed may potentially
whether, as judged by urine osmolarity, the 10-year-old reflect either rehydration or a psychological response to
children tested were or were not dehydrated.76 being allowed to drink. That psychological expectancies
However, in the afternoon, the performance on 1 of 5 are important is illustrated by a study in which both
tests was better in those who were better hydrated. In consuming caffeine and having the false belief that caf-
Sicily, an association was found between urinary osmo- feine had been consumed improved both attention and
larity and digit span, which is a measure of working motor speed.82 However, Edmonds et al.83 gave water
memory. Those who were more dehydrated had poorer to groups who were and were not given information
memories and rated themselves as less happy.81 More about the benefits of its consumption for cognitive per-
recently, there have been several intervention studies formance. It was found that water consumption, not
that are important because the findings suggest a causal raised expectations, had a beneficial effect on thirst and
relationship between fluid intake and cognitive func- letter cancellation, although digit span and reaction
tioning. If consumption of fluid enhances performance, times were not influenced. One solution to these prob-
this suggests that a deficiency existed, although it should lems is to consider not differences between groups but

Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


R
93
rather differences within groups. Benton et al.45 found a less capacity for other activities.85 It has been found that
4-fold difference in the tendency to lose body mass in the effect of water consumption on thirsty individuals
individuals subjected to an identical raised temperature. was beneficial, while the effect in the absence of thirst
Given that there is, at best, a limited ability of individ- was detrimental.9 Water consumption, however, in-
uals to appreciate their precise hydration status, relating creased alertness irrespective of the initial level of thirst.
objective changes of hydration to changes in psycholog- A later study failed to find that the level of initial thirst
ical functioning may largely prevent a placebo response influenced the response to water of those who had not
from having a differential effect. eaten or drank overnight.8 It is clear that the possibility
The widespread use of either exercise and/or expo- that thirst might influence the response to drinking
sure to heat to induce dehydration makes it difficult to needs to be further considered as it has been little stud-
distinguish the effect of hydration status from that of ied and the findings are inconsistent.
physical fatigue or changes in core temperature. A key question is how often a reduced hydration
However, Cian et al.6,7 compared dehydration induced status impacts either mood or cognition in those going
by passive exposure to heat or exercise and reported about their everyday lives? Although definitions are ar-
similar decrements in cognition, memory, and percep- bitrary, mild dehydration has been defined as a fluid
tual discrimination. Thus, neither heat nor exercise was loss of 1% of body mass. Although there are reasonably
necessary to disrupt functioning, although dehydration consistent reports that a loss of 2% has negative conse-
was a common factor. These studies did, however, in- quences (Table 1), findings to date do not allow a simi-
duce a loss of more than 2.5% of body mass and, so, are lar conclusion to be made for a decline of 1% or less of
not representative of the more moderate changes that body mass. There are, however, marked differences in
occur under everyday conditions. An intriguing finding the tendency to become dehydrated when exposed to
was that memory was impaired after dehydration as the same environmental conditions.45 There may well
well as when dehydration had been prevented by con- be much to be gained by considering individual differ-
suming water. The impairment of memory, however, ences so that those with a greater or lesser tendency to
disappeared after 3 h irrespective of whether the partici- lose body fluid can be distinguished and such differ-
pants had been rehydrated. Such studies in which dehy- ences related to changes in psychological functioning.
dration is prevented by water consumption are Benton et al.45 has developed a questionnaire that corre-
instructive. Either dehydration is not the mechanism, lates with various consequences of the response to heat.
or the consumption of water takes time to replace lost An issue that has been little considered is the influ-
bodily fluid. When water is lost, it is unclear to what ex- ence of the habitual level of fluid intake. Does adapta-
tent it is intra- or extracellular in origin and the extent tion to the habitual level of intake occur such that any
to which attempts at rehydration succeed in replacing deviation from that pattern has adverse consequences?
