You are on page 1of 15

Journal of Zoology

Journal of Zoology. Print ISSN 0952-8369

REVIEW

The evolution of ‘bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs:


biomechanics, sexual selection, social selection or
species recognition?
K. Padian1 & J. R. Horner2
1 Department of Integrative Biology and Museum of Paleontology, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA
2 Museum of the Rockies, Montana State University, Bozeman, MT, USA

Keywords Abstract
Dinosauria; functional morphology; sexual
selection; social selection; species ‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs have four main traditional explanations: mechan-
recognition. ical function, sexual selection, social selection and species recognition. Any of
these can be plausible for individual species, but they fail to be persuasive when
Correspondence other lines of evidence cannot adequately test them. The first three also fail as
Kevin Padian, Department of Integrative general propositions when phylogenetic analyses based on other characters do not
Biology and Museum of Paleontology, support scenarios of selective improvement of such functions in their clade (or the
University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA. explanation simply does not apply to any other species in the clade). Moreover, the
Email: kpadian@berkeley.edu hypothesis of sexual selection requires significant sexual dimorphism, which has
never been conclusively established in dinosaurs.
Editor: Steven Le Comber We propose instead that species recognition may have been a more general
force that drove the evolution of bizarre structures in dinosaurs. That is, the
Received 14 October 2009; revised 4 bizarre structures communicate to other individuals a variety of possible associa-
February 2010; accepted 22 April 2010 tional cues, including species identification, potential protection and social habits
and the appropriateness of potential mates. In other words, bizarre structures
doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2010.00719.x amount to an advertisement for positive association. Neither species recognition
nor any other hypothesis should be a ‘default’ explanation. Although direct
observation is impossible, we propose two tests. First, contrary to adaptive, social
or sexual selection, under the species recognition model morphology should be
expected to evolve without obvious directional trends, because the only objective is
to differ from one’s relatives. Hence, patterns of evolution of bizarre structures
should be relatively proliferative and non-directional. Second, several contem-
poraneous species should overlap in geographic range (sympatric, parapatric,
peripatric). Fossil species often show evidence of this pattern in the past by ‘ghost
ranges’ of related taxa. These tests together could reinforce or weaken an argument
for species recognition.

argue for a particular theory that explains everything than


Introduction to suggest how these kinds of evolutionary problems should
‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs and other extinct animals be addressed, and to suggest some criteria for testing them.
(e.g. Gould, 1974) are of perennial interest to paleontolo- Our hope is that others will both improve on our suggestions
gists and have become a staple of textbooks on evolution and bring new data to the questions.
because they raise perennial questions. What did these By ‘bizarre structures’ we mean features that are unusual
structures do? How did they evolve? If they were so useful, enough, to the trained eyes of paleobiologists, to invite
how did they contribute to their bearers’ evolutionary explanations beyond the basic functions of feeding, locomo-
success? If their bearers are extinct, did they become a tion, respiration and so on (Farlow & Dodson, 1974; Gould,
liability at some point? 1974; Molnar, 1977; Main et al., 2005). In many respects
In this paper, we explore the principal explanations for these structures are similar (but not necessarily analogous)
the evolution of ‘bizarre structures.’ The kinds of explana- to certain structures in living animals. They include the frills
tions we discuss include the teleology of what they were for and horns of ceratopsians, the domes of pachycephalosaurs,
and how they evolved. We recast these explanations using the crests of lambeosaurine hadrosaurs, the scute complexes
current methods of comparative biology. Our goal is less to of ankylosaurs and the plates and spikes of stegosaurs. We

Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London 3
‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs K. Padian and J. R. Horner

discuss four general types of explanations: mechanical func- Methods


tion, sexual selection, social selection and species recogni-
tion. The first two of these are pre-eminent in Although there are many approaches to explaining mor-
paleobiological explanation (e.g. Galton, 1970; Farlow & phology in extinct organisms (Hickman, 1980), inferences
Dodson, 1974; Dodson, 1975; Hopson, 1975; Farlow, about function and behavior are based on two general
Thompson & Rosner, 1976; Molnar, 1977; Buffrenil, Far- models: homology and analogy (essentially, historical and
low & de Ricqlès, 1986; etc.). The third has been advocated ahistorical explanations: Weishampel, 1997). The accepted
most recently and thoroughly by Hieronymus et al. (2009). approach to evaluating homology of function and behavior
in extinct animals is Witmer’s (1995) extant phylogenetic
The fourth has not been extensively considered by any
authors, although it has been frequently acknowledged in bracket (EPB). For this purpose, a phylogeny of living and
functional and behavioral considerations (e.g. Farlow & related fossil forms is required. The degree to which a
Dodson, 1974; Hopson, 1975; Molnar, 1977; Sampson, condition can be inferred reliably as present in an extinct
1999; Hieronymus et al., 2009). There has been an historical taxon is related to its position among living forms that are
predilection to attempt first to explain a bizarre structure in known to share the function or behavior (Fig. 1). Because
mechanical terms; if this explanation appears weak or is crocodiles and birds, the two extant brackets of extinct
contraindicated, it has been traditional to attribute the dinosaurs, share none of the bizarre structures of extinct
feature to ‘sexual display’ by virtue of its apparent useless- dinosaurs, the EPB cannot provide much direct guidance on
ness for mechanical function. In this way, sexual display has these problems. There are simply no available homologous
often become a ‘default’ explanation that was seldom structures, with the possible exceptions of the cranial crests
explicitly tested or questioned. of lambeosaurine hadrosaurs and cassowaries, and the
We acknowledge several classes of facts. First, some scutes of crocodiles and thyreophorans (which, being absent
in their respective common ancestors, must be regarded as
structures may have served more than one function. For
example, ankylosaur armor may have been defensive but parallelisms, despite an obvious homological basis in bone
also distinctive enough to have served a role in species histology: Scheyer & Sander, 2004; Main et al., 2005).
recognition. After all, exaptation is a pre-eminent factor in Analogy to living forms is the approach that remains
macroevolutionary change. Second, because many soft part when arguments of homology cannot be made, and it is even
features and also nearly all behaviors are not preserved in more problematic. The quality of an explanation depends in
the fossil record, affairs may have been far more complex part on the precision of definition of the features that are
than paleontologists can detect. We also allow that not compared, and the separation of those features (and func-
enough is known to determine the origin of some features; tions) from ancillary or irrelevant ones (Whewell, 1859;
for example, there are too few known specimens of cranially Padian, 1995; Wilson, 1998).
adorned theropod taxa such as Dilophosaurus, Cryolopho-
saurus and Carnotaurus to permit a test of evolutionary Classes of explanation of
explanations. We see no reason to be dogmatic about
particular hypotheses, and no reason not to be pluralistic
bizarre structures
about explanations when appropriate. Our goal is to pro- The two general classes of explanation of bizarre structures
pose a set of explicit tests of mechanical and behavioral in dinosaurs relate to function and display (including sexual
hypotheses that we hope will set up discriminatory criteria selection, social selection and species recognition). Each
for these kinds of explanations. kind of explanation has a long history in the literature,

Figure 1 The Extant Phylogenetic Bracket (Wit-


mer, 1995) of extinct dinosaur clades that
characteristically and independently evolved
‘bizarre structures:’ stegosaurs and ankylo-
saurs (parallel elaboration of homologously
shared dermal scutes), lambeosaurine hadro-
saurs, neoceratopsians, pachycephalosaurs
and ceratosaurian theropods. The bizarre
morphologies of these dinosaurian groups are
not shared by their living bracket, crocodiles
and birds; crocodiles have scutes, but they are
never elaborated like those of thyreophoran
dinosaurs.

4 Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London
K. Padian and J. R. Horner ‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs

