You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/232470627

Optimism.

Article · January 2009

CITATIONS READS

8 11,275

2 authors:

Charles Carver Michael F. Scheier


University of Miami Carnegie Mellon University
446 PUBLICATIONS   94,041 CITATIONS    204 PUBLICATIONS   65,584 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Facilitating Adaptation to Breast Cancer View project

Individual and Dyadic Effects of Optimism on Depression View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Charles Carver on 22 July 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/authorsrights
Author's personal copy

Review

Dispositional optimism
Charles S. Carver1 and Michael F. Scheier2
1
University of Miami, Coral Gables, FL, USA
2
Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA

Optimism is a cognitive construct (expectancies regard- Neuroscience is in preparation addressing these links),
ing future outcomes) that also relates to motivation: examination of this cognitive-affective construct, which
optimistic people exert effort, whereas pessimistic peo- has important motivational overtones, seems timely.
ple disengage from effort. Study of optimism began Variations in personality have never been popular as
largely in health contexts, finding positive associations predictors in the cognitive sciences. In part, this is probably
between optimism and markers of better psychological because the desire to make causal statements precludes
and physical health. Physical health effects likely occur reliance on correlational variables. However, the fact that
through differences in both health-promoting behaviors personality is an important determinant of many kinds of
and physiological concomitants of coping. Recently, the behavior [5] suggests that it deserves a closer look. The fact
scientific study of optimism has extended to the realm of that optimism is a facet of personality that is inherently
social relations: new evidence indicates that optimists cognitive in nature (i.e., expectancies about the future)
have better social connections, partly because they work may make it especially appealing.
harder at them. In this review, we examine the myriad This review has some boundaries that should be noted
ways this trait can benefit an individual, and our current at the outset. The term ‘optimism’ is used in diverse ways
understanding of the biological basis of optimism. in various contexts, and not all of them are reviewed
here. This review does not discuss contextualized (goal-
Introduction to dispositional optimism specific) optimism [6], which can behave differently from
The personality dimension optimism versus pessimism generalized optimism under some circumstances [7]. It
has roots both in folk wisdom and in over a century of does not discuss so-called ‘unrealistic optimism’ [8], the
expectancy-incentive motive theories. Contemporary idea that people on the whole expect better outcomes than
research into the correlates of this trait began nearly 30 are realistically justified (but see [9] for a critique of this
years ago with the creation of the Life Orientation Test, a view and [10] for evidence that the phenomenon is distinct
self-report measure of optimism, which was revised in 1994 from trait optimism). The review focuses on research in
[1] to focus its item content more closely on expectancies for which optimism is assessed by face-valid self-reports,
one’s future, the conceptual core of the trait. Optimism is holding aside research in which optimism is inferred from
one of a family of related constructs, including hope [2], patterns of causal explanation [11], in part because the
attributional style [3], and self-efficacy [4]. It differs from measures used in the two literatures are not strongly
those partly by being focused on positive versus negative correlated [12]. It is also noted that use of the labels
expectations for the future without regard to the means by ‘optimist’ and ‘pessimist’ is for convenience in description;
which such outcomes occur (you can, for example, be most research examines the construct as a continuous
optimistic because you have great confidence in your abil- distribution (although see Box 1 for an unresolved issue
ities or because you believe other people like and look out in that regard).
for you). It differs from situational expectancies in terms of
the range of situations to which the expectancy is applic-
Locating optimism in the broad matrix of personality
able and its stability over time.
Research on optimism began during the mid-1980s, before
Although it is possible to disparage this construct as
the 5-factor model of personality structure became the
merely folk psychology, optimism turns out to be a case in
default frame of reference when discussing traits. The 5-
which widespread intuition has a strong basis in reality. The
factor model [13] distills personality to five broad traits:
optimism construct has proven to be useful and relevant to a
neuroticism, extraversion, agreeableness, conscientious-
range of topic areas, and this review addresses work in
ness, and what is generally labeled ‘openness to experi-
several of them. Given current interest among cognitive
ence’. It has been suggested that optimism represents a
scientists in links between motivation and cognition (e.g.,
blend of neuroticism and extraversion [14]. However, later
a special issue of Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral
work tends to support the view that optimism is distinct
Corresponding author: Carver, C.S. (ccarver@miami.edu). from those traits [15,16], while also having some overlap
Keywords: motivation; optimism; pessimism; expectancies; health; coping.
with agreeableness and conscientiousness [17]. In sum, it
1364-6613/$ – see front matter is not easy to capture optimism well within the 5-factor
ß 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.003
viewpoint.
Trait psychologists have always been contentious about
how best to slice up personality, and disagreements have
abounded over whether one way or another is fundamental.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, June 2014, Vol. 18, No. 6 293
Author's personal copy

