You are on page 1of 9

J Light & Vis. Env. Vol.33, N0.

1, 2009 37

Research Note
Restorative Lighting Environments
-Does the Focus of Light Have an Effect on Restorative
Ex periences?
Hen Johanna NIKUNEN* and Kalevl Mlkael KORPE L

tLighting Labom:tory, Department ofElectronjis, HelsiokiUhjve ty ofTedlnohy


Departznent ofPtydio , Uhiversity ofTampere

ReceivedApril 1, 2008, Accepted January 27, 2009

A B STRACT
This study investigated how focusing light on difflerent elements of a scene affects the stress-alleviating,
restorative experience. Three night time scenes were created with the Lightscape computer program.
Each scene was illuminated in two different ways, one focusing light on roads and parking lots and the
other on vegetation, resulting altogether in six views. It was expected that focusing light on vegetation
would result in higher ratings of restorative experience. The simulated views were rated by 35 participants
using the Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) and the Restoration Outcome Scale (ROS). In accor-
dance with our hypothesls, focusing light towards greenery vs. parking lots and roads resulted in higher
PRS and ROS ratings. The results suggest that lighting offers considerable potential for enhancing the
restorativeness of urban night time environments.

KEYWORDS: restoraton, Iighting, healthy environments, urloan nightscape

l. Introduction beyond ergonomics and visual performance. Besides its


In this study, we examined the role of lighting in re- physiological effects, lighting is a health and well-being
storative experience. Restoration is a counterpoise to issue that takes effect through person-environment
stress and attentional fatiguel)-4). The attention restora- transactions; people shape and are shaped by their en-
tion theory (ART) sees restoration from attentional or viromnents4).
mental fatigue unfolding when a person encounters It has been suggested that restorative changes are
environments where experiences of fascination, being stronger with stressed individuals who have a greater
away, coherence and compatibillty become possible. In need for restoration2)13)14). However, also non-stressed
the psycho-physiological theory, Ulrich3) (see also Refer- people experience positive changes. In addition to
ences (5)-(7)) has proposed that an immediate and un- physiological, emotional and cognitive changes restora-
conscious response to nature scenes and contents is tion is also an experience in which being away, fascina-
enough to start the restoration process from stress. tion, coherence and compatibility are considered to be
Outcomes of that process include positive psychophysi- central according to ARTl).
ological changes like lowered heart rate and blood pres- Being away refers to escape and drstraction from eve-
sure, better mood, and better cognitive capabilities. En- ryday routines. It can mean physical escape from the
vironments which enable and promote these positive stressful environment, putting aside ordinary work or
changes can be called restorative and are according to mental escape. In general it means withdrawal from
both theories natural rather than built environments. the worrles and demands.
A well-articulated body of findings indicates that vis- Fascination represents things like interest and stimu-
ual exposure to enviromnents with restorative charac- lation. Fascination is caused by a stimulus that draws
teristics enhances the renewal of psychic resour sl)2)8)-14). involuntary attention. It attracts people and alloWS
It is therefore crucial to discover whether differen- them to function without directed attention and is
tially illuminated environments support restoration therefore important in restorative experience. There
differentially. This would also help in the creation of haven't really been studies on what kixids of contents
restorative environments. Examining the possibihties have this restcration property, but the possibilities
lighting may offer in producing positive emotional and might include for example sunsets, waterfalls and firel).
attentional states extends the significance of lighting Nature seems to be a good source of fascination.

