You are on page 1of 5

YEBEH-05846; No of Pages 5

Epilepsy & Behavior xxx (2018) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Epilepsy & Behavior

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/yebeh

Neuropsychological profile of mild temporal lobe epilepsy


Maria Grazia Vaccaro a,1, Michele Trimboli a,1, Cristina Scarpazza b,c, Liana Palermo d, Antonella Bruni a,
Antonio Gambardella a, Angelo Labate a,⁎
a
Department of Medical Sciences, Institute of Neurology, University Magna Græcia, Catanzaro, Italy
b
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology & Neuroscience, King's College London, UK
c
Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Italy
d
Department of Medical and Surgical Sciences, Magna Graecia University of Catanzaro, Italy

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Objective: In the current literature, whether patients with mild mesial temporal lobe epilepsy (mMTLE) have typ-
Received 22 February 2018 ical neurocognitive profile similar to patients with treatment-refractory seizures still remains unknown. The pur-
Revised 25 May 2018 pose of the present work was to analyze the neuropsychological profile in a group of consecutive patients with
Accepted 7 June 2018 mMTLE.
Available online xxxx
Methods: Forty consecutive patients whose conditions were diagnosed with mMTLE and 30 healthy controls (HC)
were evaluated with an extensive neuropsychological battery. In addition, self-report questionnaires were also
Keywords:
Mild temporal lobe epilepsy
administered to evaluate the subjective impairments in prospective and retrospective memories. Finally, the
Cognitive profile levels of depression and anxiety were evaluated using the Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II) and the State–
Verbal memory Trait Anxiety Inventory — Form Y1 (STAI-YI e 2).
Prospective memory Results: Patients with mMTLE patients showed higher BDI-II scores (15.9 ± 13.9 vs 7.2 ± 6.7; p =, 002), and
Retrospective memory higher STAI-Y1 (41.2 ± 14.6 vs 32.6 ± 9.8; p =, 005) together with both objective and subjective memory
deficits. Although BDI-II and STAI scores strongly correlated to the outcome in Rey Auditory Verbal Learning
Test (RAVLT) and prospective and retrospective memory questionnaire (PRMQ) (p b 0.0021), these results did
not change without depression scores.
Conclusion: We showed that a specific neurocognitive profile in patients with mMTLE exists. The neuropsycho-
logical features are mood depression, verbal memory immediate and delayed deficits, and subjective prospective
and retrospective memory deficits.
© 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction 90% of patients, seizures are well-controlled with a single antiepileptic


drug (AED) at low dosage, and some patients do not take medications.
Temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) is the most common type of partial ep- At long-term follow-up, seizures in one-third of patients with mMTLE
ilepsy in adulthood [1]. According to the International League Against will develop refractoriness to AEDs over time [7]. Although mMTLE
Epilepsy (ILAE) classification, two different TLE categories have been and rMTLE share very similar electroclinical and neuroimaging features,
distinguished: mesial TLE (MTLE) and lateral TLE (LTLE). Mesial tempo- a cognitive profile has been described only in rMTLE. A typical cognitive
ral lobe epilepsy represents the vast majority (more than 70%) and impairment in rMTLE (possible candidates to surgery) is characterized
comprises two subtypes: mild mesial temporal lobe epilepsy (mMTLE) by memory deficits, impaired executive functions, language, processing
and refractory mesial temporal lobe epilepsy (rMTLE) [2–4]. Mild mesial speed, intelligence, and motor dexterity [8,9]. There are several studies
temporal lobe epilepsy is defined as “benign” because it is characterized investigating the relationship between patients with rMTLE and mem-
by at least 24 months of seizure freedom with or without antiepileptic ory impairment; one of them suggests that hippocampal sclerosis influ-
medication, seizures that begin during adolescence up to middle age, ences verbal memory [10], and a second one found that the mood and
and an average age of onset at around 34 years [5–7]. In more than memory disturbances are the most salient comorbid conditions in pa-
tients with rMTLE [11,12]. Conversely, a typical cognitive mMTLE profile
has not been fully described so far. Only few studies with limited pa-
tients suggested that mMTLE shows verbal memory deficits [13–15].
⁎ Corresponding author at: Cattedra di Neurologia, Università degli Studi “Magna
Graecia”, Campus Universitario Germaneto, Viale Europa, 88100 Catanzaro, Italy.
Therefore, the aim of our study was to describe a cognitive profile
E-mail address: labate@unicz.it (A. Labate). using a large cognitive battery in a consecutive group of patients with
1
These authors contributed equally. mMTLE.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.06.013
1525-5050/© 2018 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Please cite this article as: Vaccaro MG, et al, Neuropsychological profile of mild temporal lobe epilepsy, Epilepsy Behav (2018), https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.06.013
2 M.G. Vaccaro et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior xxx (2018) xxx–xxx