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences June 2014, Vol. 18, No. 6

Box 1. One dimension or two? Given the origin of the optimism construct in a broad
Some personality dimensions are bipolar, whereas others are
view of motivation, it is natural that research has inves-
unipolar. The logic behind the optimism construct assumes a tigated its role in motivation-relevant outcomes in various
bipolar dimension, with ‘substance’ at each end and a neutral point life situations. Optimism has been linked to a greater
in the middle. However, there has been controversy about whether likelihood of completing college [22]. One study found no
optimism is a bipolar dimension or there are two separable association with first-semester law-school grades [23], but
dimensions, one pertaining to affirmation versus disavowal of
optimism, the other to affirmation versus disavowal of pessimism.
another found that optimism during the first semester of
This issue arises because the scale has both optimistically framed law school predicted higher salaries a decade later [24].
items (e.g., ‘‘I’m always optimistic about my future’’) and pessimis- One interpretation of this pattern is that optimists are no
tically framed items (e.g., ‘‘I hardly ever expect things to go my more capable than those who are less optimistic, but are
way’’) and responses to the two sets of items generally form two more persistent in their academic and professional efforts
factors. Some people think that this means there are two
substantive dimensions, whereas others think that the split is a
over time. This would be consistent with other evidence
product of method variance in responding. Sometimes separating discussed below.
those item subsets has led to better prediction of other outcomes An important characteristic of real-life contexts is that
from one or the other subset, whereas other times there has been no people generally pursue multiple goals simultaneously.
benefit. Several studies aimed at settling the issue have reached
Research has also examined the role of optimism in out-
opposite conclusions: some that a unidimensional view is best (e.g.,
[78–80]), others that there are two dimensions that should be treated comes relevant to the juggling of multiple goals. One study
separately (e.g., [81,82]). This issue, which remains unresolved, found that optimists who were pursuing multiple goals
pertains to virtually all bipolar trait scales, which typically form two were better at balancing effort expenditures [25]. Another
factors if the scales contain subsets of items affirming both poles. set of studies [26] found that optimists increase goal
For the sake of simplicity, in this article, we treat optimism–
engagement for high-priority goals (and, thus, are more
pessimism as one dimension. However, it should be kept in mind
that, in some studies, the extent to which people endorsed versus likely to attain them), and tend to decrease engagement for
rejected a pessimistic outlook was what mattered most; in other low-priority goals (Figure 1). These tendencies occurred in
studies, the extent to which people endorsed versus rejected an goal domains that included friendship formation, exercise,
optimistic outlook was what mattered most. In yet other studies, this and scholastic performance. Optimists even display
issue did not matter at all. Those who use the scale are encouraged
to continue to examine the item subsets as well as the overall scale
greater engagement in treatment programs (nutrition edu-
score, to gain further information on the issue. cation and psychotherapy), but only if the need for the
program was judged by the person to be important [27].
Optimists appear to pick and choose where to invest their
self-regulatory resources, increasing efforts when circum-
One might think of the five factors as forming domains of stances are favorable (compared with pessimists) and
content: the ‘what’ of human motives (extraversion as social tending to decrease them when circumstances are unfavor-
influence, neuroticism as threat avoidance, and agreeable- able [28]. Indeed, if they believe they are being undermined
ness as social bonds). One might think of the optimism in the workplace, they report a greater intent to quit than
dimension as part of the ‘how’ of human motives: how goals do pessimists [29].
are turned into behavior (or fail to be turned into behavior)
from an expectancy-based viewpoint. Optimism effects in relationships
As a practical matter, researchers using the optimism The principles that apply to goal attainment in other
construct have largely ignored the issue of overlap with domains also can be applied to the management of close
other traits, although they sometimes do include other relationships. This is a relatively new area of work on
traits for comparison with optimism in predictive power; optimism. Optimists seem to work harder at relationships
optimism tends to hold up well in such comparisons [18– [23,24], consistent with their greater engagement in other
20]. Those studying optimism generally have instead sim- high-priority tasks. One study of newlyweds [7] found that
ply developed their hypotheses within the framework of optimists engaged in more constructive problem solving
the expectancy-incentive motivational viewpoint, in which than did pessimists, both in a lab discussion of marital
confidence is an important determinant of effort toward issues with their partner, and outside the lab on days when
goals. That is how the construct is discussed here. there was relationship conflict (Figure 2). They also had
less decline in marital well-being over the first year of their
Optimism research reflects broader views of self- marriage.
regulation Optimists report having greater social support than do
The general approach to self-regulation on which this oper- pessimists [30–32], but there is some indication that it is
ationalization of optimism rests [21] assumes that much of the perception of support that matters rather than the
life concerns the approach of goals. Expectancies become actual provision of support [32]. Optimists thrive in a wide
important primarily when impediments appear. If the per- range of social conditions, with the result that optimism is
son is confident about eventual success, effort continues. If related to greater network size, and to ties with others that
the person is doubtful, there is a tendency to disengage cross age, educational, and racial boundaries [33]. There is
effort. Sometimes, disengagement of effort accompanies also evidence that this association works in both directions:
continued psychological engagement with the goal, yielding having strong social networks can enhance optimism [24].
distress. Sometimes, the disengagement is from the goal The social effects of optimism can be far-reaching. In one
itself, resulting in failure to attain it. Optimism versus study, optimism (assessed 10 years previously) predicted
pessimism reflects such expectancies on a broad scale. greater resilience to developing loneliness late in life [34].
294
Author's personal copy

