You are on page 1of 23

Psychological Bulletin Copyright 1994 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1994, Vol. II6, No. 1,143-165 0033-2909/94/S3.00

Multiple Identity Enactments and Multiple Personality Disorder:


A Sociocognitive Perspective
Nicholas P. Spanos

People who enact multiple identities behave as if they possess 2 or more selves, each with its own
characteristic moods, memories, and behavioral repertoire. Under different names, this phenome-
non occurs in many cultures; in North American culture, it is frequently labeled multiple personality
disorder (MPD). This article reviews experimental, cross-cultural, historical, and clinical findings
concerning multiplicity and examines the implications of these findings for an understanding of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

MPD. Multiplicity is viewed from a Sociocognitive perspective, and it is concluded that MPD, like
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

other forms of multiplicity, is socially constructed. It is context bounded, goal-directed, social be-
havior geared to the expectations of significant others, and its characteristics have changed over time
to meet changing expectations.

People who receive the diagnosis of multiple personality dis- spite much and varied criticism, this view has become highly
order (MPD) behave as if they possess two or more distinct iden- influential. Thus, from this perspective, displays of multiple
tities. They convey the impression of multiplicity by exhibiting identities reflect a mental disorder that "happens" to the person
a relatively integrated interpersonal style (i.e., a distinct person- as a result of early traumas and other experiences over which
ality) when calling themselves by one name and different inter- she or he has no control and often no memory rather than some-
personal styles when calling themselves by other names. Fre- thing that the person does in response to current contingencies,
quently, MPD patients behave as if their different identities have goals, and understandings.
their own unique memories and experiences, and many of the In this article, I argue that the disease perspective of MPD is
identities claim amnesia for the other personalities with whom fundamentally flawed. Specifically, I use experimental, cross-
they coreside. cultural, and historical findings to argue that (a) multiple iden-
Modern MPD patients are usually women with a wide range tities are usefully conceptualized as rule-governed social con-
of chronic psychiatric problems that predate their MPD diag- structions, (b) neither childhood trauma nor a history of severe
nosis (Coons, Bowman, & Milstein, 1988; Putnam, Guroff, psychopathology is necessary for the development or mainte-
Silberman, Barban, & Post, 1986; Ross, Norton, & Wozney, nance of multiple identities, and (c) multiple identities are es-
1989). These patients usually claim to have been physically or tablished, legitimated, maintained, and altered through social
sexually abused—often horrendously—in childhood (Coons & interaction.
Milstein, 1986; Ross, Miller, Bjornson, Reagor, Fraser, & An- This Sociocognitive alternative to the disease model suggests
derson, 1991; Young, Sachs, Braun, & Watkins, 1991). More- that MPD is a sociohistorical product (Hacking, 1986, 1992;
over, it is now common for investigators (e.g., Bliss, 1986; Kenny, 1986; Shorter, 1992; Spanos, 1989). In the last 2 centu-
Braun, 1990;Kluft, 1993; Putnam, 1989, 1993; Ross, 1989) to ries, a number of psychiatric syndromes (e.g., motor hysteria)
argue that MPD is a distinct mental disorder caused by severe have developed, spread, and then all but disappeared as a func-
childhood abuse. According to this hypothesis, severe trauma tion of changing conceptions held by both doctors and patients
during childhood produces a mental splitting or dissociation as concerning the ways in which distress may be legitimately ex-
a defensive reaction to the trauma. These dissociated "parts" of pressed (Shorter, 1992). In the last 20 years, the notion of
the person develop into alter identities or personalities that, in multiple personality has become commonplace in North Amer-
adulthood, periodically manifest themselves to help the individ- ican culture and is now a legitimate way for people to un-
ual cope with stressful situations, express resentments or other derstand and express their failures and frustrations, as well as a
feelings that the primary personality disavows, and so on. The covert tactic by which they can manipulate others and attain
proponents of this perspective have been highly vocal, and, de- succor and other rewards. In short, the Sociocognitive perspec-
tive suggests that patients learn to construe themselves as pos-
sessing multiple selves, learn to present themselves in terms of
Preparation of this article was supported by a grant from the Social
this construal, and learn to reorganize and elaborate on their
Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. I thank Cheryl personal biography so as to make it congruent with their under-
Burgess, Melissa F. Burgess, and Bill Jones for commenting on drafts of standing of what it means to be a multiple. These patients are
this article. conceptualized as actively involved in using available informa-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Nich- tion to create a social impression that is congruent with their
olas P. Spanos, Department of Psychology, Carleton University, B550 perception of situational demands and with the interpersonal
Loeb Building, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada K1S goals they are attempting to achieve (Spanos, Weekes, & Ber-
5B6. trand, 1985).

143
144 NICHOLAS P. SPANOS

According to this perspective, psychotherapists play a partic- tions and that, in highly hypnotizable subjects, produces distor-
ularly important part in the generation and maintenance of tions in memory and perception and facilitates the recall of "hid-
MPD. Therapists routinely encourage patients to construe den" memories. This view of hypnosis is almost always uncriti-
themselves as having multiple selves, provide them with infor- cally accepted in the MPD literature (e.g., Bliss, 1986; Ross,
mation about how to convincingly enact the role of "multiple 1989). In the last 40 years, however, a great deal of empirical
personality patient," and provide official legitimation for the evidence has challenged this view (for reviews, see Spanos,
different identities that their patients enact. 1986b; Spanos & Chaves, 1989; Wagstaff, 1981). Contrary to the
assumption frequently found in the MPD literature, more than
Identities: Singular and Plural a century of research has failed to uncover unambiguous behav-
ioral, physiological, or subjective report criteria for denoting a
In all cultures, people exhibit wide variability in their behav- uniquely hypnotic state (Barber, 1979; Fellows, 1986; Radtke &
ior across time and situations. Nevertheless, in North American Spanos, 1981; Sarbin & Coe, 1972). In addition, a large number
culture almost all people hold a subjective sense of unitary iden- of studies indicate that even the seeming dramatic behaviors as-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tity and view their diverse behavior as the product of a single self sociated with high hypnotizability (e.g., displays of age regression
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(Epstein, 1973). The self is a social product, a series of interre- and amnesia) can be accomplished by motivated control subjects
lated construals made about the first person singular by the first who have not been administered hypnotic induction procedures
person singular (Deaux, 1993). These self-construals reflect the and who display no signs of being in a "trancelike" condition
categories that each culture uses to describe its members and (Barber, 1969; Diamond, 1974; Wagstaff, 1981). Furthermore,
involve socially derived attributions that people apply to them- many of these dramatic behaviors have turned out to be much
selves. The way that people view themselves reflects the way that more mundane than they originally appeared. For example, hyp-
they are viewed by others, and it is others who provide or with- notically age-regressed subjects do not develop the psychological
hold legitimation for the varied self-presentations that people characteristics of actual children; instead, they respond like
enact (Goffman, 1959). People typically present themselves as a adults attempting to behave as if they are children. When their
unitary self enacting different roles because they are reinforced expectations of how children behave are inaccurate, their age re-
for doing so. In fact, the social, economic, and legal institutions gression performances are off the mark (Nash, 1987).
of North American culture are premised on the notion that MPD theorists frequently imply that hypnosis is a process
each person is a unitary self who is accountable for his or her that happens to a person rather than something that a person
own diverse behaviors, and it is this view of unitary self that is does (Bliss, 1986). However, there is much support for the hy-
routinely legitimated in social interaction (Halleck, 1990; Man- pothesis that hypnotic behaviors are goal-directed enactments
cuso & Sarbin, 1983; M. S. Moore, 1984). and that highly hypnotizable subjects are cognizing individuals
Social products are, of course, subject to social change. There who are attuned to even subtle interpersonal cues and who are
is nothing invariable or inevitable about the notion of a unitary invested in meeting the social demands of hypnotic situations
self, and the same social processes that validate the conception to present themselves as "good" subjects (Sarbin & Coe, 1972;
people hold of themselves as unitary selves can be used to vali- Spanos & Coe, 1992). The demands contained in hypnotic sug-
date the alternative conceptualization that people consist of gestions call for particular subjective experiences as well as cor-
multiple selves. responding overt behaviors. For instance, suggestions for amne-
sia require not only that subjects fail to report target material
Hypnosis and Multiple Personality Disorder but also that they define themselves as having forgotten that
material. However, suggested experiences such as temporary
In North American culture, those who carry out multiple forgetting or reduced pain do not occur automatically. Instead,
identity enactments are usually defined by mental health pro- such experiences must be generated by subjects who use their
fessionals as suffering from MPD. Historically, MPD has been attentional and imaginal abilities in attempting to create these
closely tied to hypnotic phenomena. Some modern investiga- subjective effects. Subjects who are unable to generate the sub-
tors (e.g., Bliss, 1986; Braun, 1990) have argued that in predis- jective experiences called for by suggestions frequently admit
posed children, trauma produces a "hypnotic state" that facili- their failures rather than fake their responses. On the other
tates the development of alter personalities. Purportedly, these hand, hypnotic responding is exceedingly easy to fake (Orne,
alters remain separated from normal consciousness by a pro- 1979), some subjects do purposely describe their experiences
cess akin to hypnotic amnesia. In addition, modern experi- inaccurately to meet test demands, and such erroneous descrip-
ments aimed at elucidating the nature of multiplicity have been tions are most likely among highly hypnotizable subjects given
conducted within a hypnotic context. Consequently, back- difficult suggestions (Burgess, Spanos, Ritt, Hordy, & Brooks,
ground information concerning hypnotic responding in general 1993; Spanos, Flynn, & Gabora, 1989; Wagstaff, 1981).
and hypnotic amnesia in particular may be useful in under-
standing MPD enactments. Hypnotic Amnesia
Amnesia has long been a hallmark of hypnotic responding,
Hypnotic Responding and during the 19th century it was thought to occur spontane-
Historically, hypnosis has been viewed as an altered state of ously as a function of the transition from being hypnotized to
consciousness that greatly increases responsiveness to sugges- being awake (Sarbin & Coe, 1972). MPD patients frequently
MULTIPLE IDENTITY ENACTMENTS AND MPD 145

behave as if one or more of their alter identities are amnesic for that are difficult to explain in terms of involuntary memory
the memories of other alters. MPD theorists typically describe dysfunction. Some report a one-way amnesia between alter
such amnesia as an involuntary and spontaneous occurrence identities, whereas others report a two-way amnesia. When
that is akin to hypnotic amnesia (Bliss, 1986; Ross, 1989). Con- switching alter identities, most such patients retain basic skills
trary to such descriptions, however, amnesic displays are not a such as reading and writing. In some early cases, however, these
common accompaniment of hypnotic performances unless the abilities were purportedly lost and had to be relearned by the
amnesia has been suggested explicitly (Coe, 1989). Spontane- new identity (Hacking, 1991; Kenny, 1986). In many modern
ous amnesia is a rare occurrence and, when it does occur, may cases, MPD patients switch back and forth between alters (and
simply reflect subjects' implicit understandings of the hypnotic thereby between sets of supposedly segregated memories) very
role. Even highly hypnotizable subjects typically recall the quickly. However, in a series of 19th-century British cases de-
contents of their hypnotic session after its termination unless scribed by Hacking (1991), these kinds of switches between al-
they are explicitly suggested to do otherwise. ters frequently involved a period of transitional sleep. Nine-
When asked to describe the experience of hypnotic amnesia, teenth-century and early-20th-century MPD patients rarely
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

subjects proffer a wide range of reports. Contrary' to the way in displayed more than two or three alter identities (Bowman,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

which hypnotic amnesia has been described in the MPD litera- 1990). Modern patients, on the other hand, display an average
ture (e.g., Bliss, 1986), many subjects describe their forgetting of 15 or more alters, and some of these patients exhibit more
as an active process that involves self-distraction and other than 100 alters (Ross, Norton, & Fraser, 1989). In other words,
strategies aimed at inhibiting recall (Spanos & Bodorik, 1977). the number of dissociated memory systems supposedly pos-
Some hypnotic subjects do report that they were unable to re- sessed by MPD patients has grown exponentially since the be-
call target material and that they perceived their amnesia as in- ginning of this century. These kinds of differences in amnesic
voluntary. Nevertheless, the available experimental data indi- displays over time and across patients suggest that MPD pa-
cate that even these subjects retain rather than lose control of tients, like hypnotically amnesic subjects, alter their patterns of
memory processes and guide their recall in terms of the social recall as a function of their understanding of what is expected.
demands to which they are exposed (Coe, 1989; Spanos & Coe, As the expectations of therapists concerning the amnesia of
1992). For example, in two separate studies (Silva & Kirsch, their patients change, patients change their amnesic displays to
1987; Spanos, Radtke, & Bertrand, 1984), highly hypnotizable meet the new expectations.
hypnotic subjects who were displaying amnesia were convinced Work with non-MPD psychiatric patients also indicates that
that they would be able to recall the forgotten information but psychogenic amnesia is a goal-directed achievement influenced
only under certain conditions. All of the subjects in one study by subjects' understandings and by legitimation received from
and almost all in the other behaved in terms of the expectations others rather than an involuntary occurrence (Kirshner, 1973).
conveyed to them by recalling and failing to recall forgotten For instance, Parfitt and Gall (1944) worked with combat veter-
material in the appropriate sequences. Relatedly, Coe and Sluis ans whose reports of amnesia prevented their return to active
(1989) exposed highly hypnotizable subjects who exhibited service. Rather than legitimating these amnesic displays, Parfitt
posthypnotic amnesia to strong and repeated demands to re- and Gall informed the patients that their memory would return
member. Under these circumstances, even subjects who had in- and continued to convey this expectation in their interactions
sisted that their amnesia was involuntary showed substantial with the patients. Parfitt and Gall did not use any treatment to
recovery of the forgotten memories. lift patients' amnesia other than telling them that they could
MPD patients are frequently described as living for years with remember. Exposure to these consistent demands to recall
alter personalities of which they are unaware (Kluft, 1985). rather than to continue forgetting led most subjects to recover
However, hypnotic amnesia is rarely complete even among their memories. On the basis of these and other clinical findings,
highly hypnotizable subjects. Moreover, among those few sub- Kirshner (1973) suggested that psychogenic amnesia can be
jects who exhibit total amnesia, more than half report postex- construed as a transitional social role that is adopted to deal
perimentally that during the amnesia test period, they con- with conflict and stress. Such an analysis may be useful for ex-
sciously remembered but failed to report at least some of the plaining how hypnotic interventions that legitimate remember-
target information (Spanos & Bodorik, 1977). ing can often induce a "sharing" of memories among the several
The "memory deficits" displayed by hypnotically amnesic identities enacted by multiples (R. B. Allison & Schwarz, 1980;
subjects vary dramatically as a function of the expectations to Bliss, 1980; Ross, 1989). The available evidence indicates that
which they are exposed. Thus, depending on the suggestions hypnotic procedures do not possess intrinsic properties that en-
they are given, these subjects behave as if they have forgotten an
entire list or only a subset of the list; all of the concrete words
1
on a list but none of the abstract words, and vice versa; the num- A number of studies have assessed psychophysiological processes
ber 4 while recalling all remaining numbers; and so on (Coe, during the various personality enactments of MPD patients and in con-
1989). Exhibiting the wide range of memory performance trol subjects (for reviews, see Brown, in press; Miller & Triggiano, 1992).
Many of the studies are anecdotal and involve numerous methodologi-
changes called for in these different experiments requires that cal shortcomings. In addition, failures to replicate are common, and the
hypnotic subjects retain rather than lose control of memory few consistent findings are difficult to interpret (Brown, in press). Even
processes and guide their amnesic displays in terms of their un- some advocates of the MPD diagnosis (Putnam, 1993) have cautioned
derstanding of what is called for by the amnesia test situation. against the idea that psychophysiologically distinct markers are likely to
MPD patients also exhibit a wide range of amnesic deficits characterize the different personalities of MPD patients.
146 NICHOLAS P. SPANOS

hance recall (Smith, 1983; Wagstaff, 1989). However, such in- students. In a typical experiment, subjects were exposed to pain
terventions may provide a legitimating context for redefining stimulation trials before and after administration of a hypnotic
the situation as one in which displays of cross-identity remem- procedure and suggestions for analgesia. Subjects were also
bering are considered role appropriate. given instructions that implied that a hidden part of them re-
In summary, MPD theorists may well be correct when they mained aware of all they experienced during the analgesia pe-
contend that the amnesia of MPD patients resembles hypnotic riod. During hypnotic analgesia testing, these subjects gave two
amnesia. However, the descriptions of hypnotic amnesia given types of pain reports. Verbal (overt) reports purportedly from
by these theorists are often misleading (e.g., Bliss, 1980, 1986). their hypnotized self usually indicated relatively low levels of
Contrary to these descriptions, the available data indicate that pain, and covert reports (numbers tapped out in a previously
hypnotic amnesia does not involve an automatic and complete taught keypressing code) supposedly from their hidden self usu-
forgetting of events. Instead, hypnotic amnesia (and most likely ally indicated high levels of pain.
much of MPD amnesia as well) involves goal-directed enact- Hilgard (1979,1991) argued that the hidden self instructions
ment aimed at meeting social expectations. Such amnesia is fre- used in these studies did not provide subjects with the idea that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