Table 1 Some proposed functions of ‘bizarre’ structures Mechanical explanations


Mechanical
Defense Explanations of individual taxa
Communication
Thermoregulation Many possible mechanical explanations have been proposed
Sensory and tested for various bizarre skeletal features of individual
Procuring food dinosaur species (Weishampel, 1981, 1997; Farke, 2004;
Display Farke, Wolff & Tanke, 2009; Hieronymus et al., 2009). In
Interspecific our view, Weishampel’s (1981) classic study of the crest of
Deter predators the hadrosaur Parasaurolophus is a model for examining
Discourage association of non-conspecifics functional inferences in extinct individual taxa. Weishampel
Intraspecific first divided all proposed hypotheses into testable and
Intrasexual untestable, and then proceeded to see if the testable ones
Establish territory could be falsified or supported by other lines of evidence. He
Ward off rivals for resources (including mates) found that most hypotheses of display and behavior could
Encourage association of conspecifics not be explicitly tested, but some mechanical functions, such
Compete for resources as snorkeling, head-butting and air storage, could be tested
Intersexual and rejected. Weishampel tested the proposed function of a
Attract mates resonance chamber by building a model of the nasal pas-
Encourage association of conspecifics
sages and diverticula, and passing a spectrum of oscillating
Note that functions of species recognition encompass interactions frequencies through them. Certain frequencies, as expected,
both between species (discourage association of non-conspecifics) resonated better than others, and Weishampel indepen-
and within species (‘encourage association of conspecifics,’ both of dently tested this outcome by determining whether the
the same sex and of different sexes). auditory organs were well attuned to those frequencies by
studying the size and morphology of the stapedial region.
Whereas this study did not ‘prove’ any particular function,
including discussions of dinosaur behavior (Horner & Gor- and could not logically rule out several weakly supported or
man, 1988; Carpenter, Hirsch & Horner, 1994; Currie & untestable explanations (see Weishampel, 1997), it is a
Padian, 1997; Farlow & Brett-Surman, 1997; Horner & model study for testing functional hypotheses of individual
Dobb, 1997; Carpenter, 1999; Weishampel, Dodson & organisms in paleobiology.
Osmolska, 2004; Hieronymus et al., 2009). We summarize But Weishampel’s approach, thorough as it was, did not
these classes of explanation in Table 1. account for all aspects of the problem, as he recognized. He
It is important that we define our terms. Mechanical noted one: the characters related to vocalizing and hearing
function refers to a specific adaptation such as feeding, reflect different (if not contradictory) respective phyloge-
locomotion, insulation or communication. Sexual selection netic brackets (Weishampel, 1997). In other words, other
is the advantage gained to access to mates when one character state distributions do not match, so they did not
sex possesses a specific feature that the other does not, apparently evolve in step. This is a specific problem for that
and uses it to attract mates or repel rivals for mates (Darwin, case. Disjunct sets of character distributions cannot support
1859, 1871). We want to emphasize here the importance a unified functional hypothesis that purports to explain the
of a discrete structure, function or behavior present in evolution of an adaptation (although in this case an exapta-
one sex but not the other, that is used for these two tion may be possible).
purposes. We also emphasize that this true sexual dimorph- One shortcoming of most functional explanations for
ism is different from a simple ‘sexual difference’ in bizarre structures in extinct dinosaurs is that the evolution
which one sex is slightly larger or more robust than the of these features and functions in a clade is very seldom
other, but possesses no particular structures for these considered. Without doing so, there is no evidence that the
purposes. (We recognize that there is debate about this function (in the sense of an adaptation) evolved at all, and
among behavioral ecologists, and we discuss it elsewhere.) therefore the hypothesized function itself must be consid-
Social selection refers to features that individuals in a ered in doubt, unless there is good independent evidence of
species use to improve their competitive advantage for it. The demonstration of its evolution requires a phyloge-
resources. Species recognition refers to features that allow netic component.
others of the same species to recognize each other for
various social purposes. Mate recognition is not the same
Phylogenetic dissection of adaptation (PDA)
thing, but it is a subset because it is important for individuals
to mate with others in the same species. We want to state When paleobiologists discuss functions of bizarre struc-
emphatically that we do not reject the possible operation of tures, they are generally discussing adaptations. It is a
any and all of these processes in extinct dinosaurs in truism of evolutionary biology that adaptations are shaped
principle. We ask how well established any and all of these by natural selection (Williams, 1992). Paleobiologists can-
are in specific cases. not assess selection in populations through generations, as

Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London 5
‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs K. Padian and J. R. Horner

microevolutionists can (e.g. Endler, 1986; Brandon, 1996).


But they can assess natural selection at a more general
hierarchical level in lineages, living and extinct, by mapping
the elaboration of structures and the improvement of
proposed functions upon phylogenies based on other char-
acters (e.g. Padian, 2001; Padian & Horner, 2002, 2004).
In order for an adaptation to be assessed (Padian, 1982,
1987), its necessary components must be identified and
separated from non-essential ones. By plotting these char-
acter states on a phylogeny built from other characters, the
Figure 2 Phylogeny as a test of the assembly of an adaptation.
assembly of the adaptation can be traced. Even after the
Consider an adaptation defined by five necessary features (1–5)
basic adaptation is assembled, further modifications can be
following a PDA analysis (Padian, 1995, 2001; see text). Phylogenetic
tracked in the same way (Padian, 2001). This method of
analysis reveals the clade that shares this function (and thereby
PDA can be formalized in the following way (modified from
shares all five features); it also reveals the order of assembly of those
Padian, 1982, 1987, 1995, 2001): five features on nodes along the spine of a more inclusive cladogram.
1. Identify the adaptation, its diagnostic (vs. merely asso- At each of the nodes along this spine (which have successive subsets
ciated) features and the groups that possess it. of those five features), a different function can be proposed and
2. Perform phylogenetic analyses of the groups, including tested; new secondary (exaptive) functions and improvements in
closest sister taxa, using all available character taxa. existing functions may also be proposed and tested.
3. Identify the phylogenetic sequence of acquisition of each
diagnostic feature of the adaptation.
4. Analyze the apparent roles, if any, of diagnostic char- pachycephalosaurs. Weishampel (1981) tested the possibi-
acters at each successive stage before the adaptation is lity of a defensive function of lambeosaurine crests and
assembled, using functional, physical, ecological, genetic concluded that the bone was too thin to have been of any
and other lines of evidence. use in this regard.
The implication of this method for the assessment of Ankylosaurs would seem to pose the least controversial
bizarre structures in dinosaurs is that, if such explanations example of a defensive function for bizarre structures, in this
are to move beyond the ad hoc, they must be able to explain case the dermal scutes (traditionally and tellingly called
the evolution of these features, the assembly of their char- ‘armor’) and tail ‘club’ (in ankylosaurids only: Carpenter,
acters and functions. In other words, at successive nodes 1997, 2001; Vickaryous, Maryanska & Weishampel, 2004).
along the spine of the cladogram, one should be able to Scutes cover the skull, the neck, the back, and much of the
point to specific characters diagnostic of the proposed tail, but there is great variety in their size, form and extent
adaptation, and assess their function with respect to the among ankylosaurs (Carpenter, 1997). This suggests that
organism as a whole. Such assessments need to take into there was no ‘optimal’ pattern of scute form and distribu-
account the roles of other features in the functional complex tion, and therefore it is difficult to propose that a defensive
in order to provide an adequate cross-test (Padian, 2001). function was successively ‘improved’ in ankylosaurs. How-
Moving to successive nodes along the spine of the clado- ever, consideration of their outgroups shows that ankylo-
gram, the evolution of the features from stage to stage saurs had more extensive dermal ossifications than the basal
should emerge. If there is no evidence for the improvement thyreophorans Scutellosaurus and Scelidosaurus (the latter
of a function or the assembly of a new one, the adaptive often considered an ankylosaur), not to mention the stego-
hypothesis fails. Therefore, functional explanations that are saurs, which lost all but the parasagittal rows (Main et al.,
not tested phylogenetically have no demonstrated evolu- 2005). This pattern points to defense as a plausible basal
tionary basis and are of limited value (Fig. 2; Weishampel, function of ankylosaur scutes, and suggests that whatever
1997). the variations in scute form and distribution, they were
‘good enough’ to serve an adequate defensive function.
Yet, as Carpenter (1997: p. 315, fig. 22.6) notes, the varia-
Testing mechanical hypotheses tion in scute form, and notably in the more conspicuous
We divide these into four general (and not mutually exclu- long neck spikes, suggests no obvious defensive strategy (see
sive) classes: defense, communication, thermoregulation also Scheyer & Sander, 2004), and may instead be primarily
and sensory function. related to display. Sexual dimorphism has not been estab-
lished, so sexual selection has no support, but social selec-
tion (Hieronymus et al., 2009) could be investigated further.
Defense
Several evolutionary strategies may have been involved
These features can be attributed to repulsion of predators here. The enlarged and fused scutes at the end of the
and to conspecifics of the same sex in agonistic behaviors ankylosaurid tail, preceded by a series of fused caudal
(non-exclusively). Notable examples are the horns and frills vertebrae, have often been invoked as a weapon, and this
of ceratopsians, the plates and spikes of stegosaurs, the seems to be supported by the enlarged areas of muscle
scutes and tail club of ankylosaurs and the domes of attachment on the pelvis, hindlimbs and transverse

6 Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London
K. Padian and J. R. Horner ‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs

because it is so similar to young Triceratops, the adult form


of Avaceratops may turn out to have been fenestrated as
well.
And horns vary widely; chasmosaurines had orbital horns
of various sizes and orientations, but most centrosaurines
had small orbital horns, and nasal horns of variable size that
show no obvious function in combat (Farke et al., 2009).
Farke (2004) used restored scale models of Triceratops to
determine how individuals might have fought each other,
interlocking horns, and Farke et al. (2009) showed that
injuries occurred significantly more often on skull bones that
would have been expected according to his predictions.
However, even if this function is plausible, it has not been
proposed and tested for other chasmosaurines, although it
was absent in centrosaurines (Farke et al., 2009). The most
recent published phylogenies of neoceratopsians (Xu et al.,
2002; Dodson, Forster & Sampson, 2004; Fig. 3) show no
directional pattern of improvement of either brow horns or
nose horns. Hence there is no evidence for adaptation to a
particular function, and other hypotheses also need to be
considered as a general explanation for the evolution of
horns and frills.
For stegosaurs, as Main et al. (2005) have shown, the
elaboration of plates and spikes shows no phylogenetic
trends in adaptation to proposed functions of thermoregu-
lation (Galton & Upchurch, 2004b). The possible function
of defense has been rejected by several authors (Buffrenil
et al., 1986; Main et al., 2005): the plates consist of a thin
layer of compact bone surrounding a central core of well-
vascularized, lattice-like (spongy) trabecular bone that
would be crushed easily by the teeth of any large theropod.
A possible function in deterring predators by making the
animal appear larger has been suggested, but again it would
not explain why Stegosaurus has large plates and those of
the contemporaneous Kentrurosaurus and others are much
smaller.
Pachycephalosaur domes have been assumed to have
been used in head-butting, ever since Colbert’s (1955) casual
Figure 3 Cladogram of advanced ceratopsians, after Dodson et al. suggestion (review in Maryanska, Chapman & Weishampel,
(2004). No specific functional or behavioral hypotheses are currently 2004). However, histological studies have shown that the
proposed (or have been tested) to account for evolutionary trends in columnar cell structure of these domes would not have
the features related to the bizarre cranial characters of these taxa. deflected the forces incurred in battering, as reasonably
proposed by Sues (1978) on the basis of biomechanical
processes of the anterior caudal vertebrae, despite some models of gross anatomy. The spongy bone that was
limits in vertical mobility (Vickaryous et al., 2004). thought to be protective of the brain during head-butting,
Most attributions of defense to the frills of neoceratop- by analogy to similar bone in bighorn sheep (Galton, 1970),
sians have focused on Triceratops (Fig. 3). This is apparently is actually characteristic of juvenile skulls; the skulls of
because Triceratops has prominent orbital horns as well as a adults, in which most head-butting would have been ex-
solid frill, so its function in ‘jousting’ is easily visualized (e.g. pected to occur, have compact bone in their external cortices
Farke, 2004). However, Triceratops is virtually (along with (Goodwin & Horner, 2004). Moreover, the spongy bone of
Avaceratops) the only neoceratopsian with a solid frill, juvenile skulls is organized in such a way that reflects radial
which is also the shortest among large neoceratopsians growth of the bone, which indicates rapid growth (Francil-
(Fig. 3). Other large neoceratopsians have substantial open- lon-Vieillot et al., 1990: p. 512). Rather than deflecting
ings in their frills, which would have been of little use in concussive forces from the brain cavity, this radial organiza-
defense. It now turns out that the adult Triceratops is in fact tion would have more likely directed them into the brain
what has been called Torosaurus, and its frill is not only cavity (Goodwin & Horner, 2004).
fenestrated but also quite thin, as in other neoceratopsians Maryanska et al. (2004) recently renewed the argument
(Scannella & Horner, in press). We hypothesize that, for mechanical agonistic behavior, but their analysis had no

Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London 7
‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs K. Padian and J. R. Horner

control for ontogeny or sexual dimorphism, so there is no (2009) noted that Hypacrosaurus altispinus had a particu-
support for assigning male status to larger and thicker larly derived and convoluted nasal chamber.
domes as they did. Moreover, the knobs and spikes that
ornamented some pachycephalosaur skulls (such as Stygi- Thermoregulation
moloch) would not have been visible until the heads were
lowered, and in any case could not have been involved in Only two kinds of dinosaurian structures have been pro-
combat (Goodwin, Rosner & Johnson, 1998). For these posed as thermoregulatory structures. The first is the plates
reasons a function in combat for both the domes and of stegosaurs. Main et al. (2005) showed that the explana-
ornamentation is implausible. Moreover, there is now evi- tion hypothesized for stegosaurs (Buffrenil et al., 1986)
dence that Stygimoloch was a subadult form of Pachycepha- could not be completely eliminated for Stegosaurus itself
losaurus, which has somewhat less extreme spikes than but was unlikely to apply to related taxa, so there was no
Stygimoloch, thus casting doubt on the functional interpre- evidence of the evolution of a functional adaptation in the
tation (Horner & Goodwin, 2009). group. The other example is the frills of ceratopsians; like
the plates of stegosaurs, these structures bear numerous
superficial vascular grooves that could be interpreted as
Communication conductors of blood vessels that could modify body core
Weishampel’s (1981, 1997) study of Parasaurolophus de- temperatures (Rigby Jr, 1990). However, this hypothesis has
scribed above was the first example of an explicit test of a never been rigorously tested, despite some intriguing evi-
hypothesis that a particular structure functioned in commu- dence (Barrick et al., 1998), and it is more conservative to
nication. As noted, this function may apply to this genus, suppose that the blood vessels nourished the rapid growth of
but it has not been proposed and tested for other lambeo- frills and plates, which seem to have become more elabo-
saurines until recently, when Evans, Ridgely & Witmer rated at the sub-adult stage (Horner & Marshall, 2002;
(2009) examined Lambeosaurus, Corythosaurus and Hypa- Dodson et al., 2004; Main et al., 2005).
crosaurus. They showed, as Weishampel (1981) had done
using Lophorhothon, that the ear region was capable of Sensory
hearing the low-frequency sounds that Weishampel calcu-
lated might have been produced by the resonating crests of Ostrom (1961, 1962) proposed that the crest of Parasaur-
these hadrosaurs. However, phylogenetic analysis of lam- olophus-enhanced olfaction: that is, an extended nasal
beosaurines (Horner, Weishampel & Forster, 2004) shows epithelium with sensory cells may have improved the ani-
no apparent trends in selection for improvement of the mal’s ability to smell. However, as Hopson (1975) noted,
features related to this function (Fig. 4), and Evans et al. lambeosaurine crest variability is too great to be explained
simply by selection for olfaction. Moreover, lambeosaurines
had no particularly specialized or enlarged olfactory lobes in
the brain, compared with other dinosaurs (Ostrom, 1961;
Corythosaurus Evans et al., 2009).

Procuring food
Bizarre structures such as tusks are used by some animals to
Hypacrosaurus procure food, but to our knowledge no such function has
been seriously proposed or tested for dinosaurs.

Lambeosaurus
Display
Display functions can be divided broadly into antagonistic
versus attractive: the repulsion of various threats versus the
attraction of potential mates (Table 1). But sometimes, as in
many mammals and some birds, these functions are related
Parasaurolophus (Darwin, 1871). Attraction only applies to the other sex of
the same species, but not all structures involved here fall into
the category of sexual selection.

Tsintaosaurus Interspecific
Hypotheses about structures that may play a role in repel-
Figure 4 Cladogram of lambeosaurine hadrosaurs, after Horner et al. ling potential predators are difficult to test. Buffrenil et al.
(2004). No specific functional or behavioral hypotheses are currently (1986) determined that the plates of stegosaurs were not well
proposed (or have been tested) to account for evolutionary trends in constructed to resist the bites of predators such as Allo-
the features related to the bizarre cranial characters of these taxa. saurus. The plates may have made the animals look larger,

8 Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London
K. Padian and J. R. Horner ‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs

and this function may also be attributed to most bizarre phenomena associated with competition for mates and
cranial structures of dinosaurs, as well as to the plates of reproductive success are interesting and important; but they
ankylosaurs (Carpenter, 1997). However, it is difficult to are not strictly part of sexual selection.) Unfortunately, this
know how to test this hypothesis. Moreover, the evolution- degree of sexual dimorphism, typical of birds and some
ary literature suggests that structures hypothesized to repel mammals, has not been sufficiently established for dino-
predators in living forms, whether by aposematic mimicry or saurs.
agonistic display, do not appear to enjoy long-term success (iii) Social selection: This concept (West-Eberhard, 1983)
unless the threat they promise can be fulfilled (Futuyma, was recently applied to dinosaurs by Hieronymus et al.
2009). (2009), who argued persuasively that the nasal cornifications
of centrosaurine ceratopsians were progressively selected for
larger size and broader display. According to them, ‘social
Intraspecific
selection occurs when there is differential success in within-
(i) Intrasexual: Females seldom contest each other, except to species competition for any limited resource.’ Two problems
establish social hierarchies (as in some mammals that travel with this definition, as applied to fossils, are that within-
in social groups or herds), but males commonly contest species phenomena can almost never be observed, and
males, among both invertebrates (notably arthropods) and competition is particularly difficult to establish in extinct
vertebrates (Darwin, 1871). In general, territory and re- forms (Benton, 1996). On the other hand, it is possible to
sources form the basis of male competition in mammals identify structures that can plausibly have functioned only
and in birds. Possession of resources is usually linked to in social interaction (as opposed to food gathering, thermo-
competitive superiority among males, and this advantage in regulation, etc.) and that are not sexually dimorphic (so are
turn makes males more able to secure females, or more not related to sexual selection), as Hieronymus et al. (2009)
attractive to females, because females are thought to per- did for centrosaurine nasal horns. However, in any case
ceive greater advantage in mating with these males. (Some social selection reduces to a kind of natural selection.
birds short-circuit the process or use a proxy to attract Moreover, these authors do not accurately distinguish
females through colorful feathers or eloquent songs [Dar- social selection and species recognition. They state (2009:
win, 1871; Andersson, 1994].) Some bizarre structures in 1394) that ‘species recognition traits are under selection only
extinct dinosaurs may have threatened rivals, but this is in the earliest stages of courtship during mating’, following
difficult to test without direct knowledge of behaviors that West-Eberhard (1983); but species recognition is simply a
are not preserved in the fossil record. matter of possessing traits that allow an individual to
(ii) Intersexual: The principal means of intersexual dis- recognize others of its species, for many functions besides
play is display for mates, traditionally called sexual display. breeding. They also state that ‘species recognition traits are
Sexual display usually implies sexual selection, and explana- only expected to occur in closely related sympatric species,’
tions of sexual selection must be evaluated much like those as opposed to being able to ‘diverge in allopatric isolated
for mechanical adaptations. In contemporary populations, populations,’ but in our view species recognition can begin
sexual selection often acts on minor features and elaborates at the population level and can easily diverge in populations
them (Mendelson & Shaw, 2005); intense sexual selection of a single species, especially if the selective change is
can result in runaway selection (Futuyma, 2009) and (or) anagenetic.
divergent selection (Kroodsma et al., 1985; Price, 1998). Contrary to West-Eberhard (1983), species recognition
Evolutionary theory holds that this kind of divergence can does not entail ‘reproductive character displacement,’ or
result in speciation (Futuyma, 2009), and that like natural necessarily any features that relate to mating, reproduction,
selection, sexual selection can be responsible for patterns of or competition among individuals of a species (Mayr, 1963).
sorting in clades (Vrba, 1984; Sampson, 1999). This could be Those other terms are the provenance of mate recognition,
shown if the characters subject to sexual selection show non- social selection, and natural selection. She rightfully criti-
random trends in clades (though the variation of the trends cizes earlier work that attributed to species recognition
themselves does not have to be directional or trendlike). many phenomena due to sexual selection or social selection
A problem with invoking sexual display as the explana- (such as the hypothesis that signal distinctiveness should be
tion of bizarre structures can be traced to Darwin’s (1871) reduced on islands and in isolated (allopatric) populations
original formation of the problem of sexual selection. (West-Eberhard, 1983: 165). That was sorted out with
Darwin emphasized that sexual selection could only apply further experimental work, but it does not nullify the con-
when one sex bears structures used in intersexual display (or cept of species recognition or imply that it is indistinguish-
agonistic behavior in intrasexual interaction). In other able from these other processes. This confusion aside, it is
words, sexual selection cannot be invoked without discrete, possible to assess the predicted effects of species recognition
qualitative features of sexual dimorphism. (We acknowledge and to separate them from those of other hypotheses.
that many neobiologists [apparently originating with West- (iv) Species recognition – Under the explanation of species
Eberhard’s, 1983 conflation of the concepts] feel that sexual recognition, bizarre structures would have no apparent
dimorphism is not necessary for sexual selection, but Dar- mechanical function and would not specifically evolve to
win defined the concept in this way and by definition he attract members of the opposite sex for mating (viz., Vrba,
cannot be wrong. This does not deny that various other 1984; Paterson, 1993); rather, they make it easier for

Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London 9
‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs K. Padian and J. R. Horner

individuals to recognize others of the same (and different) sented by width of the femur head: Raath, 1990: Fig. 7.8).
species. That is, the bizarre structures communicate to other The size-frequency distribution of femoral ‘morphs’ is also
individuals a variety of possible associational cues, includ- non-overlapping with respect to the femoral head width
ing species identification, potential protection and social (Raath, 1990: Fig. 7.10). Simply put, there are no small
habits and the appropriateness of potential mates. They are ‘robust’ morphs. Moreover, these examples are not sexual
positive indicators of beneficial social affiliations. There can dimorphism in the sense established by Darwin (and John
be a strong ontogenetic component to this process: young Hunter before him); if valid sexually, they are simply slight
neoceratopsians, pachycephalosaurs and lambeosaurs sexual differences, so they cannot be invoked to support
lacked the extent of cranial ornaments of fully grown sexual selection.
individuals, although they had rudimentary development, An alternate possibility, that these features could be
and it appears that in many cases these ornaments were ontogenetic, is suggested by Raath’s data. A broader tro-
rather rapidly developed at or around the attainment of chanter (and possibly thicker cortex, though the correlation
adult size. Larger members of a species, whether male or has not been statistically assessed) may have been acquired
female, and whether or not socially dominant, thus advertise by both males and females as they reached sexual maturity.
their biological affiliation. Sexual dimorphism has also been suggested for tyranno-
It is often difficult to differentiate among hypotheses of saurs (Carpenter, 1990; Larson, 1997), but Carr (1999) has
species recognition, social selection and mate recognition, shown that many apparently dichotomous differences in the
even in living animals. All three are forms of intra-species craniofacial skeleton, such as numbers and forms of teeth,
recognition, but less general and also different in critical are purely ontogenetic (as may be the case for Syntarsus), so
respects: it is first necessary to recognize other members of the ‘gracile’ forms are simply juveniles. We suspect that this
the species, and then to recognize (in the right seasonal and will hold for other dinosaurian species in which minor
ontogenetic contexts, because mating in most species is not variations in size and structure are found, rather than the
year-round and does not involve all members of the popula- discrete structures specified by Darwin (1859, 1871) for true
tion) individuals that could serve as potential mates or sexual selection.
rivals. This is a different process than developing gender- Other bizarre structures in theropods include cranial
specific structures that assist in the specific attraction of crests (Dilophosaurus, Monolophosaurus, Cryolophosaurus)
mates, or the repulsion of intraspecific competitors for and horns (Carnotaurus and incipient frontal structures in
mates, which is the domain of sexual selection. Below we allosaurids and tyrannosaurids); however, neither sexual
propose some tests of the species recognition hypothesis that dimorphism nor ontogenetic maturity can yet been exam-
distinguish it from the sexual selection hypothesis. In extinct ined statistically for these features.
animals only hard parts generally provide evidence, and so The argument about alleged gracile and robust dimorphic
any evolutionary hypotheses must have an evidentiary basis adult forms follows, ceteris paribus, for the studies cited on
in preservable structures. prosauropods by Galton & Upchurch (2004a: p. 257), who
provided no statistical demonstration of dimorphism, and
by Weishampel & Chapman (1990), who reached inconclu-
Dinosaurs and sexual dimorphism sive results for Plateosaurus.
Because sexual dimorphism has been so extensively invoked
to explain ‘bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs (e.g. Chapman
et al., 1997), we address it in detail here.
Ornithischia
Sample sizes in species of stegosaurs, ankylosaurs, pachyce-
phalosaurs and most ornithopods are too small to test the
Saurischia
hypothesis of sexual dimorphism; it has been proposed for
Sexual dimorphism has been proposed for several theropods hadrosaurs and ceratopsians. Goodwin (1990) noted that
(mostly basal forms assigned to ‘ceratosaurs’) and ‘prosaur- the sample of pachycephalosaurs was too small to permit
opods’ (a paraphyletic group of basal sauropodomorphs), statistical evaluation of sexual dimorphism, and Goodwin &
on the basis of an apparent difference between robust and Horner (2004); Horner & Goodwin, (2009) showed that
gracile forms (Colbert (1989, 1990) on Coelophysis; Raath most observed variation was ontogenetic, based on indepen-
(1990) on Syntarsus). Differences have been noted in the dent analysis of stage of maturity using the degree of fusion
relative thicknesses of bone walls, and in the morphology of of the cranial sutures and the progressive growth and
trochanters. Unfortunately the statistical evidence that sup- reduction of specific cranial features.
ports sexual dimorphism as an explanation for these differ-
ences is problematic. For example, Colbert (1990) produced
Hadrosaurs
considerable evidence for ontogenetic change in proportions
in Coelophysis, but his inference of sexual dimorphism Sexual dimorphism in hadrosaurs has long been accepted by
(widely accepted by other workers) was based on only two authors (e.g. Davitashvili, 1961; Hopson, 1975; Molnar,
specimens. In Syntarsus, the difference between the ‘gracile’ 1977; Weishampel, 1997; Carrano, Janis & Sepkoski, 1999);
and ‘robust’ morphs of the iliofemoralis trochanter is almost the supporting evidence can be traced almost entirely to
non-overlapping with respect to the size of the bone (repre- Dodson’s (1975) study of two genera of lambeosaurine

10 Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London
K. Padian and J. R. Horner ‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs

hadrosaurs. Dodson’s morphometric analysis suggested (Sometimes, independently) (Rarely)


