You are on page 1of 8

Neuroscience 205 (2012) 73–80

A SIMPLE METHOD FOR DETECTION OF FOOD FORAGING


BEHAVIOR IN THE RAT: INVOLVEMENT OF NMDA AND DOPAMINE
RECEPTORS IN THE BEHAVIOR
F. LI,a1 W. Y. CAO,a1 M. B. LI,a Y. XU,a J. W. ZHANG,a shortage, inclement weather, predation dangers, and re-
J. Y. ZHANG,a X. G. LUO,a R. P. DAI,b X. F. ZHOUc AND production, as it allows animals to control over food entries
C. Q. LIa* (Sherry, 1990). Indeed, hoarding anticipates future need
a
Department of Anatomy and Neurobiology, XiangYa School of Med- and allows animals to remain in its sheltered nest while
icine, Central South University, Tongzipo Road 172, Changsha feeding. However, in a natural environment it might be
410013, Hunan, China
b
disadvantageous for animals to pursue a larger food item if
Department of Anesthesia, the second XiangYa Hospital of Central the benefits do not outweigh the costs (Deacon, 2006).
South University, Ren-Min Road 86, Changsha, Hunan, China
c
Many studies suggest that foraging behavior involves
Department of Human Physiology and Center for Neuroscience,
higher cognitive functions such as effort-based decision
Flinders University, GPO Box 2100, Adelaide SA5001, Australia
making. Organisms making optimal decision require the
evaluation of both the costs and benefits of potential
Abstract—Food foraging behavior involves food removing, choices (Day et al., 2010). A question that needs further
hoarding, and competitive preying upon other animals. It is study is how the nature or amount of training on the cost-
also associated with high cognitive functions such as invest- benefit contingencies preoperatively affects choices. In
ing effort into decision making, but no established laboratory
model is available to detect the behaviors. In the present
most of operant-based tasks, pretraining is usually used to
study, we have developed a novel laboratory rodent model to ensure stable and consistent performance. Until now, tech-
detect competitive, non-competitive, and no-hurdle foraging niques available for investigating food acquisition in verte-
conditions that can mimic the corresponding environment in brates are technically challenging. When studying sponta-
nature. We found that normal rats consistently foraged the neous alternate in food foraging, the artificial nature of
food from a food container to the field and spread food into manipulation of handling the rats to the starting position is
piles in the open field. There was no difference between male
an obvious problem (Whishaw et al., 1995). Previous
and female rats in the amount of foraged food in the compet-
itive, non-competitive, and no-hurdle food foraging tests. The models simply involve measuring the amount of hoarded
amount of foraged food was consistent each day for five food transported from an external source into the home
consecutive days with a slight increase in following days. cage (Lacroix et al., 1998; Walton et al., 2009), and this
There was no significant difference in the amount of food is unsatisfactory because the rats in their studies are
foraged in the presence or absence of bedding materials. A trained before the test, a confounding factor that distorts
dramatic decrease of foraged food was found in the rats after the perspective of the true nature of the animal behavior.
administration of haloperidol (dopamine D2 receptor antago-
Mott et al. employ a T-maze task (Mott et al., 2009),
nist) in the competitive, non-competitive, and no-hurdle food
foraging tests. Treatment with MK-801 (non-competitive whereas Farrar et al. have used a concurrent fixed ratio
N-methy-D-aspartate receptor antagonist) reduced the for- 5 (FR5)/chow-feeding procedure (Farrar et al., 2007) to
aged food in the competitive food foraging test, but did not assess effort-related choice behaviors. A small labora-
affect the foraged food in the non-competitive and no-hurdle tory animal model of food foraging without prior training
food foraging tests. Our study provides a simple but consis- or manipulation is necessary to mimic the natural animal
tent analogue of natural food foraging behavior. Our study behavior. To this end, we have established a novel
also suggests that dopaminergic and glutaminergic systems
are differentially involved in the food foraging behaviors.
laboratory model including competitive, non-competi-
© 2012 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. tive, and no-hurdle food foraging paradigms. The es-
sence of the model in the competitive and non-compet-
Key words: food foraging behavior, MK-801, haloperidol, rat. itive food foraging paradigms is that food is offered in a
wire mesh container (the cage lid) on a 16-cm-high
cage, with or without a same gender residing rat, which
Food foraging behavior is a species-specific behavior that
forces test rats to make an effort energetically or psy-
involves searching for food as well as transporting, hoard-
chologically to overcome the hurdle and compete with
ing, and competitively snatching food away from other
residing rats to forage food.
animals. Hoarding, which is especially prevalent among
Recently, there has been a surge of interest in the
rodents, is an important adaptive strategy in times of food
complexity of animal cognition such as future planning and
1
These authors contributed equally to this work. decision making. There has been a growing interest in the
*Corresponding author. Tel: ⫹86-731-82650421; fax: ⫹86-731-82650426. neurobiological basis underlying decision making, with em-
E-mail address: lichangqi2003@163.com (C. Q. Li).
Abbreviations: DA, dopamine; Hal, Haloperidol; NMDA, N-methy-D- phasis placed on the different neuromodulators mediating
aspartate. these types of executive functions. The behavioral and
0306-4522/12 $36.00 © 2012 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.12.057

