You are on page 1of 18

Child Development, September/October 2021, Volume 92, Number 5, Pages 1717–1734

Values-Alignment Messaging Boosts Adolescents’ Motivation to Control


Social Media Use
Brian M. Galla Sophia Choukas-Bradley
University of Pittsburgh University of Pittsburgh and University of Delaware

Hannah M. Fiore, and Michael V. Esposito


University of Pittsburgh

Two preregistered experiments with 2,733 U.S. high school students (age range = 13–19 years) compared the
impact of different messages on adolescents’ motivation to control social media use (SMU). A traditional mes-
sage emphasized the benefits of avoiding SMU, whereas a values-alignment message framed controlling SMU
as being consistent with autonomy and social justice. Compared to no message or a traditional message, in
both studies, a values-alignment message led to greater motivation to control SMU immediately afterward,
and in Study 2, awareness of “addictive” social media designs 3 months later. As hypothesized, values-align-
ment messaging was more motivating for girls than boys. Results offer preliminary support for leveraging
adolescents’ drives for autonomy and social justice to motivate self-regulation of SMU.

How can adults motivate behavior change in ado- particularly those that grant status and respect
lescents? To prepare adolescents for adulthood, par- among peers—can thwart educational efforts that
ents and educators often encourage young people highlight long-term consequences to promote
to set and pursue goals that contribute to long-term behavior change (Steinberg, 2015; Yeager et al.,
success, from improving dietary habits and aca- 2018). One possible solution is to redirect the moti-
demic performance to reducing bullying and the vational power of reward sensitivity toward posi-
use of tobacco, drugs, and alcohol. In the United tive ends by aligning desired behavior with social
States, universally administered school-based pro- rewards that already animate adolescents (Dahl,
grams are a primary avenue for promoting positive Allen, Wilbrecht, & Suleiman, 2018; Telzer, 2016).
behavior change (Steinberg, 2014, 2015). Unfortu- Rather than treat adolescents’ sensitivity to rewards
nately, many educational efforts designed to moti- as a barrier to educational programs, we can
vate behavior change show lower effectiveness instead repurpose it as an asset to spark behavior
during adolescence (Yeager, Dahl, & Dweck, 2018). change (Bryan et al., 2016).
In this period, sensitivity to immediate rewards— The current investigation tests and extends this
emerging theory of behavior change on an outcome
that is highly relevant to adolescent development:
Research and writing were supported by funding from the
John Templeton Foundation (54929) to the first author and from social media use (SMU). Specifically, we sought to
start-up funding to the first and second authors. Study 2 in this understand how adults can instill reasons that ado-
publication was supported by Character Lab, and facilitated lescents might internalize and use to self-regulate
through the Character Lab Research Network, a consortium of
schools across the country working collaboratively with scientists their SMU. We studied high school students
to advance scientific insights that help kids thrive. (roughly at the stage of middle adolescence)
We thank Mira Patel, Brandon Baun, Gillian Murphy, Emily because they are prolific users of social media
Carrino, and Chloe Bryen for feedback on early versions of the
experimental messages. We also thank Jacqueline Nesi for feed-
back on a draft of the manuscript. We gratefully acknowledge
Christopher J. Bryan and David S. Yeager for sharing their origi- © 2021 The Authors.
nal values-alignment materials (Bryan et al., 2016), which served Child Development published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of Society
as a basis for our values-alignment message. Finally, we thank for Research in Child Development.
the students who participated in this research and the school This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons
administrators, teachers, and staff who helped facilitate it. Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to distribution in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited,
Brian M. Galla, School of Education, University of Pittsburgh, the use is non-commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made.
5940 Wesley W. Posvar Hall, Pittsburgh, PA 15260. Electronic All rights reserved. 0009-3920/2021/9205-0026
mail may be sent to gallabri@pitt.edu. DOI: 10.1111/cdev.13553
1718 Galla, Choukas-Bradley, Fiore, and Esposito

(Rideout & Robb, 2018), and are of an age when Przybylski, 2019). Empirical evidence can support
reward sensitivity may be peaking (Crone & Dahl, limiting SMU: Students who multitask with social
2012) and when many traditional behavior change media earn lower grades (Marker, Gnambs, &
programs show reduced effectiveness (Yeager et al., Appel, 2018), those who use social media at night
2018). Across two preregistered field experiments, get worse sleep (Woods & Scott, 2016), and time
we compared the effect of two different theoretical away from digital technology can improve adoles-
approaches to behavior change, operationalized via cents’ processing of social information (Uhls et al.,
proof-of-concept experimental messages, on adoles- 2014). Imposing restrictions, however, is not the
cents’ motivation to control SMU, including their same as encouraging adolescents to develop inter-
interest in learning new self-control strategies. A nalized reasons for controlling SMU.
more traditional message simulated an approach Modern school-based digital citizenship pro-
taken by many other educational programs and grams, which now reach 60,000 U.S. schools,
highlighted the long-term benefits of avoiding attempt to do this: as of 2019, at least one program
SMU, while a novel, values-alignment message has lessons encouraging adolescents to find “media
sought to align behavior change with immediate balance” by self-regulating SMU (James et al.,
social rewards. We also examined gender differ- 2019). The idea of taking control over SMU indeed
ences in the impacts of these messages. appeals to many adolescents. National surveys
show that 68% of adolescents believe SMU has a
negative impact on people their age (Rideout &
SMU as a New Target of Behavior Change Research
Robb, 2018), 41% worry they spend too much time
In less than 20 years, adolescents’ lives have on social media (Pew Research Center, 2018a), and
become saturated with social media. Representative over half (57%) have tried to cut back (Pew
surveys show that an overwhelming majority of Research Center, 2018a)—in part to get away from
U.S. adolescents (80%) aged 13 to 17 use social “digital drama” (Rideout & Robb, 2018). To be sure,
media (Rideout & Robb, 2018), which include Insta- many adolescents have favorable views of SMU.
gram, Snapchat, Facebook, Twitter, and YouTube, But these surveys suggest that a lot of adolescents
among others. Many adolescents say they spend may also be ready to change their behavior. Few
several hours every day on social media (Rideout, studies, however, have examined effective ways of
2015), and roughly half (47%) check social media “a delivering such messages to adolescents.
few times an hour” or “almost constantly” (Rideout
& Robb, 2018). Social media allow young people to
A More Traditional Approach to Motivating
socialize with friends, learn about current events,
Self-Regulation of SMU
express themselves creatively, meet new people
with shared interests, and get social support (Pew One way to motivate self-regulation is by appeal-
Research Center, 2018b)—all of which can con- ing to the long-term personal benefits of limiting
tribute to personal and social development. SMU. This approach is standard to many school-
Notwithstanding its benefits, adults remain based health programs. Programs designed to
uneasy about the influence of SMU on adolescent encourage health behavior change often attempt to
development (American Psychological Association, do so by increasing knowledge of long-term conse-
2017; James, Weinstein, & Mendoza, 2019). They quences, teaching self-regulation skills, and instill-
worry that adolescents spend too much time on ing societal expectations of positive behaviors
social media and are too attached to their phones (Steinberg, 2015; Yeager et al., 2018). Earlier ver-
(Common Sense Media, 2016a; Pew Research Center, sions of some digital citizenship programs (ones
2018a; Rosen, Cheever, & Carrier, 2008), and that it available at the time of our experiments, which did
distracts them from schoolwork and in-person inter- not yet involve lessons on “media balance;” Com-
actions (Pew Research Center, 2018a; Rosen et al., mon Sense Media, 2016b) and other educational
2008) and impairs sleep (James et al., 2019). Adults resources designed to encourage responsible SMU
also have concerns about the implications of digital in youth (e.g., Citizens Crime Commission of New
technologies and SMU for adolescents’ privacy and York City, 2015) seemed to rely on similar ideas.
exposure to cyberbullying (Anderson, 2019). They included classroom lessons that prompted
An understandable response to these worries is adolescents to reflect on possible consequences of
to impose limits on the amount of time adolescents online activity (e.g., how information posted online
spend on social media (American Psychological today may affect future opportunities) and then
Association, 2017; Anderson, 2019; Weinstein & learn strategies to change behavior. The logic of
Adolescent Values and Social Media Use 1719