that ‘procheneosaurs’ were merely juveniles of larger spe- Non-avian dinosaurs Aves
cies, and he reduced three genera and 12 species to two (No) (No)
genera (Lambeosaurus and Corythosaurus) and three species. Squamata Crocodylia
In these three species he thought he could detect sexual
differences in some cranial characters, although not at all in
postcrania; and no signal was found in most cranial char-
Sphenodontida
acters. This is a problem because there is no independent (No)
means to correlate size with age, or to identify age of a (No)
specimen on the basis of other evidence. Evans & Reisz Chelonia
(2007) have shown that this variation is ontogenetic or
characterizes chronospecies that do not overlap with each
other temporally. And moreover, these are only slight Dimorphism in ‘bizarre’ skeletal structures?
proportional differences, not discrete structural ones.
Figure 5 We ask whether there is any support in the extant phyloge-
netic bracket of extinct dinosaurs for a broader distribution of
Ceratopsians dimorphism in ‘bizarre’ skeletal structures. This question would not
be affirmatively answered by size dimorphism (as in crocodiles and
As You & Dodson (2004) note, presumed sexual differences
some other reptiles), nor by soft part structures (as in birds) or
have been postulated in Protoceratops andrewsi (Dodson,
behaviors, which cannot be assessed in fossils. The answer is
1976) and (less independently) Protoceratops hellenikorhinus
ostensibly negative.
for both cranial and post-cranial features, and some features
of the frill are dimorphic. However, as noted above, the
extent of variation in the supposedly dimorphic features was EPB does not support sexual dimorphism in non-avian
statistical (as opposed to presence/absence features of true dinosaurs on the grounds of homological comparison.
dimorphism), and although they may have supported more
conspicuous sexually dimorphic features in soft part anat-
omy that is not preserved, the statistical argument on the Comparative tests of the mate
basis of hard parts is insufficient. The kind of variation competition and species recognition
appears much more akin to the sort of differences that
characterize male and female crocodiles, which differ from
hypotheses
each other mainly at adult size, where it is mostly a matter of Vrba (1984) used the example of degree of horn differentia-
relative robusticity (Webb et al., 1978; Chabreck & Joanen, tion, which is usually greater in alcelaphine bovids (harte-
1979). beest, wildebeest, etc.) than in the related aepycerotines
If dimorphism were important in small basal ceratop- (impalas), to suggest an explanation for the greater species
sians, it should be emphasized or at least detectable in larger, diversity through time of the former clade. Sampson (1999)
more derived forms, but this does not seem to be the case. suggested that sexual selection, not just natural selection,
Lehman (1990) suggested a pattern of sexual dimorphism in could be the motor of enhanced diversity in certain sub-
Chasmosaurus and related species that could be traced clades over others. He proposed a Mate Recognition Hy-
through later ontogeny, but the small sample sizes, incom- pothesis (MRH) by which selection for positive recognition
plete preservation, and lack of association of much of this of mates could lead to increased differentiation of popula-
material, as Lehman noted, makes it difficult to evaluate tions and eventually greater rates of speciation in some
hypotheses about sexual differences, even if they are ac- lineages over others. This idea has a strong backing in recent
cepted. Ryan et al.’s (2001) study of a ceratopsian bone bed, research on the value of sexual selection in promoting
where dimorphism could be presumed to emerge, turned up differentiation of populations in a single species (e.g. An-
no significant patterns. A recent review of Ceratopsia dersson, 1994; Price, 1998; Mendelson & Shaw, 2005). But
(Dodson et al., 2004) did not accept sexual dimorphism as like any other hypothesis that involves sexual selection, a
a general feature in this clade of dinosaurs. Soft-part degree of sexual dimorphism is required that is not found in
features and behaviors that are not preserved in extinct taxa dinosaurs.
may well have contributed to sexual selection (e.g. Sampson, We propose that species recognition is a simpler and more
1997). However, to invoke them for extinct groups of general explanation for the patterns seen in the distribution
dinosaurs is outside the pale of homological and analogical of bizarre structures in dinosaurs. Structures that promote
comparison. species recognition allow individuals of a single species to
As for fossil birds, which are dinosaurs, we have almost recognize each other and distinguish conspecifics from
no information about dimorphism; long tail feathers in the members of other species. Advantages include banding
basal avialian Confuciusornis are suggestive (Chiappe et al., together for protection from predators, parental care and
1999), but this is not enough to establish evolutionary the possible location of mates. As explained above (Display:
polarity. Because dimorphism (and not just inter-sexual Intraspecific), this is a broader and more hierarchical func-
difference) is generally low in other reptiles (Fig. 5), the tion than that proposed by the Mate Recognition

Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London 11
‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs K. Padian and J. R. Horner

are expressed. Taxonomic diversity is not necessarily higher


under either model.
Second, there could be evidence that at some point,
several closely related species with divergent bizarre struc-
tures lived at the same time in environments that at least
partly overlapped. In other words, several contemporaneous
sympatric, parapatric, or partly allopatric species existed
when these lineages were diverging. These differences might
have been positively selected as a means to reinforce
associations (including mating) with appropriate conspeci-
fics. However, lineages may also continue to diverge in
Figure 6 Expected differences in macroevolutionary patterns of mor- isolation from others simply because this kind of evolution-
phology between regimes governed by natural selection (or Fisherian ary change follows a natural flexibility of phenotype. So,
sexual selection) and species recognition. Although these are ex- white-crowned sparrows diverge at the local populational
tremes of a continuum of patterns, it would be expected that natural level at a very rapid rate, changing songs in ways instantly
selection would cause more or less linear morphological trends recognizable to human birdwatchers as well as to the birds
through time as the function of a structure improved. Sexual selection
themselves (Baptista, Bell & Trail, 1993; Bell, Trail &
might also direct the orthal elaboration of a difference into a morpho-
Baptista, 1998). These songs both reinforce populational
logical trend. But when species recognition is selected for, the object
identity and allow mate recognition. But the populations
is to be recognizably different, not necessarily functionally better or
may not overlap geographically to any great extent. Drift
more visually elaborate; so a range of morphology is possible, as long
as the result is divergence. From Main et al. (2005).
may also play an important role, especially in small popula-
tions with some isolation (Mayr, 1963; Eldredge & Gould,
1972). Many evolutionary changes occur in lineages because
Hypothesis, and it does not require sexual dimorphism. It certain organisms have the evolutionary ‘habit’ of changing
can also involve many other kinds of cues than visual, let regularly, not because they are adjusting to myriad contin-
alone those related to bizarre structures. The fact that these uous demands of natural or sexual selection. Female pre-
various functions exist apart from simple mate recognition ferences can change quickly, and even ‘anticipate’ desirable
is witnessed by the appearance of bizarre structures, often in variations that later appear in males (Futuyma, 2009).
incipient form, in individuals not involved in mating at all. In this way, we predict that the species recognition
If species recognition has been important in influencing hypothesis can account for both the differentiation of
macroevolutionary trends, it should have some empirical related sympatric species and the anagenetic change in
tests by which its effects can be differentiated from those of lineages that may indeed characterize much of dinosaurian
other hypotheses. We propose two. evolution, including putative ontogenetic stages and sexual
First, the pattern of diversification of bizarre structures in dimorphs (e.g. Evans, 2007).
clades should be relatively random: it should not show
trends that could ostensibly be related to selection (merely Discussion: explaining bizarre
size-related change would not qualify). An example, neces-
sarily simplified, is presented in Fig. 6. In the diagram at left,
structures in dinosaurs
the pattern of change documented through time shows clear Morphological diversification in the bizarre structures of
directional trends. This kind of change is readily explained dinosaurs does not seem to show clear patterns of direc-
by selective forces, whether natural or sexual. The standard tional evolution within clades. To date, no satisfactory
model is of variation in populations, followed by directional adaptive explanation has been proposed and tested for the
selection. This can represent improvement of a function evolution of bizarre structures in any dinosaurian clade (not
(natural selection) or continued trends in mate preference simply an individual species). The most recent phylogenetic
(sexual selection; runaway sexual selection is an extreme analyses of these clades do not reveal trends in the morphol-
condition). A gradation of forms is expected both within ogy of these structures that indicate any directionality that
and among lineages: gradual improvement is expected in a can be attributed to adaptive improvement or sexual selec-
single lineage, whereas adaptive divergence (for ecological tion (Weishampel et al., 2004). We stress that this does not
or sexually selective reasons) should characterize differences deny the importance of mechanical adaptation, sexual selec-
among lineages. tion, or any other macroevolutionary process in dinosaurs; it
In the diagram at right in Fig. 6, however, there is no simply concludes that to date there is no evidence that it has
obvious trend in evolutionary change; the only objective of shaped any bizarre morphology in a clade. The fossil record
evolutionary change is to make a lineage different from (like the living record) provides only a sample of the
other closely related lineages (e.g. Figs 3 and 4; Main et al., diversity that has existed, and our phylogenetic reconstruc-
2005: fig. 10). This pattern represents what would be more tions would be very different with a different or more
likely expected from the species recognition model. The complete sample.
direction and degree of difference are not important or The second test of the Species Recognition model sup-
predictable; not all possible dimensions of morphospace poses that several contemporaneous lineages in a clade with

12 Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London
K. Padian and J. R. Horner ‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs

