You are on page 1of 11

ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2001, 61, 447–457

doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1610, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on

A quantitative assessment of the efficacy of an environmental


enrichment programme for giant pandas

RONALD R. SWAISGOOD*, ANGELA M. WHITE*, XIAOPING ZHOU†, HEMIN ZHANG†, GUIQUAN ZHANG†,
RONGPING WEI†, VALERIE J. HARE*, ERIN M. TEPPER* & DONALD G. LINDBURG*
*Center for Reproduction of Endangered Species, Zoological Society of San Diego
†China Research and Conservation Center for the Giant Panda, Wolong Nature Reserve, Sichuan

(Received 7 February 2000; initial acceptance 10 May 2000;


final acceptance 14 August 2000; MS. number: A8709R)

With the goal of enhancing psychological well-being, we developed an environmental enrichment


programme for eight subadult and six adult giant pandas, Ailuropoda melanoleuca, at the Wolong Breeding
Center in Sichuan, China. We used five different enrichment items, each selected because of the different
behavioural opportunities they promoted: (1) manipulable plastic objects; (2) a burlap sack full of straw;
(3) spruce branches; (4) a fruitcicle (apples frozen in a large ice block); and (5) a puzzle feeder. Each subject
received each of the five items on three occasions, for a total of 15 enrichment sessions over the course
of the 2.5-month study period. Each enrichment session was paired with a control session in which no
enrichment was present. Pandas spent significantly more time active, and displayed a greater variety of
object- and nonobject-directed behaviours when enrichment was present. We also found a statistically
significant reduction in the rate of and percentage of time engaged in the performance of stereotypic
behaviour and behaviours indicative of feeding anticipation. This effect was maintained even when
pandas were not interacting directly with an enrichment item, suggesting that enrichment had a
motivational carry-over effect in the aftermath of interactions with enrichment items. These results are
consistent with the ethological needs model of motivation in that opportunities to perform more natural
behaviour appeared to improve motivational indices of well-being. The pandas remained responsive to
enrichment across the 15 trials, providing no evidence for habituation or cross-habituation. Age proved
to be an important determinant of responsiveness; for example, adults, but not subadults, displayed a
significant preference for feeding enrichment. Future studies will assess whether these promising initial
results are maintained throughout the lives of captive giant pandas.
 2001 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour

The past decade has witnessed a remarkable rise in the abnormal behaviours (Shepherdson et al. 1998). In
importance and application of the concept of environ- addition, several studies have demonstrated a variety of
mental enrichment, both in the zoo community and positive effects of enrichment, including enhanced brain
among those striving to improve welfare in inten- function and learning ability, and reduced emotional
sively housed species used for human consumption and reactivity and indices of stress (Carlstead & Shepherdson
biomedical research (Shepherdson et al. 1998). Environ- 1994). Although many of these questions cannot be
mental enrichment is aptly defined as ‘an animal addressed in research with endangered species (e.g.
husbandry principle that seeks to enhance the quality of enrichment effects on neurological function), the
captive animal care by identifying and providing weight of evidence from these and other studies suggests
environmental stimuli necessary for optimal psychologi- a clear role for enrichment in maintaining endangered
cal and physiological well-being’ (Shepherdson 1998, species in captivity, promising to increase successful
page 1). Enriched environments can promote a greater mating and rearing of offspring, and promote the devel-
diversity of the species-typical behavioural repertoire opment of more behaviourally competent candidates for
and a concomitant reduction in stereotypic and other reintroduction to the wild.
Correspondence: R. R. Swaisgood, Center for Reproduction of
Captive environments only rarely approximate the
Endangered Species, Zoological Society of San Diego, P.O. Box 551, variety of stimuli and environmental contingencies
San Diego, CA 92112, U.S.A. (email: rswaisgood@sandiegozoo.org). present in nature. Captive environments in compari-
X. Zhou, H. Zhang, G. Zhang and R. Wei are at the Wolong Nature son with the wild are unchanging (lacking novelty),
Reserve, Wenchuan, Sichuan, People’s Republic of China, 623006. spatially limited, stimulus-poor (lacking complexity), and
0003–3472/01/020447+11 $35.00/0 447  2001 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour
448 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 61, 2

