You are on page 1of 12

This article was downloaded by: [McGill University Library]

On: 20 November 2014, At: 12:43


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office:
Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Aging & Mental Health


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription
information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/camh20

Loneliness research and interventions: A


review of the literature
a
L. ANDERSSON
a
Section of Social Gerontology , Stockholm Gerontology Research
Center , Stockholm, Sweden
Published online: 09 Jun 2010.

To cite this article: L. ANDERSSON (1998) Loneliness research and interventions: A review of the
literature, Aging & Mental Health, 2:4, 264-274, DOI: 10.1080/13607869856506

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607869856506

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the
“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our
agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the
accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views
expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views
of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and
should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall
not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages,
and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection
with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.
This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial
or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or
distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and
use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions
A gin g & M ental H ealth (1998);2(4):264± 274

O RIG INA L A RTIC LE

Loneliness research and interventions: a review of the literature

L. AN DERSSON

Section of Social Geron tology, Stockholm Gerontology Research C enter, Stockholm , Sweden

A bstract
The paper describes and reviews concepts and studies in the area of social relations and health, with special em phasis on
loneliness. Related concepts such as social networks and social support are also considered. The fundamental distinction
between the objective manifestation of being alone and the subjective manifestation of experiencing loneliness is
Downloaded by [McGill University Library] at 12:43 20 November 2014

emphasized. The second part of the paper consists of a description of various network interventions followed by an
overview and discussion of loneliness interventions.

Introduction provide som e food for thought regarding interven-


tion initiatives.
F or quite som e time, there has been a growing For obvious reasons, m ost studies re¯ ect the situ-
concern am ong policy m akers as well as am ong ation in the W estern w orld; accordingly the cultural
practitioners that social relations are vital to mental setting will occasionally be referred to in the text.
health and psychological wellbeing. In the report W hile it is quite clear that societies differ to the
`Psychogeriatric care in the com m unity’ , W H O extent that aloneness is viewed as an integral part of
(1979) has listed three levels for prevention of insti- life or one to be avoided at all costs (Bow en, 1954;
tutionalization: prim ary prevention, which m eans to D owns, 1972; Fortune, 1932; Larso n, 1990), m any
be able to rem ain at hom e; secondary prevention, ® ndings can nevertheless be generalized across dif-
w hich means discharge from hospital care as soon as ferent cultures.
possib le; and tertiary prevention, which m eans the
prevention of deterioration in long-term patients.
F our to ® ve actions are recom m ended on each level Conceptual fram ework
in order to reach the respective goals. At all levels,
prevention of social isolation or loneliness is listed as For clarity it is necessary to describe and de® ne the
necessary to w ellbeing. central concepts used throughout the text. Since
There is also a concern w ith the role of interven- there is no consensus of de® nition, the view s of the
tions and their in¯ uence on alleviating de® ciencies m ajority of scholars will be emphasized; som e
in social relations. W ith respect to loneliness, W eiss alternative standpoints w ill also be m entioned.
(1982) suggests that `it is im portant for those who
do research on loneliness to give thought to appli-
cation of their work. Concern for app lication can Social netw orks
help ensure that the research does not becom e ex-
cessively academ ic. The condition we are studying is T he concept social networks has its roots prim arily in
so disturbing that w e surely have som e responsibility anthropology and sociology. W hile conducting re-
to do w hat we can to be helpful to those who search in a ® shing parish in northern N orway,
experience it.’ Barnes (1954) noted the sim ilarity between the ways
O ne purpose of this pap er is to brie¯ y review the the individuals were connected with each other and
scienti® c efforts concerning social networks, with the pattern of a ® shing net. An early de® nition
special reference to w hat has been written about reads: `a speci® c set of linkages am ong a de® ned set
loneliness. The latter is a pertinent restraint, as the of persons, with the additional property that the
concept of networks generally has been used for characteristics of these linkages as a whole m ay be
m any purposes. In the introduction, the broader used to interpret the social behavior of the persons
perspective will be brie¯ y presented. However, the involved’ (M itchell, 1969).
chief purpose is to review the ® eld in order to Once the concept of social networks became es-

C orrespondence to: Lars Andersson, PhD , Stockholm G erontology Research Center, Box 6401, S-113 82 Stockholm,
Sweden. Tel: 46 8 6905807 . Fax: 46 8 33 52 75. E-mail: Lars.Andersson;knv.ki.se
Received for publication 2nd February 1998. Accepted 3rd June 1998.
1360-786 3/98/040264± 11 $9.00 Ó Carfax Publishing Limited
Loneliness research and interventions 265

tablish ed, divergent views of what it constituted and thus contribute to negative outcom es (G esten &
naturally emerged. Jason, 1987; Rook, 1984b; T ilburg, 1985).
The concept used m ost often in mental health Concerning loneliness, m ost scholars agree that
w ork is the ego-centered network, which consists of there is also a positive form of loneliness. It is
one particular individual and everyone that he or she voluntary, tem porary, self-in duced solitude, which
knows or interacts w ith (W einberg & M arlowe, can be constructive when used for creative work
1983). Another term for this is anchorage. Social (From m -R eichm ann, 1959). To com plicate things
netw orks can also be exam ined in terms of: further, social support has often been used in the
negative sense, i.e. it is the lack of support that has
· Size/range/extent. The num ber of people in the been em phasized in studies of social support and
network; wellbeing.
· Density. The degree to which m embers of the Although social support and loneliness m ay seem
network know each other; to be a pair of concepts that can be used concur-
· Reachability/accessibility. T he extent to which one rently, this has actually been m ore the exception
can com m unicate with other mem bers of the net- than the rule and can be traced to the developm en-
work; tal history of the concepts. Social support research
· Directionality. T he extent to which relationships has its source in the social network tradition. Lone-
are reciprocal (Auslander & Litwin, 1987; liness research, on the other hand, has its roots in
Downloaded by [McGill University Library] at 12:43 20 November 2014