Table 2 Contemporaneous occurrences of several species of related dinosaurs with ‘bizarre’ structuresa
Early Jurassic: Cryolophosaurus (Sinemurian-Pliensbachian, Ant), Dilophosaurus (Sinemurian-Pliensbachian, NAm, China)
Middle Jurassic: Stegosaurus, Lexovisaurus, ?Omosaurus (Bathonian-Callovian, Brit); Lexovisaurus (Callovian, Fr, Ger); Huayangosaurus,
unidentified stegosaur (Bathonian-Callovian, China)
Late Jurassic: Five stegosaur species (Morrison Fm., Utah), four stegosaur species (Morrison Fm., Colorado), five stegosaur species (Morrison
Fm., Wyoming) (all Kimmeridgian-Tithonian, NAm); stegosaurs, not diverse (mostly Dacentrurus armatus) (Oxfordian-Kimmeridgian, Brit;
Kimmeridgian-Tithonian, Fr, Port); at least four stegosaur species (?Oxfordian, Upper Shaximiao Fm., China)b
Early Cretaceous: Six named ankylosaur species (Cambridge Greensand, Brit), two ankylosaur species (Wessex Fm., Brit) (late Aptian).
Many species of avialians (Jiufotang and Yixian Fms., China).c
Late Cretaceous: Five neoceratopsian (‘protoceratopsids’ and relatives) species (?mid-Campanian, Djadokhta Fm., Asia).
At least four ankylosaur species, at least 10 hadrosaur species, four pachycephalosaur species, at least 10 neoceratopsian species
(Campanian, Dinosaur Park Fm., NAm);d equal or less diversity of these taxa in coeval and slightly younger formations such as the Bearpaw
Shale and the Horseshoe Canyon Fms.;e at least five pachycephalosaurs, at least three neoceratopsians (Maastrichtian, Hell Creek and Lance
Fms., Montana and neighboring states)
a
All information from Chapter 24 of Weishampel et al. (2004). Note that even though several related taxa may be present in a formation, they have
not necessarily been identified from the same localities or stratigraphic equivalences within formations. For implications, see discussion of Fig. 7
in the text.
b
The dominance of stegosaur diversity has come to light mostly in the past few decades. However, in some formations, non-‘bizarre’ dinosaurs,
such as theropods, sauropods and iguanodontids, are more diverse in the Middle and Late Jurassic.
c
Ankylosaurs replace stegosaurs in the Early Cretaceous as the most diverse ‘bizarre’ dinosaur clade. However, the non-‘bizarre’ iguanodontids
are still very diverse in places, and in China a great diversity of basal birds has been discovered in the past decade: were feathers ‘bizarre’
structures that functioned as devices of species recognition?
d
This formation also contains, among non-‘bizarre’ taxa, three tyrannosaurids, at least two ornithomimosaurs, at least three oviraptorosaurs, and
at least two dromaeosaurs.
e
The diversity of non-‘bizarre’ taxa in these formations is proportionally comparable to those in the previous note. ?Denotes uncertain identification.
Ant, Antarctica; Brit, Britain; Fm., formation; Fr, France; Ger, Germany; NAm, North America.

bizarre structures should overlap geographically to some


degree during their divergence. The geographic and tempor-
al distributions of some dinosaurian clades, including cer-
atopsians, pachycephalosaurs, lambeosaurines and
ankylosaurs, suggest that these bizarre structures appeared
in dinosaurian groups that lived at nearly the same time in
nearly the same areas (Table 2). Until recently, a salient
exception appeared to be the stegosaurs, whose low diversity
was anomalous to this general model. However, new dis-
coveries of stegosaurs have increased our knowledge of their
diversity: Carpenter (2001) estimates that at least five
stegosaur species are now known from the Morrison For-
mation of the western United States (although Galton &
Upchurch (2004b) recognize only three). This would appear
to simplify the problem, but there is an additional caveat: Figure 7 The ‘ghost’ of species recognition. Consider five species
in a lineage, distributed sequentially through time; the real extent of
the species are not all contemporaneous (K. Carpenter, pers.
temporal overlap among forms is unknown because the record
comm., 2004), and there may be geographic differentiation
is necessarily incomplete. In the figure at left, these five forms are
as well within the Morrison.
part of a single anagenetic lineage. In this case there is no evidence
The lack of contemporaneity could have several explana-
that species recognition influenced the diversification of lineages
tions, including insufficient stratigraphic sampling to estab-
(speciation), although species recognition, drift, or several other
lish that more of these species lived at the same time than it kinds of selective factors could account for the anagenetic change.
now appears. However, another approach is phylogenetic. If In the figure at right, the same five forms belong to separate line-
these five species turned out to be morphotypes of a single ages with successively ancient divergence points. This pattern
anagenetic lineage, there would indeed be no evidence for provides a basis for testing the hypothesis that species recogni-
contemporaneity. Would the hypothesis of species recogni- tion, among other factors, may have been involved in the diversifica-
tion thereby be weakened (Fig. 7, left)? In fact, such a result tion of the clade. Although as in the figure at left, no more than one of
would weaken a hypothesis of anti-hybridization, but it the five species is found at the same time as another, phylogeny
would not weaken or test the hypothesis of positive assorta- reveals their ghost lineages, and allows the inference that, for
tive mating (Paterson, 1993). However, if phylogenetic example, at time X at least four of the lineages may have been
analysis revealed that these species indeed represented contemporaneous.

Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London 13
‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs K. Padian and J. R. Horner

different lineages, and their ‘ghost ranges’ indicated that on grounds that are more stringent than weak analogies to
they must have diverged from others at an earlier time, then very different living organisms.
at one time the test of contemporaneous species would have We stress that no evolutionary hypothesis can be re-
been passed (Fig. 7, right). It is not impossible that such a garded as a ‘default’ explanation (i.e. if a certain class of
pattern could also indicate other processes than species explanation fails, then another one is automatically
recognition, such as sexual or social selection, but in concert strengthened or must be accepted by default). Hypotheses
with non-directional evolutionary change the indication must be independently tested, or they are not scientific. In
would be rather more strongly in favor of species recogni- many or most cases, definitive tests will not be possible. We
tion. Phylogenetic analysis and further biostratigraphic have proposed two tests of a Species Recognition hypoth-
sampling can test this hypothesis. esis, and there may be others. In our view, most dinosaurian
Finally, we return to the test of the Mate Recognition bizarre structures pass these tests, but they do not pass the
Hypothesis that Sampson (1999) proposed. We found that tests of adaptation or of sexual display. The importance of
in every criterion, mostly related to higher rates of specia- social selection (Hieronymus et al., 2009) remains to be
tion and habitat shifts, the concept of ‘species recognition’ tested in dinosaurs beyond individual species. This does not
could be substituted for the terms related to sexual selection mean that these structures were not adaptive or used in
without any apparent difference in results. The exception attracting mates; we simply have no evidence on these points
was his fourth criterion (speciation will often be correlated at present.
with vicariance events rather than the formation of periph- Our hypothesis is that the Species Recognition Hypoth-
eral isolates), which we suggest is untestable in the fossil esis is simpler and more general in explaining the evolution
record, and in any case would not discriminate between of bizarre structures in dinosaurs than those of mechanical
sexual selection and species recognition as a cause. In function, social selection, or sexual selection/mate recogni-
summary, the criteria for MRH and SRH are very similar tion. Rigorous tests of these complementary evolutionary
in outline; but in any given case the tests of these characters, hypotheses should be applied to other lineages besides
and perhaps the characters themselves, would be quite dinosaurs. Bizarre structures are common in many fishes,
different operationally for the same animals. For example, as well as other reptiles. In birds, sexual dimorphism,
the horns of ceratopsians might satisfy all four (five) criteria display and selection are well-established phenomena that
listed above for both MRH and SRH, but would not pass have clearly had a very strong role in shaping avian evolu-
the test of high sexual dimorphism required for sexual tion. The expression of bizarre structures in mammals,
selection; on the other hand, they appear to pass the two notably ungulates, is entailed in a constellation of ecological
tests of the species recognition hypothesis (non-directional characteristics that greatly complicate their explanation
variation of bizarre structures and several sympatric spe- (Jarman, 1974; Perez-Barberia, Gordon & Pagel, 2002).
cies). Moreover, without a clear demonstration of sexual Finally, we emphasize that a given structure may have
dimorphism, the MRH reduces to the social selection several purposes, and that even in living animals it is often
hypothesis (Hieronymus et al., 2009). difficult to determine the uses of particular structures, their
evolutionary histories, and even how the animals are com-
municating. In this respect the hypotheses of paleobiologists
Conclusions are largely interpreting the shadows on the wall of Plato’s
Our purpose is not to insist that species recognition has been cave. We persist in efforts to explain these structures because
the only cause of the evolution of bizarre structures in they were of obvious use to their bearers, and this is in
dinosaurs, nor that adaptation, social selection and sexual principle discoverable.
selection have been unimportant in dinosaurian evolution.
We merely ask in each case: how would we test this? We
conclude that the hypotheses of mechanical function and
sexual display that have predominated for decades as gen-
eral explanations of the evolution of these structures in
Acknowledgments
dinosaurian clades are unfounded. When we test the hy- We thank S. Bar-David, J. Brashares, V. de Buffrenil, K.
pothesis that presumed functions of these structures have Carpenter, P. Cross, P. Dodson, J.O. Farlow, E. Hebets, T.
evolved in their clades, we find no evidence; hence the Hieronymus, R. Irmis, C. Janis, E. Lacey, B. Lundrigan, S.
notions that these structures are ‘adaptations’ fail the Patek, A. de Ricqlès, M.J. Ryan, S.M. Sampson, K.M.
criteria proposed by evolutionary biologists (Greene, 1986; Scott, A.B. Shabel, L.M. Witmer and many other colleagues
Williams, 1992; Rose & Lauder, 1996; Padian, 2001). and reviewers for constructive comments and suggestions,
Furthermore, sexual dimorphism has not been strongly without implying their agreement with all our points. UCB
established for any bizarre structures in dinosaurian undergraduates Jasmeet K. Dhaliwal and Sylvia Moses
lineages, even though mild dimorphism has been statistically provided research support. R. Irmis and A. Lee provided
demonstrated in at least one lineage and may be plausible in technical support. This work was supported by the Univer-
others. If criteria of sexual behavior other than those based sity of California Museum of Paleontology and the Com-
on sexual selection (which requires sexual dimorphism: mittee on Research of the University of California,
Darwin (1871) are to be proposed, they should be justified Berkeley. This is UCMP Contribution No. 2012.

