You are on page 1of 3

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.5334/ai.

0408
ARCHAEOLOGY INTERNATIONAL

It has often been suggested that style


The evolutionary analysis of cultural behaviour and function in cultural artefacts can be
seen in a similar light. Some aspects of
Stephen Shennan & Mark Collard artefacts are functional and therefore will
A team of archaeologists and anthropologists from UCL and the be subject to selection in terms of how well
University of Southampton has been awarded a large grant by they perform that function; others are
the UK Arts and Humanities Research Board to set up a Centre for
much more free to vary, and it is often as­
sumed that features such as decoration on
the Evolutionary Analysis of Cultural Behaviour. 1 It is one ofonly pottery fit this description and are adap­
ten research centres to be funded in the UK under this new AHRB tively neutral. In 1995 the American ar­
scheme and is the first in the world dedicated to its subject. Here chaeologist Fraser Neiman developed this
the Centre's director and a colleague outline its aims and idea by using the mathematics of the neu­
tral theory to develop a series of expecta­
describe two of the first research projects to be undertaken.
tions about the amount of variation to be
expected in the decoration of a pottery

T
he idea that there are similari­ statistical methods derived from popula­ assemblage if the decoration was indeed
ties between the processes of tion genetics and biological systematics. neutral.6 He analyzed rim decoration on
biological and cultural evolu­ The first milestone here was the publica­ pottery assemblages from seven succes­
tion has a long history in tion in 1981 of the book Cultural trans­ sive phases of the Woodland period of the
archaeology and anthropology, mission and evolution: a quan titative American Mid-West and found that it
but in the past 25 years it has attracted approach by the famous geneticist Luca matched the neutral expectations. He
much wider interest. This new lease of Cavalli-Sforza and his mathematical col­ concluded that the patterns of variation
life for an old idea stems from the recent league Marcus Feldman.4 The methods depended on changing levels of inter­
explosion of public interest in Darwinian and ideas that have been emerging since group contact, which started low, in­
ideas and in genetics, together with then are giving archaeologists, and others creased and then declined again. The time
advances in the study of culture from an interested in the processes of cultural of highest interaction was also a time
evolutionary perspective. stability and change, new tools with when exotic trade goods were widespread.
which to approach longstanding prob­ Because the successful transmission of
The evolutionary analysis of lems. Furthermore, the fact that the tools pottery-making traditions depends on
cultural behaviour are mathematical means that models long-lasting relationships between teacher
The fundamental idea is that cultural based on them are likely to have directly and learner, he suggested that the chang­
traditions, such as those that are recog­ testable implications ing levels of intergroup contact related to
nized in the archaeological record, can be Unfortunately, most archaeologists and changes in the level of long-term residen­
regarded as similar to genetic lineages. Just anthropologists are unaware of these new tial movement of potters between groups.
as biological reproduction leads to a trans­ advances. Also, much of the work carried Richard Wilkinson and Stephen Shen­
fer of information over the generations by out has remained theoretical, and the the­ nan have recently applied and extended
genetic transmission, so the process of ac­ orists involved do not necessarily have a Neiman's methods in a study of patterns of
quiring information handed down from good grasp of archaeological and anthro­ pottery decoration from two settlements
previous generations can be seen as a proc­ pological issues and data. The new Centre of the early Neolithic Linear Pottery cul­
ess of cultural transmission, but one that aims to change this, with a series of ture in western Germany.7 The two settle­
takes place in people's minds rather than projects relating to four specific themes: ments (Fig. 1, LW8 and LB7) are located
in the D A of their reproductive cells. In • ecological dimensions of cultural evo­ within a small settlement cluster along the
other words, the way we learn from others lution shallow Merzbach valley that was totally
acts as an inheritance mechanism by pro­ • processes of cultural innovation and excavated in advance of strip mining. A
ducing behavioural similarities between transmission quantitative analysis of the frequencies of
learners and those they learn from. These • processes of cultural diversification the decorative motifs on the bodies of the
similarities cannot be explained by ge­ • spatial dimensions of cultural evolution. vessels (Fig. 2) was undertaken by the
netic transmission, nor by the fact that the We cannot describe in this short article the same method that Neiman had used. In
social and natural environments to which full range of projects that the Centre will this case the expectations of the neutral
one generation adapts tend to resemble undertake within these themes, but we can model and the amount of variation in the
those of the previous generation. briefly describe two of the initial projects. pottery assemblages of the successive site
The best known version of this idea is Both attempt to follow through the impli­ phases did not coincide. Or rather they did
Richard Dawkins' concept of the meme2 cations of regarding culture as the second for the earlier phases but not for the later
(pronounced " meem"), which he proposed system (after genetics) of human inherit­ ones , where there appeared to have been a
as the cultural equivalent of the gene, ance and to demonstrate the insights that deliberate selection of novel decoration
passed from one person to another by can be gained by adopting this approach. types, rather than simple drift. Why the
methods outside genetic control, but sub­ potters were doing this is unclear, but
ject to a cultural version of the processes Neutral evolution and pottery other evidence suggests that it may be
of selection that affect how genes spread One of the surprises in genetics in the related to a concern to establish their own
through populations. 1 960s was the discovery that much DNA local identity and distinguish themselves
Nevertheless, although the concept of did not actually code for proteins. In fact, it from neighbouring groups.
the meme has stimulated thinking about apparently did not do anything, and hence It appears that the mechanisms relevant
the way cultural information is passed on, could not be under any form of selection. It to change in the Neolithic Linear Pottery
in many ways it does not get us much fur­ was found that the only factors that culture are not the same as those that
ther than traditional vague ideas about affected the chances of a new mutation operated in the Mid-Western Woodland
similarities between biology and culture.3 surviving or going extinct were the muta­ period. Although the methods used in
It has proved scientifically more success­ tion rate and processes ofrandom drift that both case studies are based on the assump­
ful to take the analogy between genetic and were dependent on the size of the popula­ tion of neutral evolution, they do not
cultural transmission as a starting point tion. This was the basis of the neutral the­ inevitably lead to the conclusion that
for understanding cultural change through ory of evolution, which is associated with change in decoration is solely a result of
the development of mathematical and the well known geneticist Motoo Kimura. 5 innovation and drift. Rather, they provide