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences June 2014, Vol. 18, No. 6

the future will hold. However, it does not involve being


Key:
Minutes spent on pracce exams

High opmism (+1 SD) oriented preferentially to the future rather than to the
30 present [40].
Low opmism (–1 SD)
However, when optimists do think toward the future,
20 they are able to generate more vivid mental images of
positive events than are pessimists, a stronger sense of
10 ‘pre-experiencing’ those events (despite not having more
vivid imaginations in general) [41]. Consistent with this,
one imaging study found an association between disposi-
0
tional optimism and greater activation of a brain area that
is associated with imagining positive future events [42].
This frame of mind also has reverberations in the pre-
Low priority High priority
sent. Optimists appear to be more able than less optimistic
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences
people to disengage mentally from, or inhibit, physical pain
Figure 1. Optimism leads to greater effort toward high-priority, but not low- [43,44]. They are more responsive to suggestion about pain
priority goals. Some students (but not others) were given information stressing the relief in the form of placebos [45]. By contrast, more
importance of doing well on their final course exam, and indicating that preparing
for the exam should be a high priority over the following week. Displayed are pessimistic people are inclined to catastrophize pain
predicted values of time spent on practice exams as a function of this experiences, thereby making the experiences subjectively
manipulation, among students at 1 standard deviation (SD) above and below the
worse [46,47]. Illness burden, assessed by medical evalua-
sample mean of optimism. Adapted, with permission, from [26].
tions, promotes greater anxiety among persons low in
optimism, but not among those high in optimism [48].
Optimism is associated with a warm and slightly domi- If life turns seriously sour, as reflected in a lack of
nant interpersonal style, which among men results in connection with others and perceiving that one has become
greater relationship satisfaction not only for themselves, a burden on others, suicidal ideation emerges among
but also for their wives [35]. A similar effect has been found persons low in optimism, but not among those higher in
for caregiver burden among wives of men about to undergo optimism [49]. More generally, optimism reduces the
coronary artery bypass surgery [36]. Optimists handle magnitude of the association between rumination and
relationship crises more successfully than do pessimists suicidal ideation [50]. Given the adversity of an extended
[37] and they provide nurturant and involved parenting to lack of employment, optimists manage to maintain higher
their children [19,38], resulting in better adjustment of the life satisfaction, mediated partly by perceptions of family
children. support [51] Optimists report more finding of benefits in
adversity than do pessimists [52], and there is evidence
Optimism as mental orientation to experiences that this difference is mediated by differences in problem-
As noted above, optimism is the expectation that one’s own focused coping [53,54].
outcomes will generally be positive. Indeed, it incorporates
a belief that a stressful present can change to become Optimism and health
better in the future [39]. It is a viewpoint about what As noted above, research using the optimism construct
began at the interface between personality psychology
and health psychology. Given this, much of the work on
More
optimism has taken place in health-related contexts, as
construcve
people confront the transitions imposed by health crises.
1
The early work largely addressed the question of whether
High opmism (1 SD) optimists fare better emotionally and psychologically than
Daily problem- do pessimists when confronting health problems, and con-
solving index 0
firmed that the answer is yes [55]. However, more recently,
(z scores)
research has addressed the question of whether optimism
also predicts physical health [56]. Many of these studies
–1 Low opmism (–1 SD) take the form of epidemiological research, looking at long-
More term prospective associations between optimism and
avoidant
health outcomes in large samples.
Some of this research examined health outcomes per se.
0 1 2 3
As an example, one major study of cardiovascular disease,
Number of daily conflicts
using data from the Women’s Health Initiative (WHI),
TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences examined quality of life, chronic disease, morbidity, and
Figure 2. Optimism leads to a greater balance of constructive compared with mortality among over 95 000 women across an 8-year
disruptive problem solving when marital conflicts arise in day-to-day life. Spouses period [18]. All of the women were free of cardiovascular
completed 12 daily checklists of relationship conflict; they also reported how they
had responded to any conflicts that occurred, on two items reflecting constructive
disease and cancer at study entry. Optimists were less
and/or positive responses and two reflecting negative and/or avoidant behaviors. likely than pessimists to develop coronary heart disease
An index was formed by subtracting negative from positive responses. Displayed (CHD), were less likely to die from CHD-related causes,
are predicted values (standardized) of that index at values 1 standard deviation
(SD) above and below the sample mean of optimism, on days of low to higher
and had lower total mortality due to all causes, across the
conflict between partners. Adapted, with permission, from [7]. 8 years of study. Other studies found that optimism was
295
Author's personal copy