14 Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London
K. Padian and J. R. Horner ‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs

References Colbert, E.H. (1955). Evolution of the vertebrates. New York:


Wiley.
Andersson, M.B. (1994). Sexual selection. Princeton: Prince-
Colbert, E.H. (1989). The Triassic dinosaur Coelophysis. Mus.
ton University Press.
N. Arizona Bull. 57, 1–160.
Baptista, L.F., Bell, D.A. & Trail, P.W. (1993). Song learning
Colbert, E.H. (1990). Variation in Coelophysis bauri. In
and production in the white-crowned sparrow: parallels
Dinosaur systematics: approaches and perspectives: 81–90.
with sexual imprinting. Neth. J. Zool. 43, 17–33.
Carpenter, K. & Currie, P.J. (Eds). Cambridge: Cambridge
Barrick, R.E., Stoskopf, M.K., Marcot, J.D., Russell, D.A. &
University Press.
Showers, W.J. (1998). The thermoregulatory functions of
Currie, P.J. & Padian, K. (Eds). (1997). Encyclopedia of
the Triceratops frill and horns: heat flow measured with dinosaurs. San Diego: Academic Press.
oxygen isotopes. J. Vertebr. Paleontol. 18, 746–750. Darwin, C. (1859). The origin of species by means of natural
Bell, D.A., Trail, P.W. & Baptista, L.F. (1998). Song learning selection. London: J. Murray.
and vocal tradition in Nuttall’s white-crowned sparrow. Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation
Anim. Behav. 55, 939–956. to sex. London: J. Murray.
Benton, M.J. (1996). On the nonprevalence of competitive Davitashvili, L.Sh. (1961). Teoriya polovogo otbora [The
replacement in the evolution of tetrapods. In Evolutionary theory of sexual selection]. Moscow: Izdatel’stvo Akademii
paleobiology: 185–210. Jablonski, D., Erwin, D.H. & Nauk (Academy of Sciences Press).
Lipps, J.H. (Eds). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Dodson, P. (1975). Taxonomic implications of relative
Brandon, R.N. (1996). Concepts and methods in evolutionary growth of lambeosaurine hadrosaurs. Syst. Zool. 24,
biology. New York: Cambridge University Press. 37–54.
Buffrenil, V. de, Farlow, J.O. & de Ricqlès, A. (1986). Growth Dodson, P. (1976). Quantitative aspects of relative growth
and function in Stegosaurus plates: evidence from bone and sexual dimorphism in Protoceratops. J. Paleontol. 50,
histology. Paleobiology 12, 459–473. 929–940.
Carpenter, K. (1990). Variation in Tyrannosaurus rex. In Dodson, P., Forster, C.A. & Sampson, S.D. (2004). Ceratop-
Dinosaur systematics: approaches and perspectives: sidae. In The Dinosauria, 2nd edn: 494–513. Weishampel,
141–145. Carpenter, K. & Currie, P.J. (Eds). Cambridge: D., Osmolska, H. & Dodson, P. (Eds). Berkeley: University
Cambridge University Press. of California Press.
Carpenter, K. (1997). Ankylosauria. In The complete dino- Eldredge, N. & Gould, S.J. (1972). Punctuated equilibria: an
saur: 307–316. Farlow, J.O. & Brett-Surman, M.K. (Eds). alternative to phyletic gradualism. In Models in paleobiol-
Bloomington: Indiana University Press. ogy: 82–115. Schopf, T.J.M. (Ed.). San Francisco: Free-
Carpenter, K. (1999). Eggs, nests, and baby dinosaurs: a look man, Cooper.
at dinosaur reproduction. Bloomington: Indiana University Endler, J.A. (1986). Natural selection in the wild. Princeton:
Press. Princeton University Press.
Carpenter, K. (Ed.). (2001). The armored dinosaurs. Bloo- Evans, D.C. (2007) Ontogeny and evolution of Lambeosaurine
mington: Indiana University Press. dinosaurs (Ornithischia: Hadrosauridae). PhD thesis,
Carpenter, K., Hirsch, K.F. & Horner, J.R. (Eds). (1994). University of Toronto, 497pp.
Dinosaur eggs and babies. New York: Cambridge Univer- Evans, D.C. & Reisz, R.R. (2007). Anatomy and relationships
sity Press. of Lambeosaurus magnicristatus, a crested hadrosaurid
Carr, T.D. (1999). Craniofacial ontogeny in Tyrannosauridae dinosaur (Ornithischia) from the Dinosaur Park Forma-
(Dinosauria, Coelurosauria). J. Vertebr. Paleontol. 19, tion, Alberta. J. Vertebr. Paleontol. 27, 373–393.
497–520. Evans, D.C., Ridgely, R. & Witmer, L.M. (2009). Endocra-
Carrano, M.T., Janis, C.M. & Sepkoski, J.J. (1999). Hadro- nial anatomy of lambeosaurine hadrosaurids (Dinosaur-
saurs as ungulate parallels: lost lifestyles and deficient data. ia:Ornithischia): a sensorineural perspective on cranial
Acta Palaeontol. Pol. 44, 237–261. crest function. Anat. Rec. 292, 1315–1337.
Chabreck, R.H. & Joanen, T. (1979). Growth rates of Amer- Farke, A.A. (2004). Horn use in Triceratops (Dinosauria:
ican alligators in Louisiana. Herpetologica 35, 51–57. Ceratopsidae): testing behavioral hypotheses using scale
Chapman, R., Weishampel, D.B., Hunt, G. & Rasskin-Gut- models. Palaeontol. Electron. 7. Available at http://palaeo-
man, D. (1997). Sexual dimorphism in dinosaurs. In electronica.org/2004_1/horn/issue1_04.htm
Dinofest International: proceedings of a symposium held at Farke, A.A., Wolff, E.D.S. & Tanke, D.H. (2009). Evidence
Arizona State University: 83–93. Wolberg, D.L., Stump, E. of combat in Triceratops. PLoS One 4, e4252. (Online DOI:
& Rosenberg, G.D. (Eds). Philadelphia: Academy of Nat- 10.1371/journal.pone.0004252).
ural Sciences. Farlow, J.O. & Brett-Surman, M.K. (Eds). (1997). The
Chiappe, L.M., Ji, S.-A., Ji, Q. & Norell, M.A. (1999). complete dinosaur. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
Anatomy and systematics of the Confuciusornithidae Farlow, J.O. & Dodson, P. (1974). The behavioral signifi-
(Theropoda: Aves) from the late Mesozoic of northeastern cance of frill and horn morphology in ceratopsian
China. Bull. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist. 242, 1–89. dinosaurs. Evolution 29, 353–361.

Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London 15
‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs K. Padian and J. R. Horner