37 The llluminating Engineering Institute of Japan


38 J. Light & Vis. Env. Vol.33, No. 1, 2009

Coherence refers tc an environment that is under- be regarded as a tool to enhance or weaken the restora-
standable and has extent. The environment gives a feel- tiveness of urban nightscape.
ing of being in a "whole other world". It is possible to In the study reported here, we wanted to find out how
enter this world and be surrounded by it. The extent the focus of lighting affects the restorative experience.
can be either directly perceived or imagined feeling of According to our hypothesis, focusing light towards
the world continuing beyond what is perceived. greenery vs. parking lots and roads has an effect on ex-
Compatibihty refers to components like belongingness perienced restoration. The hypothesis was tcsted ex-
to the environment and being able to do things that one perimentally by computer simulations. The computer-
likes. Many people seem to experience nature as being modelled scenes were illuminated in two different ways
high in compatibility. It communicates a sense of reality creating six views. These views were rated by subjects
and offers various meaningful patterns to act. Compati- using both the Perceived Restorativeness Scale
bility can lead to a sense counectedness. This sense can (PRS)15)23)24) and, as a converging measure, the Restora-
have religious and meditative featuresD9). tion Outcome Scale (ROS), which further testing was
What is important is that a restorative experience is also one of the aims of this studyi3)25)-27). Thus, we used
possible when all these four interrelated aspects char- selfreport scales to measure the restorative potential
acterize the experience of a scene or setting. Perceived (PRS), and the actual restorative outcomes (ROS). It
restcrativeness of a scene can thus be representcd with was expected that the views where the lighting was
a measure that provides a summary score of these as- focused on the greenery would receive higher ratings on
pects of experiencel5). both PRS and ROS scales and the scenes where the
The research on restorative environments has focused lighting was focused on roads and parkurg lots would
on dayiime environments. It is important to augment have lower ratings on both scales. Because restoration
the research field with urban nightscapes to get a wider may, although not necessarily, be counected to antece-
understanding of the restorative environments. This dent stress experience according to ART, also preceding
would also be in accord with the increasing use of urban condition of stress or mental fatigue was measured by
night time environments. During the dark hours, peo- two questions.
ple may also feel more vulnerable and stressed, which
emphasizes the importance of restorative environ- 2. Method
mentsl6)17). While trees, greenery and deflected vistas'l 2.1 Stimuh
have restorative characteristics in dayiime environ- Three scenes were modelled with the Lightscape
ments, during night time they provide concealment, computer program. The general structures of the scenes
limited prospect and blocked escape, which may in- were based on real-life environments. One of the scenes
crease fearl8)-21). is from the Pitkaniemi Psychiatric Hospital area (pic-
Lighting working together with the surfaces, shapes tures 3 and 5; Figure 1). The main contents of the scene
and spaces of urban structure creates the visual image are trees, grassland and parking lots with cars. The
of urban nightscape. It may therefore have considerable other two scenes are from the Tapiola district in Espoo,
effect on the experienced restcration. Lighting can focus which is a more urban area. The first scene (pictures 1
our attention to the positive or negative restorative fea- and 4; Figure 1) contains a pedestrian area, vegetation
tures of the space and, in this way; it may enhance or and different kinds of man-made constructions. The
weaken the restorative effect. Very often, the perceived other scene (pictures 2 and 6; Figure 1) contains a large
nightscape image consists of roads, parking lots and parking lot with cars in front of a mall and trees sur-
signs, which are usually the most disliked elements of rounding the parking lot. None of the settings contains
daytime imagesl7). Besides likeability, this may also people. The study was connected to these real environ-
have an effect on the restorativeness of urban night ments because we wanted tc examine typical lighting
time environment. The fact that preference and restora- situations in common environmental settings. It was
tion are correlated to each other has both theoretical also expected that these areas would directly benefit
and empirical support in the restoration research from this research as they had some environmental im-
fieldl)3)22). This conuection leads to the suggestion that provement projects going on. Totally hypothetical situa-
focusing light on the disliked enviromnental features tions would, however, have given more freedom in set-
may have a considerable effect on restoration. On the ting configurations.
other hand, it also reveals the possibility of enhancing Computer modelled images were used because they
the restorative experience by focusing light on preferred offer the possibility of illuminating one scene in two
elements like prominent trees. Lighting may therefore different ways, which would be hard to achieve in real
*1 Deflected vista means a view that is partly covered by e.g. foliage or curviug
environments. It also enabled the subjects to assess all
pathway. the scenes at the same time in controlled conditions.

The lllum in ating Engineering Institute of Jap an 38


40 J. Light & Vis. Env. Vol.33, No. 1, 2009

Tab]e I Setting categories


Setting category scene pair I scene pair 2 scene pair 3

Focus on vegetation:
higher experienced restoration picture 5 picture 4 plcture 2

Focus on roads & parking lots:


