You are on page 1of 22

Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Contemporary Educational Psychology


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cedpsych

Confused, now what? A Cognitive-Emotional Strategy Training (CEST) T


intervention for elementary students during mathematics problem solving
Ivana Di Leo, Krista R. Muis⁎
Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, Faculty of Education, McGill University, Canada

ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT

Keywords: We developed a cognitive-emotional strategy training (CEST) intervention to teach fifth-grade students (N = 57)
Mathematics problem solving self-regulated learning strategies that can be used when confusion is experienced during mathematics problem
Cognitive-emotional strategy training solving in addition to strategies they can implement during learning to help solve them. Fifth-grade students
intervention were randomly assigned to the intervention condition or the control condition. A think-emote-aloud protocol
Elementary students
was administered to capture self-regulatory processes and emotions as students solved a complex mathematics
Self-regulated learning
problem. Using an explanatory mixed methods design, results revealed that, compared to students in the control
condition, students who received the intervention scored significantly higher on the mathematics problem,
implemented more cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies across the four phases of self-regulated
learning, expressed more positive emotions and fewer negative emotions, and were better able to regulate and
resolve their confusion when it occurred. These results extend previous findings from the strategy instruction
literature by incorporating consideration of the role of emotions during learning.

1. Introduction alarming given that negative emotions can have detrimental effects on
self-regulated learning (Muis, Psaradellis, Lajoie, Di Leo, & Chevrier,
Mathematics problem solving is complex and can be challenging, 2015), and mathematics learning and achievement (Frenzel et al.,
both cognitively and emotionally. Cognitively, effectively solving 2007; Raccanello et al., 2019) if they are not appropriately regulated or
mathematics problems involves various skills such as the ability to resolved (Munzar, Muis, Denton, & Losenno, 2020).
understand number sense, apply basic mathematics facts and mathe- Several interventions with elementary students exist in the litera-
matical reasoning, and activate relevant prior knowledge (Baroody & ture that have been effective in fostering emotion regulation (see
Dowker, 2013; Davidson & Sternberg, 1998). Because of the complexity Quoidbach, Mikolajczak, & Gross, 2015) or self-regulated learning (see
of solving mathematics problems, students must also engage in self- Dignath & Büttner, 2008). Although emotions and cognitive processes
regulated learning (De Corte, Verschaffel, & Op't Eynde, 2000; Pape & are interconnected and dynamically linked (Barrett, 2009; Blair, 2002;
Smith, 2002). Self-regulated learning facilitates students’ ability to Pekrun, 2006, 2018; Scherer, 2009), to our knowledge, there are cur-
plan, monitor, and evaluate their work, and recruit from their executive rently no interventions that take into consideration cognition-emotion
functions to organize, sustain and shift attention, inhibit distractions, relations to improve mathematics problem solving skills among ele-
utilize working memory, and maintain an appropriate level of moti- mentary students. As such, using an explanatory mixed methods ap-
vation (Cragg & Gilmore, 2014). Emotionally, as students solve complex proach (McCrudden, Marchand, & Schutz, 2019), the goal of this study
mathematics problems, they can experience positive emotions, such as was to evaluate an intervention we designed to help elementary stu-
curiosity, enjoyment, and pride, and negative emotions, like frustration, dents resolve confusion during mathematics problem solving through
anxiety, and boredom (Frenzel, Pekrun, & Goetz, 2007; Goldin, 2014; promoting emotional awareness and self-regulated learning strategies.
Op't Eynde, De Corte, & Verschaffle, 2007; Raccanello, Brondino, Moè, Prior to delineating the specific intervention, relevant theoretical and
Stupnisky, & Lichtenfeld, 2019). Of particular concern, Di Leo, Muis, empirical work is reviewed.
Singh, and Psaradellis (2019) reported that confusion and frustration
were the two most frequently expressed emotions by fifth-grade stu-
dents during a complex mathematics problem-solving task. This is


Corresponding author.
E-mail address: krista.muis@mcgill.ca (K.R. Muis).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101879

Available online 22 May 2020


0361-476X/ © 2020 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

1.1. Emotions unproductive ways. Although confusion positively predicted metacog-


nitive learning strategies, it also negatively predicted planning and
Emotions are multifaceted phenomena that include components cognitive learning strategies. Di Leo et al. (2019) also investigated the
relating to cognition, affect, physiology, motivation, and expression dynamics of emotions through emotional-state transitions. Using a
(Scherer, 2000). For example, the anxiety that students experience think-aloud protocol, students’ thoughts and emotions were captured as
about mathematics problem solving may consist of worrying about not they solved a complex mathematics problem. Concordant with D’Mello
accurately solving the problem (cognitive), feelings of nervousness and Graesser's (2012) model, patterns of emotional-state transitions
(affective), increased cardiovascular activation (physiological), im- were observed. Confusion transitioned to negative emotions, including
pulses to flee the situation (motivational), and anxious facial expression frustration and boredom, but also transitioned to curiosity. Frustration
(expressive; Scherer, 2000). Emotions are typically organized into dif- transitioned to hopelessness and confusion. Additionally, patterns were
ferent categories along two dimensions: valence (positive or negative) observed between emotions and learning strategies. In particular,
and activation (activating or deactivating arousal; Shuman & Scherer, confusion transitioned to cognitive and metacognitive learning strate-
2014). Emotions can be further classified by whether they are positive gies including help-seeking, planning, identifying important informa-
activating (e.g., curiosity, hope, enjoyment, pride), positive deacti- tion, and monitoring. This evidence suggests that confusion can be both
vating (e.g., relief), negative activating (e.g., anxiety, confusion, frus- productive and unproductive for elementary students.
tration, anger, shame), and negative deactivating (e.g., boredom, Di Leo et al.'s (2019) study further revealed that the most frequently
hopelessness, sadness; Linnenbrink, 2007; Pekrun, 2006). One addi- expressed emotions that these fifth-grade students experienced during
tional emotion, surprise, is considered neutral in valence as it can elicit mathematics problem solving were confusion and frustration. This
positive or negative arousal depending on the context (Mauss & implies that the overall experience of solving mathematics problems
Robinson, 2009). might be unpleasant for elementary students, which may constrain the
During complex learning situations like mathematics problem sol- learning strategies they implement and thus their overall performance.
ving, emotions such as surprise, confusion, and curiosity are often ex- That is, sufficient evidence reveals that experiencing confusion can be
perienced as they are triggered by cognitive incongruity, novelty, and particularly detrimental for young students (Muis et al., 2015). Al-
impasses (D’Mello & Graesser, 2012; D’Mello, Lehman, Pekrun, & though confusion is commonly experienced for all students, young
Graesser, 2014). For example, surprise is likely to be experienced when students may more frequently fall into a pattern of emotional-state
a student encounters unexpected information, curiosity when the stu- transitions of confusion to frustration and thus remain in a negative
dent experiences something new and has the desire to learn more about emotional state. Persistent or unresolved confusion can lead to greater
it, and confusion when a student encounters information that is in- opportunities to experience boredom, use fewer self-regulated learning
consistent with prior knowledge and judges that resolving the incon- strategies, and eventually disengage from the task.
gruity may be difficult (Graesser & D’Mello, 2012). Confusion may also Frustration in younger students has also been shown to lead to
arise when goals are blocked, or other discrepant events occur like disengagement (Earl, Taylor, Meijen, & Passfield, 2017). This suggests
impasses, contradictions, or unexpected feedback (Chinn & Brewer, that young students need to be explicitly taught a set of strategies they
1993). As D’Mello et al. (2014) argued, these events trigger cognitive can apply to overcome confusion when it arises during mathematics
disequilibrium (incongruity, dissonance, conflict). When cognitive dis- problem solving to avoid frustration and eventual disengagement from
equilibrium occurs, learning is disrupted, and emotions may then the task. Moreover, as Pekrun (2006) argued, confusion, frustration,
fluctuate and oscillate from one emotion to another during complex and boredom might also arise from cognitive appraisals of control over
learning and problem solving (D’Mello & Graesser, 2012). the learning task and value in the learning activity. Indeed, research has
Dynamics of emotions. D’Mello and Graesser (2012) developed a shown that lower perceived control predicts higher levels of confusion
model to explain the dynamic nature of emotional states that occur during learning and problem solving (Dowd, Araujo, & Mazur, 2015),
during complex learning. They proposed that a learner who is in the whereas higher perceived value predicts higher levels of curiosity, and
state of engagement/flow (i.e., a cognitive-affective state of positive lower levels of confusion and frustration during problem solving (Di
valence and moderate level of arousal that elicits a high state of en- Leo et al., 2019; Muis et al., 2015). This suggests that perceived control
gagement in the learning task) will experience confusion when con- should be targeted in interventions designed to teach students cognitive
fronted with cognitive incongruities, contradictions, anomalies, or im- and metacognitive strategies to resolve confusion when it occurs.
passes. The learner will then implement cognitive and metacognitive This underlines the importance, particularly for young students, to
learning strategies to overcome the incongruity. If the cognitive in- acquire a foundation of self-regulatory skills to have awareness of their
congruity (confusion) has been resolved, the learner will return to a cognitive processes, as well as emotional states, and to appropriately
state of engagement/flow and may experience enjoyment or relief for implement self-regulated learning strategies during mathematics pro-
having resolved the incongruity. However, if the learner has failed to blem solving. As such, it is critical to develop an intervention for ele-
resolve the incongruity, they might feel stuck and their confusion will mentary students that facilitates self-regulated learning while also
transition to frustration, at which point the learner may oscillate be- taking into account the role that emotions play in self-regulated
tween confusion and frustration. With persistent failure in resolving the learning (Muis, Chevrier, & Singh, 2018).
incongruity, the learner’s frustration will eventually transition to
boredom. D’Mello and Graesser's (2012) model illustrates that confu- 1.2. Self-regulated learning
sion can be productive when it is resolved and the learner transitions to
engagement/flow. However, confusion can be unproductive when the Self-regulated learning refers to the complex, interactive, and self-
learner has difficulty resolving the incongruity and will oscillate be- directive processes involved in regulating, planning, directing, and
tween confusion and frustration and then transition from frustration to evaluating one’s behavior, cognitions, and emotions for the purpose of
boredom, which subsequently leads to disengagement from the task. goal attainment in a learning context (Schunk & Zimmerman, 1997).
D’Mello and Graesser's (2012) model has been empirically sup- Several models have been developed to delineate how students engage
ported among adult students (Craig, Graesser, Sullins, & Gholson, 2004; in self-regulated learning. For example, Muis' (2007) model of self-
D’Mello & Graesser, 2012; D’Mello et al., 2014; Graesser, Chipman, regulated learning includes four phases of learning, namely, task defi-
King, McDaniel, & D’Mello, 2007) and more recently with elementary nition, planning and goal setting, enactment, and evaluation, and five
students in the fifth grade (Di Leo et al., 2019; Muis et al., 2015). For areas for regulation including cognition, motivation, affect, behavior,
example, Di Leo et al. (2019) found that fifth grade students’ confusion and context. In the first phase, students begin by defining the task,
during mathematics problem solving functioned in both productive and which is influenced by the five areas for regulation. Learning strategies