the former rather than the latter. It remains to be con- One report indicates this may be the case, although the
clusively demonstrated whether any disruption is phenomenon needs to be examined further.32 In that
caused by dehydration rather than other mechanisms. study, Pross et al.32 compared those whose habitual in-
One study found that after overnight fasting, reac- take of water was low (<1.2 L/d) or high (2–4 L/d) and
tion times were faster when water was consumed.83 In asked them to, respectively, increase or decrease their
addition, they reported that thirst affected the speed of daily intake of water. Initially the 2 groups differed little
responding. Responses were slower when no water had in their mood. However, restricting the level of intake
been consumed, but only in those reporting high sub- of those who were used to a high level of consumption
jective thirst. The responses of those who did not report decreased contentedness, calmness, positive emotions,
feeling thirsty were not affected by water consumption. and vigor. In contrast, increasing the water consump-
It was suggested that rather than affecting dehydration tion of those who were accustomed to a low intake de-
as such, any benefit of water supplementation may re- creased fatigue and confusion. Thus, increasing intake
flect alleviation of the demands placed on central pro- was beneficial in those with a low intake whereas reduc-
cessing by the subjective sensation of thirst. It should be ing intake had a negative impact on those with a habitu-
remembered, however, that this was the only significant ally higher intake. The extent to which it is necessary to
finding after taking a long test battery. The finding, establish existing levels of fluid intake when carrying
however, supports the view of Cohen84 that subjective out studies in this area needs to be established.
thirst is the mechanism by which dehydration nega- Another perspective is to consider changes in the
tively affects cognition, by directing attention away effort required when performing a task, as it is usual for
from performance. The underlying model is that atten- there to be an element of cognitive reserve, i.e., the ca-
tion has finite capacity, such that the demands placed pacity of the brain is not fully used, allowing perfor-
on it by a process such as thirst will inevitably result in mance to be maintained by working harder. The

94 Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


R
hypothesis should be considered that a degree of dehy- cognition of children improved in response to water
dration may reduce neural capacity such that even if the consumption. Although there are good reasons to be-
performance of a task is similar when euhydrated and lieve that older adults are a high-risk group, they have
under a minor degree of dehydration, the brain may been largely ignored.
need to work harder to achieve a similar performance.
The use of brain imaging is one way to consider this
Acknowledgments
proposal. To date, 1 study has used this approach and
found that the brain did need to work harder when it
was dehydrated, although the participants experienced Funding. D.B. was contracted and funded by the
up to a 2.5% loss of body mass.59 If this finding is repli- European Hydration Institute. He received financial re-
cated, the nature of the tests used in this area will need imbursement for travel and accommodation expenses
to be considered, as they will need to generate great and an honorarium from the European Hydration
cognitive demands. It should, however, be recognized Institute for his participation in the conference and for
that changes in blood flow in brain imaging studies writing this manuscript.
could be influenced by both a reduction in blood vol- Declaration of interest. The authors have no relevant in-
ume induced by dehydration, as well as an increase in terests to declare.
blood flow induced by the activity of particular areas of
the brain. Experimental designs will need to assess pa-
rameters of both of these potential changes. REFERENCES
Typically, performance has been measured over a 1. Adan A. Cognitive performance and dehydration. J Am Coll Nutr. 2012;31:71–78.
2. Gigou PY, Lamontagne-Lacasse M, Goulet EDB. Meta-analysis of the effects of pre-
relatively short period of time, whereas if dehydration exercise hypohydration on endurance performance, lactate threshold and
requires the brain to work harder to produce the same VO2max. Med Sci Sports Exer. 2010;42(5, Suppl.): 361–362.
3. Sharma VM, Pichan G, Panwar MR. Differential effects of hot humid and hot dry envi-
output, then perhaps it is the ability to maintain perfor- ronments on mental functions. Int Arch Occup Environ Health. 1983;52:315–327.
mance over a longer period that should be considered. 4. Sharma VM, Sridharan K, Pichan G, et al. Influence of heat-stress induced dehydra-
Consistent with this suggestion was the conclusion of tion on mental functions. Ergonomics. 1986;29:791–799.