M itchell, 1969; W einberg & M arlow e, 1983). research on personal relationships. This m eans that
Although there are other aspects of social networks, research on social support and loneliness has fol-
the above-m entioned are the m ost frequently stud- lowed parallel tracks with m inimal cross-fertilization
ied. as a result.
The individuals w ho are considered to be in-
cluded in a person’ s network are those w ho are seen
on a regular basis. T hey fall into the categories Objective versus subjective concepts. T here is one
relatives, friends, w ork-m ates, colleagues, etc. Ac- fundam ental distinction to be m ade concerning all
cording to Erickson (1984), w ho writes from an the concepts m entioned, that is whether they rep-
Anglo-Saxon perspective, the average social network resent an objective or a subjective phenom enon.
of an adult is usually com posed of 25 to 40 individ- W hile social netw ork alm ost exclusively has been
uals, w hereof three to six are intimates. In compari- regarded as an objective fact, and loneliness as a
son the networks of psychiatric populations are subjective one, the opinion concerning social sup-
signi® cantly sm aller in size. port is divided. Som e researchers have conceptual-
ized social support as an entirely objective
phenomenom , w hile others have conceptualized it
Social support and loneliness as entirely subjectiveÐ i.e. som ething that is per-
ceived by the recipient. In empirical research, it has
D e® nitions and mutual relationship. Tw o concepts been used both ways, even in the sam e study (Cobb,
often used by professio nals in this context are social 1976; Kahn & Antonucci, 1980; Sarason et al.,
support and loneliness. In a general w ay they can be 1983).
seen as opposite notions. Social support is the posi- W hat is valid for social support as well as for
tive pole and loneliness the negative. Social support loneliness w hen regarded as subjective phenom ena,
has been de® ned as `the degree to which a person’ s is the uncertainty which follow s from subjective
basic social needs are grati® ed through interaction m easurem ents. If someone claim s that he or she
w ith others’ (Thoits, 1982), or as `any m aterial, feels lonely, to what extent are w e prepared to
inform ational, or emotional resource that, w hen ex- accept that an observed dysfu nction is asso ciated
changed am ong individuals, is perceived by the re- with loneliness as such? A precautionary measure in
cipient as bene® cial’ (W einberg & M arlowe, 1983). dealing with self reports has been brought up by
Loneliness, on the other hand, has been de® ned as: several authors (Costa & McCrae, 1987; W atson &
`the generalized lack of satisfying personal, social, or Pennebaker, 1989). C osta and M cCrae (1987)
com m unity relationships’ (Andersson, 1993c), or as claim to have identi® ed a single pervasive traitÐ
`an enduring condition of emotional distress that neuroticism Ð that represents a general tendency to
arises w hen a person feels estranged from , m isun- express negative em otionality. W atson and Pen-
derstood, or rejected by others and/or lacks appro- nebaker (1989) report a sim ilar factor w hich they
priate social partners for desired activities, term negative affectivity and assert that it is nearly
particularly activities that provide a sense of social identical to anxiety.
integration and opportunities for emotional inti- It seems realistic to take into account som e sort of
m acy’ (Rook, 1984a). general factor which in¯ uences respondents’ an-
These are the general notions, but there are ex- swers to self-re port m easures. This in¯ uence has
ceptions. O ne exam ple can be seen concerning so- been suggested to be a personality trait (Costa &
cial support, where it has been noted that mem bers M cCrae, 1987). An alternative suggestion is that
of one’ s social network may be a source of problems state anxiety plays a central role. As soon as the
266 Lars A ndersson

T ABLE 1. Com ponent models of social support and loneliness

W eiss (1974) C obb (1979) Kahn (1979) Schaefer et al. (1981) C ohen et al. (1985)

Attachment Em otional support Affect Emotional support


Social integration Network support Belonging support
Reassurance of worth Esteem support Af® rmation Tangible aid Self-esteem support
Reliable alliance M aterial support Aid Information support Tangible support
Guidance Instrumental support Appraisal support
Opportunity for
nurturance Active support*

*Cobb (1979) de® nes active support as re¯ ecting the receipt of care or `mothering’ by the target person, whereas Weiss (1974)
views opportunity for nurturance as re¯ ecting the target person providing care to others. From Cutrona and Russell (1987).

anxiety level is affe cted, it in turn in¯ uences answers loneliness are generally m ore comm on am ong those
to various self-re port m easures. living alone. It should also be pointed out that
feelings of loneliness on the individual level m ight be
M ain effect versus buffering effect. Concerning social m ore or less stable.
support, one further distinction m ust be m ade.
Downloaded by [McGill University Library] at 12:43 20 November 2014

T he use of this concept has centered around life Social versus emotional isolation. The dichotom y
changes or life events. Social support is suggested to `social versus em otional isolation’ is im portant in
act as a buffer against detrim ental effects resulting loneliness research. Em otional isolation can be seen
from stressful situations. T his is termed the buf- as the absence of an attachm ent ® gure in one’ s life,
fering effect of social support, as opposed to the whereas social isolation m ay be regarded as the
m ain effect of social support, which refers to a direct absence of a place in an accepting com m unity
effect on w ellbeing. Explanations offered for m ain (W eiss, 1973). O ne way of exam ining these con-
effects tend to be quite general (Rook, 1984c). It cepts is by using the sam e type of fourfold typology
has been suggested that the netw ork provides the as was used in the case of loneliness/aloneness. It
individual w ith the feedback necessary for self- illustrates that an individual can be socially and
m aintenance and wellbeing and protects against dis- em otionally isolated, socially or emotionally iso-
ordered functioning (Auslander & Litwin, 1987; lated, or neither socially nor emotionally isolated
C assel, 1974). However, the proposed m odel clearly (M ullins et al., 1988). People in the ® rst group
requires further elaboration. More extensive empiri- could bene® t from both social intervention pro-
cal work has been done with the buffering m odel gram s and psych ological therapies, and are m ost in
than with the main m odel. In addition, m ore theor- need of im m ediate social intervention and counsel-
etical efforts have been m ade to interpret it. A ing.
com m only held view is that the com ponents of
social support (as illustrated in Table 1) are m ore or
C om ponents of support and loneliness
less bene® cial to the individual depending on the
particular type of stressful situation that exists. In
T he use of the terms social support or loneliness
practice, this m eans that most results from research
does not m ean that they are seen as single entities
on social support cover a limited part of social
(cf. social versus em otional isolation). O n the con-
relationships, due to the concentration on stress and
trary, there is a general consensus that different
life events in the buffering m odel.
com ponents of support and of loneliness should be
assesse d, because these com ponents seem to have
Loneliness varying effects on outcom es (C ohen & W ills, 1985;
Kessler et al.). Several com ponent m odels have been
T hus, the effects of social support have been tested suggested. Cutrona and Russell (1987) have sum -
in situations where som ething dram atic has occured. m arized som e m ajor m odels in the following m anner
In order to examine effects of enduring isolation, it (Table 1).
is m ore fruitful to approach loneliness research. W eiss’ m odel is the only one that was originally
conceived in the context of loneliness, but it in-
B eing alone versus experiencing loneliness. The rela- cludes the m ajor elements of the conceptualizations
tionship between the objective m anifestation of be- of social support (Cutrona & Russell, 1987). W hat
ing alone and the subjective m anifestation of W eiss terms provisions are social functions that m ay
experiencing loneliness is fundam ental to the under- be obtained from social relationship s. W eiss claim s
standing of the problems discussed in this review. It that all six provisions are necessary in order to avoid
is well established that a person w ho is alone or who loneliness. Attachm ent is emotional closeness from
lives alone m ay either experience loneliness or not. which one derives a sense of security; social inte-
Likew ise, a person feeling lonely may be alone, live gration is a sense of belonging to a group that shares
alone or live with others. Of course, feelings of sim ilar interests and attitudes; reassurance of worth
Loneliness research and interventions 267