2
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

that might be employed during the task definition phase include prior Emotion regulation can be described as one’s ability to control and
knowledge activation and identifying important information. For the manage one’s experience and expression of emotions (Gross, 2002). It
second phase, learners may set goals and plans to establish what they involves the attempt to increase or decrease the magnitude or duration
will do to solve the problem, including selecting appropriate learning of one’s emotions (Gross, Richards, & John, 2006; Gross, Sheppes, &
strategies. Urry, 2011; Gross & Thompson, 2007) and to reduce the urgency of the
The third phase, enactment, begins once learners implement the emotions to have control over one’s behaviors (Melnick & Hinshaw,
selected strategies to carry out the task. In the context of mathematics 2000) to maintain engagement with the task at hand (Fried, 2010).
problem solving, the enactment phase may include hypothesizing, Appropriate and effective emotion regulation depends on one’s
summarizing, help seeking, calculating/measuring, or re-reading (Muis emotional awareness and the ability to identify and appraise one’s
et al., 2015). In the last phase, individuals may evaluate the successes or emotions (e.g., Berking & Wupperman, 2012; Boden & Thompson,
failures of each phase, products created for the task, and/or perceptions 2015). Emotional awareness is considered a part of emotion regulation
about the self or context. Critical to this phase is metacognitive mon- (Gross & Thompson, 2007) and has been described as paying attention
itoring and evaluation. Strategies implemented during this phase might to one’s emotional state and having a clear understanding of one’s
include self-questioning, monitoring, judgments of learning, self-cor- emotions (e.g., Boden & Thompson, 2015; Coffey, Berenbaum, & Kerns,
recting, and evaluating (Muis et al., 2015). Muis (2007) further pro- 2003; Gohm & Clore, 2002). Facets of emotional awareness, especially
posed that metacognitive processes can occur within all phases of self- identifying the type of emotion, have been associated with emotional
regulated learning and can be ongoing throughout the learning and regulation, i.e., expressive suppression, acceptance of emotions, and
problem-solving process. As noted previously, the skills required for cognitive reappraisal (Boden & Thompson, 2015). More specifically,
each phase, including metacognitive monitoring, are critical for suc- individuals who identify the emotion type are more likely to select and
cessful mathematics problem solving (Fuchs et al., 2006; Jacobse & implement appropriate emotion regulation strategies (Boden &
Harskamp, 2012; Schoenfeld, 1994). With empirically supported Thompson, 2015).
models of self-regulated learning that provide insight into how it Teaching students to develop emotional awareness and implement
functions, it is also important to understand how to promote self- emotion regulation strategies can be done explicitly in the classroom
regulated learning. (Boekaerts, 1997) whereby an instructor uses techniques such as
Promoting self-regulated learning. Teaching students how to self- modelling strategies, which can then be used by the student (Pincus &
regulate their learning and to master complex strategies can be ac- Friedman, 2004; Schunk & Zimmerman, 2007). Direct instruction can
complished through cognitive strategy instruction (MacArthur, 2012). help bring students’ strategy use into conscious awareness as they use
Substantial evidence points to the effectiveness of cognitive strategy reflective thinking to become aware of the emotions they experience
instruction to facilitate complex tasks (Rosenshine, 1997), to promote and understand their strategy use (Schraw, Crippen, & Hartley, 2006).
the monitoring of their thinking and progress as they engage in problem Furthermore, it is possible that discussion of emotional experiences
solving (Kramarski & Mevarech, 2003; Verschaffel et al., 1999), and to within the classroom setting can further develop students’ emotion
promote mathematics automaticity and problem-solving skills (Carnine, regulation strategies (Weare, 2004) as this may increase students’
1997; Kroesbergen & Van Luit, 2003; Maccini & Hughes, 2000; knowledge of emotional expression (Denham & Kochanoff, 2002). To
Montague, Enders, & Dietz, 2011). In cognitive strategy instruction, the incorporate emotional awareness and regulation during learning, we
explicit teaching, modeling, scaffolding, and coaching of the compo- developed a cognitive-emotional strategy training intervention (CEST)
nents and steps of the strategy are considered to be effective in pro- by integrating cognitive strategy instruction with components of cog-
moting students’ achievement (Rosenshine, 1997; Schunk & nitive behavioral therapy (CBT). The CBT interventions we reviewed
Zimmerman, 1997). Rosenshine (1997) identified two important com- included those designed for children and adolescents with anxiety
ponents of strategy instruction: concrete prompts (e.g., checklists, cue disorders given their specific focus on emotion and emotion regulation.
cards) and instructional scaffolds (e.g., model using the strategy, think
aloud, begin with simplified material, anticipate difficult areas in the 1.3. Cognitive behavior therapy
material, provide correction strategies, increase students’ responsi-
bility). Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) programs that target anxiety,
Cognitive strategy instruction has been successful in promoting self- like Coping Cat (Kendall, 1994), Building Confidence (Galla, Wood, Chiu,
regulated learning to support mathematics achievement for young Langer, & Jacobs, 2012), or Healthy Minds, Healthy Schools (Montreuil &
students with various learning profiles (e.g., Case, Harris, & Graham, Tilley, 2017) focus on helping the child to: (1) recognize feelings and
1992; Cassel & Reid, 1996; Mastropieri, Scruggs, & Shiah, 1991; Mercer bodily somatic reactions to anxiety, (2) clarify thoughts about anxiety-
& Miller, 1992), with meta-analyses revealing an average effect size provoking situations (e.g., negative attributions), (3) develop coping
around 0.66 (Donker, de Boer, Kostons, Dignath van Ewijk, & van der skills (e.g., change negative self-talk to coping self-talk), and, (4)
Werf, 2014). However, such training programs have focused solely on evaluate outcomes. Behavioral training strategies include modelling,
strategies that emphasize cognitive, metacognitive, and motivational role playing and relaxation training, and cognitive strategies include
processes without consideration of the role of emotions in learning and self-control, self-observation, self-modification, and self-evaluation. For
problem solving. Given that emotions experienced during learning and example, in the STOP program (Silverman et al., 1999), children and
problem solving can facilitate or constrain self-regulated learning and adolescents are taught to identify when they are feeling anxious, to
learning outcomes (Di Leo et al., 2019; Muis et al., 2015; Pekrun, 2006, identify their anxious thoughts, then modify or restructure their an-
2018), there is a critical need to incorporate emotional components into xious thoughts by generating alternative coping thoughts and beha-
training programs that aim to improve mathematics problem solving viors, and then praise themselves for confronting their fears. These
through strategy instruction. Furthermore, self-regulated learning also programs have been shown to be effective with students as young as six
involves regulation of affective states. Therefore, it would be highly years of age (Hirshfeld-Becker et al., 2010).
relevant and important to also train students emotion regulation stra-
tegies and to monitor their emotional states and engage such strategies 2. The current study
to effectively manage their emotions and diminish the experience of
negative emotions and increase positive emotions. Taking cognitive strategy instruction and CBT together, we devel-
Emotional awareness and emotion regulation. Indeed, it is im- oped a CEST intervention that targeted training of cognitive and me-
perative that students manage and regulate their emotions in the tacognitive strategies over the four phases of self-regulated learning,
classroom setting as they carry out academic tasks (Fried, 2010, 2011). and the regulation of confusion during mathematics problem solving.

3
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

Using a mixed-methods approach, the objective of this research was Academic control scale. To measure perceived control for learning
to evaluate the efficacy of our CEST intervention with a sample of fifth- mathematics and for mathematics problem solving, participants com-
grade students. We used cognitive strategy instruction and modeling pleted the Academic Control Scale (Perry, Hladkyi, Pekrun, & Pelletier,
techniques to provide students with a repertoire of strategies that can 2001), which was previously modified and validated for use with ele-
be implemented to overcome confusion when it occurs during mathe- mentary students (see Muis et al., 2015). All participants completed this
matics problem solving. eight-item scale one week prior to commencing the study and im-
To this end, the following research questions were addressed. mediately after completing the complex problem. Students rated how
Compared to students in the control condition: (1) Will students who much they agreed to each of the eight items on a five-point Likert-scale
participate in the CEST intervention condition perform better on a ranging from “1 - Strongly Disagree” to “5 - Strongly Agree.” Sample
complex mathematics problem? (2) Will students in the intervention items include, “I am responsible for how well I do in math” and “I have
condition use more cognitive and metacognitive strategies over the four a lot of control over my grades in math.” Higher scores represent higher
phases of self-regulated learning during mathematics problem solving? perceptions of control. Cronbach’s alpha reliability estimate was 0.80 at
(3) Will students in the intervention condition experience more positive pretest and 0.85 at posttest.
emotions and fewer negative emotions? (4) Will students in the inter- Task value. The Task Value Measure (Eccles, Wigfield, Harold, &
vention condition report greater levels of perceived control (i.e., action Blumenfeld, 1993; Muis et al., 2015) was used to assess students’ per-
and outcome) during mathematics problem solving? (5) Will students in ceptions of value for mathematics problem solving. All participants
the intervention condition regulate their confusion more effectively? completed this scale one week prior to commencing the study to ensure
We hypothesized that students in the intervention condition would: (1) equivalence across groups prior to treatment. This scale assesses three
score higher on the mathematics problem; (2) engage in more cognitive dimensions of task value: intrinsic interest value (e.g., “In general, I find
and metacognitive learning strategies across the four phases of self- learning about math very interesting”), importance (e.g., “Learning
regulated learning (i.e., task definition, planning and goal setting, en- more about math is very important”), and utility value (e.g., “In gen-
actment, and evaluation); and, (3) experience more overall positive eral, learning about math is useful”). Participants rated seven items on a
emotions and fewer negative emotions, with the exception of confusion. 5-point Likert-scale ranging from “1- Not at all True of me” to “5- Very
We did not expect differences in the amount of confusion experienced True of me.” Cronbach’s alpha reliability estimate was 0.83.
as our intervention did not target a reduction in confusion but rather Emotion regulation. The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire for
how to regulate and resolve confusion when it occurred. We also ex- Children and Adolescents (ERQ-CA; Gullone & Taffe, 2012) was used to
pected students in the intervention condition to: (4) experience higher measure students’ emotion regulation strategies of cognitive reappraisal
perceptions of control (i.e., action and outcome) during problem sol- and expressive suppression to ensure equivalence between groups at
ving; and, (5) more effectively regulate their confusion during problem pretest. The ERQ-CA includes 10 items that assess the emotion reg-
solving. To capture self-regulatory processes and emotions, we used a ulation strategies of cognitive reappraisal (six items, e.g., “I control my
think-emote aloud protocol (see Di Leo et al., 2019; Muis et al., 2015). feelings about things by changing the way I think about them”) and
expressive suppression (four items, e.g., “When I’m feeling bad [e.g.,
3. Method sad, angry, or worried], I’m careful not to show it”). Items are rated on a
5-point Likert-scale ranging from “1- Strongly Disagree” to “5- Strongly
3.1. Participants Agree.” It has good internal consistency and construct validity across
age and sex (Gullone & Taffe, 2012). Cronbach’s alpha reliability esti-
All grade 5 teachers (N = 3) from one urban school were invited to mates were calculated: 0.84 for cognitive reappraisal and 0.85 for ex-
participate. All three teachers agreed. Parental consent and student pressive suppression.
assent forms were then distributed to all students in grade 5 from the Prior mathematics knowledge. To assess students’ prior mathe-
three classrooms (N = 72). Sixty-seven students from the fifth grade matics knowledge, their scores on their most recent compulsory pro-
(n = 28 girls) across the three classrooms agreed to participate (93% of vincial standards-based test were obtained1. The standardized test in-
all students invited). The mean age of the sample was 10.84 years cluded several application problems that took students approximately
(SD = 0.31). A total of 11 students were on an individualized education 40–60 min to solve, which assessed their knowledge of mathematics
plan (IEP); seven students had an adapted curriculum and no specific content learned over the school year. Given that teachers provided us
diagnosis, three students had an adapted curriculum and a diagnosis of with students’ test score, we were not able to calculate reliability.
a learning disorder, and one student had an adapted curriculum and a Intervention. The overall objective of our CEST intervention was to
diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. Eighty-five percent of the provide fifth-grade students with strategies to resolve confusion they
sample was Caucasian, with the remaining 15% being Asian (10%) or experience during complex mathematics problem solving. By explicitly
Black (5%). Students came from low to middle SES families. Only stu- teaching students learning strategies to apply during states of confu-
dents who received parental consent and provided assent participated. sion, there is greater likelihood that the experience of confusion is
productive rather than unproductive. Moreover, when students ex-
3.2. Materials perience confusion, their perceptions of control over learning (i.e., ac-
tion control and outcome control) subsequently decrease (Munzar et al.,
Because interactions between perceptions of control and value 2020), which may be a contributing factor to the reduction of learning
predict emotions experienced during learning, which subsequently strategies. To help students conceptualize their state of confusion when
predict self-regulated learning (Frenzel et al., 2007; Muis et al., 2015; it occurs, students can be told that confusion is not only commonly
Pekrun, 2006), we measured students’ perceptions of control and value experienced by everyone but is expected during complex problem-
for mathematics problem solving at pretest to ensure equivalence be-
tween groups prior to the intervention. We also measured students’
emotion regulation strategies they typically use during problem solving 1
The provincial exam that students complete each year is developed with a
to ensure equivalence between groups at pretest, as some students are
large panel of experts and educators from the province. Each question is rig-
better able to regulate their emotions compared to others (Quoidbach orously reviewed. Teachers are also trained on how to implement the tests and
et al., 2015). Moreover, as previous research has found relations be- how to grade them with specified grading rubrics. Validity and reliability of
tween prior mathematics knowledge and self-regulated learning (Muis these tests are assessed each year, and modifications are made as needed.
et al., 2015), prior mathematics knowledge was included as a covariate Unfortunately, the province does not share validity information with others
for all analyses. who are not part of this panel.

4
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

solving tasks. This message may help to normalize the state of confusion served as a visual prompt for students to help guide them through their
and may enhance perceptions of control over the task. Specifically, confusion. The worksheet included allotted space for students to write
through explicit instruction, the intervention was designed to: (1) their emotions, the issue they were having, and a list of strategies they
promote emotional awareness as it relates to problem solving through could use to overcome their issue. The worksheet included six steps to
describing and explaining various emotions; (2) describe the emotional take when confronted with confusion or a sense of being stuck. The six
state of confusion, when it can arise in the context of mathematics steps were as follows: (1) Stop and take a deep breath; (2) Think “How
problem solving, normalize the experience of confusion, describe it as do I feel right now?” and “What is my problem?”; (3) Say “I have
an alarm that should trigger the student to reflect on the cause of control, I have strategies to help me figure it out”; (4) List up to three
confusion and identify a strategy to implement to help resolve the strategies that can be used to overcome this problem; (5) Use one of
confusion; (3) teach students the cognitive and metacognitive learning those strategies; and (6) Evaluate whether it worked. If it did work,
strategies to regulate their strategy use specifically when they experi- move on but if it did not work, go back to Step 1.
ence confusion as they solve a mathematics problem; and (4) teach how In addition to the worksheet, there were two handouts that students
and when to implement learning strategies over the four phases of self- could refer to while completing the mathematics problem (see
regulated learning. Appendix A). The first handout included a list of emotions that can be
The intervention titled “I’m confused, now what?” is a training pro- experienced during mathematics problem solving, and the second
gram designed for fifth-grade students. The three-hour intervention was handout included a list of the learning strategies that could be im-
comprised of two 90-min lessons. This intervention incorporated in- plemented during mathematics problem solving. Both the comprehen-
structional procedures as well as interactive activities including group sive lists of emotions and learning strategies were discussed during the
brainstorming, think-pair-share (i.e., think about what was just learned, brainstorming sessions during the lessons on emotions and learning
pair up with a peer, and verbally share with each other what they strategies, respectively.
understand and learned), modelling of the learning strategies during Self-regulatory processes and emotions. To capture students’ self-
mathematics problem solving by the primary investigator, and oppor- regulatory processes and emotions as they occurred during problem
tunities for students to practice the strategies while solving short and solving, a think-aloud protocol (Type 1 protocol, see Ericsson and
simple mathematics problems. The primary investigator delivered the Simon, 1998) was combined with an emote-aloud protocol (Craig,
training to students in the intervention condition as a group through D'Mello, Witherspoon, & Graesser, 2008; D’Mello, Craig, Sullins, &
PowerPoint slides. Content covered in the intervention included dis- Graesser, 2006; Eva-Wood, 2004). This type of protocol provides a
cussing what a problem is and the types of problems students can come more accurate measure of students’ self-regulatory processes (see
across during mathematics problem solving; identifying different types Winne, Jamieson-Noel, & Muis, 2002) and emotions (Craig et al., 2008)
of emotions and what each emotion can feel like physiologically, de- as they spontaneously occur in real time. To train all students how to
scribing that confusion is a normal part of learning, and when specific think and emote aloud prior to commencing the students’ independent
emotions can be experienced during mathematics problem solving (e.g., study phase, the first author modeled how to think and emote aloud
surprise when you get an unexpected answer to your calculation, an- while completing a short mathematics problem. Students were in-
xious when you are worried you will not have enough time to complete structed to say everything they were thinking and feeling as they ex-
the problem, confused when the problem does not make sense and you perienced it while solving the mathematics problem. Students then had
feel stuck, frustrated when you cannot seem to get the right answer); an opportunity to practice thinking and emoting aloud with a short
taking control of one’s confusion by identifying its source; and, iden- mathematics problem unrelated to the one they were given for the
tifying the learning strategies that can be helpful to overcome problems experimental session. During the think-and-emote session, each stu-
that can arise during mathematics problem solving (e.g., re-reading the dent’s think-emote aloud was audio recorded using Apple Ear Pods with
problem and highlighting or identifying important information when a microphone connected to a digital recording device. Barriers were
you do not understand what to do, re-calculating when you are not sure placed around students and spread apart so that they could not hear
if you got the correct answer, evaluating and looking over your work to each other during problem solving.
make sure you did not forget anything, asking for help when nothing Mathematics problem. Students were given a complex mathe-
seems to be working). matics problem called The Garden Plot. This problem was appropriate
Once the lessons on emotions and learning strategies were com- for fifth-grade students and was selected from the regular curriculum by
pleted, the primary investigator modeled solving a mathematics pro- the primary investigator and the students’ teachers. The objective of The
blem while thinking and emoting aloud and implementing specific Garden Plot was to have students develop the solution to an application
strategies after expressing certain emotions and difficulties (e.g., “I do problem by selecting numerous previously acquired mathematical
not understand what the problem is asking me to do. I am feeling a little concepts and processes and applying them in a new way. Moreover, the
bit confused right now and kind of worried I will not be able to solve instructions to this problem did not suggest a procedure to follow nor
this. I am noticing my emotions. What are some strategies I can use to the specific mathematical concepts and processes to use to solve the
help me? Let me look at my worksheet. Right now, I am feeling con- problem. The problem was as follows: “Sarah is planning her kitchen
fused. I can choose certain strategies to help me out. I have control. garden. She is planting many root vegetables to last her through the
Okay, I can re-read the problem, I can highlight the important in- winter. This is a list of the vegetables and the amount of space Sarah has
formation, and I can ask my teacher for help. Let me start with re- decided to give each one: potatoes: ¼ of the garden; cabbage: 1/5 of the
reading.”). After the primary investigator solved a problem while garden; beets: 10% of the garden; carrots: 0.20 of the garden; onions
modelling the strategies, students then quietly practiced implementing and herbs: ½ of the area for the beets; turnips: the same area as the
the strategies they learned while solving a simple mathematics problem cabbage. Help Sarah plant her garden. The grid on the next page shows
(20 min). the square area of land she will use for the garden.” On the next page,
Materials used in the intervention. The primary investigator students were asked to analyze the situation, “What I know,” “What I
created a worksheet that was handed out to each student to help them am looking for,” and “What is essential to think about.” The legend for
follow a series of steps to take when they felt confused or stuck while each root vegetable was provided, along with a 10 by 10 grid on which
solving the problem (see Appendix A). Students were given this work- students had to allocate each root vegetable for planting. Students were
sheet during the first training session and used it during the practice also asked to show how they found the area for each section and to
sessions as well as during the study when solving the mathematics justify their answer.
problem. Students were told that they could refer to it if needed and
that it was not mandatory for them to use. If needed, this worksheet