lower experienced restoration picture 3 picture I picture 6

the ROS. AIL the subjects evaluated all the scenes. Pro- an effect on the perceived brightness. The black frames
jection was from the front of the screen. The counter- create simultaneous contrast with the images. When
balancing was done so that 15 subjects saw the slides in the frontal area of the view is lighter, the dark frames
original order and 20 subjects saw them in reversed enhance the brightness of the scene. When the frontal
order. The slide order was generated so that the picture area is darker, the frames enhance the darkness.
pairs would not follow each other. Although roughly same amourrt of light was used to
illuminate the scene pairs, it was also checked that
2.1.1 Lighting their perceived brightnesses and visible ilhuninated
Metal halide spotlights (BEGA 8393, wide, 70 W, areas were close to each other. Besides the brightness
23300 cd) from the Lightscape library were used to il- differences between the picture pairs, we also checked
luminate the scenes. All the luminaries in the scene whether there was significant brightness differences
pairs were at the same height. They were moved hori- between pictures in the order they were presented (Fig-
zontally to illuminate different elements of the scene. ure 1). It is possible that the brightness of previous pic-
The luminaries themselves were erased from the final ture may have an effect on the perceived brightness of
views so that the technical equipment would not attract the subsequent picture. There was no indication that
attention. It also made the device positioning easier and the presentation order and brightness changes would
freer. Photometric data (1uminance and lunuhous flux) have had an effect on the results.
was derived from the Lightscape images. There is research evidence that vertical depth is a
Besides the different elements on the scene, the per- positive predictor of preference and may therefore have
ceived brightness of the scene, vertical depth and the an effect on restoration30). The vertical depth (distance
field of view may also have some effect on restorative from the view point to the furthest visible point) of the
experience. When moving the luminaries, these factors picture pairs is roughly equal, although the spatial con-
also change. Even though the same type of luminaries figuration changes.
at the same height illunlinate the scene in all views,
different surface materials have different reflectances Test 1
and colours. Different objects also have different forms Location: Atrium-shaped lecture hall at Helsinki Uni-
and sizes that will affect the distance and angle be- versity of Technology
tween the luminary and object. These factors will have Subjects: 15 students
an effect on the surface luminance and shadow casting Slide size: roughly 170 X 120cm
and, further, on the perceived brightness. Subject distance from the screen: 3.5 - 12m
It is also possible that restorative environments ap Test 2
pear brighter due to preferences and positive attitudes Location: Atriunrshaped lecture hall at Helsinki Uni-
towards the scenes containing restorative features. Yet versity of Te chnology
another factor that may enhance the perceived bright- Subjects: 7 students
ness is the better colour contrast greenery tends to have Slide size: roughly 170 X 120cm
compared to roads and parking lots. Also, the category Subject distance from the screen: 3.5 - 10m
division in itself affects the spatial configuration of il- Pictures were presented in reversed order.
luminated surfaces; vegetation is more vertical, Test 3
whereas roads and parking lots lie on a horizontal Location: Meeting room at the University of Vaasa
plane. For this reason, the visible illuminated area from Subjects: 13 persons, staLf at the University of Vaasa
the pedestrian viewpoint tends to be larger when vege- Slide size: roughly 164 x 130cm
tation is illuminated than when parking lots are illumi- Subject distance from the screen: 2 - 6 m
nated. To balance this, a bit more light was used to il- Pictures were presented in reversed order.
luminate the parking 10ts-and-roads category.
Objects immediately around the picture will also have The subjects were free to choose their preferred view-