is the recognition of one’ s com petence by others; tionally fam ily-oriented cultures of southern Europe
reliable alliance is the assu rance that others can be than in the m ore individualistic cultures of northern
counted upon for tangible assistance; guidance is Europe (Andersso n & SundstroÈ m , 1996; Im am oulu
the assu rance that others can be counted upon for et al., 1993; JylhaÈ & Jokela, 1990).
advice or inform ation; and opportunity for nur-
turance is the sense that others rely upon one for
their wellbeing. Gender
To com plete the overview, it must be m entioned
that loneliness has also been described in term s of Although m ost studies show that wom en report
existential loneliness, m eaninglessness and self- loneliness to a higher extent than m en, som e studies
estrangement (Andersson, 1986; From m -R eich- report no differences (Andersso n, 1982; Peplau et
m ann, 1959). H owever, the latter concepts will not al., 1982; Q ureshi & W alker, 1989). Several inter-
be dealt with here. pretations of the gender difference have been sug-
gested. It has, for exam ple, been proposed that there
is a gender bias in self-d isclosure, i.e. m en are less
E pidem iology of loneliness prone to adm it feelings of loneliness (Peplau et al.,
1982). T here is, however, a confounding factor to
O ne way to identify those who experience loneliness take into consideration.
is to exam ine com m on demograph ic factors, such as
Downloaded by [McGill University Library] at 12:43 20 November 2014

age, gender, m arital status and socio-econom ic


status. It is important to note that irrespective of the M arital status
various distinctions and com ponent models of the
concept, the empirical data has m ostly been based In studies w here m arital status has been exam ined
on single-item m easures of the experience of loneli- together w ith gender, an interesting pattern em erges
ness. (Andersson, 1990; Peplau et al., 1982). Analyzing
the two factors together reveals that the group which
reports loneliness to the highest extent is non-m ar-
A ge ried m ales, followed by non-married fem ales, and in
turn by m arried fem ales, and ® nally, m arried m ales.
A curve depicting the proportions of different age T hus, it follows from these results that the rank
groups reporting loneliness assu m es the character of between the groups should have been different if
a shallow `u’ . T hus, adolescents report loneliness to gender differences in self-d isclosure were the m ajor
a somewhat higher extent than adults and young-old explanation for differences in loneliness betw een
retirees (Andersso n, 1982; 1993c; Peplau et al., m ales and fem ales. In any case, the non-m arried
1982). Although the data is less frequent, it seem s as report loneliness to a higher extent than the m arried
if there is an increase of loneliness in the highest age (Andersson, 1990; Peplau et al., 1982; Townsend,
groupsÐ from approxim ately age 75 and up. It 1973). It is im portant to notice, however, that this
should be noted that even when the proportions are association is not perfect and m any exceptions do
the sam e, the content of the loneliness feeling m ight occur.
differ in age groups (Perlm an & Peplau, 1984).
According to m any studies, at least one person in
four reports loneliness to occur constantly or fairly Socio-econom ic status
often, and a ® gure of app roxim ately 25% lonely is
quite frequent in nationw ide representative sam ples T his has not been a m ajor concern in loneliness
from different countries (Andersso n, 1982; Peplau research. Some studies have reported a relationship
et al., 1982). H ow ever, differences in the form u- between loneliness and socio-econom ic status, i.e.
lation of questions and different cultural heritage loneliness has been found to be m ore prevalent
can m ake direct com pariso ns problematic (HofstaÈ t- am ong lower incom e groups (Perlm an & Peplau,
ter, 1957). 1984).
A Eurobarom eter study w as conducted in 1992/ Apart from risk groups for loneliness identi® ed by
93 in the then 12 countries of the European Union the well-known dem ographic variables, other groups
plus Sweden. Q uestions regarding loneliness were at risk can be identi® ed as well. Exam ples of such
phrased identically in all countries. Respondents groups are: single parents; fam ily caregivers; unem-
w ere aged 60 and above. T he percentage w ho re- ployed; people who have m oved recently (either to a
ported often feeling lonely varied widely between new school, a new job or geographically); people
countries: from 4± 6% in D enm ark and Sweden, who live alone; people who have divorced or w ho
7± 9% in G erm any, the N etherlands and the UK , to have becom e widows/widowers (Perlm an & Peplau,
10± 14% in Belgium , France, Ireland, Luxem bourg 1984).
and Spain, 17% in Italy, 23% in Portugal and 36% It has been suggested that social isolation and
in Greece (Andersso n, 1993a; W alker, 1993). loneliness are asso ciated w ith lowered wellbeing and
The ® gures support earlier studies, w hich have that preventive m easures should be undertaken.
shown that loneliness is m ore frequent in the tradi- W hat support is there for such a position?
268 Lars A ndersson

Social relations and health pression and anxiety. Together, they seem to form a
syndrom e.
T here is an abundance of research which demon- The few longitudinal studies are no absolute
strates links between, on the one hand, factors such guarantee for unproblem atic conclusions. As noted
as social support, social isolation or loneliness and by Kessler and co-workers (1985), spurious asso cia-
on the other hand, various health related factors. tions can also occur in such research designs due to
Some exam ples of health related factors are de- unm easured causes of the relevant variables. It is
pression (Bragg, 1979; H orow itz et al., 1982), anxi- also possible that while social isolation and loneli-
ety (Schultz & Moore, 1984), fatigue (Berg et al., ness m ay be detrimental to health in som e cases,
1981), m ental disorder (Freeman, 1988; Hov- health problem s m ay precipitate social isolation and
aguim ian et al., 1988), backache, headache, dizzi- loneliness in others. In spite of these complications,
ness, palpitations and breathlessness (M iller & a deeper understanding of the (m ental) health ef-
Ingham , 1976), psycho som atic complaints (Stephan fects of loneliness will certainly be reached with an
et al., 1988), neuroticism (Stephan et al., 1988), increased use of longitudinal data in com bination
high consumption of hypnotics and sedatives (Berg with a control for factors such as anxiety and de-
et al., 1981), m ore vigorous decline of im m unoglob- pression.
ulin levels, higher urinary cortisol levels, lower levels
of natural killer cell activity, poorer T -lym phosyte
Interventions
Downloaded by [McGill University Library] at 12:43 20 November 2014