73
74 F. Li et al. / Neuroscience 205 (2012) 73–80

neural effects of effort-related decision making are strongly recorded with a video camera mounted directly above the testing
modulated by the availability of dopamine. Mesolimbic do- apparatus. For the time course, all rats were weighed and handled
pamine (DA) is a main component of the exertion of effort accordingly and then left undisturbed in their home cages for the
duration of the experiment.
in food-seeking behavior and effort-related decision mak-
ing (Salamone, 2009). DA depletion in the nucleus accum- Food foraging test. The test was performed after 12 h of
bens results in a reduced willingness to expend effort to food deprivation in an open field (Fig. 1). Rats have free access to
obtain rewards representing a strong reduced “wanting” in water during testing in the open field. On the days of testing, the
test male and female rats were removed from their home cages in
animals. In food-seeking behaviors, rats with impaired DA
which the rats had been residing for at least 1 week and placed on
transmission instead select the way that require less effort the open field, and the rats were allowed to become accustomed
(Farrar et al., 2007; Mott et al., 2009; Worden et al., 2009). to the open field before testing. After habituating for 2 h in the field,
Haloperidol (Hal) is an antidopaminergic drug (selective one of following sessions was then conducted. The test rat was
D2 receptor antagonist) frequently used to treat psychosis allowed to navigate to the cage and foraged food freely from 7:00
(Seeman, 2006). Other transmitters are also involved in PM to 7:00 AM of the next day. The amount of food moved to the

effort-related processes. For example, the N-methy-D-as- open field and the amount left in the food containers were sepa-
rately calculated at 7:00 AM the following day. Different rats were
partate (NMDA) receptor antagonist ketamine plays a key
used for different procedures so that no animal experienced more
role in the decision making when monkeys perform a visual than one session, and the rats were randomly assigned to proce-
search task (Shen et al., 2010). In the present study, we dures as described below.
administered MK-801 (a highly selective noncompetitive Competitive food foraging test. For testing competitive
NMDA receptor antagonist) or haloperidol 30 min before food foraging activity, one rat was placed in a small plastic
the food foraging test was performed by rats to validate our home cage (30 cm⫻18 cm⫻16 cm) with a metal wire cover
model. (Fig. 1) with 250 g of standard food pellets on the wire mesh,
and a test rat which had been deprived of food for 12 h was
placed in the open field. The rat residing in the small home cage
EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES and the test rat came from different litters and did not have any
interactions with each other before the test. The test rat was
Animals allowed to navigate to the cage and foraged food freely from
The study was carried out on male and female Sprague–Dawley 7:00 PM–7:00 AM the next day.
rats (200 –250 g) obtained from Central South University Animal Non-competitive food foraging test. For testing the non-
Services (Changsha, China). All experimental rats were housed competitive food foraging activity, the test rat was placed in the
one per cage in the wire-topped plastic home base with removable open field and allowed to navigate to the cage without any residing
wire cover (30 cm⫻18 cm⫻16 cm) and ad libitum access to water. rat in the cage, and the rat foraged food freely from 7:00 PM–7:00
AM next day. The other parts of procedures were conducted in the
The experimental protocol was approved by the Animal Care and
Use Committee of Central South University and conformed to the same manner as in the test session of the competitive food
National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Lab- foraging paradigm.
oratory Animals. All efforts were made to minimize the number of No-hurdle food foraging test. We also designed a no-hurdle
rats used and their suffering. For each treatment group in this foraging test. During the no-hurdle foraging test, 250 g of standard
series of experiments, 8 –13 animals were used. food pellets were placed in transparent bright Plexiglas tray (30
cm⫻18 cm⫻2 cm; Changsha Zhonghui Plexiglass product Tech-
Behavioral test nology Corp, LTD, Changsha, China) instead of a small wire-
topped plastic home cage, and the tray was placed within the open
The test was performed under a sound-attenuating environment in field close to one side of the wall. The other parts of the procedure
an open field, which was constructed with black wood with dimen- were conducted in the same manner as in the test sessions of the
sions of 150 cm⫻150 cm⫻50 cm (Fig. 1). All behavioral tests were competitive and non-competitive food foraging paradigms.

