You are on page 1of 19

Journal of Business and Psychology (2022) 37:263–281

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-021-09743-6

ORIGINAL PAPER

Revisiting Reciprocity: How Accountability, Proactivity,


and Interpersonal Skills Shape Obligations to Reciprocate Citizenship
Behavior
Erich C. Dierdorff 1 & Robert S. Rubin 1

Accepted: 9 March 2021 / Published online: 25 March 2021


# The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2021

Abstract
Across two studies, we directly test the widely held tenet in the scholarship of organizational citizenship behavior (OCB) that
individuals choose to engage in OCB as a result of felt obligations to reciprocate. We further examine how obligations to
reciprocate operate against a backdrop of relevant contextual factors (accountability) and individual differences (proactive
personality and interpersonal skill). Using an experimental method, we find evidence supporting the proposition that reciprocity
obligations increase OCB engagement and that these effects are amplified by contexts high in accountability and when individ-
uals possess high levels of proactive personality. In a subsequent field study, we find further corroborating evidence for the
effects of reciprocity obligations on OCB and reaffirm the moderating influences of proactive personality and accountability.
Overall, the convergent evidence supports the causal relationship between reciprocity obligations and OCB, as well as explicates
the conditions under which this foundational effect operates across individuals and varying contexts.

Keywords Organizational citizenship behavior . Reciprocity . Proactive personality . Interpersonal skills

Organizational citizenship behavior (OCB) is inextricably within the broader context of interpersonal interactions in
linked to the context of work, where these prosocial, discre- the workplace (Organ et al., 2006).
tionary actions are described as contributing to the “mainte- Fundamental to the concept of social exchange is the more
nance and enhancement of the social and psychological envi- specific notion of reciprocity, which refers to a pattern of
ronment that supports task performance” (p. 91, Organ, 1997). mutually contingent exchanges. These reciprocal exchanges
Because this behavior contributes to, and is shaped by, work form the basis of a norm of reciprocity that “evokes obliga-
context, social exchange theory (Blau, 1964) has been widely tions toward others on the basis of their past behavior” (p. 170;
applied as a central conceptual framework in the OCB litera- Gouldner, 1960). In the workplace, such obligations to recip-
ture (Bolino & Grant, 2016; Van Dyne et al., 1994; Zellars & rocate are thought to be a primary force that shapes individ-
Tepper, 2003). Social exchange theory is valuable for under- uals’ role expectations and, ultimately, the actions they choose
standing OCB because it not only articulates when individuals to perform as part of their work roles (Katz & Kahn, 1978). As
are likely to engage in socially oriented behavior in the work- such, reciprocity obligations have been widely invoked as a
place, but also proposes why such behavior is supported key explanatory mechanism for understanding why individ-
uals engage in OCB (Lester et al., 2008; Organ, 1990). In fact,
the theoretical supposition that the norm of reciprocity is a
causal mechanism underlying OCB is nearly axiomatic in
* Erich C. Dierdorff extant scholarship.
edierdor@depaul.edu Despite the widespread supposition that reciprocity obliga-
tions are an important antecedent to OCB, our current under-
Robert S. Rubin standing of the reciprocity–OCB relationship remains limited
rrubin@depaul.edu
in two significant ways. First, with a few notable exceptions
1 (Deckop et al., 2003; Korsgaard et al., 2010), the central con-
Department of Management and Entrepreneurship, Driehaus College
of Business, DePaul University, 1 E. Jackson Blvd., DePaul Center ceptual focus of the majority of past research has been toward
7034, Chicago, IL 60604, USA the overall quality of social relationships, rather than an
264 J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281