generally provide the inhabitant with little control over acquisition), but also by the behaviours that are
its environment (Carlstead 1996; Poole 1998). The latter employed to attain these endpoints (e.g. search, explore,
lack of behavioural contingency is especially evident in dig, extract, and so forth) or the information gathered
feeding practices wherein the animal is delivered its food through exploration. These models adequately explain
on a predictable schedule, and needs not perform any findings such as pigs wasting time and energy ‘rooting’
behaviour to garner access to this resource. Captive diets tokens prior to caching them in for a primary reinforcer
are also usually highly concentrated, processed foods that (i.e. food; Breland & Breland 1961) and a preference by
require little effort to consume (e.g. Lindburg 1998). The starlings to search for food hidden in holes under plastic
captive animal, therefore, is often left with a great deal of flaps rather than simply consume food made freely avail-
time, but few behavioural opportunities to occupy that able in a dish (Inglis & Ferguson 1986). Hughes & Duncan
time. One undesirable consequence of an unknown sub- go on to suggest that even if animals are well fed and
set of these factors is the development of stereotypic comfortably housed, they may suffer from a lack of
behaviours, which seem to take the place of more opportunity to express these appetitive behaviours.
‘natural’ behaviours seen among free-living animals Because it is the performance of these behaviours itself
(Hughes & Duncan 1988; Mason 1991b; Rushen et al. that is reinforcing, the inability to engage in these behav-
1993). Stereotypies are repetitive behaviours, invariant in iours may compromise the animal’s well-being, resulting
form, and have no apparent functional consequences in the performance of stereotypic behaviour in their
(Mason 1991b). The expression of stereotypies in captive stead.
animals is generally believed to reflect poor well- With these ideas in mind, we developed an enrichment
being, although it still remains unclear exactly what programme for giant pandas, Ailuropoda melanoeuca,
stereotypy performance tells us about the animal’s designed to (1) promote natural species-typical behav-
subjective experience (Mason 1991a, b). iours and reduce abnormal or stereotypic behaviours and
Researchers at zoological institutions have been (2) allow us to test some of the hypotheses pertaining to
addressing the issue of environmental enrichment with psychological well-being and enrichment. The giant
an increasingly powerful conceptual arsenal (e.g. panda is a dietary specialist, feeding almost exclusively
Hutchins et al. 1984; Carlstead & Shepherdson 1994; on bamboo (Schaller et al. 1985). Because the panda is
Carlstead 1998; Forthman 1998; Poole 1998), but, not particularly efficient at digesting this food source, it
unfortunately, the sophistication of these ideas has out- must consume approximately 14 kg/day to meet its daily
paced improvement in the application of empirical tests energetic requirements. As a consequence, the panda
of the efficacy of enrichment programmes and their spends on average 55% of its time consuming bamboo.
accompanying guiding conceptual framework. While Almost all the remaining time is spent resting, with other
major inroads have been made with studies of intensively activities occupying only 4% of its time. Because captive
housed species, zoo animal studies have been severely pandas’ diet of bamboo is supplemented with more con-
compromised by small sample size. For example, we centrated food, such as bread and milk, pandas spend less
searched the journal Zoo Biology for articles published time feeding (Mainka & Zhang 1994). As a dietary special-
during the 1990s pertaining to environmental enrich- ist with a relatively simple ecology (solitary, no complex
ment at zoological institutions, and found that the predator–prey interactions), many would predict that its
median sample size was four animals, with a maximum ethological needs are relatively simple and therefore are
of 11. less likely to develop stereotypic behaviours in captivity
Other deficiencies in studies of enrichment are not (Kreger et al. 1998; Mench 1998). Yet many pandas
necessarily limited to zoo species. For example, a rela- in captivity display a variety of stereotypies (R. R.
tively unexplored aspect of enrichment is the assessment Swaisgood, unpublished data), suggesting that their needs
of differential responsiveness to specific items as a func- are not being met in their present circumstances. Thus,
tion of the individual’s age, and subsequent efforts to an enrichment programme for pandas needs to provide
tailor enrichment programmes to specific age groups alternative ways for pandas to spend the time that they
(Mench 1998). Perhaps most important is the need to test would normally spend eating bamboo in the wild. A
whether enrichment effects behavioural change solely by common-sense solution might be to simply feed the
occupying the animal’s time, leaving less time available pandas only bamboo, but this is not a practical solution
to engage in abnormal or stereotypic behaviour, or because we cannot be certain that the bamboo we select
whether it has a long-lasting motivational effect that will meet all their nutritional needs.
carries over into the aftermath of an interaction with In the experiment described here, we tested the effects
enrichment. This idea is most effectively stated in the of five different enrichment items, each presented to the
‘ethological needs’ model of motivation advanced by animal on three separate occasions. The items provided
Hughes & Duncan (1988) and by its sister hypothesis, the included three objects (a burlap sack filled with straw,
information primacy model (Inglis et al. 1997). Briefly, moveable plastic objects and spruce branches) and two
while the types of behaviours addressed and evolutionary feeding enrichment items (fruit frozen in an ice block
explanations differ, both of these hypotheses advance the and a puzzle feeder). All of these objects, both feeding and
notion that animals have certain basic needs associated nonfeeding, were selected for their varying properties
with the appetitive phase of goal-directed activity. and consequent abilities to promote different sorts of
Thus, animals are reinforced not just by the functional behaviours. This enrichment programme is supplemental
consequences of their behaviour (e.g. food and mate to other ongoing efforts to enrich the lives of the pandas
SWAISGOOD ET AL.: ENRICHMENT FOR PANDAS 449

Table 1. Description of enrichment items used in the experiment


Enrichment item Description


Boomer Ball A hard plastic ball 25 cm in diameter.
Boomer Bobbin A hard plastic object 30 cm in height and shaped like a bobbin from a sewing
machine: wide and flat on the ends, tapering to a more narrow cylinder in the middle.
Burlap sack A large burlap sack (0.9×0.5 m) stuffed with rice straw.
Branches A pile of eight fresh-cut spruce branches, each approximately 1 m in length.
Fruitcicle A large block of ice approximately 20×30×10 cm with pieces of apple frozen on the
surface.
Puzzle feeder A large, hard segment of bamboo approximately 12 cm in diameter and 30–40 cm in
length. Both ends are naturally sealed, and a 5-cm2 hole is cut midway between the
endpoints and pieces of apple are placed inside.

at the Wolong Breeding Center. This experiment was Observations commenced after the item was placed
conducted in the spirit of a pilot study to assist in the in the enclosure and continued for 45 min. Five time
development of a long-term enrichment programme as a periods corresponding to peak activity levels (R. R.
standard husbandry practice at the this facility. Swaisgood, unpublished data) were selected for the study:
0700–0745, 0815–0900, 0900–0945, 1400–1445 and
1445–1500 hours. We employed a balanced design such
METHODS that an equal number of sessions was obtained for each
Subjects and Study Site enrichment item during each time period. All three trials
for an individual for a particular enrichment item were
Our study population consisted of 14 giant pandas: conducted during the same 45-min time period to mini-
three adult males, three adult females, three subadult mize variability associated with a particular time of day
females and five subadult males. Subadults ranged from and maximize our ability to detect any habituation effects
1.5 to 4.5 years of age, with reproductive maturity occur- across trials. To give the pandas more opportunities to
ring at 5.5 years of age (Schaller et al. 1985). During the interact with the enrichment items and encourage any
course of this study, all pandas were housed in enclosures possible habituation, enrichment items were left in the
consisting of an outdoor portion measuring approxi- pandas’ enclosures until approximately 1700 hours.
mately 98 m, and a 36 m indoor bedroom. Pandas Approximately 24–48 h prior to the enrichment presen-
are able to interact through cage bars with neighbouring tation, we observed the panda for a 45-min control
individuals in adjoining enclosures, but opportunities for session with no enrichment present. Each control session
physical contact is minimal. For details of housing and occurred at the same time of day as the enrichment trial
diet, refer to Swaisgood et al. (1999). None of the with which it was paired. Thus, each enrichment trial was
subjects had had any previous exposure to an enrichment paired with its own control trial 1–2 days prior to the
programme. enrichment trial to control for any temporal changes in
motivation across the study period, and at the same
time of day as the enrichment trial to control for any
Experimental Design and Procedures time-of-day effect.
Table 1 describes each of the six enrichment objects,
selected for the diverse array of sensory properties and
Dependent Variables
behavioural opportunities that they afford (e.g. move-
ability, manipulability, malleability, consumability, etc.). We set a timer to emit an audible signal at 1-min
The Boomer Ball and Bobbins were presented simul- intervals, and used instantaneous point sampling to esti-
taneously and analysed as a single enrichment treatment. mate the percentage of time spent in various activities.
The remaining four items were provided separately, for a We recorded one of the following mutually exclusive
total of five different enrichment treatments over the and exhaustive behaviours at the point sample. (1)
2.5-month study period. All objects were novel to the Stereotypic: any of the invariant, highly repetitive
subjects at the onset of the study. Each subject was given behaviours observed previously in giant pandas (R. R.
each of the five treatments on three different occasions Swaisgood, unpublished data). These include head toss,
for a total of 15 enrichment trials. We chose this design so pirouette, cage climb, tongue flick, pace, self-bite, paw
that we could assess the relative efficacy of several differ- suck and body sway. (2) Feeding Anticipation: operation-
ent enrichment items, ensure robust results, and provide ally defined as being alert, within one body length of the
some opportunity for habituation to specific items, as area where food is commonly delivered, and oriented in
well as cross-habituation to multiple exposures to an the direction from which food is brought. (3) Object-
array of items. By ‘habituation’ we mean any evidence of Directed: in contact with and actively interacting with an
decreased responsiveness to enrichment items resulting object. Further divided into three subcategories: (a)
from repeated presentations. Object Play/Manipulate: panda engages in any of the
450 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 61, 2