quently denned by subjects as involving voluntary self-distrac- they had a hidden self or with the idea that hidden and overt
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tion, it is rarely complete even in highly hypnotizable subjects, reports should be different. Instead, the hidden self was suppos-
and, when it is complete, it frequently involves conscious with- edly an unsuggested accompaniment of hypnotic analgesia that
holding and is typically of short duration. Hypnotic subjects remained separated from normal consciousness by an "amne-
retain rather than lose control of memory processes and, in that sic barrier" unless and until it was contacted by the hypnotist.
way, display the particular memory deficits called for by the test Contrary to this view, a number of studies (Spanos, Flynn, &
situation (Coe, 1989;Spanos, 1986b). Gwynn, 1988; Spanos, Gwynn, & Stam, 1983; Spanos & Hew-
Braun (1990) suggested that the segregated memories dis- itt, 1980) demonstrated that the direction of hidden reports var-
played by different alters reflect state-specific recall. Application ied with the expectations conveyed by hidden self instructions.
of this idea to the amnesia seen in MPD patients suggests that Thus, depending on the implications contained in their instruc-
experiences acquired in one psychological state (i.e., Identity A) tions, subjects exhibited hidden selves that reported less pain
will not be remembered when the person is in a different state than, more pain than, or an amount of pain equal to that of
(i.e., Identity B). However, these experiences will be easily re- their hypnotized selves.
membered when the person is again in the first state (i.e., Iden- The creation of hidden selves that respond to instruction by
tity A). This formulation cannot account for the one-way am- behaving as if they possess information of which the person's
nesia that is commonly reported by MPD patients (e.g., Identity "normal self" is unaware has been documented in studies on
A is aware of its own memories and those of Identity B, but hypnotic age regression, blindness, eye-witness recall, and am-
Identity B is aware only of its own memories). In addition, this nesia as well as pain reduction (Spanos et al., 1988; Spanos,
hypothesis cannot explain cases in which people who display Gwynn, Comer, Baltruweit, & deGroh, 1989; Spanos & Mclain,
distinct identities show no cross-identity amnesia. 1986; Spanos et al., 1984). Moreover, hypnotic procedures are
Heterosexual male transvestites in North American culture not required to produce hidden selves. Two studies (Spanos &
alternate between a male and female identity. When enacting Bures, 1993; Spanos, deGroot, Tiller, Weekes, & Bertrand,
their female identity, they typically dress as women, act as 1985) found that nonhypnotic high hypnotizables reported ex-
women, and refer to themselves with a woman's name (Tala- periencing hidden selves as frequently as did corresponding
mini, 1982). Nevertheless, transvestites do not display between- hypnotic subjects.
identities amnesia. While enacting their female self, they recall In summary, the enactment of hidden or dissociated selves
and discuss their masculine self, and vice versa (Docter, 1988). by hypnotic and nonhypnotic subjects involves strategic, rule-
These findings indicate that amnesia between identities does governed self-presentation. In these studies, information about
not arise automatically as a function of the differences in psy- the characteristics of the hidden selves was provided by experi-
chological functioning that are involved in enacting one identity mental instructions, and subjects guided their experiences and
as opposed to another. Instead, whether or not amnesia is asso- behaviors in terms of these role prescriptions. Thus, the charac-
ciated with alter identity enactments appears to depend on the teristics of hidden self enactments varied as a function of the
expectations and purposes associated with those enactments. performance expectations transmitted to subjects.

Experimental Creation of Multiplicity


Past-Life Regression Experiments
In the last 20 years, two lines of experimental research have
examined variables that influence the development of multi- Several studies have examined factors that influence the for-
plicity. The first was initiated by Hilgard (1979) and revolved mation of multiple selves by using the phenomenon of past-life
around his notion of a hidden observer or hidden self. The sec- hypnotic regression. Although some believers in reincarnation
ond research line has dealt with the phenomenon of past-life hold that people can be hypnotically regressed back to previous
hypnotic regression. lives (e.g., Wambaugh, 1979), the available evidence suggests in-
stead that past-life experiences are fantasy constructions (Baker,
Hidden Observer Experiments 1992; Spanos, Menary, Gabora, DuBreuil, & Dewhirst, 1991).
Hilgard (1979, 1991) conducted a series of studies that led to These experiences are important because they shed light on the
the elicitation of "hidden selves" in highly hypnotizable college processes by which people come to treat their fantasies as real
MULTIPLE IDENTITY ENACTMENTS AND MPD 147

and because past-life identities are similar in many respects to equally likely to enact past-life identities, but those assigned to
the multiple identities of MPD patients. Like MPD patients, the imaginary creation condition assigned significantly less
subjects who exhibit past-life identities behave as if they are in- credibility to these identities than did those told that reincarna-
habited by more than one self. Like the secondary selves of tion was scientifically credible. In short, prior information from
MPD patients, those exhibited by past-life responders often dis- authority figures influences not only the characteristics and
play moods and personality characteristics that are different memories that people attribute to their multiple identities but
from the person's primary self, have a different name than the also the degree to which they come to believe in the reality of
primary self, and report memories of which the primary self these identities.
was previously unaware. Just as MPD patients come to believe Taken together, the experimental data indicate that multiple
that their alter identities are real personalities rather than self- identities are social creations that can be elicited easily from
generated fantasies, many of the subjects who enact past lives many normal people. When the identity to be constructed is
continue to believe in the reality of their past lives after termi- relatively complex, as in past-life regression studies, subjects
nation of the hypnotic procedures. draw on information from a wide array of sources (e.g., TV
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Kampman (1976) found that 41% of highly hypnotizable shows, historical novels, aspects of their own past, and wish-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

subjects manifested evidence of a new identity and called them- fulfilling daydreams) to flesh out the newly constructed identity
selves by different names when hypnotically regressed beyond and to provide it with the history and characteristics that are
their birth. Contrary to the notion that the development of called for by their understanding of the current task demands.
multiple identities is a sign of mental illness, Kampman's past- These studies indicate that the development of multiple identi-
life responders scored higher on measures of psychological ties is not related to psychopathology and that men are as adept
health than did subjects who failed to exhibit a past life. as women at creating such identities. Although none of these
In a series of experiments, Spanos, Menary, et al. (1991) also studies obtained information about whether subjects had been
obtained past-life identity enactments after hypnotic regression abused as children, the fact that psychopathology failed to pre-
suggestions. Frequently, the past-life identities were elaborate, dict either the development of these identities or the extent to
had their own names, and described their lives in great detail. which subjects construed them to be real rather than imagined
Subjects who reported past lives scored higher on indexes of makes it unlikely that early abuse played an important role in
hypnotizability and fantasy proneness, but no higher on indexes these regards.
of psychopathology, than those who did not exhibit a past life.
The characteristics that subjects attributed to their past lives
were influenced by experimenter-transmitted expectations. For Cross-Cultural Studies on Spirit Possession
instance, subjects provided with prehypnotic information
about the likely characteristics of their identities were much Multiple self enactments occur in most but not all cultures
more likely than controls to incorporate these characteristics (Bourguignon, 1976). In many traditional societies and in some
into their descriptions of their past-life selves (Spanos, Menary, subcultural contexts in North American society, multiple self
et al., 1991, Experiment 2). In a different study (Spanos, Men- enactments take the form of spirit possession. In these cases, it
ary, et al., 1991, Experiment 3), some subjects were informed is believed that the human occupant of the body is temporarily
prehypnotically that children in past eras were frequently displaced by another self or selves that are defined as spirits who
abused, whereas those in the other condition were given no in- temporarily take over control of the body. Frequently, the hu-
formation about abuse. The past-life identities of subjects given man self claims amnesia for the periods during which the spirit
abuse information reported significantly higher levels of child- selves are in control (Bourguignon, 1976; Lewis, 1987).
hood abuse than did the past-life identities of control subjects. The frequency with which possession occurs varies greatly
In summary, these studies indicate that the personal attributes from one society to another. In some societies that hold posses-
and memory reports elicited from subjects who enact past-life sion beliefs, only a relatively small percentage of the population
identities are influenced by the beliefs and expectations con- is ever defined as actually possessed. For instance, Wijesinghe,
veyed by the experimenter/hypnotist. Subjects shape the attri- Dissanayake, and Mendis (1976) reported an incidence of 0.5%
butes and biographies attributed to their past-life identities to for a semiurban population in Sri Lanka; Carstairs and Kapur
correspond to their understandings of what significant others (1976) found a period prevalence rate in a rural population on
believe these characteristics to be. the west coast of South India of 2.8%; and Venkataramaiah,
The extent to which subjects assigned credibility to their past- Mallikarjunaiah, Chandra, Rao, and Reddy (1981) reported a
life identities correlated significantly with the degree to which prevalence of 3.7% in a different South Indian rural population.
they placed credence in reincarnation before the experiment In other societies, the rates of possession are extremely high. For
and the extent to which they expected to experience a real past instance, in the villages of the Malagasy, speakers of Mayotte,
life. However, subjects' beliefs in the reality of their past lives Lambek (1980) reported that 39% of the adult women and 8%
were also influenced by information from the experimenter. of the adult men were considered to be possessed. Relatedly,
Spanos, Menary, et al. (1991, Experiment 4) informed subjects Harper (1963) reported that 20% of the women among the
in one condition that past-life identities were interesting fanta- Havik Brahmins in Mysore, India, experienced possession, and
sies but were certainly not evidence of real past lives. Those in Boddy (1988) found that in different years 42% and 47% of ever-
another condition were informed that reincarnation was a sci- married women more than 15 years of age in the village of Ho-
entifically credible notion. Subjects in the two conditions were friyat in Northern Sudan had succumbed to possession. When
148 NICHOLAS P. SPANOS

considering only women between the ages of 35 and 55 years, These members frequently reconstruct their biographies to ac-
Boddy (1988) reported that 66.6% had experienced possession. centuate differences between their preconversion and postcon-
In most traditional societies that hold possession beliefs, pos- version identities. In so doing, they emphasize their first glosso-
session occurs much more frequently in females than in males. lalic experience as a marker of what they and their community
This, however, is not invariably the case, and in some societies view as a critical transition point between a discarded sinful
possession occurs with equal or almost equal frequency in the identity and a new consecrated identity (Hine, 1970; Maloney
two sexes (Lewis, 1987). If nothing else, the marked differences & Lovekin, 1985).
between societies in rates of possession, coupled with the very Another form of possession with a long history in Western
high rates of possession in some societies, should make one societies is spirit mediumship (R. L. Moore, 1977). During the
wary of explanations of multiple identity development that em- late 19th century, at the height of interest in mediumship in
phasize the importance of stable personality or cognitive char- America, spirit mediums were often female, and adoption of
acteristics (such as high fantasy proneness or high hypnotizabil- the medium role was a vehicle through which women could cir-
ity) as necessary predisposing factors. Sex ratios and the pro- cumvent some of the restrictions associated with the female role
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

portion of community members affected also vary widely in and earn an independent living. North American mediums usu-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

some North American groups that display spirit possession. ally became possessed by a control spirit who served as an in-
termediary to the spirit world and who summoned the spirits of
Two North American Examples of Spirit Possession departed relatives, who in turn possessed and spoke through the
medium. Typically mediums reported amnesia for the period
One relatively common form of possession experience in during which the spirits occupied their bodies.
North American society is religious glossolalia (Mine, 1969). Although less common than previously, spirit mediums con-
Glossolalia involves semantically meaningless vocal utterances tinue to operate in many North American cities. Biscop (1981,
that sound languagelike and that are sometimes mistaken by 1985) conducted a participant observation study of a spiritist
naive listeners as a foreign language. Glossolalia is frequently church in Canada and documented the process by which nov-
spoken in certain Christian religious settings. It may be accom- ices were socialized into the role of medium. Part of this process
panied by dramatic behaviors including convulsions, profuse involved formal teaching, but much of it involved repeated op-
sweating, eye closure, and an apparent loss of consciousness, portunities to observe more experienced mediums, coupled
but it often occurs in the absence of all such dramatic accom- with encouragement in small-group settings in which novices
paniments (Spanos & Hewitt, 1979). Whether or not glossolalia were provided with helpful feedback. Mediumship involves
is accompanied by dramatic displays appears to depend on the learning how to "read" the client to obtain information about
expectations of the glossolalics and their audience and the the deceased that can then be fed back to the client as proof of
norms of the particular setting in which it is displayed. Tradi- the deceased's survival. At times, mediums go to great lengths
tionally, glossolalia has been interpreted in Christian circles as and use much trickery to obtain information and provide con-
possession by the Holy Spirit, who speaks his own language (the vincing performances (Keene, 1976). Biscop (1981) interviewed
glossolalia) through the possessed person. six North American mediums at length. Most reported rela-
Glossolalia is learned behavior, and rates of glossolalia differ tively happy childhoods, and, in marked contrast to MPD pa-
dramatically across different religious groups as a function of tients, only one reported that she had been sexually abused as a
expectations concerning who will and will not manifest the phe- child.
nomenon. In some congregations, glossolalia is encouraged and The characteristics of possession displays in other cultures
occurs in all or almost all members. In other congregations, it is also vary greatly both between societies and within societies.
relatively rare (Samarin, 1972). Glossolalia can be easily Possession is not a unitary phenomenon; it differs dramatically
learned through modeling and practice (Spanos, Cross, Lepage, depending on the status of the possessed person, the context in
& Coristine, 1986), and congregations that encourage glossola- which the possession occurs, and the meaning attributed to the
lia typically provide the novice with much encouragement and possession both by the possessed individual and by his or her
coaching and multiple opportunities to closely observe other audience (Krippner, in press; Lewis, 1987).
glossolalics (Maloney & Lovekin, 1985). Glossolalics do not
score higher than nonglossolalics on measures of hypnotizabil- Ritual Possession
ity, imaginative activity, or psychopathology (Richardson, 1973;
Spanos & Hewitt, 1979), and the ability to learn glossolalia is In many societies, spirit possession occurs as part of helping
unrelated to either hypnotizability or imaginative activity rituals. The medium becomes possessed by a spirit or by suc-
(Spanos, Cross, et al., 1986). Glossolalia can occur with equal cessive spirits, and it is the spirits who diagnose the client, pre-
frequency in men and women, and when sex differences do oc- scribe treatments, or offer advice for problems in living. The
cur they reflect local custom rather than intrinsic gender differ- rituals can be private consultations involving only the medium
ences (Samarin, 1972). and client or public ceremonies involving one or more mediums
In some religious communities, the first manifestation of and large audiences (Lee, 1989; Lewis, 1987). The structure of
glossolalia is interpreted as a sign of salvation that signals full these rituals illustrates the social nature of multiple identity en-
acceptance into the religious group. In describing their conver- actments and the dependence of such enactments on social val-
sion, new members typically draw a sharp distinction between idation. The medium and the audience hold complementary
their new (postconversion) identity and their old life of sin. expectations concerning the behaviors that define the medium
MULTIPLE IDENTITY ENACTMENTS AND MPD 149

as possessed by a particular spirit and the behaviors that define tions of clients and other audience members (Krippner, 1989;
members of the audience as validating the spirit presentations. Morton, 1977; Peters, 1981).
The medium presents as a specific spirit by enacting the behav- Becoming a spirit medium, like becoming a glossolalic, some-
ioral displays that the audience identifies with that spirit. The times involves the possessed organizing their biographies to cor-
audience, in turn, validates the medium's presentation by re- respond with implicit societal conceptions concerning the
sponding in a manner that is congruent with the particular meaning of possession. In some societies, for instance, the pos-
spirit identity being presented (Firth, 1967; Lambek, 1988; session careers of mediums are described in highly stereotyped
Lee, 1989). fashion and include a series of stock background events (e.g.,
For instance, the transition from the medium's human per- fleeing into the wilderness) that explain why they were singled
sonality to that of a spirit is marked by readily identifiable signs out by the spirits for possession (Morton, 1977). Investigators
(e.g., shaking and eye closure) that the audience has learned to of mediums in traditional cultures (Krippner, 1989; Leacock &
interpret in terms of spirit possession. Similarly, the transition Leacock, 1972) have often commented that these individuals
from one spirit identity to another is marked by recognizable usually appear to be well adjusted, mentally healthy, and com-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

changes in behavior such as changes in voice, personality, and petent.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