explicit focus on reciprocity obligations. For example, several moderating influences on OCB (e.g., Dierdorff et al., 2012;
studies have supported the positive relationship between OCB Farh et al., 1990; Rubin et al., 2013). Taken collectively, the
and perceptions of supportive or high-quality relationships preceding theory and research suggest that by constraining
with one’s peers, supervisor, and organization (e.g., Lynch contextual examinations to only social elements renders in-
et al., 1999; Masterson et al., 2000; Ozer, 2011; Settoon complete our present understanding of how social exchange
et al., 1996; Smith et al., 2020; Wayne et al., 1997). The and reciprocity obligations influence OCB.
theoretical interest of such research, however, resides in ex- With the above in mind, the purpose of the current research
amining the nature of social exchanges among individuals, not was to examine the causal linkages between the norm of rec-
the direct influence of reciprocity within these exchange rela- iprocity and decisions to engage in OCB, and how work con-
tionships. As an example, though firmly rooted in social ex- text creates boundary conditions for these causal linkages.
change theory, studies that reveal positive associations be- Work role performance is a function of the person and the
tween OCB and leader-member exchange emphasize the gen- context of work (Hattrup & Jackson, 1996), and this fact has
eral quality of exchange relationships as opposed to the spe- been recognized in social exchange approaches applied to
cific influence of reciprocity itself.1 Such approaches are valu- OCB (e.g., theory of other orientation; Meglino &
able because they depict the connection between high-quality Korsgaard, 2004, 2006). We thus followed this interactionist
interpersonal relationships and OCB, yet tell us little about the approach by incorporating relevant individual differences
precise antecedent role that reciprocity obligations play in (Tett & Burnett, 2003) and a mutually corresponsive situation-
performing OCB. al variable (Ostroff & Schulte, 2007). In essence, OCB is
Second, and more significant, is that the relationship be- discretionary workplace behavior (Organ, 1988) involving ac-
tween reciprocity obligations and OCB has largely been stud- tions that are affiliative and promotive in nature (Van Dyne
ied without regard for the work context in which this relation- et al., 1995). As such, scholars have argued that relevant indi-
ship exists. The overall paucity of contextual examinations is a vidual differences include the proficiency to effectively be-
significant concern for OCB research in general (Bommer have in a cooperative or prosocial manner (Dudley &
et al., 2007) and is particularly important for research that Cortina, 2008; Grant & Mayer, 2009) as well as the tendencies
invokes social exchange theory because the theory necessarily toward achievement and initiating change at work
involves a focus on situational contingencies. That is, one of (LePine et al., 2002; Bergeron et al., 2014; Parker, 1998).
the basic tenets of social exchange theory is that social situa- Consistent with these arguments, we examined two personal
tions generate normative definitions or “rules of exchange” attributes that reflect how effectively individuals interact with
among individuals (Emerson, 1976). This stipulation places others (interpersonal skill) and their behavioral tendencies for
a primacy on the social or interpersonal elements of context, achievement and change (proactive personality). As men-
which is among the reasons for its value in understanding an tioned above, OCB is thought to be a consequence of reci-
inherently prosocial behavior such as OCB. Yet, these mutual procity obligations and, thus, arises from normative situational
exchanges are not only linked to social context, but exist in a rules of social exchange. This implies that mutually corre-
broader context of work that also entails salient asocial ele- sponsive situations would be task contexts where external,
ments (Katz & Kahn, 1978). Obligations to reciprocate with normative cues are more (or less) prevalent. We examined this
OCB therefore occur within a general milieu that is comprised stipulation by studying how levels of accountability in the task
of more than just interpersonal features. For example, it has context, which reflect external cues of evaluation and justifi-
been long recognized in role theory (Biddle, 1979) and more cation of one’s actions, impinge upon the reciprocal ex-
recently in theories of work context (Johns, 2006) that influ- changes that predict OCB.
ential elements of work context extend to the structural and The specific integration of interpersonal skill, proactive
informational features of the discrete task context (Morgeson personality, and accountability into the examination of the
& Dierdorff, 2011). Consistent with this supposition, research effects of reciprocity obligations on OCB offers several con-
has shown that elements of task context often exert tributions. First, although scholars have identified the interper-
sonal skill as an important antecedent to OCB (Dudley &
1 Cortina, 2008; Motowildo et al., 1997), others have noted that
This conceptual focus derives from the assumption that leader-member ex-
change (LMX) relationships are a result of reciprocal and favorable interper- empirical verifications of this supposition are sparse. For ex-
sonal interactions (see Dienesch & Liden, 1986). The emphasis on the overall ample, Bolino and Grant (2016) observed, “there has also
quality of social exchanges is also apparent in measures of LMX, which
been surprisingly little attention given to the role of knowl-
consist of general dimensions such as contribution, affect, loyalty, and profes-
sional respect (Liden & Maslyn, 1998), rather than specific items that assess edge, skills, and abilities as drivers of prosocial behaviors and
reciprocity obligations in the LMX relationship. Finally, a basic premise of an influence on their effects” and more detailed examinations
LMX theory is that employees who have “higher quality LMX relationships are “sorely needed” (p. 35). Second, while ample research has
‘pay back’ their leaders by engaging in citizenship…” (p. 269, Ilies et al.,
2007) and yet, the obligation to reciprocate high-quality LMX with OCB is demonstrated a connection between proactive personality and
largely assumed rather than directly assessed. prosocial behaviors like OCB (Spitzmuller et al., 2015), the
J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281 265