these approaches is supported by decision-making contribute knowledge and skills that promote proso-
theories showing the predictive power that percep- cial, beyond-the-self goals (Fuligni, 2019).
tions of long-term consequences can have on behav- Several results in the developmental science liter-
ior change (Ajzen, 1991) and by research on self- ature suggest that values-alignment approaches
control showing that focusing on distant conse- might be more effective for motivating adolescents
quences is one way to overcome immediate tempta- to control their SMU. Studies of adolescents’ neural
tion (Kober, Kross, Mischel, Hart, & Oschner, 2010). functioning show that acting independently from
But there are reasons to question whether this adult control and in ways that are of benefit to
tactic would optimally motivate adolescents to con- others elicits activation in regions associated with
trol their SMU. Despite being knowledgeable about reward processing (Telzer, 2016). Adolescents with
long-term consequences, adolescents often do not greater activation in reward processing regions dur-
behave in ways that take into account what might ing prosocial decisions are not only less likely to
happen in the future (Steinberg, 2008). Adolescents engage in risk-taking behaviors (Telzer, Fuligni,
act more impulsively than children and adults do Lieberman, & Galvan, 2013), but are less likely to
in part because they are more sensitive to environ- find risk-taking rewarding (Telzer, 2016). These
mental rewards (Telzer, 2016). Positive social feed- data suggest that the “warm glow” adolescents feel
back—which elicits positive emotions and conveys during autonomous, prosocial behavior could offset
to the individual that they are respected and held the immediate rewards they might otherwise gain
in esteem—is an especially powerful reward during from risky behaviors (Telzer, 2016). In a similar
adolescence (Yeager et al., 2018). Adolescents read- way, framing self-regulation of SMU as a way to
ily realign their goals toward the good feelings that pursue autonomy and social justice could provide a
result from behaving in ways their peers value, viable in-the-moment substitute to the social
even if upon reflection they might endorse opposite rewards that come from habitual use. From a social
goals (Crone & Dahl, 2012; Yeager et al., 2018). psychological perspective, values-alignment
Seeking to change SMU encapsulates this conun- approaches can be considered a “wise” intervention
drum: Social media taps into key adolescent moti- because they target key psychological processes
vators, including interpersonal feedback (Nesi, underlying the desired behavior (Walton, 2014).
Choukas-Bradley, & Prinstein, 2018), making it To align self-regulation of SMU with autonomy
highly immediately rewarding (Sherman, Payton, and social justice motives, we adapted a message
Hernandez, Greenfield, & Dapretto, 2016) and thus used by Bryan et al. (2016), which originally was
reinforcing (Lindstr€om, Bellander, Chang, Tobler, & designed to change adolescents’ preferences for
Amodio, 2019). Although adolescents may recog- junk food, to focus instead on social media. The
nize the long-term benefits of controlling SMU, values-alignment message was presented as an
appealing to such perceptions may do little in the expose of how social media companies utilize so-
moment to offset the rewards of social media. called “addictive designs” (e.g., likes) for the pur-
pose of keeping users online as long as possible, to
maximize advertising revenue. The message also
A Values-Alignment Approach to Motivating
stressed social justice implications of companies
Self-Regulation of SMU
gaining financially from using designs engineered
In contrast to highlighting the long-term benefits to be hard to resist. Raising awareness of industry
of avoiding SMU, we tested another approach that practices, we speculated, could prompt adolescents
sought to make behavior change more immediately to view self-regulating SMU as a way to join a
rewarding. We did so by framing the motivation to counter social movement and assert independence
control SMU as being aligned with social values that from companies seeking to control and profit from
many adolescents care about: autonomy and social their behavior. Thus, a behavior that might serve
justice (Bryan, Yeager, & Hinojosa, 2019; Bryan et al., longer-term goals (i.e., self-regulating SMU) could
2016). Autonomy—thinking and acting on one’s potentially also feel immediately rewarding by
own, without coercion—is important to adolescents symbolically fulfilling important values that are
because it demonstrates that they are competent peo- shared by their peers (Bryan et al., 2016).
ple, worthy of respect (Yeager et al., 2018). Social jus- In some ways, this message captures what many
tice refers to having a sensitivity toward and desire adolescents already think about the technology
to correct societal unfairness (Bryan et al., 2016; Yea- industry. One nationally representative survey
ger et al., 2014). Working toward social justice pro- found that 72% of teens believe technology compa-
vides opportunities to join social movements and nies “manipulate people into spending more time on
1720 Galla, Choukas-Bradley, Fiore, and Esposito

devices” (Rideout & Robb, 2018). In that survey, one Furthermore, girls report greater psychological
16-year-old girl said, “The companies that own these dependence on social media (Boer, Stevens, Stevens,
social media should stop scamming teenagers into Finkenauer, & van den Eijnden, 2020) and make
spending a majority of their time on their sites” (p. more comparisons to their peers’ lives while using it
29). Until now, the power of such beliefs to shape (Nesi & Prinstein, 2015). To the extent that adoles-
motivation has not been tested empirically. cent girls are more emotionally invested in (and pos-
Our overall theory of change, supported in part sibly more negatively affected by) SMU than boys,
by prior research by Bryan et al. (2016) on junk food we believed it was possible that girls would be more
preferences and by evaluations of the truthâ anti-to- motivated to control their SMU after viewing a mes-
bacco campaign on adolescent smoking (Evans et al., sage that raised awareness about design features that
2004; Hersey et al., 2005), is depicted in stylized form might intensify dependence.
in Figure 1. We hypothesized that our message
would: (a) strengthen the belief that social media
The Current Investigation
companies use addictive design features to entice its
users, which would lead to (b) greater perceived Across two preregistered field experiments, we
alignment between self-regulating SMU and auton- hypothesized that compared to receiving no mes-
omy and social justice, which would (c) enhance sage or a traditional message highlighting the long-
overall motivation to control SMU, which would cul- term personal benefits of avoiding SMU, a values-
minate in (d) longer-term behavior change. alignment message would boost adolescents’ imme-
diate motivation to control their SMU and promote
longer-term changes in social media awareness and
Examining Heterogeneity of Messaging Effects:
behavior. We also tested psychological processes
Gender Differences
leading to immediate and longer-term outcomes, as
Another goal of the current investigation was to outlined in our theory of change (Figure 1). Finally,
examine the role of gender in the effectiveness of val- we examined gender as a possible moderator of
ues-alignment messaging. We focused on gender messaging effects. In Study 1, we conducted
given the growing body of research indicating that exploratory, nondirectional tests of moderation, and
SMU may have greater significance in the lives of then in Study 2, we tested the confirmatory hypoth-
adolescent girls than boys. For example, girls are esis that, compared to boys, a values-alignment
more likely than boys to report that social media is message would more strongly impact girls’ motiva-
“very” or “extremely” important for friendships, and tion to control SMU.
they place greater emphasis on getting a lot of likes
and are more worried about being excluded from
social media posts (Rideout & Robb, 2018). Girls
Study 1: Initial Evaluation of Messaging Effects
are also more likely to report that they spend too
much time on social media (Pew Research Center, In Study 1, high school students were randomly
2018a) and that they take more breaks from SMU assigned to read a values-alignment message, a
due to “digital drama” (Rideout & Robb, 2018). traditional message, or no message at all. A

Assessed immediately after reading experimental messages

Experimental Manipulation Process Variable 1 Process Variable 2 Immediate Outcome Longer-Term Outcome

Foster a perceived
Reading an exposé alignment between
that raises awareness Strengthen the belief self-regulating social
Strengthen overall
about how social that social media media use and acting
motivation to control Result in greater
media companies use companies use independently from
adult control while also
social media use, behavior change
“addictive designs” to addictive designs,
which, in turn, will…
maximize advertising which will… working toward social
revenue will… justice,
which will…

Figure 1. Stylized theory of change for the values-alignment message. The model proposes that reading an expose that details how
social media companies use “addictive designs” will: (1) strengthen the belief that social media companies use addictive design features,
which will (2) foster greater perceived alignment between self-regulating social media use and autonomy and social justice, which will
(3) strengthen overall motivation to control social media use, which will culminate in (4) longer-term behavior change.
Adolescent Values and Social Media Use 1721