response to phytohemagglutinin (Kiecolt-G laser et


al., 1984a; 1984b), high frequency of seeking m edi-
U nder norm al circum stances, people turn prim arily
cal advice (Berg et al., 1981), alcohol use (Bell,
to fam ily, friends, relatives, neighbors, co-w orkers,
1956; N erviano & G ross, 1976), self-re ported m em-
etc. for help and support; that is, to w hat is com -
ory problem s (Bazargen & Barbre, 1992), suicide
m only referred to as the social network (social sup-
(Diam ant & W indholz, 1981; Trout, 1980),
port netw ork, inform al support system ). If no one in
schizophrenia (Jaco, 1954; Kohn & Clausen, 1955),
this prim ary net of relationships is available som e
and m ortality (Berkm an & Sym e, 1979; House et
individuals seek assistance from professionals such
al., 1982). Recent reviews of the area include C ohen
as physicians, teachers and clergy, as well as from
and McKey (1984), Ell (1984), C ohen and W illis
bartenders and beauticiansÐ at least in som e W est-
(1985), Gottlieb (1985) and Saraso n and Saraso n
ern cultures (Eddy et al., 1970). Although the infor-
(1985), W eeks (1994) and W enger et al., (1996).
m al helpers are the m ain source of support, there is
Thus, the research im pressively docum ents that
no doubt that professionals can play an im portant
social isolation and persistent loneliness present po-
role. However, it can be a role different from that
tential risks for em otional and physical disorders
for which m ost professionals traditionally have been
am ong a range of populations and situations (Ell,
trained (Garbarino, 1983). In the case of severe
1984; Rook, 1984a).
problems, the professional can work with a client in
O ne observation emanating from the factors
a variety of therapeutic m anners. Additionally, the
nam ed above, is that the in¯ uencial factors, w hether
professional can act as a contact between individuals
social isolation or loneliness, are not disease speci® c.
and social networks. Auslander and Litwin (1987)
It should be noted that m ost of the studies have
point out that there is no com m on conception as to
been correlational in nature. It would therefore be
what constitutes a network intervention. Their ow n
hazardous to m ake causal interpretations. A rela-
de® nition is `a planned activity by a professional that
tionship between social isolation and m ortality can
aim s to in¯ uence the functioning of an existing
be spurious, because both of the factors are related
inform al network or to bring about the creation of a
to poverty, for exam ple.
social network where one did not previously exist
Caution is warranted before unquestionably ac-
due to absence or inaccessibility’ . From this point
cepting long lists of correlates to a self-assessed
on, the review will em phasize interventions conduc-
variable. Andersson (1993b), for exam ple, statisti-
ted by professionals rather than by inform al groups.
cally controlled for the effect of anxiety when ana-
One m ay get the im pression that there is an
lyzing loneliness in com bination w ith a num ber of
abundance of interventions in this area. Biegel
health related variables. All of the signi® cant corre-
(1985) has presented an overview of social network
lations vanished with the exception of the corre-
interventions with special reference to the elderly
lation between loneliness and depression.
(Table 2).
Loneliness w as assessed with a single item m easure:
`Does it ever happ en that you feel lonely?’ . T he
question could be answ ered on a four-point scale N etwork interventions
from : `Yes, often’ to `No, never’ . The other vari-
ables included subjective health, psychosom atic Based on conceptualizations by Froland and co-
sym ptom s, health care use, self-esteem, and de- workers (1981), Biegel (1985) has identi® ed seven
pression. In the end there rem ained a hard core of types of network intervention: (1) clinical treatm ent,
variablesÐ the very variables w hich have been con- (2) fam ily caretaker enhancem ent, (3) case m anage-
sistently found connected with loneliness: de- m ent, (4) neighborhood helping, (5) volunteer link-
Loneliness research and interventions 269

T ABLE 2. Social network interventions with the elderly

Type of network Locus Level


intervention Client group Helper type of help of help*

1. Clinical treatment An elderly individual Professional helpers, Non-geographic T, R


family, neighbors,
friends
2. Family caretaker Family mem bers of Family, neighbors, Non-geographic P, T
enhancement: the elderly friends, professional
education and training helpers
Direct services
3. Case m anagement An elderly individual Agency staff and Neighborhood, T
personal network non-geographic
4. Neighborhood helping An elderly individual Natural helpers, Neighborhood P, T, R
or at-large elderly in role-related helpers
the neighborhood (gatekeepers)
P, T, R
5. Volunteer linking An elderly individual Volunteers Neighborhood
community P, T, R
Downloaded by [McGill University Library] at 12:43 20 November 2014

6. Mutual aid/self-help At-large elderly in People with similar Neighborhood,


the neighborhood problems community or
non-geographic P, T, R
7. Comm unity At-large elderly in Lay and professional Neighborhood
em powerment the neighborhood helpers

*P 5 Prevention; T 5 Treatment; R 5 Rehabilitation; from Biegel (1985).

ing, (6) m utual help/self-he lp, and (7) com m unity into those which approach existing self-h elp groups
em powerm ent. and those whose aim is to create arti® cial netw orks.
In clinical treatment, the professional works as a In either case the approach is focused on a group of
catalyst rather than as a therapist, assessing prob- clients, and the professio nal coordinates various ac-
lems through a social systems, rather than an in- tivities for the group. The groups m ay be divided
trapsychic, perspective. T he aim is to assess and into those which m eet in response to a com m on
strengthen existing support system s. An intervention problem, and those w hich m eet to exchange ser-
can consist of involving all signi® cant m em bers of vices. T he relationship is generally collegial.
an individual’ s social network in the speci® c prob- Com munity empowerment aim s at creating groups
lem, as well as the creation of new networks. with collective strength, to gain access to resources
The aim of family caretaker enhancement is to ease or organizations that are supposed to service them.
the burden for fam ily caretakers. The professio nal T he m ethod used here is to identify inform al leaders
can provide assistance to fam ily caretakers through in the com m unity and to facilitate their cooperation.
education and training or by supplying various ser- T he professional can also act in an advisory ca-
vices. pacity.
In case management the professional should func- It is obvious from the descriptions above that
tion as a coordinator of various public and private aloneness or loneliness are generally not the core
services in order to address fragm entation, lack of concepts. Instead, the network interventions are
accessab ility and lack of accountability. m ore social policy oriented. In som e cases they act
The aim of neighborhood helping is to establish as substitutes for de® cient social services. In som e
social support within a geographically de® ned com- cases, they help orient am ong the options for help
m unity. The professional accomplishes this w ith the and care. And in som e other cases, they m ight lead
aid of natural helpers, w ho have been identi® ed as to the form ation of pressure groups to demand im -
pro® cient in this m atter. T hey provide em otional proved conditions.
support as well as problem -centered services. These program s have also been widely used in the
Volunteer linking m eans that the professio nals train m ental health context, among clients w ith speci® c
and supervise inform al helpers who provide support psych iatric diagnoses.
to individuals in need. T he inform al helpers are Although prevention of aloneness and loneliness
recruited through som e speci® c characteristic or ex- can be effected by these program s, it is not their
perience that they have. The relationship is clearly m ain purpose. N evertheless, program s m ay be ini-
one-sidedÐ from the professional, via the informal tiated when the primary purpose is to offer social
helpers, to the helped individual. support. H ow ever, in m ost cases the arrangements
M utual aid/self-help interventions can be divided are far beyond the support sphere.
270 Lars A ndersson

T ABLE 3. Overview of interventions

Goal of Individual Group Environmental


intervention approaches approaches approaches

Facilitate Cognitive Social skills Network building


social bonding behavior therapy training for (intentional and
lonely college students, unintentional)
Client-centered socially isolated
therapies children and dating Restructuring social
anxiety settings (e.g. school
Psychodynamic classroom modi® cation)
therapies Shyness groups
E nhance cop ing Improving Support groups
solitary skills for bereavem ent, divorce
(pleasurable and other social losses
scheduling)
Self-help groups
Prevention Early intervention C ommunity awareness
with high-risk and educational
groups (e.g. children programs
Downloaded by [McGill University Library] at 12:43 20 November 2014

of divorce)
Removing obstacles
to social contact

Adapted from Rook (1984).