one–zero activities listed under this category below; (b) to reflect an array of common ‘normal’ behaviours that
Feed: panda consumes any food item derived from an could be construed as an index of psychological well-
enrichment item; and (c) Object Investigation: panda being. Although such an index is not a proven manifes-
sniffs or licks object. (4) Several other behaviours (e.g. tation of well-being (indeed, there are no undisputed
feed, locomote, rest) not relevant to analyses performed indices), environmental exploration, play and the expres-
for this paper. (5) Active: panda engaged in some active sion of species-typical behaviours are at least correlated
behaviour, that is, not sleeping, standing, or other- with other measures of well-being (Chamove & Anderson
wise resting (not mutually exclusive of the behaviours 1989; Broom & Johnson 1993; Poole 1998); therefore, the
identified above). We also recorded all-occurrences of question of causation aside, this measure may bear some
Stereotypic behaviour (except pacing) and Feeding relationship with well-being. In addition, if animals have
Anticipation, as defined above. Separate bouts of the need to express some specific behaviour patterns,
behaviour were separated by more than 5 s. then the performance of a greater variety of behaviours
Because of the vigour and complexity of interactions will increase the probability that one or more important
with enrichment items, we recorded the occurrence of behaviours are expressed (see Discussion).
specific behaviours directed to the enrichment item dur-
ing 1-min intervals using one–zero sampling. Those
behaviours subsumed under the general category Object Data Analysis
Play/Manipulation include the following. (1) Paw: panda
pushes, swats or rolls item on the ground using its Residual plots were inspected for deviations from
forepaws. (2) Manipulate: panda picks up, holds and normality and the homogeneity assumption was tested
rotates object in forepaws and/or hindpaws. (3) Bite: with Hartley’s Fmax test (Sokal & Rohlf 1998). Where
panda bites, mouths or chews object. (4) Roll: panda rolls necessary, we arcsine-transformed data based on percent-
side-to-side or somersaults on top of or while holding ages and log-transformed those based on rates. If, follow-
object in mouth or paws. (5) Head-Push: panda pushes ing transformation, the data still did not meet the
object with head, causing it to move. (6) Pursue: panda assumptions of parametric tests, we employed a non-
causes object to move and follows it for at least 2 m. (7) parametric counterpart. Parametric tests were comprised
Shake: panda holds object in mouth and shakes head of a repeated measures factorial analysis of variance
vigorously from side to side. (8) Pounce: panda rears up (ANOVA) with two within-subjects (experimental con-
and jumps forcefully on top of object. (9) Carry: panda dition, trial) and two between-subjects (sex, age class)
holds object in mouth or paws and carries or drags object factors. We excluded control trials from the analysis and
at least 2 m. We also recorded two behaviours under the employed the same statistical model (with enrichment
general category of Object Investigation using one–zero item replacing experimental condition) to examine the
sampling. (10) Investigate: pandas places nose within effects of different enrichment items. Type III ANOVA
5 cm of object and sniffs or licks the object. (11) Flehmen: was used because it provides a conservative adjustment
while Investigating an object, panda raises upper lip, for unequal sample sizes.
baring the teeth, and inhales deeply. A final category of
object interaction not included in the above categories is
RESULTS
(12) Scent Anoint: panda picks up object and rubs it onto
its pelage, often accompanied by movements reminiscent Description of Responses to Enrichment
of a cat washing its face.
We used the above one–zero behaviours, together with The pandas almost invariably approached the enrich-
several other behaviours from our standard ethogram, to ment item as soon as they became aware of its presence. A
calculate a Behavioural Diversity index. Each time a typical interaction began with an investigatory phase in
particular behaviour was observed at least once during an which the subject, sometimes displaying signs of caution,
observation session, a score of ‘1’ was recorded for that approached and sniffed and gently touched the object
behaviour. In addition to the 12 one–zero behaviours with the forepaw. A manipulatory phase generally fol-
defined above, we recorded the following 20 behaviours: lowed the initial investigation, in which the subject
five elements of locomotor play (playful run, jump, picked up the object and rotated the object repeatedly in
somersault, headstand and roll without an object), loco- its forepaws, examining it closely both olfactorily and
mote, feed on bamboo, climb, water splash, water roll, tactilely. Almost invariably the panda rolled on to its back
social play, nonplay social interaction, scent-mark, body and manipulated the object with all four paws simul-
rub, and chemosensory investigation of six different sub- taneously. This slow methodical examination typically
strates (concrete floor, dirt/grass, wall, cage bars, wood gave way to more vigorous play, particularly among
and large stone/vertical concrete structure). Thus, an subadults, including behaviours such as Bite, Roll, Shake,
individual could receive a maximum score of ‘32’ for Pursue, and Pounce. Although different enrichment items
an observation session. Other behaviours, such as promoted different kinds of behaviours, pandas did not
Stereotypic behaviours and Feeding Anticipation, were show any overt preferences for one enrichment item over
omitted from the Behavioural Diversity index because any other, as evidenced by our failure to obtain any
they are generally believed to indicate poor well-being significant effects of enrichment item on the percentage
(Mason 1991a, b; Shepherdson et al. 1998). Behaviours of time spent interacting with enrichment (data not
included in the Behavioural Diversity index were chosen shown; but see adult/subadult comparisons below).
SWAISGOOD ET AL.: ENRICHMENT FOR PANDAS 451

