You are on page 1of 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/21692780

Fine Motor Activities in Elementary School: Preliminary Findings and


Provisional Implications for Children With Fine Motor Problems

Article  in  The American journal of occupational therapy.: official publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association · November 1992
DOI: 10.5014/ajot.46.10.898 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

364 1,243

2 authors, including:

Sharon Cermak
University of Southern California
206 PUBLICATIONS   7,251 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Sensory Processing in ASD View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Sharon Cermak on 01 June 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Fine Motor Activities in ine rl1owr:,kills mJy h<.:con-,ide.:n:d e.:nahling bella\'·

Elementary School: F ioni and may affect the child in school 10 se.:ver:ll
waF;. They may influence the quality and quantity
of the.: child'~ learn;;lg ,wd achievement in the da~~room.
the validity of a~se~smcnt~ for in~tructi()nal planning.
Preliminary Findings educational placement and eligibility for service~, and the.:

and Provisional development of the child'" ,;::Jf-esteem and motivation


(Cermak & H-::'lderson, 1990; Levine, 1987).
For example, ~!..her; r, child with fine motor probl<.:ms
Implications for must copy math problems o"t of a textbook before even
beginning to solve them, she m~y not physically be able to
Children With Fine do enough probleiBs to get~he necessary practice, thus

Motor Problems her achievement in math may be affected. Another child


may not be able to demonstrate that he know~ how to do
long division if he cannot write legibly enough to read
oock to himself correctly Lhe numbers he has written.
Kathleen McHale, Sharon A. Cermak This situation may lead the teacher to assume that the
child does not know how to do long division, thus iHu~­
trating the effect of fine motor skills on assessment for
Key Words: apraxia. coordination function.
in~truction. A third child whc. writes vel)' slowly may not
handwriting. learning disorders.
be able to complete many math problems on the annual
occupational therapy treatment
timed test that determines math group placement. He
may then be placed in an inappropriate math group.
Ustly, if a child with fine mo~or problems is graded in
handWriting by the same standards as children without
This study was designed to obtain a detailed picture of fine motor problems, the child's self-esteem may suffer.
the fine motor requirements in regular elementary The incidence of fine motor difficulties among chil­
school classrooms. This knowledge is cn·tical for occu­
dren i~ not readily available because fine motor difficulties
pationaltberapisJs in working u'itb children with fine
motor and handwn'ting problems who are main­ is not one of the standard reporting categories for federal,
streamed into regular classes. The allocation of lime state, or local rccords. Howcver, available research shows
to fine motor activities and the ~l'pes offine motor that betw<.:en 90% (Tarnopol & Tarnopol, 19-:--) and 98%
tasks children are expected to perform in elementary (Clement~. 1966) of children with learning disabilities or
school U'e7l? int'estigated through the obsen1ation ofsix minimal brain damage demonstrate either fine motor dif­
elementa,,' school classrooms. A un'tlen minute·hr· ficultie~ or poor, slow, and labored handwriting. Al­
minute record of one u'hole day's actil'ities in each though the federal goverT'mel"t repons that about S% of
classroom showed that 30% to 60% of the day u'as allo­ the school-aged population receives sef\;ces for learning
cated to fine motor actiL'ities, u'ith un'ling tasks pre­ disahilities (l'.S. DepJr1.ment of Education, 198'). in pr:lC­
dominating over other manipulatil'e tasks. Implica­ tice. the.: number of <:hiidren rccei\'ing such ~ef\'ices is
tiolls for children U!ith fine motor difficulties include
often considerably higher. Forexampk:, in .~!a~sachusetts
t/.7t' need for modifications to l'ollime of u'ork, ~rpes of
tasks. and rnaten'als. in 19H8. 16,"/% of the school·aged population receiwd
~e.:f\·il·<':s for Ie.:Jrning disabilitie~ (~1a~sachusetts Depan·
ment of Education. personal communication. February
10. 1989), In fact, e~timates of the number of children
wi~:1 iearning disabi1itie~ have ranged as high a~ 3090 of
the ::1t,dent population (Cermak & Henderson. 1990). On
,h=:.>::;i~ of Tarnopol and Tarnopol's (19~-:') finding that
90";'; ()f :;hildre.:n with learning disabilities han: fine motor
K.Ilhlt:t:n McHalt:..\ttl! is a consultant and founder of the: As­ or handwriting difficulties as .....ell. one may estimJte thJt
soc:i:ltion for Children With Fine Motor Difficulties. PO Box at least 4.5'?u of school·aged children (90<?o of th<.: Depan­
!l29. :'\eedham, MA 02194. mcni of Educatjon'~ S90 figure cited aoon:) will have seri­
ous fine motor problem~ in connection with their learn·
Sharon A. Cermak. F~II), OTR L. is Associate Professor of Occupa·
ing di~abiliti<.:s. Fine motor difficu)tie.:s greatly affect
Ilona! Ther:lp~' and Co-Director, I'eurobcha\'ioral Rehahilit;j­
childn:n'~ performance in the.: classroom, Younc:s.
tllm Research Center, Boston L'niversil\', Sargent Collt:gt:.
Boston. Massachusens. Rusner, and Webb (1983) repone.:d that of th<.: 199 chil­
dren with learning disabililie~ th<.:y studied. "/7% were
Tbi.< Qrticl~ u'as QI:Cl'p/~dfor puhlicalion .'Im· /5. /w.!
Initially rc.:fc:rr<.:d for handwriting problt:ms.