values-alignment message sought to frame a moti- school computers during school hours. Activities
vation to control SMU as being aligned with desires were presented via Qualtrics Survey Software. Stu-
for autonomy and social justice, whereas a tradi- dents did not have contact with the research team.
tional message sought to motivate behavior change The experiment was fully blinded.
by highlighting long-term personal benefits of Data were collected during the 2017–2018 aca-
avoiding SMU. We hypothesized that students who demic year. Approximately 3 months before the
saw a values-alignment message would be more experimental session, students completed a baseline
motivated to control SMU immediately afterward survey in which they provided basic demographic
and exhibit greater awareness of their SMU and information and answered questions about their
engage in more behavior change over subsequent social media beliefs, attitudes, and behaviors, in
weeks. Exploratory tests explored gender differ- addition to topics unrelated to social media (e.g.,
ences in the messages’ effects on motivation to con- academic motivation, mindfulness). During the
trol SMU. experimental session, students were randomly
assigned at the individual level to either a values-
alignment message, traditional message, or no-treat-
Method ment control. Students did their assigned activities
and then completed a battery of self-report and
Participants
behavioral willingness measures. Finally, students
The analytic sample included 858 first- and at two of the three schools completed a brief fol-
third-year students from three public high schools low-up survey assessing the short-term enduring
(two suburban, one rural) in the Northeastern Uni- impact of the messages (the third school did not
ted States. The sample reflects the maximum num- participate in the follow-up session, as acknowl-
ber of students who were eligible and available to edged in the preregistration plan). A total of 331
participate on the days approved by the schools. (out of 428) students accessed the follow-up survey.
Post hoc power analysis using G*Power v3.1 (Faul, Due to the timing of state-standardized testing, one
Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007) shows that school administered the follow-up survey 2 weeks
N = 858 and a = .05 provides 74.8% power to after the experimental session, and the other admin-
detect a one-way, omnibus effect of Cohen’s d = .20 istered it approximately 6 weeks after. Students
(a conventionally small effect size). The sample was were given a class period (~40 min) to complete the
split equally across first-year (50.93%) and third- baseline and experimental surveys, and approxi-
year (49.07%) students. According to students’ mately 15 min to complete the follow-up survey.
self-reports, they were M = 15.47 years old (SD =
1.13 years, range = 14–18); 44.76% were girls and
Experimental Messages for Studies 1 and 2
44.41% were boys; 74.36% were White, 4.66% Black,
1.75% Asian, 1.17% Hispanic, and 7.46% mixed or The two messages (values-alignment, traditional)
other races and ethnicities (including American involved brief reading and writing activities deliv-
Indian, Middle Eastern, and Pacific Islander). ered online via Qualtrics. The core content for each
Demographic information was self-reported during was delivered with text that was supplemented
a baseline survey 3 months prior to random assign- with images, graphs and captions, and sources.
ment and is thus only available for a maximum of Both messages involved three writing exercises: one
767 students (89.4%) who completed both the base- at the halfway point, where students were invited
line and experimental surveys. See Table S1 for to write about what information they found most
school-level demographic information (National interesting so far, and two at the end where stu-
Center for Education Statistics, 2020). dents were invited to answer questions related to
their assigned message’s content. The full experi-
mental messages are freely available to researchers
Procedure
upon request subject to terms of a Material Transfer
This study was approved by the University of Agreement; the Supporting Information provides
Pittsburgh Human Research Protection Office. sample excerpts from each message.
Schools sent an informational letter about the study Values-alignment message. Our message
and an opt-out permission form to parents. Students adhered to the structure and incorporated some of
provided assent before completing study materials. the values-alignment message text used in Bryan
Classroom teachers introduced the study to students, et al. (2016), but the content was redesigned to
who then completed all activities independently on focus on social media (rather than junk food). As in
1722 Galla, Choukas-Bradley, Fiore, and Esposito

Bryan et al. (2016), our message used a two- and to be a catalyst for positive social change (by
pronged approach to frame controlling SMU as a demanding more humane technology designs).
means of pursuing autonomy and working toward To facilitate internalization of this information,
social justice. the message ended with two techniques that are
First, the message was presented as an expose of common to wise interventions (Walton, 2014):
industry practices. It was written to have an excit- descriptive norms and saying-is-believing. To create
ing and subversive feel by giving information that a supportive descriptive norm, following prior
social media companies supposedly would not research by Bryan et al. (2016), the message sug-
want their users to know (Yeager et al., 2018). It gested that a lot of other high school students feel
drew upon various sources (e.g., popular press arti- shocked and angry about what social media compa-
cles, books, blogs, videos) to describe how social nies are doing, and that they plan to fight back by
media companies use “addictive designs” to entice learning new strategies to take back control over
adolescents and others into spending more time on their SMU. The message then showed three stories
their platforms in order to maximize advertising attributed to high school social media users (the sto-
revenue. The message included descriptions of ries were either adapted from Bryan et al. (2016) or
how: (1) the pull-to-refresh and like features can were developed based on interviews and focus
hook users by exploiting intermittent variable groups with individuals who piloted early versions
rewards, much like slot machines do; (2) Snap- of the message). The stories sought to depict the per-
streaks can hook users by exploiting desires for ception of anger directed at social media companies,
social reciprocity; and (3) bottomless newsfeeds can and to suggest that this anger could be harnessed
hook users by removing stopping cues (e.g., Alter, into positive, prosocial action (Bryan et al., 2016). In
2017; Bosker, 2016; Harris, 2016; Lewis, 2017; Mor- the saying-is-believing exercise, again following
gans, 2017). The goal in highlighting such designs Bryan et al. (2016), students were asked to write brief
was to depict controlling SMU as a way to reassert essays explaining why they thought other people
autonomy from companies trying to control and were angry about what social media companies
profit from them (see also, Bryan et al., 2019). were doing and what new actions they could take to
Second, the message emphasized societal conse- fight back and regain control of their SMU.
quences and harmful effects of these designs on Traditional message (active control). The active
adolescents’ changing views of themselves and their control message was designed to simulate an
social worlds. It described results of national sur- approach that is more common to universal behav-
veys and data collected during the baseline survey ior change programs delivered in schools (Bryan
(presented anonymously) showing adolescents’ self- et al., 2019; Yeager et al., 2018)—that is, by high-
reports of feeling “addicted to their phones” (Com- lighting the potential long-term benefits of avoiding
mon Sense Media, 2016a), worrying about missing SMU, teaching self-regulation strategies, and affirm-
out, and feeling pressure to be constantly on social ing societal values of positive behavior. It paralleled
media, and results from epidemiological data show- the values-alignment message in structure but dif-
ing a possible connection between the rise in SMU fered in content.
and documented generational increases in mental The traditional message was inspired in part by
health problems (Twenge, Joiner, Rogers, & Martin, existing digital citizenship curricula available at the
2018). It included short profiles of industry execu- time (Common Sense Media, 2016b) and by the
tives and engineers who would not allow their chil- healthy eating control condition as described in
dren to use the technology they helped create Bryan et al. (2016). However, the traditional message
(Alter, 2017) or who have since spoken out against used original content developed by the study
these addictive designs (e.g., Lewis, 2017). The mes- authors. It drew upon various sources (e.g., popular
sage also presented results from the baseline survey press articles, news and other websites, books, scien-
highlighting how these designs may be impacting tific studies) to describe how reducing or eliminating
participants themselves and their friends (i.e., in SMU might benefit health (by increasing sleep qual-
terms of how much time was being “stolen” by ity), social functioning (by increasing communication
social media companies to make more advertising skills), performance in school (by reducing dis-
revenue). Finally, it suggested that more and more tractibility), and privacy (by reducing likelihood of
adolescents were beginning to take back control personal information being spread online). Consis-
over their SMU as an act of rebellion. The goal in tent with the Bryan et al. (2016) healthy eating con-
providing this information was to depict controlling trol condition, after reading about these various
SMU as a way to join a growing cultural movement long-term benefits, students read tips ostensibly
Adolescent Values and Social Media Use 1723

endorsed by adults about different self-control in learning new self-control strategies to cut back on
strategies they could use to eliminate or reduce their SMU. These were the only three variables assessed
SMU. Strategies included deactivating social media at baseline that matched the primary outcomes
accounts, deleting smartphone social media applica- described in the preregistration plan. We note that
tions, giving social media login information to par- the items used to assess these constructs were not
ents (to self-impose time limits), and installing all the same as those used to assess the same con-
smartphone applications (or Internet browser exten- structs during the experimental session.
sions) to monitor and limit how much time is spent Immediate outcomes
on social media. To instill a descriptive norm, the Motivation to control SMU. Four items
message presented stories attributed to adults (e.g., assessed students’ intentions to reduce SMU (e.g.,
parent) in support of the benefits that may accumu- “I’m really going to try to limit my time on social
late from staying off social media (Bryan et al., 2016). media”). Three items assessed students’ interest to
For the saying-is-believing exercise, students were learn new strategies for controlling their SMU (e.g., “I
then asked to write brief essays explaining why they want to learn new strategies to take control over my
think it is beneficial to eliminate or reduce social SMU”). To assess behavioral willingness, students
media from their life and what new actions they responded “yes” or “no” to two different choices
could take to follow the advice of the program and involving SMU. First, they indicated their willingness
the adults who endorsed it (Bryan et al., 2016). to use a smartphone application (“app”) that keeps
The traditional message served as a strong con- track of how much time they spend on social media.
trol because it conveyed information about the They read that these apps could help them better
potential long-term personal benefits of avoiding control their SMU. Students who answered “no”
SMU and provided explicit recommendations for were directed to the next question. Those who
strategies that students might use to control their answered “yes” were given suggestions about possi-
SMU. ble apps to try. Second, students indicated their will-
No-treatment control (passive control). Students ingness to join the “Time Well Spent” movement and
assigned to the passive control condition completed demand better technology from social media compa-
the baseline survey (for a second time in Study 1 or nies (in Study 1 only, this question was framed to
for the first time in Study 2) in lieu of receiving a students as “signing a petition”). Those who
values-alignment or traditional message. answered “no” were directed to the end of the sur-
vey. Students who answered “yes” were given a
brief description of the Center for Humane Technol-
Measures for Studies 1 and 2
ogy (and their “Time Well Spent” initiative), includ-
The measures described here were mentioned ing a URL to its website, where they could receive
explicitly in the preregistration plans for Studies 1 periodic informational emails, learn tips for how to
and 2. They were all created originally for our manage smartphone usage, and read about the orga-
experiments. Unless otherwise stated, the following nization’s efforts to promote ethical and empower-
measures were identical across Studies 1 and 2. ing technology design.
Other measures were collected during the experi- Process variables. Eight items assessed stu-
ments but were not part of the preregistration plan dents’ beliefs about social media companies’ use of addic-
and so are not described. Scale scores for each con- tive design features to hook users (e.g., “Social media
struct were calculated by averaging items, such that companies use science and engineering to make their
higher scores indicated greater agreement with the apps more addictive”). Using prior research as a
construct. Item order was randomized within each guide (Bryan et al., 2016), six items assessed the
measure, but all measures were presented in a fixed degree to which students viewed reducing SMU as
order (that mirrored our theory of change) across being aligned with autonomy and social justice. Three
students. All continuous self-report items were items assessed autonomy (e.g., “Cutting back on
rated from 1 = not at all true to 5 = completely true. social media is a way to stand up to people who are
Descriptive statistics are provided in Table S2. The trying to control us”), and three items assessed social
Supporting Information also includes a full list of justice (e.g., “When I cut back on social media, I’m
items. demanding better technology for other kids and
Baseline (prerandom assignment) measures. In teens”). Autonomy and social justice measures were
Study 1, students responded to items assessing be- significantly correlated (Study 1: r = .66, p < .001;
liefs about social media companies’ use of addictive Study 2: r = .67, p < .001). As in Bryan et al. (2016),
design features, intentions to reduce SMU, and interest we combined these items into a single composite.
1724 Galla, Choukas-Bradley, Fiore, and Esposito