The llluminating Engineering Institute of Japan 40


J. Light & Vis. Env. Vol.33, No. I, 2009 41

ing position within the two test spaces. studies suggesting that 2-factor solution is preferable
than 4-factor one in PRS measures24).
2.2 Subjects Preceding condition of stress or mental fatigue was
There were 35 subjects, 19 men and 16 women, who measured by two questions. One question scored tired-
evaluated all the scenes. Their age varied from 20 to 58 ness and the other stress level. Thirteen reported being
years. The mean age was 29 years (20-29 years, 26 sub- stressed (score 4-6 on 0-6 scale) before the test and 15
jects; 30-39 years, 3 subjects; 40-49 years, 2 subjects; reported being tired (score 4-6 on 0-6 scale) before the
50-59 years, 4 subjects). The subjects were unaware of test. If the total score of these two questions was 6 or
the purpose of the study and participated in the ex- more, the subject was considered to be in a state of
periment voluntarily without course credit. mental fatigue. Eighteen subjects formed the fatigued
group and 17 the non-fatigued group. The purpose was
2.3 Measures to see if the antecedent condition affected the PRS and
Experienced restoration measures were obtained by ROS scores. There was no special antecedent stress
the Perceived Restorativeness Scale (PRS) and the Res- manipulation. The subjects participated in the test dur-
toration Outcome Scale (ROS) instruments based on ing their normal day.
attention restoration theory. PRS scores for Being Away, Respondents indicated on a 7-point scale (0=Not at all,
Fascination, Coherence, and Compatibility that reflect 6=Completely) the extent to which the given statement
the potential to be restored according to attention resto- fitted their experience of the given scene. All subjects
ration theory. There were 16 statements concerning flLled both forms, PRS being administered first and
PRS. All of these were used to compute a mean sum- ROS second.
mary score of perceived restorativeness. In the PRS sec-
tion, the subjects were asked to imagine themselves in 2.4 Statistical analysis and validr'ty checks
the environment each view presented and respond to Repeated measures and one-way ANOVAs, together
the statements based on that assumptionl5)23)24). PRS with linear correlation (SPSS program), were used in
has been used with stressed9)3D and with non-stressed the validity checks and tests for experimental effects. In
individualsl5). repeated measures ANOVA, the scene pairs were speci-
ROS measures positive affect and reflection, which fied as a priori contrasts. There were five missing val-
are the possible restoration outcomes according to at- ues in the data matrix; Multivariate-Normal Missing-
tention restoration theory. However, restoration out- Value Imputation (NCSS-program) was used to fill in
comes might also include increase in subjective vital- the missing values.
ity32)33). Thus, for purposes of further theoretical devel-
opment of the restorative experience scale, items such 3. Results
as "I would be ready to meet difficult challenges" of sub- Calculations of internal consistency (Cronbach's a)
jective vitality were added. A positive state was scored showed that the coefficients were high across all pic-
with statements like "I feel peaceful and relaxed". tures (a>0.88; Table 2). The focus"oflight-on-greenery
Statements like "It feels easy to make plans for the fu- category got the hlghest mean ratings in both PRS and
ture" referred to reflection. A mean summary score of ROS ratings. Tests were conducted using repeated
the 15 items in ROS was used in subsequent statistical measures ANOVA with contrast tests (SPSS). The
analysis. In the ROS section, the subjects were asked to states of mental fatigue and gender were set as be-
imagine that they had spent some time in the environ- tween-subjects factors. The setting category effect was
ment presented by each slide and respond to the state- significant for both PRS (F(5,155)=17.061, p<0.001,
ments based on that assunrption. The ROS section fol- power=1.0) and ROS (F(3.955,122.597)=20.058, p<0.001,
lowed the PRS section in order to give the subjects more power=1.0) (Greenhouse-Geisser corrected) scores in all
time to experience the environment. There were 15 picture pairs, and was in the expected direction, with
statements concerning ROS13)26)27). the focus-on-greenery category having a higher mean in
ROS is a newer tool than PRS, which has mainly been both PRS and ROS ratings. The PRS scores corre-
used in the previous studies on restorative experience. sponded strongly with ROS scores (r=0.636, p<0.001).
ROS measures were used in this study as a converging The ROS ratings were higher than PRS ratings in all
measure to give more weight to the results and to assist pictures.
in the scale's further development. Because we wanted The main effect of mental fatigue as a between-
to examine how PRS and ROS scores converge with subjects factor was insigni icant in both overall PRS
each other, a mean suJnmary score, instead of separate (F(1,33)=0.140, p=0.710, power=0.065) and ROS
factor scores, was used on both scales. The use of a (F(1,33)=0.283, p=0.598, power=0.081) ratings (re-
mean smnmary score is also supported by previous peated measures ANOVA with picture pairs as con-

41 The lllum in ating Engineering Institute of Jap an


42 J Light & Vis. Env. Vol.33, No. 1, 2009

Table 2 Mean ratings (M), standard deviations (SD), Cronbach's Alphas (a) and the significance levels of contrast tests (p) (ANOVA) of
PRS and ROS scales between piciure pairs 4-1 , 5-3, and 2-6
PRS ROS

n
M SD p power
35 35 35
M SD p power
35 35 35
picture 4 3.4 0.9 O.94 3.6 0.9 0.95
picture 1 2.4 0.8 O.88 <0.001 o.998 2.9 0.9 O.91 O.OOI O.914
pieture 5 2.9 O.8 0.89 3 .4 O. 8 0.92
picture 3 2.2 0.8 O.91 O.OO1 0.877 2.9 0.9 0.93 0.008 O.816
picture 2 2.6 0.9 o.93 3.2 O.8 0.90
pieture 6 2.1 o.8 0.88 o.003 0.941 2.7 O.8 0.90 <0.001 0.791
Contrasts: 4- l