Fig. 1. Photographs display the competitive food foraging paradigm from 7:00 PM to 7:00 AM the next day under 12-h fasting conditions. A small
wire-topped plastic home cage (30 cm⫻18 cm⫻16 cm) with 250 g standard rodent food pellets on the wire mesh was placed in the open field. (A) A
photograph of the open field with a control rat before the test. (B) A photograph of the open field with the control rat shown in (A) after the test. The
control rats spread food into piles in the field.
F. Li et al. / Neuroscience 205 (2012) 73–80 75

In these experiments, the amount of food carried to the open RESULTS


field was considered the amount of food foraged per night.
A simple method to evaluate food foraging behavior
The effects of several variables on the competitive in rats
food foraging behavior
We found that naive rats without training or manipulation
Male rat was used to evaluate the effects of variables on the foraged food pellets from the food container to the field and
competitive food foraging behavior. All the experiments were car-
spread food into piles in the open field in three procedures
ried out under 12-h fasting conditions except the experiment that
compared food foraging behavior under fasting and non-fasting (Fig. 1B). Few rats scattered food everywhere in the open
conditions. field. The amounts of food foraged by male rats in the com-
petitive, non-competitive, and no-hurdle food foraging tests
The effect of fasting versus non-fasting on the food foraging
behavior. One group of testing rats were deprived of food for were 151.54⫾23.07 g, 174.77⫾18.97 g, and 156.43⫾20.39
12 h (fasting during 7:00 AM–7:00 PM) but given free access to g, respectively. The amount of food foraged by female rats in
water before the experiment. Other groups of testing rats were the competitive, non-competitive, and no-hurdle food forag-
given free access to food during the experiment. The competitive ing tests were 157.75⫾27.40 g, 180.38⫾16.62 g, and
food foraging test was conducted as described in section Com-
182.48⫾21.11 g, respectively. Two-way ANOVA indicated
petitive food foraging test.
no effects of test type (F(2,48)⫽0.583, P⫽0.562) and animal
The effect of a clean floor versus floor with bedding materials gender (F(1,48)⫽0.516, P⫽0.477) or their interaction (F(2,48)⫽
on the competitive food foraging behavior. The effect of clean 0.146, P⫽0.865) (n⫽8 per group) (Fig. 2).
floor versus floor with bedding materials on the competitive food
foraging behavior was investigated. Bedding material was spread
over the entire field at 5:00 PM. The competitive food foraging test The effect of fasting on the competitive food
was conducted as described in section Competitive food foraging foraging behavior
test.
The activities in animals with fasting were increased in the
The procedures were conducted for five consecutive days in competitive food foraging behavior test compared with
a rat. After the completion of the competitive food foraging test
non-fasting rats (160.56⫾30.23 g vs. 53.11⫾15.0 g). Stu-
at 7:00 AM each day, the rats were put back to their home cages
until the next session at 7:00 PM of the same day (fasting from 7:00 dent’s test indicated that the activities were significantly
AM to 7:00 PM, testing from 7:00 PM to 7:00 AM the next day), and increased after fasting in relative to no fasting (t⫽⫺5.701,
the tests were repeated for five consecutive days. P⬍0.0001, n⫽13) (Fig. 3A).
Effects of a higher degree of difficulty on the competitive and
non-competitive food foraging tests in rats. As showed in Fig. The effect of bedding in the floor of open field on
4A, B, the food container (16 cm in height) was placed upon a 50 competitive food foraging behavior
cm⫻35 cm⫻20 cm (length⫻width⫻height) plastic cage. It means
that the test rat which is in the open field had to climb total of We compared the amounts of food foraged by rats in the
36-cm-high barrier to get the food. The competitive and non- field with and without bedding. As shown in Fig. 3B, there
competitive food foraging test was conducted as described in was no significant difference in the two groups with or
section Competitive food foraging test and in section Non-com-
without bedding (162.38⫾31.03 g vs.155.64⫾45.37 g)
petitive food foraging test, respectively.
(t⫽⫺0.270, P⫽0.790, n⫽12).
The effects of pharmacological manipulations on the
food foraging test
One group of male rats were injected subcutaneously in a volume
of 1.0 ml/kg body weight with Hal(0.1mg/kg s.c.; Sigma Chemical,
St. Louis, MO, USA; it was dissolved in 10% glacial acetic acid,
brought to volume with saline, and the pH was adjusted to 6.5 with
NaOH). The dose of haloperidol used in the present study was
based upon previously performed research (Bardgett et al., 2009;
Farrar et al., 2007). A second group of rats were injected with
MK-801 (0.2 mg/kg s.c., Sigma) which was dissolved in sterile
saline and injected based on previous report with some modifica-
tion (Mishima et al., 2002). Control rats received vehicle (saline)
injections by the same route. Competitive, non-competitive, and
no-hurdle food foraging behavior were studied 30 min after injec-
tion of haloperidol or MK-801 or vehicle.