5
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

Table 1
Coding scheme for learning strategies in think-emote-aloud protocol.
Level (Macro) / Micro Definition Examples

Level 1 – Task Definition A learner generates a perception about the task, context, and the Prior knowledge activation, beliefs, motivation, and knowledge of
self in relation to the task. External and internal conditions play a strategies are activated during this level.
major role.

Prior Knowledge Activation Searching for or explicitly recalling relevant prior knowledge. “Oh I remember now! 0.20 means 20 out of 100.”
“100 divided by 20, which is 5. I already know that.”
“This is percentage because 10 is a tenth of 100 and 100 always goes
with percentage.”
“And a quarter of a hundred is 25. I know that because of money.”
“100 divided by 4 that equals to 25. I’m not even going to do the
calculation because I know what it equals to because you know, it’s one
of the things that you remember.”

Identifying Important Recognizing the usefulness of information. “It’s the same area as the cabbage. I have to write that down because it’s
Information good information, it’s important.”
“Summarizing important information” as part of the intervention. “Hold on I need to remember this, let me write it down. Onions and herbs
are half so they equal 5.”

Level 2 – Planning and Goal Setting The learner begins to devise a plan to solve the problem and sets E.g., Planning to use means-ends analysis, trying trial and error,
goals. identifying which part of the problem to solve first, solving it within a
specific amount of time.

Making / Restating a Plan Stating what approach will be taken, what strategy will be used to “I’m going to highlight the things I’m going to put in there so I remember
solve the problem, or what part of the problem will be solved in it and I don’t have to keep looking for it.”
some sequence. This includes restating plans. “I think I’m going to ask an adult, but first, I’m going to do my other
strategies.”
“First, I’m going to highlight the important information.”

Setting / Restating a Goal A goal is modeled as a multifaceted profile of information, and “I need to fill in 25 of the spaces for potatoes.”
each standard in the profile is used as a basis to compare the “I have to colour in 10.”
products created when engaged in the activity. This includes “I need to do onions and herbs. So it’s a half, so I need to stop at the half
restating goals. part.”

Level 3 – Enactment Enactment occurs when the learner begins to work on the task by
applying tactics or strategies chosen for the task.

Hypothesizing Making predictions. “I think the garden is worth 100.”


“Carrots is 0.20 of the garden, so I guess that’s 20%.”

Summarizing Summarizing what was just read in the problem statement. “So basically, I have to help her plant her garden with all the vegetables
and stuff.”
“Turnips is the same as the cabbage. So that means the cabbage is 1 fifth
“Summarize what I need to do to solve the problem” as part of the so turnips will be the same.”
intervention.

“Summarize what I need to do to move on” as part of the


intervention.

Help Seeking Asking for help from a teacher, peer, or other source. Help seeking “Do I write P for potatoes?”
for information or help seeking for evaluation. “Wait, so I have a question. Are the onions and herbs the same?”

“Ask my teacher” as part of the intervention.

Highlighting / Labeling / Highlighting information, labeling information as part of the “Okay, let me put the 10 beets [writing] b,b,b,b,b,b,b,b,b,b.”
Colouring / Drawing / Writing problem-solving process, or taking notes in reference to the “This is beets, this is potatoes, this is cabbage [writing].”
problem. Making a drawing to assist learning or as part of solving “I’m highlighting this with my yellow highlighter.”
the problem. “I’m just going to write cabbage equals 1 fifth of the garden [writing] 1
fifth of the garden.”
“Highlight the important information” as part of the intervention.

Calculating / Measuring Solving equations, measuring, or other similar features. “Ok, long division. 5 goes into 1 zero times. 5 goes into 10 two times,
minus that by ten equals 0. Bring down the 0.5 goes into 0 zero times. So,
“Re-calculating” as part of the intervention. 20 times 1 equals 20 and so 20 squares are cabbage.”
“10, 20, 30, 40, 50, 60, 70, 80, 90, 100, that’s 100 squares in the grid.”

Re-Reading Re-reading a section of the problem, word for word. Important that “I’m going to go back to the first page. Onions and herbs are half of the
it is word for word, otherwise it is summarizing. area for the beets[re-reading]”
“OK. I’m going to re-read the whole entire problem.”
“Re-read the question” as part of the intervention.

Making Inferences aking inferences based on information read or products created “So, um, that should be, if I’m right, it should be 5 because beets are 10.”
from solving the problem. “Turnips equals to 20 because 1 fifth of a hundred is 20. So, let’s do that.”
self-explanation). Explaining why something was done. Key word “I’m guessing that 0.20 is 20 because it’s not a whole, but it’s 20, so
is “because.” yeah.”

Level 4 – Monitoring and Various types of reactions and reflections are carried out to Products created are compared to the standards set via metacognitive
Evaluation evaluate the successes or failures of each level or products created monitoring. Monitoring and evaluation can include any facet listed
for the task, or perceptions about the self or context. Reaction and above (e.g., progress, motivation, plans, goals, strategies, products like
reflection also includes judgments and evaluations of performance answers or drawings made).
on a task as well as the attributions for success or failure.
(continued on next page)

6
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

Table 1 (continued)

Level (Macro) / Micro Definition Examples

Self-Questioning Posing a question. “Where did I go wrong?”


“Carrots 0.20 of the garden. Well is that 20%?”
“Figure out what my problem is” as part of the intervention. “So how many squares are there in the grid?”
“So now what am I going to do?”

Monitoring Monitoring something relative to goals. “Now I’m going to make sure I have all of them. 1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10.
Ok.”
“Check/review my work” as part of the intervention. “I am working on the 1 fifth for cabbage.”
“So, I found carrots, cabbage, beets, and potatoes. So, I’m missing onions
and herbs, and turnips.”
“The turnips is my last one. After this, I’ll be done”
“Turnips, I did that. It’s only onions and herbs I have left.”

Judgment of Learning Learner is aware that something is unknown or know; not fully “I think I’m feeling pretty confident with what I’m doing now.”
understood, or completely understood. “Okay, I think I’m getting it.”
“I have no clue what I’m doing right now.”

Self-Correcting Correcting one’s mistakes. “I messed up. I’m going to get my eraser and erase this.”
“[counting from 1] … 17, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22, oops I passed it. …
[counting from 1] … 18, 19, 20.”
“Now, let’s go on to 0 on 20. No, oops, 0 point 20.”

Evaluation Judging whether goals have been met, whether a particular “So, potatoes are 1 fourth of the garden. So that would be 100 divided 4
strategy is working, whether the answer is correct, whether the is 25. So yes, that’s good okay.”
work is neat, etc. Judgment of all facets that fall under monitoring. “So, I don’t think I got it. I think I have to do it over again.”
[after calculating] “20. Perfect! Which means I got it right.”
“Check/review my answer” as part of the intervention. [checking over work] “So 10% of 100 equals 10. Ok so I got that right.”

Control Changing strategy when monitoring or evaluation results in a “Okay. I’m just going to do this a different way.” [after starting to count
determination that goal has not been met. the squares in the grid]
“So, 1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8… [counting to 37 by digit]. Wait. Maybe I can go by
“Tell myself I have control” as part of the intervention. tens. Maybe I can count by tens. 10, 20, 30, 40… [counting to 100 by
tens].”

Task Difficulty Statements reflecting the difficulty or easiness of a task. “So this is pretty easy.”
“This just got tricky.”
“It looks so simple but it’s really hard.”

3.3. Coding & Perry, 2007; Pekrun, Goetz, Frenzel, Barchfeld, & Perry, 2011) as well
as Craig et al.’s (2008) and D’Mello and Graesser’s (2011) definitions of
Self-regulatory processes and emotions. The think-emote-aloud emotions. Overall, 13 emotions were included: surprise, curiosity, en-
audio recordings were transcribed verbatim, then segmented and coded joyment, pride, hope, relief, confusion, frustration, boredom, anxiety,
for self-regulatory processes and emotions. All transcripts were coded hopelessness, shame, and anger. Based on these two coding schemes,
under blinded conditions (i.e., learning condition was unknown to en- the primary investigator and one previously trained graduate student
sure unbiased coding). Think-emote-aloud protocols ranged from 9 to (see Di Leo et al., 2019; Muis et al., 2015) independently coded 10% of
40 min. Muis et al.'s (2015) coding scheme specific to mathematics the transcripts. Inter-rater agreement was established at 91%, and
problem solving was used to code for self-regulatory processes (see disagreements were resolved through discussion. The primary in-
Table 1). Nineteen learning strategies were coded: prior knowledge vestigator coded the remaining transcripts.
activation, identifying important information, making/restating a plan, Mathematics problem-solving outcome. The standardized pro-
setting/restating a goal, hypothesizing, summarizing, help-seeking, vincial grading scheme was used to calculate students’ total score on the
coordinating informational sources, highlighting/colouring/drawing, problem. This mathematics problem consisted of three scoring com-
calculating/measuring, re-reading, making inferences, self-questioning, ponents including analysis (worth 30%), application (worth 50%), and
monitoring, judgment of learning, self-correcting, evaluation, control, justification (worth 20%). For the analysis component, students were
and task difficulty. Of these 19 strategies, 11 were explicitly taught as required to identify the information that was provided in the problem
part of the intervention (see Table 1). These learning strategies were statement in terms of essential information to solve the problem and
categorized into four macro-level phases of self-regulated learning (see what information they had to use to solve it. For the application com-
Greene & Azevedo, 2009) including task definition (e.g., identifying ponent, students were required to use calculations and apply mathe-
important information), planning and goal setting (e.g., making a plan), matical concepts and processes to solve the problem. Finally, for the
enactment (e.g., hypothesizing, re-reading, highlighting/labelling, co- justification component, students had to adequately justify their work
ordinating informational sources, summarizing), and monitoring and and calculations and provide a concluding statement. To establish inter-
evaluation (e.g., self-correcting, self-questioning, monitoring progress, rater agreement, the primary investigator and one previously trained
evaluating if answer is correct). graduate student graded 10% of the problems. Inter-rater agreement
To code for emotions, Di Leo et al.'s (2019) coding scheme for was established at 98%. The primary investigator graded the remaining
emotions specific to mathematics problem solving was used (see problems. Cronbach’s reliability estimate for the three scored compo-
Table 2). This coding scheme was developed using the control-value nents of the mathematics problem was 0.83.
theory of achievement emotions (Pekrun, 2006; Pekrun, Frenzel, Goetz,

7
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

Table 2
Coding scheme for emotions in think-emote-aloud protocol.
Emotion Description/Definition Example

Curiosity Interest, intrigued “Wait, how about the turnips?”


“Hmm I wonder if 1 fifth could be about 50? or another 25? Or maybe…”
“Ooooh, how many squares are there in here? [referring to the grid]. Let me see”

Enjoyment Excited, enthusiastic, happy “I’m enjoying this right now because I like math very much.”
“So I feel calm and kind of enjoying this.”
“I’m happy I know what I’m doing.”
Surprise Astonished, amazed “So that’s one line for the beets. Woah, what? One line?… Alright.”
“I feel surprised that I actually got the answer right.”

Confusion Puzzled, mixed up “So 10% every ten? What? Wait. That doesn’t make sense.”
“Just by the look of it, I feel a bit confused because there’s a lot squares and I’m a
bit confused at what to do with them.”
“I’m so confused right now, I just can’t count. We have to colour it in and it
confuses me because, uh, how do I do this?!”

Frustration Irritated, dissatisfied “Okay then beets are wrong. No no no no no. Ugh! I feel annoyed.”
“Okay, I’m annoyed right now.”
“Ah I did it all wrong! Argh, I have to erase all of it.”

Boredom Dull, monotonous “Yeah, this problem is pretty easy and I’m pretty bored.”
“I’m bored to death by this math problem.”
“In my opinion, I find this boring.”

Anxiety Worried, nervous “What’s really worrying me is I don’t know if I’m getting the 100 divided by 5. I
don’t know if I’m getting that right.”
“So I’m not the best at fractions so I’m kind of scared.”
“I feel nervous because of all the percentages.”

Pride The state of being proud. A feeling of happiness when you do something “[at the end of the problem] I’m proud of myself.”
good or difficult. “I feel proud that I finished.”