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences June 2014, Vol. 18, No. 6

Box 2. Origins of optimism and pessimism


Key:
Where do individual differences in optimism and pessimism come Low opmism (332)
from? Clearly, there is some sort of genetic involvement in this trait. Middle opmism (298)

opmism subsample
Heritability estimates hover around 30%, depending on the study 60

Percent of each
High opmism (352)
and the algorithm used to estimate heritability [83–85]. A family–
pedigree paradigm has also yielded associations between optimism 40
levels of parents and offspring in two separate cohorts [86]. More
recently, evidence has been found that associations between
optimism and longevity may also have genetic underpinnings [87].
20
Researchers have been trying (with mixed success) to identify
specific genomic elements that might underlie variations in
optimism and pessimism. One study [88] implicated the oxytocin Current No regular Low fruit
receptor gene (rs53576), but this finding failed to replicate in another smoker exercise and veg (≤2)
study [89]. A haplotype for the mineralocorticoid receptor gene, TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences
which helps to regulate stress arousal, has also been related to
optimism in one study, but only among men [72]. The only Figure 3. Optimism is associated with more health-promoting and less health-
published genome-wide study to date [90] failed to identify any impairing behavior. Optimists are less likely to smoke, more likely to exercise, and
correlate of optimism. have more healthy diets compared with pessimists in the form of higher fruit and
vegetable consumption (displayed are percentages of subjects who reported two
On the environmental side, early childhood experience has also
or fewer servings per day). Adapted, with permission, from [61].
been linked to adult optimism. This research [91] measured
optimism in a cohort of adults aged 24–27 years. The researchers
also had access to socioeconomic status (SES) of the participants’ take a proactive approach to health promotion. They are
families during their early childhood (ages 3–6 years) as well as the less likely to smoke, more likely to exercise, have more
participants’ current SES. Childhood SES predicted optimism in
healthy diets, and are more likely to improve their diets
adulthood, even after controlling for adult SES.
An apparently unexamined possibility is whether optimism is than are pessimists [18,61,69–71] (Figure 3). These beha-
cultivated directly through parental transmission (e.g., through vioral pathways are all consistent with the motivated goal
parental modeling). Another possibility is that optimism could arise pursuit that optimists display in other domains, as
via instruction from parents to children on the use of adaptive reviewed above.
coping strategies. Use of such strategies might lead to coping
successes, thereby laying the foundation for future optimism. It
Another reason for better health follows from the better
would also be useful to explore these possibilities in future research. profile of emotional responses to adversity displayed by
optimists (less distress and more positive emotions). This
pattern of overall emotional experiences, which follows
protective against stroke [57] and against increase in in part from the coping reactions that optimists use [55],
carotid artery blockage over a 3-year span [58], and that doubtlessly results in lower physiological strain over time,
optimists were less likely to be rehospitalized by 8 months resulting in better health [72,73].
following coronary bypass surgery [59]. It is noteworthy
that all of these effects remained even when depressive Is optimism ever bad?
symptoms were controlled. Thus, despite the fact that A question that has been raised virtually from the incep-
pessimism is a symptom of depression, pessimism is not tion of research on the optimism construct is whether
merely depression under another name. there are circumstances in which optimism is undesirable.
Other studies have examined health markers that Are optimists ever worse off than pessimists? A common
might be thought of as physiological pathways to health proposal is that a bad outcome disconfirms an optimist’s
problems. Optimism has been linked to lower levels of two expectations so badly that the experience may be worse
markers of inflammation [60], to better antioxidant levels for the optimist than for a pessimist, who expected a bad
[61], to better lipid profiles [62], and to lower cortisol outcome and had the expectation confirmed. Although
responses under stress [63]. There is also evidence that this is a plausible argument, we know of no evidence that
optimistic children show more adaptive sleep profiles [64]. supports it. Rather, as noted above, optimists endorse
The latter study used the Parent-rated Life Orientation the belief that a stressful present can change to a better
Test [65] to assess optimism in 8-year-olds. It is noteworthy future [39]. Put differently, the view that ‘things are
that associations between optimism and markers of good bad now, but they will get better’ promotes better func-
health appear so early in life (see also Box 2). tioning than ‘things are bad now and they are going to stay
Research has also examined differences in immune that way.’
response. There is evidence not only of stronger immune However, there are rare findings of adverse effects of
responses among optimists [66,67] but also of lower optimism. Two recent findings come from contexts in which
immune responses among optimists under conditions of too much confidence and persistence might create pro-
high challenge [68]. Segerstrom [68] argues that the blems. One is gambling. Research on gambling has found
reduction under high challenge reflects greater behavioral that optimists had more positive expectations for gambling
engagement with the challenge, which induces suppres- than did pessimists, and were less likely to reduce their
sion of immune responses to conserve energy for the betting after poor outcomes [74]. Participants in that
behavior. research were not people with actual gambling problems.
Why do optimists have better health outcomes than do However, the outcome suggests the possibility of a vulner-
pessimists? Two possibilities stand out. One is motiva- ability to such problems.
tional and behavioral. Part of remaining healthy is doing In another recent study, lower levels of optimism
the right things and avoiding the wrong things. Optimists among entrepreneurs (in combination with relatively high
296
Author's personal copy