Follow-up outcomes. Six items assessed the significantly by condition or condition-by-gender


degree to which students made changes in their SMU. (Table S5). Students assigned to a values-alignment
Three items assessed reductions in SMU (e.g., “I’ve message (M = 28.99, SD = 19.32) or traditional mes-
tried to cut back on social media”), and three items sage (M = 30.34, SD = 19.68) wrote similar numbers
assessed the use of new strategies to control the use of words on average across the three writing
of social media (e.g., “I’ve tried new strategies to prompts (t(534) = 0.80, p = .423; also see Table S20).
take control over my SMU”). Four items assessed the
degree to which students were mindful of urges to use
Main Effects on Immediate Outcomes
social media (e.g., “I’ve been more aware of how often
I feel the urge to check social media”). Finally, six Motivation to control SMU. As shown in
items assessed the degree to which students were Table 1, students assigned to a values-alignment
aware of how certain social media design features tried to message had stronger intentions to reduce SMU (F
keep them online longer (e.g., “I’ve noticed how social (2, 805) = 7.26, p = .001) and greater interest in
media apps are like slot machines”). learning new self-control strategies (F(2, 804) = 6.09,
p = .002) than did students in either a traditional
message (ps < .005, ds = .30 and .27) or no-treat-
Analysis Plan for Studies 1 and 2
ment control (ps < .005, ds = .26 and .25). Students
We preregistered the study design, hypotheses, who saw a values-alignment message were also
and analysis plan prior to data collection: Study 1: more willing to join the “Time Well Spent” move-
https://aspredicted.org/gc2s3.pdf; Study 2: ment (aka “sign a petition,” Wald v2(2) = 9.25,
https://osf.io/qhgd5. Deidentified data and syntax p = .010) compared to students in a traditional mes-
are available on the Open Science Framework sage (p = .003) but not a no-treatment control
(https://osf.io/thbx4/). Full details of the analytic (p = .079). There was no effect of condition (Wald
plan are provided in the Supporting Information. v2(2) = 3.30, p = .192) on willingness to use a time-
Briefly, we fit one-way analysis of variance monitoring app. See Table S7 for pairwise compar-
(ANOVA) and logistic regression to test preregis- isons and effect size estimates.
tered confirmatory hypotheses on immediate and Process variables. As predicted, students
follow-up outcomes. We used indirect effect analy- assigned to a values-alignment message held a
sis with 5,000 bootstrapped samples and bias-cor- stronger belief that social media companies use
rected 95% CI to test the preregistered theory of addictive design features (F(2, 810) = 11.10,
change. Finally, we used factorial ANOVA and p < .001) compared to students in both a traditional
logistic regression to examine gender moderation message (p < .001, d = .41) and no-treatment control
effects on immediate and follow-up outcomes. Sen- (p = .014, d = .21); the no-treatment control also
sitivity analyses, reported in the Supporting Infor- held a stronger belief compared to the traditional
mation, show that all preregistered confirmatory message (p = .024, d = .20). Also consistent with
tests were robust to: (a) missing data (Tables S14– hypotheses, students who saw a values-alignment
S16), and (b) corrections for multiple comparisons message perceived greater alignment between
(with the exception of some condition-by-gender reducing SMU and pursuing autonomy and social
interaction effects in Study 2, Tables S17–19). The justice (F(2, 808) = 7.33, p = .001) compared to stu-
Supporting Information also provides details about dents in a traditional message (p < .001, d = .33)
the study samples (Figures S1 and S2), data exclu- and (at significance threshold) a no-treatment con-
sions, the experimental messages and wording trol (p = .050, d = .17). The traditional message and
changes across Studies 1 and 2, minor deviations no-treatment control did not differ significantly
from the preregistration plan, and results of tests (p = .058, d = .16). See Table 1. (Analyzing auton-
on ancillary preregistered outcomes (Table S9). omy and social justice separately revealed similar
effects; Table S9.)

Results
Main Effects on Follow-Up Outcomes
Preliminary Analysis
Two-to-six weeks after the experimental session,
Random assignment was effective (see Table S3). conditions did not differ significantly on behavior
Data were available for 93.4% to 94.8% of the imme- change (F(2, 314) = 2.43, p = .090), mindfulness of
diate outcomes and 73.6% to 74.8% of the follow-up SMU (F(2, 317) = 0.23, p = .797), or awareness of
outcomes; rates of missing data did not differ addictive social media design features (F(2,
Adolescent Values and Social Media Use 1725

Table 1
Effect of Condition on Immediate Outcomes

Study 1 Study 2

Test of between-condition Test of between-condition


Variables n M SD difference n M SD difference

Motivation to control social media use


Intentions to reduce social media use
No-treatment control 275 2.58b 0.99 F(2,805) = 7.26, p = .001 567 2.70b 1.12 F(2, 1,676) = 16.18, p < .001
Traditional message 266 2.53b 0.99 555 2.69b 1.15
Values-alignment message 267 2.84a 1.05 557 3.03a 1.18
Interest in learning strategies for taking control over social media use
No-treatment control 275 2.76b 0.98 F(2,804) = 6.09, p = .002 566 2.80b 1.09 F(2, 1,677) = 17.80, p < .001
Traditional message 266 2.74b 1.01 556 2.87b 1.11
Values-alignment message 266 3.02a 1.04 558 3.18a 1.15
Willingness to use a smartphone time-monitoring application*
No-treatment control 276 37.3%a Wald v2(2) = 3.30, p = .192 568 33.8%a,b Wald v2(2) = 9.20, p = .010
Traditional message 265 34.7%a 555 29.7%b
Values-alignment message 265 42.3%a 558 38.4%a
Willingness to join "Time Well Spent" movement*
No-treatment control 276 23.2%a,b Wald v2(2) = 9.25, p = .010 566 17.5%a,b Wald v2(2) = 8.04, p = .018
Traditional message 264 18.6%b 553 13.6%b
Values-alignment message 261 29.9%a 557 19.9%a
Process variables
Belief that social media companies use addictive design features
No-treatment control 274 3.36b 0.69 F(2,810) = 11.10, p < .001 580 3.22b 0.64 F(2, 1,720) = 165.43, p < .001
Traditional message 270 3.23c 0.67 568 3.05c 0.72
Values-alignment message 269 3.50a 0.71 575 3.77a 0.74
Appeals to autonomy and social justice
No-treatment control 277 2.44a,b 0.87 F(2,808) = 7.33, p = .001 573 2.26b 0.85 F(2, 1,699) = 50.68, p < .001
Traditional message 266 2.30b 0.83 562 2.25b 0.87
Values-alignment message 268 2.59a 0.87 567 2.73a 1.02

Note. Unadjusted means (M) and standard deviations (SD) are displayed. Different subscript letters indicate M are different at p < .05.
*The between-condition difference and pairwise comparisons are based on logistic regression with full-information maximum likelihood
to account for missing data, but descriptive statistics (n, M, SD) are based on observed data.