2. Materials and method evaluated attention with an interference procedure (times in s); the
Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (MWCST) [28] was a modification
2.1. Participants of the original Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, and it tested the abstract
reasoning, decision making, planning, conceptual shifting, learning
In this study, 40 consecutive patients with a diagnosis of mMTLE new rules, and the ability to change cognitive strategies depending on
were enrolled from February 2015 to June 2017. Patients were recruited changes in environmental conditions [29]; the Digit Span: Forwards
from the Outpatients Epilepsy Centre of Magna Graecia University of Ca- and Backwards task [30] was used to assess the short-term verbal mem-
tanzaro, Italy. The mMTLE of all patients was diagnosed according to the ory; the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCFT) [31,32] was used
ILAE classification of seizures [16] by two trained epileptologists (AG to investigate visuospatial constructional functions, visuographic mem-
and AL). Exclusion criteria were as follows: patient age is less than 18 ory, and some aspects of planning and executive functions, with imme-
or more than 70 years; patient has other neurological, psychotic disor- diate and delayed recalls; the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test
ders (excluding depression and anxiety) or comorbidities; and patient (RAVLT) [33,34], which includes measures of immediate and delayed
has generalized epilepsy, other partial epilepsies, or rMTLE. We decided recalls, was used to assess verbal learning and memory; and the Short
not to exclude patients with clinically relevant depression or anxiety as Tale (ST) [35,31], which includes measures of immediate and delayed
these psychiatric symptoms are often associated with epilepsy [17–20]. recalls, was used to assess the episodic memory. In addition, the self-re-
Thirty healthy individuals matched in age, gender, and level of educa- ported prospective and retrospective memory questionnaire (PRMQ)
tion to the patients with mMTLE were also recruited. Healthy individ- [36] was used to measure the subjective impairment in daily life in pro-
uals with Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) [21] score equal or spective memory (PM) and retrospective memory (RM). It is a self-re-
less than 25/30, neurological or psychiatric diseases, and drug addiction port questionnaire, and it is made up of sixteen items: eight asking
were excluded. Both patients and healthy controls (HC) provided writ- about PM (i.e., the ability to remember to accomplish an intended action
ten informed consent to participate in the study. The study was ap- at some point in the future such as remembering to take a tablet after
proved by the local Research Ethics Committee in accordance with the lunch) and eight about RM failures. Participants were asked to rate
Helsinki Declaration. In each patient, the diagnosis of MTLE was mainly how often they believe to fail in daily life activities requiring PM or
based on typical temporal auras or interictal electroencephalographic RM from very often (rated as 5 in a Likert scale) to never (rated as 1
(EEG) discharges with a maximum over the temporal lobes. Any sug- in a Likert scale) [37,38]. In this way, the higher the PRMQ score is, the
gestion of seizure onset outside the mesial temporal structures by semi- higher the subjectively perceived problems in PM and RM are. Although
ology or EEG findings was an exclusion criterion. Lateralization was the PRMQ is widely used in literature [39–44], it has never been used to
based on lateralized EEG discharges, with or without lateralized seizure study the cognitive impairment in epilepsy. The Beck Depression Inven-
features. Epileptiform discharges were diagnosed in the presence of tory Second Edition (BDI-II) [45] was used to evaluate depressive symp-
focal spikes or sharp waves followed by slow waves, and they always in- toms of both groups; the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory — Form Y1
volved the temporal regions. (STAI-YI) provides scores for state anxiety; and State–Trait Anxiety In-
All patients underwent 3-Tesla magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) ventory — Form Y2 (STAI-YII) [46] provides scores for trait anxiety
test to evaluate the presence of abnormal temporal lobe features, levels. The entire neuropsychological battery lasted around 60 to 90
mainly the presence of hippocampal sclerosis. Multiple scalp EEG re- min.
cordings were used to detect interictal EEG abnormalities. At baseline,
all participants underwent a battery of standardized neuropsychological 2.3. Statistical analysis
(NPC) tasks and several neuropsychiatric interviews. All tests and ques-
tionnaires were administered by the same neuropsychologist expert Categorical variables are expressed as count (percentage) and con-
(MGV) blinded to clinical information. tinuous variables as mean (SD) or median (range), as appropriated. Be-
tween-group differences were tested using the chi-square for
2.2. Neuropsychological battery categorical variables and the two independent samples t-test or the
Mann–Whitney U tests for categorical and continuous variables, de-
The general cognitive functions of all patients and HC were evalu- pending on whether the data were or were not normally distributed.
ated by a battery of standardized neuropsychological tests. The follow- The normal distribution of the data was tested using the Kolmogorov–
ing tests were administered: MMSE [21] was used to screen cognitive Smirnov test (d-value) on each variable. The partial eta squared (η2p)
impairment; the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) was was reported as an estimate of effect size [47]. Note that 0.2 is consid-
used as a measure of lexical stock, ability to access the lexicon, and cog- ered a small effect, 0.5 a medium effect, and 0.8 a large effect [47]. The
nitive flexibility [22–25]; Stroop Color and Word Test (Stroop) [26,27] results from the neuropsychological tests were corrected for multiple