Interventions against loneliness the three app roaches above. T hey do not have to be
run by professionals and they do not attempt to
An overview, w hich is more pivotal for the purpose change the lonely person. Rather they provide the
of this paper is put forth by Rook (1984d). T he opportunity to avoid loneliness through com panion-
presentation of the categories in T able 3 is an adap- ship. T hese program s are generally offered to house-
tation of Rook’ s (1984d) disposition. She describes bound individuals.
three general goals of loneliness interventions. First, Among the group approaches, Rook m entions so-
social bonding is facilitated by providing new oppor- cial skills training, shyness groups, depression
tunities for social contact, by providing support in a groups and sem inars for the separated or the be-
transitional period, or by helping lonely people to reaved. T hese program s are typically aim ed at target
relate to others. A second goal is not to alleviate groups.
loneliness itself, but to prevent loneliness from Social skills training has been used with groups
evolving into other serious problem s. This can be such as socially isolated children, students, and ado-
achieved by helping the individual cope with loneli- lescents w ith dating anxiety. M any of the program s
ness. The third goal is to try to prevent loneliness for socially isolated children have been developed
from occuring in the ® rst place. for school settings. T he techniques used with adults
Som e exam ples of individual approaches m en- are basically the sam e as those employed with chil-
tioned include cognitive behavioral therapy, inter- dren/youth. According to Rook, m ost social skills
personal therapies, psychodynam ic therapies and training program s emphasize skills needed to initiate
`friendly visitor’ program s. contact, but do not em phasize those needed to
Cogn itive behavioral therapy aim s at im proving the m aintain contact. Som e characteristics com m on to
lonely individual’ s behavior in social situations. It lonely people and thus subject to training are low
has been shown that lonely people can show greater self-d isclosure (Solano et al., 1982), reluctance to
m istrust and hostility in interactions (Jones, 1982). take social risks (Jones et al., 1981) and high self-fo-
T he therapy is intended to help individuals recog- cus (Jones, 1982).
nize and correct this behavior. Shyness groups have been suggested by Pilkonis
Interpersonal therapies are built on the client± thera- and Zim bardo (1979) in order to teach shy people
pist relationship. In these sessions the clients are to restructure social encounters.
trained to express them selves and in the long run to Depression groups m ight in som e cases be useful for
change their self-perceptions, i.e. the belief that their lonely people due to the close association betw een
real self is unlovable. depression and loneliness which was shown above.
Psychodynamic therapies are also built on the cli- As noted by Rook, self-re inforcem ent training, often
ent± therapist relationship . T he approach is develop- utilized in treatm ent of depression, m ight be useful
m ental and attem pts to identify childhood for lonely people.
experiences which have negatively affe cted adult Seminars for speci® c target groups, such as the sepa-
adjustm ent. rated or the bereaved, naturally differ from the other
Friendly visitor program s are quite different from app roaches. Loneliness that results from a loss
Loneliness research and interventions 271

rather than from enduring circum stances has to be `related to affectional ties’ (Cutrona & Russell,
treated separately, both theoretically and practically. 1987).
Finally, Rook m entions som e exam ples of environ- W ith these conceptualizations as a starting point,
mental approaches, such as netw ork building, restruc- an intervention could either be based on any num -
turing social settings and removing obstacles to ber of the six speci® ed categories, or on two or three
social contact. m ore general concepts. Here it is not relevant to
N etwork building is concerned with creating new debate whether a certain conceptual level is m ore
opportunities for contact w hen the existing opportu- basic than another. The point is rather to illustrate
nities are lim ited. Rook m akes three recom m enda- that the socio-psyc hological factors are interm in-
tions with regard to these interventions. The ® rst is gled, and in practice it is realistic to expect that if
that the interventions should be designed in ways som e com ponents on a particular conceptual level
that distract the lonely individuals from their self- are chosen as core concepts for an intervention,
consciousness. The second recom m endation is that other com ponents on the sam e conceptual level
interventions should be designed to provide a basis m ight be in¯ uenced too, through a com m on factor.
for identi® cation w ith com mon goals. T he third N evertheless, a certain sensitivity is needed to
recom m endation stresses the im portance of label- determ ine the m ost appropriate level of speci® city in
ling: `Labels that emphasize social de® cits are likely conducting interventions. W hile on the one hand, it
to m ake participants feel stigm atized.’ is reasonable to approach som eone who has always
Downloaded by [McGill University Library] at 12:43 20 November 2014

Restructuring social settings involves efforts such as lived alone in a different m anner from som eone w ho
m odifying classro om s in schools or any interior has just lost a spouse, unnecessary specialization
m odi® cation that brings people together. Obviously, m ay, on the other hand, be counterproductive.
the design of comm on space can help facilitate or Another com plication can be illustrated using the
obstruct social contacts. categories `being alone’ and `feeling lonely’ . From
Removing obstacles to social contact concerns activi- the point of view of interventions, the categories
ties such as offering transportation to the elderly or represented by those who neither feel lonely nor are
relief to those caring for chronically disabled fam ily alone, and by those who both feel lonely and are
m em bers. alone, are not particularly troublesom e. But what
As is evident from this overview, effo rts differ about those who are alone but do not feel lonely,
m arkedly. T here are exam ples of both prevention and those w ho feel lonely, despite not being alone?
and treatment; of individual, group level and en- Studies of the relationship between social relations
vironm ental efforts. T he interventions also differ in and health outcom es which distinguish betw een
their ® nancial requirem ents as well as other re- these categories are scarce (W enger et al., 1996).
sources needed for their accom plish m ent. T here are m ore studies which have com pared the
W ith regard to social support and the distinction relationship betw een health related variables (or
between m ain effect and buffering effect, G esten psych ological wellbeing) and quantity versus quality
and Jason (1987) note that evidence for m ain effects of support (Foire et al., 1986; Gottlieb, 1985; Hen-
encourages developm ent of interventions in normal derson et al., 1981). The conclusion from these
populations, whereas evidence for buffering effects studies is that subjective variables are stronger pre-
calls for a risk group approach. As a result, it has dictors of outcom e than are objective variables. O ne
been claim ed that both m odels are lim ited as a basis com plication is that the self-re ported feelings of
for developing interventions (Auslander & Litwin, loneliness or support m ay be part of the syndrom e
1987): the m ain effect m odel is too general; the of negative affectivity. N evertheless, from a preven-
buffering effect model is too restricted. tive perspective, it is of m ajor importance that the
The review of interventions suggests that although loneliness concept is included in discussions of poss-
there are a large num ber to choose am ong, only a ible interventions in the area. In practical arrange-
few have been developed speci® cally to deal with m ents, the objective dim ensions of the network are
different expressions of isolation. com paratively easy to control. If the subjective fac-
The discussion of com ponents of social support tors are ignored, however, crucial interpretations of
and loneliness (Table 1), brings up a fundamental outcom es are deliberately lost.
aspect of interventions, namely the conceptual Thus, it would seem reasonable to offer individu-
level at which interventions should be applied. T he als from the two groups `alone/not lonely’ and
behavioral scientist’ s conceptualizations, to som e `lonely/not alone’ the opportunity to participate in
extent, resem ble the physicist’ s search for the ele- an intervention, particularly if other signs of lowered
m ents of m atter. Once a certain level with a few wellbeing are present. Am ong the `alone/not lonely’
com ponents is reached, new ® ndings lead to the one can ® nd the life-long isolates, som e of w hom are
unveiling of another level with m ore components, quite satis® ed with the state of things. There are also
etc. O ne exam ple of this is W eiss’ (1974) six com- individuals with low ered wellbeing who are alone,
ponents of the single entity loneliness, which have but do not necessarily express their dissatisfaction in
been m erged into two basic categories: assistance- terms of loneliness. Either they m ay not want to
related and non-assista nce-related, with the second reveal their loneliness due to the stigma attached to
category being divided into `self-esteem related’ and it (From m -R eichmann, 1959), or they fail to recog-
272 Lars A ndersson