Statistical analysis
The results are presented as mean⫾SEM. Statistical differences
were determined using one way or two-way ANOVA followed by Fig. 2. The amount of foraged food was calculated in three kinds of
post hoc Dunnett testing or Tukey post hoc multiple comparison food foraging tests under 12-h fasting conditions. Two-way ANOVA
test where appropriate. Unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test was indicated no effects of both test type (F(2,48)⫽0.583, P⫽0.562) and
used if only two groups were applied. P⬍0.05 was considered animal gender (F(1,48)⫽0.516, P⫽0.477) and their interactions (F(2,48)⫽
statistically significant. 0.146, P⫽0.865) (n⫽8 per group).
76 F. Li et al. / Neuroscience 205 (2012) 73–80

Fig. 3. Effects of variables on competitive food foraging behavior. (A) The effect of fasting on competitive food foraging behavior. Student’s test
indicated that foraging activity was significantly increased after fasting (t⫽⫺5.701, P⬍0.0001, n⫽13). (B) The effect of floor condition (with or without
bedding) on competitive food foraging behavior. Student’s test indicated that there was no significant difference in the amount of foraged food with
or without bedding materials on the floor (t⫽⫺0.270, P⫽0.790, n⫽12). (C) The amount of foraged are consistent but slightly increased when the same
procedure was repeated for five consecutive day on competitive food foraging behavior (fasting during 7:00 AM–7:00 PM; test during 7:00 PM–7:00 AM).
There was no significant difference in foraged food in the different time points with one-way ANOVA (F(4,48)⫽1.739, P⫽0.164, n⫽8).

The effect of repeating the same procedure for five vs. 97.03⫾16.21 g, t⫽4.373, P⫽0.001, n⫽8) (Fig. 4C). In
consecutive days on the competitive food foraging addition, when the difficulty was added the amount of
behavior foraged food in the competitive food foraging test was less
than in the non-competitive food foraging test(F(3,31)⫽
The amount of foraged food was very consistent when
11.983, P⬍0.001, n⫽8) (Fig. 4C).
the same procedure was repeated for five consecutive
days (fasting during 7:00 AM–7:00 PM; testing during 7:00
The effect of the DA receptor antagonist haloperidol
PM–7:00 AM). Although there was a slight increase in the
and the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 injection on
amount of food foraged with increased number of tests,
the three kinds of food foraging behavior
there was no significant difference with one-way ANOVA
(F(4,48)⫽1.739, P⫽0.164, n⫽8) (Fig. 3C). Control rats with vehicle injection and experimental rats
with Hal injection or MK-801 injection were tested for the
The effect of degree of difficulty on the competitive three kinds of food foraging test procedures. The amount
and non-competitive food foraging tests in rats of foraged food of vehicle rats in competitive, non-compet-
We found that naive rats foraged mostly food pellets from itive, and no-hurdle food foraging tests was 165.92⫾24.73 g,
the food container to the field into piles in the open field 174.72⫾18.67 g, and 156.61⫾20.02, respectively. After
after adding the difficulty in the non-competitive food for- pretreatment with haloperidol, the amount of foraged food
aging tests (Fig. 4A), but only a little food pellets was in competitive, non-competitive, and no-hurdle food forag-
spread into the open field after adding the difficulty in the ing tests was 96.43⫾21.14 g, 74.41⫾14.63 g, and 58.55⫾
competitive food foraging tests (Fig. 4B). Quantitative anal- 16.24 g, respectively, being significantly decreased (P⬍0.01,
ysis revealed that after adding the difficulty, the foraged n⫽8) compared with that of vehicle groups (Fig. 5). Pretreat-
food per night was slightly decreased in non-competitive ment of MK-801 significantly decreased the amount of for-
food foraging test (170.85⫾26.8 g vs. 152.05⫾22.6 g, aged food in competitive food foraging tests in comparison
t⫽1.424, P⫽0.176, n⫽8). In the competitive food foraging with the vehicle treatment (71.32⫾7.1 g vs. 165.92⫾24.73
tests, the foraged food per night was significantly de- g, P⬍0.01, n⫽8). There were no significant differences
creased after adding the level of difficulty (153.81⫾22.8 g between the MK-801 treatment group and the vehicle
F. Li et al. / Neuroscience 205 (2012) 73–80 77

