You are on page 1of 10

Are Infant-Toddler Social-Emotional and Behavioral

Problems Transient?
MARGARET J. BRIGGS-GOWAN, PH.D., ALICE S. CARTER, PH.D.,
JOAN BOSSON-HEENAN, B.A., AMANDA E. GUYER, PH.D., AND SARAH M. HORWITZ, PH.D.

ABSTRACT
Objective: To examine the persistence of parent-reported social-emotional and behavioral problems in infants and toddlers.
Method: The sample comprised 1,082 children ascertained from birth records. Children were 12 to 40 months old in year 1
(1998 Y 1999) and 23 to 48 months old in year 2 (1999 Y 2000). Eighty percent participated in year 1 and 91% were retained in
year 2. Social-emotional and behavioral problems were measured by high scores (Q90th percentile) on the Internalizing,
Externalizing, and/or Dysregulation domains of the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA). Parents
reported on sociodemographic factors, family life impairment, parenting stress, and family functioning. Results: Among
children with any high ITSEA domain score in year 1, 49.9% had persistent psychopathology, as indicated by the continued
presence of a high score in year 2. In multivariate analyses, persistence was significantly more likely when parents reported
co-occurring problems (i.e., problems in multiple ITSEA domains), high family life disruption, and high parenting distress in
year 1. Homotypic persistence rates (i.e., same domain persistence) ranged from 38% to 50%. Only for dysregulation was
homotypic persistence greater when co-occurring problems were present than for dysregulation alone. Persistence patterns
were similar for boys and girls. Conclusion: Findings indicate that infant-toddler social-emotional/behavioral problems are
not transient and highlight the need for early identification, multidomain and family assessment, and effective early
intervention. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 2006;45(7):849 Y 858. Key Words: infant-toddler, persistence, social-
emotional, behavioral problems.

Despite increasing consensus that psychopathology significance and to the development of appropriate
exists in infancy and toddlerhood (National Center intervention services for children with significant
for Toddlers and Families, 1994; Zeanah, 2000), psychopathology. Early childhood (birth to 3 years) is
relatively little is known about the course and characterized by rapid developmental change and
persistence of early-emerging social-emotional and consequently many parents and professionals believe
behavioral problems. An understanding of the extent that early social-emotional and behavioral problems are
to which these early-emerging problems persist over developmentally transient (e.g., Bthe terrible twos[) and
time is essential to our understanding of their clinical likely to diminish as children grow older. This view
conflicts with a small but growing body of evidence that
Accepted January 31, 2006. some early-emerging social-emotional and behavioral
Dr. Briggs-Gowan and Ms. Bosson-Heenan are with the Department of
problems persist (Fischer et al., 1984; Keenan and
Psychiatry, University of Connecticut Health Center, Farmington CT; Dr.
Carter is with the Psychology Department, University of Massachusetts, Boston; Wakschlag, 2000; Lavigne et al., 1998; Mathiesen and
Dr. Horwitz is with Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland; Dr. Guyer is Sanson, 2000) and may be a barrier preventing children
with the National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, MD. from receiving needed intervention services. In this
This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health grant
R01MH55278. article, patterns of persistence of psychopathology,
Correspondence to Dr. Margaret J. Briggs-Gowan, Department of Psychiatry, measured by extreme social-emotional/behavioral prob-
University of Connecticut Health Center, 263 Farmington Avenue, Farmington, lems, and factors related to persistence are examined in
CT, 06030; e-mail: Briggsgowan@psychiatry.uchc.edu.
0890-8567/06/4507 Y 0849Ó2006 by the American Academy of Child
a representative community sample of young children.
and Adolescent Psychiatry. Psychopathology in young children is often concep-
DOI:10.1097/01.chi.0000220849.48650.59 tualized as falling along the three broad domains of

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:7, JULY 2006 849

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
BRIGGS-GOWAN ET AL.