dress. In some cultures, particular spirits dance to some tunes The spirit possession enactments of mediums are responsive
but not others; in others, each change in spirit identity is marked to sociocultural changes, and in some circumstances possession
by replacing a scarf of one color with a scarf of another. When becomes a vehicle for expressing resistance to externally im-
behaving as a warrior spirit, the medium struts to and fro in an posed authority (Stoller, 1989). For instance, during the French
aggressive and threatening manner and the audience responds colonial period, native African mediums among the Songhay
accordingly by being quiet and respectful. When presenting as became possessed by a new group of spirits that aped colonial
a tiger spirit, the medium may walk on all fours and growl while officials and burlesqued French colonial society with displays of
the audience backs away. When presenting as a risque spirit, the exaggerated and satiric behavior. Later, when French rule was
medium jokes with the audience, which responds with relax- replaced by a puritanical Islamic state, mediums expressed
ation and laughter (Krippner, 1989; Lambek, 1989; Lee, 1989; their resistance by enacting scatological and overtly sexual pos-
Saunders, 1977). The result of these mutually supporting in- session displays that violated the official Islamic moral code
teractions is the construction of spirit possession as a social re- (Stoller, 1989). The responsiveness of possession displays to cul-
ality (Schieffelin, 1985). tural changes illustrates the constructive and goal-directed na-
Often, ritual possession ceremonies involve a number of peo- ture of possession and the importance of ongoing situational
ple who are possessed simultaneously (Lewis, 1987). Further- factors (as opposed to idiosyncratic psychological ones) in de-
more, in some ceremonies, a single spirit can move from pos- termining both the character of possession displays and the his-
torical changes that occur in the nature of those displays.
sessing one person to possessing another. However, the same
spirit cannot possess two people simultaneously. Thus, ceremo-
nies that involve the simultaneous possession of several people Peripheral Possession
by spirits that move between mediums involve a good deal of
Lewis (1987) distinguished between central and peripheral
coordination. The various mediums must be aware of their own
possession. In central possession, the medium is possessed by
performances as well as those of other mediums to keep their
the major deities of the society, and the possession performances
changing roles distinct (Lambek, 1988; Lee, 1989).
serve to publicly reaffirm and support the central values of the
The rule-governed nature of ritual possession is also illus- society. Typically the medium in central possession is a re-
trated by the preparation required for a convincing perfor- spected member of the community. In contrast, peripheral pos-
mance. The props for differentiating spirit identities must be session afflicts socially marginal and oppressed members of the
readily available. For instance, when presenting as a tiger spirit, community. In this case, the possessing spirits are capricious
one medium would bite and suck at the patient's body until he and often amoral members of the pantheon, and possession is
produced from his mouth a black substance that he called black associated with illness, emotional distress, or both.
pus and blood. The blood he probably produced by biting his Lewis (1987) hypothesized that peripheral possession often
own cheek, and the "black pus" he produced by putting ashes constitutes a strategy used by the socially powerless to manipu-
into his mouth before the ceremony and before becoming pos- late their social superiors. Typically, peripheral possession oc-
sessed (Peters, 1981). curs in people low in the social hierarchy who are experiencing
Becoming a spirit medium usually involves an extensive so- high levels of psychological or interpersonal stress. The stress is
cialization process, and once an individual becomes a medium often manifested in psychophysiological symptoms (e.g., head-
periodic possession may be a lifelong occurrence. In some soci- aches) that are interpreted as initial manifestations of posses-
eties, mediumship runs in families, and particular spirits move sion. The possessing spirit makes numerous demands that must
from possessing a parent to possessing one of his or her children. be met by the family of the possessed. Possession is considered
In other cases, mediums are former patients who apprentice involuntary, and the disruptive behavior and unusual demands
with their healer. Frequently, there are a number of different are attributed to the possessing spirit rather than the possessed
paths into mediumship within the same society. Regardless of person. Frequently, peripheral possession is chronic, and new or
the path taken, however, the medium must learn the rules re- recurring stresses lead to a recurrence of symptoms (Saunders,
quired to give convincing performances that meet the expecta- 1977; Ward, 1989).
150 NICHOLAS P. SPANOS

Many traditional societies are strongly patriarchal, and the syncratic psychological causes. The contagion that occurs in
women in these societies often have few rights and are hemmed these cases results from social factors, from the common under-
in by many social restrictions. Consequently, in most of these standings held by participants about what constitute legitimate
societies it is women rather then men who resort to peripheral means of expressing dissatisfaction, and from the effects of ob-
possession as an interpersonal strategy for improving their lot. serving displays of possession and the consequences of those dis-
For example, a woman who is in an unhappy marriage to an plays.
inattentive or brutal husband and lives in a culture that restricts In summary, possession phenomena underscore the rule-gov-
married women almost exclusively to the home may become erned and social nature of multiple identity displays. Enact-
possessed by a spirit who demands an expensive public cere- ments of spirit possession are learned patterns of social re-
mony that includes rich delicacies, new clothing, and interac- sponding. Possessed individuals enact spirit identities that cor-
tion with other women. Moreover, when enacting the spirit role, respond to their understandings and expectations of possession.
the woman can voice unflattering and insulting remarks to her Possession enactments are public and involve interaction with
husband that would not be tolerated if they were defined as an audience that legitimates the enactments. These enactments
coming from her rather than from her spirit (Constantinides,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

occur in a wide range of circumstances, are carried out by very


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1977). Possession is a public event, and the norms of the com- different kinds of people, and serve a number of different social
munity may demand that the husband abide by the requests of functions. Possession is sometimes symptomatic of severe stress
the spirit despite his personal feelings toward his wife and de- and accompanied by symptoms of psychopathology. At other
spite the substantial expense involved. times, however, it is enacted by well-adjusted individuals who
In many societies where peripheral possession occurs, the do not manifest high levels of psychopathology.
aim of treatment is not to expel the spirit but to bring it under
control. Frequently, this taming process is accomplished by the Historical Manifestations of Demonic Possession
possessed joining a possession cult (Lewis, 1987). Here the pos-
sessed individual joins with other possessed individuals under The idea that demons can enter into people and take over
the tutelage of a shaman. The shaman is herself possessed but their functioning entered Western European history as an ac-
has learned to control her possessing spirits and use them as companiment of Christianity (Spanos, 1983a). Although infor-
spiritual advisors. The women in the cult meet regularly to hold mation concerning the rate of possession in earlier centuries is
feasts and dances in honor of the spirits and to seek spiritual sparse, it seems clear that this rate varied dramatically in
advice from the spirits of the shaman (Lewis, 1987; Morton, different historical eras (Oesterreich, 1966). Possession and ex-
1977). Frequently, these cult groups foster a high level of cohe- orcism were frequently used as proselytizing tools to impress
siveness and appear to provide tangible psychological benefits and convert unbelievers. Consequently, possession appears to
for their adherents (Galanter, 1990; Morton, 1977). have been a relatively common occurrence in the early church
Rates of peripheral possession can change within a culture. In while Christianity struggled for supremacy among numerous
societies that condone possession beliefs, cultural changes that competing religions. After Christianity became the state reli-
increase stress levels often produce increased rates of possession gion of the Roman Empire, the frequency of possession and ex-
(Ackerman & Lee, 1981; Phoon, 1982; Sharp, 1990; Teoh, Soe- orcisms appears to have waned.
wondo, & Sidhartha, 1975). For instance, several investigators Beginning in the 11th century with the gradual breakdown
(Ackerman & Lee, 1981; Ong, 1988; Phoon, 1982) have de- of feudalism, Western Europe experienced increased politico-
scribed small epidemics of possession in female Malaysian fac- religious turmoil that, in the 16th century, culminated in the
tory workers. These women frequently carry out boring, repeti- Reformation and in the breakup of Western Christianity into
tive work for very low wages under poor working conditions. competing sects (Russell, 1980). This period also saw the devel-
The antiunion policies of the factories effectively prevent orga- opment and elaboration of the mythology of satanism. Accord-
nized protest. Under these circumstances, epidemics of spirit ing to the tenets of this mythology, there existed an international
possession that involve convulsions and bizarre behavior be- satanic conspiracy bent on destroying Christianity. The agents
come a way of venting distress and frustration, obtaining time of this conspiracy were witches who supposedly worshiped Sa-
off from work, and rebelling against authority. Because the spir- tan at secret meetings where they desecrated the symbols of
its rather than the possessed women are blamed for the distur- Christian worship and engaged in cannibalism, murder, and
bances, possession displays are a safe, albeit indirect, way of ex- sexual orgies (Cohn, 1975). Modern historians have rejected the
pressing grievances. In a factory studied by Ackerman and Lee notion that there actually was a satanic conspiracy or that those
(1981), increases in the frequency of possession episodes fol- accused of witchcraft belonged to a large-scale conspiracy. In-
lowed a change from relaxed to stricter management. Moreover, stead, the idea of a satanic conspiracy existed only in the imagi-
the possession displays occurred only among the Malaysian nation. It existed first in the imagination of cultural elites who
workers whose cultural beliefs made possession an acceptable established the administrative machinery and legal categories
vehicle for the expression of dissatisfaction. Chinese and Indian that made satanic witchcraft a crime and then spread down the
workers in the same factory never exhibited displays of posses- social scale to become part of the taken-for-granted belief sys-
sion. tem of much of the populous (Cohn, 1975; Lamer, 1981; Rus-
Epidemic possession affects numerous people in close prox- sell, 1980; Spanos, 1978).
imity within a short period of time and, therefore, cannot be Possession and exorcism again became common between the
accounted for by theories of multiplicity that emphasize idio- 15th and 17th centuries because, during this period, possession
MULTIPLE IDENTITY ENACTMENTS AND MPD 151

became associated with witchcraft. Both Catholics and Protes- ing, procedures that were much less likely to elicit demon self
tants believed that witches, through Satan's intercession, could enactments.
send demons to possess people. However, the indwelling demons Detailed information concerning role prescriptions was con-
could be coerced by authorities to name the witch that sent veyed to both Catholic and Protestant demoniacs outside of the
them. The accused witch could then be arrested, tortured into exorcism situation. The sources of this information could in-
confessing her involvement in a nonexistent satanic conspiracy, clude explicit coaching by parties who held a vested interest
and, in many cases, executed. Thus, during this period demo- in the demoniacs giving convincing performances, exposure to
niacs frequently functioned as witch finders, and those who con- other more practiced demoniacs, and conversations about the
trolled the demoniacs had a powerful weapon to use against po- occurrence and timing of symptoms that were, held in the de-
litical, social, or personal rivals (Spanos, 1978). moniac's presence (Harsnett, 1599, 1603; Thomas, 1971;
Walker, 1981).
A number of potent social psychological factors converged in
The Socialization of Demoniacs leading potential demoniacs to define themselves as possessed.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

These individuals shared the same belief system as the commu-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

The idea of demon possession was taken for granted in early nity that labeled them and, therefore, tended to interpret their
modern Europe, and the major components of that role were own illness of behavioral deviations in the same terms as their
well-known (Thomas, 1971). The subtle aspects of the role were neighbors and clerical superiors (Thomas, 1971). In some cases,
transmitted through the demoniac's exposure to clerical ex- demoniacs were made dependent for the satisfaction of their
perts. Demonic possession was used as one explanation for cer- physical and social needs on those who labeled them. The label-
tain physical symptoms or for behavior that was socially disrup- ers consistently interpreted the experiences of demoniacs in
tive or considered abnormal. During the initial stages of posses- terms of possession and isolated the demoniacs from others who
sion, the demoniac's symptoms were often ambiguous. might offer nondemonic interpretations of these events (Spanos,
Frequently, these symptoms began to correspond to "official" 1983a).
stereotypes of demonic possession as the demoniac gained in- Individuals sometimes denied that they were possessed; how-
formation about those stereotypes (Spanos, 1983b; Spanos & ever, these denials were routinely construed by authorities as
Gottlieb, 1976; Walker, 1981). indications of a wily demon attempting to escape divine pun-
The symptom displays of the possessed sometimes varied ishment. Continued refusal to define oneself as possessed and
with local beliefs and practices, and these variations demon- act accordingly frequently led to threats of perpetual damna-
strate the social and rule-governed nature of possession. For in- tion and sometimes to punishment administered in the guise of
stance, both Catholic and Protestant demoniacs regularly con- benevolently motivated attempts to free hapless victims from
vulsed and displayed a variety of sensory-motor deficits. In ad- demonic control (Harsnett, 1599; Spanos, 1983a).
dition, Catholic demoniacs invariably exhibited direct evidence
of indwelling demon selves. These demon selves spoke in voices Reasons for Adopting the Possessed Role
different from that of the possessed person, had their own
names, and displayed their own unique demonic personalities. People did not always avoid being labeled demonically pos-
Protestant demoniacs of the same period rarely displayed de- sessed. Like sufferers of peripheral possession in other cultures,
mon selves (Walker, 1981). the demonically possessed could use their enactments strategi-
Protestant-Catholic variation in the frequency of demon self cally. Those who became demoniacs usually had little social
enactments reflected the different practices toward the pos- power, were hemmed in by social restrictions, and had few sanc-
sessed adopted by these religions. During Catholic exorcisms, tioned avenues for protesting or improving on their situations.
the priest communicated directly with the indwelling demon. Given the patriarchal and misogynistic culture of early modern
The priest made a clear distinction between the possessing de- Europe, it is not surprising that adult demoniacs were much
mons and the person possessed. When questioning a demon, the more frequently women than men (Oesterreich, 1966). Child
exorcist expected to be answered by the demon and not by the demoniacs, on the other hand, were frequently of both sexes
person possessed (Oesterreich, 1966). Before beginning the ex- (Spanos, 1983a).
orcism rite, the priest contacted and questioned the demons to For the socially powerless, demonic possession offered nu-
obtain their names, number, reasons for possessing the person, merous advantages. Its adoption could lead to a dramatic rise in
hour they entered the body, and length of time they proposed to social status. On one hand, demoniacs were viewed as the help-
stay (Kelly, 1974; "The Roman Ritual of Exorcism," 1614/ less victims of satanic influence and consequently received sym-
1976). During the exorcism, the demons were often questioned pathetic attention and a reduced workload. On the other hand,
repeatedly and at great length about their motives, earthly ac- they were sometimes treated as awesome seers whose affliction
complices, status in the social structure of hell, and so forth placed them in direct contact with the supernatural and whose
(e.g., Michaelis, 1613). In short, Catholic exorcism procedures performances commanded fearful respect and attention. De-
strongly cued demon self enactments as a central component of moniacs sometimes became the star attractions in what the
the demonic role. In contrast, Protestants rarely used formal community considered a deadly serious combat between the
exorcism procedures because direct communication with de- forces of heaven and hell (Spanos, 1983a; Walker, 1981). Like
mons was shunned as sinful (Thomas, 1971). In place of exor- peripheral possession in other cultures and MPD in North
cism, Protestant demoniacs were treated with prayer and fast- American culture, demonic possession was often chronic. Fre-
152 NICHOLAS P. SPANOS

quently, these individuals were possessed by many demons that port never having seen a case of MPD (Chordoff, 1987; Merskey,
had to be individually exorcised over a long period of time. 1992).
Moreover, even the successful exorcism of all the demons was At present, MPD appears to be a culture-bound syndrome.
no guarantee that they would not return. Thus, once possession The explosion of cases since 1970 has been largely restricted to
had been legitimated, it remained an option that could be used North America. Despite its turn-of-the-century prominence as
as the situation required. a center for the study of MPD, the diagnosis is very rarely made
Like the ritual ceremonies conducted by spirit mediums in in modern France. It is also very rare in Great Britain (Al-
other cultures, the performances of demoniacs often required dridge-Morris, 1989; Fahy, 1988), Russia (R.M.Allison, 1991),
forethought and the judicious use of props. For instance, the and India (Adityhanjee & Khandelwal, 1989), and a recent sur-
spitting up of pins or nails that were supposedly used by in- vey in Japan failed to uncover even a single case (Takahashi,
dwelling demons to torture the demoniac internally was a fairly 1990).
regular feature of English and Continental possession cases Modestin (1992) surveyed all of the psychiatrists in Switzer-
(Notestein, 1911; Oesterreich, 1966). Obviously, the demoniacs land concerning the frequency with which they had seen pa-
had to place the pins in their mouths before their performance
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tients with MPD. Depending on how it was calculated, the prev-