312) = 1.98, p = .140; (see Table 2 and Table S8 for preregistration plan. We corrected these ambiguities
pairwise comparisons). in the preregistration plan for Study 2, which
enabled a single test of our full theory of change.
Theory of Change
Gender Differences
Indirect effect analysis revealed support for a pre-
liminary theory of change. Compared to a no-treat- Exploratory analyses revealed that two of the six
ment control and traditional message, students in a condition-by-gender interaction effects on immediate
values-alignment condition held a stronger belief outcomes were significant. The first, on the belief
that social media companies use addictive designs, that social media companies use addictive designs (F
which, in turn, significantly predicted many of the (2, 727) = 3.22, p = .040), indicated a slight enhance-
remaining outcomes specified in Figure 1 (see ment effect for both girls and boys assigned to a val-
Table S10). We did not test the full theory of change ues-alignment message. The second interaction
in Study 1 because we realized after the fact that our effect, on intentions to reduce SMU (F(2, 720) = 3.60,
preregistration plan did not unambiguously describe p = .028), indicated an enhancement effect for girls,
the entire sequence of hypothesized psychological but not boys, assigned to a values alignment mes-
changes. Thus, in Study 1, we only fit models based sage. No significant interaction effects were observed
on the most straightforward interpretation of the for follow-up outcomes (see Table S12).
1726 Galla, Choukas-Bradley, Fiore, and Esposito

Table 2
Effect of Condition on Follow-Up Outcomes

Study 1 Study 2

Test of between-condition Test of between-condition


Variables n M SD difference n M SD difference

Mindfulness of social media use


No-treatment control 111 2.77a 0.98 F(2,317) = 0.23, p = .797 401 3.00a 1.06 F(2, 1,269) = 0.74, p = .480
Traditional message 106 2.71a 1.09 430 3.09a 1.20
Values-alignment message 103 2.67a 1.02 441 3.09a 1.14
Behavior change
No-treatment control 111 2.28a 1.05 F(2,314) = 2.43, p = .090 398 2.59a 1.12 F(2, 1,266) = 0.63, p = .533
Traditional message 105 2.02a 0.94 431 2.52a 1.13
Values-alignment message 101 2.29a 0.99 440 2.51a 1.16
Awareness of addictive social media design features
No-treatment control 111 2.65a,b 1.06 F(2,312) = 1.98, p = .140 397 2.76b 1.13 F(2, 1,257) = 5.63, p = .004
Traditional message 104 2.50b 1.14 428 2.75b 1.16
Values-alignment message 100 2.81a 1.14 435 2.99a 1.20

Note. Unadjusted means (M) and standard deviations (SD) are displayed. Different subscript letters indicate M are different at p < .05.

in a different part of the country, and with a longer


Discussion
follow-up period. We also addressed the limitations
Study 1 provided initial support for the hypothesis of the original preregistration plan by more clearly
that aligning behavior change with intrinsically specifying models for binary outcomes and the com-
rewarding adolescent values of autonomy and social plete theory of change (Figure 1). Based on explora-
justice would boost motivation (Bryan et al., 2016). tory findings from Study 1, we also added a
Compared to students who read a traditional mes- preregistered confirmatory hypothesis that a values-
sage highlighting long-term personal benefits of alignment message would be more effective at moti-
avoiding SMU, those who read a brief values-align- vating girls to control their SMU compared to boys.
ment message that framed controlling SMU as a
social justice-oriented, autonomous behavior were
more motivated to self-regulate their SMU immedi- Method
ately afterward. Though there were no main effects of
Participants
a values-alignment message on follow-up outcomes,
theory of change analysis revealed that strengthening The analytic sample included 1,875 first- through
the belief that social media companies use addictive fourth-year students from one public, suburban
designs did predict greater awareness of addictive high school in the Southeastern United States. The
social media design features in the weeks following sample size again reflects the maximum number of
viewing the message. This finding accords with students who were eligible and available to partici-
results of national surveys showing that a majority of pate on the testing days provided by the schools.
adolescents believe technology companies try to Power analysis using G*Power v3.1 showed that a
manipulate them into using their platforms (Rideout sample of 1,875 and a = .05 provides 97.9% power
& Robb, 2018), but until now had not been examined to detect a one-way, omnibus effect and a condi-
as a possible motivator of behavior change. Finally, tion-by-gender interaction effect of Cohen’s d = .20.
Study 1 provided suggestive evidence that a values- According to official school records, students were
alignment message was more effective for motivating M = 15.92 years old (SD = 1.22 years, range = 13–
girls to control their SMU compared to boys. 19); 39.89% of the sample were first-year students,
24.32% second-year, 19.84% third-year, and 15.95%
fourth-year; 50.72% were girls and 49.28% were
boys; 48.16% were White, 34.35% Hispanic, 7.41%
Study 2: Replication of Messaging Effects
Asian, 7.15% Black, and 2.83% mixed or other races
Study 2 was a direct replication in another sample of and ethnicities (including American Indian and
high school students, conducted 1 year after Study 1, Pacific Islander); 24.0% were eligible for free or
Adolescent Values and Social Media Use 1727

reduced-price lunch; 9.07% were English language message had stronger intentions to reduce SMU (F(2,
learners; and 15.41% had an individualized educa- 1,676) = 16.18, p < .001) and greater interest in learn-
tion plan. Information about race/ethnicity was ing new self-control strategies (F(2, 1,677) = 17.80,
missing for two (0.1%) students. See Table S1 for p < .001) than did students in either a traditional
aggregate, school-level demographic information. message (ps < .001, ds = .29 and .27) or no-treatment
control (ps < .001, ds = .29 and 0.33). Students who
Procedure saw a values-alignment message were also more
willing to join the “Time Well Spent” movement
Data for Study 2 were collected and de-identified
(Wald v2(2) = 8.04, p = .018) and use a time-monitor-
by the Character Lab Research Network (CLRN)
ing app (Wald v2(2) = 9.20, p = .010) compared to
before being shared with our team. The CLRN is a
students in a traditional message (ps = .005 and .002,
consortium of schools working collaboratively with
respectively) but not a no-treatment control
scientists to advance character development research
(ps = .295 and .112, respectively). The traditional
(https://characterlab.org/research-network/). Study
message and no-treatment control did not differ sig-
2 was approved by the University of Pittsburgh
nificantly on any outcome (see Table 1).
Human Research Protection Office and the CLRN.
To gauge the effect of a values-alignment message
Study 2 was conducted during the 2018-2019
on overall motivation to control SMU, following our
academic year, approximately 1 year after Study 1.
preregistration plan, we averaged z-scores of the four
Students were given a class period (~45 min) to
motivation measures (rs = .23–.72, ps < .001) to form
complete experimental and follow-up surveys during
a composite. Students assigned to a values-alignment
regular school hours. We made every effort to conduct
message had stronger overall motivation to control
a precise replication, and thus purposefully kept the
SMU (F(2, 1,688) = 16.35, p < .001) compared to both
experimental materials and procedures as similar to
the traditional message (t(1,688) = 5.31, p < .001,
Study 1 as was possible. The following are notable
95% CI [.198, .430], d = .31) and no-treatment control
exceptions: (1) there was no baseline (prerandom
(t(1,688) = 4.50, p < .001, 95% CI [.149, .380], d = .26).
assignment) survey; and (2) the follow-up survey was
The traditional message and no-treatment control
administered approximately 3 months after the exper-
did not differ (t(1,688) = 0.83, p = .404, 95% CI
imental session. A total of 1,296 (69.1%) students
[ .067, .165], d = .05).
accessed the follow-up survey.
Process measures. Students assigned to a val-
ues-alignment message again held a stronger belief
Results and Discussion that social media companies use addictive designs (F
(2, 1,720) = 165.43, p < .001) and perceived greater
Preliminary Analysis
alignment between reducing SMU and autonomy
Random assignment was effective (Table S4). Data and social justice (F(2, 1,699) = 50.68, p < .001) com-
were available for 89.4% to 91.9% of the immediate pared to students in either a traditional message
outcomes and 67.2% to 67.8% of the follow-up out- (ps < .001, ds = .99 and .51) or no-treatment control
comes. Rates of missing data for the immediate (but (ps < .001, ds = .80 and .50; see Table 1).
not follow-up) outcomes showed differential attrition
by condition and condition-by-gender, with girls in Main Effects on Follow-Up Outcomes
the traditional message and values-alignment mes-
sage having more missing data than girls in the no- At 3-month follow-up, students assigned to a
treatment control (there were a few condition differ- values-alignment message reported greater aware-
ences across boys, too, but these were less consis- ness of addictive social media design features in
tently observed; see Table S6). As in Study 1, their daily life (F(2, 1,257) = 5.63, p = .004) com-
students assigned to a values-alignment message pared to a traditional message and the no-treatment
(M = 41.52, SD = 30.05) or traditional message control (ps < .01, ds = .20 and .20, respectively). See
(M = 41.78, SD = 28.90) wrote similar numbers of Table 2. Condition was not a significant predictor
words on average across the three writing prompts of behavior change (F(2, 1,266) = 0.63, p = .533) or
(t(1,196) = 0.16, p = .877; see also Table S21). mindfulness of SMU (F(2, 1,269) = 0.74, p = .480).