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences June 2014, Vol. 18, No. 6

entrepreneurial experience) predicted better performance that investigations of optimism and health will continue,
in new ventures, defined as employment growth and rev- with an emphasis on exploration of mechanisms of action.
enue [75]. However, this study had an unusual sample: Optimism is a construct that illustrates the importance
entrepreneurs, who (as the authors pointed out) are espe- of recognizing that cognitive, emotional, and motivational
cially optimistic as a group. The fact that less optimism processes are intertwined. At its center, optimism is a
predicted better outcomes in a subset of that sample led cognitive construct, an expectancy. However, by virtue of
those authors to suggest that optimism has curvilinear its connotations of valence (expecting either good or bad),
effects in some contexts. Despite the infrequency of such this dimension also has emotional overtones. Finally, the
reversals (and the abundance of positive associations evidence reviewed here makes clear that this expectancy
between optimism and motivational outcomes), this possi- also has motivational implications. It seems impossible to
bility warrants study. untangle these threads from each other, and it seems wise
to recognize their inextricability.
Becoming more optimistic
Given that optimism is beneficial in many life domains, a Acknowledgment
reasonable question is how to become more optimistic. Preparation of this review was supported by a grant from the National
Recent research has found that 2 weeks of daily 5-min Center for Complementary and Alternative Medicine (AT007262).
sessions of imagining one’s best possible self can increase
optimism, at least temporarily [76]. Others have trained References
people to make systematically more optimistic explana- 1 Scheier, M.F. et al. (1994) Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism
tions for events [3]. Indeed, it might be argued that the (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self-esteem): a reevaluation of the
Life Orientation Test. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 67, 1063–1078
broad range of cognitive-behavioral therapies used in clin-
2 Snyder, C.R. (1994) The Psychology of Hope: You Can Get There From
ical practice for conditions such as depression [77] typically Here, Free Press
involve efforts to induce people to approach their lives in 3 Seligman, M.E.P. (1991) Learned Optimism, Knopf
more optimistic ways. 4 Bandura, A. (1997) Self-efficacy: The Exercise of Control, Freeman
Still, optimism is a personality trait. Without manip- 5 Roberts, B.W. et al. (2007) The power of personality: the comparative
validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive
ulation, it generally remains relatively stable over ability for predicting important life outcomes. Perspect. Psychol. Sci.
extended periods, at least in the absence of major life 2, 313–345
transitions [55]. This raises questions about whether pro- 6 Armor, D.A. and Taylor, S.E. (1998) Situated optimism: specific
cedures such as mental simulations can be expected to outcome expectancies and self-regulation. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol.
have pervasive or long-lasting effects. (Indeed, questions 29, 209–379
7 Neff, L.A. and Geers, A.L. (2013) Optimistic expectations in early
about longevity and pervasiveness can also be raised about marriage: a resource or vulnerability for adaptive relationship
formal therapies.) Changing a person’s overall outlook on functioning? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 105, 38–60
life can be done, but it is not a simple matter. 8 Weinstein, N.D. (1989) Optimistic biases about personal risks. Science
246, 1232–1233
9 Harris, A.L. and Hahn, U. (2011) Unrealistic optimism about future life
Implications and concluding remarks
events: a cautionary note. Psychol. Rev. 118, 135–154
The recent research reviewed here extends previous work 10 Klein, W.P. and Zajac, L.E. (2009) Imagining a rosy future: the
in several directions: to consider more closely the role of psychology of optimism. In Handbook of Imagination and Mental
optimism in prioritization of goals; to consider how opti- Simulation (Markman, K.D. et al., eds), pp. 313–329, Psychology
mists handle the demands of close relationships; and to Press
11 Peterson, C. and Villanova, P. (1988) An expanded attributional style
examine how optimism relates to diverse markers of phy-
questionnaire. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 97, 87–89
sical well being. Most of the evidence, both old and new, 12 Hirsch, J.K. and Conner, K.R. (2006) Dispositional and explanatory
suggests that optimism is a desirable property to have. style optimism as potential moderators of the relationship between
Clearly, there are gaps in knowledge (Box 3). Research will hopelessness and suicidal ideation. Suicide Life Threat. Behav. 36,
continue to explore the possibility that optimism is proble- 661–669
13 McCrae, R.R. and Costa, P.T., Jr (1997) Personality trait structure as a
matic in certain domains, for example, and some work human universal. Am. Psychol. 52, 509–516
should test for curvilinear effects to determine more clearly 14 Marshall, G.N. et al. (1992) Distinguishing optimism from pessimism:
what happens at the highest levels of optimism. It is likely relations to fundamental dimensions of mood and personality. J. Pers.
Soc. Psychol. 62, 1067–1074
15 Alarcon, G.M. et al. (2013) Great expectations: a meta-analytic
Box 3. Outstanding questions examination of optimism and hope. Pers. Individ. Differ. 54, 821–827
 Is the presence of optimism different from the absence of 16 Kam, C. and Meyer, J.P. (2012) Do optimism and pessimism have
pessimism? different relationships with personality dimensions?. A re-
 Do effects of optimism overlap or differ from effects of other examination. Pers. Individ. Differ. 52, 123–127
personal strengths? 17 Sharpe, J. et al. (2011) Optimism and the big five factors of personality:
 What are the origins of optimism? beyond neuroticism and extraversion. Pers. Individ. Differ. 51,
 What neural processes subserve optimism? 946–951
 What biobehavioral mechanisms underlie optimism effects on 18 Tindle, H.A. et al. (2009) Optimism, cynical hostility, and incident
health? coronary heart disease and mortality in the women’s health initiative.
 What techniques might foster change from pessimism to Circulation 120, 656–662
optimism? 19 Heinonen, K. et al. (2006) Parents’ optimism is related to their ratings
 How hard is it to change from a relative pessimist to a relative of their children’s behaviour. Eur. J. Pers. 20, 421–445
optimist? 20 Räikkönen, K. and Matthews, K.A. (2008) Do dispositional pessimism
 Under what circumstances might it be bad to be too optimistic? and optimism predict ambulatory blood pressure during schooldays
and nights in adolescents? J. Pers. 76, 605–630