Fig. 4. Effects of different degrees of difficulty on the competitive and non-competitive food foraging tests in rats under 12-h fasting conditions.
Photographs display the non-competitive and competitive food foraging paradigm from 7:00 PM to 7:00 AM the next day after adding the difficulty.
(A) A photograph of the open field with a test rat in the non-competitive food foraging tests after one night foraging freely. The photograph
showed that all of food pellets from the food container were spread into piles in the open field. (B) A photograph of the open field with a test
rat in the competitive food foraging tests after one night foraging freely. The photograph showed that only a small food pellets were spread into
the open field, considerable food pellets were remained in the food container. (C) Effects of different levels of difficulty on the competitive and
non-competitive food foraging tests in rats. A dramatic decrease was found in the foraged food (g/12 h) of rats after adding the degree of difficulty
in the competitive food foraging behavior. There was no significant difference of foraged food in the non-competitive food foraging tests between
the conventional and higher degree of difficulty. The amount of foraged food was fewer in the competitive food foraging test when the difficulty
was added than in the non-competitive food foraging test in the two levels of difficulty. ** P⬍0.01 represents statistically significant differences
compared with conventional degree of difficulty group in the competitive food foraging behavior. ## P⬍0.01 represents statistically significant
differences compared with non-competitive food foraging tests groups (n⫽8 per group). For interpretation of the references to color in this figure
legend, the reader is referred to the Web version of this article.

treatment group in non-competitive and no-hurdle food investing (Rushworth et al., 2004; Walton et al., 2009).
foraging tests (P⬎0.10) (Fig. 5). The amount of food eaten In fact, it is an important model of a highly integrative
per night in vehicle, haloperidol, and MK-801 treatment method for exploring numerous questions through dif-
rats were 18.63⫾3.9 g, 23.11⫾8.1 g, and 17.47⫾6.0 g, ferent disciplines (Pravosudov and Smulders, 2010).
respectively, one-way ANOVA indicated that haloperidol Many studies explore the influence of a variety of factors
and MK-801 treatment did not induced any effect on eaten on the foraging behavior: risk sensitivity, presence of
activity (F(2,29)⫽2.278, P⬎0.05, n⫽10). competitors, and predation risk, and so forth (Arcis and
Desor, 2003). However, these studies have used ani-
DISCUSSION mals that are required manipulated or trained, which
The food foraging paradigm has played a major role in may distort the interpretation of data or generate condi-
the conceptual framework of behavioral science. An tioning effects. To study the foraging behaviors, it is
increasing body of evidence indicates that food foraging necessary to establish a method that can imitate and
activity is associated with decision making and effort reflect the nature of the food foraging behavior so that a
78 F. Li et al. / Neuroscience 205 (2012) 73–80