nize it. Instead they can report som atic problems or B ARN ES , J.A. (1954). C lass and committees in a Norwe-
gian island parish. H um an Relations , 7 , 39± 58.
vague feelings of dissatisfaction (Peplau, 1955;
B AZARG EN , M . & B AR BRE , A.R. (1992). Self-reported
Rook, 1984d). Am ong the `lonely/not alone’ we mem ory problem s among the Black elderly. Education al
® nd, for exam ple, m arried people who have com e to Gerontology , 18 (1), 71± 82.
feel lonely in marriage. Their need m ay often be as B ELL , R.G. (1956). Alcohol and loneliness. Journal of
m uch social as emotional. Socia l Therapy , 2 , 171± 181.
B ENU M , K., A N ST ORP , T., D ALG ARD , O.S. & S Á R ENSEN , T.
Finally, the ® rst step in selecting an intervention is
(1987). Social network stimulation. Health promotion
to specify the particular purpose of the effort and the in a high risk group of middle-aged women. Acta Psy-
particular target group. H ow ever, regardless of the chiatrica Scandina vica , Suppl. 337 (76), 33± 41.
delineations used, the vast majority of interventions, B ERG , S., M ELLSTR OÈ M , D., P ERSSO N , G. & S VAN BO RG , A.
in any category, have not been acceptably evaluated. (1981). Loneliness in the Swedish aged. Journal of
Gerontology , 36 (3), 342± 349.
M ost often the `evaluations’ have consisted of anec-
B ERKM AN , L.F. & S Y M E , S.L. (1979). Social networks,
dotal reports or insuf® ciently designed studies with- host resistance and mortality: a nine-year follow-up
out control group or before± after m easurem ents. It study of Alameda county residents. Am erican Journal of
is therefore dif® cult or im possib le to determine the Epidemiology , 109 , 186± 204.
B IEG EL , D.E. (1985). The application of network theory
scienti® c outcom es of such interventions. In Rook’ s
and research to the ® eld of aging. In: J.W. S AU ER &
report (1984d), her general conclusion was that R.T. C OW AR D (Eds), Socia l support networ ks and the care
`with the exception of social skills approaches, virtu- of the elderly. Theory, research and practice . New York:
Downloaded by [McGill University Library] at 12:43 20 November 2014

ally no publish ed evaluation studies of interventions Springer Publishing Com pany.


developed speci® cally for the problem of de® cient B O W EN , E. (1954). Return to laughter. New York: Harper.
B RAGG , M .E. (1979). A comparative study of loneliness
interpersonal bonds exist.’ T horoughly evaluated in-
and depression. Dissertation Abstracts , 39 , 79: 13710.
terventions which m eet reasonable standards both C A SSEL , J. (1974). Psychosocial processes and stress. In-
w ith regard to feasibility and outcom e are, to a large ternatio nal Journal of Hum an Services , 4 , 471± 482.
extent, still lacking. H owever, som e evaluations of C O BB , S. (1976). Social support as a moderator of life
m ore or less successfu l interventions have been pub- stress. Psych osom atic M edicine , 38 , 300± 314.
C O BB , S. (1979). Social support and health through the
lished (Andersson, 1984; 1985; Benum et al., 1987;
life course. In: M .W. R ILEY (Ed.), Aging from birth to
H eller et al., 1991; Richm ond & Picken, 1986). death: interdisciplinary perspectiv es (pp. 93± 106). Boul-
Irrespective of whether these evaluated interventions der, CO: Westview Press.
are used as m odels or not, it is im portant that those C O H EN , S. & M C K AY , G. (1984). Social support, stress,
w ho engage in interventions against loneliness care- and the buffering hypothesis. A theoretical analysis. In:
A. B AUM , S.E. T AYLO R & J.E. S IN GER (Eds), H andbook
fully evaluate their efforts in order to forward the of psych ology and health . Hillsdale, NY: Erlbaum.
knowledge of how health and wellbeing m ay be C O H EN , S. & W ILLS , T.A. (1985). Stress, social support
effected by alleviating loneliness. and the buffering hypothesis. Psychology Bulletin , 98 ,
310± 357.
C O STA , P.T. & M C C RAE , R.R. (1987). Neuroticism, so-
matic complaints, and disease: is the bark worse than
References
the bite? Journa l of Persona lity , 55 , 229± 316.
C U TRO NA , C .E. & R U SSELL , D.W . (1987). The provisions
A N DER SSO N , L. (1982). Interdisciplinary study of loneli- of social relationships and adaptation to stress. Advances
nessÐ with evaluation of social contacts as a means in Persona l Relation ships , 1 , 37± 67.
towards improving competence in old/age. Acta Socio- D IAM ANT , L. & W IN DH O LZ , G . (1981). Loneliness in col-
logica , 25 , 75± 80. lege students: some therapeutic considerations. Journal
A N DER SSO N , L. (1984). Intervention against loneliness in of C ollege Stud ent Personne l , 22 , 515± 522.
a group of elderly womenÐ a process evaluation. H u- D OW N S , J. (1972). The N avajo . New York, NY: Holt,
m an Relation s , 37 , 295± 310. Rinehart & Winston.
A N DER SSO N , L. (1985). Intervention against loneliness in E D D Y , W.B., P AAP , S.M . & G LAD , D.D. (1970). Solving
a group of elderly womenÐ an impact evaluation. Socia l problems in living: the citizen’ s viewpoint. M ental H y-
Science and M edicine , 20 , 355± 364. giene , 54 (1), 64± 72.
A N DER SSO N , L. (1986). A m odel of estrangementÐ E LL , K. (1984). Social networks, social support, and
including a theoretical understanding of loneliness. Psy- health status: a review. Social Services R eview , 58 (1),
chologica l Reports , 58 , 683± 695. 133± 149.
A N DER SSO N , L. (1990). Om ensamhetens innehaÊ ll och E R ICKSO N , G.D. (1984). A framework and themes for
aÈ ldres upplevelser. Tidskriften AÈ ldrecentrum , 1± 2 , 9± 13. social network intervention. Fam ily Process , 23 (2), 187±
A N DER SSO N , L. (1993a). A È ldre i Sverige och E uropa . AÈ de- 198.
lutvaÈrderingen 93:4. Socialstyrelsen: Stockholm. F IORE , J., C O PPEL , D.B., B ECK ER , J. & C O X , G.B. (1986).
A N DER SSO N , L. (1993b). Loneliness and its relationship Social support as an adjustment. Am erican Journal of
with misery. Psychological Reports , 73 , 584± 586. Com munity Psych ology , 14 , 93± 111.
A N DER SSO N , L. (1993c). Loneliness. In: R. K ASTENB AUM F OR TUN E , R. (1932). Sorcerers of Dobu . New York: Dut-
(Ed.), The encyclop edia of adult developm ent . (pp. 282± ton.
285) Phoenix: The Oryx Press. F REEM AN , H. (1988). Psychosocial effects of social iso-
A N DER SSO N , L. & S UN DSTROÈ M , G. (1996). Social net- lation in the elderly. Danish M edical Bulletin , Special
works of elderly people in Sweden. In: H. L IT W IN (Ed.), Supplement Series, 6 , 13± 23.
The social netw orks of older people: a cross-national analysis F RO LA ND , C., P ANCO AST , D.L., C H APM AN , N.J. & K IM-
(pp. 15± 29). W estport, CT: Praeger Publishing. BO KO , P.J. (1981). H elping netw orks and hum an services .
A U SLAN DER , G.K. & L IT W IN , H. (1987). The parameters Beverly Hills, C A: Sage.
of network intervention: a social work application. So- F RO M M -R EICH M ANN , F. (1959). Loneliness. Psychiatry ,
cial Services Review , 61 (2), 305± 318. 22 (1), 1± 15.
Loneliness research and interventions 273