Table 1
Demographic, clinical, and neuroimaging features of healthy controls (HC) and patients with mild mesial temporal lobe epilepsy (mMTLE).

HC mMTLE p value
n = 30 n = 40

Gender (F)a 20 (66.6%) 27 (67.5%) 0.941


Age (years)b 43.4 ± 16.5 44.3 ± 12.9 0.790
Education (years)c 13 [5–17] 13 [5–17] 0.763
Duration of illness (years)b – 15.8 ± 11.6 –
Febrile seizures, n – 22 –
Simple partial seizures (e.g., dejàvù)/m (n), mean (CI) – 0.7 (0.2–6.2) –
Number of AEDs (mean ± SD) – 1.1 ± 0.6 –
MRI evidence of hippocampal sclerosis (n) – 8 –
BDI-IIb 7.2 ± 6.7 15.9 ± 13.8 0.002
STAI-YIb 32.6 ± 9.8 41.2 ± 14.6 0.005
STAI-YIIb 35.4 ± 11.4 41.6 ± 13.2 0.046

Results were expressed using row number percentage (a) for dichotomous variable and mean ± standard deviation (b) or median [range] (c) for continuous variables depending on
whether or not the data were normally distributed. Statistical analyses were performed using chi-square (a) for dichotomous variable, two independent samples t-test (b) or Mann–Whit-
ney (c) for continuous variables depending on whether or not the data were normally distributed. CI: confidence interval; m: month; n: number.

Please cite this article as: Vaccaro MG, et al, Neuropsychological profile of mild temporal lobe epilepsy, Epilepsy Behav (2018), https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.06.013
M.G. Vaccaro et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior xxx (2018) xxx–xxx 3

Table 2
Neuropsychological profile of healthy controls (HC) and patients with mild temporal lobe
epilepsy (mMTLE).

