You are on page 1of 6

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/331641271

Integrative Complexity

Chapter · February 2019


DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_1942-1

CITATIONS READS

4 2,396

2 authors:

Vera Békés Peter Suedfeld


Yeshiva University University of British Columbia - Vancouver
52 PUBLICATIONS   315 CITATIONS    336 PUBLICATIONS   7,099 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Long-term Effects After Prolonged Spaceflight (LEAPS) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Vera Békés on 10 March 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


I

Integrative Complexity Introduction

Vera Békés1 and Peter Suedfeld2 IC is unique among cognitive complexity formu-
1
Ferkauf Graduate School of Psychology, lations in that it does not only consider the amount
Yeshiva University, Bronx, NY, USA of different information, perspectives, or dimen-
2
Department of Psychology, University of British sions noticed and processed regarding one issue
Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada (differentiation) but also incorporates perceiving
the connections among these divergent dimen-
sions (integration). These recognized connections
may be interactions, trade-offs, synthesis, or
Synonyms
belonging under a unifying schema. IC thus
only refers to the structure of thinking and not its
Cognitive complexity; Cognitive structure; Cog-
content; it considers how people think about an
nitive style; Conceptual complexity; Conceptual
issue, independently of what they think about
systems; Flexibility; Interactive complexity;
it. Consequently, any idea can be expressed at
Metacognition; Meta-complexity; Wisdom
any level of IC (Suedfeld 2010).
In general, low IC is characterized by rigid,
black-and-white thinking, intolerance for ambigu-
Definition ity and uncertainty, a desire for rapid closure, and
not recognizing the validity of other viewpoints.
Integrative complexity (IC) refers to the structure Conversely, high IC is marked by flexible, broad
of information processing, independently of its thinking that recognizes multiple aspects and pos-
content. It entails two aspects: differentiation and sible interpretations of an issue and sees connec-
integration, where differentiation refers to the tions and dynamic tensions between perspectives
extent of perceiving a variety of dimensions and (Suedfeld 1985).
perspectives when considering an issue, whereas
integration is a capacity to create conceptual con-
nections among these different dimensions and IC, Intellect, and Personality
perspectives.
Despite the required cognitive capacity to perform
interactively complex thinking, the association
between IC and intelligence has been found
to be weak. Instead of an aspect of intelligence,
© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019
V. Zeigler-Hill, T. K. Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_1942-1
2 Integrative Complexity

IC is better conceptualized as a thinking style that IC and Age


involves high levels of information search, flexi-
bility, and tolerance for ambiguity, for uncertainty, Despite of suppositions about individuals becom-
and for lack of closure (Suedfeld 2010). IC has ing more sophisticated in their reasoning over the
been found to be associated with divergent think- lifespan, IC and age has not been shown to be
ing (Suedfeld and Coren 1992) and to certain consistently associated. On the contrary, there is
personality characteristics, such as openness, cre- some evidence for a decrease in IC in older indi-
ativity, and relative lack of compliance and con- viduals (Pratt et al. 1990) A phenomenon of “ter-
scientiousness (Tetlock et al. 1993). Individuals minal drop” in IC during the last few years of an
with high IC have been found to be more extra- individual’s life, independently of the cause of
verted, gregarious, and socially adept, motivated death (i.e., protracted illness or sudden death),
to seek power (Coren and Suedfeld 1995), and has also been demonstrated (Suedfeld 1985;
less authoritarian and dogmatic (Schroder Suedfeld and Piedrahita 1984).
et al. 1967).
The perception of those with high IC varies.
Generally they are seen as moderate, independent,
IC and Situational Factors
and flexible with good social, decision-making,
and information processing skills, high in initia-
The general cognitive capacity for complex think-
tive and self-objectivity (Tetlock et al. 1993).
ing has been found to be a relatively stable indi-
However, they might also appear as procrastina-
vidual characteristic, whereas IC greatly depends
tors, overly anxious (Coren and Suedfeld 1995),
on the actual situation and circumstances. Besides
antagonistic, and even narcissistic (Tetlock
personal characteristics, the actual level of IC is
et al. 1993).
affected by other internal factors (e.g., motivation,
fatigue) as well as by external circumstances such
as time pressure and perceived danger.
IC and Political Views The level of IC demonstrated in each situation
can be described based on the cognitive manager
There is strong evidence for an association model (Suedfeld 2010). Independently of individ-
between political preferences and interactively ual differences in cognitive capabilities, complex
complex thinking style. Moderate political information processing requires certain psycho-
views, especially left-of-center on the political logical and material resources, such as cognitive
spectrum, are generally linked to more cognitive effort, time, collecting information, and so on, and
flexibility when compared to those on the right or individuals have limited capacity to maintain a
(the further) left. One plausible explanation is that high level of cognitive functioning. A good cog-
those who believe in multiple competing political nitive manager would devote only as much com-
values employ a more complex approach and thus plexity to a given situation as it minimally
demonstrate higher IC than those emphasizing requires. Situations that are considered more
one value above the others. In addition, those important or complex will call for more cognitive
with less complex thinking styles may feel threat- investment, whereas less important or simpler
ened by the prospect of living in a more open, situations would be solved by less cognitive
changing, and potentially chaotic society and thus effort. Accordingly, IC increases when the
be inclined toward more conservative or extreme person tries to find a solution to a problem
ideologies (Tetlock 1986). deemed significant, and it decreases and returns
Integrative Complexity 3