internalizing, externalizing, and dysregulation (National Mathiesen and Sanson, 2000), Bat-risk[ samples (Rose
Center for Toddlers and Families, 1994; Zeanah, 2000). et al., 1989; Shaw et al., 1998), and samples enriched for
Information about clusters of social-emotional/behavioral psychopathology by overselection of children with high
problems can be assessed in each domain and examined scores on symptom checklists (Lavigne et al., 1998).
dimensionally or by assigning cutpoints. Scores above the Despite methodological differences, these studies have
cutpoints reflect the presence of multiple behaviors within yielded fairly consistent evidence that parental reports of
a given domain, which is used to indicate psychopa- infant-toddler internalizing and externalizing problems
thology. Psychopathology also may be assessed using psy- correlate with later social-emotional/behavioral problems
chiatric classification systems. However, young children (Briggs-Gowan and Carter, 1998; Fischer et al., 1984;
also may evidence social-emotional/behavioral problems Mathiesen and Sanson, 2000; Mesman et al., 2001; Rose
in the internalizing, externalizing, and/or dysregulation et al., 1989; Shaw et al., 1998; Smith et al., 2004). Some
domains that do not occur so frequently or intensely as to of these studies have suggested that stability may be more
be considered clinically significant psychopathology consistent and stronger for externalizing problems than
(Carter and Briggs-Gowan, 2005). For the purpose of for internalizing problems (Briggs-Gowan and Carter,
clarity in this article, psychopathology is used to refer to 1998; Mesman and Koot, 2001l Mesman et al., 2001).
psychiatric disorders or high levels of social-emotional/ Many of these studies have focused on homotypic
behavioral problems. Social-emotional/behavioral prob- stability, that is, the stability of behaviors within the
lems refer to dimensionally measured problem behaviors same domain over time. Within early childhood, several
that include both normal and atypical ranges of behavior. studies suggest low to moderate homotypic stability in
In early childhood, internalizing problems include early internalizing problems, with correlations from 0.23
difficulties with anxiety, depression/withdrawal, fears, to 0.52 (Briggs-Gowan and Carter, 1998; Fischer et al.,
and shyness/inhibition. Externalizing problems include 1984; Mathiesen and Sanson, 2000). Early childhood
aggression, overactivity, impulsivity, and inattention. internalizing problems also have been linked with
Recently, extreme problems in the regulation of state, school-age internalizing problems; however, results
affect, and sensory processing have been addressed in the have been inconsistent, with one study indicating greater
regulatory disorders of the Diagnostic Classification stability among boys (Mesman et al., 2001) and another
System for 0Y3 (National Center for Toddlers and greater stability among girls (Fischer et al., 1984).
Families, 1994). Although conceptualizing extreme When examined dimensionally, externalizing prob-
regulatory problems as a form of psychopathology is lems demonstrate moderate to strong stability, with
controversial, there is considerable evidence that children longitudinal correlations from 0.31 to 0.70 (Briggs-
with difficult temperament are vulnerable to developing Gowan and Carter, 1998; Mathiesen and Sanson, 2000;
social-emotional/behavioral problems and psychiatric Mesman et al., 2001; Rose et al., 1989; Shaw et al.,
disorders (Bates et al., 1985; Guerin et al., 1997; Keenan 1998; Smith et al., 2004). Notably, toddler externalizing
et al., 1998; Prior et al., 1993; Shaw et al., 1996). This behaviors have significantly predicted externalizing
suggests that difficulties with regulation may play a role disorders at age 5 (Keenan et al., 1998). Although two
in the emergence or maintenance of psychopathology. studies found no sex differences in the stability of
Therefore, it is important to examine the extent to which externalizing problems (Fischer et al., 1984; Smith et al.,
extreme dysregulation persists and/or contributes to the 2004), one study reported stronger longitudinal path-
persistence of psychopathology in other areas. ways for boys than girls (Mesman et al., 2001).

Stability in Social-Emotional/Behavioral Problems Categorical Persistence


Most studies that have followed children beginning in Although the stability of early psychopathology is more
early childhood have employed dimensional measures to relevant to decisions about clinical intervention than the
assess aspects of internalizing and externalizing problems stability of dimensional reports of social-emotional/
and/or Bdifficult temperament.[ Longitudinal studies behavioral problems, few studies have examined the
that have examined the stability of social-emotional/ persistence of psychopathology defined using symptom
behavioral problems have varied considerably in sample cutpoints or diagnostic classifications. Mathiesen and
type, including community samples (Fischer et al., 1984; Sanson (2000) found that 37% of 18-month-olds with

850 J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:7, JULY 2006

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
ARE INFANT-TODDLER PROBLEMS TRANSIENT?

moderately high emotional/behavioral problems con- systems, such as the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric
tinued to have problems in the same area at 30 months. Association, 1994), addressing the role of impairment in
Similarly, in the study of Lavigne et al. (1998), ap- persistence is clearly important.
proximately half of 2-year-olds who met criteria for a Although some studies have sought to identify risk
psychiatric disorder continued to meet criteria for a dis- factors for persistent psychopathology, relatively few
order 1 year later. Furthermore, toddlers extreme in be- factors have been identified. Mathiesen and Sanson
havioral inhibition have been found to be at increased risk (2000) found that although many factors were associ-
of later internalizing disorders (Biederman et al., 2001; ated with the onset of toddlers_ psychopathology, none
Schwartz et al., 1999). Most of these studies do not appear of a comprehensive array of risk factors, including
to have examined sex differences in persistence rates. sociodemographic factors, maternal symptomatology,
However, Lavigne and colleagues (1998) reported that social support, and life events, distinguished children
internalizing disorders were more persistent among 2- and with stable versus remitting psychopathology. In
3-year-old boys (50%) than among girls (29%), and that contrast, Mesman and Koot (2001) found that stressful
there was no sex difference in the persistence of life events and physical health problems were associated
externalizing disorders. Information concerning the het- with risk of later disorder. Finally, Lavigne and
erotypic continuity of early social-emotional/behavioral colleagues (1998) found that low family cohesiveness
problems is limited. Although some studies have suggested was associated with persistence of early externalizing
that internalizing problems may decrease the risk of disorders, but that negative maternal affect and child
developing externalizing problems (Mesman et al., 2001; cognitive level were not associated with persistence of
Schwartz et al., 1999), others have found no link between either internalizing or externalizing disorders.
problems in one domain and risk of developing problems An understanding of the persistence, or course, of
in another (Lavigne et al., 1998; Mesman and Koot, psychopathology in young children is critical to
2001). A high level of co-occurrence across domains, improving identification and intervention efforts in
noted by Lavigne and colleagues (1998), may underlie early childhood. Identifying those children whose
these conflicting results. difficulties are particularly intransigent would aid
decision making regarding eligibility for targeted
prevention and intervention programs. In this work,
Contributing Factors the persistence of high levels of parent-reported
Some studies have indicated greater persistence internalizing, externalizing, and dysregulation problems
among children with higher levels of social-emotional/ is examined in a sociodemographically diverse, repre-
behavioral problems (Lavigne et al., 1998; Mathiesen sentative, community sample of children first studied at
and Sanson, 2000; Prior et al., 1992). This is not approximately 1 to 3 years of age. Patterns of overall
surprising because the greater the number or severity of persistence, homotypic persistence, and heterotypic
problems, the greater the behavioral change needed for persistence are examined, with attention paid to the role
difficulties to remit. Persistence also may be greater among that having problems in multiple or co-occurring areas
children whose difficulties interfere with their ability to may have on persistence. In addition, factors that may
engage in age-appropriate activities and/or negatively affect contribute to persistence are examined, including child
social relationships (i.e., impairment). Restricted exposure sex, child age, problem severity, disruptions in family
to developmentally appropriate situations and activities routine attributed to child behavior, sociodemographic
may constrain a child_s opportunities to learn to master risk, and potential risks and stressors within the family.
challenging tasks, such as learning to control aggressive
impulses when frustrated by peer interactions. Such
associations between impairment and persistence have METHOD
been identified in older children (Costello et al., 1999).
Participants
However, Lavigne and colleagues (1998) found that in
young children, persistence of externalizing disorders was The sample studied is an age- and sex-stratified healthy birth
cohort randomly selected from birth records at the State of
not related to a global measure of impairment. Given the Connecticut Department of Public Health for children born in
increased focus on impairment in current diagnostic a Standard Metropolitan Statistical Area of the 1990 Census