HC mMTLE p K–S d Cohen's


n = 30 n = 40 d

MMSEa 27.9 ± 1.6 27.1 ± 1.5 0.06 0.12 0.51


ROCF copya 24.3 ± 4.2 27.8 ± 5.2 0.004 0.13 0.75
ROCF delay 30 sa 14.4 ± 4.9 12.5 ± 5.7 0.152 0.08 0.35
ROCF delay 20 mina 13.8 ± 4.7 12.1 ± 5.1 0.156 0.06 0.34
RAVLT immediate recalla 51.0 ± 41.4 ± 9.4 0.001* 0.07 0.92
12.2
RAVLT delayed recalla 11.6 ± 3.4 8.5 ± 3.8 0.001* 0.11 0.85
SRT total scorea 11.7 ± 2.9 9.0 ± 4.2 0.005 0.11 0.72
SRT immediate recalla 5.4 ± 1.8 4.4 ± 2.1 0.055 0.16 0.50
SRT delayed recalla 6.2 ± 1.6 4.5 ± 2.5 0.003 0.15 0.78
COWATa 38.2 ± 29.7 ± 0.015 0.12 0.79
14.4 12.1
MCST categoriesb 5 [1–6] 5 [1–6] 0.19 0.21°
MCST perseverative 2 [0–21] 4 [0–15] 0.03 0.207°
errorsb
MCST total errorsa 6.6 ± 7.2 8.4 ± 6.2 0.292 0.15 0.27
Digit span forwarda 5.4 ± 1.1 4.6 ± 1.1 0.007 0.18 0.72 Fig. 1. Correlation between BDI-II and PRMQ retrospective (PRMQ R).
Digit span backwarda 3.5 ± 1.0 2.7 ± 1.0 0.003 0.19 0.9
Stroop test interferencea 26.7 ± 8.1 27.7 ± 0.737 0.11 0.08
14.6
0.001). The results of these neuropsychological tests strongly correlated
PRMQ prospectivea 17.2 ± 5.4 22.5 ± 5.7 0.001* 0.12 0.95
PRMQ retrospectivea 17.0 ± 4.1 22.3 ± 5.8 b0.001* 0.14 1.04 with the BDI-II score but not with the STAI-YI and STAI-YII scores (Table
3). In particular, the RAVLT delayed score negatively correlated with the
Results were expressed using mean ± standard deviation (a) or median [range] (b) de-
pending on whether or not the data were normally distributed. Statistical analyses were BDI-II (r = − 0.386, p = 0.001; indicating that the higher the depres-
performed using two independent samples t-test (a) or Mann–Whitney test (b) depend- sion score, the lower the delayed recall) (Fig. 1). Similarly, the PRMQ
ing on whether or not the data were normally distributed. K–S: Kolmogorov–Smirnov. scores positively correlated with the BDI- II (r = 0.449 and r = 0.461
Asterisk (*) denotes statistical significance after multiple comparison correction. Open cir- for the prospective and retrospective indces, respectively, both p b
cle (°) denotes that the data are not normally distributed
0.001; indicating that the higher the depression score, the higher the
perceived prospective and retrospective memory difficulties) (Figs.
comparisons (Bonferroni's correction) and considered significant if p b 2 and 3). The correlation between RAVLT immediate score and BDI-II
0.05/18 = 0.0027 (as 18 different neuropsychological tests were ad- is not significant after Bonferroni correction (r = 0.338, p = 0.005).
ministered). Clinical and neuropsychological data were correlated One way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) on neuropsychological per-
using Pearson's correlation. formance, using group as independent factor (two levels: controls and
patients with mMTLE) and controlling for BDI score, was performed in
3. Results order to rule out the hypothesis that the worse neuropsychological per-
formance of patients with mMTLE was due to their higher depression
Neither patients nor HC were excluded or dropped out during the and not to a real impairment in cognitive functioning. Interestingly,
study. None of the controls showed interictal or ictal EEG abnormalities; the results still remained significant (RAVLT delayed: F = 5.24, p =
interictal spikes or sharp waves were detected on the left side (22/40) 0.025; PRMQ P: F = 5.39, p = 0.024; PRMQ R: F = 8.047, p = 0.006),
or right side (18/40) in the patient group. As summarized in Table 1, pa- thus, confirming that patients with mMTLE performed worse than con-
tients with mMTLE did not differ from HC in the sociodemographic fea- trols in these neuropsychological tests.
tures, age, gender, and level of education. Patients with mMTLE showed
significantly higher depression (p = 0.002) and anxiety (p = 0.005 and 4. Discussion
p = 0.046) scores compared with the HC as expressed by the BDI-II,
STAI-YI, and STAI-YII questionnaire scores, respectively (Table 1). The purpose of the present study was to describe NPC features in a
In detail, at the neuropsychological assessment, patients with consecutive group of patients with mMTLE. In fact, mMTLE has been ex-
mMTLE showed lower performance than controls in the majority of tensively described [4,48,14,15] and represents a syndrome itself. How-
the neuropsychological tests particularly at RAVLT, short tale test ever, a typical cognitive profile of this peculiar population is still
(SRT), Digit Span, and PRMQ questionnaire as summarized in Table 2. unknown. We, thus, proceed to comparing patients with mMTLE with
After correction for multiple comparisons, significant differences be- HC to study whether, even if mMTLE is defined as mild or “benign”,
tween patients and controls were captured at RAVLT immediate recall they carry a typical cognitive profile at onset. In fact, neurologically
(51.0 ± 12.2 and 41.4 ± 9.4, t = 3.648, degrees of freedom (df) = 68, healthy patients with mMTLE might manifest important neuropsycho-
p = 0.001), RAVLT delayed recall (11.6 ± 3.4 and 8.5 ± 3.8, t = 3.400, logical deficits since onset independent from depression or anxiety.
df = 68, p = 0.001), PRMQ prospective (PRMQ P) (17.2 ± 5.4 and We clearly showed that firstly, the NPC assessment is important
22.5 ± 5.7, t = −3.631, df = 68, p = 0.001), and PRMQ retrospective even in patients with milder and subtle form of epilepsy and secondly,
(PRMQ R) (17.0 ± 4.1 and 22.3 ± 5.8, t = − 4.034, df = 68, p b patients' awareness of memory impairment could prevent a negative

Table 3
Correlations between clinical and neuropsychological features.