Main Effects on Immediate Outcomes Theory of Change


Motivation to control SMU. Replicating results Indirect effect analysis supported our overall the-
of Study 1, students assigned to a values-alignment ory of change (Figure 1), both when comparing a
1728 Galla, Choukas-Bradley, Fiore, and Esposito

values-alignment message to a traditional message results. The first variable was a process measure for
(indirect effect = .041, 95% CI [ .059, .027]), and the information conveyed in the traditional message
when comparing a values-alignment message to a (e.g., “Staying off social media helps me protect my
no-treatment control (indirect effect = .032, 95% privacy”), measured at immediate posttest. The tra-
CI [ .046, .021]; see Figure S5 and Table S11). ditional message and values-alignment message did
not differ significantly on this variable (Study 1:
p = .179; Study 2: p = .324), but both tended to out-
Gender Differences
perform the no-treatment control (ps = .134 to
Three of the six condition-by-gender interaction < .001). Thus, students assigned to the traditional
effects on immediate outcomes were significant (or message internalized the core information at higher
on the threshold of significance): (a) belief that rates than would be expected had they not received
social media companies use addictive designs (F(2, any message (Table S9).
1,717) = 6.32, p = .002); (b) alignment of autonomy The second variable was self-reported time spent
and social justice with reducing SMU (F(2, on social media, informed by prior research (Woods
1,696) = 4.26, p = .014); and (c) willingness to use a & Scott, 2016), measured during the follow-up ses-
time-monitoring app (Wald v2(2) = 5.97, p = .050; sion. In Study 1, neither the main effect of condition
Figure 2). Consistent with hypotheses, girls in a val- nor condition-by-gender interaction effect were sig-
ues-alignment message reported the highest levels nificant (F(2, 325) = 1.16, p = .315; F(2, 292) = 0.27,
on each outcome. No significant interactions were p = .760, respectively). Likewise, in Study 2, the
observed for follow-up outcomes (see Table S13). main effect of condition was not significant (F(2,
1,254) = 0.35, p = .705; see Tables S9, S12, and S13).
However, in Study 2, and consistent with the
preregistered findings, the data revealed a signifi-
Non-Preregistered Exploratory Analysis
cant condition-by-gender interaction effect (F(2,
To promote greater transparency of the research 1,251) = 7.12, p = .001). Boys assigned to a values-
findings, we briefly report results of analyses using alignment message reported spending more time
two variables that were not preregistered, but on social media 3 months later compared to boys
whose relevance to the current study may appear assigned to the no-treatment control (p = .007), but
straightforward. See Supporting Information for full not compared to the traditional message (p = .349).

100%

90% NTC
Smartphone Time-Monitoring Application

80% TM
VAM
(Estimated Marginal Means)

70%
Willingness to Use a

60%

50%

40%

30%

20%

10%

0%
Boy Girl

Figure 2. Condition-by-gender interaction effect on willingness to use a smartphone time-monitoring application to control social media
use (Study 2). NTC = no-treatment control; TM = traditional message; VAM = values-alignment message. Error bars are 1 SE.
Adolescent Values and Social Media Use 1729

Contrariwise, girls assigned to a values-alignment appealing to autonomy and social justice, two val-
message reported spending less time on social ues whose fulfillment is intrinsically rewarding to
media compared to the no-treatment control adolescents (Bryan et al., 2016). By raising aware-
(p = .009), but not compared to the traditional mes- ness about social media designs that entice people
sage (p = .091; see Table S13 and Figure S6). into spending more time online and about the toll
it might take on society, the message encouraged
adolescents to view self-regulating SMU as an
opportunity to make an independent, socially con-
General Discussion
scious choice. Because adolescence is a developmen-
The current investigation tested a theory of adoles- tal period in which individuals reprioritize their
cent behavior change that argues that adolescents’ goals toward immediate social rewards (Crone &
sensitivity to immediate social rewards—often con- Dahl, 2012), framing self-regulation as a means of
sidered a barrier to traditional educational pro- fulfilling such intrinsically rewarding goals could
grams—can be repurposed to encourage greater help to offset the hedonic pleasure provided by
self-regulation (Bryan et al., 2016; Telzer, 2016; Yea- SMU (Sherman et al., 2016). Pursuing goals for
ger et al., 2018). We tested this hypothesis in the intrinsic rather than extrinsic reasons is known to
context of a novel, developmentally relevant out- support behavioral persistence in adolescents
come: adolescents’ motivation to self-regulate their (Galla, Amemiya, & Wang, 2018; Woolley & Fish-
SMU. Across two preregistered field experiments bach, 2016).
with 2,733 adolescents, we found some evidence in Results extend prior efforts to harness adoles-
support of this theory. Adolescents who read a cents’ autonomy and social justice motives (Bryan
brief message that aligned the desired behavioral et al., 2016; Hersey et al., 2005), not only by
outcome with their drives for autonomy and social focusing on another outcome relevant to adoles-
justice had stronger motivation to control SMU cent development but also by considering hetero-
compared to those who saw no message or who geneity of effects. We hypothesized and found
read a message that conveyed information about that a values-alignment message had a stronger
the benefits of avoiding SMU. This boost in motiva- effect for girls than for boys. Results of non-pre-
tion predicted self-reported change in social media registered, exploratory tests (in Study 2) also
behaviors, including attempts to cut back and try showed that a values-alignment message led to
new self-control strategies, up to 3 months later. In less self-reported SMU 3 months later for girls,
Study 2, values-alignment messaging also led to whereas it led to more self-reported time on social
greater awareness of “addictive designs” 3 months media for boys.
later. In both studies, preregistered moderation It is possible that the values-alignment message’s
analyses indicated that a values-alignment message content—which described prominent “peer feed-
was more effective at motivating girls than boys. back” features such as likes and Snapstreaks—more
effectively captured girls’ SMU experiences, thereby
increasing their receptivity to it. As discussed ear-
Implications for Science and Practice
lier, adolescent girls are more likely than boys to
Results of this investigation add new empirical report emotional investment in the interpersonal
support to the theory that adolescents’ sensitivity to connection and feedback provided by social media,
immediate rewards can be leveraged to create inter- and that they spend too much time on social media
nalized motivation for behavior change. A common and have taken breaks to escape “digital drama”
approach for encouraging adolescents to set and (Pew Research Center, 2018a; Rideout & Robb,
pursue positive long-term goals is to highlight 2018). They also report greater preoccupation with
future consequences of the actions they take now and social comparison on social media (Boer, Ste-
(Steinberg, 2015). While this approach is supported vens, et al., 2020; Nesi & Prinstein, 2015). Empha-
by decision-making theories, in practice, such sizing how design features might subtly intensify
appeals often show reduced effectiveness during compulsive use and social evaluation could be
adolescence (Yeager et al., 2018). Our traditional more pertinent for girls. Our findings do not neces-
message was no more effective than receiving no sarily mean that this approach cannot also motivate
message on motivation. boys (see Bryan et al., 2019), but future research is
Why was a values-alignment message more com- necessary to unpack these gender differences to
pelling to teens? We theorize that it increased the optimize values-alignment messages for various
immediate symbolic value of controlling SMU by demographic subgroups.
1730 Galla, Choukas-Bradley, Fiore, and Esposito

What are the practical implications of this inves- empirical rebuttals to that study (e.g., Heffer, Good,
tigation? Many school-based educational programs Daly, MacDonell, & Willoughby, 2019) and to the
that focus on long-term consequences show reduced argument that SMU is consequentially associated
effectiveness for motivating self-regulation during with adolescents’ mental health (Orben & Przybyl-
adolescence (Yeager et al., 2018)—presumably ski, 2019). We should note that the values-align-
because appealing to distant outcomes is an uphill ment message was not intended to reduce
battle against adolescents’ drives for immediate adolescents’ depressive symptoms (and we did not
rewards (Steinberg, 2015). But this does not mean include any such measures in our study), nor does
that any effort by adults to help adolescents inter- the message’s argument need to hinge on a connec-
nalize positive long-term goals is destined to fall tion to depressive symptoms. But in light of knowl-
short. Our findings highlight the possibility that edge gained in recent years, we believe it would be
instead of trying to fight against adolescents’ sensi- prudent in the future to remove the suggestion that
tivity to rewards, it might be better to make the act the rise in depressive symptoms may be connected
of behavior change more immediately rewarding: to increased SMU until further research has been
In this case, by reframing desired behavior as an conducted.
opportunity for adolescents to make independent Furthermore, the term “addictive” to characterize
choices that contribute positively to the world. This technology use is increasingly being debated, even
suggests that desires for autonomy and social when used colloquially. Our values-alignment mes-
justice be given consideration in the creation of sage followed other academics who have used it to
adult-delivered messages targeting adolescents’ describe design features intended to capture and
technology choices. sustain users’ attention (Alter, 2017) and national
campaigns that have used it to raise awareness
about these design practices (Morgan, 2018). A
Limitations and Future Directions
recently revised school-based digital citizenship cur-
Our experiments have numerous limitations. riculum now also includes the same term that our
Practically, this investigation should not be con- message used, “addictive design,” to educate stu-
strued as a critique of any specific digital citizen- dents about features of an “app that are intended
ship program since the content of our traditional to hook the user into frequent use” (Common Sense
message was derived from many sources (and also Media, 2019b). However, others argue that such ter-
because one key digital citizenship program avail- minology risks creating a moral panic (Orben,
able during our experiments was redesigned in 2020). To avoid these concerns, if the term “addic-
2019; Common Sense Media, 2019a). This investiga- tive designs” continues to be used, it will be impor-
tion should also not be taken as a recommendation tant to explicitly define what, exactly, it means, and
that the specific values-alignment message exam- perhaps also to provide examples of “humane
ined here be used as part of a broad-based digital designs” (e.g., options to disconnect for a period of
citizenship curriculum. The message we designed is time), as one digital citizenship curriculum now
a proof-of-concept preliminary demonstration, and does (Common Sense Media, 2019b).
its contribution is primarily for advancing behavior The expose-style framing of the values-alignment
change theory. Revisions, updates, and further message, adapted from other campaigns aimed at
research are required before it could contribute changing adolescents’ health behaviors (Allen, Val-
directly to ongoing intervention efforts for fostering lone, Vargyas, Healton, & Foundation, 2010; Bryan
more responsible and intentional SMU. Given the et al., 2016), is likely not the only way to harness
speed at which social media sites can evolve— adolescents’ desires for autonomy and social justice.
alongside an evolving body of research on their Rather than framing controlling SMU as a fight
individual and societal effects—it is certain that any against injustice, another approach is to have high
message targeting SMU will also need to evolve, school students give advice to younger students.
too. For example, when describing the harmful For example, high school students could write
effects of industry practices on society, the message essays advising younger students about how “ad-
briefly referenced a study that used nationally rep- dictive designs” enhance the interests of technology
resentative data and documented possible connec- companies (by increasing advertising revenue), and
tions between the observed epidemiological rise in about self-control strategies they use that can coun-
adolescent depressive symptomatology and teract this effect by making SMU more aligned with
expanded use of digital technology, including SMU personal goals. Giving advice to younger students
(Twenge et al., 2018). There have since been is a saying-is-believing exercise that has been
Adolescent Values and Social Media Use 1731