mechanisms. As the foraging behavior is associated with


the process of decision making and effort investing (Rush-
worth et al., 2004; Walton et al., 2009), this model may be
important in exploring the neurobiological mechanisms of
high integrative brain functions.
In addition, we have studied that the effects of several
variables on competitive food foraging behavior using the
established animal model. Notably, we found that the
amount of foraged food in fasted animals was significantly
increased. It is a fact that the motivation for foraging and
hoarding more food tends to be activated when the rats are
experiencing a certain extent of hunger. The increase in
the foraged food after fasting indicates the state of satiety
is important for foraging behavior. In the present study
Fig. 5. Effects of haloperidol or MK-801 on three kinds of food forag- using the open field, which was a black floor constructed of
ing tests in rats under 12 h fasting conditions. A significant decrease
wood, we found that there was no significant difference in
was found in foraged food (g/12 h) of rats after administration of
haloperidol in the three kinds of food foraging behavior. A reduced the amount of foraged food with or without bedding mate-
foraged food (g/12 h) in rats after administration of MK-801 was only rials on the floor. We had thought that foraging behavior
found in the competitive food foraging behavior. ** P⬍0.01, ## P⬍0.01 would be affected if the open field was covered with bed-
(n⫽8 per group). Vel: vehicle, Hal: haloperidol. ding material. Some studies demonstrated that environ-
quantitative method can be directly used to examine ment structure can affect the foraging behavior. For exam-
what neural mechanisms are implicated in the behavior. ple, increased illumination can reduce gerbil’s foraging
The present study aimed to establish the method and activity in areas without cover (Arcis and Desor, 2003).
characterize it with different paradigms. More interestingly, when a rat was tested using same
Firstly, we successfully created a simple and practical procedure for five consecutive days, the amount of foraged
laboratory model that can quantitatively detect the natural food is very consistent with only slight increase in the
food foraging behavior without any artificial intervention or competitive food foraging tests. This result suggests that
training. In our experiment, we tested competitive, non- our method is very robust and reproducible. The data also
competitive, or no-hurdle food foraging conditions where suggest that the prior experience does not significantly
different animals were used respectively. These para- affect subsequent foraging behavior. This result also sug-
digms represent three different situations: invest a larger gests that our method can be used to investigate interven-
psychological effort plus a physical effort to gain reward, ing factors or neurobiological mechanisms in the same rats
invest a physical effort to obtain food, or exert no signifi- with internal controls by repetitive testing.
cant physical effort to get food. The method works well and There was no difference in the amount of foraged food
allows animals to forage food freely within the field. In among three kinds of food foraging tests, which means that
nature, animals constantly face the risk of encountering under certain level of difficulty animals would like to carry
predators. They usually need to compete with other ani- all the food outsides to its territory. To determine whether
mals and overcome hurdles when foraging food. There- the foraging behavior was affected after exceeding con-
fore, we designed the competitive food foraging activity ventional level of difficulty, we designed another test to
and non-competitive food foraging activity tests in which increase the level of difficulty during which they had to put
animals must conquer the hurdle of a 16-cm-high plastic more effort to climb a 36-cm-high hurdle in the competitive
cage to get the food. We also designed the no-hurdle food and non-competitive conditions. After adding the level of
foraging tests that allows an animal free access to food difficulty, we found that foraged food was significantly de-
held in a bright tray with 2-cm high without physical hur- creased in the competitive food foraging tests. However,
dles. During the competitive food foraging test, the rat the higher degree of difficulty reduced food foraging activ-
which was resident in the small home cage would shriek ity, but it did not reach significance in the non-competitive
and threat the invader (test) rat that wanted to forage food food foraging tests. These data suggest that the variables
on the wire mesh. In the presence of a hungry conspecific, such as the physical hurdle and the presence of a com-
the resident rat is at risk on losing its food. Even without petitor may interact with each other. When facing a com-
bodily touching and fighting, this response implied a com- petitor and a high level of difficulty rats foraged less food.
petition or conflict. We designed the food foraging test In the absence of a competitor, this level of the difficulty did
during the night without any human interruption to mimic not affect the activity. This result suggests that rats had to
the natural food hunting behavior of rats. This is a big invest a larger psychological effort plus a more physical
advantage as our method avoids many confounds of arti- effort to gain reward.
ficial laboratory training and humanly handling. The over- Extensive studies have demonstrated that cost/benefit
training of a task leads to behavior autonomy and develops decisions regarding relative effort are mediated by distrib-
a habit without voluntary control by the mouse (Killcross uted dopaminergic and glutaminergic neural circuits (Flo-
and Coutureau, 2003). Human handling can generate con- resco et al., 2008). We hypothesize that the dopaminergic
ditioning effect, which may complicate neurobiological and glutaminergic systems might be involved in the forag-
F. Li et al. / Neuroscience 205 (2012) 73–80 79