21
ARCHAEOLOGY INTERNATIONAL

an ancestral assemblage (branching) ,


N

1
0 km whereas i n ethnogenesis a new cultural
r
-, ... - � assemblage arises through the blending of
N
'

elements of two or more contemporaneous

1
entities. Currently, most authors consider
blending to be far more important than
branching in the generation of cultural
entities. However, most assessments ofthe
relative importance of phylogenesis and
ethnogenesis in human culture have so far
been theoretical and qualitative, or both;
only a few attempts have been made to
address the problem quantitatively. More­
over, most studies have focused on ethno­
graphic data rather than archaeological
evidence.
We have suggested in a recent paper
. Aachen
that the issue of distinguishing phylogen­
esis from ethnogenesis in any particular
LW9 case is very similar to problems that have
already been successfully addressed in the
field of evolutionary classification in biol­
ogy, in estimating the descent relation­

E
ships among species.9 Therefore, methods
g that have been developed in that field of
biology could potentially be applied to the
analysis of archaeological data.
What the two problems have in com­
LW16 mon is that they both require similarities
existing within a group of species or
archaeological assemblages to be divided
into those that are the result of shared
ancestry and those that are the result of
other mechanisms, for example diffusion.
In biology this task is accomplished by
generating a tree structure that links the
, ' species concerned in such a way that the
,· number of hypotheses of change required
: LWZ to account for the observed distribution of
similarities is minimized. Using this tree
structure it is then possible to classify the
: similarities as either resulting from shared
ancestry or not. The first group of similar­
, '
ities suggests relationships that are com­
patible with the tree structure, whereas the
second implies relationships that conflict
\ house with the tree structure.
' earthwork
Influenced by a phylogenetic analysis
of stone tools by Robert Foley,10 we have
suggested that this approach could be used
D
maximum extent of
archaeological finds
to assess the relative contributions of
branching and blending processes to
m 200
patterns in the archaeological record. If a
statistically robust tree structure can be
derived from a group of archaeological
Figure 1 The distribution ofNeolithic Linear Potte1y (Bandkeramic) settlements in the assemblages, then phylogenesis can rea­
middle Merzbach valley. sonably be inferred to have played a more
important role than ethnogenesis in the
a baseline of great predictive value that Cultural origins: branching or generation of the assemblages. Con­
enables not just neutrality but also alterna­ blending? versely, if such a tree structure cannot be
tives to be identified. If these methods had The second project is concerned with identified , then ethnogenesis can be
not been used, any view about the neutral­ another issue of great general interest: the inferred to be the most important process.
ity or active role of pottery-decoration way in which new cultures or subcultures We have analyzed a dataset consisting
style in any given case would be purely a originate and the way in which these of the same patterns of pottery decoration
matter of personal preference. Such ques­ processes relate to genetic and linguistic analyzed in the neutral-evolution study/
tions are of interest to many archaeolo­ patterns in human history. Recent discus­ but this time from all seven of the early
gists, including those who do not favour sions of cultural change have focused on Neolithic settlements in the area (Fig. 1 ) ,
the evolutionary approach, but the latter two processes that J. H. Moore has termed i n order t o see if there i s evidence for
do not have the methods with which to "phylogenesis" and " ethnogenesis".8 In branching or blending in the relationships
answer them. phylogenesis a new cultural entity is the between the decorative patterns at the dif­
result of descent with modification from ferent settlements in the different phases.

22
ARCHAEOLOGY I NTERNATIONAL

Figure 2 Examples of early (left) a n d late (righ t) Neolithic Linear Pottery (Ban dkeramic) vessels from the Merzbach valley.