5-3
2-6

Tabie 3 The significan of mental fatigue (p) as a between-subjects factor (one-way ANOVA) in PRS and ROS ratings of different pic-
tures, mean ratings (M) and standard deviations (SD) of PRS and ROS scales by fatigued and non-fatigued
Fatigued Non- Fatigued
PRS ROS PRS PRS
n=3 5 n=3 5 n=18
ROS
n= 1 8 n=1 7 n= i 7
ROS

picture l
p power p power
O.225 O.224 O.i63 0.283
M SD
2.2 O.7
M SD
2.70.9
M2.6 SD
0.9
M3.2 SD 0.8
picture 2 0.180 O.265 O.127 O.331 2.3 l .O 2,9 0.8 2.8 o.7 3.3 0.7
picture 3 O.637 O.075 0.838 O.055 2.1 0.9 2.9 l .O 2.3 0.8 3.0 0.6
pieture 4 O.045 0.524 0.458 0.113 3.7 l.O 3.8 l .O 3.1 0.7 3.5 0.9
picture 5 O.913 0.051 O.949 0.050 2.9 0.9 3 *4 O.8 2.9 o.7 3 *4 o.6
picture 6 O.524 O.096 O.949 0.050 2.0 O.6 2.7 O.8 2. l o.9 2.7 o.9

Table 4 The significance of gender as a between-subjects factor (p) (one way ANOVA), mean ratings (M) and siandard deviations (SD) of
PRS and ROS scales by women and men
Men Women
PRS ROS PRS
n=35 n=35 n= 1 9
ROS
n=19
PRS ROS
n= { 6 n= 1 6
p power p power M SD M S.D M SDO.9 M2.8SD
picture 1 0.031 O.5902.7O.53
o.6 O.094
3.0 0.8 2. i .O

picture 2 O.758 O.060 0.938 0.05i 2.5 o.7 2.5 O.9 2.6 l .1 3.5 0.7
picture 3 0.014 O.717 O.144 O.307 2.6 0.7 3.l 0.9 l .9 0.8 2.7 O.g
picture 4 O.159 0.288 0.136 0.318 3 .2 0.9 3 *4 1.i 3.7 l .O 3.9 O.7
picture 5 O.346 O.153 0.368 O.144 3.0 0.7 3,5 0.9 2.8 O.9 3 ,2 O.8
pieture 6 O.447 0.116 0.679 O.069 2.l 0.8 2.7 0.8 l .9 O.7 2.8 0.8

a]1 pictures (mean values) 2.7 o.4 3 0.4 2.5 o.7 3.2 O.5

trasts). It was, however, significant in picture pair 4- 1 p=0.831, power=0.055) (repeated measures ANOVA
(p=0.025, power=0.629) in PRS ratings. To see if the with picture pairs as contrasts and gender as a be-
fatigue had any special response pattern to either of the tween-subjects factor). It was, however, significant in
setting categories, we tested the significance of mental picture pair 4-1 in PRS ratings (p=0.017, power=0.688).
fatigue as a between-subjects factor in all pictures. To look at the gender-specific responses a bit closer, we
Mental fatigue was a significant factor only in picture 4 tested the significance of gender as a between-subjects
in PRS ratings (p=0.045, one-way ANOVA with mental factor in all pictures. Gender was significant in the rat-
fatigue as a between-subjects factor; Table 3) so that ings of pictures I and 3 in PRS ratings (p=0.031;
those who were fatigued evaluated it as more restora p=0.014, one-way ANOVA with gender as a between-
tive than non-fatigued. The category effect was insig subjects factor; Table 4) so that men evaluated them as
nificant in all other pictures. more restorative. Both pictures belonged to the focus-
The main effect of gender as a between-subjects factor on-parking lots category. In ROS ratings, gender was an
was insignificant in both overall PRS (F(1,33)=1.424, insignificant factor in all pictures. Table 4 presents the
p=0.241, power=0.212) and ROS ratings (F(1,33)=0.047, mean values and standard deviations by gender. The