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:7, JULY 2006 851

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
BRIGGS-GOWAN ET AL.

(Briggs-Gowan et al., 2001). Children likely to have developmental employed (Carter and Briggs-Gowan, 2005; Carter et al., 2003). The
delays caused by prematurity, low birth weight, or birth complica- Internalizing domain measures general anxiety, depression/withdraw-
tions were excluded, as were parents unable to participate in English al, inhibition to novelty/shyness, and separation distress. The
and families that had moved out of the state. Of an eligible sample Externalizing domain measures aggression/defiance, peer aggression,
of 1,605, 1,280 families participated (80% response rate) in year 1. and activity/impulsivity. The Dysregulation domain measures
These participants were sociodemographically similar to families problems with regulation of state, affect, and sensory processes across
living in the region (Briggs-Gowan et al., 2001). Of year 1 the areas of negative emotionality, eating, sleep, and sensory
participants, 1,169 (91.3%) participated in the year 2 survey. sensitivities. Items are rated on a 3-point scale from not true/rarely
Seventeen children were subsequently deemed ineligible because of to very true/often. Across several studies, the ITSEA has demonstrated
significant developmental delays identified during the study (e.g., acceptable internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and validity
autism spectrum disorders). relative to other parent report checklists and independent behavioral
Longitudinal analyses were restricted to children with complete observations (Carter and Briggs-Gowan, 2005). Domain cutpoints
ITSEA data, the same respondent over time, and a minimum are set at the 90th percentile. A Total ITSEA Problems T score was
interval of 6 months between the year 1 and year 2 surveys. Most of calculated as the sum of the three domain scores standardized within
the analyzed sample (89%) had a 9- to 15-month interval (mean 6-month age bands and sex.
12.3 months; SD = 1.8 months); 5.5% had 6- to 8-month intervals, Family Life Impairment Scale (FLIS). The 21-item FLIS assesses
and 5.8% had 15- to 22-month intervals. The final sample the extent to which the parent views the young child_s behavior,
included in analyses (N = 1,082) was similar to the sample (n = 178) personality, or special needs as limiting the child_s participation in
not available for analyses (because of nonparticipation, respondent activities that are typical for families with young children (e.g., visiting
change, incomplete data, or time interval restrictions) with respect family, dining at family restaurants, going grocery shopping) or as
to child sex, marital status, and poverty status, but differed negatively affecting the parent (e.g., by placing restrictions on the
significantly (p G .05) on year 1 child age in months (mean 24.2, parent_s socialization with friends or intimate time with her partner).
SD = 7.0 versus mean 28.1, SD = 7.5, respectively), minority The FLIS was developed for this study as a brief measure that would be
ethnicity (36% versus 61%), and respondent educational attain- appropriate for a normative sample of children, most of whom would
ment of high school degree or less (27% versus 41%). Weights were not have disabilities or chronic health problems.
employed in all analyses to adjust for potential biases in the retained Worry About Child. Parents rated their level of worry about their
sample. Weights accounted for unequal selection probabilities, child_s behavior, social development, and emotional development
differential nonresponse in the initial study, and differential on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all worried to 5 = extremely worried).
attrition. Information from birth records concerning sociodemo- Parenting Stress Index Short Form. The Parent Distress (PD) and
graphic background (e.g., parental age, education and race) and Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction (PCDI) scales were
birth status (e.g., birth weight and gestational age) were used to employed (Abidin, 1990). PD addresses general stress and
adjust for differential nonresponse and attrition in calculating final dissatisfaction in the parenting role. PCDI measures whether the
sampling weights. Weighting resulted in nonwhole integers for cell child meets parental expectations and reinforces the parent. These
counts; these were rounded in the results to aid readability. scales have acceptable reliability and validity. Clinical cutpoints
The majority of children ($97%) were within the target child age available from the author (Abidin) were employed.
ranges of 12 to 35 months in year 1 and 24 to 47 months in year 2. Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Inventory. The 20-
The remaining children were slightly outside these age ranges item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Inventory self-
because of variability in the length of time needed to locate subjects report checklist assesses adult depressive symptoms. It has
and obtain participation. In year 1, 547 were 12 to 23 months, 508 demonstrated high internal consistency (coefficient ! from .84 to
.90) and modest test-retest reliability (r = 0.51 to 0.67; Radloff,
were 24 to 35 months, and 27 were 36 to 40 months. In year 2, 12
1977). A clinical cutpoint of 16 was employed.
children were 23 months, 532 were 24 to 35 months, 513 were 36
Beck Anxiety Inventory. This self-report measure consists of
to 47 months, and 25 were 48 months.
statements that describe common symptoms of anxiety (Beck et al.,
Boys and girls were equally represented (49.7% boys). Children
1988). Symptoms are rated on a 4-point scale from Bnot at all
were ethnically diverse (64.1% white, 18.7% black, 4.7% Hispanic,
bothered[ to Bseverely bothered.[ Psychometric properties are
9.2% multiethnic minority, 2.0% Asian, and 1.3% other ethnicity).
adequate. A clinical cutpoint of 16 was employed.
Most respondents were mothers (97.7%) and married/cohabiting
Family Environment Scale (FES). The FES Expressiveness and
with a partner (79.0%). Approximately 27% of respondents had
Conflict subscales were employed (Moos and Moos, 1983). These
completed a high school education or less, 32% had some education
subscales have acceptable reliability and validity in measuring
beyond high school, and 41% had a college degree or more. Median
disruptions in family functioning. Cutpoints indicating Conflict
annual income was $50,000, with 19.3% of families living on
scores in the upper 80th percentile and Expressiveness scores in the
incomes below the poverty line and 16.0% living in borderline lowest 20th percentile were employed.
poverty (i.e., below 185% of poverty).
Life Events. The child stressful life events measure, developed as a
companion to the ITSEA, comprises 14 items that children may
Measures have experienced in their lifetime (e.g., hospitalization, injury,
separation from parent, violence). The parent life events comprises
Sociodemographic Variables. Parents answered questions about 38 items selected from the Life Events Inventory (Cochrane and
sociodemographic factors, such as child sex, age, ethnicity, parental Robertson, 1973), based on having the highest severity weights and
education, marital status, and household income. greatest applicability to young families.
Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA). The Social support was measured with the Tangible and Emotional
Internalizing, Externalizing, and Dysregulation domains of the Informational Support measures of the Medical Outcomes Study
ITSEA, a 166-item parent report measure of social-emotional/ (Sherbourne and Stewart, 1991). The scales comprise 12 items and
behavioral problems and competencies in 12- to 48-month-olds, were have demonstrated adequate psychometric properties.