role that reciprocity plays in this connection remains largely The obligation to reciprocate is central to understanding
untested even though research shows that employees often why individuals engage in OCB because these behaviors are
take the initiative to engage in helping behavior (Grant et al., a key way that individuals contribute to the social context of
2009). Finally, previous research has revealed rather equivo- the workplace as well as fulfill the social obligations that result
cal results for the associations between accountability and from ongoing social exchanges (Cropanzano & Mitchell,
OCB. This has led scholars to call for more nuanced exami- 2005; Organ, 1988). For example, research has generally sup-
nations of the influences of accountability (Hall et al., 2017) as ported the notion that individuals are more likely to engage in
well as for specific examinations of accountability as a con- OCB when they work in contexts of high-quality social ex-
textual moderator of individual performance of OCB (Rubin changes with their leaders or the organization in general
et al., 2013). (Eisenberger et al., 2001; Masterson et al., 2000), as well as
Our multi-method research consisted of two studies. Our when receiving OCB from their coworkers (Deckop et al.,
first study comprised two samples (N1 = 146, N2 = 265) and 2003). These studies offer indirect support that social ex-
used an experimental vignette methodology (Aguinis & change relationships are positively correlated with whether
Bradley, 2014) demonstrating that individual differences in or not individuals will perform OCB.
proactive personality and situational differences in account- Responding to reciprocity obligations by engaging in OCB
ability are key moderators of the antecedent effects of reci- is consistent with a functional approach to understanding why
procity obligations on decisions to engage in OCB. We then individuals perform these behaviors (Rioux & Penner, 2001),
conducted a second study using a field sample (N = 300) to where OCB can be utilized to satisfy goals such as maintain-
replicate and extend our experimental findings. This second ing mutually beneficial social relationships (Halbesleben &
study’s results confirm the norm of reciprocity hypothesis, Wheeler, 2011). As such, OCB is an important way individ-
replicate the moderating roles of accountability and proactive uals can fulfill the obligation to reciprocate the benefits re-
personality, and further demonstrate a more nuanced connec- ceived from others in the workplace, especially if those bene-
tion between reciprocity, accountability, and interpersonal fits are in the form of assistance or support (Bowling et al.,
skills when understanding individual performance of OCB. 2005; Deckop et al., 2003). Even more so, the felt pressure to
Taken collectively, our results provide causal evidence for reciprocate reflects a norm of reciprocity and stems from the
the norm of reciprocity effect on decisions to engage in dissonance that is created by the inequity of the social ex-
OCB and reveal the conditions under which this foundational change (Adams, 1965). Individuals typically strive to resolve
effect operates across individuals and elements of work this dissonance by allocating resources to the relationship
contexts. (Perugini et al., 2003) so as to avoid debt in social exchanges
(Blau, 1964). Along these lines, research has found that when
individuals feel that they are receiving more than reciprocat-
Study 1: The Effects and Boundary Conditions ing, they tend to increase cooperation (Clark & Sefton, 2001)
of Reciprocity Obligations on OCB and engage in more OCB (Halbesleben & Wheeler, 2011).
The above theory and related research suggests that the reci-
A central proposition of social exchange theory is that a series procity obligations should prompt individuals to perform
of interdependent and contingent interpersonal interactions OCB.
generates obligations toward others (Emerson, 1976). In work
settings, the application of social exchange theory presumes & Hypothesis 1: Obligations to reciprocate increase deci-
that workers form distinct exchange relationships with others sions to engage in OCB.
(e.g., direct reports, coworkers, supervisors) and that these
social connections hold implications for work-related behav-
ior because individuals feel obligated to return the benefits Individual and Contextual Moderators of Reciprocity
they receive from others (Cropanzano & Mitchell, 2005). Obligations
This obligation is referred to as the norm of reciprocity and,
from a social perspective, entails the expectation or “rule” of Beyond the individual propensity to reciprocate, essential to
behaving in a reciprocal manner toward other social actors all work role enactment is the premise that elements of work
(Gouldner, 1960). One way the social obligation to recipro- context are likely to impinge upon the extent to which behav-
cate manifests is when one acts to repay a benefit received ior is displayed. Within the OCB literature, work context has
from another person (Blau, 1964). This form of expectation demonstrated considerable potency in delineating the confines
has been referred to as a “paying you back” obligation and in which citizenship behavior is enacted. One category of
reflects the internalized acceptance of a social norm or pre- work context that bears relevance to OCB is referred to as task
ferred way to act: in this case, the norm of reciprocity as it context (Johns, 2006). Task context describes the structural
pertains to OCB (Korsgaard et al., 2010; Perugini et al., 2003). and informational elements of the work environment in which
266 J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281