898 exloht-r 19?2, \'olu",/! ..j {). Numlwr 10


·\taIlY Ilt/H.-r studl.'nls may ha\'l.' finl.' mowr prohlems siles for Ihis slud~'. The six cla.sses consisled of 1\\'0 Classl's

I'
th;.It arc due primarily ttl a physic:l1 uisahility, such as each from Gratll:s 2, oi, and 6. Two of the six rbsses

cl'rl.'hral paby, mU:Kular uyslroph~', juwnile anhrilis, Ilr oh.~l.'rvL'd were .~plit gr:Jdes. onl' a split first and sl.'cond

0' spin:1 hifiua. Andrl.'ws :mu Elkins (.I 91'l.l) rl'poned thl: re­ gr;llle, the other a split fiflh and sixth grade. \X'here a

sults of a SUf\'L'y in Auslralia of rq,~ul;lr dassroom teachers schoul system offered a choice of c.:Iasst:s within a grade

of children Wilh spina bifk!a anu hydrocephalus, Thirty­ level and the classes had been groupt:d hy ahjlit~" I ill'

nine percent of the students were perc.:cived as experience c:Iass representing a middle academic group was selel'led

ing some degn:e of difficulty with fine motor tasks, includ­ for ohsef\ation, Where mort: than two such c:Iasses at :J

ing writing, urawing, using an eraser, ruling lines, grade level were available for observation, the panicip:Jl­

sleadying papers, laking books from the desk, turning ing class was chosen randomly. The inclusion of splil­

pages, using scissors, and doing craft activities, grade classrooms was not pan of the design of the study.

Children who have attention deficit disorders with or but rather reflects the frequency with which one of lhe

without learning disabilities may also have difficulty with school distril1s uses split-grade classrooms.