852 J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:7, JULY 2006

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
ARE INFANT-TODDLER PROBLEMS TRANSIENT?

Procedure Dysregulation, 39.7% versus 37.7%, respectively (p > .05).


Parents were invited to participate via mail and telephone. In addition, there were no significant difference between
Most parents answered the questions as self-report question- younger and older children in rates of homotypic
naires. Less than 5% of parents were read questions verbatim by persistence of Internalizing (35.8% versus 39.9%),
research staff. Informed consent procedures were followed. All of
the procedures were approved by the institutional human subjects Externalizing (55.7% versus 43.4%), or Dysregulation
review board. Parents who declined participation were not (38.8% versus 38.7%) problems.
contacted further. Parents received $25 for participating. Similar Heterotypic persistence was defined as the presence of
procedures were used in both years.
problem(s) in different areas over time. Of the 110
children with persistent problems, only 11.8% (n = 13)
RESULTS
had problems solely in a new area in year 2. All shifts
were from one pure group to another pure group and
Initial Problems
most (12/13) involved Dysregulation (4 Dysregulation
In the overall sample, 20.3% of children were reported to Internalizing, 3 Dysregulation to Externalizing, 5
to have any ITSEA problem, defined as one or more Externalizing to Dysregulation, and 1 Internalizing to
Internalizing, Externalizing, and/or Dysregulation score Externalizing).
above the 90th percentile based on the sex of the child Rates of Persistence in Pure and Co-occurring Groupings.
and 6-month age bands (Table 1). Approximately 14.2% As a first step in examining the possible influence of co-
of children had a single high domain score (i.e., Bpure[ occurring problems (present in 30.1% of children with
problems) and 6.1% had multiple high domain scores Any Problem), mutually exclusive groups were formed
(i.e., Bco-occurring[ problems in two or three do-
mains). Notably, 30.1% of children with Any Problem
TABLE 1
had co-occurring problems. Rates of problems were Rates of Initial Problems and Overall Persistence
similar for boys and girls across all pure and co- (Weighted) (N = 1,082)
occurring groups. In year 2, similar rates of problems Any ITSEA
were reported across these groupings. Year 1 Problem Year
ITSEA 2 (Overall
Persistence Problem(s) Persistence)
NY2
Overall Problem Persistence. The rate of overall Year 1 ITSEA Domains NY1(%) (% NY2/NY1)
problem persistence, defined as the presence of Any
Any internalizing problem 105 (9.7) 55 (52.0)
ITSEA Problem in year 2 among children with any
Any externalizing problem 101 (9.4) 64 (63.3)
ITSEA problem year 1, was 49.9% (Table 1) and Any dysregulation problem 105 (9.7) 61 (58.0)
was similar for boys (47.6%) and girls (52.6%). Simi- Any problem (Internalizing, 220 (20.3) 110 (49.9)
lar rates of persistence were obtained among younger Externalizing, and/or
children (12Y23 months: 54.5%) and older children Dysregulation domain)
(24Y40 months: 45.3%), # 2 = 1.62, not significant. No problem 862 (79.7) 120 (14.0)
Mutually exclusive groupings
Although not a focus of this article, the incidence of Internalizing only 57 (5.3) 19 (33.5)A
any ITSEA problem in year 2 among children with no Externalizing only 51 (4.7) 26 (51.1)A,B
year 1 problem was 14.0%. Dysregulation only 45 (4.2) 15 (33.6)A
Homotypic and Heterotypic Persistence. Homotypic Internalizing and 7 (0.6) 4 (55.2)A,C
persistence was defined as the presence of a high score in Externalizing
Internalizing and 15 (1.4) 11 (73.2)B,C
the same domain over time, regardless of the presence of
Dysregulation
co-occurring problems in other domain(s). Homotypic Externalizing and 18 (1.7) 14 (78.2)B,C
persistence rates were 37.8% for the Internalizing domain, Dysregulation
49.9% for the Externalizing domain, and 38.7% for the Internalizing, 26 (2.4) 21 (79.2)C
Dysregulation domain, with no significant differences in Externalizing, and Dysregulation
rates across the domains. Rates of homotypic persistence Note: Cells that do not share a common superscript letter differ
were comparable for boys and girls: Internalizing, 30.4% significantly, p G .01. ITSEA = Infant-Toddler Social and
versus 46.1%; Externalizing, 47.4% versus 52.7%; and Emotional Assessment; Y1 = year 1; Y2 = year 2.