Relief The removal or lightening of something painful or distressing. “Finally, finally, finally, finally I am done! Hallelujah.”
“I am relieved that it’s over.”
“I think I am doing okay so far, I am feeling relieved.”

Hope To want something to happen or be true and think that it could happen or be “So if that’s 20 of a hundred, it’s 20 squares. I hope so.”
true. “I’m not the best at fractions, so hopefully I get them right.”

Hopelessness Having no hope, no expectation of good or success. Incapable of solution, “I’m probably gonna get this all wrong.”
management, or accomplishment. “I give up.”
“I keep doing this wrong, this is impossible.”

Anger To become angry. No participant expressed anger in this study.

Shame A feeling of guilt, regret, or sadness that you have because you know you No participant expressed shame in this study.
have done something wrong.

3.4. Procedure problems in mind. The following week, using stratified random sam-
pling based on SES and race within classrooms, all participating stu-
In February of the academic year, parental consent and student dents were given a number (from 1 to 67) and then were randomly
assent were collected prior to the intervention and mathematics pro- assigned to one of two conditions: the intervention condition (n = 34;
blem solving sessions. Fig. 1 presents the procedure visually. One week 18 girls) or the control condition (n = 33; 10 girls). After random as-
prior to the study, all participating students completed the Academic signment to condition, students were separated based on their assigned
Control Scale, Task Value Scale, and Emotion Regulation Questionnaire condition (i.e., intervention or control).
for Children and Adolescents in their regular classroom. The primary Once students were separated into their respective groups on Day 1,
investigator read all the items for questionnaires out loud to the stu- intervention students were delivered the first 90-min intervention
dents and answered any questions they had. Prior to reading each item training lesson in their regular classroom while students in the control
and instructions, the primary investigator discussed with students the condition were relocated into the library and practiced solving
type of problem she would be giving them the following week. Because mathematics problems and engaged in the regular mathematics curri-
application problems are given to students on a weekly basis, they were culum to ensure equal time on task. Students in the control condition
familiar with the type of problem they would solve for this study. did not receive any strategy instruction or training on emotional
Students were asked about what kinds of application problems they had awareness during this time. On Day 2, students in the intervention
already done that year, discussed how challenging and confusing those condition received the second and final 90-min intervention training
problems can be, and were then told to complete the scales with those lesson while students in the control condition continued their activities

8
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

Fig. 1. Procedure flow diagram.

from Day 1. For both conditions, each lesson/activity was divided into Table 3
30-min segments to best maximize students’ attention. On Day 3, all Means and standard deviations for each variable by condition.
students were first trained on how to think and emote out loud and Intervention Control
were given two simple practice problems to solve. Condition Condition
All students were given the same mathematics problem, which took
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
students from 9 min to 40 min to complete. Students were spread out
Prior knowledge 76.06% (5.68) 77.91% (6.49)
and barriers were placed between students to ensure they could not Math achievement 66.20% (20.68) 52.75% (20.80)
hear one another. Three trained graduate students and the primary Task definition 4.10 (2.86) 0.68 (1.04)
investigator circulated to ensure students continued to talk out loud and Planning and goal setting 10.10 (6.99) 3.87 (3.30)
responded to questions students had while solving the problem. Enactment (cognitive learning 31.57 (16.22) 22.94 (191.06)
strategies)
Students were prompted to keep talking if they were silent for more
Evaluation (metacognitive learning 29.83 (19.35) 18.29 (9.05)
than five seconds. Teachers also remained in the classrooms and an- strategies)
swered any questions students had while solving the problem. After Perceived Control at Posttest 4.04 (0.75) 3.92 (0.97)
completing the problem, students once again completed the Academic Surprise 0.24 (0.51) 0.13 (0.34)
Control Scale. The primary investigator then returned one week later to Enjoyment 0.52 (0.79) 0.16 (0.45)
Curiosity 0.41 (0.57) 0.03 (0.18)
give the intervention training to students who were assigned to the Pride 0.59 (0.50) 0.16 (0.37)
control condition. Hope 0.21 (0.49) 0.03 (0.18)
Relief 0.41 (0.73) 0.03 (0.18)
Confusion 1.79 (2.41) 2.00 (2.28)
3.5. Data analysis Anxiety 0.34 (0.55) 0.29 (0.94)
Frustration 0.17 (0.38) 0.84 (1.64)
Boredom 0.41 (0.63) 0.52 (0.85)
The purpose of this study was to examine whether our CEST inter- Hopelessness 0.10 (0.31) 0.19 (0.60)
vention targeting confusion and promoting self-regulatory processes
would lead to higher mathematics problem-solving outcomes, more Note. Prior knowledge and mathematics achievement are presented as percen-
learning strategies across the four phases of self-regulated learning, tages. Learning strategies and emotions are presented as average frequencies.
greater perceptions of control, and more positive emotions and fewer
negative emotions during problem solving. To this end, two ANCOVAs strategies used, perceptions of control, and experienced emotions. To
and two MANCOVAs (with prior mathematics knowledge as a covariate control for Type I errors, alpha was set at 0.01. Table 3 presents the
for all analyses) were conducted to establish whether there were group means and standard deviations for each variable by condition, and
differences in mathematics problem-solving outcomes, learning Table 4 presents a zero-order correlation matrix between all variables.

9
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

Table 4
Zero-order correlations between all variables.
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16

** **
1. Surprise 0.26* 0.14 0.18 0.32 0.38 −0.02 0.07 0.18 0.11 −0.06 0.05 0.09 0.19 0.05 −0.16
2. Enjoyment 0.21 0.17 0.46** 0.54** −0.01 −0.05 0.33** 0.05 0.02 0.12 0.07 0.13 0.15 0.19
3. Curiosity −0.05 0.40** 0.35** 0.07 −0.08 −0.05 −0.07 −0.05 0.18 0.19 0.33** 0.16 0.12
4. Hope 0.13 0.37** 0.03 −0.06 0.11 −0.01 0.11 0.20 0.23 0.23 0.10 0.22
5. Pride 0.39** 0.11 0.13 0.18 0.05 −0.06 0.13 0.37** 0.42** 0.28* 0.20
6. Relief 0.23 0.03 0.19 0.13 −0.13 0.32* 0.26* 0.37** 0.08 0.13
7. Confusion 0.47** 0.16 0.44** −0.02 0.23 0.51** 0.48** −0.06 −0.01
8. Frustration −0.07 0.35** 0.07 −0.17 0.04 0.07 −0.08 −0.21
9. Anxiety 0.01 −0.06 0.15 0.13 0.19 0.02 0.11
10. Hopelessness −0.10 0.06 0.13 0.04 −0.02 −0.10
11. Boredom −0.14 −0.13 −0.18 −0.19 −0.11
12. Planning and Goal Setting 0.56** 0.61** 0.20 0.34**
13. Cognitive Strategies 0.81** 0.19 0.38**
14. Metacognitive Strategies 0.14 0.32*
15. Academic Control post intervention 0.30*
16. Math Performance Outcome

*
p = .05.
**
p < .01.

4. Results (Gullone & Taffe, 2012) was administered to establish whether there
were differences in students’ emotion regulation strategies of cognitive
4.1. Preliminary analyses reappraisal and expressive suppression at baseline (prior to treatment).
Analyses revealed that there were no significant differences in ex-
Assumptions. Skewness and kurtosis values were examined for pressive suppression, t(65) = −0.15, p = .88 (Mcontrol = 2.88,
normality for prior mathematics knowledge, mathematics problem- SD = 0.75, Mintervention = 2.91, SD = 0.81) or cognitive reappraisal t
solving outcome, macro-level learning strategies, and emotions. Prior (65) = −0.39, p = .69 (Mcontrol = 3.57, SD = 0.94),
mathematics knowledge, mathematics problem-solving outcome, and Mintervention = 4.65, SD = 0.77). These results indicate groups were
learning strategies were within the acceptable range for skewness and equivalent in emotion regulation strategies prior to the intervention.
kurtosis (using Tabachnick and Fidell’s [2013] criteria of <|3| for
skewness and <|8| for kurtosis). Emotions fell within the range of <|8|
for kurtosis with a range from −2.37 to 7.05 but were positively 4.2. Main analyses
skewed and fell outside the range of <|3| for skewness, with a range
from −0.85 to 4.84. Normality was not expected for emotions ex- Mathematics problem-solving outcome. An ANCOVA was con-
pressed in the think-emote-aloud (see Di Leo et al., 2019). Since the ducted to address the first research question regarding whether students
measurement of emotions was on a ratio scale with a meaningful zero, in the intervention condition would perform better on the mathematics
scores were not transformed (see Tabachnick & Fidell, 2013). Think- problem than students in the control condition. Results revealed a
emote-aloud audio recordings ranged from 9 to 40 min. There were no significant difference between groups on mathematics problem-solving
significant differences in length of time spent thinking-emoting aloud, t outcome after controlling for prior mathematics knowledge, F(1,
(65) = 1.14, p = .26, between the control condition (M = 20.39 min, 64) = 7.11, p < .01, partial η2 = 0.10, Cohen’s d = 0.65. Specifically,
SD = 7.30) and the intervention condition (M = 18.03 min, students in the intervention condition (M = 66.36, SD = 20.68) per-
SD = 6.33) nor were there differences between conditions on verbosity formed significantly better than students in the control condition
(i.e., number of words spoken per minute), t(65) = 1.01, p = .32 (M = 52.59, SD = 20.80), with a medium effect size.
(average spoken words per minute was 95 for both conditions), or Learning strategies across the four phases of self-regulated
frequency of emotions expressed (χ2 = 0.02, p = .87). Finally, given learning. To answer the second research question regarding whether
that students were nested in classrooms, we calculated the intraclass students in the intervention condition would use more learning strate-
correlation coefficient (ICC) to determine whether multi-level analyses gies across the four phases of self-regulated learning compared to stu-
were necessary. That is, we examined the amount of variance in dents in the control condition, MANCOVA results revealed a significant
mathematics problem-solving outcome at the classroom level. The ICC difference between groups, Pillai’s Trace = 0.561, F(4, 57) = 16.93,
was 0.02, which indicates that nested analyses were not necessary as p < .001, partial η2 = 0.56. As hypothesized, there were significant
only 2% of the variance in students’ outcomes on the mathematics differences between groups on task definition, F(1, 59) = 41.12,
problems was attributable to classroom-level factors. p < .001, partial η2 = 0.42, planning and goal setting F(1, 59) = 22.72,
Task value and academic control at pretest. Perceptions of task p < .001, partial η2 = 0.29, enactment of learning strategies F(1,
value and academic control were measured one week before the study 59) = 6.93, p < .01, partial η2 = 0.11, as well as evaluation, F(1,
commenced to determine whether there were group differences. 59) = 10.57, p = .002, partial η2 = 0.16. Specifically, the intervention
Analyses revealed no significant differences in perceptions of task condition engaged in more task definition, planning and goal setting,
value, t(65) = 0.54, p = .59, between the control condition (M = 3.94, enactment of cognitive learning strategies, and evaluation than the
SD = 0.77) and the intervention condition (M = 3.83, SD = 0.83). control condition, with large effect sizes for all four macro processes2.
There was also no significant difference in perceptions of academic Table 5 presents the frequency of each learning strategy by condition.
control, t(65) = −0.35, p = .72, between the control condition
(M = 4.00, SD = 0.69) and the intervention condition (M = 4.06, 2
With regard to the specific strategies students were taught as part of the
SD = 0.69). These results indicate that students in both groups had
intervention, analyses of the micro-level strategies from students’ think alouds
equal levels of task value and academic control prior to beginning the
revealed that the intervention condition significantly engaged in more of those
study. strategies, with the exception of re-reading (no difference), and help-seeking
Emotion regulation. The ERQ for Children and Adolescents (fewer instances), compared to the control group. See Table 5.

10
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

Table 5
Frequency of learning strategies used by condition.
Intervention Condition Control Condition

Frequency Range Percentage Frequency Range Percentage

Prior Knowledge Activation 72 0–9 3.25 3 0–1 0.20


Identifying Important Information 47 0–6 2.12 18 0–3 1.19
Making a Plan 248 0–29 11.20 89 0–9 5.89
Setting a Goal 45 0–7 2.03 31 0–4 1.40
Hypothesizing 20 0–3 0.90 1 0–1 0.06
Summarizing 36 0–4 1.63 21 0–2 1.39
Help Seeking 10 0–2 0.45 28 0–7 1.85
Highlighting / Labeling / Drawing 245 0–23 11.06 198 0–16 13.08
Calculating 296 0–28 13.36 284 0–27 18.76
Re-Reading 42 0–9 1.90 58 0–4 3.83
Making Inferences 134 0–11 6.05 100 0–10 6.61
Self-Questioning 110 0–13 4.97 74 0–12 4.89
Monitoring 528 0–42 23.84 311 0–24 20.54
Judgment of Learning 99 0–20 4.47 67 0–7 4.43
Self-Correcting 48 0–6 2.17 41 0–7 2.71
Evaluation 169 0–30 7.63 132 0–10 8.72
Control 21 0–4 0.95 16 0–3 1.06
Task Difficulty - Difficult 21 0–4 0.95 28 0–6 1.85
Task Difficulty - Easy 24 0–4 1.08 14 0–4 0.92
Total Learning Strategies 2,215 0–42 100 1,514 0–24 100

Emotions. To assess the third research question regarding whether 4.3. Explanatory trend analyses of think-emote-aloud transcripts
students in the intervention condition expressed more positive emo-
tions and fewer negative emotions than students in the control condi- To answer the fifth research question regarding whether students in
tion, MANCOVA results revealed a significant difference between the intervention condition were better able to regulate their emotions
groups, Pillai’s Trace = 0.41, F(11,46) = 2.93, p = .005, partial compared to students in the control condition, we conducted a chi-
η2 = 0.41. As predicted, there was no significant difference in the square test in addition to an explanatory trend analysis (McCrudden
frequency of confusion expressed between groups F(1, 59) = 2.42, et al., 2019) of students’ transcripts. In total, 60 student transcripts
p = .49. Moreover, as predicted, results revealed a significant differ- were examined with 29 transcripts from the intervention condition and
ence between groups on frustration F(1, 59) = 5.36, p = .01, partial 31 transcripts from the control condition. This approach also allowed us
η2 = 0.08, curiosity F(1, 59) = 2.44, p = .001, partial η2 = 0.18, en- to develop a better understanding of similarities and differences in the
joyment F(1, 59) = 5.06, p = .01, partial η2 = 0.08, pride F(1, expression of emotions and use of learning strategies as a function of
59) = 7.22, p < .001, partial η2 = 0.24, and relief F(1, 97) = 7.41, learning condition. Given that the think-emote aloud data was quali-
p = .009, partial η2 = 0.12. Specifically, students in the intervention tatively rich, it was important to explore them further with the goal of
condition expressed less frustration, but more curiosity, enjoyment, identifying the ways in which students dealt with and managed the
pride and relief than students in the control condition, with large effect confusion they experienced. To this end, the following paragraphs
sizes. Table 6 presents the frequency of each emotion by condition. provide examples of how students’ confusion transitioned to frustration,
Perceived control. To address the fourth research question re- how students regulated their confusion, and how students used learning
garding whether students in the intervention condition experienced strategies when they experienced confusion.
more perceived control during mathematics problem solving than stu- Initial analyses of students’ transcripts revealed that expressions of
dents in the control condition, ANCOVA results revealed no significant confusion ranged from zero to eight times in the intervention condition
difference between groups, F(1, 64) = 0.47, p = .50. and from zero to seven times in the control condition. Students who did
not express confusion at all were eliminated from this exploration. We
then used two markers to gauge how students responded to states of