RAVLT immediate RAVLT delayed PRMQ prospective PRMQ retrospective

BDI-II r = −0.338, p = 0.005 r = −0.386, p = 0.001* r = 0.499, p b 0.001* r = 0.461, p b 0.001*


STAI-YI r = −0.084, p = 0.502 r = −0.197, p = 0.111 r = 0.297, p = 0.026 r = −0.260, p = 0.053
STAI-YII r = −0.077, p = 0.543 r = −0.198, p = 0.113 r = 0.361, p = 0.007 r = 0.332, p = 0.014

Asterisk (*) denotes statistical significance after multiple comparison correction.

Please cite this article as: Vaccaro MG, et al, Neuropsychological profile of mild temporal lobe epilepsy, Epilepsy Behav (2018), https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.06.013
4 M.G. Vaccaro et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior xxx (2018) xxx–xxx

long-term alteration in neuronal plasticity and memory function may


be the consequence of even single or brief repetitive seizures and
cause secondary hippocampal volume loss [7,53,54]. Furthermore, we
cannot exclude that AEDs might have negatively influenced the cogni-
tive profile of patients with mMTLE. Indeed, there is a possibility that
a longer exposure to AEDs might also contribute to verbal learning im-
pairment in our patients. However, this should not represent our case
because the vast majority of our sample was taking a single AED at
low dosages that carries a modest, dose-related, adverse cognitive effect
compared with the contribution of high dosages and polytherapies [55].
Few limitations need to be accounted for. The sample size was not
very large; however, the cohort with mMTLE was very homogeneous
representing a specific syndrome. Secondly, we used PRMQ that is a
measure of self-rated memory failures and therefore cannot be treated
as though it were a direct measure of memory performance [38], but
the results of memory deficits are confirmed by objective neuropsycho-
logical assessment particularly with verbal memory test (RAVLT).
To our knowledge, the current work might represent the first neuro-
Fig. 2. Correlation between BDI-II and PRMQ prospective (PRMQ P). psychological study carried out in consecutive patients with mMTLE.
We demonstrated that even with patients with mMTLE and a very
mild profile, there are cognitive dysfunctions mainly on memory defi-
impact of cognitive deficits on everyday life allowing physicians to set cits as part of the clinical spectrum of this syndrome. We believe that
rehabilitation programs [49]. Interestingly, we observed that patients this information is crucial because neuropsychology plays a very impor-
with mMTLE showed a cognitive impairment that can be part of the syn- tant role in the treatment of epilepsy [56-60], providing information on
drome itself. Indeed, we found verbal memory deficits at RAVLT delay as the effects of seizures on higher cortical functions through the measure-
well as subjective memory deficits in the daily life using a self-reported ment of behavioral abilities and disabilities. Larger longitudinal long-
questionnaire exploring PM and RM [37–39]. Although PM and RM im- term studies together with advanced neuroimaging correlation com-
pairments might have very serious implications for the health of pa- paring patients with refractory TLE are necessary to better characterize
tients with epilepsy such as forgetting to take tablets [50], PM and RM the NPC profile in mMTLE and to look for potential biomarkers of
subjective assessments in patients with mMTLE have received little at- refractoriness.
tention to date. In contrast to Rayner's [51,52] results, showing that sub-
jective memory complaints in patients with refractory focal epilepsy
were often due to mood dysfunction, our results suggested that subjec- Acknowledgments
tive memory complaints are independent from mood dysfunction. An-
other intriguing finding in our study is that patients with mMTLE The authors wish to thank Dr. Genevieve Rayner for providing En-
showed higher score on BDI-II compared with HC. However, when con- glish language proofreading for an early version of the manuscript.
trolling for the BDI-II score, we found the same significant difference be-
tween controls and patients on memory performance, suggesting that Disclosure of conflicts of interest
memory deficits are not due to depressive symptoms. We can hypothe-
size that our results are mainly related to the underlying epileptic pro- The authors have no conflicts of interest.
gression itself that might cause additional damage to temporal This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agen-
structures dealing to remodeling of synaptic organization and memory cies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
deficit because epilepsy in our cohort is very mild with few simple par-
tial seizures at long-term follow-up. In accordance with this hypothesis, Ethical publications statement
good evidence from experimental and imaging studies indicate that
We confirm that we have read the journal's position on issue in-
volved in ethical publication and affirm that this report is consistent
with those guidelines.