fine motor tasks (Denckla, Rudel, Chapman, & Krieger,


1985). Lastly, Gubbay (1985) found that 5% of children
Procedure Ii·
without learning disabilities nevenheless have major d,
problems because of clumsiness. A written record of activities on a minute-by-minute h:Jsis I!!
"
~U
It seems that, at a minimum, nearly 10% of elemen· was made for one whole academic <.Jay in each of thl' six
i!
tary school-aged children may experience major difficulty dassrooms. The major indicators of fine motor requirl'­ :,1
With fine motor tasks. We derived this figure by combin­ ments were allocation of time to fine mOlor lasks and the !j!
I..
ing the estimate<! minimum of 4,5% of children who have types of tasks children were eXJ1Ccted to perform. All q,
both learning disabilities and fine motor difficulties (Tar­ .c:Iassroom uhsef\'ations and coding were done hy the first II'
III
"}
nopol & Tarnopol, 19n; C.S. Department of Education, author.
1987) and the finding of 5% of children without learning
disabilities who have major problems with fine motor
During observations, the time at which a panil'ular
task or acti\;ty began was recorded un an ohsef\';ltion
~i:1l'
tasks (Gubbay, 1985), Figures on lhe incidence of fine data sheet, and that task was described. 'X'hen the task or
motor difficullies among all children with physical disabil­ activity changed, the time of the change and the nature ()f
ities and attention deficits were not available, but would the new activiry were recorded, All obsef\'ations began or
probably increase this figure even after overlap between ended at the time that a school administrator had ideIHi­
categories is taken into consideration. In a typical class­ fled as the beginning or ending of the school day. LUllch­
room, then, the average te:Jcher will probably have at lime, which was -I; min. was not inc:luded in thL' ObSl'I,\,;I­
leasl two students each year whose fine mOlor difficulties tions or cakulatjons. Thus each class was ohsef\'ed for 'i
;In: serious enough to require special education or other hr 1-; min.
sef\'ices. In one of the second'W:lde and one of lhe founh·
Although it is unifomlly recognizeu that elementary grade classes, soml' of lhe students changed re:ll hers fill'
school work reqUires fine motor tasks. there is little docu­ particular suhjel'ts. For example. in onl.' fourth·gr:ll!L·
mentation about either the percentage of time spent on class, 9 students lefl to rl'ceh'e their m:Hh in.struniol1 in ;1
.such t:Jsk.s or lhL' spl'cific types of tasks with fine mOlor uif/\:rent cl:Jssroom. Because some stU(ll.'ntsslayed in the
requiremL'nts that childn:n are expected to perform. This original cbssrool11. the ohsl:f\'atioll l'l>l1tilll1e<l in the
knowledgl.' i:- crilicil for occupatilln:J1 therapists hecause original c:Iassroom. Howc\'er. in onl' six[h-,~ralk' cLi's. :111
most referrals for Occup;ltional lher..lpy for children in Ilfthl.' students I11m'ed as a group 10 diffl.'n,..nr ll'adll.'rs (, Ir
schools involw problems in handwriting and fine motor math and sodal studies. In th:Jt l':JSl'. rhe oh'l'!'Yarions
skills (Cermak. 1991), Toohtain a detailed pictureofthl: took place in three differl'nt c1assrollms as till' Slll<!cJlI­ I'
fint: motor requirements of work in regular elementary mO\'L'd from teacher to leacher. \x'hen instrlll'tillll:tI i;
/'.
school dassnx>ms, the first author conducted day-long groups wilhin a class panicipated in separate al-tl\·itics.
observation~ in sLx dassrooms across the different years
of elementary sch(x>!. The method and findings from this
time allocations and tasks Wl'rL' recorded sep;lr:ltl'h' f( Ir
each grou r,
l.;
I'I
study (~1c:Hak', 1987 ) art: presented, followed by a discus­ After each observation was completed. all tasks \\l'rl' I
~ion of somt: of lhe implications for children with fine assigned to one of four cltegoriL's: fine I11otor lasks. illtl'­
InUlor problems who are in regular classrooms. grated fine motor tasks. other academic lasks. ;lOll III ll1­
academic activities. Fine motor t(lsks were tho:-l' fllr
which pcrform:Jnce required :1 I11ajor USl.' of OIll.'·S halld,.
Method
such as writing Wilh :I pencil. usi'ng .sci.s:-ors. :lnd usill).! .1
Subjects computer kc:~ho:lrd. IlIIeRraled.!il1C! motor tasJ..'s indud,
Six classes from two middle·incomt: suburban public ed activities in which fine l110wr and mhl'r ;lC:I(k'mi.' t;lsk,
school dislrins in New England served as observation occurred simult:lnl'ously_ For exampk'. sonll:lillll'S :1

'1"" American .loun/al oj Occupaliollol Tberapr


lc:acher instructed orally while children complcl<.:d work Table 1
shel'ts. The category of other academic tasks included Time Allocated to Tasks In Regular Classrooms
tasks for which performance did not require frequent use TOlal Time
AJI()(JleU (0
of one's hands. such as reading or group discussion. Non·
Fonc ~Olor
academic ac/il'i/ies included those activities that seemed
Time
_____ __ on _
Tasb
__ (min) anu
_ _ _ _ _ Intcgrated
to be dther functional (e.g., the collecting of lunch mono
ey) or transitional (e.g., moving from one classroom to Fine Integr:lleu Other Non· Fine Motor
Graue MolOr' Fine MOlllr' AcadL'Tllic" 3c3demicd Tasks (%)"
another) rather than instructional.
Interrater reliability within these six classrooms was 2 128 1S 119 113 oj;