Table 6
Frequency of emotions that occurred by condition.
Intervention Condition Control Condition

Frequency Range Percentage Frequency Range Percentage

Surprise 7 0–2 4.64 4 0–1 2.94


Enjoyment 15 0–3 9.93 5 0–2 3.68
Curiosity 12 0–2 7.95 1 0–1 0.74
Pride 17 0–1 11.26 5 0–1 3.68
Hope 6 0–2 3.97 1 0–1 0.74
Relief 12 0–3 7.95 1 0–1 0.74
Confusion 52 0–8 34.44 62 0–7 45.59
Anxiety 10 0–2 6.62 9 0–5 6.62
Frustration 5 0–1 3.31 26 0–5 19.12
Boredom 12 0–2 7.95 16 0–3 11.76
Hopelessness 3 0–1 1.99 6 0–3 4.41
Total emotions 151 – 100 136 – 100

11
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

confusion. First, given that Di Leo et al. (2019) found that confusion students monitored their emotions, particularly their state of confusion,
frequently transitioned to frustration when students did not regulate or monitored their progress and the learning strategies they selected, and
resolve their confusion, we investigated whether students who ex- evaluated whether the strategy was successful in resolving confusion. It
pressed confusion also expressed frustration. In this regard, a transition appeared that confusion served as a cue to alert students that a learning
from confusion to frustration without resolving confusion would in- strategy needed to be implemented or perhaps that a strategy needed to
dicate that students were not effectively regulating their confusion. be changed. For example, one student worked through her confusion
Second, we explored whether students acknowledged their confusion using strategies that were taught during the training: “Okay so I’m
when it occurred, and what regulatory strategies they implemented, if confused. I’ll start with what I know, that um, potatoes are one fourth of
any, to resolve their confusion. the garden. But how many spaces does she have in her garden? I don’t
Transitions from confusion to frustration. With regard to tran- feel that good. I don’t know how much she has, like how big her garden
sitions from confusion to frustration, analyses revealed that in the in- is? I’m gonna fill in this paper thing (referring to the worksheet she was
tervention condition, 16 of 29 students (55.17%) expressed at least one given to help work through confusion). So, stop, take a breath. How do I
instance of confusion. Of these 16 students, three (18.75%) expressed feel right now? I feel confused and unsure what is my problem. I don’t
frustration following confusion. In contrast, 21 of 31 students (67.74%) know if I should… I don’t know how much space she has in her garden.
in the control condition expressed at least one instance of confusion. Of Um, I have control, I have strategies to help me figure it out. I have a list of
those who expressed confusion, 14 (66.67%) expressed frustration im- strategies. I’m gonna ask a teacher. I don’t feel that confident right now,
mediately following confusion. This difference in transition from con- I feel like it’s a little confusing for me. Um okay, I’m gonna break down
fusion to frustration was statistically significant, χ2 = 8.39, p = .003, the question and I’m also going to read the question again. So, the
which suggests that students in the control condition were not as ef- strategy that I’m gonna be using right now is ask a teacher.” Here, the
fective in regulating their confusion compared to students in the in- student experienced confusion and verbally acknowledged her state of
tervention condition. In the control condition, these expressions of confusion and reflected on how it made her feel. She then decided to
confusion to frustration were often cyclical in nature wherein students look at the worksheet and the list of learning strategies that were
toggled from confusion to frustration back to confusion, much like what provided to all students in the intervention condition.
Di Leo et al. (2019) and D’Mello and Graesser (2014) reported in their After asking for help, this student counted the squares in the garden.
research. “So, I know that there’s a hundred squares in all. So now I’m gonna
Regulating confusion: Control group. These findings may suggest work on the potatoes. I feel better now. So, it worked, my strategies
that students in the control condition may not have had the appropriate worked. Now I feel like I’m kind of relieved because now I’m able to get
strategies to overcome their confusion and, as such, experienced frus- past my problem.” This student monitored her emotions and evaluated
tration, especially when confusion could not be resolved. To illustrate, whether her strategy worked and how that made her feel. Two minutes
after reading the problem, one student in the control condition ex- later, this student expressed confusion again. “So, one fifth, maybe it’s a
pressed confusion at the onset of problem solving and then immediately half of 10? I think I’m confused. Let me rethink it. I know it’s a quarter
expressed frustration and boredom and a desire to disengage from the but I’m not sure what I’m supposed to do right now. Now I’m feeling
task: “Oh my God, oh my God, I don’t feel good about this. It looks hard. again confused. But this time, I’m not gonna ask a teacher, I’m gonna
I literally understand nothing. I don’t get it [confusion]. 10% of the use another strategy. I’m gonna break it down another way and think
garden?! This is complicated. Oh my goodness I literally understand about it another way.” Although confused, this student did not express
nothing. I really don’t want to do this, I don’t feel like doing it. Ugh! any other negative emotions and continued to solve the problem by
[frustration] Oh my God. In my opinion, I find this boring [boredom].” recalculating and evaluating her work. At the end of the problem, the
This student did not attempt to regulate her confusion by identifying student reflected on her problem solving: “I stopped and took a deep
the source of that confusion or what strategy she could use to resolve it. breath and told myself I have control. And for emotions, I was a little
Another student in the control condition also exhibited frustration proud of myself.” This student took the cue of feeling confused to then
and then a desire to give up after experiencing confusion: “I don’t even stop and think about how to overcome it and identified which strategies
know if I did it right” then told the research assistant “I don’t think I can might work best and evaluated whether those strategies helped her
finish it. I’m basically doing trial and error and I’m calculating but it’s resolve confusion. Indeed, there is evidence that students in the inter-
still not working.” This student restarted his calculations at every in- vention condition attempted a variety of different strategies once they
stance of confusion. Although recalculating is a learning strategy and expressed a state of confusion. What was particularly noteworthy is that
the student evaluated the efficacy of the strategy, this student continued students in the intervention condition asked a teacher for help a total of
to implement the same strategy despite its lack of success and then 10 times, whereas students in the intervention condition asked a tea-
wanted to stop working on the problem altogether. The research as- cher for help a total of 28 times. This suggests that students in the in-
sistant encouraged the student to continue. The student continued tervention condition took more ownership over their learning. In other
solving the problem and again expressed confusion and then frustra- words, they engaged in more self-regulated learning.
tion: “The cabbage, wait, did I put the cabbage? I don’t have turnips?? Another example of this was observed among a student who ex-
[confusion] What? Ugh!!! [frustration] Can I stop?” This example pressed confusion eight times while problem solving. The following
supports D’Mello and Graesser's (2012) model that confusion can be excerpt is one instance of her confused state, her reflection on her
unproductive when it persists or when the student does not feel capable emotional state, and the actions she took to overcome her confusion:
of managing or overcoming the impasse on their own. This pattern of “Wait, what am I doing? What am I doing? Okay now I’m just going
confusion-frustration-disengagement was observed among 14 students crazy. Okay so re-read was a good strategy, I just didn’t re-read prop-
in the control condition. erly. I’m feeling unorganized and this usually happens to me in all my
Regulating confusion: Intervention group. In contrast to students problems where it’s one part of the problem that messes up all my
in the control condition, when students in the intervention condition calculations and I’m confused and I don’t know what I’m gonna do.
experienced confusion, they were likely to acknowledge their state of Usually I’m in resource and they help me out, and I have other tools that
confusion and devise a plan of what to do next. These patterns were can help or I’d be standing next to the teacher with other students and
consistent with the instruction and is evidence of treatment uptake by we’d be helped, but I mean, now I can’t do that, so I have to re-read.”
the students in the intervention condition. Once confused, these This student acknowledged her confusion, monitored her emotions, and

12
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

identified one strategy to use. This student went on to successfully solve 5. Discussion
the problem.
Like many students in the intervention condition, this student also Confusion, a negative activating emotion, can be related to optimal
reflected on the emotions she experienced and the learning strategies achievement outcomes when students appropriately implement
she used after completion of the problem: “So, I’m done. I sometimes learning strategies to resolve the confusion (D’Mello et al., 2014).
felt, actually, usually I feel anxious. I felt anxious before the test. I felt a However, confusion can have detrimental effects on learning among
little frustrated, I have to be honest because at some points in the test I elementary students (e.g., Di Leo et al., 2019; Muis et al., 2015). Given
had trouble getting the answer. I felt confused a lot and I used the list of this, research calls have been made to teach elementary students ap-
strategies. So I did use the deep breath one, I did re-read, I asked an propriate skills to resolve their confusion when it occurs during
adult for help, I reviewed my work, and I broke down the question. 1, 2, learning (Di Leo et al., 2019; Muis et al., 2015). The purpose of this
3, 4, 5. I used 5 strategies. And now that it’s done, I feel proud.” This study was to examine the efficacy of a CEST intervention we developed
student reflected on her problem-solving experience and expressed to make students more aware of their emotional states during learning,
having experienced frustration as a result of having difficulty finding and to equip them with the self-regulated learning strategies they can
the correct answer. She also reflected on the fact that she experienced use to overcome confusion during mathematics problem solving
confusion and used numerous strategies to overcome it and evaluated through explicit instruction. Pekrun's (2006) control-value theory of
whether those strategies worked. achievement emotions, D’Mello and Graesser's (2012, 2014) frame-
Another student in the intervention condition read the problem and work, cognitive strategy instruction methods (e.g., Montague, 1992,
expressed some anxiety, “I’m not the best at fractions so I’m kind of 2008; Montague, Applegate, & Marquad, 1993) and cognitive beha-
scared.” She then reread and identified the important information in the vioral therapy (Montreuil & Tilley, 2017) served as the foundations
text and planned her next action, “I’ll start with what I know” and then from which to develop the intervention. Overall, results were consistent
the student wrote down the important information (i.e., the quantity of with hypotheses: students in the intervention condition scored higher
space in fractions that each vegetable takes up within a 10 × 10 grid), on the mathematics problem, engaged in more cognitive and meta-
which consisted of the fractions. She then stated the following: “I may cognitive learning strategies, experienced more positive and fewer ne-
have to use the strategy list after if I really get stuck” perhaps sug- gative emotions, and better regulated their confusion compared to
gesting that while she might be in a mild state of confusion, it was not students in the control condition. Each of these results are discussed in
sufficient to immediately implement a strategy on the strategy list. turn, followed by a discussion of broader educational implications. We
However, she did make a plan to consult the list if she felt it was ne- end with limitations and future directions.
cessary, thus engaging her metacognitive knowledge (knowing when to
use the strategy list). Later, as the student carried out her calculations, 5.1. Mathematics problem solving outcome
she expressed confusion again and identified the reason for her confu-
sion: “Now I’m doing the carrots, okay now I’m confused. Let me look at To our knowledge, this study is the first to incorporate theoretical
my strategies list. Take a deep breath (the student takes a deep breath). considerations of emotions into an intervention to promote self-regu-
Okay, so I need to figure out what I’m stuck on and why I’m confused. lated learning and learning outcomes among elementary students
So, I think I’m confused because I kind of forgot what 0.20 is.” during mathematics problem solving. Although emotions and cognitive
This student became confused again and stated: “I’m really not sure processes have been increasingly integrated within theoretical models
about the carrots, I don’t think that’s right. I really don’t know actually, of self-regulated learning (Efklides, 2011; Muis et al., 2018), they have
I’m feeling confused, like really confused.” This student’s confusion not been integrated in practice through interventions for mathematics
appeared to be significant enough as she once again looked at her list of learning and problem solving. As such, findings from this research fill a
possible strategies: “I’m gonna look at my strategies. Stop and take a gap in the literature. Unlike previous interventions designed to support
deep breath. I think I’m gonna re-do the calculations because I don’t middle school students’ strategy use and self-regulated learning during
think I did that right. So, let me try this again. I think I’ll do better this mathematics problem solving (e.g., Krawec, Huang, Montague,
time.” Here, the student experienced recurring confusion and stopped Kressler, & Melia de Alba, 2013; Montague, 2008; Perels, Dignath, &
to review the list of strategies, evaluated her work thus far, changed her Schmitz, 2009; Perels, Gürtler, & Schmitz, 2005), this intervention was
strategy of attacking the problem, and expressed a sense of hope for the developed to take into consideration the role that emotions play in
outcome. facilitating or constraining self-regulated learning. Moreover, in addi-
Similar to the previous student, once this student completed the tion to normalizing the experience of confusion, students were taught to
problem, she reflected on the emotions she experienced and the stra- reflect on both their cognitive processes and emotional experiences.
tegies that she implemented: “I’m done now. I felt surprised when I got This combined training led to higher scores on the mathematics pro-
the paper because I didn’t think it would be a lot of fractions. So, I was a blem among students who received the intervention compared to stu-
little bit curious. I was proud and relieved when it was over. I was really dents in the control condition.
confused. From my strategy list, I stopped and took a deep breath at the Indeed, what might have contributed to the positive results of this
beginning when I didn’t understand and figured out what I was stuck intervention was its integrated nature to support regulatory strategies
on. I re-read the problem, highlighted important information, sum- including the monitoring of cognitive processes and emotions. Previous
marized the important information, re-calculated, and checked and research has demonstrated that monitoring progress is a particularly
reviewed my work.” This final reflection was a common occurrence critical part of the self-regulated learning process (Winne & Perry,
among students in the intervention condition, perhaps as they had been 2000). The mechanisms that underlie self-regulation, whether it be for
monitoring their emotions and strategy use throughout problem sol- behavior, cognitions, learning, or emotions, are the same (e.g., Perry,
ving. This type of post-problem-solving reflection might be helpful for Hutchinson, Yee, & Määttä, 2018) and involve metacognition, moti-
students in developing self-regulated learning skills as they demonstrate vation, strategic action (Winne, 2018), and understanding, labelling,
the capacity to be aware of their problem-solving experience, how they and controlling emotions (Perry et al., 2018). Given this, incorporating
felt, what strategies they used, what worked, and what might not have emotional awareness into the intervention may have helped students to
worked. regulate their confusion, thereby freeing up more cognitive resources to
continue to implement the learning strategies necessary to succeed.