The first set of analyses focused on the should not simply assume that the cultural 4. L. Cavalli-Sforza & M . Feldman, Cultural
assemblages from the four settlements that entities and assemblages they study are transmission and evolution : a qu antita­
have evidence for occupation throughout the result of ethnogenesis. Instead, the tive approach (Princeton, New Jersey:
relative contribution of ethnogenesis and Princeton University Press, 1 9 8 1 ) .
the whole of the ten-phase period. It was
5 . M . Kimura, The neutral allele theory of
conjectured that, if the branching or phyl­ phylogenesis to the generation of the
m olecular evolution (Cambridge: Cam­
ogenesis hypothesis is correct, analyses of assemblages needs to be determined bridge University Press, 1983).
the assemblages should divide them into empirically on a case-by-case basis.U A 6. F . D. Neiman, " S tylistic variation in evo­
the same groups in consecutive phases. On second implication of the results is that lutionary perspective: inferences from
the other hand, if the ethnogenesis or the processes of colonization and group decorative diversity and interassemblage
blending hypothesis is accurate, the analy­ fission (when people from existing settle­ distance in Illinois Woodland ceramic
ses should separate the settlements into ments establish new ones), which are assemblages" , American Antiquity 60, 7-
different groups in consecutive phases. usually assumed to have driven the Linear 3 6 , 1995.
The four settlements were divided into the Pottery early Neolithic expansion into 7. S. Shennan & R. Wilkinson, "Ceramic
style change and neutral evolution: a case
same groups in six of the instances in Europe, appear to have the cultural con­
study from Neolithic Europe", submitted
which consecutive phases could be com­ sequences we might expect, and that they to American An tiq uity
pared. In the remaining three instances, were perhaps associated with correspond­ 8. J. H. Moore, "Putting anthropology back
the settlements were divided into different ing linguistic and genetic dispersals. It is together again: the ethnogenetic critique
groups in consecutive phases. These particularly striking, given the extensive of cladistic theory, American Anthropol­
results are not wholly compatible with intersite interaction and close relation­ ogist 96 , 3 70-96, 1 994; and "Ethnogenetic
either hypothesis. Rather, they indicate ships that can be assumed to have existed, theory" , National Geographic Research
that phylogenesis and ethnogenesis were that the cultural consequences of group and Exploration 10, 1 0-2 3, 1 994.
both involved in the generation of the pot­ fission so clearly involve cultural differen­ 9 . M. Collard & S. Shennan, "Processes of
culture change in prehistory: a case study
tery assemblages. tiation and branching, even at the local
from the European Neolithic", in Arch ae­
The second set of analyses focused on scale of the sites analyzed in this study. ogenetics: DNA and the population pre­
the three instances in the ten-phase period history of Europe, C. Renfrew & K. Boy le
in which a new pottery assemblage Notes (eds) , 89-97 (Monograph, McDonald
appears. It was reasoned that, if the phyl­ 1 . The team consists of Stephen Shennan Institute for Archaeological Research,
ogenesis hypothesis is correct, an analysis and Mark Lake (Institute of Archaeology) , Cambridge, 2000).
of the decoration frequencies for one of the Mark Collard and Ruth Mace (UCL Anthro­ 1 0 . R. Foley, "Hominid species and stone tool
newly founded assemblages derive from a pology), and James Steele and Tim assemblages: how are they related?",
Sluckin (Archaeology and Mathematics, Antiquity 6 1 , 3 80-9 2 , 1 9 8 7 .
single parent in the preceding phase. Con­
Southampton). Stephen Shennan is the 1 1 . S e e also P. Bellwood, "Phylogeny versus
versely, if the ethnogenesis hypothesis is Centre's Director and James Steele its reticulation in prehistory" , Antiquity 70,
correct, such an analysis should group the Associate Director. The grant, which is for 88 1-90, 1996.
newly established assemblage with never a period of five years from January 2001,
less than two of the potential ancestral will fund post-doctoral research assist­
assemblages. The second set of analyses ants at UCL and Southampton, computer
support the phylogenesis rather than the equipment for mathematical and spatial
ethnogenesis hypothesis. Two of the anal­ modelling, a programme of conferences
yses offer strong support for the idea that and workshops, and support for visiting
scholars. In addition to the grant from the
newly founded assemblages derive from a
AHRB , members ofthe Centre will seek fur­
single ancestral assemblage through ther funding for additional projects, and
descent with modification. The results of for the continuation of the Centre beyond
the third analysis were more ambiguous, the initial five years.
but their simplest interpretation also sup­ 2. See pp. 206-7 in R. Dawkins , The selfish
ports the notion that the newly founded gene (Oxford: Oxford University Press,
assemblages had a single parent from 1 976).
those in the preceding phase. 3 . M. Lake, "Digging for memes: the role of
The two sets of analyses of the data from material objects in cultural evolution", in
Cognition and material culture: the
the settlements in the Merzbach valley are
archaeology of symbolic storage, C. Ren­
not compatible with the assertion that frew & C. Scarre (eds), 7 7-88 (Monograph ,
cultural assemblages arise predominantly McDonald Institute for Archaeological
through blending. One important implica­ Research, Cambridge, 1998).
tion of these results is that archaeologists

23

View publication stats

You might also like