2 (Splil) 166 6 Il6 57 5S

not calculated; however, at a later date, four }-hr class­ oj 70 28 145 72 31

room observations were videotaped and independently 4 80 17 147 71 31

sl"Ored by both the first author and another trained rater. 6 90 14 137 74 33

6 (Split) 180 9 65 61 60

Results indicated interrater agreement of more than 95%


',W = 119.50 = oi2. h.u = 1;,SD = ~. '){ = 112.SD = 33. u.\I = 70.50
on the cooing of both fine motor and integrated fine '" 9. ~M '" 43. 50 = 12.
motor activities for time allocation and type of task.
The key factor in selecting a category for the activi·
ties dUring a particular span of time was always the teach­
er's instructions to the class or group as a whole, rather rate instructional groups, the results reflect an average of
than the activities of particular children within the group. the time allocations for the different groups. However, in
For example. if the teacher assigned the task of writing split-grade classes, only the results for the grade actually
sentences from vocabulary words, the task was catego­ being observed are reported.
rized as a fine motor task from the time the teacher The types of fine motor tasks that occurred in the
indicatl..'l! that students should begin work until the teach­ classrooms are shown below. Only tasks actually ob­
er gave instructions for all students to begin some other served in the six classrooms were induded. For reporting
type of actiVity. In some classes, children who had fin­ purposes, tasks were grouped under two major headings:
ished a particular academic task were allowed to deter· paper·and·pencil tasks and manipulative tasks. The cate­
mine how they would use their remaining free time. gory of paper·and-pencil tasks, which included all tasks
Thus, in one class, children who finished writing their requiring the use of pencil, pen, crayon, or marker on
spelling words might listen to tapes, whereas in another paper, contained tht: following:
class, children who finished a small group discussion
• Cop~ing from text or the board
might do their math homework. In this study, however,
all activities were categorized according to the teacher's • Doing rcpetitive writing
instructions and the time the teacher allowed for task • Writing headings on paper
l"Ompletion. not according to how particular children • Writing from dictation
used the time. • Taking notes
• Complcting commerdJI work shcets. workbooks,
All tasks thaI met the definition offine motor, that is,
or tests
th:1t required mJjor use of one's hands, were identified
and described in detail on the observation data sheets. • Correcting work rapidly
• Answering questions from text
The academic subjel"l, the precise task, and the materials
u..ed were reconJcd. For example, rather than just de­ • Doing creative writing
. .crihing a task as /l'ri/ing, the firsl author described it as • DraWing.
l"llpving .sentences from a texth<xlk onto paper with a The category of maniplllatj\·l' t;lsk,. which indude:s
pencil. tasks that require manil1ulatin~ objl'n~ rathl'r than writ­
After the obscC"ations were completed, the duration ing with an implement, contained the follOWing:
(If time: spent on each of the four categories of tasks was
• Folding paper to make margins and dividing lines
calculated with the data from the obseC"ation data sheets.
The percentages of time allocated to fine motor and inte­ • Cutting or pasting or Doth
grated fine motor tasks wcre thcn calculated. A list of all • Using a computer
• Manipulating objects such as seclls or plam,.
tasks assigned was then developed. and tasks were
grouped together for reporting purposes. For all fine motor acti\'ities for the sLx d;l.... ruom~.
8;% of the time was spent on paper-and-pt'ncil tasks and
15% was spent on manipulative tasks. Subheadings under
Results
each major category of task also rdlcL"! groupings of simi­
Findings regarding actual time and the percentJge of the lar tasks. For exampic, in the catcgory of paper-and-pen.
day spent on fine motor ta:.ks and integrated fine motor cil tasks, the subheading of repetitive: writing includes the
tasks are shown in Table 1. Where classes contained sepa- specific tasks of the repetitive writing of spelling words,