The llluminating Engineering Institute of Japan 42


J. Light & Vis. Env. Vol.33, No.1, 2009 43

category difference is higher in women's responses, es- lowing studies. However in this study the viewing angle
pecially on the PRS scale, where women's mean score in was so small for most of the subjects, i.e., the picture
the focus-on-parking-lots category is 2 and, in the focus- covered so small area in the field of view, that it was
on-vegetation category, 3. The corresponding figures possible to see the whole image with one glance. To our
with men are 2.5 and 2.9. In ROS ratings, the parking knowledge calibration of the view angle or the use of a
lots category got a mean score of 2.8 and vegetation fixed point on a scene where the participants should
category 3.5 in the womenjs responses, whereas the cor- focus has not been used in any of the previous studies of
responding figures were 2.9 and 3.1 in the men:s re- landscape preference or restoration.
s ponses. The subjects attended the experiment durixrg their
normal day and did not undergo any antecedent stress
4. Discussion manipolation. Therefore, the setting category efflect
The results support the hypothesis that focusing light might have been stronger after a more stressfLll situa-
on the different elements of the scene affects the experi- tion, but the results suggest that lighting manipulation
enced restoration. As expected, focusing light on vegeta- is a strong enough effect to enhance restorative experi-
tion resulted in higher ratings of restorative experience ence, even in everyday conditions in our world of cogni-
than when the light was focused on parking lots and tive overload. The results, however, do not support the
roads. The ratings on the Perceived Restoration Scale, view that mental fatigue is a significant factor in ex-
i.e., the qualities related to the potential of restorative perienced restoration evaluations. Mental fatigue as a
experience, were lower than on the Restoration Out- between-subjects factor was signiflcant in only one pic-
come Scale, i.e., the present positive feelixrgs and ease of ture pair in PRS ratings and none in ROS ratings.
reflection, in all pictures. This may have been due to the When looking at the picture-specific PRS and ROS tat-
subjects in general having a relatively positive mental ings, it was significant only in picture 4 in PRS ratings,
state and there being no antecedent stress condition, which was also assessed as the most restorative picture.
both of which were reflected in the ROS scores. The dif- In general, the results of different category pictures do
ferences between the two category ratings were also not indicate that the mentaJly fatigued would differen-
greater on the PRS than on the ROS. There may be two tiate between categories in their responses. This may be
reasons for this. One possibility is that it was easier to due to the small number of subjects. It is, however, also
assess perceived restoration potential than restoration possible that the environnrent has to be very restcrative,
outcomes. Another possibility is that, because the resto- like in picture 4, in order for the difference to occur be-
ration outcornes are more connected with the subjecfs tween the fatigued and non-fatigued. The presentation
present mental state, the preceding positive mood left medium might also have an effect on the results. In real
less space for variation, whereas the perceived potential environments, the restorative experience might be
(relating more to the perception of surroundings) en- stronger.
ables more variation. PRS scores corresponded tc ROS Gender as a between-subjects factor was also signifi-
scores strongly, following them at a slightly lower level. cant in only one picture pair in PRS ratings and insig-
Both scales showed good sensitivity in detecting the nificant in all ROS ratings. When looking at the pic-
setting category effect. This study thus encourages their ture-specific ratings, gender was a significant factor in
fufther use and development. two pictures in the parking lots category. This may be
The present study used as a setting category division due to women:s more negative attitude tcwards the
greenery contra parking lots and roads. Therefore, the parking lots category compared to melQis. The subjects
results cannot be generalised tc all scene contents. on this research were predominantly fairly young, with
These contents are, however, very common in urban a mean age of 29. It is possible that young men may
nightscapes, and thus allow further application in de- have a more positive attitude towards parking lots and
sign efforts. Because the research was simulation based cars than other age groups and women. The degree to
and there were no objective measures concerning the which the results can be generalized to other age
renewal of directed attention capacity, it is not possible groups needs further research. It is possible that young
to make definitive conclusions. Further research using people appreciate more urban scene contents that indi-
converging operations is thus needed. The results are, cate possibilities for social encounters, than older people
however, highly suggestive and in line with previous do. Women also tended to give more differentiated rat-
findings . ings to different categories so that the more restorative
This study can also be criticized for the lack of cali- scenes scored lrlgher and less restorative scenes lower
bration of the subjective scale since it is not possible to than the corresponding ratings in men's responses. Men
determine what the subjects were actually looking at. tended to have more moderate differences between the
Thus the calibration needs further attention in the fol- ratings of these two categories. The results indicate