297
Author's personal copy

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences June 2014, Vol. 18, No. 6

21 Carver, C.S. and Scheier, M.F. (1998) On the Self-Regulation of 48 Hirsch, J.K. et al. (2012) Illness burden and symptoms of anxiety
Behavior, Cambridge University Press in older adults: optimism and pessimism as moderators. Int.
22 Solberg Nes, L. et al. (2009) Optimism and college retention: mediation Psychogeriatr. 24, 1614–1621
by motivation, performance, and adjustment. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 39, 49 Rasmussen, K.A. and Wingate, L.R. (2011) The role of optimism in
1887–1912 the interpersonal-psychological theory of suicidal behavior. Suicide
23 Rand, K.L. et al. (2011) Hope, but not optimism, predicts academic Life Threat. Behav. 41, 137–148
performance of law students beyond previous academic achievement. 50 Tucker, R.P. et al. (2013) Rumination and suicidal ideation: the
J. Res. Pers. 45, 683–686 moderating roles of hope and optimism. Pers. Individ. Differ. 55,
24 Segerstrom, S.C. (2007) Optimism and resources: effects on each other 606–611
and on health over 10 years. J. Res. Pers. 41, 772–786 51 Duffy, R.D. et al. (2013) Examining a model of life satisfaction among
25 Segerstrom, S.C. and Solberg Nes, L. (2006) When goals conflict but unemployed adults. J. Couns. Psychol. 60, 53–63
people prosper: the case of dispositional optimism. J. Res. Pers. 40, 52 Lechner, S.C. et al. (2006) Curvilinear associations between benefit
675–693 finding and psychosocial adjustment to breast cancer. J. Consult. Clin.
26 Geers, A.L. et al. (2009) Dispositional optimism and engagement: the Psychol. 74, 828–840
moderating influence of goal prioritization. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 96, 53 Büyükaşik-Çolak, C. et al. (2012) Mediating role of coping in the
913–932 dispositional optimism–posttraumatic growth relation in breast
27 Geers, A.L. et al. (2010) Dispositional optimism, goals, and engagement cancer patients. J. Psychol. 146, 471–483
in health treatment programs. J. Behav. Med. 33, 123–134 54 Llewellyn, C.D. et al. (2013) Assessing the psychological predictors of
28 Pavlova, M.K. and Silbereisen, R.K. (2013) Dispositional optimism benefit finding in patients with head and neck cancer. Psychooncology
fosters opportunity-congruent coping with occupational uncertainty. 22, 97–105
J. Pers. 81, 76–86 55 Carver, C.S. et al. (2010) Optimism. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 879–889
29 Britton, A.R. et al. (2012) Is the glass really half-full? The reverse- 56 Rasmussen, H.N. et al. (2009) Optimism and physical health: a meta-
buffering effect of optimism on undermining behavior. Pers. Individ. analytic review. Ann. Behav. Med. 37, 239–256
Differ. 52, 712–717 57 Kim, E.S. et al. (2011) Dispositional optimism protects older
30 Srivastava, S. et al. (2006) Optimism in close relationships: how seeing adults from stroke: the health and retirement study. Stroke 42,
things in a positive light makes them so. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 143– 2855–2859
153 58 Matthews, K.A. et al. (2004) Optimistic attitudes protect against
31 Assad, K.K. et al. (2007) Optimism: an enduring resource for romantic progression of carotid atherosclerosis in healthy middle-aged
relationships. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 93, 285–297 women. Psychosom. Med. 66, 640–644
32 Vollmann, M. et al. (2011) Social support as mediator of the stress 59 Tindle, H. et al. (2012) Optimism, response to treatment of depression,
buffering effect of optimism: the importance of differentiating the and rehospitalization after coronary artery bypass graft surgery.
recipients’ and providers’ perspective. Eur. J. Pers. 25, 146–154 Psychosom. Med. 74, 200–207
33 Andersson, M.A. (2012) Dispositional optimism and the emergence of 60 Roy, B. et al. (2010) Association of optimism and pessimism with
social network diversity. Sociol. Q. 53, 92–115 inflammation and hemostasis in the multi-ethnic study of
34 Rius-Ottenheim, N. et al. (2012) Dispositional optimism and loneliness atherosclerosis (MESA). Psychosom. Med. 72, 134–140