900 Octobe,. /991. Vulum.. -16. .\'lImbe,. 10


handwriting practice:. and the: r<.:pe:titivc: writing of vo­ until junior high sch~xli or high school whe:n there b
cabulary L1dinilions. more: emphasis on such task~ as taking notes during oral
instruction.
Discussion An interesting finding e:merged whe:n data from the:
classrooms were examined. Although only two split-grade
Allucation of Time classrooms were observed, the two highest per<:l:ntage~
There was a considerable range in the length of time and oftime allocated to fine motor tasks were in these classes.
percentage of the day allocated to all types of fine motor In the two split grades observed. teachers organized the
tasks in the classrooms obsen'ed (see Table 1). The range students into many separate small instructional groups.
i extended from 31 % to 60% of the academic day. This In the first- and second-grade classroom there were four
ft finding indicates that different classrooms may have dif­ separate groups, two for reading and two for math. In the
ferent levels of fine motor demands. The variations may fifth· and sixth-grade classroom there were five separate
result from factors such as teacher style, classroom and groups, three for reading and two for math. The high
school organization. and curriculum decisions. Results percentages of time on fine motor tasks resulted from the
from the small sample of six classrooms did not indicate a need to keep students in some groups involved in seat
relation betwee:n increasing grade level and increasing work while the teacher conducted oral instruction with
percentage of time allocated to tasks with fine motor other groups.
demands. Rathe:r, it seemed that in some elementary
school classrooms. the primary learning modality was fine
7)pes of Tasks
motor activity. whereas in other classrooms, fine motor
activity played less of a role in learning because group Paper-and-pencil tasks are listed according to the degree
discussion or oral instruction predominated. of student control over the volume and content of task
Equally imponant. however, is the finding that in all perfonnance as detennined by task analysis done by the

the classrooms observed, a substantial pan of the day was authors. The list begins with tasks that offer little student
spent on fine motor tasks. Thus. it seems that regardless control in these areas. such as copying from a textbook or
of the differences between classrooms in the degree of Writing spelling words repeatedly. Creative writing and
emphasis on fine motor tasks, all the classrooms ob­ drawing, tasks with the highest degree of student defini­
served had a high level of fine motor demands. It should tion of the task, are listed near the end of paper-and­
be emphasized that these observations were made in only pencil tasks. The manipulative tasks listed require high
one day in each of the six classrooms. Therefore, broad fine motor skills but do not involve the use of a v.Titing
conclusions ahout c1emental'\' school classrooms in gen­ implement on paper. They focus on working with materi­
eral would not be: warranted. als or tools. For example. in one founh-grade class. the
A third finding rdates to the time allocated to inte­ students prepared a science project that involved cutting
~r3ted fine motor tasks. Integrated tasks may present a seed in half, gluing it to a piece of paper, and cO\'ering
increased difficuftics for the child who must concentrate the seed with a piece of adhesive-backed transparent
on thc physical aet of writing or using hands while also plastic.
focusin~ on the content of the oral instruction being pre· Several patterns related to types of fine motor activi­
~e:nte:d. Fine motor activities were classified as integrated ties were identified during the observations. ~iost time
t:Jsks only whe:n fine: motor and other academic tasks spent on fine motor tasks involved some kind of writing
Ill.:currcd simultaneously. If the tc:acher's instructions to with paper and pencil rather than manipulating Othe:r
:he clas:'> cre:ated a houndJl'\' betwee:n types of tasks. the types·of materials or tools or using markers or crayons.
t:Jsks we:re: not con"ide:red to be occurring simultaneous· Two t\'pes of fine motor activities occurred in e:vel'\' n:gu­
ly, for example:. if the te:acher said, "I want you to put lar classroom observed. First. children were instnKte:d to
your pencib down and liste:n for a minute," that statement fold paper to make margins or diViding lines heron: d( ling
cre::lte:d a clear demarcation between time spent on fine their work. Second. reading workhooks or work sheeh
motor tasks and time spent attending to oral instruction. were used in all classes, The fine mo[Or demands of thl'
In general. the: classes obsen'ed did not seem to reading workbooks could be de:scrihcd as minimal. Ex­
spe:nd large percentages of the day engaged in integrated cept for comprehension questions. the: [asks in the reJd­
t:l~k.'i. \X'ith the exception of onc founh·grade class, no ing workbooks involved either circling numhe:rs for
':ISS spent more than 5% of the day on integrated tasks. multiple-choice questions or writing sjn~de letters or
I the founh-grade class in which 990 of the day was spent words,