43 The llluminating Engineering Institute of Japan


44 J. Light & Vis. Env. Vol.33, No. 1, 2009,

that women may show greater response to the envi- on the experience of fear and insecurity. This study thus
romnent in psychological measures, which is in line regarded lighting as a potential way to increase the re-
with previous findings2). There is, however, not enough storative quality of nightscapes. It also expanded the
gender-specific data to make any definitive conclusions. time span of restorative environments into nightscapes
Whether these differences are also consistent in physio- and provided a new context in which restoration might
10gical measures needs further research as well. occur. The llnexplored role of lighting may be an impor-
In our experiment changes in the focus of light did tant factor in restorative experience and thus needs
also change the visible scene coniiguration from a more fufther investigation.
horizontal (parking lots) to a more vertical one (green-
ery) which may also have affected on the results. There Acknowledgement
is however research evidence that in daytime environ-
ments people prefer greener surroundings also when Research reported in thls paper was supported by a
the spatial configuration is the same (urban alleys vs. grant from the Kone foundation.
natural pathways)34). In urban alleys fear tends to be a
more common reaction than a sense of mystery whereas References
the reverse is true in natural surroundings. The au-
thors also have prelnninary research evidence, that (1) Kaplan, R. and Kaplan, S.: The Experience of Na-
when the spatial configuration and lighting setting are ture: A Psychological Perspective, New York: Cam-
the same, natural scenes are considered as more re- bridge (1989).
storative, preferred and less frightening than urban (2) Ulrich, R.S.: Natural versus urban scenes; Some
ones also during night time. Scenes containing both psychophysiological effects, Environment and Be-
elements were assessed close to natural ones. However havior, 13, pp.523 556(1981).
in this study we wanted to concentrate on the possibili- (3) Ulrich, R.S.: Human response to vegetation and
ties of lighting manipulation, not the actual changes in landscapes, Landscape and Urban Plauning, 13,
scene contents, might offer in enhancing the restora- p p.29-44(1986).
tiveness of urban nightscapes. (4) Hartig, T.: Nature experience in transactional per-
The relationship between fear and restoration needs spective, Landscape and Urban Planning, 25, pp.17-
fufther exploration, bearing in mind that fear is often a 36(1993).
prominent factor in night time environmental experi- (5) Hietanen, J.K. and Korpela, K.M.: Do both negative
encel7). While trees, greenery and deflected vistas have and positive environmental scenes elicit rapid affec-
restorative characteristics in daytime environments, tive processing?, Environment and Behavior, 36,
during night time they provide conceallnent, Iinilted p p.558 577(2004).
prospect and blocked escape, which may increase fear20). (6) Hietanen, J.K., Klemettil , T., Kettunen, J.E. and
An interesting extension would also be to examine the Korpela, K.M.: What is a nice smile like that doing
whole restoration-fear preference pattern in urban in a place like this? Automatic affective r6sponses to
nightscapes. The importance of these environmental environments irrfluence the recognition of facial ex-
factors is also visible in modern migration, where peo- pressions, Psychological Research; doi: 10.1007/
ple are increasingly moving from urban centres to sub- s00426-006-0064-4(2006).
urban or rural areas as they seek better housing envi- (7) Korpela, K., Klemettil , T. and Hietanen, J.: Evi-
ronments and an escape from urban stressors. dence for rapid affective evaluation of environ-
In this study a bit less light was actually used to illu- mental scenes, Environment and Behavior, 34-5,
minate the more restorative scene pairs, which shows p p.478 494(2002).
that energy issues do not necessarily contradict the (8) Berto, R.: Exposure to restorative enviromnents
creation of restorative lighting enviromnents. At the helps restore attentional capacity, Journal of Envi-
time of growing health care expenditures and energy rornnental Psychology, 25, pp.249-259(2005).
concerns, the results are highly encouraging. The re- (9) Hartig, T., Mang, M. and Evans, G.W. : Restorative
sults indicate that the focus of light- and scene-contcnt effects of natural environment experience, Envi-
issues need further attention in the creation of healthy ronment and Behavior, 23, pp.3 26(1991).
and energy-conscious urban environments. (10)Kaplan, R.: Some Psychological benefits of Gar-
In conclusion, the present study offers insights into dening, Environment and Behavior, 5 2, pp.145-
how lighting can be used to enhance the experienced 162(1973).
restoration of night time environments. The possibili- (11)Kaplan, R.: Some psychological benefits of an Out-
ties of gaining positive experiences in nightscapes have door Challenge Program, Environment and Behav-
received very little attention when the focus has been ior 6, pp.101-116(1974).