roles are performed (Morgeson & Dierdorff, 2011). Prior re- Though certain task contexts can increase the salience of
search has revealed considerable influence of task context, social exchanges, individuals can differ in how they tend to
including factors that augment (e.g., autonomy) and attenuate react to these environmental norms (Cropanzano &
(e.g., ambiguity) OCB (Dierdorff et al., 2012; Podsakoff et al., Mitchell, 2005). Social exchanges, especially those
1996; Podsakoff et al., 1997). reflected by reciprocity obligations, essentially represent
In the present study, we focus on the potential influence of work situations where the need take action is created (i.e.,
a prototypical task context variable, namely, accountability. to behave reciprocally). This suggests that action or
Accountability reflects the extent to which “one’s decisions or approach-oriented dispositions are relevant to social ex-
actions will be evaluated by a salient audience and that re- changes. One such dispositional construct is that of proac-
wards or sanctions are believed to be contingent on this ex- tive personality, defined as a person’s propensity to initiate
pected evaluation” (p. 134; Hall & Ferris, 2011). As Hall et al. change in work situations (Bateman & Crant, 1993).
(2017) noted, accountability goes beyond an expectation of Proactive individuals look for opportunities to act, demon-
evaluation and incorporates the belief that one’s performance strate initiative, take action, and seek to control their envi-
requires an “account-giving” or explanation of one’s work. ronments, whereas individuals who are low in proactivity
Thus, a primary outcome in contexts of heightened account- tend to be relatively passive and content to endure their
ability is that individuals typically increase their awareness of, circumstances (Crant, 2000). Proactivity is thought to en-
and attentiveness to, external performance standards. It is thus hance effectiveness in part because proactive individuals
unsurprising that some research finds strong accountability are more likely to both recognize and create conditions that
contexts are associated with increases in role strain (Hall facilitate their performance (Greguras & Diefendorff,
et al., 2006) and decreases in prosocial behaviors like OCB, 2010). Research has generally supported this supposition,
as employees narrow their attentional capacity to focus on task with meta-analytic evidence of positive relationships be-
performance directly linked with visible rewards or results tween proactive personality and task performance
(Mitchell et al., 1998; Rubin et al., 2013). Yet at the same (Thomas et al., 2010), and facets of OCB such as voice
time, other research has shown the positive effects of account- and taking charge (Fuller & Marler, 2009). In addition,
ability on OCB (Hall et al., 2009; Hall & Ferris, 2011). Bergeron et al. (2014) found proactive personality was pos-
Regarding these mixed results, Hall et al. (2017) suggest that itively related to the frequency with which individuals per-
the relationship between accountability and OCB “is compli- form OCB.
cated and warrants further empirical scrutiny” (p. 10). We These previous empirical findings indicate that individ-
posit that more clarity can be achieved when one considers uals with higher proactive personality are more likely to
how causal mechanisms involved in the social exchange pro- engage in OCB. Relevant to the current study, we argue
cess are likely impacted by environments high in that proactive personality is also likely to factor into the
accountability. process whereby individuals fulfill reciprocity obligations
Social exchange theory holds that over time, the rules of with OCB. As noted above, individuals with higher proac-
social exchange create a “normative definition of the situation tive tendencies are more likely to seek control of their
that forms among or is adopted by the participants in an ex- situations, search the environment for information and op-
change relation” (p. 351, Emerson, 1976). These normative portunities, and take action to affect change in their work
definitions become internalized and reciprocity obligations situations. These behavioral tendencies are likely to ampli-
become progressively guided by such norms. In high account- fy the effects of increased reciprocity obligations on deci-
ability contexts, the adherence to internalized reciprocal sions to perform OCB. For example, not fulfilling the norm
norms is likely amplified as accountability shines a spotlight of reciprocity is akin to failing to “control” the expected
on the normative rules of social exchange. In this sense, high social exchange within the work situation. Moreover, the
accountability contexts create “strong situations” whereby in- proclivity to search for information makes proactive indi-
dividuals are likely to interpret appropriate courses of action in viduals more attuned to the social cues in their work situ-
uniform ways and regulate behavior accordingly (Meyer et al., ations (Ashford & Black, 1996; Ashford & Cummings,
2010; Mischel, 1977). Thus, in conditions of high account- 1985), which would include noticing that others have ex-
ability, reciprocity obligations likely increase as contextual tended help and the concomitant obligation to repay such
cues for desired behavior are more readily visible to others. help. Finally, the tendency to proactively take action is
We therefore argue that accountability magnifies the effects of known to facilitate the incorporation of discretionary be-
reciprocity on decisions to engage in OCB. havior into one’s performance of work (Morrison &
Phelps, 1999; Parker, 1998). For these reasons, we hypoth-
& Hypothesis 2: The relationship between the obligation to esize that the effects of reciprocity obligations will be
reciprocate and OCB is amplified in conditions of high stronger when individuals are higher in proactive person-
accountability. ality as compared to those lower in this disposition.
J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281 267