Farlow, J.O., Thompson, C.V. & Rosner, D.E. (1976). Plates Horner, J.R. & Gorman, J. (1988). Digging dinosaurs: the
of the dinosaur Stegosaurus: forced convection heat loss search that unraveled the mystery of baby dinosaurs. New
fins? Science 192, 1123–1125. York: Workman.
Francillon-Vieillot, H., de Buffrénil, V., Castanet, J., Gérau- Horner, J.R. & Marshall, C.L. (2002). Keratin-covered
die, J., Meunier, F.J., Sire, J.Y., Zylberberg, L. & de dinosaur skulls. J. Vertebr. Paleontol. 22 (Suppl.), 67A.
Ricqlès, A. (1990). Microstructure and mineralization of Horner, J.R., Weishampel, D.B. & Forster, C.A. (2004).
vertebrate skeletal tissues. Skeletal biomineralization: pat- Hadrosauridae. In The Dinosauria, 2nd edn: 438–463.
terns, processes and evolutionary trends, Vol. 1: 471–530. Weishampel, D., Osmolska, H. & Dodson, P. (Eds).
Carter, J.G. (Ed.). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Futuyma, D.J. (2009). Evolution, 2nd edn. Sunderland: Jarman, P.J. (1974). The social organization of antelope in
Sinauer. relation to their ecology. Behaviour 68, 231–264.
Galton, P.M. (1970). Pachycephalosaurids: dinosaurian Kroodsma, D.E., Baker, M.C., Baptista, L.F. & Petrinovich,
battering rams. Discovery 6, 23–32. L. (1985). Vocal ‘‘dialects’’ in Nuttall’s white-crowned
Galton, P.M. & Upchurch, P. (2004a). Prosauropoda. In The sparrow. Curr. Ornithol. 2, 103–133.
Dinosauria, 2nd edn: 232–258. Weishampel, D., Osmolska, Larson, P. (1997). The king’s new clothes: a fresh look at
H. & Dodson, P. (Eds). Berkeley: University of California Tyrannosaurus rex. In Dinofest International: proceedings
Press. of a symposium held at Arizona State University: 65–71.
Galton, P.M. & Upchurch, P. (2004b). Stegosauria. In The Wolberg, D.L., Stump, E. & Rosenberg, G.D. (Eds).
Dinosauria, 2nd edn: 343–382. Weishampel, D., Osmolska, Philadelphia: Academy of Natural Sciences.
H. & Dodson, P. (Eds). Berkeley: University of California Lehman, T.M. (1990). The ceratopsian subfamily Chasmo-
Press. saurinae: sexual dimorphism and systematics. In Dinosaur
Goodwin, M.B. (1990). Morphometric landmarks of pachy-
systematics: approaches and perspectives: 211–229. Carpen-
cephalosaurid cranial material from the Judith River
ter, K. & Currie, P.J. (Eds). Cambridge: Cambridge
Formation of northcentral Montana. In Dinosaur
University Press.
systematics: approaches and perspectives: 189–201.
Main, R.P., de Ricqlès, A., Horner, J.R. & Padian, K. (2005).
Carpenter, K. & Currie, P.J. (Eds). Cambridge:
The evolution and function of thyreophoran dinosaur
Cambridge University Press.
scutes: implications for plate function in stegosaurs. Paleo-
Goodwin, M.B., Buchholz, E.A. & Johnson, R.E. (1998).
biology 31, 293–316.
Cranial anatomy and diagnosis of Stygimoloch spinifer
Maryanska, T., Chapman, R.E. & Weishampel, D.B. (2004).
(Ornithischia: Pachycephalosauria) with comments on
Pachycephalosauria. In The Dinosauria, 2nd edn: 464–477.
cranial display structures in agonistic behavior. J. Vertebr.
Weishampel, D., Osmolska, H. & Dodson, P. (Eds).
Paleontol. 18, 363–375.
Berkeley: University of California Press.
Goodwin, M.B. & Horner, J.R. (2004). Cranial histology of
Mayr, E. (1963). Animal Species and Evolution. Cambridge:
pachycephalosaurs (Ornithischia: Marginocephalia) re-
veals transitory structures inconsistent with head-butting Belknap Press, Harvard University.
behavior. Paleobiology 30, 253–267. Mendelson, T.C. & Shaw, K.L. (2005). Rapid speciation in an
Gould, S.J. (1974). The evolutionary significance of bizarre arthropod. Nature 433, 375.
structures: antler size and skull size in the ‘‘Irish Elk,’’ Molnar, R.E. (1977). Analogies in the evolution of display
Megaceros giganteus. Evolution 28, 191–220. structures in ornithopods and ungulates. Evol. Theory 3,
Greene, H.W. (1986). Diet and arboreality in the Emerald 165–190.
Monitor, Varanus prasinus, with comments on the study of Ostrom, J.H. (1961). Cranial morphology of the hadrosaurian
adaptation. Fieldiana Zool. 31, 1–12. dinosaurs of North America. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist. Bull.
Hickman, C.S. (1980). Gastropod radulae and the assessment 122, 35–186.
of form in evolutionary paleontology. Paleobiology 6, Ostrom, J.H. (1962). The cranial crests of hadrosaurian
276–294. dinosaurs. Postilla 62, 1–29.
Hieronymus, T.L., Witmer, L.M., Tanke, D.H. & Currie, P.J. Padian, K. (1982). Macroevolution and the origin of major
(2009). The facial integument of centrosaurine ceratopsids: adaptations: vertebrate flight as a paradigm for the analysis
morphological and histological correlates of novel skin of patterns. Proc. Third N. Am. Paleontol. Convention 2,
structures. Anat. Rec. 292, 1370–1396. 387–392.
Hopson, J.A. (1975). The evolution of cranial display struc- Padian, K. (1987). Presence of the dinosaur Scelidosaurus
tures in hadrosaurian dinosaurs. Paleobiology 1, 21–43. indicates Jurassic age for the Kayenta Formation (Glen
Horner, J.R. & Dobb, E. (1997). Dinosaur lives: unearthing an Canyon Group, northern Arizona). Geology 17, 438–441.
evolutionary saga. New York: HarperCollins. Padian, K. (1995). Form versus function: the evolution of a
Horner, J.R. & Goodwin, M.B. (2009). Extreme cranial onto- dialectic. In Functional morphology in vertebrate paleontol-
geny in the Upper Cretaceous dinosaur Pachycephalosaurus. ogy: 264–277. Thomason, J.J. (Ed.). Cambridge: Cam-
PLoS One 4, e7626, doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007626 bridge University Press.

16 Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London
K. Padian and J. R. Horner ‘Bizarre structures’ in dinosaurs

Padian, K. (2001). Cross-testing adaptive hypotheses: phylo- Vickaryous, M.K., Maryanska, T. & Weishampel, D.B.
genetic analysis and the origin of bird flight. Am. Zool. 41, (2004). Ankylosauria. In The Dinosauria, 2nd edn:
598–607. 363–392. Weishampel, D., Osmolska, H. & Dodson, P.
Padian, K. & Horner, J.R. (2002). Typology versus transfor- (Eds). Berkeley: University of California Press.
mation in the origin of birds. Trends Ecol. Evol. 17, Vrba, E.S. (1984). Evolutionary pattern and process in the
120–124. sister-group Alcelaphini-Aepycerotini (Mammalia: Bovi-
Padian, K. & Horner, J.R. (2004). Dinosaur Physiology. In dae). In Living fossils: 62–79. Eldredge, N. & Stanley, S.M.
The Dinosauria, 2nd edn: 660–671. Weishampel, D., (Eds). Berlin: Springer.
Osmolska, H. & Dodson, P. (Eds). Berkeley: University of Webb, G.J.W., Messel, H., Crawford, J. & Yerbury, M.J.
California Press. (1978). Growth rates of Crocodylus porosus (Reptilia:
Paterson, H.E.H. (1993). Evolution and the recognition concept Crocodylia) from Arnhem Land, Northern Australia. Aust.
of species: collected writings. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Wildl. Res. 5, 385–399.
University Press. Weishampel, D.B. (1981). Acoustic analyses of potential
Perez-Barberia, F.J., Gordon, I.J. & Pagel, M. (2002). The vocalization in lambeosaurine dinosaurs (Reptilia:
origins of sexual dimorphism in body size in ungulates. Ornithischia). Paleobiology 7, 252–261.
Evolution 56, 1276–1285. Weishampel, D.B. (1997). Dinosaurian cacophony:
Price, T. (1998). Sexual selection and natural selection in bird inferring function in extinct organisms. Bioscience 47,
speciation. Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. B353, 251–260. 150–158.
Raath, M.A. (1990). Morphological variation in small ther- Weishampel, D.B. & Chapman, R.E. (1990). Morphometric
opod teeth and its meaning in systematics: evidence from study of Plateosaurus from Trossingen (Baden-Wurttem-
Syntarsus rhodesiensis. In Dinosaur systematics: approaches burg, Federal Republic of Germany). In Dinosaur sys-
and perspectives: 91–106. Carpenter, K. & Currie, P.J. tematics: approaches and perspectives: 43–52. Carpenter, K.
(Eds). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. & Currie, P.J. (Eds). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Rigby, J.K. Jr (1990). Thermoregulation in latest dinosaurs. Press.
J. Vertebr. Paleontol. 9 (Suppl.), 36A. Weishampel, D.B., Dodson, P. & Osmolska, H. (Eds). (2004).
Rose, M.R. & Lauder, G.V. (Eds). (1996). Adaptation. San The Dinosauria, 2nd edn. Berkeley: University of Califor-
Diego: Academic Press. nia Press.
Ryan, M.J., Russell, A.P., Eberth, D.E. & Currie, P.J. (2001). West-Eberhard, M.J. (1983). Sexual selection, social competi-
The taphonomy of a Centrosaurus (Ornithischia: Ceratop- tion, and speciation. Quart. Rev. Biol. 58, 155–183.
sidae) bone bed from the Dinosaur Park Formation (Upper Whewell, W. (1859). The history of inductive sciences, from the
Campanian), Alberta, Canada, with comments on cranial earliest to present times. New York: Appleton.
ontogeny. Palaios 16, 482–506. Williams, G.C. (1992). Natural selection: domains, levels, and
Sampson, S.D. (1997). Dinosaur combat and courtship. In The challenges. New York: Oxford University Press.
complete dinosaur: 383–393. Farlow, J.O. & Brett-Surman, Wilson, E.O. (1998). Consilience: the unity of knowledge. New
M.K. (Eds). Bloomington: Indiana University Press. York: Random House.
Sampson, S.D. (1999). Sex and destiny: the role of mating Witmer, L.M. (1995). The Extant Phylogenetic Bracket
signals in speciation and macroevolution. Hist. Biol. 13, and the importance of reconstructing soft tissues in
173–197. fossils. In Functional morphology in vertebrate paleontology:
Scannella, J.B. & Horner, J.R. (in press). Torosaurus Marsh, 19–33. Thomason, J.J. (Ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge
1891 is Triceratops Marsh, 1889 (Ceratopsidae: Chasmo- University Press.
saurinae): synonomy through ontogeny. J. Vertebr. Xu, X., Makovicky, P.J., Wang, X.L., Norell, M.A. &
Paleontol. You, H.L. (2002). A ceratopsian dinosaur from China
Scheyer, T.M. & Sander, P.M. (2004). Histology of ankylo- and the early evolution of Ceratopsia. Nature 416,
saur osteoderms: implications for systematics and function. 314–317.
J. Vertebr. Paleontol. 24, 874–893. You, H. & Dodson, P. (2004). Basal Ceratopsia. In The
Sues, H.D. (1978). Functional morphology of the dome in Dinosauria, 2nd edn: 478–493. Weishampel, D., Osmolska,
pachycephalosaurid dinosaurs. Neues Jahrbuch fuer H. & Dodson, P. (Eds). Berkeley: University of California
Geologie und Palaeontologie Monatshefte 1978, 459–472. Press.

Journal of Zoology 283 (2011) 3–17


c 2010 The Authors. Journal of Zoology
c 2010 The Zoological Society of London 17

You might also like