shown to motivate behavior change in high school results generalize to other sociocultural contexts.
students (Eskreis-Winkler, Milkman, Gromet, & For example, it remains unknown whether values-
Duckworth, 2019), presumably because it boosts alignment messaging would be more effective in
confidence (Eskreis-Winkler, Fishbach, & Duck- countries with higher rates of problematic SMU
worth, 2018), but it is also a way to be of service to (Boer, van den Eijnden, et al., 2020). Future research
others. could also examine the impact of values-alignment
Scientific limitations also suggest directions for messages for younger adolescents (ages 11–13) on
future research. First, our dependent measures their motivation to take proactive steps toward
focused on a motivation to self-regulate—and in using social media intentionally. Adolescents’ sensi-
many cases, reduce—SMU. We are quick to point tivity to rewards increases with the onset of pub-
out that our measures of self-regulation included erty (Crone & Dahl, 2012), and so possibly, early
more than just cutting back SMU (e.g., using time- adolescents could be responsive to messaging that
monitoring apps, joining a social movement, leverages these emerging desires (Yeager et al.,
becoming more aware of social media design fea- 2018). But because fewer middle school youth use
tures in everyday life). Notably, cutting back SMU social media compared to high school youth (Ride-
(at least temporarily) does appear to be a relevant out, 2015), the impacts of such messages may be
goal for many adolescents (Pew Research Center, moderated by whether these younger adolescents
2018a), and setting time limits is an option for stu- and their peers have started interacting through
dents in a revised digital citizenship curriculum on social media.
finding “media balance” (Common Sense Media, Although the results provided evidence for our
2019c). But reducing overall SMU is not the only or theory of change, we cannot rule out the possibility
even the “optimal” outcome to study. Future that other differences between the values-alignment
research could examine whether values-alignment and traditional messages may have influenced
messaging increases SMU that is subjectively per- results. For example, there were small differences in
ceived to be more prosocial or personally meaning- word counts across some individual writing prompts
ful and less regrettable or distracting (Sagioglou & (Tables S20 and S21) which may indicate variation in
Greitemeyer, 2014). Raising awareness of industry cognitive processing of the messages. Also, as
practices may also increase adolescents’ interest in reported in the Supporting Information, students
learning about the broader implications of SMU reported feeling that the values-alignment message
(e.g., for privacy) and in using that interest as an was more important than the traditional message.
“opportunity to find more balance in their digital Nonetheless, students assigned to the traditional
lives” (Common Sense Media, 2019b). message internalized its core idea (that avoiding
Another limitation is that results are based exclu- social media offers long-term personal benefits) bet-
sively on a single set of self-reported outcomes that ter than those in a no-treatment control, and further,
were created or adapted for this investigation. such beliefs were positively associated with motiva-
Future research could use sensor data to capture tion to control SMU. This suggests that, overall, the
objective SMU or other behavioral outcomes (e.g., traditional message successfully activated the key
changing privacy settings). Relatedly, there were theoretical process, but future work could refine the
few direct effects of condition on longer-term out- messages’ designs to provide a purer test of theory.
comes and not all the condition-by-gender interac-
tion effects survived corrections for multiple
Conclusion
comparisons. Future research is required to repli-
cate and extend these findings using alternative Adolescent development is now entwined with
long-term outcomes. social media. Beyond imposing restrictions on ado-
The external validity of this investigation cannot lescents’ use of social media, a growing societal
be assumed for adolescents who do not share need will be to understand how adults can
demographic characteristics of those included in empower adolescents to take stock of their SMU so
our samples. Though the sample sizes provided that they can make more informed and intentional
power to detect small main and interactive effects, choices. The reach of digital citizenship programs in
we relied on convenience samples to test our over 50% of U.S. schools is a big step in this direc-
hypotheses. Participants were sampled from mostly tion (Common Sense Media, 2019d). The current
White, suburban schools where about a quarter of research suggests that leveraging adolescents’
students received financial assistance. Future drives to act independently from adult control and
research is required to understand whether our to serve a larger societal purpose, two highly
1732 Galla, Choukas-Bradley, Fiore, and Esposito

rewarding adolescent values, may be one approach Common Sense Media. (2016a). Dealing with devices: The
for instilling a motivation for self-regulating social parent-teen dynamic. Retrieved from https://www.
media use. commonsensemedia.org/technology-addiction-concern-
controversy-and-finding-balance-infographic
Common Sense Media. (2016b). Our k-12 digital citizenship
curriculum. Retrieved from https://www.commonsense
References
media.org/sites/default/files/uploads/classroom_c
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organi- urriculum/cs_digitalcitizenshipcurric_2016_release.pdf
zational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179– Common Sense Media. (2019a). Digital citizenship curricu-
211. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T lum. Retrieved from https://www.commonsense.org/
Allen, J. A., Vallone, D., Vargyas, E., Healton, C. G., & education/digital-citizenship/curriculum?grades=9,10,
Foundation, A. L. (2010). The truthâ campaign: Using 11,12
counter marketing to reduce youth smoking. In B. J. Common Sense Media. (2019b). Can media be addictive?
Healey & R. S. Zimmerman (Eds.), The new world of Retrieved from https://www.commonsense.org/educa
health promotion: New program development, implementa- tion/digital-citizenship/lesson/can-media-be-addictive
tion, and evaluation (pp. 195–215). Burlington, MA: Jones Common Sense Media. (2019c). The health effects of screen-
and Bartlett Publishers. time. Retrieved from https://www.commonsense.org/
Alter, A. L. (2017). Irresistible: The rise of addictive technol- education/digital-citizenship/lesson/the-health-effects-
ogy and the business of keeping us hooked. London, UK: of-screen-time
Penguin Press. Common Sense Media. (2019d). Digital citizenship.
American Psychological Association. (2017). Stress in Retrieved from https://www.commonsense.org/educa
America: Coping with change, part 2: Technology and social tion/digital-citizenship
media. Retrieved from https://www.apa.org/news/pre Crone, E. A., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adoles-
ss/releases/stress/2017/technology-social-media.pdf cence as a period of social–affective engagement and
Anderson, M. (2019, March 22). How parents feel about— goal flexibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 636–
and manage—their teens’ online behavior and screen time. 650. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3313
Retrieved from https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-ta Dahl, R. E., Allen, N. B., Wilbrecht, L., & Suleiman, A. B.
nk/2019/03/22/how-parents-feel-about-and-manage- (2018). Importance of investing in adolescence from a
their-teens-online-behavior-and-screen-time/ developmental science perspective. Nature, 554, 441–
Boer, M., Stevens, G., Finkenauer, C., & van den Eijnden, 450. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature25770
R. (2020). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder-symp- Eskreis-Winkler, L., Fishbach, A., & Duckworth, A. L.
toms, social media use intensity, and social media use (2018). Dear Abby: Should I give advice or receive it?
problems in adolescents: Investigating directionality. Psychological Science, 29, 1797–1806. https://doi.org/10.
Child Development, 91, e853–e865. https://doi.org/10. 1177/0956797618795472
1111/cdev.13334 Eskreis-Winkler, L., Milkman, K. L., Gromet, D. M., &
Boer, M., van den Eijnden, R. J. J. M., Boniel-Nissim, M., Duckworth, A. L. (2019). A large-scale field experiment
Wong, S.-L., Inchley, J. C., Badura, P., . . . Stevens, G. shows giving advice improves academic outcomes for
W. J. M. (2020). Adolescents’ intense and problematic the advisor. Proceedings of the National Academy of
social media use and their well-being in 29 countries. Sciences of the United States of America, 116, 14808–
Journal of Adolescent Health, 66, S89–S99. https://doi. 14810. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1908779116
org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.02.014 Evans, W. D., Price, S., Blahut, S., Hersey, J., Niederdeppe,
Bosker, B. (2016, November). The binge breaker. The J., & Ray, S. (2004). Social imagery, tobacco independence,
Atlantic. Retrieved from https://www.theatlantic.com/ and the truth campaign. Journal of Health Communication,
magazine/archive/2016/11/the-binge-breaker/501122/ 9, 425–441. https://doi.org/10.1080/1081073049050413
Bryan, C. J., Yeager, D. S., & Hinojosa, C. P. (2019). A val- Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007).
ues-alignment intervention protects adolescents from G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis pro-
the effects of food marketing. Nature Human Behaviour, gram for the social, behavioral, and biomedical
3, 596–603. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0586-6 sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.
Bryan, C. J., Yeager, D. S., Hinojosa, C. P., Chabot, A., https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
Bergen, H., Kawamura, M., & Steubing, F. (2016). Har- Fuligni, A. J. (2019). The need to contribute during ado-
nessing adolescent values to motivate healthier eating. lescence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14, 331–
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the Uni- 343. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691618805437
ted States of America, 113, 10830–10835. https://doi.org/ Galla, B. M., Amemiya, J., & Wang, M.-T. (2018). Using
10.1073/pnas.1604586113 expectancy-value theory to understand academic self-
Citizens Crime Commission of New York City. (2015). control. Learning and Instruction, 58, 22–33. https://doi.
Social media and violence. Retrieved from http://www. org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.04.004
nycrimecommission.org/pdfs/social-media-violence- Harris, T. (2016, May 18). How technology hijacks people’s
packet.pdf minds—from a magician and Google’s design ethicist.
Adolescent Values and Social Media Use 1733