Percentage of time interacting with enrichment varied percentage of time spent in Stereotypic behaviour
from about 28% with the Ball/Bobbin to 36% with the (F1,395 =12.8, P=0.0004; Fig. 1h). Similarly, Feeding
Fruitcicle. These analyses also revealed that all five Anticipation was reduced even when pandas were not
enrichment items yielded similar levels of ‘improvement’ interacting directly with the enrichment item (F1,395 =8.2,
among all of our behavioural measures (e.g. stereotypy P=0.004; Fig. 1d).
performance).
Age and Sex Differences in Responsiveness to
Effects of Enrichment on Behaviour: Enrichment Enrichment
versus Control Trials
Although there were pronounced and highly signifi-
Our findings demonstrate that enrichment altered sev- cant main effects of age on most of the behavioural
eral aspects of the pandas’ behaviour in that the presence measures reported here (see Fig. 1a–h), these results are
of enrichment produced significant main effects for all of beyond the scope of this paper. Sex effects were minimal,
the behaviours measured. Pandas spent a significantly but likewise are unrelated to the topic at hand. Of interest
greater proportion of their time Active during enrichment for this paper are how sex and age interact with exper-
than control trials (F1,395 =25.7, P<0.0001; Fig. 1a). imental condition (enrichment versus control). Sex did
Pandas also displayed greater Behavioural Diversity when not interact significantly with experimental condition
enrichment was present (F1,395 =406.3, P<0.0001; Fig. 1b). (F1,395 <2.5, NS), strongly suggesting that males and
However, Object-Directed behaviour was only rarely females do not differ with regard to responsiveness to
observed during control trials (XSE=0.30.07 behav- enrichment. In contrast, age of the subject interacted
iours for subadults and zero for adults), probably because significantly with experimental condition for two
there were few objects that were moveable in the measures. The presence of enrichment appeared to bring
enclosures. Pandas were occasionally seen to play with about a more dramatic reduction in Feeding Anticipation
bamboo, bread, porridge pans and loose cement blocks. among subadults than adults (experimental con-
Since object play behaviours were difficult to perform dition*age class statistical interaction: F1,395 =4.5, P=0.04;
during control trials, we eliminated object play from our Fig. 1c), and was also more effective at promoting higher
diversity index and reanalysed the occurrence of the activity levels among subadults (F1,395 =14.2, P=0.0002;
remaining behavioural variables to determine whether Fig. 1a). We also examined the percentage of time spent
the presence of enrichment affected the expression of interacting with enrichment (including Feeding) with
behaviours not immediately directed towards enrich- regard to feeding versus nonfeeding enrichment. The
ment items. The resulting analysis still yielded signifi- results of this test revealed a significant interaction
cant results, with pandas displaying a mean of 5.5 between age class and enrichment type (F1,55 =5.1,
0.2 nonobject-directed behaviours during control trials P=0.03). Separate analyses for each age class showed that
and 7.80.3 during enrichment trials (F1,395 =59.5, adults (22.24.4% time spent with feeding versus 9.7
P<0.0001). 1.5% for nonfeeding enrichment; F1,26 =6.3, P=0.02), but
In addition to enhancing the occurrence of ‘desirable’ not subadults (40.74.0% versus 44.24.2%; F1,38 =0.1,
behaviours, the enrichment programme was also associ- P=0.79), displayed a significant preference for feeding
ated with a significant reduction in ‘undesirable’ behav- enrichment. However, these differences did not arise
iours. For example, Feeding Anticipation occurred from a tendency of adults to spend more time Feeding per
significantly less frequently in the presence of enrich- se: adults spent a mean of 9.7% of their time Feeding, in
ment than when enrichment was absent (F1,395 =21.7, comparison with 12.9% for subadults.
P<0.0001; Fig. 1c). This same effect was also evident for
the percentage of time engaged in Feeding Anticipation
Evidence for Habituation across Trials
(F1,395 =31.6, P<0.0001; data not shown). In addition,
pandas displayed Stereotypic behaviour less frequently We were unable to detect any evidence that the pandas
(F1,395 =7.6, P=0.006; Fig. 1e) and spent a smaller pro- habituated to these enrichment items across the 15 trials,
portion of time engaged in Stereotypic behaviour despite the fact that all items were left in the enclosures
(F1,395 =17.9, P<0.0001; Fig. 1g) when enrichment was until the end of the day. Although trial produced a
present. Stereotypic behaviours that contributed to rate significant main effect for two behaviours, the pattern of
estimates were predominantly composed of head toss, change was inconsistent with habituation. Indeed, the
pirouette, cage climb and tongue flick, whereas those percentage of time spent in Stereotypic behaviour actu-
contributing to the percentage of time estimates primar- ally decreased significantly across trials (F1,154 =3.7,
ily consisted of pace, self-bite, paw suck and cage climb. P=0.03: 4.51.4, 2.10.9 and 1.80.7% for trials 1, 2
To test whether enrichment produced a motivational and 3, respectively). Conversely, the percentage of time
carry-over effect in the aftermath of enrichment inter- spent in Object-Directed behaviour with enrichment
actions during an enrichment trial, we calculated esti- items was highest in trial 3 (F1,154 =3.7, P=0.03:
mates of Stereotypic behaviour when the panda was not 29.92.9, 26.52.9 and 35.93.4%). Behavioural
interacting with the enrichment item. Analysis of these Diversity also increased by the end of the study, albeit not
data revealed that even when pandas were not quite significantly (F1,154 =2.9, P=0.06: 11.70.6, 11.2
enrichment-directed, they displayed a significant reduc- 0.6, and 12.90.7). Trial effects did not approach signifi-
tion in both the rate (F1,395 =3.9, P=0.04; Fig. 1f) and cance for any other behavioural variable (F1,154 <2.3, NS).
452 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 61, 2

100 18
90 (a) 16 (b)

Behavioural diversity
80 14
70
%Time active

12
60
10
50
8
40
30 6
20 4
10 2
0 0
Adult Subadult Adult Subadult
7 7
Feeding anticipation bouts/h