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

in anticipation of spitting them out later and in anticipation of alence rate ranged between 0.5% and 1.0%. More interesting,
the effect that such displays would produce in their audience. Modestin noted that 90% of the respondents had never seen a
In summary, demonic enactments constituted coordinated, case of MPD, whereas 3 psychiatrists had each seen more than
goal-directed self-presentations aimed at conveying and sus- 20 MPD patients. From the data presented in this article, it
taining the impression that the actors were possessed by evil appears that 66% of the MPD diagnoses were made by only 6 of
spirits. As in cross-cultural manifestations of spirit possession, 655 (0.09%) psychiatrists. Unlike North American samples in
a major feature of the demonic role involved conveying the im- which women are at least three times more likely than men to
pression that behaviors were no longer governed by the actor. receive an MPD diagnosis, Modestin found that 51% of the
However, conveying this impression convincingly required that MPD diagnoses were given to men.
demoniacs retain behavioral control and gear their enactments The historical changes in the prevalence of MPD, the sub-
to contextual demands in a manner consistent with their audi- stantial national differences in prevalence and in gender ratios,
ence's conception of what constituted possession. and the large differences in the frequency with which different
clinicians make the diagnosis are difficult to account for parsi-
Multiple Personality Disorder moniously in terms of a disease perspective. They are, however,
reminiscent of the historical changes in the prevalence of de-
Cases that today would be labeled as MPD were reported in- monic possession, the large cross-cultural differences in the
frequently during the first three quarters of the 19th century. prevalence of spirit possession, and the large differences in rates
An increase in the number of cases was reported in the last of glossolalia seen between different churches in North Amer-
quarter of the 19th and the early years of the 20th century, and ica. In short, the prevalence data for MPD suggest that MPD,
most of these were from either France or the United States like possession phenomena, is a social creation that varies in
(Hacking, 1986). By the end of the first 2 decades of the 20th frequency as a function of the expectations for its occurrence
century, the number of cases had dropped substantially, and that are transmitted to patients. The most important sources of
from the 1920s to 1970 only a handful were reported such expectations are therapists committed to the MPD diag-
worldwide. Since 1970, the number of cases reported has in- nosis, but other sources (e.g., media) have become increasingly
creased astronomically, and thousands have now been reported important as information about MPD has infused into the
(Putnam, 1989). Investigators who are sympathetic to the MPD wider culture.
diagnosis are particularly likely to find high rates of occurrence An alternative to the social creation hypothesis holds that the
of the disorder. For instance, Bliss, Larson, and Nakashima incidence of MPD has not really increased. According to this
(1983) reported that 60% of the 45 patients admitted to a single view, MPD was previously (and often continues to be) misdiag-
inpatient ward with auditory hallucinations were actually nosed as schizophrenia, psychopathy, and various other disor-
suffering from MPD. Bliss and Jeppsen (1985) reported that ders (R. B. Allison & Schwarz, 1980; Rosenbaum, 1980; Ross,
approximately 8 of 50 (16%) sequentially admitted inpatients Norton, & Wozney, 1989). Purportedly, the current increased
on an acute psychiatric ward and 9 of 100 (9%) private psychi- prevalence of the disorder is apparent rather than real and re-
atric outpatients suffered from MPD. Ross, Anderson, Fleisher, flects the fact that actual MPD cases are incorrectly diagnosed
and Norton (1991) diagnosed 3.3% of a sample of 299 psychi- less frequently than previously. This account is also sometimes
atric inpatients as suffering from MPD. Despite the high rates used to explain national differences in MPD rates (Altrocchi,
of occurrence found in these studies and the high rates reported 1992). According to this view, psychiatrists in other countries
more informally by numerous other clinicians who are strong have not yet developed the diagnostic acumen required to rec-
supporters of the MPD diagnosis (e.g., R. B. Allison & Schwarz, ognize MPD.
1980; Braun, 1984; Kluft, 1982), some studies have reported A major problem with this account is that the symptoms of
very low rates of occurrence. Thus, Merskey and Buhrich MPD are, in fact, quite distinctive. A patient who calls herself
(1975) found no MPD among 89 patients diagnosed with dis- Mary on one day and Jane on another and who behaves very
sociative or conversion disorders. In addition, a number of differently as Mary than as Jane is unlikely to go unrecognized
highly experienced clinicians with many years of practice re- as a candidate for an MPD diagnosis by even an inexperienced
MULTIPLE IDENTITY ENACTMENTS AND MPD 153

clinician. To deal with this difficulty, proponents of the disease ence between therapists who diagnose MPD and those who do
model argue that such patients are skilled at hiding their multi- not is the use by the former of hypnotic procedures for uncov-
plicity and reveal it only when those with acumen enough to ering hidden memories. Wilbur (1984) described the process:
correctly interpret the subtle signs use diagnostic procedures "The patient is hypnotized and each alternate, in turn, is asked
that bring forth the multiple identities. In fact, the proponents to tell what precipitated it into the life of the birth personality"
of the MPD diagnosis agree that most patients who eventually (p. 28). Bliss (1986) elaborated on the procedure:
receive this diagnosis do not enter therapy complaining about
multiple personalities, do not exhibit clear-cut evidence of I then suggest that the [hypnotized] patient look into her mind to
multiplicity, and do not know that they possess alter identities see if there is anyone or anything there. If anything or a person is
identified I want to know who it is. [Once an alter has been identi-
(R. B. Allison, 1978; Franklin, 1988; Wilbur, 1984). These in- fied] I then make a rapid survey by asking the personality . . . if
vestigators regard MPD as a "hidden syndrome" in which 80% she has a name; how long she has been there; the patient's age when
of such patients were unaware of the existence of alters before she [the alter] came; whether the patient knows her; whether she
entering treatment with the therapist who uncovered their ever takes over the body; whether she ever directs or influences the
patient when the patient has the body; her mission or function; and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

multiplicity (Kluft, 1985). From a sociocognitive perspective, whether there are other people back there, (pp. 196-197)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

these data suggest instead that the procedures used to diagnose


MPD often create rather than discover multiplicity. Given the highly leading nature of these procedures, it comes
as no surprise that there often are "other people back there,"
Teaching Multiplicity and they are identified and surveyed in the same manner. The
flavor of such interviews is conveyed in the following verbatim
Proponents of the MPD diagnosis have described a large and excerpt from a hypnotic interview with a suspected murderer
diverse number of signs that supposedly might indicate this dis- named Ken Bianchi. Because it was conducted in a forensic
order and that can be used to justify probing for confirmation. context, Bianchi's interview was videotaped. After a hypnotic
Some of these signs include depression, somataform symptoms, induction procedure, the clinician proceeded as follows:
headaches, periods of missing time, impaired concentration,
hallucinations, sexual dysfunctions, fatigue, and drug abuse I've talked a bit to Ken but I think that perhaps there might be
(Coons et al., 1988; Ross, Norton, & Wozney, 1989). There are another part of Ken that I haven't talked to. And I would like to
many more. R. B. Allison (1978) even suggested that a smooth communicate with that part. And I would like that other part to
complexion might indicate MPD because the regular switching come and talk to me.. . . And when you are here, lift the left hand
off the chair to signal to me that you are here. Would you please
of alter personalities supposedly prevents the formation of wrin- come part so I can talk to you.. . . Part would you come and lift
kles. Even this truncated list makes it clear that a large set of Ken's hand to indicate to me that you are here? . . . Would you
presenting symptoms can be used as possible indicators of talk to me part by saying "I'm here." (Schwarz, 1981, pp. 142-143)
MPD.
Once the diagnosis is suspected, it is common practice to ask During this interview, Bianchi displayed a second personality
leading and explicit questions in an attempt to confirm it. Put- named Steve who confessed to the murders with which Ken had
nam (1989), for example, asked such questions as "Do you ever been charged. The clinician who conducted this interview
feel as if you are not alone, as if there is someone else or some pointed out that he did nothing unusual in this case and used
other part watching you?" (p. 90). Others apply stronger pres- such interview procedures regularly to diagnose MPD (Wat-
sure. Merskey (1992) recently reviewed a large number of MPD kins, 1984). In other words, leading hypnotic interviews that
cases from the 20th century and earlier that provided informa- repeatedly inform the patient that he or she has other parts that
tion about treatment procedures and symptomatology. His re- can be addressed and communicated with as if they were sepa-
view included well-known cases such as those of Sally Beau- rate people are used routinely when diagnosing MPD.
champ (Prince, 1908), The Three Faces of Eve (Thigpen & Spanos, Weekes, and Bertrand (1985) used the hypnotic in-
Cleckley, 1957), and Sybil (Schreiber, 1973), as well as many terview that was employed with Bianchi to test the hypothesis
lesser known ones. Merskey (1992) found that highly leading that such procedures can provide even naive subjects with the
and suggestive procedures have long been routine in the diagno- information required to enact multiple identities. The subjects
sis of MPD. In some cases, therapists insisted to doubting pa- were college students asked to pretend that they were in Bi-
tients that they were multiples and even supplied the patients anchi's situation, had been accused of committing a series of
with names for their alters. Along these lines, it is worth noting murders, and had been remanded for a psychiatric interview.
that Allison and Schwarz (1980) contended that patients are Subjects were told nothing about multiple personality. They
frequently reluctant to accept that they are multiples and, under were simply instructed to use whatever background informa-
these circumstances, should be actively persuaded by their ther- tion they possessed and whatever they could glean from their
apist. interview to behave the way they believed an accused in that
Perhaps the most common procedure used to elicit evidence situation would behave.
of multiplicity is the use of highly leading hypnotic interviews Subjects in one group were administered an interview mod-
during which alter personalities are explicitly suggested and ex- eled closely on the one used with Bianchi. Role-playing control
plicitly asked to "come forth" and talk with the therapist (Alli- subjects were also interviewed. These subjects were told that
son & Schwarz, 1980; Bliss, 1980, 1986; Brandsma & Ludwig, personality was complex and involved walled-off thoughts and
1974). In fact, R. B. Allison (1978) stated that the major differ- feelings, but the interviewing "psychiatrist" never informed the
154 NICHOLAS P. SPANOS

controls that they possessed another part that could be commu- produce their secular counterparts: multiple indwelling person-
nicated with directly. alities.
Most of the role players given the Bianchi interview enacted
symptoms of MPD by (a) adopting a different name, (b) refer-
Motivations, Legitimation, and Multiple Personality
ring to their primary personality in the third person, and (c)
displaying amnesia for their alter personalities after termination
Disorder
of the hypnotic interview. None of the role-playing controls dis- Information about MPD is widespread in North American
played any of these symptoms. culture, and the major components of the role are now well
In a second session, the "psychiatrist" again contacted the known to the general public. Popular TV movies like Sybil and
alter personalities of the role-playing multiples. These subjects popular biographies like The Minds of Billy Milligan (Keyes,
maintained their role successfully in the second session by ex- 1981) provide extensive information about the symptoms of
hibiting marked and consistent differences between their pri- MPD, and MPD patients, along with their psychiatrists, are
mary and alter personalities on a variety of psychological tests. sometimes even featured on popular TV talk shows. In all of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Role-playing controls performed similarly on the two adminis- these sources, MPD patients are shown in an attractive light as
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

trations of the test. A replication of this study by Rabinowitz people with dramatic symptoms who, with the help of devoted
(1989) yielded similar findings. and empathic therapists, surmount numerous obstacles to
Spanos, Weekes, Menary, and Bertrand (1986) extended the eventually gain self-esteem, dignity, health, happiness, and
findings of Spanos, Weekes, and Bertrand (1985) and Rabinow- much sympathetic attention from high-status others. In short,
itz (1989) by exposing role-playing multiples to interviews that the idea of being a multiple, like the idea of suffering from pe-
focused on their childhood experiences. The task of the role ripheral possession or demonic possession, may provide some
players was not, of course, to describe their actual childhoods people with a viable and face-saving way to account for personal
but, to use cues gleaned from the interviews to present them- problems as well as a dramatic means for gaining concern and
selves as actual patients. Like the histories given by actual MPD attention from significant others. The role of the media in fos-
patients, the role-playing multiples gave negatively toned de- tering MPD was evident in a report by Gruenwald (1971) con-
scriptions of childhood, described their parents as punitive and cerning a 17-year-old, hospitalized female patient. This pa-
rejecting, described an early onset (before 10 years of age) for tient's first enactment of an alter personality occurred the day
their alter personalities, and described their alters as "taking after seeing the movie The Three Faces of Eve on television.
over" to handle difficult situations and express strong emotions. Relatedly, Fahy, Abas, and Brown (1989) reported on a patient
The findings of these studies are straightforward. They demon- who presented symptoms of MPD and who had seen the movie
strate that the interviewing procedures used routinely to diag- The Three Faces of Eve and read the book Sybil (Schreiber,
nose MPD convey all of the information required to allow even 1973).
psychiatrically unsophisticated subjects to enact the cardinal Thigpen and Cleckley (1957), the authors of The Three Faces
symptoms of multiplicity. of Eve, commented on the attractions of an MPD diagnosis.
All of the subjects in these studies had been explicitly asked After publication of their famous book, they were frequently
to fake their responses, and, consequently, it can be argued that contacted by people who displayed symptoms similar to those
people who were not faking would be unlikely to develop reported in the book for Eve and who sought out the authors to
multiple identities after exposure to leading hypnotic in- legitimate their self-diagnosis of multiplicity:
terviews. This argument is contradicted by the evidence de- [Many of these patients] appeared to be motivated (either con-
scribed earlier that demonstrated that hypnotic procedures that sciously or unconsciously) by a desire to draw attention to them-
were less leading than the one used with Bianchi led regularly selves. Certainly a diagnosis of multiple personality attracts a good
to enactments of hidden selves and past-life personalities in deal more attention than most other diagnoses. Some patients ap-
pear to be motivated by secondary gain associated with avoiding
nonsimulating college students (Spanos & Hewitt, 1980; responsibility for certain actions. (Thigpen & Cleckley, 1984, p. 64)
Spanos, Menary, etal., 1991).
Hypnotic interviews used to diagnose MPD are highly remi- Given the attractions of the MPD diagnosis and the wide-
niscent of Catholic exorcism procedures. They have, of course, spread knowledge of its symptoms, it is not surprising that pa-
been secularized to meet the materialist assumptions of 20th- tients sometimes present such symptoms in the absence of cu-
century psychiatry, but the major components are the same. ing from therapists. Nevertheless, people are unlikely to sustain
The hypnotist has replaced the priest, and he or she searches enactment of such a role in the absence of legitimation. For in-
out and obtains the names of alters rather than demons. The stance, the therapist who treated the case described by Fahy et
priest "discovered" when the demons entered the body; the hyp- al. (1989) directed attention away from the patient's alters and
notist "discovers" when each alter split from the "birth person- focused on her other problems in living. In the absence of the
ality." The priest discovered why each demon entered the per- therapist's legitimation, the patient's MPD enactments went
son; the hypnotist discovers the supposed trauma that led to the into sharp decline.
formation of each alter. The priest discovered the number of The importance of interpersonal legitimation in the mainte-
indwelling demons; the hypnotist discovers the number of al- nance of alter identity enactments was demonstrated by Koh-
ters, and so on. Exorcisms led regularly to the production of lenberg (1973). Kohlenberg described how the psychiatric staff
multiple indwelling demons, and hypnotic interviews appear to of a ward that housed a multiple were sensitized to his three
MULTIPLE IDENTITY ENACTMENTS AND MPD 155

different personality enactments and interacted with each per- to adopt a view of themselves that is congruent with the view
sonality in a different manner. To demonstrate the importance conveyed to them by their therapist. Adoption of this view in-
of contextual variables in maintaining multiple identity enact- volves clients coming to construe their various "symptoms"
ments, baseline rates of occurrence were assessed for the behav- (e.g., mood swings, shameful or unrepresentative behaviors,
iors associated with each of the patient's three personalities. Af- ambivalent feelings, hostile fantasies, forgetfulness, guilt-induc-
terward, the behaviors associated with only one of the personal- ing sexual feelings, and bad habits) as the results of personified
ities were selectively reinforced. The behaviors associated with alter selves. In North American culture, it is common for people
the reinforced personality increased dramatically in frequency. to describe uncharacteristic or ambivalent feelings and behav-
In later extinction trials, the frequency of occurrence of these iors metaphorically as resulting from different parts of them-
behaviors decreased to baseline levels. selves (e.g., "One part of me wanted to do it but another part
Currently, the legitimation of MPD often involves a social said no" or "I'm of two minds about the issue"). A sociocogni-
dimension that transcends patient-therapist interactions that tive analysis suggests that the development of MPD involves a
occur in the consulting room. In some respects, advocacy of reification of such metaphors that leads both the client and the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the MPD diagnosis has taken on the characteristics of a social therapist to construe the client as possessing multiple selves
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

movement (Mulhern, 1991b, in press). MPD patients, along (Sarbin, in press).