References

[1] Manford M, Hart YM, Sander JW, Shorvon SD. National General Practice Study of Ep-
ilepsy (NGPSE): partial seizure patterns in a general population. Neurology 1992;42
(10):1911–7.
[2] Volcy Gómez M. Mesial temporal lobe epilepsy: its physiopathology, clinical charac-
teristics, treatment and prognosis. Rev Neurol 2004;38(7):663–7.
[3] Salanova V, Markand ON, Worth R. Clinical characteristics and predictive factors in
98 patients with complex partial seizures treated with temporal resection. Arch
Neurol 1994;51(10):1008.
[4] Labate A, Mumoli L, Curcio A, Tripepi G, D'Arrigo G, Ferlazzo E, et al. Value of clinical
features to differentiate refractory epilepsy from mimics: a prospective longitudinal
cohort study. Eur J Neurol 2018:22.
[5] Labate A, Gambardella A, Andermann E, Aguglia U, Cendes F, Berkovics SF, et al. Be-
nign mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Nat Rev Neurol 2011;7:237–40.
[6] Labate A, Ventura P, Gambardella A, Le Piane E, Colosimo E, Leggio U, et al. MRI ev-
idence of mesial temporal sclerosis in sporadic “benign” temporal lobe epilepsy.
Neurology 2003;60:560–3.
[7] Labate A, Aguglia U, Tripepi G, Mumoli L, Ferlazzo E, Baggetta R, et al. Long-term out-
come of mild mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. A prospective longitudinal cohort
Fig. 3. Correlation between BDI-II and RAVLT. study. Neurology 2016;86(20):1904–10.

Please cite this article as: Vaccaro MG, et al, Neuropsychological profile of mild temporal lobe epilepsy, Epilepsy Behav (2018), https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.06.013
M.G. Vaccaro et al. / Epilepsy & Behavior xxx (2018) xxx–xxx 5