Feeding anticipation bouts/h


(c) (d)
6 6

5 5

4 4

3 3

2 2

1 1

0 0
Adult Subadult Adult Subadult
30 30
(e) (f)
25 25
Stereotypic bouts/h

Stereotypic bouts/h

20 20

15 15

10 10

5 5

0 0
Adult Subadult Adult Subadult
16 16
(g) (h)
14 14
%Time stereotypic

%Time stereotypic

12 12
10 10
8 8
6 6
4 4
2 2
0 0
Adult Subadult Adult Subadult
Figure 1. Behavioural effects of enrichment as a function of panda age class. (a) Percentage of time Active; (b) Behavioural Diversity; (c) rate
of Feeding Anticipation (bouts/h); (d) rate of Feeding Anticipation when not interacting with enrichment item; (e) rate of Stereotypic
behaviour; (f) rate of Stereotypic behaviour when not interacting with enrichment item; (g) percentage of time performing Stereotypic
behaviour; (h) percentage of time performing Stereotypic behaviour when not interacting with enrichment. Values are X+SE. : Control;
: enrichment.

DISCUSSION met, at least during the admittedly brief 2.5-month


period during which this study was conducted. When
This investigation of our first enrichment efforts with the enrichment was present, pandas spent a greater pro-
giant pandas at the Wolong Breeding Center achieved a portion of their time Active, and displayed a greater
measure of success in that all of our identified goals were variety of Object-Directed and other behaviours than
SWAISGOOD ET AL.: ENRICHMENT FOR PANDAS 453

during control observations in which enrichment was interpretation of these apparent behavioural needs, argu-
absent. This addressed our concerns regarding excessive ing that it is not the performance of these appetitive
inactivity and probable depauperate behavioural diversity behaviours that is important, but rather the information
among captive pandas. Enrichment also promoted a con- gathered through the exploratory process associated with
comitant reduction in the rate of and proportion of time locating and extracting hidden or dispersed food
spent engaged in Stereotypic behaviour and Feeding resources (Inglis & Ferguson 1986; Inglis et al. 1997).
Anticipation. Alleviating Feeding Anticipation is of These researchers do not relate their model (the infor-
special importance because this activity is closely associ- mation primacy model) to suffering or stereotypy, yet this
ated with the development of stereotypies (Falk 1977; bridge is easily built, as implied in Mench’s (1998) linkage
Hughes & Duncan 1988; Mason 1991b). It is during of information primacy with enrichment needs (see also
this time that appetitive behaviour is activated, wherein Shepherdson et al. 1993). Following Toates (1983),
the animal is motivated to find and acquire food. Mench also reduces the emphasis of these models on
Depauperate environments apparently do not provide feeding behaviour, expanding them to include the con-
appropriate opportunities for animals to channel this struction of a cognitive map of other relevant environ-
motivation, and subsequently many animals develop mental dimensions. It is in this more general spirit with
rigid, entrenched stereotypic routines (e.g. adjunctive which we approached our ‘test’ of the ethological needs
behaviour) while waiting for food delivery. Reduction model.
of Feeding Anticipation may, therefore, remove the If enrichment is to have an effect on stereotypic behav-
motivational context in which many stereotypies arise. iour via the reduction of ethological needs, then it must
Finally, age played a prominent role in governing respon- reduce the occurrence of stereotypies even when the
siveness to enrichment, an effect which centred primarily animal is not actively interacting with the enrichment
around subadults’ penchant for play and adults’ interest item. Thus, overall rates or time allocated to stereotypy in
in feeding. Subadults showed no overall preference the presence and absence of enrichment is insufficient
between the feeding (fruitcicle, puzzle feeder) and non- to test the ethological needs model. To adequately test
feeding enrichment items (burlap sack, ball/bobbin, enrichment efficacy, therefore, it is necessary to factor out
branches), whereas adults significantly preferred feeding the time spent interacting with enrichment and analyse
enrichment. how the animal spends the remainder in the aftermath of
An interesting, but ultimately unanswerable, question an enrichment interaction. Taking this idea to its logical
arising from this research is how these behavioural effects conclusion, we need furthermore to demonstrate that
of enrichment relate to the subjective experience of the enrichment carried out throughout an animal’s life can
animal. Do these measures of reduced abnormal behav- meet its ethological needs such that the development of
iour, increased behavioural diversity and so forth indicate stereotypies is prevented in the first place.
improved ‘psychological well-being’ of the animal? The latter ambitious goal is beyond the scope of this
Although the driving force behind our enrichment pro- paper, but our results clearly show that enrichment
gramme was guided by the nebulous concept of well- affected the pandas’ motivation to engage in Stereotypic
being, this thorny issue raises a host of problems behaviour and Feeding Anticipation in the aftermath of
regarding its measurability and interpretation (Mason enrichment interactions. After factoring out the time
1991a, b; Dawkins 1990). In light of these limitations, it is spent with enrichment, pandas still performed stereo-
perhaps best for our present purposes to limit our evalu- typies at a lower rate and spent less time engaged in
ation to that which is quantifiable, and maintain the stereotypic behaviour. Feeding Anticipation was con-
hope that these effects are manifestations of improved comitantly reduced, suggesting that feeding motivation
well-being and reflect meaningful enhancement of living may play a role in stereotypy performance (cf. Falk 1977).
conditions for the pandas. Also consistent with the ethological needs model is the
observation that enrichment promoted a greater diversity
Motivational Effects of Enrichment: Support for of nonobject-directed activities, again suggesting a carry-
over effect that not only reduced abnormal behaviour,
the Ethological Needs Model?
but also encouraged a more natural behavioural reper-
The ethological needs model of motivation posits that toire. (Note, however, that while this analysis may test
animals may suffer if they are unable to carry out normal some predictions of the ethological needs model, it does
behaviour patterns (Hughes & Duncan 1988). When the not address the more practical question of long-term
environment affords few opportunities to perform certain effects on motivation.)
natural behaviours, particularly those associated with Qualitative observations also support these conclu-
feeding, animals often display these activities even when sions. On several occasions we observed that episodic
they yield no functional consequence. For example, interactions with enrichment items appeared to break the
chickens conditioned to stand on a platform for 15 s will ‘momentum’ toward stereotypic behaviour. For example,
ground-scratch while waiting, even though this behav- behaviours such as head toss, pirouette, cage climb and
iour is not part of the conditioned response (Breland & pace seemed to ‘build up’ after 2–4 min of fast-paced,
Breland 1961). If behavioural opportunities are severely apparently ‘anxious’ locomotion, often associated with
constrained (i.e. in an impoverished environment), many Feeding Anticipation. Sometimes pandas appeared to
animals develop stereotypies that replace these natural break away from this somewhat predictable trajectory to
behaviours. Inglis and his colleagues offer a different investigate, consume, or play with enrichment items.
454 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 61, 2