in older men. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 27, 151–159 61 Boehm, J.K. et al. (2013) Association between optimism and serum
35 Smith, T.W. et al. (2013) Optimism and pessimism in social context: an antioxidants in the Midlife in the United States study. Psychosom.
interpersonal perspective on resilience and risk. J. Res. Pers. 47, 553– Med. 75, 2–10
562 62 Boehm, J.K. et al. (2013) Relation between optimism and lipids in
36 Ruiz, J.M. et al. (2006) Does who you marry matter for your health? midlife. Am. J. Cardiol. 111, 1425–1431
Influence of patients’ and spouses’ personality on their partners’ 63 Jobin, J. et al. (2013) Associations between dispositional optimism
psychological well-being following coronary artery bypass surgery. J. and diurnal cortisol in a community sample: When stress is
Pers. Soc. Psychol. 91, 255–267 perceived as higher than normal. Health Psychol. http://dx.doi.org/
37 Andersson, M.A. (2012) Identity crises in love and at work: 10.1037/a0032736
dispositional optimism as a durable personal resource. Soc. Psychol. 64 Lemola, S. et al. (2011) Sleep quantity, quality and optimism in
Q. 75, 290–309 children. J. Sleep Res. 20, 12–20
38 Taylor, Z.E. et al. (2012) Dispositional optimism: a psychological 65 Lemola, S. et al. (2010) A new measure for dispositional optimism and
resource for Mexican-origin mothers experiencing economic stress. pessimism in young children. Eur. J. Pers. 24, 71–84
J. Fam. Psychol. 26, 133–139 66 Kohut, M.L. et al. (2002) Exercise and psychosocial factors modulate
39 Chang, E.C. et al. (2013) An examination of optimism/pessimism and immunity to influenza vaccine in elderly individuals. J. Gerontol. A:
suicide risk in primary care patients: does belief in a changeable future Biol. Med. Sci. 57A, M557–M562
make a difference? Cogn. Ther. Res. 37, 796–804 67 Szondy, M. (2004) Optimism and immune functions. Mentalhigiene es
40 Busseri, M.A. et al. (2013) Are optimists oriented uniquely toward the Pszichoszomatika 5, 301–320
future? Investigating dispositional optimism from a temporally- 68 Segerstrom, S.C. (2006) How does optimism suppress immunity?
expanded perspective. J. Res. Pers. 47, 533–538 Evaluation of three affective pathways. Health Psychol. 25, 653–657
41 Blackwell, S.E. et al. (2013) Optimism and mental imagery: a possible 69 Giltay, E.J. et al. (2007) Lifestyle and dietary correlates of dispositional
cognitive marker to promote well-being? Psychiatry Res. 206, 56–61 optimism in men: the Zutphen elderly study. J. Psychosom. Res. 63,
42 Sharot, T. et al. (2007) Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias. 483–4904
Nature 450, 102–105 70 Steptoe, A. et al. (2006) Dispositional optimism and health behaviour in
43 Geers, A.L. et al. (2008) Dispositional optimism and thoughts of well- community-dwelling older people: associations with healthy ageing.
being determine sensitivity to an experimental pain task. Ann. Behav. Br. J. Health Psychol. 11, 71–84
Med. 36, 304–313 71 Hingle, M.D. et al. (2014) Optimism and diet quality in the Women’s
44 Goodin, B.R. et al. (2013) Testing the relation between dispositional Health Initiative. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
optimism and conditioned pain modulation: Does ethnicity matter? J. j.jand.2013.12.018
Behav. Med. 36, 165–174 72 Klok, M.D. et al. (2011) A common and functional mineralocorticoid
45 Geers, A.L. et al. (2010) Dispositional optimism predicts placebo receptor haplotype enhances optimism and protects against depression
analgesia. J. Pain 11, 1165–1171 in females. Transl. Psychiatry 1, e62
46 Goodin, B.R. et al. (2013) The association of greater dispositional 73 Wrosch, C. et al. (2013) Goal adjustment capacities, subjective well-
optimism with less endogenous pain facilitation is indirectly being, and physical health. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 7, 847–
transmitted through lower levels of pain catastrophizing. J. Pain 14, 860
126–135 74 Gibson, B. and Sanbonmatsu, D.M. (2004) Optimism, pessimism,
47 Hanssen, M.M. et al. (2013) Optimism lowers pain: evidence of the and gambling: the downside of optimism. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull.
causal status and underlying mechanisms. Pain 154, 53–58 30, 149–160