ing behaviors. We undertook to test the role of neurotrans- food foraging behavior test indicating that the role of MK-
mitter receptors (NMDA or DA receptor) in food foraging 801 on foraging behavior was a specific effect. It is rea-
behavior with the established model by assessing the abil- sonable to argue that the observed decrease in the amount
ity of their antagonists in this behavior. Dopamine released of foraged food could have been due to blockade of NMDA
in the nucleus accumbens is thought to contribute to the receptor, which resulted in the disruption of competitive
decision to exert effort to seek reward (Nicola, 2010). The activity. Our study suggests that NMDA receptor may be
amount of consumed food was not significantly different involved in the regulating the social competitive activity.
after injection of haloperidol, a dopamine D2 receptor an- We found that the quantity of consumed food was not
tagonist, indicating that the D2 receptor has no significant significantly different between the injection group and con-
effect on food consumption in our model, which is consis- trol group, indicating that NMDA had no effect on appetitive
tent with previous studies (McFarland and Ettenberg., behavior.
1998) that suggest that dopamine transmission may not be Our report describes an experimental method that al-
necessary for the demonstration of normal food-seeking lows an animal to freely forage food in the field with or
behavior. However, we found a clear impact of dopaminer- without a physical or psychological barrier to overcome,
gic modulation of the foraging behavior. The injection of allowing an investigation of behaviors that can mimic food
haloperidol caused an obvious reduction in food foraged foraging-related behaviors in nature. Based on the well-
activity during the competitive, non-competitive, and no- validated animal paradigm, this study assessed the effects
hurdle foraging behavior test compared with the control of several variables on the established competitive food
group, suggesting that the D2 receptor is involved in food foraging test. Most importantly, we elucidated the role
foraging behavior. Many studies indicated that the disrup- played by two neurotransmitter systems in the behavior
tion of dopaminergic transmission decreases effort-based using our established models. In doing so, we provided
decision making (Floresco et al., 2008). Bardgett et al.’s unambiguous and strong evidence to support the notion
results showed that rats were more likely to choose the that a dramatic decrease of foraged food was found in the
smaller reward arm after haloperidol treatment (Bardgett et rats after administration of haloperidol in the competitive,
al., 2009), suggesting that D2 receptor is involved in deci- non-competitive, and no-hurdle food foraging tests. Treat-
sion making. We found that the foraging behavior of three ment with MK-801 reduced the foraged food in the com-
different paradigms tested in the present studies was in- petitive food foraging test, but did not affect the foraged
hibited after haloperidol treatment. It suggests that there food in the non-competitive and no-hurdle food foraging
may be subtle difference in neurobiological mechanisms tests. Our data support the notion that competitive food
between decision making and foraging behavior. In our foraging activity may be associated with the function of
model, the animals would not only make effort to obtain both dopamine and excitatory amino acids. We believe
food but also overcome hurdles for ongoing transfer of the that this method could be of great utility in studies striving
food into the field, which may associate with the reinforce- to determine which brain regions are involved in cues of
ment process of a goal-directed behavior. Soto et al.’s food foraging behavior associated with reinforcement pro-
study demonstrated that food’s reinforcing effectiveness cess and effort-related decision making.
was lowest in dopamine D2 receptor knockout mice indi- In light of the complexity of reinforcement process and
cated by the behavioral economic analysis of decreases in effort-related decision making, the intrinsic mechanism of this
consumption with increases in price (Soto et al., 2011). state should be further studied. Using the simple and robust
It should be emphasized that we cannot completely model described, this study opens future avenues for analy-
rule out the possibility of the effect of haloperidol on loco- sis of this complex animal behavior by manipulation of factors
motor activities, which may affect the foraging behavior. such as levels of neurochemical substrates. Such advances
However, we choose the low dose of haloperidol injection will be crucial for refining and developing future models of
(0.1 mg/kg) that does not affect the locomotor functions as human psychopathology. Future work will aim to elucidate
tested in the previous studies (Salamone et al., 2009). whether other neurotransmitters, such as 5-hydroxytrypta-
Salamone et al. found that only higher doses of haloperidol mine or opioids, are biologically relevant in foraging behavior,
(0.4 mg/kg) produced motor impairments that interfere with as well as the involvement of specific brain regions.
feeding behavior (Salamone et al., 1990). The duration of
the foraging test may reduce the possibility that the forag- Acknowledgments—This study was supported by National Natural
ing reduction was due to haloperidol-induced locomotor Science Foundation of China (30971533 to C.Q.L.).
dysfunction, as the catalepsy effect induced by the halo-
peridol treatment (4 mg/kg, i.p.) was attenuated after 3-h
injection (Adams et al., 1997). In our experiment, the du-
REFERENCES
ration of 12 h provides enough time for the food foraging
behavior. Nonetheless, our results indicate that the de- Adams MR, Brandon EP, Chartoff EH, Idzerda RL, Dorsa DM, McK-
night GS (1997) Loss of haloperidol induced gene expression and
crease in the altered amount of foraged food after halo-
catalepsy in protein kinase A-deficient mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U
peridol treatment is most likely due to the specific effects of S A 94:12157–12161.
the drugs on foraging behavior. Arcis V, Desor D (2003) Influence of environment structure and food
However, after MK-801 injection (0.2mg/kg s.c.), the availability on the foraging behavior of the laboratory rat. Behav
foraging activity in rats was only reduced in the competitive Processes 60:191–198.
80 F. Li et al. / Neuroscience 205 (2012) 73–80