Following these interactions, the panda’s demeanor often and indeed any study aimed at quickly arresting negative
appeared to be calmer for several minutes, as evidenced consequences of suboptimal captive conditions, it is
by a slow-paced gait and reduced Feeding Anticipation. neither practical nor ethical to engage in a long-term
This disruptive effect may explain why pandas displayed series of investigations aimed at understanding the effect
less stereotypy even when they were not interacting with of selective deprivation of specific behaviour patterns.
the enrichment item.
How specific are these results to the ethological needs Mimicking Behavioural Opportunities Present in
model of motivation? A test of the ethological needs
the Wild
model requires that any motivational effects be achieved
solely via the performance of specific behaviours, not The limited space available in the confines of captive
their functional consequences. While it might be argued environments makes it difficult to recreate the animal’s
that the two feeding enrichment items achieved their natural environment in miniature. Wild-living animals
effects via the functional consequence of food acqui- encounter a temporally and spatially variable environ-
sition, the same cannot be said for the three nonfeeding ment, such that they are exposed to nearly constant
enrichment items. These items provided no secondary change at some level: trees, bushes, rocks, terrain, bodies
reinforcer, only the reward associated with the oppor- of water, conspecifics, other forms of wildlife. All of
tunity to explore, manipulate and play. That is, all they these aspects of the natural environment have varying
provided was the opportunity to perform certain behav- properties that afford infinite behavioural opportunities.
iours, strongly implicating behavioural performance as Certainly, a great deal of the ‘enrichment’ in a wild
the causal variable in reducing motivation to engage in animal’s life stems from the sheer variety to which it is
stereotypic behaviour. It might be argued, however, that exposed. Thus, practitioners of enrichment must selec-
sensory feedback and the opportunity to investigate these tively choose items that afford opportunities to express
objects played a causal role (i.e. information primacy those behaviours that occur naturally in the wild. This
model), rather than the performance of the behaviour notion is based in part on the hypothesis that animals
itself. Without highly invasive experimental interven- have ethological needs wherein they are motivated to
tion, such as deafferentation, these alternatives remain perform certain behaviours even when their most basic
empirically indistinguishable. Regardless, whether these needs of food and comfort are met (Hughes & Duncan
effects arise from performance of these behaviours or the 1988). In the case of the giant panda, it is difficult to
information obtained from them is of little practical determine just what its ethological needs might be
consequence as long as stereotypic motivation is miti- because so little is known about its behavioural repertoire
gated and well-being promoted. Moreover, appealing to in the wild. As solitary animals that only encounter
some version of the ethological needs/information pri- conspecifics occasionally (Schaller et al. 1985), it seems
macy model seems to offer a plausible motivational likely that most needs for social interaction are met at a
explanation for our results. captive facility such as Wolong. Each panda has access
It should be noted that our experimental design did not through cage bars to other pandas, and occasional full
allow us to determine which behaviours were important contact when placed together during mating intro-
to perform to reduce these putative ethological needs. We ductions. Opportunities for chemical communication,
agree with Veasey et al. (1996, page 17), skeptics of the pandas’ primary means of communication throughout
ethological needs model, that it is not necessary for an most of the year, might also constitute a suitable etho-
animal to perform all of the behaviours occurring among logical need, but the management strategy at Wolong
wild-living individuals, but also concur that the ‘expres- (Swaisgood et al. 2000) probably meets this need. The
sion of some behaviours is essential for adequate welfare’. clear majority of a wild panda’s activity budget is filled
Indeed, it may not be the expression of any particular with feeding on bamboo (55%, Schaller et al. 1985), while
behaviour that is important, and any number of alterna- at the Wolong Breeding Center pandas spend only 25% of
tive behaviours may provide sufficient substitution for their time feeding (Mainka & Zhang 1994). In addition,
those performed in the wild (see below). The fact that pandas spend a greater proportion of their time inactive
all five enrichment items effected similar degrees of in captivity (61% versus 41%).
enhancement in plausible measures of well-being, yet Our enrichment programme addressed some of these
each produced a distinct set of behaviours, implies that perceived problems regarding activity budget and prob-
not one, but some unknown combination of several able depauperate behavioural diversity. Because increas-
behaviours promoted by these enrichment items consti- ing time allocated to bamboo consumption was not a
tute important ethological needs. Of course, our research practical solution (they are fed bamboo ad libitum), we
would be strengthened if we could determine precisely sought to occupy their time in alternative activities. The
which behaviours are important, but the fact that enrich- two feeding enrichment items, in particular, were pro-
ment did lead to a substantive increase in behavioural vided in the hope of stimulating feeding-related activi-
diversity at least maximizes the probability that one or ties. Although the behavioural tasks required to extract
more behavioural needs were satisfied. On a more practi- food from these items deviates from those associated with
cal level, even in species more amenable to experimental bamboo consumption, the opportunity to employ more
manipulation, it is extremely difficult to deprive individ- appetitive feeding behaviours may partially fulfill this
uals of opportunities to perform specific behaviours, and putative ethological need. The pandas’ foraging strategy
measure the effects of such deprivation. In our research, requires both a great deal of processing (e.g. mastication)
SWAISGOOD ET AL.: ENRICHMENT FOR PANDAS 455