5. Gopinathan PM, Pichan G, Sharma MA. Role of dehydration in heat stress-induced
Grandjean and Grandjean,86 who noted that when an variations in mental performance. Arch Environ Health. 1988;43:15–17.
association was reported between dehydration and cog- 6. Cian C, Barraud PA, Melin B, et al. Effects of fluid ingestion on cognitive function
after heat stress or exercise-induced dehydration. Int J Psychophysiol.
nition, it tended to involve complex tasks that, it was 2001;42:243.
reasonable to assume, demanded more processing ca- 7. Cian C, Koulmann N, Barraud PA, et al. Influences of variations in body hydration
on cognitive function: effects of hyperhydration, heat stress, and exercise-induced
pacity. The fact that cognitive reserve declines with dehydration. J Psychophysiol. 2000;14:29–36.
age87 is another reason to consider samples of older 8. Neave N, Scholey AB, Emmett JR, et al. Water ingestion improves subjective alert-
ness, but has no effect on cognitive performance in dehydrated healthy young
adults who also are at risk of reduced kidney efficiency. volunteers. Appetite. 2001;37:255–256.
9. Rogers PJ, Kainth A, Smit HJ. A drink of water can improve or impair mental per-
formance depending on small differences in thirst. Appetite. 2001;36:157–158.
CONCLUSION 10. Shirreffs SM, Merson SJ, Fraser SM, et al. The effects of fluid restriction on hydra-
tion status and subjective feelings in man. Brit J Nutr. 2004;91:951–958.
11. Patel AV, Mihalik JP, Notebaert AJ, et al. Neuropsychological performance, postural
Although it has been suggested that many in the general stability, and symptoms after dehydration. J Athletic Training. 2007;42:66–75.
population are dehydrated to the extent that mood and 12. Szinnai G, Schachinger H, Arnaud MJ, et al. Effect of water deprivation on
cognitive-motor performance in healthy men and women. Am J Physiol–Reg
cognition are disrupted, the influence of a reduced hy- Integrative Comp Physiol. 2005;289:R275–R280.
dration status, at the degree that will occur in those who 13. Lieberman HR, Bathalon GP, Falco CM, et al. Severe decrements in cognition func-
tion and mood induced by sleep loss, heat, dehydration, and undernutrition dur-
have not exercised, has been subject to little study. In ing simulated combat. Biol Psychiat. 2005;57:422–429.
particular, there has been virtually no attempt to look at 14. Grego F, Vallier JM, Collardeau M, et al. Influence of exercise duration and hydra-
changes in hydration status in the range that occurs in tion status on cognitive function during prolonged cycling exercise. Int J Sports
Med. 2005;26:27–33.
those leading a sedentary life in a temperate climate. 15. Serwah N, Marino FE. The combined effects of hydration and exercise heat stress
When dehydration reduces body mass by more than 2% on choice reaction time. J Sci Med Sport. 2006;9:157–164.
16. Petri NM, Dropulic N, Kardum G. Effects of voluntary fluid intake deprivation on
due to the experience of a high ambient temperature or mental and psychomotor performance. Croatian Med J. 2006;47:855–861.
exercise, there are consistent reports that mood is influ- 17. Tomporowski PD, Beasman K, Ganio MS, et al. Effects of dehydration and fluid in-
gestion on cognition. Int J Sports Med. 2007;28:891–896.
enced, fatigue is greater, and alertness is lower. The ef- 18. Edwards AM, Mann ME, Marfell-Jones MJ, et al. Influence of moderate dehydra-
fects on cognition, however, have been less consistent. tion on soccer performance: physiological responses to 45 min of outdoor match-
play and the immediate subsequent performance of sport-specific and mental
There consequently remains a need for studies of indi- concentration tests. Br J Sports Med. 2007;41:385–391.
viduals losing up to 1% of body mass. In addition, only 19. Backhouse SH, Biddle SJH, Williams C. The influence of water ingestion during
prolonged exercise on affect. Appetite. 2007;48:193–198.
a few studies have looked at females and there has been 20. Baker LB, Conroy DE, Kenney WL. Dehydration impairs vigilance-related attention
little attempt to consider the effect of hormones on kid- in male basketball players. Med Sci Sports Exer. 2007;39:976–983.