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:7, JULY 2006 853

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
BRIGGS-GOWAN ET AL.

based on the combination of problems in year 1 (Table 1). occurring group than in the pure group, with increased
Across the pure groups, overall persistence rates ranged RR ranging from approximately 2.9 for Externalizing to
from 33.5% for Internalizing only to 51.1% for 6.7 for Dysregulation. However, the impact of co-
Externalizing only, with no significant differences in occurring problems on homotypic persistence varied
rates across the pure groups. In the co-occurring groups, with domain. For Dysregulation, homotypic persis-
persistence rates ranged from 55.2% for Internalizing/ tence was approximately 4.5 times more likely in the
Externalizing to 79.2% for Internalizing/Externalizing/ co-occurring group (53.5%) than in the pure group
Dysregulation, with no significant differences in persis- (20.1%). Yet, co-occurring problems had no signifi-
tence across the co-occurring groups. The co-occurring cant effect on the homotypic persistence of Inter-
groups tended to be significantly more persistent than the nalizing or Externalizing problems.
pure groups. However, Externalizing only and Internal- Severity. Because the impact of co-occurrence on
izing/Externalizing were exceptions to this pattern: the persistence may be related to overall problem severity,
Externalizing only group differed only from the Internal- an analysis of variance (ANOVA) was examined in
izing/Externalizing/Dysregulation group and the Internal- which the Total ITSEA Problems T score was the
izing/Externalizing group did not differ from any other dependent variable and overall persistence status and
group. When children with co-occurring problems were child sex were independent variables. The overall model
compared with children with pure problems, those was significant, F3,216 = 9.10, p G .0001, as was the
with co-occurring problems were approximately 4.7 effect of persistence status, F1,216 = 26.84, p G .0001,
times more likely to have Any Problem in year 2, #2 = with higher Total Problems in the persistent group
22.09, p G .0001, relative risk ratio (RR) = 4.65, 95% (mean = 66.83, SD = 8.91) than in the remitting group
confidence interval (CI) = 2.43Y8.86. (mean = 61.07, SD = 7.61). There was no significant
Bivariate analyses were employed to examine whether, sex effect (F1,216 = 0.01) nor was there a significant
for each domain separately, the presence of co-occurring interaction between sex and persistence status (F1,216 =
problems significantly increased the likelihood of (1) 0.88; data not shown).
overall problem persistence and (2) homotypic persis- Factors Related to Overall Problem Persistence. As a
tence within that domain (Table 2). For all domains, first step in identifying sociodemographic and parent/
overall persistence was significantly more likely in the co- family functioning factors associated with increased risk

TABLE 2
Effect of Co-occurrence on Rates of Overall and Homotypic Persistence Among Children With Any Problem, Year 1
Year 2 ITSEA Status
Overall Problem Homotypic
Persistence NY2 Persistence NY2
Year 1 ITSEA Status NY1 (%)a (%)a
Internalizing
Pure Internalizing 57 19 (33.5) 18 (31.9)
Co-occurring Internalizing 48 35 (74.0) 21 (44.9)
Relative risk (95% CI) 5.63 (2.42 Y 13.17) 1.74 (0.78 Y 3.85)
Externalizing
Pure Externalizing 51 26 (51.1) 21 (41.4)
Co-occurring Externalizing 50 38 (75.7) 29 (58.4)
Relative risk (95% CI) 2.98 (1.28 Y 6.94) 1.99 (0.90 Y 4.38)
Dysregulation
Pure Dysregulation 45 15 (33.6) 9 (20.1)
Co-occurring Dysregulation 59 46 (77.3) 32 (53.5)
Relative risk (95% CI) 6.74 (2.83 Y 16.04) 4.57 (1.88 Y 11.12)
Note: Relative risks shown in bold are statistically significant (i.e., confidence intervals exclude 1.0). ITSEA =
Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment; Y1 = year 1; Y2 = year 2 ; CI = confidence interval.
a
% = NY1/NY2.

854 J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:7, JULY 2006

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
ARE INFANT-TODDLER PROBLEMS TRANSIENT?

TABLE 3
Bivariate Analysis of Factors Related to Persistence Among Children With Any Problem in Year 1
Factor Remit NFactor/NRemit(%) Persist (Any Problem) NFactor/NPersist(%)
Child factors
Sex (male) 58/103 (56.2) 50/100 (50.4)
Age group (24 Y 40 mo) 58/103 (56.8) 46/100 (46.1)
Co-occurring problems 15/103 (14.8) 43/100 (43.5)y
Sociodemographics
Resp. educ. e high school 36/103 (34.7) 38/109 (38.3)
Poor/borderline poor 44/103 (43.2) 52/99 (53.1)
Single parent 31/103 (30.5) 36/100 (35.6)
Minority ethnicity 48/103 (46.8) 55/100 (55.5)
Parenting stress
Parent worry about child 36/103 (34.5) 45/100 (44.7)
Family life impairment 43/103 (41.4) 67/100 (66.9)***
PSI/parenting distress 7/103 (6.7) 26/100 (25.6)***
PSI/P-C dysfunction 7/103 (6.6) 15/100 (15.1)*
Parent/family factors
Depressive symptoms 24/103 (23.5) 36/100 (36.2)*
Anxiety symptoms 3/103 (3.0) 14/100 (14.4)**
FES expressiveness 20/100 (20.2) 14/99 (13.8)
FES conflict 17/97 (17.2) 22/99 (22.0)
Parent stressful events 9/103 (8.8) 14/100 (14.5)
Child stressful events 8/103 (7.6) 11/100 (10.5)
Low social support 22/101 (21.5) 27/98 (28.1)
Note: Sample size reduced due to elimination of 17 subjects with missing covariate data.
Significance evaluated with continuity adjusted #2. Resp. educ. e high school = respondent
educational attainment of less than or equal to a high school degree; PSI = Parenting Stress Index; PSI/
P-C = Parent-Child Dysfunctional Interaction; FES = Family Environment Scale.
* p G .10; ** p G .01; *** p G .001; y p G .0001.