to baseline once the strategy is successful (or the cognitive resources (such as professional chal-
problem gets otherwise resolved or becomes lenges) and decrease when they seem to be out
unimportant). However, if the problem remains of the person’s control and thus deemed to not be
unresolved, maintaining an elevated level of worth the personal investment (e.g., a tense polit-
investment is necessary, which might eventually ical situation) and/or are prolonged and thus even-
lead to exhaustion and the depletion in the indi- tually exhaust the person’s inner resources (for
vidual’s cognitive resources and consequently to a instance, organized persecution during the
reduction in IC. In this case, the person may Holocaust).
become overly frustrated, tired, or stressed to con-
tinue processing the information on a high level of
complexity and might perceive the situation as IC and Political Violence
unsolvable, look for a simplifying strategy, or
get distracted by other problems. The concept of IC is most often examined and
discussed in the context of political psychology.
The most robust finding is that IC in speeches
IC and Performance tends to drop when political tension grows and
there is a threat of impending domestic or interna-
Higher IC does not always lead to better out- tional violence (Suedfeld and Rank 1976;
comes. In fact, the optimal level of IC seems to Suedfeld and Tetlock 1977). This “war crisis
be determined by the nature of the task. Complex effect” has been consistently found to occur dur-
activities, intellectual tasks, and collaborative ing the escalation of political tension before the
efforts, when there is no urgency, do require inter- outbreak of wars. On the other hand, when inter-
actively complex thinking for an optimal result national confrontations are resolved through
(Gruenfeld and Hollingshead 1993). On the other peaceful negotiations, increased or at least
hand, some tasks can be more effectively solved maintained levels of IC have been demonstrated;
by a more simplistic approach. These may be and, interestingly, the IC level of the negotiators
simple, insignificant, familiar, or unresolvable gets mirrored, i.e., change in one party evokes
tasks, problems outside the control of the individ- similar change in the other (Tetlock 1985).
ual, or situations that require rapid decisions;
investing cognitive resources in these cases may
be unnecessary or even counterproductive (e.g., Assessment
Tetlock and Boettger 1989).
IC can be measured on any type of verbal text.
Given the interest in political psychology among
IC and Life Events IC researchers, archival data of policy statements,
speeches, diplomatic documents, and personal
Research shows inconsistent associations letters have most frequently been analyzed. The
between various life events and levels of IC: dur- scoring system has been established by Baker-
ing negative life events, IC was found to either Brown et al. (1990). First the text is divided to
increase or decrease, whereas positive life events thematic units (i.e., segments of text related to one
were associated with higher IC or with no change idea, usually a paragraph in length). Several
in IC (De Vries et al. 1995; Suedfeld and Bluck trained judges then independently rate the level
1993; Suedfeld and Pennebaker 1997; Suedfeld of IC from 1 to 7 as it occurs in the text based on
et al. 1998; Suedfeld and Granatstein 1995). The the rating manual. Although the scoring system
mixed results support the theory that various fac- has been found to be reliable, its application is
tors determine the level of actual cognitive invest- somewhat time-consuming and labor-intensive.
ment. IC might increase during life events that Applications of computerized scoring systems
appear to be controllable if one invests enough has been proposed (Conway et al. 2014; Young
4 Integrative Complexity