,
. III inte:grated tasks. most of the integrated time occurred In five out of six classrooms. children were expected
f while the children were preparing a science project. As to copy from textbooks and do repetitive writing for such
they worked on a plant hooklet. the teacher continued tasks as copying spelling words or vocabulal'\' definition."
oral instru<:tion ahout their plants. It may be that high Cre:ative writing and answering questions in se:ntences.
allocations of time to integrated tasks are not evident tasks in which the child does have a great degre:e of

Til,. Amt'rican foun/al of Occupatiol/al Tberapy 901


l'IlOtrol ovcr the: volume and conte.:nt of writing. o\.Turre:d l'hild with hill' IlH !lor prol>k'll1" I," plJll'<! III t hat cia",." The
in fpur out of ~ix c1a~srooms, dfel't Oil line 1I11J!llr (k'm:lIHI.., of so many in.,tnll'tion;J1
group:- l'an be.: rl'dul'l'll. For e.:xJmple.:, in rhl' fll'.,t- and
s<':l"lllld·grJlIl" dassrlllllll (JiJslTv<.:d for this study, an :Jiue.:
Implications for Children With Fine MOlor

took 9 studl'nts inlo ;J separate.: room for computl'r in·


Difficulties

strunion whill' anothe.:r group was instructl'd in math in


The:: findings re::garding the:: amount of time:: allocate.:d to the dassroom hy the.: tl';Jc!K'r, Thus, th<.: fin<.: motor aeth'·
fine motor tasks and the types of tasks that children an: ity of thos<.: 9 childrl.."n w:ts of a diffe.:rent intl'nsity than it
expected to perform raise important issues about provid· would h~I\,(: hc<.:n if th<.:y had bee.:n assi~n<.:d the.: more.:
ing appropriate education for children with fine motor USU;J\ type of s<.:at work while: the other group was re.:ce.:i\'­
difficulties in the regular classroom. In all classrooms ob­ ing dirl'Ct instrul'tion.
served, at least one third of the day was allocated to fine Th<.: typ<.:s of ta~ks assigned affe.:ct thl.." child with finl.."
motor activity. and in two out of six of the classrooms motor difficulties, h<.:c;Juse tasks th<.:mse.:\v<.:s ma\' prl.."s<.:nt
observed. at 'least 55% of the day was allocated to fine differ<.:nt r<.:tjuirements rt'latin~ to the amount. size. I<.:g·
motor tasks, thus it seems that fine motor tasks are an ihility, and spacing of writing or to the comple.:xity and
integral part 'of elementary school education. Providing difficulry of work with particular materials, For example. a
an appropriate education for a child with known fine math ditto with answer spaces designed for a child with
motor problems in regular classes may therefore reqUire handwriting uf avera~t' size may not offer enough work
a teacher to make needed modifications, proVide alterna· space for a child whose handWriting is larger than aver·
tive modes of response and learning, or reduce the vol· agL', Different tasks also may offer the child differl'nt b'­
ume of written work while ensuring that adequate prac· ds of contrul over the volume and content of writing, For
tice fOF learning new tasks has been proVided. Careful example. assignin~ ;J child the task of writing a stOl'\' with
monitoring of the chikl's work and learning is required to sre::lling words allows fJr more latitude in volume of writ­
ensure that an appropriate match between fine motor ing than dues assigning the task of copying fiv<.: para­
demands, environmental adaptations, and the child's graphs from a soci;JI stuui<.:s text. Thus, the tyre:: of t;Jsks
ability is occurring, assi~ned may increase or decrl'3St: th.e nee.:d for n1odifka­
Given that a range in fine motor requirements wa<; tion~ to work.