with therapists, participate regularly in MPD workshops and
conferences, and both patients and therapists frequently have Child Abuse and Multiple Personality Disorder
access to national newsletters that provide updated information
about the syndrome (Victor, 1993). Along with their individual Most studies find that MPD patients report extremely high
therapy, many patients participate in MPD self-help and ther- rates of childhood sexual or physical abuse, or both (e.g., Coons
apy groups that provide ongoing legitimation for their multiple et al., 1988; Ross, Miller, et al., 1991; Ross, Norton, & Wozney,
self enactments. Patients in one highly vocal subset who appear 1989). These findings are the major source of empirical support
to enjoy their MPD status have even asserted their right to re- for the hypothesis that MPD results from early trauma. As de-
main multiples (Mulhern, 1991b, in press). Some therapists scribed earlier, however, data obtained from North American
employ MPD patients as cotherapists to help convince skeptical spirit mediums, as well as from experimental subjects who re-
new patients that their MPD diagnosis is accurate (Allison & port past-life identities, indicate that multiplicity can occur in
Schwarz, 1980). Perry (1992) estimated that 17% of the thera- the absence of early child abuse. In addition, the correlational
pists treating MPD are themselves patients or former patients nature of the MPD/child abuse findings precludes their estab-
diagnosed with MPD or other dissociative disorders (see also lishing a causal relationship between abuse and MPD. More-
Mulhern, in press). These therapists, who help to socialize new over, the frequent reports of child abuse from MPD patients can
patient recruits into the MPD role, are reminiscent of those in be accounted for without positing that abuse causes MPD.
traditional cultures who, after their own possession, join and The sexual abuse of children appears to be a relatively com-
sometimes become leaders of the possession cults that shape and mon occurrence in North American culture, and such abuse
legitimate the spirit possession enactments of new members. occurs more frequently in girls than in boys (Finkelhor, 1979).
In summary, the importance of psychotherapy and therapy- For instance, depending on the criteria for defining abuse and
related social supports in the genesis and maintenance of MPD the characteristics of the samples assessed, rates of reported
would be hard to overemphasize. Therapists are, after all, typi- childhood sexual abuse in women ranged from a low of'27% to a
cally viewed by their clients as competent experts whose opin- high of 62% in a series of studies reviewed by Pope and Hudson
ions are highly valued and whose suggestions are treated seri- (1992). Even higher rates have been reported in some clinical
ously. In addition, psychotherapy clients are often insecure, un- samples. Briere and Zaidi (1989) found that 70% of 50 consec-
happy people with a strong investment in winning the concern, utive female patients visiting an urban emergency room re-
interest, and approval of their therapist. This is likely to be par- ported a history of child sexual abuse. The short-term and long-
ticularly true of the polysymptomatic, chronically disturbed term psychological effects of child sexual abuse are by no means
women with a long history of psychiatric involvement who are clear because of serious methodological problems in the studies
typically diagnosed as MPD sufferers. Given these circum- that have addressed these issues (Beitchman, Zucker, Hood, Da-
stances, mutual shaping between therapists "on the lookout" Costa, & Akman, 1991; Beitchman et al., 1992). Nevertheless,
for signs of MPD and clients involved in creating an impression people who were abused as children often report a range of psy-
that will elicit approval is likely to lead to enactments of chiatric symptoms and undoubtedly come to the attention of
multiple personality that confirm the initial suspicions of the mental health professionals at least as frequently as the non-
therapist and, in turn, lead the therapist to encourage and vali- abused. Consequently, the high rate of abuse in MPD patients
date more elaborate displays of the disorder (Sutcliffe & Jones, can be partly explained by the fact that a substantial proportion
1962). In addition, the newsletters, therapy groups, workshops, of chronically disturbed people who seek psychiatric help (par-
and informal interactions with other multiples that have be- ticularly women) are likely to report a history of child abuse. In
come an important part of the social life of many MPD patients addition, because some clinicians consider a history of abuse to
serve to continually shape and legitimate multiple self enact- be a possible sign of MPD, they may be more likely to expose
ments. abused than nonabused patients to leading hypnotic interviews
This analysis does not imply that MPD patients are typically and other "diagnostic" procedures that generate displays of
faking their multiplicity. Instead, it suggests that patients come multiplicity. To the extent that this occurs, the idea that early
156 NICHOLAS P. SPANOS

abuse is associated with MPD becomes a self-fulfilling proph- for the first time during therapy (Loftus, 1993b; Wakefield &
ecy. Underwager, 1992).
Some patients who receive an MPD diagnosis do not remem- The strong connection between child abuse and MPD is of
ber having been abused in childhood until their multiplicity is recent origin. Cases reported in the early part of the 20th cen-
"discovered" in therapy. In these cases, the patient's alter per- tury and before were much less likely than modern cases to be
sonalities report abuse that had purportedly been dissociated associated with reports of child abuse (Bowman, 1990; Kenny,
from the memory of the presenting personality. Under these cir- 1986). Moreover, the abuse that was reported in these early
cumstances, there is reason to believe that such newly "remem- cases lacked the lurid ritualistic elements that are becoming an
bered" abuse is often fabricated. In other words, patients who increasingly prominent characteristic of the abuse memories
develop the expectation that they must have been abused may proffered by modern MPD patients. An association between
construct fantasies of such abuse. Given their expectations and MPD and child abuse first came to prominence in the 1970s
the validation of these fantasies as memories by their therapists, with the concurrent rise in public interest in child sexual abuse.
the fantasies are experienced as real memories of abuse (Loftus, In the early 1970s, the book Sybil (Schreiber, 1973) described
1993b; Spanos, Burgess, & Burgess, in press).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the sadistic childhood abuse purportedly suffered by an MPD


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

According to MPD proponents, abuse that occurs during patient. The book and later television movie were exceedingly
childhood is often so traumatic that it is forgotten (i.e., dissoci- popular and Sybil, even more than Eve (Thigpen & Cleckley,
ated from the primary personality). This hypothesis predicts 1957) in the previous decade, became a model of the MPD sur-
that children who have suffered documented psychological vivor that greatly influenced the expectations of therapists and
trauma will later be unable to recall that trauma. However, the patients alike (Putnam, 1989). Consequently, when MPD pa-
available data gathered in prospective studies of traumatized tients claim that they cannot remember having been abused,
children suggest instead that children who are more than 4 or 5 therapists tend to disbelieve them (Bliss, 1986). Instead, thera-
years of age at the time that they are traumatized usually re- pists may prod them repeatedly in an attempt to unearth such
member rather than forget the traumatic events years after their memories. When patients believe they may be fantasizing rather
occurrence (Femina, Yeager, & Lewis, 1990; Terr, 1988, 1990). than remembering abuse, their uncertainty may be presented
Femina et al. (1990) found that people who were abused as chil- to them as evidence that they are unwilling to face the fact of
dren sometimes denied the abuse when interviewed years later. their abuse (Bliss, 1986). In short, MPD therapists appear to
In a second interview, however, these subjects invariably ac- frequently use leading and suggestive procedures to elicit abuse
knowledged remembering the abuse and described their earlier memories from their patients (Mulhern, 1991 a).
denials as due to factors such as embarrassment about what had A number of years after the publication of Sybil, a book titled
occurred rather than memory loss. Obviously, the findings of Michelle Remembers (Smith & Pazder, 1980) reported on ritual
these studies do not demonstrate that people never forget early satanic tortures that a woman had purportedly experienced
abuse. Nevertheless, they suggest that reports of previously for- during childhood and then forgotten until they were recovered
gotten childhood trauma that emerge for the first time during during therapy. Michelle's story became a part of the propa-
therapy should be treated cautiously rather than accepted as ac- ganda used by the evangelical Christian movement that became
curate descriptions of early events.2 increasingly prominent in many facets of American social and
It is now generally acknowledged that memory involves re- political life during the 1980s. This movement reinvigorated the
constructive elements and is strongly influenced by current be- mythology of satanism. Like its 16th- and 17th-century prede-
liefs and expectations (Friedman, 1993; Loftus, 1979). People cessor, this reinvigorated mythology holds that there exists a
typically organize their recall of past events in a way that makes powerful but secret international satanic conspiracy that carries
sense of their present situation and is congruent with their cur- out heinous crimes. These crimes supposedly include the kid-
rent expectations. The specifics of what they recall are fre- napping, torture, and sexual abuse of countless children as well
quently in error and involve a mixture of correctly remembered
and misremembered information that is often impossible to 2
It is frequently hypothesized in the MPD literature that children
disentangle. Frequently, there is little or no correlation between react to abuse and other trauma by imagining that they are elsewhere or
the accuracy of recall and the confidence that people place in that the negative events are happening to another (imaginary) person
their recall. It is not unusual for people to be convinced about (e.g., Bliss, 1986). This is an interesting hypothesis that deserves to be
the accuracy of a remembrance that turns out to be false (Lof- examined in studies of abused children rather than through the retro-
tus, 1979; Wells, Ferguson, & Lindsay, 1981). In addition, lead- spective reports of adult MPD patients. Note, however, that many chil-
ing questions and other suggestive interview procedures can dren do not appear to respond in this way to abuse (Terr, 1990). More-
produce a substantial deterioration in recall accuracy even over, even if it should turn out that some children do respond in this way,
when subjects remain highly confident of their inaccurate re- there is no reason to assume that such responding involves a "hypnotic
state," that the abuse events will be forgotten, or that hypnotic proce-
membrances (Spanos, Gwynn, et al., 1989). Contrary to popu-
dures facilitate the accurate recall of forgotten traumatic events. The
lar belief, hypnotic procedures do not reliably enhance the ac- frequency with which adults report that they forgot early abuse for some
curacy of recall and, at least under some circumstances, may period of time varies greatly across studies. Such variability is probably
lead subjects to become even more overconfident than usual in influenced by differences across studies in interview methodologies and
their inaccurate recall (Smith, 1983; Spanos, Quigley, Gwynn, the criteria used to define forgetting. The difficulties involved in draw-
Glatt, & Perlini, 1991). The fallibility of memory should be kept ing inferences about memory processes on the basis of such reports have
in mind when examining reports of child abuse that emerge been discussed by Loftus, Polonsky, and Fullilove (in press).
MULTIPLE IDENTITY ENACTMENTS AND MPD 157

as murder, forced pregnancies, and cannibalism (Bromley, vestigation, and many of the events to which he confessed were
1991; Hicks, 1991; Lyons, 1988). suggested to him by the police officers and psychologist who
Large numbers of therapists who identified themselves as ac- interrogated him. The events to which he confessed were legiti-
tive Christians joined the MPD movement in the 1980s (Mul- mized as memories rather than fantasies by his fundamentalist
hern, in press), and soon accounts like those of Michelle began minister. Ofshe interrogated the suspect with the leading ques-
to be reported by the alters of MPD patients (Eraser, 1990; tions and guided fantasy procedures used by the police and sug-
Young et al., 1991). By the mid-1980s, 25% of MPD patients in gested to him that he had committed a set of abuse events that
therapy had recovered memories of ritual satanic abuse, and, by the police agreed had not occurred. The suspect readily con-
1992, the percentage of patients recovering such memories was fessed to the false events and, after the interrogation, insisted
as high as 80% in some treatment facilities (Mulhern, in press). that those events had really occurred. Although the suspect had
If the ritual satanic crimes "remembered" by MPD patients no psychiatric history before his arrest, he was diagnosed by
actually occurred, it would necessitate the existence of a monu- one psychologist involved in the case as suffering from MPD.
mental criminal conspiracy that has been in existence for at Whitley (1992) described several former patients from the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

least 50 years and has been responsible for the murder of thou- same psychiatric clinic who reported ritual abuse memories
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sands of people (Hicks, 1991). Law enforcement agencies during their therapy but later disavowed the reality of these re-
throughout North America have investigated numerous allega- ports. The patients (some of whom were diagnosed as multiples)
tions of satanic abuse made by MPD patients but have been described their reports of early abuse as fantasies that were sug-
unable to substantiate the existence of the requisite criminal gested and encouraged by their therapists and by copatients in
conspiracy (Lanning, 1989). It is important to understand that psychotherapy groups. These patients reported a great deal of
criminal conspiracies are very difficult to hide. This, of course, interpersonal pressure to generate reports of severe abuse and
is particularly true when large numbers of conspirators are in- to define those reports as memories as opposed to fantasies.
volved and the crimes include murder and cannibalism, which Some patients report memory fragments or dreams with sa-
leave physical evidence that is difficult to eliminate (Lanning, tanic content and only afterward are exposed to hypnotic in-
1989). Thus, the repeated failure of law enforcement agencies terviews aimed at confirming such abuse. However, because
to obtain support for the satanic abuse allegations of MPD pa- many MPD patients are enmeshed in a social network in which
tients constitutes strong evidence that the vast majority of these they hear about satanic abuse from other patients, therapists,
allegations are false and that the "memories" on which they are and shared newsletters and in which they or their fellow patients
based are fantasies rather than remembrances of actual events attend workshops devoted to such abuse, "spontaneous"
(Hicks, 1991). Like the satanic conspiracy of the 16th and 17th dreams and memories of this kind are hardly surprising and
centuries, the modern conspiracy exists only in the imagina- do not provide serious evidence of actual ritual abuse. In this
tions of its propagators. context, it is worth recalling the ease with which highly hypno-
Recently, Bottoms, Shaver, and Goodman (1991) surveyed tizable college students were induced to enact past-life person-
psychologists across the United States about the frequency with alities who "remembered" that they had been abused as chil-
which they had seen patients who reported ritual abuse memo- dren when the expectation of such abuse had been conveyed to
ries. Seventy percent of the respondents reported that they had them before their hypnotic regression (Spanos, Menary, et al.,
never seen such patients. A small minority, however, reported 1991). Also relevant are the reports of glossolalics in some con-
having seen large numbers of patients who reported ritual gregations and spirit mediums in some cultures who reorganize
abuse. This pattern of findings suggests that therapists who ob- their biographies to bring them into line with cultural expecta-
tain such reports regularly may play an active role in shaping tions concerning the past histories of possessed people.
the ritual abuse memories of their patients. The reporting of ritual satanic abuse is not the only unusual
Mulhern (199la) described the hypnotic procedures some- characteristic of the child abuse reported by MPD patients. For
times used to elicit satanic ritual abuse memories in MPD pa- instance, some evidence suggests that the populations from
tients: which subjects are selected influence the incidence with which
MPD patients report child abuse histories. Ross, Norton, and
During hypnotic interviews clinicians explicitly described satanic Fraser (1989) found that a sample of American psychiatrists
ritual scenes or displayed pictures of satanic symbols to a patient;
then addressed "all parts of the patient's mind" or "everyone in- reported a much higher prevalence of child sexual abuse in their
side," requesting that any part who recognized the satanic material MPD patients (81.2%) than did Canadian psychiatrists (45.5%).
so indicate either by a nod of the head or by prearranged yes, no Relatedly, Ross (1991) reported that subjects drawn from a non-
and stop ideomotor signals . . . is it possible that these clinicians clinical population and diagnosed with MPD reported much
never paused to consider just what kind of message a patient would lower rates of child abuse than MPD patients drawn from clin-
receive from a clinician who is holding up snapshots and asking if
the patient can identify people as leaders of a group of cannibalistic ical populations. The findings of these studies challenge the idea
devil worshippers, (p. 610) of a causal relationship between child abuse and MPD.
Studies on the sexual abuse of children (reviewed by Wake-
The importance of leading interrogations in obtaining mem- field & Underwager, 1991) indicate that the abusers are very
ories of ritual abuse was underscored in the case of a suspect unlikely to be female and that this is particularly true when the
described by Ofshe (1992) who confessed to raping and ritually victim is female. Some studies of hospitalized adolescents who
abusing his own children as part of a satanic cult. None of the had been sexually abused as children reported that the perpe-
satanic elements of his story could be confirmed by police in- trators were never female (e.g., Sansonnet-Hayden, Haley, Mar-
158 NICHOLAS P. SPANOS

riage, & Fine, 1987). Given the consistency of these findings, also include requests to construct an as if scenario: to behave
those reported by Ross, Miller, et al. (1991) for MPD patients and experience as if one has alter identities.
raise suspicions about validity. These investigators reported MPD proponents interpret the finding that MPD patients fre-
that 15.7% of their MPD patients had been sexually abused by quently attain high hypnotizability to mean that hypnotizabil-
their mothers; 2.9%, 10.8%, and 21.6%, respectively, had been ity scores reflect individual differences in the capacity for disso-
so abused by their stepmothers, other female relatives, and other ciation (Putnam, 1993). This hypothesis predicts a substantial
females. Unfortunately, Ross, Miller, et al. (1991) did not pro- correlation between hypnotizability and independent indexes of
vide the total percentage of patients abused by a female; how- dissociation. Stava and Jaffa (1988) developed several objective
ever, that total obviously must have been quite substantial and indexes of dissociative capacity (e.g., degree of success at divid-
well above what would be expected on the basis of other studies ing attention and performing two tasks simultaneously). None
of child abuse. Ross, Miller, et al. (1991) also reported on the age of these correlated significantly with hypnotizability. Bernstein
of earliest sexual abuse for their subjects. A substantial 26.6% and Putnam (1986) developed a questionnaire index of dissoci-
reported being sexually abused before 3 years of age, and 10.6% ation, and Putnam (1989) reported that it correlated signifi-
reported being sexually abused before their first birthday. The
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cantly with hypnotizability. However, two studies (Nadon,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