and handling effort (stripping leaves, peeling culms), but diminishment of its efficacy. Given that we maintain
does not require much searching. The puzzle feeder sufficient variability in the objects we provision, we are
requires significant handling time, whereas the fruitcicle encouraged that the programme will maintain its practi-
promotes the processing phase of feeding in that the cal utility (cf. Weld & Erwin 1990; Crockett 1998).
animal must chew the ice block vigorously to extract food Indeed, we have continued and expanded this pro-
items. Although these foraging tasks differ in the exact gramme since its inception; although monitored through
form of behaviours associated with feeding on bamboo, it less labour-intensive and quantifiable means, we are
might be argued that they promoted analog behav- encouraged that the pandas have continued to interact
iours and thereby satisfied some ethological need (sensu with these enrichment items for nearly a year. We will
Veasey et al. 1996). The remaining enrichment items did follow up this programme with detailed quantitative
not address any specific known behaviours that pandas assessments at periodic intervals throughout the lives of
perform in the wild, but may none the less provide these pandas, thereby enabling the long-term effective-
behavioural opportunities present in the wild. In ness and developmental consequences of the programme
addition, the handling of objects incorporated some of to be monitored. It is our goal to develop an enrichment
the same behavioural elements observed when feeding on programme/husbandry protocol that ultimately alters the
bamboo. For example, the branches in particular pro- developmental trajectory such that abnormal behaviours
moted behaviour resembling bamboo feeding, includ- never develop.
ing stripping of the needles with the forepaws and Regardless of whether this lofty goal is attained, we
the bark with the teeth. While wild pandas would hope that enrichment will have a beneficial effect on the
not encounter burlap sacks, balls or bobbins, they cer- breeding success at the facility. As discussed by Carlstead
tainly encounter other manipulable objects that they & Shepherdson (1994), environmental enrichment can
investigate, manipulate or play with occasionally. promote captive propagation in a variety of ways. For
Practitioners of enrichment programmes have long example, pairing for mating purposes or the birth of an
debated the relative merits of ‘artificial’ versus ‘natural’ offspring can represent a significant challenge to an
enrichment (e.g. Lindburg 1998; Poole 1998). Of course, animal kept in a sterile, unchanging environment. That
the ‘naturalness’ of enrichment items varies along a captive giant pandas may suffer from such deficits is
continuum from only those biotic and abiotic commodi- suggested in the fear-induced maternal abandonment
ties occurring in the natural environment of the species observed in some panda mothers (Zhang et al. 2000) and
to natural items that do not occur in the species’ range to the inappropriate behavioural responses that plague
natural objects modified by humans to completely many attempts at mating introduction (Lindburg et al.
human-made objects. Initially, our philosophical stance 1998; R. R. Swaisgood, unpublished data). Enrichment
fell in the ‘natural’ camp, but practicality of implemen- can provide the environmental complexity and novelty
tation, efficacy of enrichment items at producing the required to develop the ability to cope with such changes,
desired effect, and constraints operating in certain captive as suggested by the reduced emotional responsiveness
environments have swayed us towards a balanced observed in animals kept in enriched environments
approach including both ‘artificial’ and ‘natural’ enrich- (Renner & Rosenzweig 1986, 1987). Enriched environ-
ment as needs dictate (cf. Veasey et al. 1996). However, all ments have also proved effective at alleviating stress
of the enrichment items provisioned during this study, (Carlstead & Shepherdson 1994; Carlstead 1996), and
regardless of their location on the naturalness con- presumably may mitigate its negative consequences for
tinuum, were equal in their overall efficacy with regard to health and reproduction. Promotion of physical exercise,
occupying time, promoting Activity and Behavioural as suggested by the vigour with which many pandas
Diversity, and reducing Stereotypic behaviour and interacted with the enrichment items, may also yield
Feeding Anticipation. If it is the performance of these beneficial effects on health. Reintroduction of captive
behaviours that is important, then the goal of enrich- giant pandas to the wild is an important long-term goal of
ment programmes should be to promote such behaviour China’s giant panda breeding programme (MacKinnon et
using whatever means, natural or artificial, that prove al. 1989), and thus the improved behavioural competence
most provocative of the desired outcome. and postrelease survival associated with enriched en-
vironments for some species (Sheperdson 1994; Miller et
al. 1998) would be an additional welcome consequence.
Implications for Husbandry and Captive Breeding
Finally, the enrichment programme described in the
One limitation of the present study was its short-term present study is not occurring in a vacuum, but rather is
nature (2.5 months) and rather limited opportunity for accompanied by other efforts, such as the creation of
habituation to take place. However, although several large naturalistic outdoor enclosures, changes in diet and
studies have found substantive habituation even after a feeding protocols, and other measures to enrich the
single trial (reviews in Shepherdson et al. 1998), we found pandas’ lives in captivity.
no evidence of it across three trials. We also found no
evidence of cross-habituation across the 15 trials; that is, Acknowledgments
exposure to one type of enrichment did not reduce
responsiveness to others. This initial finding provides We thank the keepers at the Wolong Breeding Center for
hope that the continued application of this enrichment their assistance in carrying out this programme and
programme (in expanded form) will not lead to the China’s State Forestry Administration for its support. This
456 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 61, 2