21. Adam GE, Carter R, Cheuvront SN. Hydration effects on cognitive performance
ney function. High-risk groups also need to be exam- during military tasks in temperate and cold environments. Physiol Behav.
ined. Four intervention trials have found that the 2008;93:748–756.

Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


R
95
22. D’Anci KE, Vibhakar A, Kanter JH, et al. Voluntary dehydration and cognitive per- 55. Mellanby AR, Reveley MA. Effects of acute dehydration on computerized tomo-
formance in trained college athletes. Percept Mot Skills. 2009;109:251–269. graphic assessment of cerebral density and ventricular volume. The Lancet.
23. Bandelow S, Maughan R, Shirreffs S, et al. The effects of exercise, heat, cooling 1982;2:874.
and rehydration strategies on cognitive function in football players. Scand J Med 56. Dickson JM, Weavers HM, Mitchell N, et al. The effects of dehydration on brain
Sci Sports. 2010;20 (Suppl. 3):148–160. volume—preliminary results. Int J Sports Med. 2005;26:481–485.
24. Ganio MS, Armstrong LE, Casa DJ, et al. Mild dehydration impairs cognitive perfor- 57. Duning T, Kloska S, Steinstrater O, et al. Dehydration confounds the assessment of
mance and mood of men. Brit J Nutr. 2011;106:1535–1543. brain atrophy. Neurology. 2005;64:548–550.
25. Armstrong LE, Ganio MS, Casa DJ, et al. Mild dehydration affects mood in healthy 58. Kempton MJ, Ettinger U, Schmechtig A, et al. Effects of acute dehydration on brain
young women. J Nutr. 2012;142:382–388. morphology in healthy humans. Hum Brain Mapp. 2009;30:291–298.
26. Streitburger D-P, Moeller HE, Tittgemeyer M, et al. Investigating structural brain 59. Kempton MJ, Ettinger U, Foster R, et al. Dehydration affects brain structure and
changes of dehydration using voxel-based morphometry. PLoS ONE. function in healthy adolescents. Human Brain Mapp. 2011;32:71–79.
2012;7:e44195. 60. Hocking C, Silberstein RB, Lau WM, et al. Evaluation of cognitive performance in
27. Edmonds CJ, Crombie R, Gardner MR. Subjective thirst moderates changes in the heat by functional brain imaging and psychometric testing. Comp Biochem
speed of responding associated with water consumption. Frontiers Hum Neurosci. Physiol. 2001;128:719–734.
2013;7:363. 61. Nestor SM, Rupsingh R, Borrie M, et al. Ventricular enlargement as a possible mea-
28. Pross N, Demazieres A, Girard N, et al. Influence of progressive fluid restriction on sure of Alzheimer’s disease progression validated using the Alzheimer’s disease
mood and physiological markers of dehydration in women. Brit J Nutr. neuroimaging initiative database. Brain. 2008;13:2443–2454.
2013;109:313–321. 62. Smith SM, Zhang YY, Jenkinson M, et al. Accurate, robust, and automated
29. Ely BR, Sollanek KJ, Cheuvront SN, et al. Hypohydration and acute thermal stress longitudinal and cross-sectional brain change analysis. Neuroimage. 2002;17:
affect mood state but not cognition or dynamic postural balance. Eur J App 479–489.