Bardgett ME, Depenbrock M, Downs N, Points M, Green L (2009) Pravosudov VV, Smulders TV (2010) Integrating ecology, psychology
Dopamine modulates effort-based decision making in rats. Behav and neurobiology within a food-hoarding paradigm. Philos Trans R
Neurosci 123:242–251. Soc Lond B Biol Sci 365:859 – 867.
Day JJ, Jones JL, Wightman RM, Carelli RM (2010) Phasic nucleus Rushworth MF, Walton ME, Kennerley SW, Bannerman DM (2004)
accumbens dopamine release encodes effort- and delay-related Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex. Trends Cogn
costs. Biol Psychiatry 68:306 –309. Sci 8:410 – 417.
Deacon RM (2006) Assessing hoarding in mice. Nat Protoc 1: Salamone JD (2009) Dopamine, effort, and decision making: theoret-
2828 –2830. ical comment on Bardgett et al. (2009). Behav Neurosci 123:
Farrar AM, Pereira M, Velasco F, Hockemeyer J, Müller CE, Salamone 463– 467.
JD (2007) Adenosine A(2A) receptor antagonism reverses the Salamone JD, Farrar AM, Font L, Patel V, Schlar DE, Nunes EJ,
effects of dopamine receptor antagonism on instrumental output Collins LE, Sager TN (2009) Differential actions of adenosine A1
and effort-related choice in the rat: implications for studies of and A2A antagonists on the effort-related effects of dopamine D2
psychomotor slowing. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 191:579 –586. antagonism. Behav Brain Res 201:216 –222.
Floresco SB, Tse MT, Ghods-Sharifi S (2008) Dopaminergic and Salamone JD, Zigmond MJ, Stricker EM (1990) Characterization of the
glutamatergic regulation of effort- and delay-based decision mak-
impaired feeding behavior in rats given haloperidol or dopamine-
ing. Neuropsychopharmacology 33:1966 –1979.
depleting brain lesions. Neuroscience 39:17–24.
Killcross S, Coutureau E (2003) Coordination of actions and habits in
Seeman P (2006) Targeting the dopamine D2 receptor in schizophre-
the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cereb Cortex 13:400 – 408.
nia. Expert Opin Ther Targets 10:515–531.
Lacroix L, Broersen LM, Weiner I, Feldon J (1998) The effects of
Shen K, Kalwarowsky S, Clarence W, Brunamonti E, Paré M (2010)
excitotoxic lesion of the medial prefrontal cortex on latent inhibition,
Beneficial effects of the NMDA antagonist ketamine on decision
prepulse inhibition, food hoarding, elevated plus maze, active
processes in visual search. J Neurosci 30:9947–9953.
avoidance and locomotor activity in the rat. Neuroscience 84:
Sherry DF (1990) Food hoarding in animals. Science 250:1602–1603.
431– 442.
McFarland K, Ettenberg A (1998) Haloperidol does not affect motiva- Soto PL, Grandy DK, Hursh SR, Katz JL (2011) Behavioral economics
tional processes in an operant runway model of food-seeking of food reinforcement and the effects of prefeeding, extinction, and
behavior. Behav Neurosci 112:630 – 635. eticlopride in dopamine D2 receptor mutant mice. Psychopharma-
Mishima K, Fujii M, Aoo N, Yoshikawa T, Fukue Y, Honda Y, Egashira cology (Berl) 215:775–784.
N, Iwasaki K, Shoyama Y, Fujiwara M (2002) The pharmacological Walton ME, Groves J, Jennings KA, Croxson PL, Sharp T, Rushworth
characterization of attentional processes using a two-lever choice MF (2009) Comparing the role of the anterior cingulate cortex and
reaction time task in rats. Biol Pharm Bull 25:1570 –1576. 6-hydroxydopamine nucleus accumbens lesions on operant effort-
Mott AM, Nunes EJ, Collins LE, Port RG, Sink KS, Hockemeyer J, based decision making. Eur J Neurosci 29:1678 –1691.
Müller CE, Salamone JD (2009) The adenosine A2A antagonist Whishaw IQ, Coles BL, Bellerive CH (1995) Food carrying: a new
MSX-3 reverses the effects of the dopamine antagonist haloperidol method for naturalistic studies of spontaneous and forced alterna-
on effort-related decision making in a T-maze cost/benefit proce- tion. J Neurosci Methods 61:139 –143.
dure. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 204:103–112. Worden LT, Shahriari M, Farrar AM, Sink KS, Hockemeyer J, Müller
Nicola SM (2010) The flexible approach hypothesis: unification of CE, Salamone JD (2009) The adenosine A2A antagonist MSX-3
effort and cue-responding hypotheses for the role of nucleus ac- reverses the effort-related effects of dopamine blockade: differen-
cumbens dopamine in the activation of reward-seeking behavior. tial interaction with D1 and D2 family antagonists. Psychopharma-
J Neurosci 30:16585–16600. cology (Berl) 203:489 – 499.

(Accepted 28 December 2011)


(Available online 8 January 2012)

You might also like