The llluminating Engineering Institute of Japan 44


J Light & Vis. Env. Vol.33, No. 1, 2009 45

(12)Kaplan, S.: The restorative benefits of nature: To- (24)Hartig, T., Korpela, K., Evans, G.W. and G rlixrg, T.:
ward an integrative framework, Journal of Envi- Validation of a measure of perceived environmental
ronmental Psychology, 15, pp.169- 182(1995). restorativeness, G6teborg Psychological Reports, 26-
(13)Staats, H., Kieviet, A. and Hartig, T.: Where to re- 7, G6teborg University(1996).
cover from attentional fatigue: An expectancy value URL: http ://www.psy.gu.se/download/gpr967.pdf
analysis of environmental preference, Journal of (25)Hartig, T., Lindblom, K. and Ovefelt, K.: The Home
environmental Psychology, 23, pp.147- 157(2003). and Near-Home Area Offer Restoration Opportuni-
(14)Ulrich, R.S., Simons, R.F., Losito, B.D.. Fiorito, E., ties Differentiated by Gender. Scandinavian Hous-
Miles, M.A. and Zelson, M.: Stress recovery during ing and Planning Research, 15, pp.283-296(1998).
exposure to natural and urban environments, Jour- (26)Kaplan, S., Bradwell, L.V. and Slakter, D.B.: The
nal of Environmental Psychology, 11, pp.201- Musemn as restorative environment Environment
230(1991). and Behavior, 25-6, pp.725 742(1993).
(15)Hartig, T., Korpela, K., Evans, G.W. and G rling, T.: (27)Korpela, K., Yl6n, M., ヲbrrvainen, L. and Silvenno-
A measure of restcrative quality in environments, inen, H.: Determinants of restorative experiences in
Scandinavian Housing and Planning Research, 14, everyday favourite places, Health & Place 2007.
pp.175-194(1997). doi: 10. 10 16/ j .healthplace.2007. 10.008 .
(16)Fisher, B.S. and Nasar, J.L.: Fear of crime in rela- (28)Trent, R.B., Neumann, E. and Kvashny, A.: Presen-
tion to three exterior site features: Prospect, refuge tation mode and question format artifacts in visual
and escape. Environment and Behavior, 24, pp.35 assessment research. Landscape and Urban Plan-
65(1992). ning, 14, pp.225 235(1987).
(17)Nasar, J.L.: The Evaluative image of the city, Thou- (29)Newsham, G.R., Seetzen, H., Veitch, J.A. and
sand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, (1998) . Chaudhuri, A.: Whitehead LA. Lighting quality
(18)Herzog, T. and Kropscott, L.S.: Legibility, Mystery, evaluations using images on a high dynamic range
and Vlsual Access as Predictors of Preference and display, Paper presented at ARCCfEAAE 2002 In-
Perceived Danger in Forest settings without path- ternational Conference on Architectural Research,
ways, Environment and Behavior, 36-5, pp.659- Montreal, QC(2002).
677(2004). (30)Herzog, T. and Smith, G.A.: Danger, mystery and
(19)Herzog, T. and Kutzli, G.: Preference and perceived environnrental preference, Environment and Be-
danger in field/forest settings, Environment and haviol 20-3, pp.320-344(1988).
Behavior, 34-6, pp.819-835(2002). (31)Bodin, M. and Hartig, T.: Does the outdoor envi-
(20)Nasar, J.L., Fisher, B. and Grannis, M.: Proximate ronment matter for psychological restoration gained
physical cues to fear of crime, Landscape and urban through running?, Psychology of Sports and Exer-
plauning, 26, pp. 161-178(1993). cise, 4, pp.141-153(2003).
(21)Van den Berg, A.E. and Ter Heijne, M.: Fear versus (32)Nix, G.A., Ryan, R.M., Manly, J.B. and Deci, E.L.:
fascination: An exploration of emotional responses Revitalization through self-regulation: The effects of
to natural threats, Journal of Environmental Psy autonomous and controlled motivation on happiness
chology, 25, pp.261-272(2005). and vitality, Journal of Experimental Social Psy
(22)Van den Berg, A.E., Koole, S.L, and Van der Wulp, chology, 35, pp.266 284(1999).
N.Y. : Environmental preference and restoration: (33)Ryan, R.M. and Frederick, C.: On energy, personal-
(How) are they related?. Journal of Environmental ity and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic re-
Psychology, 23 pp.135- 146(2003). flection of well-being, Journal of Personali:ty, 65,
(23)Lamnann, K.. Garling, T. and Stcrmark, K.M.: Rat- p p.529-565(1997).
ing scale measures of restorative components of en- (34)Herzog, T. and Miller, E.: The role of mystery in
viromnents, Journal of Environmental Psychology, perceived danger and environmental preference,
21, pp.3 1-44(2001). Environment and Behavior, 30-4, pp.429-449(1998).

45 The llluminating Engineering Institute of Japan

You might also like