fact that these ages are much younger than the age at which Hoyt, Register, & Kihlstrom, 1991; Spanos, Arango, & de
abuse typically begins (see Wakefield & Underwager, 1992) is Groot, 1993) found that dissociation scores correlated signifi-
problematic. More troubling is that these data were based en- cantly with hypnotizability only when both indexes were as-
tirely on the retrospective reports of the patients. Studies on sessed in the same context. When dissociation was assessed in a
recall of early memories have usually indicated that people are context that subjects did not associate with their hypnotizabil-
unable to recall events that occurred before 3 years of age, and ity testing, it failed to correlate significantly with hypnotizabil-
these studies have reported uniformly that subjects are unable ity. These findings indicate a lack of any intrinsic relationship
to recall events before their first birthday (Howe & Courage, between dissociation scores and hypnotizability. Whether or not
1993; Loftus, 1993a). Ross, Miller, et al. (1991) said nothing these dimensions are found to be related appears to be depen-
about corroboration of their patients' early memories, and one dent on expectations generated by testing both dimensions in
is left wondering how these patients were able to remember the same context.
what happened to them before the age of 1 or 2 years. Most studies report no differences between the sexes in hyp-
In summary, the high rates of child abuse reported by MPD notizability (for a review, see deGroh, 1989). In North Ameri-
patients do not constitute good evidence that such abuse causes can studies, however, women are assigned the MPD diagnosis
multiplicity. A number of different noncausal factors probably much more frequently than men, whereas the one Swiss study
contribute to the high rates of child abuse reported by these (Modestin, 1992) found that men were assigned the diagnosis
patients (e.g., high base rates for abuse in chronic psychiatric slightly more frequently than women. These findings indicate
populations, the use of abuse histories as a criterion for con- that the relationship between MPD and hypnotizability must
ducting MPD-eliciting hypnotic interviews, and treatment-in- be moderated by other variables. The hypothesis that this mod-
duced fabrication). No doubt, many people who become MPD erator is early child abuse requires that child abuse occur at
patients were abused during childhood. Nevertheless, most peo- least three times more frequently in girls than in boys in North
ple who suffer even severe child abuse do not exhibit MPD, and America but with about equal frequency in boys and girls in
many people who have not been abused can easily and quickly Switzerland. This hypothesis further suggests that the combina-
be induced to display multiplicity (e.g., college students given tion of high hypnotizability and early child abuse is much rarer
past-life regression suggestions and mentally healthy spirit in Great Britain and in other societies with very low rates of
mediums). Taken together, these findings argue against a MPD than it is in North America. The available evidence does
causal relationship between child abuse and later displays of not indicate that high hypnotizability is more common in
multiplicity. North America than in Great Britain, nor does the evidence
indicate very low rates of child abuse in Great Britain (La Fon-
Hypnotizability and Multiple Personality Disorder taine, 1990).3

MPD patients frequently attain high scores on standardized 3


MPD advocates sometimes postulate that early abuse leads to the
hypnotizability scales (Bliss, 1980,1986; Bliss & Larson, 1985). use of dissociation as a characteristic coping style (e.g., Putnam, 1993).
The suggestions included on these scales are communications According to this hypothesis, a history of child abuse should be associ-
that call for the construction of "as if" situations (Bertrand, ated with both high hypnotizability and high scores on attribute mea-
1989). Thus, suggestions for arm levitation, hallucination, and sures of dissociation. Although several studies have indicated that hyp-
amnesia are, in effect, tacit requests to use imaginative and notizability correlates significantly with severity of childhood punish-
ment and with child abuse (Hilgard, 1970; Nash & Lynn, 1986; Nash,
other cognitive abilities to behave as if one's arm is rising by Lynn, & Givens, 1984), more recent studies have failed to replicate this
itself, as if one is unable to remember, and so forth. Behaving as relationship (DiTomasso & Routh, 1993; Nash, Hulsey, Sexton, Harral-
if an imaginary scenario is true involves creating the requisite son & Lambert, 1993). Several studies (e.g., Chu & Dill, 1990; DiTo-
subjective experiences as well as generating the requisite behav- nasso & Routh, 1993; Nash et al., 1993) have reported significant rela-
iors (Sarbin & Coe, 1972; Spanos, Rivers, & Ross, 1977). The tionships between a history of child abuse and dissociation scores. How-
hypnotic interviews typically used to "call up" alter personali- ever, the correlations between these variables are often of small
ties, like the standardized suggestions on hypnotizability scales, magnitude (e.g., correlations between sexual abuse and dissociation and
MULTIPLE IDENTITY ENACTMENTS AND MPD 159

An alternative hypothesis holds that high hypnotizables are hypnotic procedures. In other words, almost all MPD patients
particularly likely to respond with displays of multiplicity to have been repeatedly administered hypnotic procedures and
the cuing contained in leading interviews. This hypothesis holds have responded repeatedly to these procedures by displaying al-
that hypnotizability is a stable, traitlike index of suggestibility ter personalities. Given this, MPD patients are very likely to
and that high hypnotizables constitute only about 10% of the construe themselves as highly hypnotizable and are likely to be
population. Because high hypnotizables are more suggestible motivated to respond to communications delivered in a hyp-
than moderate or low hypnotizables, they are particularly likely notic context. Consequently, when they are tested for hypnotiz-
to respond to the strong cuing for MPD contained in leading ability, they tend to respond in terms of the motivations and
diagnostic interviews (Ganaway, 1989;Mulhern, 1991b). expectations derived from their earlier hypnotic experiences
This suggestibility trait hypothesis accounts for why MPD and, therefore, attain high hypnotizability scores.
patients typically score as high hypnotizables. Moreover, it can This contextualist hypothesis does not deny that individual
account for different cross-national gender ratios and incidence differences on some cognitive or interactional style dimensions
rates by suggesting that they reflect the expectations of thera- may influence the ease with which people carry out multiple
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

pists who use leading hypnotic procedures. Those who believe identity enactments. For example, some clinical reports de-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

that MPD is relatively common will use such procedures fre- scribe MPD patients as highly imaginative .people with rich fan-
quently, those who believe that it is more common in women tasy lives who have spent much time covertly rehearsing and
than men will use such procedures more frequently with becoming absorbed in a range of "make believe" activities (e.g.,
women than with men, and so on. However, the suggestibility Keyes, 1981). Given appropriate motivations, it is easy to un-
trait hypothesis cannot account parsimoniously for the very derstand that such people might be particularly adept at enact-
high rates of multiplicity seen in some traditional cultures (e.g., ing multiple identities when contextual inducements call for
47% of the women in some villages) or for the very high rates of such enactments.
demonic possession seen in some group possession cases (e.g., Relatedly, some clinical studies describe many MPD patients
the case in which all or almost all of the nuns in a particular as exhibiting symptoms of borderline personality disorder (e.g.,
convent displayed possession simultaneously; Huxley, 1952). Horevitz & Braun, 1984). Borderlines are described as display-
This hypothesis also has difficulty with the findings that glosso- ing sudden mood shifts; rapid, marked changes in attitude; im-
lalics do not exhibit particularly high hypnotizability (Spanos pulsivity; and sudden, inappropriate anger. When provided with
& Hewitt, 1979) and that, in some congregations, glossolalia oc- the idea of MPD, such people may find it particularly easy to
curs in all or almost all members (Goodman, 1972). conceptualize their rapid shifts in mood and behavior as stem-
An alternative to the suggestibility trait hypothesis holds that ming from different selves at war with one another.
a great many people who do not typically attain high hypnotiz- Many MPD patients are diagnosed as psychopaths at some
ability scores possess the cognitive abilities required to enact the point in their careers (Bliss, 1986). In short, they are often adept
as if scenarios contained in hypnotic suggestions and in hyp- at altering their self-presentations to manipulate others. Given
notic interviews that call for multiplicity (Spanos, 1986a). that they see it in their best interests, such people may be par-
Along these lines, a number of recent studies have demonstrated ticularly adept at enacting multiplicity.
that short training procedures aimed at altering subjects' inter- In summary, there may be numerous cognitive propensities
pretations and attitudes produce large gains in hypnotizability and interactional styles that influence the ease with which indi-
(e.g., Gorassini, Sowerby, Creighton, & Fry, 1991; Spanos, Ga- viduals carry out multiplicity enactments. On the other hand,
bora, Jarrett, & Gwynn, 1989; Spanos, Lush, & Gwynn, 1989). motivational and contextual demand variables will undoubt-
Thus, a much greater proportion of people than indicated by edly interact with and may at times even override the effects of
conventional hypnotizability testing possess the cognitive abili- such individual difference variables. In addition, it is important
ties required to experience the as if scenarios traditionally asso- to keep in mind that individual difference variables that might
ciated with hypnotic suggestions. facilitate multiple identity enactments in one cultural context
The leading interviews used to diagnose MPD are frequently may be unimportant or even a hindrance to such enactments in
conducted in a hypnotic context, and the psychotherapeutic other cultures. For example, borderline characteristics might
procedures used with these patients almost always make use of facilitate a self-definition of multiplicity when multiplicity is de-
fined as a pathology and when erratic and unpredictable behav-
iors are congruent with the multiple identity role. However, the
between physical punishment and dissociation of .21 and .18, respec- same characteristics might well hinder displays of multiplicity
tively; DiTomasso & Routh, 1993), and Nash et al. (1993) found that in a shaman who is required to display restraint and good judge-
the relationship between these variables fell to nonsignificance after ment and whose multiple identity enactments entail regular
controlling statistically for dysfunctional family environment. In two conformance with ritual requirements.
recent studies, Council (1993a, 1993b) found that the magnitude of the In summary, cross-cultural differences in the rates with
relationship between reported abuse history and dissociation scores was
dependent on whether the dissociation scale was completed before or which multiplicity is displayed are very large, and in some cul-
after subjects reported their abuse history. Significant correlations be- tures substantial proportions of the population display multi-
tween these variables emerged only when subjects completed the disso- plicity at some point in their lives. These considerations suggest
ciation scale after reporting their abuse history. These findings raise the that the cognitive abilities required to experience and enact
possibility that the relationship between abuse history and dissociation multiplicity are fairly common and that the frequency of such
is an expectancy-mediated artifact. enactments is limited as much, and perhaps more, by
160 NICHOLAS P. SPANOS

contextual considerations (e.g., the practices of different thera- ports of child abuse have gone from occasional accompani-
pists and the opportunity to observe more practiced demoniacs) ments of early cases to the ritual satanic abuse of today.
as by stable individual differences in cognitive abilities. Changes of these kinds are difficult to deal with from a per-
spective that explains identity enactments as symptoms caused
Conclusions by past traumas rather than as expectancy-guided displays that
change with new information concerning role demands. As in-
Multiple identity enactments occur in most human societies. dicated earlier, role changes of this kind are commonly seen in
Nevertheless, the frequency of these enactments, their behav- historical and cross-cultural displays of possession. Songhay
ioral components, the conditions under which they occur, and mediums, for example, changed the characteristics of their pos-
the characteristics of those who display them differ dramatically session displays in response to changes among the ruling elites
between cultures and sometimes within cultures as well. When (Stoller, 1989), and the characteristics of demonic possession
examined across cultures and historical eras, the rule-governed changed to meet the different requirements of particular reli-
nature of multiple identity enactments and their embeddedness gious communities (Spanos, 1983a; Walker, 1981). Relatedly,
within a legitimizing social matrix become clear.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

the recent association of MPD with reports of ritual satanic


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Each culture develops its own indigenous theory of multiple abuse is much more likely to reflect therapy-induced fabrica-
identity enactments. These local theories reflect local social tions generated by the infusion of evangelical Christian ideology
structures and institutions, and they translate into culturally into the MPD movement than the existence of a 50-year-old
specific expectations that guide both the performance of secret conspiracy that has been responsible for the murder of
multiple identity enactments and the reactions of audiences to thousands without leaving a trace of evidence.
these enactments. Thus, the theory that the Holy Ghost speaks Like other local theories, the disease theory fails to provide a
through people and, in so doing, endorses them spiritually pro- general account of multiplicity that takes into consideration its
vides a local explanation for glossolalia and helps to establish cross-cultural and transhistorical manifestations. I suggest that
motivations and expectations for its perpetuation. However, lo- the local theory of MPD be abandoned and the phenomenon of
cal theories of multiplicity are designed to explain only local multiplicity viewed from a sociocognitive and historical per-
displays, and their deficiencies as general theories of multiplicity spective.
become obvious when these enactments are compared across
historical and cultural contexts. References
The disease theory of MPD is a local theory. Because the pro-
ponents of this theory are invariably mental health profession- Ackerman, S. E., & Lee, R. L. M. (1981). Communication and cognitive
als, the displays of multiplicity that they observe are usually lim- pluralism in a spirit possession event in Malaysia. American Ethnol-
ited to those distressed people who go to them for help. This ogist, 8, 789-799.
limitation profoundly influences the manner in which these in- Adityhanjee, R., & Khandelwal, S. (1989). Current status of multiple
personality disorder in India. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146,
vestigators conceptualize multiplicity. The disease theory of 1607-1610.
MPD is based on the idea that unhappiness or behavioral devi- Aldridge-Morris, R. (1989). Multiple personality: An exercise in decep-
ance in adulthood stems from particular traumatic events oc- tion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
curring in childhood. The particular childhood traumas on Allison, R. B. (1978). On discovering multiplicity. Swedish Journal of
which modern MPD proponents focus are physical abuse and, Hypnosis, 2, 4-8.
especially, sexual abuse. Because of its emphasis on childhood Allison, R. B., & Schwarz, T. (1980). Minds in many pieces. New York:
antecedents and on the notion that "symptoms" reflect uncon- Rawson Wade.
scious defenses, this approach tends to greatly de-emphasize the Allison, R. M. (1991). Travel log: In search of multiples in Moscow.
social and strategic nature of multiple identity enactments and American Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 12, 51-56.
Altrocchi, J. (1992). "We don't have that problem here": MPD in New
the roles played by the institutionalized contexts that encour-
Zealand. Dissociation, 5, 109-110.
age, shape, and legitimize these enactments. In particular, this Baker, R. A. (1992). Hidden memories. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus.
emphasis deflects attention away from the role clinicians them- Barber, T. X. (1969). Hypnosis: A scientific approach. New York: Van
selves play in cuing and legitimizing manifestations of multi- Nostrand Reinhold.
plicity. It also deflects attention from the marked changes in Barber, T. X. (1979). Suggested ("hypnotic") behavior: The trance par-
symptomatology that have occurred in MPD over the years, adigm versus an alternative paradigm. In E. Fromm & R. E. Shor
changes that illustrate the role of social factors in shaping MPD (Eds.), Hypnosis: Developments in research and new perspectives (pp.
displays. Since the 19th century, for example, the number of 217-271). Chicago: Aldine.
personalities per patient has jumped from 2 or 3 to often more Beitchman, J. H., Zucker, K. J., Hood, J. E., DaCosta, G. A., & Akman,
than 20 and sometimes into the hundreds. Early cases were D. (1991). A review of short-term effects of child sexual abuse. Child
marked by transitional periods of sleep and convulsions, which Abuse and Neglect, 15, 537-556.
Beitchman, J. H., Zucker, K.. J., Hood, J. E., DaCosta, G. A., Akman,
are uncommon today. The alters of early patients were human. D., & Cassavia, E. (1992). A review of long-term effects of child sexual
Recently, however, scholarly articles have been devoted to ani- abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 16, 101-118.
mal alters (e.g., Hendrickson, McCarty, & Goodwin, 1990), and Bernstein, E. M., & Putnam, F. W. (1986). Development, reliability,
the alters "discovered" by some clinicians now exhibit the char- and validity of a dissociation scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental
acteristics of semi-spiritual self helpers (R. B. Allison & Diseases, 174, 727-734.
Schwarz, 1980) or reincarnated past lives (Krippner, 1986). Re- Bertrand, L. D. (1989). The assessment and modification of hypnotic
MULTIPLE IDENTITY ENACTMENTS AND MPD 161