G A RBARINO , J. (1983) Social support networks: Rx for the childhood to old age. Developm ent Review , 10 , 155± 183.
helping professions. In: K. W H IT TAKER , J. G A RBARINO et M ILLER , P.M. & I N GH AM , J.G. (1976). Friends, con® dants
al. (Eds), Social support netw orks: inform al helping in the and symptoms. Social Psychiatry , 11 , 51± 58.
hum an services . New York: Aldine Publishing Com pany. M IT CHELL , J.C. (1969). The concept and use of social
G ESTEN , E.L. & JASO N , L.A. (1987). Social and community networks. In: J.C. M ITCH ELL (Ed.), Social networks in
interventions. A nnual Review of Psych ology , 38 , 427± 460. urban situa tions . Manchester: M anchester University
G O T TLIEB , B.H. (1985). Social networks and social sup- Press.
port: an overview of research, practice and policy impli- M ULLINS , L.C., J OHN SO N , D.P. & A N D ERSSO N , L. (1988).
cations. Health E ducation Q uarterly , 2 (1), 5± 22. Social and emotional isolation among the elderly: a
H ELLER , K., T H O M PSO N , M.G., T R UEBA , P.E., H O G G , J.R. conceptual view of loneliness. Danish M edical Bulletin ,
& V LACH O S- W EBER , I. (1991). Peer support telephone Special Supplement Series, 6 , 26± 29.
dyads for elderly women: was this the, wrong interven- N ERVIAN O , N.J. & G RO SS , W.F. (1976). Loneliness and
tion? Am erican Journa l of Com m unity Psychology , 19 (1), locus of control for alcoholic m ales: validity against
53± 74. Murray Need and Cattell Trait Dim ensions. Journal of
H EN DERSO N , S., B YRN E , D.G. & D UNCAN -J ON ES , P. Clinica l Psych ology , 32 , 479± 484.
(1981). N eurosis and the socia l env ironm ent . Sydney: P EPLAU , H.E. (1955). Loneliness. Am erican Journal of
Academic Press. Nursing , 55 , 1476± 1481.
H O FSTAÈ TTER , P.R. (1957). G ruppendyna m ik. Kritik der P EPLAU , L.A., B IKSON , I., R O OK , K. & G O O D SH ILD S , J.
M assenpsychologie . Hamburg: Rowohlt. (1982). Being old and living alone. In: L.A. P EPLAU , D.
H O RO W IT Z , M .L., F REN CH , R. D E S. & A ND ERSO N , C.A. P ERLM A N (Eds), Loneline ss: a sourcebook of current theory,
(1982). The prototype of a lonely person. In: L.A. research and therapy . New York: Wiley-Interscience.
P EPLAU & D. P ERLM AN (Eds), Loneliness: a sourceb ook of P ERLM AN , D. & P EPLAU , L.A. (1984). Loneliness research:
Downloaded by [McGill University Library] at 12:43 20 November 2014