observed within the same grade level, placing the child in A panicular issue is the frequency with which repet·
a class in which the requirements lie at the lower end of itiw writing tasks we.:re assigned to childr<.:n, The t'duca­
the rang<.: may be the prt:ferred coursl' of action when a til lilaI \'alm: of such tasks for children who haw difficu!!\'
choice of <:lasses <.:xists. However, in the present study we thinking whil<.: f(X,'using on handwriting must be c;Jrefully
<.:xamine.:d only time allocated to fine motor tasks and did l'onsidl.."r<.:d. Spelling prete:sts or oral vocabulary pretL'sts
not conside.:r the volume of work to be done in the time could e.:liminate.: the ne.:ed to have students copy entire.:
allocated, In making decisions about a child's education, spelling oF\'lll'ahubr\' lists O\'l'r al--\ OVl'r, In math, rcquir­
we would also n<.:ed to consider thiS factor, ing stuuL'nts to copy a numhl'rof cumple.:x math problcm~
Ao;, mentioned earlier. a substantial amount of time onto another pi<':l'e: of pape:r hefore.: they h<.:gin to soh'e
allocate.:d to integrated tasks can present a prohlem for thl' prohlems ,'oulll he.: l'lirninate.:u through the.: usc of 3
the child who has difficulty thinking while Writing, How· photol,'(lpying enlarging m:ll'hine.:, A..o;, an ;Jltl'Fn;Jtiw, thl"
ever. the:se: six ohsl'l'\'ations do not indicate that ;J suh· SllJdl'l1t l"l luld use: thl' workhook version of thl' tl'xtbook u'
stantial amount of time: is ;Jllocate:d to integratt'u tasks in that is JV:lilahlL',
elementary school. It should hl' note.:d that our opc:ration· Th<.: pr:1l'till' I If ha\'mg l'hildn:n fold pJlx'r tll l1lakl'
al definition of inte.:gr;Jtcd fine motor tasks influl'nceu margins and <hiding linl's on their papl'r:-., whkh Ul.'.
these findings. 'X'c defin<.:u inleRraledjine mOlor !clsks as l'llrn:d in l'\'<':~' l"la,.,..,rool11, is a frequ<.:ntly rl'l'(lmml'ntk'd
simultaneous occurrcnce of fin<.: motor activities and oral modific;Jtion '!(lr childr<.:n with handwriting pr()bk:rn~,
instruction by the tcach<.:r. Many of the acti\'ities that we Th<.: lI"l' of re.:ading workbooks with mil1lmal fine m()[or
uefin<.:d as fin\.' motor activities, such as writing sentences <k'mands i,,, also h<.:lpful fDr thl' integratiun of L'hi/th':")
with wKahulary words. also demanded thinking whil<.: with fine 1111 III lr pn lhll"ms into rl'gular c1a:-:-rnoms,
writing, Simpk' llHldifil·:1tiDns fm handling more: Il'ngtl1\' J"
Split-grade class<.:s are often viewed as ad\'antageous signml'nts in reading workhooks could induuL' allowing
for the child with special nl"Cds because a greater spread tilt' child to r<.:mm'l' the pa~l' from th<.: workhook and to
of ahility is present to enahle the child's needs to Ix: mer. use.: a typt'\\Tit<.:r t, It\')1<': answers directly onto the PJgl'. In
However, this type of daso;, mi~ht not be an optimal onl" sixth·gradl" cl:l.,sroom ()h~el'\'ed. a typewrit<.:r wa"
choil'l.: for a child with fine mo ,r prohlems because of uSl'd f(lr this pUrp(l:-l' hy se.:\'l"rJI stude.:nts. nont' uf whom
th<.: high p<.:rcentag<.: of time Spl'nt on fine motor anivi· had known finl' l11otor diftkulties,
til'S. Th<.: organization of dassroom activities in a split­ In suml11JI\', tl1l' rl"sult~ of this study 3re exdting but
wadI..' da~~room must he <.:x;Jmined car<.:fully before a l11u:-t hI.' \'il'\wd a." prl'lil11in:l~'. Mowl<.:ug<.: of the: hl'a\~'

902
t
i
finc motor demands of regular dassroo01s should assist losh, "10'>, and Ihl' btl' Eli.sa Bonaventura. Phil. Th,s srudv \\,;b