13
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

Students in the intervention condition were taught that when faced knowledge and identified more important information during the task
with confusion, they can purposefully select from their repertoire of definition phase of self-regulated learning, made more plans during the
strategies, when to apply them, to monitor their progress, and evaluate planning and goal-setting phase of learning, and hypothesized and
whether the strategies were effective. This intervention focussed on summarized more in the enactment phase of self-regulated learning
having students bring awareness to their emotional state and under- compared to students in the control condition. Finally, students in the
stand that emotions occur and are indeed part of learning and problem intervention condition monitored their progress more than students in
solving. Although directly controlling emotions (e.g., through cognitive the control condition. By training students to implement strategies
reappraisal or expressive suppression) was not a component of the in- across the various phases of self-regulated learning, this helped to foster
tervention, the intervention did promote the regulation of emotions better learning outcomes. Implementing more effective strategies to
indirectly as evidenced by the explanatory trend analysis whereby reduce confusion may have also helped to increase positive emotions
students recognized and labelled their confusion and regulated it and decrease negative emotions.
through applying learning strategies to resolve confusion.
Bringing one’s awareness and attention to one’s emotional state and 5.3. Emotions
to internal and external experiences of the present moment through
mindfulness techniques is an emerging area of research (Diamond & In support of our hypotheses, students in the intervention condition
Lee, 2011; Kabat-Zinn, 2003). Specifically, classroom- and school-based expressed more positive emotions and fewer negative emotions than
mindfulness interventions are gaining popularity as they have demon- students in the control condition. Specifically, students in the inter-
strated effectiveness in promoting students’ regulation for attention, vention condition expressed more curiosity, enjoyment, pride, and re-
inhibition, emotional regulation and overall emotional well-being and lief and less frustration than students in the control condition. The five
mental health (e.g., Britton et al., 2014; Flook et al., 2010; Greenberg & most frequently expressed emotions among students in the intervention
Harris, 2012; Kuyken et al., 2013; Van de Weijer-Bergsma, Formsma, condition were primarily positive emotions (i.e., three out of five)
Bruin, & Bögels, 2012). Such interventions are drawn from cognitive- whereas the five most frequently expressed emotions among students in
behavioral theoretical frameworks, which emphasize the reciprocal the control condition were all negative emotions. That is, the most
relationship between emotions and cognitions (Beck, 2011). As such, commonly occurring emotions in the intervention condition were
these interventions include mindful awareness of one’s emotional state, confusion (34.4%), pride (11.26%), curiosity, relief, and boredom (all
actively describing and labeling emotions, and engaging in regulating 7.95%). In contrast, the five most frequently occurring emotions in the
and managing the emotional state such as through cognitive reappraisal control condition were confusion (45.59%), frustration (19.12%),
or through mindfulness techniques including acceptance of the emotion boredom (11.76%), anxiety (6.62%), and hopelessness (4.41%). These
or thought and deep breathing (e.g., Burrows, 2017; Diamond & Lee, findings suggest that the intervention helped to promote positive
2011; Willis & Dinehart, 2014). Empirical evidence reveals that mind- emotions during mathematics problem solving.
fulness interventions have beneficial effects for students’ emotion-reg- These findings are particularly noteworthy. As discussed previously,
ulation skills and overall regulatory skills (e.g., Flook et al., 2010). it was not expected that students in the intervention condition would
Findings from our research provide preliminary evidence that this in- experience less confusion than students in the control condition.
tervention may promote the monitoring of emotions and learning Rather, it was expected that students would react differently to confu-
strategies during a specific academic task. sion when it arose in terms of implementation of strategies to resolve
confusion. Resolution of confusion more likely led to relief, joy, or other
5.2. Learning strategies positive emotions rather than more negative emotions like frustration
(Munzar et al., 2020). That is, we expected students in the intervention
Previous research on cognitive strategy instruction has demon- condition to be less likely to fall into a cyclical trap of confusion-frus-
strated the effectiveness of interventions designed to improve in- tration-boredom-disengagement, as has been reported in previous stu-
dependent and self-regulated use of strategies (for a review, see dies (Di Leo et al., 2019; D’Mello & Graesser, 2012) than students in the
MacArthur, 2012). Specifically, strategy instruction in the area of control condition. Our findings support previous research and provide
mathematics has been shown to increase the repertoire of effective evidence that this intervention was successful in helping students avoid
strategies and to improve strategic knowledge, application of strategies, the negative confusion-frustration loop.
and problem-solving performance and accuracy (Case et al., 1992; It may also be the case that, because the intervention focused on the
Krawec et al., 2013; Montague, 2008; Montague et al., 2011). Con- normalization of confusion and the ability to manage that confusion
sistent with this literature, our CEST intervention focused on teaching through implementation of learning strategies, when students did ex-
students a variety of strategies necessary to help them solve a complex perience confusion, they were more capable of regulating that emotion
mathematics problem. In addition to promoting self-regulated learning, compared to students in the control condition. Evidence from trend
our goal was to normalize the experience of confusion, and to bring analyses revealed that students in the intervention condition were
students’ attention to the kinds of learning strategies they could im- better able to regulate their confusion compared to students in the
plement when they experienced confusion. Results from our research control condition. As such, it is possible that by teaching students to be
provide evidence that direct instruction with modeling plus scaffolded more aware of their emotional states, when negative emotions arise,
practice improved students’ implementation of these strategies, parti- they might be more likely to take action to regulate them. Awareness is
cularly following confusion. the foundation of self-regulated learning (Muis, 2007) and it is likely
That is, results revealed that, across the four phases of self-regulated that the intervention helped promote awareness. Given that there were
learning, students in the intervention condition engaged in more cog- no significant differences in emotion regulation strategies between both
nitive and metacognitive learning strategies than those in the control groups prior to the intervention, it may the case that increased fre-
condition. This provides evidence that the intervention was beneficial quency of positive emotions in the intervention condition is related to
in students’ implementation of learning strategies during complex students’ regulation of confusion when it occurred. Overall, these
mathematics problem solving. Specifically, compared to the control findings indicate that the intervention promoted more positive emo-
condition, students in the intervention condition activated more prior tions and fewer negative emotions, with the exception of confusion.

14
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

With regard to confusion, results revealed no difference in expres- Saarni, 2003), emotion regulation (Garner & Hinton, 2010; Metz et al.,
sion of confusion between groups. This was not surprising as confusion 2013), and mindfulness (Mendelson et al., 2010; Schonert-Reichl &
is the result of a cognitive incongruity or impasse (D’Mello & Graesser, Lawlor, 2010; Schonert-Reichl, Lawlor, Abbott, Thomson, Oberlander,
2011, 2012). Because the mathematics problem was the same for both & Diamond, 2015; Zenner, Herrnleben-Kurz, & Walach, 2014). How-
groups, we expected that all students would experience relatively si- ever, these programs are often couched in social relations rather than
milar occurrences of confusion. Furthermore, differences were not ex- specifically during learning and are aimed at improving emotional de-
pected since this intervention was not meant to reduce the experience velopment, social-emotional functioning, and school readiness rather
of confusion during mathematics problem solving, but rather the ne- than improving the ability to regulate and manage one’s emotions that
gative emotions that are most likely to occur following confusion, such arise specifically from academic tasks. Our intervention takes this ap-
as frustration, boredom, and hopelessness. An important goal of the proach to becoming more aware of one’s emotions during a learning
intervention was to normalize the experience of confusion and to model context and focuses on helping students to manage those emotions
to students what learning strategies they could implement to resolve it. through enacting various learning strategies. As such, these broader
As previous research has shown, when confusion is resolved through curricular objectives could readily be refocused on specific learning
appropriate learning strategies, this leads to higher levels of enjoyment contexts with some teacher training for implementation.
and better learning outcomes (D’Mello et al., 2014; Munzar et al., Second, numerous school programs exist, particularly at the sec-
2020). ondary level, that teach strategies for general academic success and that
help students learn how to learn (e.g., Mind Pop Program - Mind, plan,
5.4. Relations between emotions and learning strategies organize, and prioritize; Clark & Leech-Pepin, 2017). Additionally, in the
province of Québec, mathematics problems are structured to help stu-
According to Pekrun (2006; Pekrun & Stephens, 2012), emotions dents activate prior knowledge, identify what they know from the
predict the types of cognitive and metacognitive learning strategies problem statements, evaluate what they need to do to solve the pro-
students use during learning and problem solving (Pekrun et al., 2007). blem, and justify their solutions. Although providing prompts for stu-
These emotions can hinder or facilitate learning (Baker, D’Mello, dents helps, not all students take advantage of these prompts or know
Rodrigo, & Graesser, 2010; D’Mello & Graesser, 2011; Pekrun, Goetz, how to use them (Muis et al., 2015). As such, students need direct and
Daniels, Stupinsky, & Perry, 2010). That is, positive emotions, such as explicit instruction with modeling, and practice with scaffolding to
enjoyment and curiosity, can facilitate learning (Pekrun, Elliot, & support the development of these skills (Carnine, 1997; Perry et al.,
Maier, 2009) whereas negative emotions, like boredom and frustration, 2018). Although some teachers engage in direct instruction of learning
can hinder learning (Pekrun et al., 2011). These relations are said to strategies to support the development of self-regulated learning, not all
occur through self-regulatory processes including motivation and cog- teachers do this (Perry, Brenner, & MacPherson, 2015). Teaching stu-
nitive and metacognitive learning strategies (Op't Eynde et al., 2007; dents strategies for learning is a complex task for teachers, and they
Pekrun, Goetz, Titz, & Perry, 2002). need to be supported in this regard (Perry et al., 2018). As Coburn and
Although Pekrun (2006; Pekrun & Stephens, 2012) proposed that Penuel (2016) suggested, one way to support teachers’ implementation
emotions predict learning strategies, it could also be the case that of self-regulated learning-promoting practices is through research-
teaching students the appropriate learning strategies to use when spe- practice partnerships with teachers.
cific emotions arise facilitates more positive emotions after resolution Finally, a unique feature of this research was that it was conducted
of negative emotions. In this way, patterns of relations between emo- in an authentic learning environment, i.e., in the students’ regular
tions and learning strategies may be more reciprocal than linear. The classrooms. Many interventions that aim to promote self-regulated
positive increase in emotions in the intervention condition could have learning to improve mathematics problem solving have taken place
been a function of successful implementation of the learning strategies outside the classroom with little connection made to the natural setting
students were taught. If students experienced greater success due to of students’ regular courses and classrooms (e.g., Perels et al., 2005).
these strategies, this could increase the experience of positive emotions. This has implications on the problem-solving skills students transfer
Reciprocally, an increase in positive emotions may have also increased from one context to another (Mayer & Wittrock, 1996). Implementing
use of those strategies, resulting in a positive feedback loop in the in- such an intervention within a classroom setting strengthens the ecolo-
tervention condition. Future research could test this hypothesis via an gical validity of our results to the students’ everyday academic lives and
in-depth analysis of sequential relations between emotions and learning suggests a promising future for this intervention that can be integrated
strategies (Di Leo et al., 2019). Indeed, the increase in cognitive and into the curricula.
metacognitive processes and positive emotions may have been one key
factor in the increase in students’ problem-solving outcomes in the in- 5.6. Limitations and future directions
tervention condition compared to students in the control condition.
Regardless of the mechanisms responsible for the increase in learning Although the results of the current study contribute to the literature
outcomes with the intervention group, our CEST intervention is some- on improving problem-solving and self-regulated learning skills in
thing that teachers could easily integrate into their curriculum. We elementary students, it is important to consider the limitations of this
discuss the educational implications next. research study. The first limitation pertains to the length of the inter-
vention. Although the present intervention was effective at demon-
5.5. Educational implications strating immediate results, it was conducted within one week and is
considered a brief intervention. Other cognitive strategy instruction
There are several educational implications of this research. First, it interventions have ranged in length between less than one week to over
is important to recognize that many school- and classroom-based pro- one month (see Xin & Jitendra, 1999). This was not a longitudinal
grams aim to promote emotional awareness and the development of design and the intervention was completed within one week, thus the
emotion regulation at the elementary and secondary school levels (e.g., maintenance of these skills was not assessed at multiple time points
Broderick & Frank, 2014; Metz et al., 2013). Many programs focus on after the intervention. It is possible that these skills might not be
recognizing emotions and emotional competence (Buckley, Storino, & maintained over time without practice and teacher support. Therefore,