current theory, research and therapy . New York: Wiley- a survey of em pirical ® ndings. In: L.A. P EPLAU & S.E.
Interscience. G OLD STO N (Eds), Preventing the harmful consequ ences of
H O USE , J., R O BBINS , C. & M ETZNER , H.L. (1982). The severe and persistent loneliness . (pp. 13± 46). W ashington,
association of social relationships and activities with DC: National Institute of Mental Health (NIM H),
mortality: prospective evidence from the Tecumseh Washington Government Printing Of® ce.
community health study. A merican Journa l of E pidem iol- P ILK ON IS, P.A. & Z IM BARD O , P.G. (1979). The personal
ogy , 116 , 123± 140. and social dynamics of shyness. In: C .E. I ZARD (Ed.),
H O VAGU IMIAN , T., G RA B , B. & S TU CKELBE RG ER , A. (1988). Em otions in persona lity and psych opathology (pp. 133±
Psychosocial problems and the health of the elderly with 160). New York: Plenum Press.
special referen ce to social isolation. Danish M edical Q URESH I , H. & W ALK ER , A. (1989). The caring relationship .
Bulletin , Special Supplement Series, 6 , 2± 7. Elderly people and their families . Basingstoke: M acmillan.
I M AM O ULU , E.O., K UÈ LLER , R., I M AM O ULU , V. & K UÈ LLER , R ICH M ON D , L.T. & P ICK EN , E.D . (1986). A m odel for
M. (1993). The social psychological worlds of Swedes working with lonely clients: Sadler revisited. Psychoth er-
and Turks in and around retirement. Journa l of C ross- apy Patient , 2 (3), 109± 130.
Cultural Psych ology , 24 (1), 26± 41. R O O K , K.S. (1984a). Promoting social bonding. Strategies
J ACO , E.G. (1954). The social isolation hypothesis and for helping the lonely and socially isolated. A merican
schizophrenia. Am erican Sociology Review , 19 , 567± 577. Psych ology , 39 (12), 1389± 1407.
J ON ES , W.H. (1982). Loneliness and social behavior. In: R O O K , K.S. (1984b). The negative side of social interac-
L.A. P EPLAU & D. P ERLM AN (Eds), Loneline ss: a source- tion: impact on psychological well-being. Journal of
book of current theory, research and therapy . New York: Personality and Social Psychology , 46 , 1097± 1108.
Wiley-Interscience. R O O K , K.S. (1984c). Research on social support, loneli-
J ON ES , W.H., F REEM O N , J.E. & G O SW ICK , R.A. (1981). The ness, and social isolation. Toward an integration. In: P.
persistence of loneliness: self and other determ inants. S H AVER (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology ,
Journal of Personality , 49 , 27± 48. volume 5. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
J YLH AÈ , M . & JO KELA , J. (1990). Individual experiences as R O O K , K.S. (1984d ). Interventions for loneliness: a review
culturalÐ a cross-cultural study on loneliness among the and analysis. In: L.A. P EPLAU & S.E. G O LD STO N (Eds),
elderly. Ageing and Society , 10 , 295± 315. Preventin g the harmful consequ ences of severe and persistent
K AH N , R.L. (1979). Aging and social support. In: M.W . loneline ss (pp. 47± 79). W ashington, DC : National Insti-
R ILEY (Ed.), A ging from birth to death: interd isciplinary tute of M ental Health (NIM H), Washington Govern-
perspectives . Boulder, CO: W estview Press. (pp. 77± 91) ment Printing Of® ce.
K AH N , R.L. & A N TO NU CCI , T. (1980). Convoys over the S AR ASO N , I.G., L EVIN E , H.M ., B ASH AM , R.B. & S ARASO N ,
life-course: attachment, roles and social support. In: P.B. B.R. (1983). Assessing social support: the social support
B A LTES & O. B RIM (Eds), Life-span develop m ent and questionnaire. Journal of Persona lity and Social Psy-
behavior , volume 3. Lexington, MA: D C Heath. chology , 44 , 127± 139.
K ESSLER , R.C., P R ICE , R.H. & W OR TM AN , C.B. (1985). S AR ASO N , I.G. & S A RASO N , B.R. (Eds). (1985) Social sup -
Social factors in psychopathology: stress, social support, port: theory, research, and applications . The Hague: Mat-
and coping processes. Annua l Review of Psych ology , 36 , inus Nijhof.
531± 572. S CH AEF ER , C ., C O YN E , J.C . & L AZARUS , R.S. (1981). The
K IECOLT -G LASER J.K., G ARN ER , W., S PEICH ER , C ., P EN N , health-related functions of social support. Journal of
G.M., H O LLIDAY , J. & G LASER , R. (1984a). Psychosocial Beh avioral M edicine , 4 , 381± 406.
modi® ers of immuno-competence in medical students. S CH ULT SZ , N.R., M OORE , D. J R . (1984). Loneliness: corre-
Psych osom atic M edicine 46 , 7± 14. lates, attributions, and coping among older adults. Per-
K IECOLT -G LASER , J.K., G ARN ER , W., S PEICHER , C., P EN N , sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin , 10 (1), 67± 77.
G.M., H O LLID AY , J. & G LASER , R. (1984b). Urinary S O LAN O , C .H., B ATTEN , P.G. & P ARISH , E.A. (1982).
cortisol levels, cellular immunocompetency and loneli- Loneliness and patterns of self-disclosure. Journa l of
ness in psychiatric inpatients. Psych osom atic M edicine , 46 , Personality and Social Psychology , 43 (3), 524± 531.
15± 23. S T EPH AN , E., F AÈ TH , M. & L AM M , H. (1988). Loneliness as
K O H N , M .L. & C LAUSEN , J.A. (1955). Social isolation and related to various personality and environmental mea-
schizophrenia. Am erican Sociology Review , 20 , 265± 273. sures: research with the German adaptation of the
L ARSSO N , R.W. (1990). The solitary side of life: an UCLA loneliness scale. Society Behaviour Persona lity ,
examination of the time people spend alone from 16 (2), 169± 174.
274 Lars A ndersson

T H OITS , P.A. (1982). Conceptual, methodological and W EEK S , D.J. (1994). A review of loneliness concepts, with
theoretical problems in studying social support as a particular referen ce to old age. Interna tional Journa l of
buffer against life stress. Journa l of H ealth and Socia l Geriatric Psych iatry , 9 , 345± 355.
Behavior , 23 , 145± 159. W EIN BERG , R.B. & M ARLO W E , H.A. JR . (1983). Recogniz-
T ILBU RG , T. VAN . (1985). Supportive relationships and ing the social in psychosocial competence: the import-
loneliness: suggestions for the improvement of support ance of social network interventions. Psych osocial
networks as guidelines for research and policy. In: J.A. Rehabilitation Journa l , 6 (4), 25± 34.
Y OD ER (Ed.), Support netw orks in a caring com munity. W EISS , R. (1973). Lonelines s: the experience of em otiona l and
Research and policy, fact and ® ction (pp. 215± 225) Dor- socia l isolation . Cambridge, MA: The M IT Press.
drecht: M artinus Nijhoff Publishers. W EISS , R. (1974). The provisions of social relationships.
T O W NSEND , P. (1973). Isolation and loneliness in the In: Z. R UBIN (Ed.), Doing unto others . (pp. 17± 26). En-
aged. In: R. W EISS Loneline ss: the experience of em otiona l glewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
and social isolation . Cambridge, M A: The M IT Press. W EN GER , G.C., D AVIES , R., S H AH T AH M ASEBI, S. & S CO TT ,
T RO UT , D.L . (1980). The role of social isolation in sui- A. (1996). Social isolation and loneliness in old age:
cide. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior , 10 , 10± 23. review and m odel re® nement. Ageing and Society , 16 ,
W ALKER , A. (1993). Age and attitud es. M ain results from a 333± 358.
Eurobarom eter survey . Commission of the European W HO (1979). Psychogeriatric care in the com m unity . Public
Comm unities. Health in Europe 10. Copenhagen: W HO Regional
W ATSO N , D. & P ENN EBAK ER , J.W. (1989). Health com- Of® ce for Europe.
plaints, stress and distress: exploring the central role of
negative affectivity. Psychology Review , 96 (2), 234± 254.
Downloaded by [McGill University Library] at 12:43 20 November 2014

You might also like