susceptibility. In N. P. Spanos & J. F. Chaves (Eds.), Hypnosis: The N. P. Spanos & J. F. Chaves (Eds.), Hypnosis: The cognitive-behav-
cognitive-behavioral perspective (pp. 18-31). Buffalo, NY: Prometh- ioral perspective (pp. 418-436). Buffalo, NY: Prometheus.
eus. Coe, W. C., & Sluis, A. S. E. (1989). Increasing contextual pressures
Biscop, P. (1981). By spirit possessed. Unpublished master's thesis, De- to breach posthypnotic amnesia. Journal of Personality and Social
partment of Anthropology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, British Psychology, 57, 885-894.
Columbia, Canada. Cohn, N. (1975). Europe's inner demons. New York: Basic Books.
Biscop, P. (1985). There is no death: Belief and the social construction Constantinides, P. (1977). Ill at ease and sick at heart: Symbolic behav-
of reality in a Canadian spiritualist church. Unpublished doctoral ior in a Sudanese healing cult. In I. Wilson (Ed.), Symbols and senti-
dissertation, Department of Anthropology, Simon Fraser University, ments (pp. 61-84). NY: Academic Press.
Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada. Coons, P. M., Bowman, E. S., & Milstein, V. (1988). Multiple personal-
Bliss, E. L. (1980). Multiple personalities: A report of 14 cases with ity disorder: A clinical investigation of 50 cases. Journal of Nervous
implications for schizophrenia and hysteria. Archives of General Psy- and Mental Disease, 176, 519-527.
chiatry, 37, 1388-1397. Coons, P. M., & Milstein, V. (1986). Psychosexual disturbances in
Bliss, E. L. (1986). Multiple personality, allied disorders and hypnosis. multiple personality: Characteristics, etiology, and treatment. Jour-
New York: Oxford University Press. nal of Clinical Psychiatry, 47, 106-110.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Bliss, E. L., & Jeppsen, E. A. (1985). Prevalence of multiple personality Council, J. R. (1993a, June). The annoying itch of context effects. Paper
among inpatients and outpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry, presented at the Nags Head Conference on Personality and Social
142,250-251. Behavior, Highland Beach, FL.
Bliss, E. L., & Larson, E. M. (1985). Sexual criminality and hypnotiz- Council, J. R. (1993b). Context effects in personality research. Current
ability. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 173, 522-526. Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 31-34.
Bliss, E. L., Larson, E. M., & Nakashima, S. R. (1983). Auditory hallu- Deaux, K. (1993). Reconstructing social identity. Personality and Social
cinations and schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, Psychology Bulletin, 19, 4-12.
777,30-33. deGroh, M. (1989). Correlates of hypnotic susceptibility. In N. P.
Boddy, J. (1988). Spirits and selves in northern Sudan: The cultural Spanos & J. F. Chaves (Eds.), Hypnosis: The cognitive-behavioral per-
theraputics of possession and trance. American Ethnologist, 15, 4- spective (pp. 32-63). Buffalo, NY: Prometheus.
27. Diamond, M. J. (1974). Modification of hypnotizability: A review. Psy-
Bottoms, B. L., Shaver, P. R., & Goodman, G. S. (1991, August). Profile chological Bulletin, 81, 180-198.
of ritual and religion related abuse allegations reported to clinical DiTomasso, M. J., & Routh, D. K. (1993). Recall of abuse in childhood
psychologists in the United States. Paper presented at the 99th Annual and three measures of dissociation. Child Abuse and Neglect, 17,
Convention of the American Psychological Association, San Fran- 477-485.
cisco, CA. Docter, R. F. (1988). Transvestites and transexuals: Toward a theory of
Bourguignon, E. (1976). Possession. San Francisco: Chandler & Sharp. cross-gender behavior. New \fork: Plenum.
Bowman, E. S. (1990). Adolescent multiple personality disorder in the Epstein, S. (1973). The self concept revisited: Or a theory of a theory.
nineteenth and early twentieth centuries. Dissociation, 3, 179-187. American Psychologist, 28, 404-416.
Brandsma. J. M., & Ludwig, A. M. (1974). A case of multiple personal- Fahy, T. A. (1988). The diagnosis of multiple personality disorder: A
ity: Diagnosis and treatment. International Journal of Clinical and critical review. British Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 597-606.
Experimental Hypnosis, 22, 216-233. Fahy, T. A., Abas, M., & Brown, J. C. (1989). Multiple personality. A
Braun, B. G. (1984). Hypnosis creates multiple personality: Myth or symptom of psychiatric disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 154,
reality? International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypno- 99-101.
sis, 32, 191-197. Fellows, B. (1986). The concept of trance. In P. L. N. Naish (Ed.), What
Braun, B. G. (1990). Multiple personality disorder: An overview. Amer- is hypnosis?(pp. 37-58). Philadelphia: Open University Press.
ican Journal of Occupational Therapy, 44, 971-976. Femina, D. D., Yeager, C. A., & Lewis, D. O. (1990). Child abuse: Ado-
Briere, J., & Zaidi, L. Y. (1989). Sexual abuse histories and sequelae in lescent records vs. adult recall. Child Abuse and Neglect, 14, 227-
female psychiatric emergency room patients. American Journal of 231.
Psychiatry, 146, 1602-1607. Finkelhor, D. (1979). Sexually victimized children. New York: Free
Bromley, D. G. (1991). Satanism: The new cult scare. In J. T. Richard- Press.
son, J. Best, & D. G. Bromley (Eds.), Thesatanism scare (pp. 49-72). Firth, R. (1967). Ritual and drama in Malay spirit mediumship. Com-
Chicago: Aldine. parative Studies in Society and History, 9, 190-207.
Brown, P. (in press). Toward a psychobiological study of dissociation. Franklin, J. (1988). Diagnosis of covert and subtle forms of multiple
In S. J. Lynn (Ed.), Dissociation: Clinical, theoretical and research personality disorder. Dissociation, 2, 27-33.
perspectives. Washington, DC: Guilford Press. Fraser, G. A. (1990). Satanic ritual abuse: A cause of multiple personal-
Burgess, C. A., Spanos, N. P., Ritt, J., Hordy, T, & Brooks, S. (1993). ity disorder. Journal of Child and Youth Care, Special Issue, 55-66.
Compliant responding among high hypnotizables: An experimental Friedman, W. (1993). Memory for the time of past events. Psychological
demonstration. Unpublished manuscript, Carleton University, Ot- Bulletin, 7/5,44-66.
tawa, Ontario, Canada. Galanter, M. (1990). Cults and zealous self-help movements: A psychi-
Carstairs, G. M., & Kapur, R. L. (1976). The great universe ofKota. atric perspective. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 543-551.
London: Hogarth Press. Ganaway, G. K. (1989). Historical versus narrative truth: Clarifying the
Chordoff, P. (1987). More on multiple personality disorder [letter to the role of exogenous trauma in the etiology of MPD and its variants.
editor]. American Journal of Psychiatry 144, 124. Dissociation, 2, 205-220.
Chu, J. A., & Dill, D. L. (1990). Dissociative symptoms in relation to Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New \brk:
childhood sexual abuse. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 887- Doubleday.
892. Goodman, F. D. (1972). Speaking in tongues: A cross-cultural study of
Coe, W. C. (1989). Posthypnotic amnesia: Theory and research. In glossolalia. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
162 NICHOLAS P. SPANOS

Gorassini, D. R., Sowerby, D., Creighton, A., & Fry, G. (1991). Hyp- In R. P. Kluft (Ed.), Childhood antecedents of multiple personality
notic suggestibility enhancement through brief cognitive skill train- (pp. 197-238). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
ing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 289-297. Kluft, R. P. (1993). Multiple personality disorders. In D. Spiegel (Ed.),
Gruenwald, D. (1971). Hypnotic techniques without hypnosis in the Dissociative disorders: A clinical review (pp. 17-44). Lutherville,
treatment of a dual personality. Journal of Nervous and Mental Dis- MD: Sidran.
ease, 153, 41-46. Kohlenberg, R. J. (1973). Behavioristic approach to multiple personal-
Hacking, 1. (1986). The invention of split personalities. In A. Donagan, ity: A case study. Behavior Therapy, 4, 137-140.
A. N. Perovich, Jr., & M. V. Wedin (Eds.), Human nature and natural Krippner, S. (1986). Cross-cultural approaches to multiple personality
knowledge (pp. 63-85). New York: D. Reidel. disorder: Therapeutic practices in Brazilian spiritism. Humanistic
Hacking, I. (1991). Double consciousness in Britain 1815-1875. Disso- Psychologist, 14, 177-193.
ciation, 4, 134-146. Krippner, S. (1989). A call to heal: Entry patterns in Brazilian medi-
Hacking, I. (1992). Multiple personality and its hosts. History of the umship. In C. W. Ward (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness and
Human Sciences, 5, 3-31. mental health: Theoretical and methodological issues (pp. 186-206).
Halleck, S. L. (1990). Dissociative phenomena and the question of re- Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

sponsibility. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hyp- Krippner, S. (in press). Cross-cultural treatment perspectives of dissoc-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

nosis, 38, 2W-1\4. iative disorders. In S. J. Lynn (Ed.), Dissociation: Clinical, theoretical
Harper, E. B. (1963). Spirit possession and social structure. In B. Rat- and research perspectives. NY: Guilford.
man (Ed.), Anthropology on the march, (pp. 56-62) London: Oxford La Fontaine, J. (1990). Child sexual abuse. Cambridge, England: Polity
University Press. Press.
Harsnett, S. (1599). Discovery of thefraudulent practices of John Darrel. Lambek, M. (1980). Spirits and spouses: Possession as a system of com-
London: John Wolfe. munication among the Malagasy speakers of Mayotte. American Eth-
Harsnett, S. (1603). A declaration of egregious popish impostures. Lon- nologist, 7,318-331.
don: I. Roberts. Lambek, M. (1988). Spirit possession/spirit succession: Aspects of so-
Hendrickson, K. M., McCarty, T., & Goodwin, J. M. (1990). Animal cial continuity among Malagasy speakers in Mayotte. American Eth-
alters: Case reports. Dissociation, 4, 218-221. nologist, 15, 710-731.
Hicks, R. D. (1991). In pursuit of Satan. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus. Lambek, M. (1989). From disease to discourse: Remarks on the con-
Hilgard, E. R. (1979). Divided consciousness in hypnosis: The implica- ceptualization of trance and spirit possession. In C. A. Ward (Ed.),
tions of the hidden observer. In E. Fromm & R. E. Shor (Eds.), Hyp- Altered states of consciousness and mental health: Theoretical and
nosis: Developments in research and new perspectives (pp. 45-79). methodological issues (pp. 36-62). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Chicago: Aldine. Lanning, K. V. (1989). Satanic, occult and ritualistic crime: A law en-
Hilgard, E. R. (1991). A neodissociation interpretation of hypnosis. In forcement perspective. Police Chief, 56, 62-85.
S. J. Lynn & J. Rhue (Eds.), Theories of hypnosis: Current models Lamer, C. (1981). Enemies of God: The witch-hunt in Scotland. Balti-
and perspectives (pp. 83-104). New York: Guilford Press. more: Johns Hopkins University Press.
Hilgard, J. R. (1970). Personality and hypnosis: A study of imaginative Leacock, S., & Leacock, R. (1972). Spirits of the deep: A study of an
involvement. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Afro-Brazilian cult. New York: Anchor Books.
Hine, V. H. (1969). Pentecostal glossolalia: Toward a functional reinter- Lee, R. L. M. (1989). Self-presentation in Malaysian spirit seances: A
pretation. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 8, 212-226. dramaturgical perspective on altered states of consciousness in heal-
Hine, V. H. (1970). Bridge burners: Commitment and participation in ing ceremonies. In C. W. Ward (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness
a religious movement. Sociological Analysis, 31,61 -66. and mental health: Theoretical and methodological perspectives (pp.
Horevitz, R. P., & Braun, B. G. (1984). Are multiple personalities bor- 251-266). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
derlines? An analysis of 33 cases. Psychiatric Clinics of North Amer- Lewis, I. M. (1987). Ecstatic religion. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
ica, 7, 69-87. Loftus, E. F. (1979). Eyewitness testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
Howe, M. L., & Courage, M. L. (1993). On resolving the enigma of University Press.
infantile amnesia. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 305-326. Loftus, E. F. (1993a). Desperately seeking memories of the first few
Huxley, A. (1952). The devils ofLoudon. New York: Harper & Row. years of childhood: The reality of early memories. Journal of Experi-
Kampman, R. (1976). Hypnotically induced multiple personality: An mental Psychology: General, 122, 274-277.
experimen tal study. International Journal of Clinical and Experimen- Loftus, E. F. (1993b). The reality of repressed memories. American Psy-
tal Hypnosis, 24, 215-227. chologist, 48, 518-537.
Keene, M. L. (1976). The psychic mafia. New York: Dell. Loftus, E. F, Polonsky, S., & Fullilove, M. T. (in press). Memories of
Kelly, H. A. (1974). The devil demonology and witchcraft. New \brk: childhood sexual abuse: Remembering and repressing. Psychology of
Doubleday. Women Quarterly.
Kenny, M. G. (1986). The passion of Ansel Bourne: Multiple personality Lyons, A. (1988). Satan wants you: The cult of devil worship in America.
and American culture. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution New York: Mysterious Press.
Press. Maloney, H. N., & Lovekin, A. A. (1985). Glossolalia: Behavioral sci-
Keyes, D. (1981). The minds of Billy Milligan. New York: Bantam ence perspectives on speaking in tongues. New \brk: Oxford Univer-
Books. sity Press.
Kirshner, L. A. (1973). Dissociative reactions: An historical review and Mancuso, J. C., & Sarbin, T. R. (1983). The self narrative in the enact-
clinical study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 49, 698-711. ment of roles. In T. R. Sarbin & K. E. Schiebe (Eds.), Studies in social
Kluft, R. P. (1982). Varieties of hypnotic interventions in the treatment identity (pp. 233-253). New York: Praeger.
of multiple personality. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 24, Merskey, H. (1992). The manufacture of personalities: The production
230-240. of multiple personality disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 160,
Kluft, R. P. (1985). The natural history of multiple personality disorder. 327-340.
MULTIPLE IDENTITY ENACTMENTS AND MPD 163

Merskey, H., & Buhrich, N. A. (1975). Hysteria and organic brain dis- gapore: Some patterns and modes of presentation. In M. J. Collinger,
ease. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 48, 359-366. J. W. Pennebaker, & L. R. Murray (Eds.), Masspsychogenic illness: A
Michaelis, S. (1613). The admirable history of the possession and con- social psychological analysis (pp. 21-52). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
version of a penitent woman. London. Pope, H. G., & Hudson, J. I. (1992). Is childhood sexual abuse a risk
Miller, S. D., & Triggiano, P. J. (1992). The psychophysiological investi- factor for bulimia nervosa? American Journal of Psychiatry, 149,
gation of multiple personality disorder: Review and update. American 455-463.
Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 35,47-61. Prince, M. (1908). The dissociation of a personality (2nd ed.). London:
Modestin, J. (1992). Multiple personality disorder in Switzerland. Longmans Green.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 88-92. Putnam, F. W. (1989). Diagnosis and treatment of multiple personality
Moore, M. S. (1984). Law and psychiatry: Rethinking the relationship. disorder. New York: Guilford Press.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. Putnam, F. W. (1993). Dissociative phenomena. In D. Spiegel (Ed.),
Moore, R. L. (1977). In search of white crows. New York: Oxford Uni- Dissociative disorders: A clinical review (pp. 1-16). Lutherville, MD:
versity Press. Sidran.
Morton, A. (1977). Dawit: Competition and integration in an Ethiopian Putnam, F. W., Guroff, J. J., Silberman, E. K., Barban, L., & Post,
R. M. (1986). The clinical phenomenology of multiple personality
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Wuqabi cult group. In V. Crapanzano & V. Garrison (Eds.), Case