& Hypothesis 3: The relationship between the obligation to description of the required task. After indicating they under-
reciprocate and OCB is amplified for individuals with stood and agreed to proceed, participants were randomly
more proactive personalities. assigned to read the experimental vignettes. After reading
the assigned vignette, participants were asked to indicate the
likelihood they would perform OCB, followed by other items
that measured the manipulation, demographics, and other
Study 1: Method study 1 variables. The experimental vignette conditions used
in sample 1 and sample 2 are provided in the Appendix.
Participants and Design Beyond tests pertinent to our three hypotheses, we also exam-
ined the possibility of an additional three-way interaction be-
Two samples were collected for study 1. The first sample was tween reciprocity obligations, accountability, and proactive
used to examine the efficacy of our experimental vignette personality. This supplemental analysis was exploratory in
methodology (i.e., testing the experimental manipulation of nature.
reciprocity obligations) and to provide initial evidence perti-
nent to Hypothesis 1. Participants in the first sample were 146 Procedure and Measures (Sample 1)
individuals enrolled in a graduate school of business at a uni-
versity in the Midwestern United States who volunteered to A single-factor between-subjects design with random assign-
participate and earned extra credit across multiple sections of a ment was used in sample 1, where we manipulated the inten-
management course. The mean age of the sample was 31.4 sity of reciprocity obligations.
and 56% was female. In terms of work experience, 91% of the
sample had more than 5 years of experience and 65% with Experimental Conditions We created three distinct vignettes
more than 8 years. As discussed below, results were favorable that described work situations where the extent to which one’s
and supported the manipulation of reciprocity obligations. coworker had helped on previous projects was varied in order
A second sample was collected and used to provide repli- to manipulate the intensity of reciprocity obligations. After the
cation evidence for Hypothesis 1, as well as direct tests of vignettes, individuals were presented with a decision to offer
Hypotheses 2 and 3. This second sample consisted of partic- their coworker help on a new project with which the coworker
ipants recruited from the United States using Amazon’s was struggling to complete the assigned tasks. We were care-
Mechanical Turk (Rand, 2012). A total of 265 individuals ful to minimize other factors that could influence the vignette
comprised this second sample. The mean age of the sample manipulations. For example, other scenario details were held
was 36.34 (SD = 11.61), 45% was female, and 81% had more constant across the three vignettes, a prior “history” was
than 5 years of work experience. The racial composition of the established with the coworker (2 years of working on other
sample was 82% White, 6% Black, 9% Asian, and 3% indi- projects), and the new project’s tasks were described as inde-
cated “other.” Several quality control checks were used to pendent (versus interdependent) in nature. In the high
detect careless responding (Meade & Craig, 2012). First, in- obligation condition, the coworker (named “Tom”) was de-
structions specified that individuals have at least 2 years of scribed as regularly helping on tasks that were specifically
work experience, which allowed a crosscheck with a survey assigned to the participant such that the coworker had helped
item asking years of work experience. Second, full completion much more in the past compared to the participant. The low
of all study measurements was required and verified. Third, obligation condition described the participant as regularly
responses more than one standard deviation below the average helping to complete the coworker’s assigned tasks such that
study completion time were flagged for review. Finally, we the participant had helped much more in the past than the
examined within-person response patterns to ensure that indi- coworker. The equal obligation condition described previous
viduals, especially those with fast response times, were not helping to be evenly distributed (i.e., the coworker and partic-
simply providing identical ratings on consecutive items or ipant had previously helped each other equally).
other careless patterns (e.g., strict increasing/decreasing se-
quences). No participants were identified as careless OCB Intentions The dependent variable measured participants’
respondents. intentions to perform OCB and was derived from the interper-
sonal citizenship behavior scale by Settoon and Mossholder
General Procedure (2002). Three items from this scale were used along with the
initial prompt, “How likely is it that you would…” These
Our approach followed recommendations for an experimental items included “assist Tom [the coworker’s name] with the
vignette study design (Aguinis & Bradley, 2014). Participants assignments, even when assistance is not directly requested,”
were first informed that the experiment was intended to study “go out of your way to help Tom with project-related prob-
how people make decisions at work and provided a brief lems,” “take on extra responsibilities in order to help Tom
268 J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281