research was funded by the Zoological Society of San of the International Symposium on the Protection of the Giant
Diego. We also wish to thank Charles and Susan Lang for Panda, Ailuropoda melanoleuca (Ed. by A. Zhang & G. He),
their donation in support of this project. Ronald R. pp. 67–71. Chengdu, China: Sichuan Publishing House of Science
Swaisgood’s position was generously funded by a do- and Technology.
MacKinnon, J., Bi, F., Qiu, M., Fan, C., Wang, H., Yuan, S., Tian, A.
nation from J. Dallas Clark. The research presented here
& Li, J. 1989. National Conservation Management Plan for the Giant
was evaluated and approved by the Animal Behavior Panda and Its Habitat. Hong Kong: China Alliance Press.
Society’s Animal Care Committee on 6 September 2000. Mainka, S. A. & Zhang, H. 1994. Daily activity of captive giant
pandas (Ailuropoda melanoleuca) at the Wolong Reserve. Zoo
Biology, 113, 13–20.
References Mason, G. J. 1991a. Stereotypies and suffering. Behavioral Processes,
Breland, K. & Breland, M. 1961. The misbehavior of organisms. 25, 103–115.
American Journal of Psychology, 16, 681–684. Mason, G. J. 1991b. Stereotypies: a critical review. Animal Behaviour,
Broom, D. M. & Johnson, K. G. 1993. Stress and Animal Welfare. 41, 1015–1037.
London: Chapman & Hall. Mench, J. A. 1998. Environmental enrichment and the importance
Carlstead, K. 1996. Effects of captivity on the behavior of wild of exploratory behavior. In: Second Nature: Environmental
mammals. In: Wild Mammals in Captivity (Ed. by D. G. Kleiman, Enrichment for Captive Animals (Ed. by D. J. Shepherdson, J. D.
M. E. Allen, K. V. Thompson & S. Lumpkin), pp. 317–333. Mellen & M. Hutchins), pp. 30–46. Washington, D.C.:
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Smithsonian Institution Press.
Miller, B., Biggins, D., Vargas, A., Hutchins, M., Hanebury, L.,
Carlstead, K. 1998. Determining the causes of sterotypic behaviors
in zoo carnivores: toward appropriate enrichment strategies. In: Godbey, S., Anderson, S., Wemmer, C. & Oldemeier, J. 1998.
The captive environment and reintroduction: the black-footed
Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals (Ed.
by D. J. Shepherdson, J. D. Mellen & M. Hutchins), pp. 172–183. ferret as a case study with comments on other taxa. In: Second
Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press. Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals (Ed. by
D. J. Shepherdson, J. D. Mellen & M. Hutchins), pp. 97–112.
Carlstead, K. & Shepherdson, D. J. 1994. Effects of environmental
Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press.
enrichment on reproduction. Zoo Biology, 13, 447–458.
Poole, T. B. 1998. Meeting a mammal’s psychological needs:
Chamove, A. S. & Anderson, J. R. 1989. Examining environmental
basic principles. In: Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for
enrichment. In: Psychological Well-being of Primates (Ed. by E.
Captive Animals (Ed. by D. J. Shepherdson, J. D. Mellen & M.
Segal), pp. 183–202. Philadelphia: Noyes.
Hutchins), pp. 83–94. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution
Crockett, C. M. 1998. Psychological well-being of captive non-
Press.
human primates: lessons from laboratory studies. In: Second
Renner, M. J. & Rosenzweig, M. R. 1986. Object interactions in
Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals (Ed. by D. J.
juvenile rats (Rattus norvegicus): effects of different experiential
Shepherdson, J. D. Mellen & M. Hutchins), pp. 129–152.
histories. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100, 229–236.
Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press.
Renner, M. J. & Rosenzweig, M. R. 1987. Enriched and Impoverished
Dawkins, M. S. 1990. From animal’s point of view: motivation, Environments: Effects on Brain and Behavior. New York: Springer-
fitness and animal welfare. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 1–61.
Verlag.
Falk, J. L. 1977. The origin and functions of adjunctive behavior. Rushen, J., Lawrence, A. B. & Terlouw, E. M. C. 1993. The
Animal Learning and Behavior, 4, 325–335. motivational basis of stereotypies. In: Stereotypic Animal Behaviour
Forthman, D. L. 1998. Toward optimal care for confined ungulates. (Ed. by A. Lawrence & J. Rushen), pp. 41–64. Wallingford: CAB
In: Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Ani- International.
mals (Ed. by D. J. Shepherdson, J. D. Mellen & M. Hutchins), Schaller, G. B., Hu, J., Pan, W. & Zhu, J. 1985. The Giant Pandas of
pp. 236–261. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press. Wolong. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Hughes, B. O. & Duncan, I. J. H. 1988. The notion of ethological Shepherdson, D. J. 1994. The role of environmental enrichment in
‘need’, models of motivation and animal welfare. Animal the captive breeding and reintroduction of endangered species.
Behaviour, 36, 1696–1707. In: Creative Conservation: Interactive Management of Wild and
Hutchins, M., Hancocks, D. & Crockett, C. 1984. Naturalistic Captive Animals (Ed. by G. Mace, P. J. S. Olney & A. Feistner),
solutions to behavioral problems of captive animals. Zoologische pp. 167–177. London: Chapman & Hall.
Garten, 54, 28–42. Shepherdson, D. J. 1998. Tracing the path of environmental
Inglis, I. R. & Ferguson, N. J. K. 1986. Starlings search for food enrichment in zoos. In: Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment
rather than eat freely-available, identical food. Animal Behaviour, for Captive Animals (Ed. by D. Shepherdson, J. Mellen & M.
34, 614–616. Hutchins), pp. 1–12. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution
Inglis, I. R., Forkman, B. & Lazarus, J. 1997. Free or earned food? A Press.
review and a fuzzy model of contrafreeloading. Animal Behaviour, Shepherdson, D. J., Carlstead, K., Mellen, J. D. & Seidensticker, J.
53, 1171–1191. 1993. The influence of food presentation on the behavior of small
Kreger, M. D., Hutchins, M. & Fascione, N. 1998. Context, ethics, cats in confined environments. Zoo Biology, 12, 203–216.
and environmental enrichment for zoos and aquariums. In: Second Shepherdson, D. J., Mellen, J. D. & Hutchins, M. (Eds) 1998.
Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals (Ed. by Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for Captive Animals.
D. J. Shepherdson, J. D. Mellen & M. Hutchins), pp. 59–82. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press.
Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press. Sokal, R. & Rohlf, F. 1998. Biometry. New York: W. H. Freeman.
Lindburg, D. G. 1998. Enrichment of captive mammals through Swaisgood, R. R., Lindburg, D. G. & Zhou, X. 1999. Giant pandas
provisioning. In: Second Nature: Environmental Enrichment for discriminate individual differences in conspecific scent. Animal
Captive Animals (Ed. by D. J. Shepherdson, J. D. Mellen & Behaviour, 57, 1045–1053.
M. Hutchins), pp. 262–276. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Swaisgood, R. R., Lindburg, D. G., Zhou, X. & Owen, M. A. 2000.
Institution Press. The effects of sex, reproductive condition and context on discrimi-
Lindburg, D. G., Huang, X. M. & Huang, S. Q. 1998. Reproductive nation of conspecific odours by giant pandas. Animal Behaviour,
performance of giant panda males in Chinese zoos. In: Proceedings 60, 227–237.
SWAISGOOD ET AL.: ENRICHMENT FOR PANDAS 457

Toates, F. M. 1983. Exploration as a motivational and learning Weld, K. & Erwin, J. 1990. Provision of manipulable objects to
system: a cognitive incentive view. In: Exploration in Animals and cynomolgus macaques promotes species-typical behavior.
Humans (Ed. by J. Archer & L. I. A. Birke), pp. 55–71. Berkshire: American Journal of Primatology, 20, 243.
Van Nostrand Reinhold. Zhang, G. Q., Swaisgood, R. R., Wei, R. P., Zhang, H. M., Han,
Veasey, J. S., Waran, N. K. & Young, R. J. 1996. On comparing the H. Y., Li, D. S., Wu, L. F., White, A. M. & Lindburg, D. G. 2000.
behaviour of zoo housed animals with wild conspecifics as a A method for encouraging maternal care in the giant panda. Zoo
welfare indicator. Animal Welfare, 5, 13–24. Biology, 19, 53–63.

You might also like