15
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

future studies could investigate the length of time that is ideal for such (e.g., Perels, Merget-Kullman, Wende, Schmitz, & Buchbinder, 2009),
training among inclusive classrooms and assess the maintenance of the there continues to be a lack of empirically-tested training programs
intervention over time. (Dignath & Büttner, 2008). Therefore, future research might endeavour
Another limitation is that there was no within-subject analysis for to design and test teacher training and professional development pro-
pre- and post-intervention time points. Although significant differences grams aimed at training teachers to promote self-regulation of learning
were found between groups, we were unable to determine whether and academic emotions as students solve complex problems.
there were intervention effects at the individual level. It would be in- Finally, one last limitation of our study is the sample size. Although
teresting to identify the extent to which an individual improved based results from our study hold promise, much larger samples with a greater
on the intervention. This could reveal aspects of the intervention that variety of students is needed to more rigorously evaluate the efficacy of
are effective or perhaps not effective for students with certain learning our intervention. Until we replicate these results with larger samples,
profiles. In addition, assessment of cognitive and emotional regulation our results must be interpreted with caution. Although our sample was
strengths and weaknesses at the individual level would be beneficial to heterogeneous in terms of gender, SES, and race, it was predominantly
determine what skills or deficits the students currently possess and white and limited to a bilingual context, thereby limiting the general-
whether deficits improve post-intervention. Pre-intervention assess- izability of the results to these specific population characteristics.
ment could determine such strengths and weaknesses, and the pre- and Future research should expand on the range of students sampled, types
post-intervention within-subject analysis could provide information of complex mathematics problems used, and grades who participate.
regarding specific factors that helped contribute to better self-regulated With some refining, our intervention could be modified for slightly
learning strategies and better learning outcomes. Furthermore, it would younger and older students to benefit a broader range of grades.
be interesting to assess how each student engaged in problem solving
before the intervention and determine whether their overall strategies 6. Conclusion
improved. Even though the think-emote-aloud provided rich informa-
tion, it would be beneficial to interview students after they solved the Collectively, these findings provide insight into cognitive-emotional
problem. Students could be directly asked how they dealt with the strategy instruction and the need for explicitly teaching young students
confusion they experienced, how they think it affected them, and how self-regulated learning strategies to overcome their confusion. Previous
they felt being confused. This could help researchers gain more un- interventions focused solely on the cognitive component of problem
derstanding of the role of confusion during learning. solving and excluded the emotional components. This study is the first
Additionally, a researcher delivered this intervention within a to include emotions and target confusion within an intervention for
classroom setting. As noted above, teachers’ role in promoting self- mathematics problem solving among elementary students. However,
regulated learning is critical (Azevedo, Moos, Greene, Winters, & given that there were only two conditions, i.e., intervention and con-
Cromley, 2008; Paris & Paris, 2001), thus future work should train trol, more work is needed to better assess how much benefit the current
teachers on how to improve students’ self-regulation of emotions and intervention, i.e., cognitive-emotional strategy instruction, has over
learning during mathematics problem solving. It would be ideal for cognitive strategy training or emotional awareness/regulation training
teachers to continuously model self-regulated learning strategies and alone. As such, it is recommended that future research include three
emotional processes, model monitoring and labelling of their emotions experimental conditions, i.e., cognitive-emotional strategy instruction,
as they experience them while engaging in an academic task such as cognitive strategy instruction alone, and emotional awareness/regula-
mathematics problem solving. Teachers’ modeling of emotion regula- tion training alone. Nevertheless, this research adds to the current lit-
tion through emoting aloud during problem solving tasks can help erature on relations between emotions and self-regulated learning and
promote students’ emotional awareness during academic tasks. This broadens the literature on cognitive strategy instruction as it in-
may then help students understand and use appropriate language for corporates emotions. This intervention helped to promote the use of
the emotions they experience. Continuously thinking and emoting cognitive and metacognitive strategies within a classroom context,
aloud and modeling may promote students’ understanding of how which is critical for self-regulated learning intervention efficacy (Hattie,
emotions and learning strategies play a role in learning. Biggs, & Purdie, 1996). The focus of the intervention was to teach
However, the quality and effectiveness of the training is dependent students how to be active learners and conscious of their emotions and
on the individual who is teaching the skills (Dignath, Büttner, & progress during problem solving. The results of this study extend pre-
Langfeldt, 2008). Findings from meta-analyses revealed that the effect vious findings of strategy instruction and offers suggestions for fur-
sizes of self-regulated learning training programs were smaller when thering exploration in this field. It is important that in moving forward,
teachers trained students than when researchers trained students pedagogical design and academic interventions are affectively suppor-
(Dignath & Büttner, 2008; Dignath et al., 2008). Taken together, even tive and promote active awareness and mindfulness of academic emo-
though cognitive strategy instruction is beneficial for students, it cannot tions and learning strategies.
be assumed that teachers implicitly know how to provide this type of
training and know what and how to model the specific skills. Indeed, Funding
research on teacher beliefs, knowledge, and classroom practice of self-
regulated learning reveal that there are gaps in teachers’ knowledge, Funding for this work was provided by a grant to Krista R. Muis
personal practice, and classroom practice of self-regulated learning from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada
(e.g., Dignath-van Ewijk & van der Werf, 2012; Spruce & Bol, 2015). (435-2014-0155), and from the Canada Research Chair program.
Therefore, teachers likely need to receive explicit and direct training on Correspondence concerning this article can be addressed to Krista R.
how to teach young students skills to promote the monitoring of their Muis, Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, Faculty
emotions and self-regulated learning skills. Although there are teacher- of Education, McGill University, 3700 McTavish Street, Montreal, QC,
training programs to promote self-regulated learning in the classroom H3A 1Y2, or via email at krista.muis@mcgill.ca.

16
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

Appendix A

17
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

18
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

References externally-facilitated regulated learning more effective than self-regulated learning


with hypermedia? Educational Technology Research and Development, 56(1), 45–72.
Baker, R. S., D’Mello, S. K., Rodrigo, Ma. M. T., & Graesser, A. C. (2010). Better to be
Azevedo, R., Moos, D. C., Greene, J. A., Winters, F. I., & Cromley, J. G. (2008). Why is frustrated than bored: The incidence, persistence, and impact of learners’

19
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

cognitive–affective states during interactions with three different computer-based Learning and Instruction, 22(2), 145–157.
learning environments. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 68(4), D'Mello, S., & Graesser, A. (2014). Confusion and its dynamics during device compre-
223–241. hension with breakdown scenarios. Acta Psychologica, 151, 106–116.
Baroody, A. J., & Dowker, A. (2013). The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: D’Mello, S., Lehman, B., Pekrun, R., & Graesser, A. (2014). Confusion can be beneficial for
Constructive adaptive expertise. New York, NY: Routledge. learning. Learning and Instruction, 29, 153–170.
Barrett, L. F. (2009). Variety is the spice of life: A psychological construction approach to Donker, A. S., de Boer, H., Kostons, D., Dignath van Ewijk, C. C., & van der Werf, M. P. C.
understanding variability in emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 23(7), 1284–1306. (2014). Effectiveness of learning strategy instruction on academic performance: A
Beck, J. S. (2011). Cognitive behavior therapy: Basics and beyond. New York, NY: Guilford meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 11, 1–26.
Press. Dowd, J., Araujo, I., & Mazur, E. (2015). Making sense of confusion: Relating perfor-
Berking, M., & Wupperman, P. (2012). Emotion regulation and mental health: Recent mance, confidence, and self-efficacy to expressions of confusion in an introductory
findings, current challenges, and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 25, physics class. Physical Review Special Topics - Physics Education Research, 11(1),
128–134. https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevSTPER.11.010107.
Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological Eccles, J., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., & Blumenfeld, P. (1993). Age and gender differ-
conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, ences in children's self-and task perceptions during elementary school. Child
57(2), 111–127. Development, 64(3), 830–847.
Boden, M. T., & Thompson, R. J. (2015). Facets of emotional awareness and associations Efklides, A. (2011). Interactions of metacognition with motivation and affect in self-
with emotion regulation and depression. Emotion, 15(3), 399–410. regulated learning: The MASRL model. Educational Psychologist, 46(1), 6–25.
Boekaerts, M. (1997). Self-regulated learning: A new concept embraced by researchers, Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1998). How to study thinking in everyday life:
policy makers, educators, teachers, and students. Learning and Instruction, 7(2), Contrasting think-aloud protocols with descriptions and explanations of thinking.
161–186. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 5(3), 178–186.
Britton, W. B., Lepp, N. E., Niles, H. F., Rocha, T., Fisher, N. E., & Gold, J. S. (2014). A Earl, S. R., Taylor, I. M., Meijen, C., & Passfield, L. (2017). Autonomy and competence
randomized controlled pilot trial of classroom-based mindfulness meditation com- frustration in young adolescent classrooms: Different associations with active and
pared to an active control condition in sixth-grade children. Journal of School passive disengagement. Learning and Instruction, 49, 32–40.
Psychology, 52(3), 263–278. Eva-Wood, A. L. (2004). Thinking and feeling poetry: Exploring meanings aloud. Journal
Broderick, P. C., & Frank, J. L. (2014). Learning to BREATHE: An intervention to foster of Educational Psychology, 96(1), 182.
mindfulness in adolescence. New Directions for Youth Development, 142, 31–44. Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., & Goetz, T. (2007). Girls and mathematics—A “hopeless” issue?
Buckley, M., Storino, M., & Saarni, C. (2003). Promoting emotional competence in chil- A control-value approach to gender differences in emotions towards mathematics.
dren and adolescents: Implications for school psychologists. School Psychology European Journal of Psychology of Education, 22(4), 497–514.
Quarterly, 18(2), 177–191. Fried, L. (2011). Teaching teachers about emotion regulation in the classroom. Australian
Burrows, L. (2017). Safeguarding mindfulness in schools and higher education: A holistic and Journal of Teacher Education (Online), 36(3), 1–11.
inclusive approach. London, England: Routledge. Fried, L. (2010). Understanding and enhancing emotion and motivation regulation
Carnine, D. (1997). Instructional design in mathematics for students with learning dis- strategy use in the classroom. International Journal of Learning, 17(6), 115–129.
abilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30(2), 130–141. Flook, L., Smalley, S. L., Kitil, M. J., Galla, B. M., Kaiser-Greenland, S., Locke, J., ... Kasari,
Case, L. P., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (1992). Improving the mathematical problem- C. (2010). Effects of mindful awareness practices on executive functions in elemen-
solving skills of students with learning disabilities: Self-regulated strategy develop- tary school children. Journal of Applied School Psychology, 26(1), 70–95.
ment. The Journal of Special Education, 26(1), 1–19. Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., Compton, D. L., Powell, S. R., Seethaler, P. M., Capizzi, A. M., ...
Cassel, J., & Reid, R. (1996). Use of a self-regulated strategy intervention to improve word Fletcher, J. M. (2006). The cognitive correlates of third-grade skill in arithmetic,
problem-solving skills of students with mild disabilities. Journal of Behavioral algorithmic computation, and arithmetic word problems. Journal of Educational
Education, 6(2), 153–172. Psychology, 98(1), 29.
Chinn, C., & Brewer, W. (1993). The role of anomalous data in knowledge acquisition: A Galla, B. M., Wood, J. J., Chiu, A. W., Langer, D. A., Jacobs, J., et al. (2012). One year
theoretical framework and implications for science instruction. Review of Educational follow-up on modular cognitive behavior therapy for the treatment of pediatric an-
Research, 63(1), 1–49. https://doi.org/10.2307/1170558. xiety disorders in an elementary school setting. Child Psychiatry & Human
Clark, A., & Leech-Pepin, S. (2017). Mind Pop: Mindful, plan, organize, prioritize. Development, 43(2), 219–226.
Strategies for student success. Retrieved from http://www.royalwestacademy.com/ Garner, P. W., & Hinton, T. S. (2010). Emotional display rules and emotion self-regula-
wp-content/uploads/2017/09/SOS-MindPOP-info-Package.pdf. tion: Associations with bullying and victimization in community-based after school
Coburn, C. E., & Penuel, W. R. (2016). Research–practice partnerships in education: programs. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 20(6), 480–496.
Outcomes, dynamics, and open questions. Educational Researcher, 45(1), 48–54. Goldin, G. A. (2014). Perspectives on emotion in mathematical engagement, learning, and
Coffey, E., Berenbaum, H., & Kerns, J. (2003). Brief report. Cognition and Emotion, 17(4), problem solving. In R. Pekrun, & L. Linnenbrink-Garcia (Eds.). International handbook
671–679. of emotions in education (pp. 401–424). New York, NY: Routledge.
Cragg, L., & Gilmore, C. (2014). Skills underlying mathematics: The role of executive Gohm, L. C., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Four latent traits of emotional experience and their
function in the development of mathematics proficiency. Trends in Neuroscience and involvement in well-being, coping, and attributional style. Cognition and Emotion,
Education, 3(2), 63–68. 16(4), 495–518.
Craig, S. D., D'Mello, S., Witherspoon, A., & Graesser, A. (2008). Emote aloud during Graesser, A., Chipman, P., King, B., McDaniel, B., & D'Mello, S. (2007). Emotions and
learning with AutoTutor: Applying the Facial Action Coding System to cogniti- learning with auto tutor. Frontiers in Artificial Intelligence and Applications, 158,
ve–affective states during learning. Cognition and Emotion, 22(5), 777–788. 569–571.
Craig, S., Graesser, A., Sullins, J., & Gholson, B. (2004). Affect and learning: An ex- Graesser, A., & D’Mello, S. (2012). Emotions during the learning of difficult material. The
ploratory look into the role of affect in learning with AutoTutor. Journal of Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 57, 183–225. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-
Educational Media, 29(3), 241–250. 12-394293-7.00005-4.
Davidson, J. E., & Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Smart problem solving: How metacognition Greenberg, M. T., & Harris, A. R. (2012). Nurturing mindfulness in children and youth:
helps. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.). Metacognition in educa- Current state of research. Child Development Perspectives, 6(2), 161–166.
tional theory and practice (pp. 47–68). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc. Greene, J. A., & Azevedo, R. (2009). A macro-level analysis of SRL processes and their
De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., & Op't Eynde, P. (2000). Self-regulation: A characteristic and relations to the acquisition of a sophisticated mental model of a complex system.
a goal of mathematics education. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.). Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34(1), 18–29.
Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 687–726). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences.
Denham, S., & Kochanoff, A. T. (2002). Parental contributions to preschoolers' under- Psychophysiology, 39(3), 281–291.
standing of emotion. Marriage & Family Review, 34(3–4), 311–343. Gross, J. J., Richards, J. M., & John, O. P. (2006). Emotion Regulation in Everyday Life.
Di Leo, I., Muis, K. R., Singh, C., & Psaradellis, C. (2019). Curiosity... Confusion? In D.
Frustration! The role and sequencing of emotions during mathematics problem sol- Gross, J. J., Sheppes, G., & Urry, H. L. (2011). Emotion generation and emotion regula-
ving. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 58, 121–137. tion: A distinction we should make (carefully). Cognition and emotion (Print), 25(5),
Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function develop- 765–781.
ment in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333(6045), 959–964. Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion Regulation: Conceptual Foundations. In
Dignath, C., & Büttner, G. (2008). Components of fostering self-regulated learning among J. J. Gross (Ed.). Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 3–24). The: Guilford Press.
students. A meta-analysis on intervention studies at primary and secondary school Gullone, E., & Taffe, J. (2012). The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire for Children and
level. Metacognition and Learning, 3(3), 231–264. Adolescents (ERQ–CA): A psychometric evaluation. Psychological Assessment, 24(2),
Dignath, C., Büttner, G., & Langfeldt, H. P. (2008). How can primary school students learn 409.
self-regulated learning strategies most effectively?: A meta-analysis on self-regulation Hattie, J., Biggs, J., & Purdie, N. (1996). Effects of learning skills interventions on student
training programmes. Educational Research Review, 3(2), 101–129. learning: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 66(2), 99–136.
Dignath-van Ewijk, C., & van der Werf, G. (2012). What teachers think about self-regu- Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Masek, B., Henin, A., Blakely, L. R., Pollock-Wurman, R. A.,
lated learning: Investigating teacher beliefs and teacher behavior of enhancing stu- McQuade, J., ... Biederman, J. (2010). Cognitive behavioral therapy for 4-to 7-year-
dents’ self-regulation. Education Research International, 4, 1–10. old children with anxiety disorders: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting
D'Mello, S. K., Craig, S. D., Sullins, J., & Graesser, A. C. (2006). Predicting affective states and Clinical Psychology, 78(4), 498.
expressed through an emote-aloud procedure from AutoTutor's mixed-initiative dia- Jacobse, A. E., & Harskamp, E. G. (2012). Towards efficient measurement of metacog-
logue. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 16(1), 3–28. nition in mathematical problem solving. Metacognition and Learning, 7(2), 133–149.
D'Mello, S., & Graesser, A. (2011). The half-life of cognitive-affective states during Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and
complex learning. Cognition and Emotion, 25(7), 1299–1308. future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 144–156.
D’Mello, S., & Graesser, A. (2012). Dynamics of affective states during complex learning. Kendall, P. C. (1994). Treating anxiety disorders in youth: Results of a randomized