Physiol. 2013;113:1027–1034. 63. Frisoni GB, Fox NC, Jack CR, et al. The clinical use of structural MRI in Alzheimer
30. Lindseth PD, Lindseth GN, Petros TV, et al. Effects of hydration on cognitive func- disease. Nat Rev Neurol. 2010;6:67–77.
tion of pilots. Military Med. 2013;178:792–798. 64. El-Sharkawy AM, Sahota O, Maughan RJ, et al. The pathophysiology of fluid and
31. Shirreffs SM, Merson SJ, Fraser SM, et al. The effects of fluid restriction on hydra- electrolyte balance in the older adult surgical patient. Clin Nutr. 2014;33:6–13.
tion status and subjective feelings in man. Brit J Nutr. 2004;91:951–958. 65. Alison SP, Lobo DN. Fluid and electrolytes in the elderly. Curr Opin Clin Nutr
32. Pross N, Demazières A, Girard N, et al. Effects of changes in water intake on mood Metab Care. 2004;7:27–33.
of high and low drinkers. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e94754. 66. Lindeman RD, Tobin J, Shock NW. Longitudinal studies on the rate of decline in re-
33. Norman GR, Sloan JA, Wyrwich KW. Interpretation of changes in health-related nal function with age. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1985;33:278–285.
quality of life: the remarkable universality of half a standard deviation. Med Care. 67. Epstein M. Aging and the kidney. J Am Soc Nephrol. 1996;7:1106–1122.
2003;41:582–592. 68. Kenney WL, Chiu P. Influence of age on thirst and fluid intake. Med Sci Sports
34. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd edn. New Exer. 2001;33:1524–1532.
Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1988. 69. Phillips PA, Johnston CI, Gray L. Disturbed fluid and electrolyte homoeostasis fol-
35. Stachenfeld NS. Hormonal changes during menopause and the impact on fluid lowing dehydration in elderly people. Age Ageing. 1993;22:S26–S33.
regulation. Reprod Sci. 2014;21:555–561. 70. Raman A, Schoeller DA, Subar AF, et al. Water turnover in 458 American adults
36. Prashad DN, Fletcher PA, Cooper M. Exercise-induced changes in urinary water 40–79 yr of age. Am J Physiol–Renal Physiol. 2004;286:F394–F401.
and mineral output during the menstrual cycle. Brit J Sports Med. 1987;21:9–12. 71. Suhr JA, Hall J, Patterson SM, et al. The relation of hydration status to cognitive
37. Pechere-Bertschi A, Burnier M. Gonadal steroids, salt-sensitivity and renal function performance in healthy older adults. Int J Psychophysiol. 2004;53:121–125.
Curr Opin Nephrol Hypertens. 2007;16:16–21. 72. Suhr JA, Patterson SM, Austin AW, et al. The relation of hydration status to declar-
38. Tulin A. Menstrual syndrome comparison of athletes and non-athletes. Health ative memory and working memory in older adults. J Nutr Health Aging.
Med. 2012;6:3035–3042. 2010;14:840–843.
39. Emans J, Grace E, Woods ER. Adolescents compliance with the use of oral contra- 73. Renneboog B, Musch W, Vandemergel X, et al. Mild chronic hyponatremia is asso-
ceptives. J Am Med Assoc. 1987;257:3377–3381. ciated with falls, unsteadiness, and attention deficits. Am J Med. 2006;119:71.e1–
40. Rosenburg MJ, Waugh MS. Oral contraceptive discontinuation: a prospective eval- 71.e8.
uation of frequency and reasons. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 1998;179:577–582. 74. Ainslie PN, Campbell IT, Frayn KN, et al. Energy balance, metabolism, hydration,
41. Kleitman N, Titelbaum S, Feiveson P. The effect of body temperature on reaction and performance during strenuous hill walking: the effect of age. J Appl Physiol.
time. Am J Physiol. 1938;121:495–501. 2002;93:714–723.
42. Holland RL, Sayers JA, Keatinge WR, et al. Effects of raised body temperature on 75. D’Anci KE, Constant F, Rosenberg IH. Hydration and cognitive function in children.
reasoning, memory, and mood. J Appl Physiol. 1985;59:1823–1827. Nutr Rev. 2006;64:457–464.