of overall persistence (persist versus remit), bivariate # 2 in which Total ITSEA Problems and Co-occurring
tests were employed (Table 3). Persistence was signifi- Problems were entered in the first model step indicated
cantly more likely in the presence of high year 1 comparable model fit (likelihood ratio = 40.82, p G .0001)
parental anxiety, parenting distress, and FLIS disrup-
tion in family life than when each of these factors was
not high. Persistence status was not significantly TABLE 4
associated with any other factor tested. Multivariate Logistic Regression Examining Factors Related to
Overall Problem Persistence Among Children With Any Problem
A multivariate stepwise logistic regression model was
in Year 1 (n = 203)
employed to further examine these associations. The
Estimate SE RR 95% CI
model included all variables that met a significance level of
p G .10 in the bivariate analyses, thus restricting the Intercept Y 1.01 0.25 V V
Co-occurring 1.36 0.36 3.89*** 1.93 Y 7.85
number of variables in the model, as was necessary because problems
of power constraints. Overall problem persistence was PSI/parenting 1.38 0.48 3.96** 1.55 Y 10.13
significantly more likely when co-occurring problems distress
were present (RR = 3.89), with high parenting distress High family life 0.75 0.31 2.11* 1.14 Y 3.92
(RR = 3.96) and with high FLIS disruption (RR = 2.11) impairment
Likelihood ratio (5,203) = 39.13, p G .0001
(Table 4). Parental depressive symptoms, anxiety symp-
toms, and PSI-PC did not remain in the final model. No Note: PSI = Parenting Stress Index.
significant interactions were present. A competing model * p G .05; ** p G .01; *** p G .001.

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:7, JULY 2006 855

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
BRIGGS-GOWAN ET AL.

and the same pattern of significant effects. Results problems than children with pure problems, and 3.9
indicated shared variance between Total Problems and times more likely after controlling for parenting distress
Co-occurrence; although Total Problems was not signifi- and family disruption. Thus, comprehensive assessment
cant (p G .20), co-occurrence had smaller RR than in the tools capable of identifying problems in multiple
first model (RR = 2.56, 95% CI 1.00Y6.53, p G .05). domains may aid efforts to identify children at par-
ticular risk of persistent problems.
There may be several explanations for the link
DISCUSSION
between co-occurrence and overall persistence. For
A primary aim of this work was to document the example, when problem behaviors are of greater
persistence of parent-reported psychopathology in number or severity, it is simply less probable that all
infants and toddlers in a representative healthy birth problems will drop below cutpoint thresholds. It also is
cohort. Our findings indicate that even at this young possible that difficulty with emotion regulation man-
age, elevations in social-emotional and behavioral ifests across multiple areas, such as aggression/defiance,
problems are not transient. Approximately half of the separation distress, and sleep problems. Co-occurring
infants and toddlers who were reported to have high problems also may indicate a greater progression of
social-emotional and behavioral problems continued to psychopathology, in which problems that began in one
have such problems approximately 1 year later. area become more pervasive and affect functioning in
Furthermore, rates were comparable for younger and another area. For example, oppositionality first
older children. This level of persistence is consistent expressed as defiance may develop an aggressive
with findings reported by others who have studied this component and come to be expressed as power struggles
phenomenon in early childhood (Fischer et al., 1984; at mealtime or bedtime. Our multivariate findings
Lavigne et al., 1998; Mathiesen and Sanson, 2000) and indicated that total problems and co-occurrence shared
parallels rates of persistence documented in school-age variance; thus, persistence is likely related to both
children (Briggs-Gowan et al., 2003). Furthermore, severity and pervasiveness across domains.
although many studies have examined homotypic Furthermore, co-occurring problems were associated
stability using symptom counts, fewer have examined with increased homotypic persistence of dysregulation
homotypic persistence using cutpoints or diagnostic problems (co-occurring 54%, pure 20%), but not with
status (Lavigne et al., 1998; Mathiesen and Sanson, increased persistence of internalizing or externalizing
2000). Our findings indicate substantial homotypic problems. The presence of problem behaviors in other
stability in cutpoint status, with rates of 38% for domains may make it more challenging for parents to
Internalizing, 39% for Dysregulation, and 50% for help their children to develop more appropriate
Externalizing. These early, stable, and distinctive regulatory skills. Furthermore, the relatively low
manifestations of psychopathology support the pres- stability of dysregulation when not accompanied by
ence of early differentiation in psychopathological problems in other areas suggests that the dysregulation
behaviors that may be signs of later disorders or construct measured does not have a strong tempera-
represent the presence of early childhood disorders. mental component.
In addition to contributing to our understanding of Factors other than co-occurrence also affect persis-
the unfolding of early psychopathology, these findings tence, as evidenced by the fact that approximately half of
have implications for identifying children whose social- children with persistent problems had initial pure
emotional/behavioral problems are most likely to problems. Controlling for co-occurrence, persistence
persist and who are likely to have the greatest need of was more likely when parents reported high levels of
intervention. Remarkably, persistent social-emotional/ parenting distress and/or disruption in family life because
behavioral problems were reported in approximately of their child_s behavior. This is consistent with studies of
75% of children with co-occurring problems. Children older children that have linked greater parental burden
with co-occurring problems represented 30% of with symptom severity, impairment, and service use
children with any initial problem and nearly half of (Angold et al., 1998). Although there are likely
children whose problems persisted. These children were bidirectional influences between child psychopathology
approximately 4.7 times more likely to have persistent and parenting distress and family disruption, these