Thrive Global. Retrieved from https://medium.com/ Nesi, J., & Prinstein, M. J. (2015). Using social media for
thrive-global/how-technology-hijacks-peoples-minds- social comparison and feedback-seeking: Gender and
from-a-magician-and-google-s-design-ethicist-56d62ef popularity moderate associations with depressive
5edf3 symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43,
Heffer, T., Good, M., Daly, O., MacDonell, E., & Wil- 1427–1438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0020-0
loughby, T. (2019). The longitudinal association between Orben, A. (2020). The Sisyphean cycle of technology pan-
social-media use and depressive symptoms among ado- ics. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15, 1143–1157.
lescents and young adults: An empirical reply to Twenge https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620919372
et al (2018). Clinical Psychological Science, 7, 462–470. Orben, A., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). The association
https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702618812727 between adolescent well-being and digital technology
Hersey, J. C., Niederdeppe, J., Evans, W. D., Non- use. Nature Human Behaviour, 3, 173–182. https://doi.
nemaker, J., Blahut, S., Holden, D., . . . Haviland, M. L. org/10.1038/s41562-018-0506-1
(2005). The theory of "truth": How counterindustry Pew Research Center. (2018a). How teens and parents navi-
campaigns affect smoking behavior among teens. Health gate screen time and device distractions. Retrieved from
Psychology, 24(1), 22–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278- https://www.pewinternet.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/
6133.24.1.22 9/2018/08/PI_2018.08.22_teens-screentime_FINAL.pdf
James, C., Weinstein, E., & Mendoza, K. (2019). Teaching Pew Research Center. (2018b). Teens, social media, and tech-
digital citizens in today’s world: Research and insights nology 2018. Retrieved from http://assets.pewresearch.
behind the Common Sense K–12 Digital Citizenship Cur- org/wp-content/uploads/sites/14/2018/05/31102617/
riculum. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media. PI_2018.05.31_TeensTech_FINAL.pdf
Kober, H., Kross, E. F., Mischel, W., Hart, C. L., & Osch- Rideout, V. (2015). The Common Sense census: Media use
ner, K. N. (2010). Regulation of craving by cognitive by tweens and teens. Common Sense Media. Retrieved
strategies in cigarette smokers. Drug and Alcohol Depen- from https://www.commonsensemedia.org/research/
dence, 106, 52–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcde the-common-sense-census-media-use-by-tweens-and-
p.2009.07.017 teens
Lewis, P. (2017, October 6). “Our minds can be hijacked”: Rideout, V., & Robb, M. B. (2018). Social media, social life:
The tech insiders who fear a smartphone dystopia. The Teens reveal their experiences. Common Sense Media.
Guardian. Retrieved from https://www.theguardian.c Retrieved from https://www.commonsensemedia.org/
om/technology/2017/oct/05/smartphone-addiction- research/social-media-social-life-2018
silicon-valley-dystopia Rosen, L. D., Cheever, N. A., & Carrier, L. M. (2008). The
Lindstr€om, B., Bellander, M., Chang, A., Tobler, P. N., & association of parenting style and child age with paren-
Amodio, D. M. (2019). A computational reinforcement tal limit setting and adolescent MySpace behavior. Jour-
learning account of social media engagement. https://doi. nal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29, 459–471.
org/10.31234/osf.io/78mh5 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.07.005
Marker, C., Gnambs, T., & Appel, M. (2018). Active on Sagioglou, C., & Greitemeyer, T. (2014). Facebook’s emo-
Facebook and failing at school? Meta-analytic findings tional consequences: Why Facebook causes a decrease
on the relationship between online social networking in mood and why people still use it. Computers in
activities and academic achievement. Educational Psy- Human Behavior, 35, 359–363. https://doi.org/10.1016/
chology Review, 30, 651–677. https://doi.org/10.1007/ j.chb.2014.03.003
s10648-017-9430-6 Sherman, L. E., Payton, A. A., Hernandez, L. M., Green-
Morgan, D. (2018, February 5). "Truth about tech" cam- field, P. M., & Dapretto, M. (2016). The power of the
paign takes on tech addiction. CBS News. Retrieved from like in adolescence: Effects of peer influence on neural
https://www.cbsnews.com/news/truth-about-tech-ca and behavioral responses to social media. Psychological
mpaign-against-tech-addiction/ Science, 27, 1027–1035. https://doi.org/10.1177/
Morgans, J. (2017, May 17). Your addiction to social media is 0956797616645673
no accident. Vice. Retrieved from https://www.vice. Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on
com/en_us/article/vv5jkb/the-secret-ways-social-media- adolescent risk-taking. Developmental Review, 28, 78–106.
is-built-for-addiction https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002
National Center for Education Statistics. (2020). Common Steinberg, L. (2014). Age of opportunity: Lessons from the
Core of Data (CCD): Public school data 2018–2019, 2019– new science of adolescence. New York, NY: Eamon
2020 school years. Retrieved from https://nces.ed.gov/ Dolan/Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
ccd/schoolsearch/index.asp Steinberg, L. (2015). How to improve the health of
Nesi, J., Choukas-Bradley, S., & Prinstein, M. J. (2018). American adolescents. Perspectives on Psychological
Transformation of adolescent peer relations in the social Science, 10, 711–715. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916
media context: Part 1—A theoretical review and appli- 15598510
cation to dyadic peer relationships. Clinical Child and Telzer, E. H. (2016). Dopaminergic reward sensitivity can
Family Psychology Review, 21, 267–294. https://doi.org/ promote adolescent health: A new perspective on the
10.1007/s10567-018-0261-x mechanism of ventral striatum activation. Developmental
1734 Galla, Choukas-Bradley, Fiore, and Esposito

Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 57–67. https://doi.org/10. Woods, H. C., & Scott, H. (2016). #Sleepyteens: Social
1016/j.dcn.2015.10.010 media use in adolescence is associated with poor sleep
Telzer, E. H., Fuligni, A. J., Lieberman, M. D., & Galv an, quality, anxiety, depression and low self-esteem. Journal
A. (2013). Ventral striatum activation to prosocial of Adolescence, 51, 41–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ad
rewards predicts longitudinal declines in adolescent olescence.2016.05.008
risk taking. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 45– Woolley, K., & Fishbach, A. (2016). For the fun of it: Har-
52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2012.08.004 nessing immediate rewards to increase persistence in
Twenge, J. M., Joiner, T. E., Rogers, M. L., & Martin, G. long-term goals. Journal of Consumer Research, 42, 952–
N. (2018). Increases in depressive symptoms, suicide-re- 966. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucv098
lated outcomes, and suicide rates among U.S. adoles- Yeager, D. S., Dahl, R. E., & Dweck, C. S. (2018). Why
cents after 2010 and links to increased new media interventions to influence adolescent behavior often fail
screen time. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(1), 3–17. but could succeed. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702617723376 13, 101–122. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617
Uhls, Y. T., Michikyan, M., Morris, J., Garcia, D., Small, 722620
G. W., Zgourou, E., & Greenfield, P. M. (2014). Five Yeager, D. S., Henderson, M., Paunesku, D., Walton, G.
days at outdoor education camp without screens M., D’Mello, S., Spitzer, B. J., & Duckworth, A. L.
improves preteen skills with nonverbal emotion cues. (2014). Boring but important: A self-transcendent pur-
Computers in Human Behavior, 39, 387–392. https://doi. pose for learning fosters academic self-regulation. Jour-
org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.036 nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 559–580.
Walton, G. M. (2014). The new science of wise psycholog- https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037637
ical interventions. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 23(1), 73–82. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0963721413512856 Supporting Information
Weinstein, N., & Przybylski, A. K. (2019). The impacts of
motivational framing of technology restrictions on ado- Additional supporting information may be found in
lescent concealment: Evidence from a preregistered the online version of this article at the publisher’s
experimental study. Computers in Human Behavior, 90, website:
170–180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.053 Appendix S1. Supplemental Online Material.

You might also like