and Hermann 2014); however, despite their rela- De Vries, B., Blando, J., & Walker, L. J. (1995). The review
tively reliable complexity scores, they are unable of life’s events: Analyses of content and structure. In
B. Haight & J. Webster (Eds.), The art and science of
to assess the integration component (Suedfeld reminiscing: Theory, research, methods, and applica-
2010). tions (pp. 123–137). Washington, DC: Taylor &
Francis.
Gruenfeld, D. H., & Hollingshead, A. B. (1993). Socio-
cognition in work groups: The evolution of group inte-
Conclusion grative complexity and its relation to task performance.
Small Group Research, 24, 383–405.
The concept of IC is most often examined and Pratt, M. W., Pancer, M., Hunsberger, B., & Manchester, J.
(1990). Reasoning about the self and relationships in
discussed in the context of political psychology,
maturity: An integrative complexity analysis of indi-
but it has also been applied fruitfully to various vidual differences. Journal of Personality and Social
other areas; the study of IC has increased our Psychology, 59, 575–581.
understanding of a wide variety of issues in polit- Schroder, H. M., Driver, M. J., & Streufert, S. (1967).
Human information processing: Individuals and
ical, social, and personality psychology. Research
groups functioning in complex social situations. Holt:
continues to focus on factors affecting differences Rinehart and Winston.
in IC in various settings, possible procedures to Suedfeld, P. (1985). APA presidential addresses: The rela-
facilitate changes in IC, and the implications of tion of integrative complexity to historical, profes-
sional, and personal factors. Journal of Personality
such changes both on the individual and group
and Social Psychology, 49(6), 1643.
levels. Suedfeld, P. (2010). The cognitive processing of politics
and politicians: Archival studies of conceptual and
integrative complexity. Journal of Personality, 78(6),
1669–1702.
Cross-References Suedfeld, P., & Bluck, S. (1993). Changes in integrative
complexity accompanying significant life events:
▶ Cognitive Complexity Historical evidence. Journal of Personality and Social
▶ Dogmatism Psychology, 64(1), 124.
Suedfeld, P., & Coren, S. (1992). Cognitive correlates
▶ Explanatory Style of conceptual complexity. Personality and Individual
▶ Field Independence Differences, 13, 1193–1199.
▶ Flexibility Suedfeld, P., & Granatstein, J. L. (1995). Leader complex-
▶ High Levels of Information Search ity in personal and professional crises: Concurrent
and retrospective information processing. Political
▶ Lack of Closure Psychology, 16, 509–522.
▶ Need for Closure Suedfeld, P., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1997). Health outcomes
▶ Need for Cognition and cognitive aspects of recalled negative life events.
▶ Personal Constructs Psychosomatic Medicine, 59, 172–177.
Suedfeld, P., & Piedrahita, L. E. (1984). Intimations of
▶ Tolerance for Ambiguity mortality: Integrative simplification as a precursor of
▶ Uncertainty death. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
47(4), 848.
Suedfeld, P., & Rank, A. D. (1976). Revolutionary leaders:
Long-term success as a function of changes in concep-
References tual complexity. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 34, 169–178.
Baker-Brown, G., Ballard, E. J., Bluck, S., De Vries, B., Suedfeld, P., & Tetlock, P. E. (1977). Integrative complex-
Suedfeld, P., & Tetlock, P. E. (1990). Coding manual ity of communications in international crises. Journal
for conceptual/integrative complexity. Berkeley: of Conflict Resolution, 21, 427–441.
University of British Columbia and University of Suedfeld, P., Fell, C., & Krell, R. (1998). Structural aspects
California. of survivor’s thinking about the holocaust. Journal of
Conway, L. G., III, Conway, K. R., Gornick, L. J., & Traumatic Stress, 11(2), 323–336.
Houck, S. C. (2014). Automated integrative complex- Tetlock, P. E. (1985). Accountability: A social check on
ity. Political Psychology, 35, 603–624. the fundamental attribution error. Social Psychology
Coren, S., & Suedfeld, P. (1995). Personality correlates of Quarterly, 48, 227–236.
conceptual complexity. Journal of Social Behavior and
Personality, 10, 229–242.
Integrative Complexity 5

Tetlock, P. E. (1986). A value pluralism model of ideolog- Tetlock, P. E., Peterson, R. S., & Berry, J. M. (1993).
ical reasoning. Journal of Personality and Social Flattering and unflattering personality portraits of
Psychology, 50, 819–827. integratively simple and complex managers. Journal
Tetlock, P. E., & Boettger, R. (1989). Accountability: of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 500–511.
A social magnifier of the dilution effect. Journal of Young, M. D., & Hermann, M. G. (2014). Increased com-
Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 388–398. plexity has its benefits. Political Psychology, 35(5),
635–645.

View publication stats

You might also like