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

studies in spirit possession (pp. 193-234). New York: Wiley. disorder: A review of 100 recent cases. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
Mulhern, S. (199 la). Letter to the editor. Child Abuse and Neglect, ]5, 47, 285-293.
609-611. Rabinowitz, F. E. (1989). Creating the multiple personality: An experi-
Mulhern, S. (1991b). Satanism and psychotherapy: A rumor in search mental demonstration for an undergraduate abnormal psychology
of an inquisition. In J. T. Richardson, J. Best, & D. G. Bromley (Eds.), class. Teaching of Psychology, 16, 69-71.
The satanism scare (pp. 145-172). Chicago: Aldine. Radtke, H. L., & Spanos, N. P. (1981). Was I hypnotized? A social psy-
Mulhern, S. (in press). Le trouble de lapersonnalite multiple a la recher- chological analysis of hypnotic depth reports. Psychiatry, 44, 359-
che du trauma perdu [The trouble with research on multiple person- 367.
ality and childhood trauma]. In M. Gabel, S. Lebovici, & P. Mazet Richardson, J. T. (1973). Psychological interpretations of glossolalia: A
(Eds.), Inceste. Paris: Press Universitaire de France. reexamination of research. Journal for the Scientific Study of Reli-
Nadon, R., Hoyt, I. P., Register, P. A., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1991). Ab- gion, 12, 199-207.
sorption and hypnotizability: Context effects reexamined. Journal of The Roman ritual of exorcism. (1976). In M. Malachi, Hostage to the
Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 144-153. devil. New York: Reader's Digest Press. (Original work published
Nash, M. R. (1987). What, if anything, is regressed about hypnotic age 1614)
regression? A review of the empirical literature. Psychological Bulle- Rosenbaum, M. (1980). The role of the term schizophrenia in the de-
tin, 102, 42-52. cline of the diagnosis of multiple personality. Archives of General Psy-
Nash, M. R., Hulsey, T. L., Sexton, M. C, Harralson, T. L., & Lambert, chiatry, 37, 1383-1385.
W. (1993). Long-term sequelae of childhood sexual abuse: Perceived Ross, C. A. (1989). Multiple personality disorder: Diagnosis, clinical
family environment, psychopathology, and dissociation. Journal of features and treatment. New \brk: Wiley.
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 276-283. Ross, C. A. (1991). Epidemiology of multiple personality disorder and
Nash, M. R., & Lynn, S. J. (1986). Child abuse and hypnotic ability. dissociation. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 14, 503-517.
Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 5, 211-218. Ross, C. A., Anderson, G., Fleisher, W. P., & Norton, G. R. (1991). The
Nash, M. R., Lynn, S. J., & Givens, D. L. (1984). Adult hypnotic sus- frequency of multiple personality disorder among psychiatric inpa-
ceptibility, childhood punishment, and child abuse: A brief commu- tients. American Journal of 'Psychiatry, 148, 1717-1720.
nication. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypno- Ross, C. A., Miller, S. D., Bjornson, L., Reagor, P., Fraser, G. A., &
sis, 32, 6-11. Anderson, G. (1991). Abuse histories in 102 cases of multiple person-
Notestein, W. (1911). A history of witchcraft in England. New York: ality disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 36, 97-101.
Crowell. Ross, C. A., Norton, C. R., & Fraser, G. A. (1989). Evidence against the
Oesterreich, T. K. (1966). Possession: Demoniacal and other. Secaucus, iatrogenesis of multiple personality disorder. Dissociation, 2, 61 -65.
NJ: Citadel Press. Ross, C. A., Norton, R., & Wozney, K. (1989). Multiple personality
Ofshe, R. J. (1992). Inadvertent hypnosis during interrogation: False disorder: An analysis of 236 cases. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry,
confession due to dissociative state; mis-identified multiple personal- 34, 414-418.
ity and the satanic cult hypothesis. International Journal of Clinical Russell, J. B. (1980). A history of witchcraft. London: Thames & Hud-
and Experimental Hypnosis, 40, 125-156. son.
Ong, A. (1988). The production of possession: Spirits and the multina- Samarin, W. J. (1972). Tongues of men and angels: The religious lan-
tional corporation in Malaysia. American Ethnologist, 15, 28-42. guage ofpentecostalism. New York: Macmillan.
Orne, M. T. (1979). On the simulating subject as a quasi-control group Sansonnet-Hayden, H., Haley, G., Marriage, K.., & Fine, S. (1987). Sex-
in hypnosis research: What, why and how. In E. Fromm & R. E. Shor ual abuse and psychopathology in hospitalized adolescents. Journal
(Eds.), Hypnosis: Developments in research and new perspectives (pp. of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26,
519-566). Chicago: Aldine. 753-757.
Parfitt, D. N., & Gall, C. M. C. (1944). Psychogenic amnesia: The re- Sarbin, T. R. (in press). Dissociation: State, trait or skill? Contemporary
fusal to remember. Journal of Mental Science, 90, 511-531. Hypnosis.
Perry, N. E. (1992). Therapists' experiences of the effects of working Sarbin, T. R., & Coe, W. C. (1972). Hypnosis: A social-psychological
with dissociative patients. Paper presented at the Ninth International analysis of influence communication. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
Conference on Multiple Personality/Dissociation, Arlington, VA. Winston.
Peters, L. G. (1981). An experiential study of Nepalese shamanism. Saunders, L. W. (1977). Variants in Zar experience in an Egyptian vil-
Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 13, 1-26. lage. In V. Crapanzano & V. Garrison (Eds.), Case studies in spirit
Phoon, W. H. (1982). Outbreaks of mass hysteria at workplaces in Sin- possession (pp. 177-191). New York: Wiley.
164 NICHOLAS P. SPANOS

Schieffelin, E. L. (1985). Performance and the cultural construction of visual hallucinations in hypnotic subjects: When no means yes. Brit-
reality. American Ethnologist, 12, 707-724. ish Journal of Experimental and Clinical Hypnosis, 6, 63-67.
Schreiber, F. R. (1973). Sybil. New York: Warner. Spanos, N. P., Flynn, D. M., & Gwynn, M. I. (1988). Contextual de-
Schwarz, T. (1981). The hillside strangles. A murderer's mind. New mands, negative hallucinations and hidden observer responding:
York: New American Library. Three hidden observers observed. British Journal of Experimental
Sharp, L. A. (1990). Possessed and dispossessed youth: Spirit possession and Clinical Hypnosis, 5, 5-10.
of school children in northwest Madagascar. Culture Medicine and Spanos, N. P., Gabora, N. J., Jarrett, L. E., & Gwynn, M. I. (1989).
Psychiatry, 14, 339-364. Contextual determinants of hypnotizability and of the relationship
Shorter, E. (1992). From paralysis tofatigue: A history of psychosomatic between hypnotizability scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
medicine in the modern era. New York: Free Press. chology, 57,27'1-278.
Silva, C. E., & Kirsch, I. (1987). Breaching hypnotic amnesia by ma- Spanos, N. P., & Gottlieb, J. (1976). Ergotism and the Salem village
nipulating expectancy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 96, 325- witch trials. Science, 194, 1390-1394.
329. Spanos, N. P., Gwynn, M. I., Comer, S. L., Baltruweit, W. J., & deGroh,
Smith, M. C. (1983). Hypnotic memory enhancement of witnesses: M. (1989). Are hypnotically induced pseudomemories resistant to
Does it work? Psychological Bulletin, 94, 387-407.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cross-examination? Law and Human Behavior, 13, 271-289.


Smith, M., & Pazder, L. (1980). Michelle remembers. New York: Pocket
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Spanos, N. P., Gwynn, M. I., & Stam, H. J. (1983). Instructional de-


Books. mands and ratings of overt and hidden pain during hypnotic analge-
Spanos, N. P. (1978). Witchcraft in histories of psychiatry: A critical sia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 479-488.
analysis and an alternative conceptualization. Psychological Bulletin, Spanos, N. P., & Hewitt, E. C. (1979). Glossolalia: A test of the "trance"
55,417-439. and psychopathology hypotheses. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
Spanos, N. P. (1983a). Demonic possession: A social psychological anal- 88, 427-434.
ysis. In M. Rosenbaum (Ed.), Compliance behavior (ftp. 149-198). Spanos, N. P., & Hewitt, E. C. (1980). The hidden observer in hypnotic
New York: Free Press. analgesia: Discovery or experimental creation? Journal of Personality
Spanos, N. P. (1983b). Ergotism and the Salem witch panic: A critical and Social Psychology, 39, 1201 -1214.
analysis and an alternative conceptualization. Journal of the History Spanos, N. P., Lush, N. I., & Gwynn, M. I. (1989). Cognitive skill train-
of the Behavioral Sciences, 19, 358-368. ing enhancement of hypnotizability: Generalization effects and
Spanos, N. P. (1986a). Hypnosis and the modification of hypnotic sus- trance logic responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
ceptibility: A social psychological perspective. In P. L. N. Naish (Ed.), 56, 795-804.
What is hypnosis?(pp. 85-120). Philadelphia: Open University Press. Spanos, N. P., & Mclain, J. M. (1986). Hypnotically created pseudo-
Spanos, N. P. (1986b). Hypnotic behavior: A social-psychological inter- memories: Memory distortions or reporting biases? British Journal
pretation of amnesia, analgesia and "trance logic." Behavioral and of Experimental and Clinical Hypnosis, 3, 155-159.
Brain Sciences, 9, 449-502.
Spanos, N. P., Menary, E., Gabora, N. J., DuBreuil, S. C., & Dewhirst,
Spanos, N. P. (1989). Hypnosis, demonic possession and multiple per- B. (1991). Secondary identity enactments during hypnotic past-life
sonality: Strategic enactments and disavowels of responsibility for ac- regression: A sociocognitive perspective. Journal of Personality and
tions. In C. A. Ward (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness and mental
Social Psychology, 61, 308-320.
health: Theoretical and methodological issues (pp. 96-124). New-
Spanos, N. P., Quigley, C. A., Gwynn, M. I., Glatt, R. L., & Perlini,
bury Park, CA: Sage.
A. H. (1991). Hypnotic interrogation, pretrial preparation, and wit-
Spanos, N. P., Arango, M., & de Groot, H. P. (1993). Context as a mod-
ness testimony during direct and cross-examination. Law and Hu-
erator in relationships between attribute variables and hypnotizabil-
man Behavior, 15, 639-653.
ity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 71-77.
Spanos, N. P., Radtke, H. L., & Bertrand, L. D. (1984). Hypnotic am-
Spanos, N. P., & Bodorik, H. L. (1977). Suggested amnesia and disor-
nesia as a strategic enactment: Breaching amnesia in highly hypno-
ganized recall in hypnotic and task-motivated subjects. Journal of Ab-
tizable subjects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47,
normal Psychology, 86, 295-305.
1155-1169.
Spanos, N. P., & Bures, E. (1993). Pseudomemory creation and reversal
in hypnotic, imagination control and simulating subjects. Unpub- Spanos, N. P., Rivers, S. M., & Ross, S. (1977). Experienced involuntar-
lished manuscript, Carleton University, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada. iness and response to hypnotic suggestions. Annals of the New York
Spanos, N. P., Burgess, C. A., & Burgess, M. F. (in press). Past life iden- Academy of Sciences, 296, 208-221.
tities, UFO abductions and satanic ritual abuse: The social construc- Spanos, N. P., Weekes, J. R., & Bertrand, L. D. (1985). Multiple per-
tion of "memories." International Journal of Clinical and Experi- sonality: A social psychological perspective. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
mental Hypnosis. chology, 94, 362-376.
Spanos, N. P., & Chaves, J. F. (1989). Hypnosis: The cognitive-behav- Spanos, N. P., Weekes, J. R., Menary, E., & Bertrand, L. D. (1986).
ioral perspective. Buffalo, NY: Prometheus. Hypnotic interview and age regression procedures in the elicitation
Spanos, N. P., & Coe, W. C. (1992). A social psychological approach to of multiple personality symptoms: A simulation study. Psychiatry, 49,
hypnosis. In E. Fromm & M. R. Nash (Eds.), Contemporary hypnosis 298-311.
research (pp. 102-130). New York: Guilford Press. Stava, L. J., & Jaffa, M. (1988). Some operationalizations of the neodis-
Spanos, N. P., Cross, W. P., Lepage, M., & Coristine, M. (1986). Glos- sociation concept and their relationship to hypnotic susceptibility.
solalia as learned behavior: An experimental demonstration. Journal Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 989-996.
of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 21-23. Stoller, P. (1989). Stressing social change in Songhay possession. In
Spanos, N. P., deGroot, H. P., Tiller, D. K., Weekes, J. R., & Bertrand, C. A. Ward (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness and mental health:
L. D. (1985). "Trance logic" duality and hidden observer responding A cross cultural perspective(pp. 267-284). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
in hypnotic, imagination control and simulating subjects. Journal of Sutcliffe, J. P., & Jones, J. (1962). Personal identity, multiple personality
Abnormal Psychology, 94, 611-623. and hypnosis. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Spanos, N. P., Flynn, D. M., & Gabora, N. J. (1989). Suggested negative Hypnosis, 10,231-269.
MULTIPLE IDENTITY ENACTMENTS AND MPD 165

Takahashi, Y. (1990). Is multiple personality disorder really rare in Ja- A critical review of the literature. American Journal of Forensic Psy-
pan? Dissociation, 3, 57-59. chology, 9, 43-69.
Talamini, J. T. (1982). Boys will be girls: The hidden world of the het- Wakefield, H., & Underwager, R. (1992). Recovered memories of al-
erosexual male transvestite. Lanham, MD: University Press of Amer- leged sexual abuse: Lawsuits against parents. Behavioral Sciences and
ica. the Law, 70,483-507.
Teoh, J., Soewondo, S., & Sidharta, M. (1975). Epidemic hysteria in Walker, D. P. (1981). Unclean spirits. Philadelphia: University of Penn-
Malaysian schools: An illustrative example. Psychiatry, 38, 258-268. sylvania Press.
Terr, L. (1988). What happens to early memories of trauma? A study of Wambaugh, H. (1979). Life before life. New York: Bantam.
twenty children under age five at the time of documented traumatic Ward, C. A. (1989). Possession and exorcism: Psychopathology and psy-
events. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent chotherapy in a magico-religious context. In C. A. Ward (Ed.), Al-
Psychiatry, 27, 96-104. tered states of consciousness and mental health: Theoretical and
Terr, L. (1990). Too scared to cry. New York: Harper & Row. methodological issues (pp. 125-144). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Thigpen, C. H., & Cleckley, H. M. (1957). The three faces of Eve. New Watkins, J. G. (1984). The Bianchi (L.A. hillside strangler) case: Socio-
path or multiple personality. International Journal of Clinical and
York: Fawcett.
Experimental Hypnosis, 32, 67-101.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Thigpen, C. H., & Cleckley, H. M. (1984). On the incidence of multiple


Wells, G. L., Ferguson, T. J., & Lindsay, R. C. L. (1981). The tractability
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

personality disorder. International Journal of Clinical and Experi-


of eyewitness confidence and its implications for triers of fact. Journal
mental Hypnosis, 32, 63-66.
of Applied Psychology, 66, 688-696.
Thomas, K. (1971). Religion and the decline of magic. New York: Whitley, G. (1992, January). Abuse of trust. D Magazine, pp. 36-39.
Scribner. Wijesinghe, C. P., Dissanayake, S. A. W, & Mendis, N. (1976). Posses-
Venkataramaiah, V., Mallikarjunaiah, M., Chandra, C. R., Rao, C. K., sion trance in a semi-urban community in Sri Lanka. Australian and
& Reddy, G. N. (1981). Possession syndrome: An epidemiological New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 11, 93-100.
study in West Karnataka. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 23, 213-218. Wilbur, C. B. (1984). Treatment of multiple personality. Psychiatric An-
Victor, J. S. (1993). Satanic panic: The creation of a contemporary leg- nals, 14,21-31.
end. Peru, IL: Open Court. Young, W. C., Sachs, R. G., Braun, B. G., & Watkins, R. T. (1991).
Wagstaff, G. F. (1981). Hypnosis, compliance and belief. New York: St. Patients reporting ritual abuse in childhood: A clinical syndrome.
Martin's Press. Report of 37 cases. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15, 181-189.
Wagstaff, G. F. (1989). Forensic aspects of hypnosis. In N. P. Spanos &
J. F. Chaves (Eds.), Hypnosis: The cognitive-behavioral perspective Received June 28,1993
(pp. 340-357). Buffalo, NY: Prometheus. Revision received December 8,1993
Wakefield, H., & Underwager, R. (1991). Female child sexual abusers: Accepted December 9,1993 •

Low Publication Prices for APA Members and Affiliates


Keeping You Up-to-Date: All APA members (Fellows; Members; Associates, and Student
Affiliates) receive-as part of their annual dues-subscriptions to the American Psychologist and
APA Monitor.
High School Teacher and International Affiliates receive subscriptions to the APA Monitor,
and they can subscribe to the American Psychologist at a significantly reduced rate.
In addition, all members and affiliates are eligible for savings of up to 60% (plus a journal
credit) on all other APA journals, as well as significant discounts on subscriptions from coop-
erating societies and publishers (e.g., the American Association for Counseling and Develop-
ment, Academic Press, and Human Sciences Press).
Essential Resources: APA members and affiliates receive special rates for purchases of APA
books, including the Publication Manual of the APA, the Master Lectures, and Journals in Psychol-
ogy: A Resource Listing for Authors.
Other Benefits of Membership: Membership in APA also provides eligibility for low-cost
insurance plans covering life, income protection, office overhead, accident protection, health
care, hospital indemnity, professional liability, research/academic professional liability, stu-
dent/school liability, and student health.
For more information, write to American Psychological Association,
Membership Services, 750 First Street, NE, Washington, DC 20002-4242, USA

You might also like