43. Wright KP, Hull JT, Czeisler CA. Relationship between alertness, performance, and 76. Bar-David Y, Urkin J, Kozminsky E. The effect of voluntary dehydration on cogni-
body temperature in humans. Am J Physiol–Reg Integ Comp Physiol. tive functions of elementary school children. Acta Paediatr. 2005;94:1667–1673.
2002;283:R1370–R1377. 77. Benton D, Burgess N. The effect of the consumption of water on the memory and
44. Negoro H, Kanematsu A, Yoshimura K, et al. Chronobiology of micturition: puta- attention of children. Appetite. 2009;53:143–146.
tive role of the circadian clock. J Urol. 2013;190:843–849. 78. Edmonds CJ, Burford D. Should children drink more water?: the effects of drinking
45. Benton D, Young H, Jenkins K. The development of the predisposition to dehydra- water on cognition in children. Appetite. 2009;52:776–779.
tion questionnaire. Appetite. 2015;87:76–80. 79. Edmonds CJ, Jeffes B. Does having a drink help you think? 6–7-year-old children
46. Maughan RJ, Shirreffs SM. Hydration and performance during Ramadan. J Sports show improvements in cognitive performance from baseline to test after having a
Sci. 2012;30:S33–S41. drink of water. Appetite. 2009;53:469–472.
47. Grandjean AC, Reimers KJ, Bannick KE, et al. The effect of caffeinated, non-caffein- 80. Benton D. Dehydration influences mood and cognition: a plausible hypothesis?
ated, caloric and non-caloric beverages on hydration. J Am Coll Nutr. Nutrients. 2011;3:555–573.
2000;19:591–600. 81. Fadda R, Rapinett G, Grathwohl D, et al. Effects of drinking supplementary water
48. Dore C, Wheeler EF, Elneil H. Water balance and body-weight—studies in a tropi- at school on cognitive performance in children. Appetite. 2012;59:730–737.
cal climate. Ann Human Biol. 1975;2:25–33. 82. Dawkins L, Shahzad F-Z, Ahmed S, et al. Expectation of having consumed caffeine
49. Adolph EF. Physiology of Man in the Desert. New York: Interscience Press; 1947. can improve performance and mood. Appetite. 2011;57:597–600.
50. Perrier E, Vergne S, Klein A, et al. Hydration biomarkers in free-living adults with 83. Edmonds CJ, Crombie R, Ballieux H, et al. Water consumption, not expectancies
different levels of habitual fluid consumption. Brit J Nutr. 2012;109;1678–1687. about water consumption, affects cognitive performance in adults. Appetite.
51. Cutler R. Formation and absorption of cerebrospinal fluid in man. Brain. 2013;60:148–153.
1968;91:707–720. 84. Cohen S. After effects of stress on human performance during a heat acclimatiza-
52. Bull J. The volume of the cerebral ventricles. Neurology. 1961;11:1–9. tion regimen. Aviat Space Environ Med. 1983;54:709–713.
53. Gullans S, Verbalis J. Control of brain volume during hyperosmolar and hypo- 85. Kahneman D. Attention and Effort. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc; 1973.
osmolar conditions. Ann Rev Med. 1993;44:289–301. 86. Grandjean AC, Grandjean NR. Dehydration and cognitive performance. J Am Coll
54. Lee R, Abraham R, Quinn C. Dynamic physiologic changes in lumbar CSF volume Nutr. 2007;26:549S–554S.
quantitatively measured by three-dimensional fast spin-echo MRI. Spine. 87. Benton D. Neurodevelopment and neurodegeneration: are there critical stages
2001;26:1172–1178. for nutritional intervention? Nutr Rev. 2010;68:S6–S10.

96 Nutrition ReviewsV Vol. 73(S2):83–96


R

View publication stats

You might also like