856 J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:7, JULY 2006

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
ARE INFANT-TODDLER PROBLEMS TRANSIENT?

associations highlight the need to include both children pathology and disrupted/distressed parenting may be
and parents in assessment and intervention. mutually maintaining, thus emphasizing the need for
The lack of sex differences is notable and contrasts parentYchild interventions.
with some prior evidence of sex differences in problem
persistence and stability. This difference may reflect in Disclosure: The Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment
part that the ITSEA employs age- and sex-based (ITSEA) is licensed for publication with Harcourt Assessment. Drs.
cutpoints. Thus, although there are sex differences in Carter and Briggs-Gowan will receive royalties from its publication.
The other authors have no financial relationships to disclose.
some ITSEA subscales (Carter et al., 2003), when
compared within age and sex bands, patterns of co-
occurrence, overall persistence, and homotypic conti- REFERENCES
nuity did not differ for boys and girls.
Abidin RR (1990), Parenting Stress Short-Test Manual. Charlottesville, VA:
Pediatric Psychology Press
American Psychiatric Association (1994), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
Limitations of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American
The reliance on parental reports without corrobora- Psychiatric Association
Angold A, Messer SC, Stangl D, Farmer EM, Costello EJ, Burns BJ (1998),
tion from direct observation or another informant Perceived parental burden and service use for child and adolescent
constrains our ability to establish the clinical signifi- psychiatric disorders. Am J Public Health 88:75 Y 80
Bates JE, Maslin CA, Frankel KA (1985), Attachment security mother-child
cance of the problem behaviors studied. Reporting interaction, and temperament as predictors of behavior-problem ratings
biases or individual differences in thresholds applied at age three years. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 50:167x Y 193x
Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA (1988), An inventory for
when reporting problems behaviors may affect parental measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult Clin
reports. Nevertheless, parental perceptions of extreme Psychol 56:893 Y 897
Biederman J, Hirshfeld-Becker DR, Rosenbaum JF et al. (2001), Further
social-emotional/behavioral problems, even if not evidence of association between behavioral inhibition and social anxiety
initially clinically significant, may become so when in children. Am J Psychol 158:1673 Y 1679
maintained over a period of years. Additional research is Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS (1998), Preliminary acceptability and
psychometrics of the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment
warranted to examine the contributions of tempera- (ITSEA): a new adult report questionnaire. Infant Ment Health J
ment, emotion regulation, and symptom clusters to the 19:422 Y 445
Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS, Skuban EM, Horwitz SM (2001), Prevalence of
developmental course of psychopathology. Ideally, this social-emotional and behavioral problems in a community sample of 1-
future research would incorporate observational methods and 2-year-old children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 40:811 Y 819
Briggs-Gowan MJ, Owens PL, Schwab-Stone ME, Leventhal JM, Leaf PJ,
and newly available structured diagnostic interviews Horwitz SM (2003), Persistence of psychiatric disorders in pediatric
(Del Carmen-Wiggins and Carter, 2004) to allow careful settings. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 42:1360 Y 1369
Carter AS, Briggs-Gowan MJ (2005), Infant Toddler and Brief Infant
characterization of temperament, emotional regulation, Toddler Social Emotional Assessment (ITSEA) (BITSEA) Manual. San
and psychopathology. Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment
Carter AS, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Jones SM, Little TD (2003), The Infant-
Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA): factor structure,
Clinical Implications reliability, and validity. J Abnorm Child Psychol 31:495 Y 514
Cochrane R, Robertson A (1973), The life events inventory: a measure
Most broadly, the high rates of persistence observed of the relative severity of psycho-social stressors. J Psychosom Res
17:135 Y 139
contrast with a view of early problems as developmen- Costello EJ, Angold A, Keeler GP (1999), Adolescent outcomes of
tally transient. Early social-emotional/behavioral pro- childhood disorders: the consequences of severity and impairment.
blems appear to be especially intransigent when J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 38:121 Y 128
Del Carmen-Wiggins R, Carter AS, eds. (2004), Handbook of Infant and
problems are present in multiple domains and when Toddler Mental Health Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press
parents report high distress in parenting and disruption Fischer M, Rolf JE, Hasazi JE, Cummings L (1984), Follow-up of a
preschool epidemiological sample: cross-age continuities and predictions
in family routines. Results also indicate that many of later adjustment with internalizing and externalizing dimensions of
parents who reported persistent psychopathology were behavior. Child Dev 55:137 Y 150
Guerin DW, Gottfried AW, Thomas CW (1997), Difficult temperament
not worried about their child_s behavior, thus empha- and behaviour problems: a longitudinal study from 1.5 to 12 years. Int J
sizing the need for systematic early identification Behav Dev 21:71 Y 90
Keenan K, Shaw D, Delliquadri E, Giovannelli J, Walsh B (1998), Evidence
strategies, such as screening with normed instruments. for the continuity of early problem behaviors: application of a
Finally, evidence that persistence was related to both co- developmental model. J Abnorm Child Psychol 26:441 Y 452
Keenan K, Wakschlag LS (2000), More than the terrible twos: the nature
occurrence of problems across domains and disrupted/ and severity of behavior problems in clinic-referred preschool children.
distressed parenting suggests that child psycho- J Abnorm Child Psychol 28:33 Y 46

J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:7, JULY 2006 857

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
BRIGGS-GOWAN ET AL.