[8] Kellermann TS, Bonilha L, Lin JJ, Hermann BP. Mapping the neuropsychological pro- [35] Barigazzi R, Della Sala S, Laiacona M, Spinnler H, Valenti V. La Memoria di Prosa:
file of temporal lobe epilepsy using cognitive network topology and graph theory. Taratura di un test. Ricerche Psicol 1987;1:49–80.
Epilepsy Behav 2016;63:9–16. [36] Crawford JR, Smith G, Maylor EA, Della Sala S, Logie RH. The prospective and retro-
[9] Hermann BP, Seidenberg M, Schoenfeld J, Davies K. Neuropsychological characteris- spective memory questionnaire (PRMQ): normative data and latent structure in a
tics of the syndrome of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Arch Neurol 1997;54(4): large non-clinical sample. Memory 2003;11(3):261–75.
369–76. [37] Smith G, Della Sala S, Logie RH, Maylor EA. Prospective and retrospective memory in
[10] Sidhu MK, Stretton J, Winston GP, Symms M, Thompson PJ, Koepp MJ, et al. Factors normal ageing and dementia: a questionnaire study. Memory 2000;8(5):311–21.
affecting reorganisation of memory encoding networks in temporal lobe epilepsy. [38] Cona G, Bisiacchi PS, Sartori G, Scarpazza C. Effects of cue focality on the neural
Epilepsy Res 2015;110:1–9. mechanism of prospective memory: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Sci
[11] Wrench JM, Wilson SJ, Bladin PF, Reutens DC. Hippocampal volume and depression: Rep 2016;6:25983.
insights from epilepsy surgery. Send to J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2009;80(5): [39] Sullivan KL, Woods SP, Bucks RS, Loft S, Weinborn M. Intraindividual variability in
539–44. neurocognitive performance is associated with time-based prospective memory in
[12] Tellez-Zenteno JF, Patten SB, Jetté N, Williams J, Wiebe S. Psychiatric comorbidity in older adults. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2018;20:1–11.
epilepsy: a population-based analysis. Epilepsia 2007;48:2336–44. [40] Marcone S, Gagnon JF, Desjardins C, David AC, Postuma RB, Montplaisir J, et al. Pro-
[13] Gambardella A, Aguglia U, Chifari R, Labate A, Manna I, Serra P, et al. ApoE epsilon4 spective memory in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder with or without mild
allele and disease duration affect verbal learning in mild temporal lobe epilepsy. cognitive impairment: a preliminary study. Clin Neuropsychol 2018;7:1–23.
Epilepsia 2005;46:110–7. [41] Papaliagkas V, Papantoniou G, Tsolaki M, Moraitou D. Self-report instruments of cog-
[14] Alessio A, Kobayashi E, Damasceno BP, Lopes-Cendes I, Cendes F. Evidence of mem- nitive failures as screening tools for subjective cognitive impairment in older adults.
ory impairment in asymptomatic individuals with hippocampal atrophy. Epilepsy Hell J Nucl Med 2017;20:58–70 [Suppl].
Behav 2004;5(6):981–7. [42] Woods SP, Weinborn M, Li YR, Hodgson E, Ng AR, Bucks RS. Does prospective mem-
[15] AlQassmi A, Burneo JG, McLachlan RS, Mirsattari SM. Benign mesial temporal lobe ory influence quality of life in community-dwelling older adults? Neuropsychol Dev
epilepsy: a clinical cohort and literature review. Epilepsy Behav 2016;65:60–4. Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2015;22(6):679–92.
[16] Fisher RS, Cross JH, French JA, Higurashi N, Hirsch E, Jansen FE, et al. Operational clas- [43] Singer JJ, Falchi M, Macgregor AJ, Cherkas LF, Spector TD. Genome-wide scan for pro-
sification of seizure types by the International League Against Epilepsy: position spective memory suggests linkage to chromosome 12q22. Behav Genet 2006;36(1):
paper of the ILAE Commission for Classification and Terminology. Epilepsia 2017; 18–28.
58(4):522–30. [44] Bigdeli I, Farzin A, Talepasand S. Prospective memory impairments in schizophrenic
[17] Lespinet V, Bresson C, N'Kaoua B, Rougier A, Claverie B. Effect of age of onset of tem- patients. Iran J Psychiatry Behav Sci 2014;8(4):57–63.
poral lobe epilepsy on the severity and the nature of preoperative memory deficits. [45] Beck Aaron T, Steer Robert A, Brown Gregory K. In: Ghisi M, Flebus GB, Montano A,
Neuropsychologia 2002;40(9):1591–600. et al, editors. BDI-II Beck Depression Inventory — II. In Manuale. Adattamento
[18] Rayner G, Wilson SJ. The role of epilepsy surgery. In: Mula M, editor. Neuropsychiat- italiano (a cura di). Giunti OS Organizzazioni Speciali; 2006.
ric symptoms in epilepsy. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing; [46] Spielberger CD. STAI state–trait anxiety inventory. Forma Y. Manuale. O. S.
2016. p. 303–32. Organizzazioni Speciali; 1996.
[19] Boylan LS, Flint LA, Labovitz DL, Jackson SC, Starner K, Devinsky O. Depression but [47] Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. San Diego, CA: Aca-
not seizure frequency predicts quality of life in treatment-resistant epilepsy. Neurol- demic Press; 1969.
ogy 2004;62:258–61. [48] Aguglia U, Gambardella A, Le Piane E, Messina D, Oliveri RL, Russo C, et al. Mild no
[20] Dumbray S, Naik S. Anxiety and depression levels in clients with epilepsy in selected lesional temporal lobe epilepsy. A common unrecognized disorder with onset in
hospital of Pune City. Nurs J India 2014;105(4):160–3. adulthood. Can J Neurol Sci 1998;25:282–6.
[21] Measso G, Cavarzeran F, Zappalà G, Lebowitz BD, Crook TH, Pirozzolo FJ, et al. The [49] Jackson CF1, Makin SM, Baker GA. Neuropsychological and psychological interven-