20
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 100–110. Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2009). Achievement goals and achievement
Kramarski, B., & Mevarech, Z. R. (2003). Enhancing mathematical reasoning in the emotions: Testing a model of their joint relations with academic performance. Journal
classroom: The effects of cooperative learning and metacognitive training. American of Educational Psychology, 101(1), 115–135.
Educational Research Journal, 40(1), 281–310. Pekrun, R., Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., & Perry, R. P. (2007). The control-value theory of
Krawec, J., Huang, J., Montague, M., Kressler, B., & Melia de Alba, A. (2013). The effects achievement emotions: An integrative approach to emotions in education. In P. A.
of cognitive strategy instruction on knowledge of math problem-solving processes of Schutz, & R. Pekrun (Eds.). Emotion in education (pp. 13–36). San Diego, CA: Elsevier
middle school students with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 36(2), Academic Press.
80–92. Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Daniels, L., Stupinsky, R. H., & Perry, R. (2010). Boredom in
Kroesbergen, E. H., & Van Luit, J. E. (2003). Mathematics interventions for children with achievement settings: Exploring control-value antecedents and performance out-
special educational needs: A meta-analysis. Remedial and Special Education, 24(2), comes of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 531–549.
97–114. Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Frenzel, A. C., Barchfeld, P., & Perry, R. P. (2011). Measuring
Kuyken, W., Weare, K., Ukoumunne, O. C., Vicary, R., Motton, N., Burnett, R., ... Huppert, emotions in students’ learning and performance: The Achievement Emotions
F., et al. (2013). Effectiveness of the Mindfulness in Schools Programme: Non-ran- Questionnaire (AEQ). Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36(1), 36–48.
domised controlled feasibility study. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 203(2), Pekrun, R., Goetz, T., Titz, W., & Perry, R. P. (2002). Academic emotions in students' self-
126–131. regulated learning and achievement: A program of qualitative and quantitative re-
Linnenbrink, E. A. (2007). The role of affect in student learning: A multi-dimensional search. Educational Psychologist, 37(2), 91–105.
approach to considering the interaction of affect, motivation, and engagement. In P. Pekrun, R., & Stephens, E. J. (2012). Academic emotions. In K. Harris, S. Graham, & T.
A. Schutz, & R. Pekrun (Eds.). Emotion in education (pp. 107–124). San Diego, CA: Urdan (Eds.). APA Educational Psychology Handbook: Individual differences and cultural
Academic Press. and contextual factors (pp. 3–31). Washington, DC: American Psychological
MacArthur, C. A. (2012). Strategies instruction. In K. R. Harris, S. Graham, & T. Urdan Association.
(Eds.). Educational psychology handbook Application to learning and teaching (pp. 379– Perels, F., Dignath, C., & Schmitz, B. (2009). Is it possible to improve mathematical
401). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. achievement by means of self-regulation strategies? Evaluation of an intervention in
Maccini, P., & Hughes, C. A. (2000). Effects of a problem-solving strategy on the in- regular math classes. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 24(1), 17–31.
troductory algebra performance of secondary students with learning disabilities. Perels, F., Gürtler, T., & Schmitz, B. (2005). Training of self-regulatory and problem-
Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 15(1), 10–21. solving competence. Learning and Instruction, 15(2), 123–139.
Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T. E., & Shiah, S. (1991). Mathematics instruction for Perels, F., Merget-Kullmann, M., Wende, M., Schmitz, B., & Buchbinder, C. (2009).
learning disabled students: A review of research. Learning Disabilities Research & Improving self-regulated learning of preschool children: Evaluation of training for
Practice, 6(2), 89–98. kindergarten teachers. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79(2), 311–327.
Mauss, I. B., & Robinson, M. D. (2009). Measures of emotion: A review. Cognition and Perry, N. E., Brenner, C. A., & MacPherson, N. (2015). Using teacher learning teams as a
Emotion, 23(2), 209–237. framework for bridging theory and practice in self-regulated learning. In T. Cleary
Mayer, R. E., & Wittrock, M. C. (1996). Problem-solving transfer. In D. C. Berliner, & R. C. (Ed.). Self-regulated learning interventions with at-risk youth: Enhancing adaptability,
Calfee (Eds.). Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 47–62). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence performance, and well-being (pp. 229–250). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Erlbaum Associates Inc. Association.
McCrudden, M. T., Marchand, G., & Schutz, P. (2019). Mixed methods in educational Perry, N. E., Hutchinson, L. R., Yee, N., & Määttä, E. (2018). Advances in studying
psychology inquiry. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 57, 1–8. classrooms as contexts for supporting young children’s self-regulation and self-
Melnick, S. M., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2000). Emotion regulation and parenting in AD/HD and regulated learning. In D. Schunk, & J. Greene (Eds.). Handbook of Learning and
comparison boys: Linkages with social behaviors and peer preference. Journal of Performance. New York, NY: Routledge.
abnormal child psychology, 28(1), 73–86. Perry, R. P., Hladkyi, S., Pekrun, R., & Pelletier, S. (2001). Academic control and action
Mendelson, T., Greenberg, M. T., Dariotis, J. K., Gould, L. F., Rhoades, B. L., & Leaf, P. J. control in college students: A longitudinal study of self-regulation. Journal of
(2010). Feasibility and preliminary outcomes of a school-based mindfulness inter- Educational Psychology, 93, 776–789.
vention for urban youth. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(7), 985–994. Pincus, D. B., & Friedman, A. G. (2004). Improving children’s coping with everyday stress:
Mercer, C. D., & Miller, S. P. (1992). Teaching students with learning problems in math to Transporting treatment interventions to the school setting. Clinical Child and Family
acquire, understand, and apply basic math facts. Remedial and Special Education, Psychology Review, 7(4), 223–240.
13(3), 19–35. Quoidbach, J., Mikolajczak, M., & Gross, J. J. (2015). Positive interventions: An emotion
Metz, S. M., Frank, J. L., Reibel, D., Cantrell, T., Sanders, R., & Broderick, P. C. (2013). regulation perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 141(3), 655–693.
The effectiveness of the learning to BREATHE program on adolescent emotion reg- Raccanello, D., Brondino, M., Moè, A., Stupnisky, R., & Lichtenfeld, S. (2019). Enjoyment,
ulation. Research in Human Development, 10(3), 252–272. boredom, anxiety in elementary schools in two domains: Relations with achievement.
Montague, M. (1992). The effects of cognitive and metacognitive strategy instruction on Journal of Experimental Education, 87(3), 449–469.
the mathematical problem solving of middle school students with learning dis- Rosenshine, B. V. (1997, March). The case for explicit, teacher-led, cognitive strategy
abilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25(4), 230–248. instruction. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association,
Montague, M. (2008). Self-regulation strategies to improve mathematical problem solving Chicago, IL. Retrieved January 2019, from http://www.formapex.com/
for students with learning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 31(1), 37–44. telechargementpublic/rosenshine1997a.pdf.
Montague, M., Applegate, B., & Marquard, K. (1993). Cognitive strategy instruction and Scherer, K. R. (2000). Psychological models of emotion. In J. C. Borod (Ed.). The neu-
mathematical problem-solving performance of students with learning disabilities. ropsychology of emotion (pp. 37–62). New York, NY: Oxford University Press Inc.
Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 8(4), 223–232. Scherer, K. R. (2009). The dynamic architecture of emotion: Evidence for the component
Montague, M., Enders, C., & Dietz, S. (2011). Effects of cognitive strategy instruction on process model. Cognition and Emotion, 23(7), 1307–1351.
math problem solving of middle school students with learning disabilities. Learning Schoenfeld, A. H. (1994). Reflections on doing and teaching mathematics. In A. H.
Disability Quarterly, 34(4), 262–272. Schoenfeld (Ed.). Mathematical thinking and problem solving (pp. 53–70). New York,
Montreuil, T. C., & Tilley, M. (2017). Healthy minds, healthy schools: Strategies and activities NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.
for happy and successful learners. Toronto, ON: Canadian Scholars. Schonert-Reichl, K. A., & Lawlor, M. S. (2010). The effects of a mindfulness-based edu-
Muis, K. R. (2007). The role of epistemic beliefs in self-regulated learning. Educational cation program on pre-and early adolescents’ well-being and social and emotional
Psychologist, 42(3), 173–190. competence. Mindfulness, 1(3), 137–151.
Muis, K. R., Chevrier, M., & Singh, C. A. (2018). The role of epistemic emotions in per- Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Oberle, E., Lawlor, M. S., Abbott, D., Thomson, K., Oberlander, T.
sonal epistemology and self-regulated learning. Educational Psychologist, 53(3), F., et al. (2015). Enhancing cognitive and social–emotional development through a
165–184. simple-to-administer mindfulness-based school program for elementary school chil-
Muis, K. R., Psaradellis, C., Lajoie, S. P., Di Leo, I., & Chevrier, M. (2015). The role of dren: A randomized controlled trial. Developmental Psychology, 51(1), 52–66.
epistemic emotions in mathematics problem solving. Contemporary Educational Schraw, G., Crippen, K. J., & Hartley, K. (2006). Promoting self-regulation in science
Psychology, 42, 172–185. education: Metacognition as part of a broader perspective on learning. Research in
Munzar, B., Muis, K. R., Denton, C. A., & Losenno, K. (2020). Elementary students’ cog- Science Education, 36(1–2), 111–139.
nitive and affective responses to impasses during mathematics problem solving. Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2007). Influencing children’s self-efficacy and self-
Journal of Educational Psychology. https://doi-org.proxy3.library.mcgill.ca/10.1037/ regulation of reading and writing through modeling. Reading & Writing Quarterly,
edu0000460. 23(1), 7–25.
Op't Eynde, P., De Corte, E., & Verschaffel, L. (2007). Students' emotions: A key com- Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (1997). Developing self-efficacious readers and wri-
ponent of self-regulated learning? In P. A. Schutz, & R. Pekrun (Eds.). Emotion in ters: The role of social and self-regulatory processes. In J. T. Guthrie, & A. Wigfield
education (pp. 185–204). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press. (Eds.). Reading engagement: Motivating readers through integrated instruction (pp. 34–
Pape, S. J., & Smith, C. (2002). Self-regulating mathematics skills. Theory into Practice, 50). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.
41(2), 93–101. Shuman, V., & Scherer, K. R. (2014). Concepts and structures of emotions. In R. Pekrun, &
Paris, S. G., & Paris, A. H. (2001). Classroom applications of research on self-regulated L. Linnenbrink-Garcia (Eds.). International handbook of emotions in education (pp. 13–
learning. Educational Psychologist, 36(2), 89–101. 35). New York, NY: Routledge.
Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: Assumptions, Silverman, W. K., Kurtines, W. M., Ginsburg, G. S., Weems, C. F., Lumpkin, P. W., &
corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educational Carmichael, D. H. (1999). Treating anxiety disorders with group cognitive-behavioral
Psychology Review, 18(4), 315–341. therapy: A randomized clinical trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
Pekrun, R. (2018). Control-value theory: A social-cognitive approach to achievement 67(6), 995–1003.
emotions. In G. A. D. Liem & D. M. McInerney (Eds.), Big theories revisited 2: A Spruce, R., & Bol, L. (2015). Teacher beliefs, knowledge, and practice of self-regulated
volume of research on sociocultural influences on motivation and learning (pp. 162- learning. Metacognition and Learning, 10(2), 245–277.
190). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing. Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using Multivariate Statistics (6th ed.). Boston, MA:

21
I. Di Leo and K.R. Muis Contemporary Educational Psychology 62 (2020) 101879

Allyn Bacon. learning. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(1), 9–20.


Van de Weijer-Bergsma, E., Formsma, A. R., de Bruin, E. I., & Bögels, S. M. (2012). The Winne, P. H., Jamieson-Noel, D., & Muis, K. R. (2002). Methodological issues and ad-
effectiveness of mindfulness training on behavioral problems and attentional func- vances in researching tactics, strategies, and self-regulated learning. In P. R. Pintrich,
tioning in adolescents with ADHD. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 21(5), & M. L. Maehr (Vol. Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement: New directions in
775–787. measures and methods. 12. Advances in motivation and achievement: New directions in
Verschaffel, L., De Corte, E., Lasure, S., Van Vaerenbergh, G., Bogaerts, H., & Ratinckx, E. measures and methods (pp. 121–155). Bingley, England: Emerald Publishing Limited.
(1999). Learning to solve mathematical application problems: A design experiment Winne, P. H., & Perry, N. E. (2000). Measuring self-regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P.
with fifth graders. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 1(3), 195–229. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.). Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 531–566). San Diego,
Weare, K. (2004). Developing the emotionally literate school. London, UK: Paul Chapman CA: Academic Press.
Publishing A SAGE Publications Company. Xin, Y. P., & Jitendra, A. K. (1999). The effects of instruction in solving mathematical
Willis, E., & Dinehart, L. H. (2014). Contemplative practices in early childhood: word problems for students with learning problems: A meta-analysis. The Journal of
Implications for self-regulation skills and school readiness. Early Child Development Special Education, 32(4), 207–225.
and Care, 184(4), 487–499. Zenner, C., Herrnleben-Kurz, S., & Walach, H. (2014). Mindfulness-based interventions in
Winne, P. H. (2018). Theorizing and researching levels of processing in self-regulated schools—a systematic review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(603), 1–20.

22

You might also like