Lavigne JV, Arend R, Rosenbaum D, Binns HJ, Christoffel KK, Gibbons versus stable behavior problems in a normative sample: infancy to school
RD (1998), Psychiatric disorders with onset in the preschool years: I. age. J Pediatr Psychol 17:423 Y 443
Stability of diagnoses. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 37: Radloff LS (1977), The CES-D scale: a self report depression scale for
1246 Y 1254 research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1:385 Y 401
Mathiesen KS, Sanson A (2000), Dimensions of early childhood behavior Rose SL, Rose SA, Feldman JF (1989), Stability of behavior problems in very
problems: stability and predictors of change from 18 to 30 months. young children. Dev Psychopathol 1:5 Y 19
J Abnorm Child Psychol 28:15 Y 31 Schwartz CE, Snidman N, Kagan J (1999), Adolescent social anxiety as an
Mesman J, Bongers IL, Koot HM (2001), Preschool developmental outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood. J Am Acad Child
pathways to preadolescent internalizing and externalizing problems. Adolesc Psychiatry 38:1008 Y 1015
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 42:679 Y 689 Shaw DS, Owens EB, Vondra JI, Keenan K, Winslow EB (1996), Early risk
Mesman J, Koot HM (2001), Early preschool predictors of preadolescent factors and pathways in the development of early disruptive behavior
internalizing and externalizing DSM-IV diagnoses. J Am Acad Child problems. Dev Psychopathol 8:679 Y 699
Adolesc Psychiatry 40:1029 Y 1036 Shaw DS, Winslow EB, Owens EB, Vondra JI, Cohn JF, Bell RQ (1998),
Moos RH, Moos BM (1983), Family Environment Scale. In: Family Health The development of early externalizing problems among children from
and Neighborhood Questions. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists low-income families: a transformational perspective. J Abnorm Psychol
Press 26:95 Y 107
National Center for Toddlers and Families (1994), Diagnostic Classification: Sherbourne CD, Stewart AL (1991), The MOS Social Support Survey. Soc
0-3. Diagnostic Classification of Mental Health and Developmental Sci Med 32:705 Y 714
Disorders of Infancy and Early Childhood. Washington, DC: National Smith L, Calkins SD, Keane SP, Anastopoulos AD, Shelton TL (2004),
Center for Toddlers and Families Predicting stability and change in toddler behavior problems:
Prior M, Smart D, Sanson A, Oberklaid F et al. (1993), Sex differences in contributions of maternal behavior and child gender. Dev Psychol
psychological adjustment from infancy to 8 years. J Am Acad Child 40:29 Y 42
Adolesc Psychiatry 32:291 Y 304 Zeanah CHJ (2000), Handbook of Infant Mental Health, 2nd ed. New York:
Prior M, Smart D, Sanson A, Pedlow R, Oberklaid F (1992), Transient The Guilford Press

Comparison of Complementary and Alternative Medicine Use: Reasons and Motivations Between Two Tertiary
Children_s Hospitals D.R. Cincotta, N.W. Crawford, A. Lim, N.E. Cranswick, S. Skull, M. South, C.V.E. Powell

Aims: To compare prevalence, reasons, motivations, initiation, perceived helpfulness, and communication of complementary and
alternative medicine (CAM) use between two tertiary children_s hospitals. Methodology: A study, using a face-to-face questionnaire, of
500 children attending the University Hospital of Wales, Cardiff, UK, was compared to an identical study of 503 children
attending the Royal Children_s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia. Results: One year CAM use in Cardiff was lower than Melbourne
(41% v 51%; OR = 0.67, 95% CI 0.52j0.85), reflected in non-medicinal use (OR = 0.41, 95% CI 0.29 Y 0.58), and general
paediatric outpatients (OR = 0.38,95% CI 0.21 Y 0.67). Compared to Melbourne, factors associated with lower CAM use
in Cardiff included families born locally (father: OR = 0.58, 95% CI 0.44 Y 0.77) or nontertiary educated parents (mother: OR =
0.54, 95% CI 0.38 Y 0.77). Cardiff participants used less vitamin C (OR = 0.31, 95% CI 0.18 Y 0.51) and herbs (OR = 0.49, 95%
CI 0.34 Y 0.71), attended less chiropractors (OR = 0.25, 95% CI 0.06 Y 0.37), and naturopaths (OR = 0.08, 95% CI 0.02 Y 0.33),
but saw more reflexologists (OR = 3.33, 95% CI 1.08 Y 10.29). In Cardiff, CAM was more popular for relaxation (OR = 1.92,
95% CI 1.03 Y 3.57) but less for colds/coughs (OR = 0.4, 95% CI 0.27 Y 0.73). Most CAM was self-initiated (by parent) in
Cardiff and Melbourne (74% v 70%), but Cardiff CAM users perceived it less helpful (OR = 0.46, 95% CI 0.31 Y 0.68). Non-
disclosure of CAM use was high in Cardiff and Melbourne (66% v 63%); likewise few doctors/nurses documented recent
medicinal CAM use in inpatient notes (0/21 v 2/22). Conclusions: The differences in CAM use may reflect variation in
sociocultural factors influencing reasons, motivations, attitudes, and availability. The regional variation in use and poor
communication highlights the importance of local policy development. Archives of Disease in Childhood 2006;91:153 Y 158.

858 J. AM. ACAD. CHILD ADOLESC. PSYCHIATRY, 45:7, JULY 2006

Copyright @ 2006 American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

You might also like