Mini-Mental State Examination: normative study of an Italian random sample. tions for people with newly diagnosed epilepsy. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2015
Dev Neuropsychol 1993;9(2):77–85. (7).
[22] Malek-Ahmadi M, Small Brent J, Raj A. The diagnostic value of controlled oral word [50] Thompson PJ, Corcoran R. Everyday memory failures in people with epilepsy.
association test-FAS and category fluency in single-domain amnestic mild cognitive Epilepsia 2017;33(Suppl. 6):S18–20.
impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2011;32:235–40. [51] Rayner G, Wrench JM, Wilson SJ. Differential contributions of objective memory and
[23] Caltagirone C, Gainotti G, Carlesimo GA, Parnetti L, Fadda L, Gallassi R, et al. Batteria mood to subjective memory complaints in refractory focal epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav
per la valutazione del deterioramento (Parte I): descrizione di uno strumento di 2010;19(3):359–64.
diagnosi neuropsicologica. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr 1995;56(4):461–70. [52] Rayner G. The contribution of cognitive networks to depression in epilepsy. Epilepsy
[24] Caltagirone C, Gainotti G, Carlesimo GA, Parnetti L, Fadda L, Gallassi R, et al. Batteria Curr 2017;17(2):78–83.
per la valutazione del deterioramento (Parte II): descrizione di uno strumento di [53] Briellmann RS, Mark Wellard R, Masterton RA, Abbott DF, Berkovic SF, Jackson GD.
diagnosi neuropsicologica. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr 1995;56(4):471–88. Hippocampal sclerosis: MR prediction of seizure intractability. Epilepsia 2007;48
[25] Costa A, Bagoj E, Monaco M, Zabberoni S, De Rosa S, Papantonio AM, et al. Standard- (2):315–23.
ization and normative data obtained in the Italian population for a new verbal flu- [54] Chang YC, Huang CC, Huang SC. Long-term neuroplasticity effects of febrile seizures
ency instrument, the phonemic/semantic alternate fluency test. Neurol Sci 2014; in the developing brain. Chang Gung Med J 2008;31(2):125–35 [Review].
35(3):365–72. [55] Bengzon J, Kokaia Z, Elmér E, Nanobashvili A, Kokaia M, Lindvall O. Apoptosis and
[26] Scarpina F, Tagini S. The Stroop Color and Word Test. Front Psychol 2017;8:557. proliferation of dentate gyrus neurons after single and intermittent limbic seizures.
[27] Amato MP, Portaccio E, Goretti B, Zipoli V, Ricchiuti L, De Caro MF, et al. The Rao's Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1997;94(19):10432–7.
Brief Repeatable Battery and Stroop Test: normative values with age, education [56] Tramoni E, Felician O, Barbeau EJ, Guedj E, Guye M, Bartolomei F, et al. Long-term
and gender corrections in an Italian population. Mult Scler 2006;12(6):787–93. consolidation of declarative memory: insight from temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain
[28] Caffarra P, Vezzadini G, Dieci F, Zonato F, Venneri A. Modified card sorting test: nor- 2011;134(Pt 3):816–31.
mative data. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2004;26(2):246–50 Online. [57] Hendriks MP, Aldenkamp AP, Alpherts WC, Ellis J, Vermeulen J, van der Vlugt H. Re-
[29] Grippa E, Sellitto M, Scarpazza C. Multiple sclerosis reduces sensitivity to immediate lationships between epilepsy-related factors and memory impairment. Acta Neurol
reward during decision making. Behav Neurosci 2017;131(4):325–36. Scand 2004;110(5):291–300.
[30] Monaco M, Costa A, Caltagirone C, Carlesimo GA. Erratum to: forward and backward [58] Voltzenlogel V, Hirsch E, Vignal JP, Valton L, Manning L. Preserved anterograde and
span for verbal and visuo.spatial data: standardization and normative data from an remote memory in drug-responsive temporal lobe epileptic patients. Epilepsy Res
Italian adult population. Neurol Sci 2013;34:749–54. 2015;115:126–32.
[31] Carlesimo GA, Buccione I, Fadda L, Graceffa A, MauriI M, Lorusso S, et al. [59] Wilson SJ, Baxendale S, Barr W, Hamed S, Langfitt J, Samson S, et al. Indications and
Standardizzazione di due test di memoria per uso clinico. Breve racconto e figura expectations for neuropsychological assessment in routine epilepsy care: report of
di Rey. Nuova Riv Neurol 2002;12(1):1–13. the ILAE Neuropsychology Task Force, Diagnostic Methods Commission, 2013–
[32] Caffarra P, Vezzadini G, Dieci F, Zonato F, Venneri A. Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: 2017. Epilepsia 2015;56(5):674–81.
normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurol Sci 2002;22(6):443–7. [60] Jones-Gotman M, Smith ML, Risse GL, Westerveld M, Swanson SJ, Giovagnoli AR,
[33] Carlesimo GA, Caltagirone C, Gainotti G. The mental deterioration battery: normative et al. The contribution of neuropsychology to diagnostic assessment in epilepsy. Ep-
data, diagnostic reliability and qualitative analyses of cognitive impairment. Eur ilepsy Behav 2010;18:3–12.
Neurol 1996;36:378–84.
[34] Carlesimo GA, Sabbadini M, Fadda L, Caltagirone C. Word-list forgetting in young
and elderly subjects: evidence for age-related decline in transferring information
from transitory to permanent memory condition. Cortex 1997;33(1):155–66.

Please cite this article as: Vaccaro MG, et al, Neuropsychological profile of mild temporal lobe epilepsy, Epilepsy Behav (2018), https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.06.013

You might also like