1 Ol'(upational therapists working in the schools in rrl>vid­


ing more appmrrialt: consu!'ation. HoweVl..·r, Ihios study is
based on limited ohservations. The sample was sm:1I1­
l'ompil'tl'd as pan of [he requiremcnts for the first author's
dq{ree of master of l'du<.:ation, Rhode Island College.
This paper \\'a- written under the auspiccs of the :"eurolx:·
ha\,j()ral Rl'hahilital1lm Research Center. Boston, Massachu·
only six different c1assruoms and only two split-grade selts, a center for scholarship and research in oc<.:upational
classrooms were observed. Mureover, each class was oh· therapy at Boston L'niversity funded by lhe American Occup:!­
served for only one day. AlthouRh we a[lcmpted 10 select tiona) Therapy foundation and the American Occupational
Therapy A~sociati()n.
typica: days, with such a shan observation an atypical day
wuuld have skewed the results. In additiun, the issue of
References
interrater reliability of coding was nOl fully addressed in
this study in that on-site imerrater reliability was not cal­ Andrl"Ws. R., & Elkins. J. (1981). The mana.cwm!!nt (lnd
I. educalion of cbildrell u:i/b spina hifida and hydrocephalus
culated. It is possible that rating from a videotape results
Canherra, Australia: Australian Education Research and Devel­
in higher (or lower) imerrater reliability of coding com­ opment Commillt:e. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service
pared with rating from on-site observation. This study r\o. ED 242 162)
should be replicated With a larger sample with Wider Cemlak. S. (1991) Somatodyspraxia. In A. Fisher, E. Mur­
geographic representation and over a longer period of ray. & A. Bundy (Eels.), Sensory- integration: Theory- and prac­
time, and on-site reliability must be examined. tice (pr. 13&-170). Philadelphia: F. A. Davis. .
Cermak, S., & Henderson, A. (1990). Learning disabilities.
Direct observation of the classroom provides helpful In D, l:mphrcd (Ed.)J...· eurological rehahilitation (2nd ed., pp.
information about classroom organization, the use of 2R3-331). SI. Louis: Mosby.
time for academic activities, and the tasks that children Clements. S. D. (1966),.t1inimal brain dysfunction in chil·
will be expected to perfonn. Classroom observation pro­ dren: TenninolO1{1' alld identlficatioll (NI!':DB Monograph l'o.
vides invaluable infonnation to the therapist consulting 3). WashinglOn, DC: L'.S. Departmenl of Health. Education and
\X'elfare,
with the teacher and can help to identify potential prob· Den<.:kla, M., Rudel, R., Chapman. c.. & Krieger, J. (1985).
lem areas for a child With fine motor problems, Appropri­ ~ll>l()r proficienC"!' in dyslexic children with and without allen·
ate modifications and personalized instruction can then tiol1al disorders. Archil'es of Neurology, 42, 228-231.
be anticipated. Necessary plans can be implemented be­ Guhha~', S. S. (1985). Clumsiness In P. Vinken, G. Bru~'n. &

fore or during the performance of academic tasks, rather H, Klawans (Eds.), Handbook ofclinical neurology (Vol. 2. pp.
159-167), New York: Elsevier.
than after the child has experienced failure at a panicular l.evine. M, D. (19IP). DeI'elopmentall'oriatloll ond I!!am·
task. "''hen comhined with careful monitori.lg of the 11I,I!. di.~or("·rs. Camhridge. /l.1A: Educ3lOrs Puhlishini! Service.
child's work and learning. knowledge and antlciratiun ~1rllalc. K. (19~-), Intf!grat;'lf?, children Il'llh jin!! motor
of fX.>tential problem areas will help ensure that the dijli'cultif!s 11110 rl!J.(ular classrooms. Cnpublished master's tht'­
child receives an appropriate ellul'ation in the regular sis, Rhode Island Colle~t:. Pru\'idence. Rhode Island.
Tarnopol. 1.., & Tamopol. M. (1977). Brain/unction a1l(1
classroom... '""lIding di>;(lhilitif!s. Baltimore: L'niversily Park Press.
[·.S. Dl'partmcnt of Education. (198-). ft.'imh amllwl n'·
/X)rt to CunRr!!ss. \\·:JshlOglOn. DC.
Acknowledgments
Younes, R.. Rosner. B.. & \\'ebh, G. (1983). :"euroimmatur·
\\'e thank the Special Education Dl'panment at Rhode Island it\ of Il':Jrning-disahled ('hildren: A controlled ~tudy. Dl'l'IdofJ­
College, Providence. Rhode !slant!. espeCially A. Anthony An· IIIl'/I/(I1 .I/I'dicille a/lll C/)ild Nf!urolol-..'1'. .15. 5~-l-';-9.

The I\.meriam jUUM/(l1 uJ Occupatiunal Th"mpl' 903

View publication stats

You might also like