298
Author's personal copy

Review Trends in Cognitive Sciences June 2014, Vol. 18, No. 6

75 Hmieleski, K.M. and Baron, R.A. (2009) Entrepreneurs’ optimism and 84 Mosing, M. et al. (2009) Genetic and environmental influences on
new venture performance: a social cognitive perspective. Acad. optimism and its relationship to mental and self-rated health: a
Manage. J. 52, 473–488 study of aging twins. Behav. Genet. 39, 597–604
76 Meevissen, Y.M.C. et al. (2011) Become more optimistic by imagining a 85 Mosing, M.A. et al. (2010) Sex differences in the genetic architecture of
best possible self: effects of a two week intervention. J. Behav. Ther. optimism and health and their interrelation: a study of Australian
Exp. Psychiatry 42, 371–378 and Swedish twins. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 13, 322–329
77 Beck, A.T. (1976) Cognitive Therapy and the Emotional Disorders, 86 Rius-Ottenheim, N. et al. (2012) Parental longevity correlates with
International Universities Press offspring’s optimism in two cohorts of community-dwelling older
78 Chiesi, F. et al. (2013) The accuracy of the life orientation test-revised subjects. Age 34, 461–468
(LOT-R) in measuring dispositional optimism: evidence from item 87 Mosing, M.A. et al. (2012) Genetic influences on life span and its
response theory analyses. J. Pers. Assess. 95, 523–529 relationship to personality: a 16-year follow-up study of a sample of
79 Rauch, W.A. et al. (2007) Method effects due to social desirability as a aging twins. Psychosom. Med. 74, 16–22
parsimonious explanation of the deviation from unidimensionality in 88 Saphire-Bernstein, S. et al. (2011) Oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is
LOT-R scores. Pers. Individ. Differ. 42, 1597–1607 related to psychological resources. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108,
80 Segerstrom, S.C. et al. (2011) Optimism and pessimism dimensions in 15118–15122
the Life Orientation Test-Revised: method and meaning. J. Res. Pers. 89 Cornelis, M.C. et al. (2012) Oxytocin receptor (OXTR) is not associated
45, 126–129 with optimism in the Nurses’ Health Study. Mol. Psychiatry 17,
81 Glaesmer, H. et al. (2012) Psychometric properties and population- 1157–1159
based norms of the life orientation test revised (LOT-R). Br. J. Health 90 Eriksson, N. et al. (2010) Web-based, participant-driven studies
Psychol. 17, 432–445 yield novel genetic associations for common traits. PLoS Genet. 6,
82 Rauch, W.A. et al. (2008) An IRT analysis of the personal optimism e1000993
scale. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 24, 49–56 91 Heinonen, K. et al. (2006) Socioeconomic status in childhood and
83 Plomin, R. et al. (1992) Optimism, pessimism, and mental health: a adulthood: associations with dispositional optimism and pessimism
twin/adoption analysis. Pers. Individ. Differ. 13, 921–930 over a 21-year follow-up. J. Pers. 74, 1111–1126

299

View publication stats

You might also like