complete work assignments,” and “help Tom when he is run- rated two items used for manipulation checks. Similar to sam-
ning behind in his work activities.” The items were rated using ple 1, the first item read, “In the scenario, Avery [the co-
a 5-point scale: 1 = extremely unlikely, 2 = unlikely, 3 = worker’s name] gave me a great deal of help on past projects”
somewhat likely, 4 = likely, and 5 = very likely. Coefficient which checked for the manipulation of reciprocity obligations.
alpha for the scale was .91. The second item was a manipulation check for accountability
and read, “In the scenario, my individual actions were highly
Manipulation Check Participants rated an item that served as visible to my boss and coworkers.” Both items were rated
the manipulation check for the experimental conditions for using a 5-point agreement scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 =
reciprocity obligations. This item read, “In the scenario, disagree, 3 = neither disagree nor agree, 4 = agree, 5 = strongly
Tom gave me a great deal of help on past projects” and was agree). Means tests indicated that both manipulations were
rated using a 5-point agreement scale (1 = strongly disagree, 2 effective. For the reciprocity obligation manipulations, the
= disagree, 3 = neither disagree nor agree, 4 = agree, 5 = high obligation condition was significantly higher than the
strongly agree).2 low condition (high obligation: M = 4.70, SD = .52; low ob-
We expected that intentions to perform OCB would be ligation: M = 1.85, SD = 1.02; t = 27.99, df =250, p < .01). For
stronger in the high obligation condition compared to the the accountability manipulation, the high accountability con-
equal and low obligation conditions. Comparing means across dition was significantly higher than the low accountability
the three conditions supported the manipulation (F = 128.40, condition (high accountability: M = 4.57, SD = .56; low ac-
df = 2, p < .01, η2 = .64). In the high obligation condition, countability: M = 1.82, SD = .57; t = 38.80, df =250, p < .01).
participants’ ratings were higher than both the equal and low Proactive personality was measured using a 6-item scale
conditions (high obligation: M = 4.32, SD = .59; equal obli- (Claes et al., 2005). Participants were asked to indicate the
gation: M = 3.43, SD = .73; low obligation: M = 2.05, SD = accuracy with which each item described them using a 5-
.92). Importantly, ratings for the manipulation check were point scale (1 = very inaccurate, 2 = inaccurate, 3 = neither
significantly higher in the high obligation condition compared inaccurate nor accurate, 4 = accurate, 5 = very accurate). The
to the low obligation condition (t = 15.79, df =114, p < .01) six items were as follows: “if I see something I don’t like, I fix
and the equal obligation condition (t = 6.15, df = 88, p < .01). it”; “no matter what the odds, if I believe in something I will
Manipulation check ratings were also significantly higher in make it happen”; “if I believe in an idea, no obstacle will
the equal obligation condition than in the low obligation con- prevent me from making it happen”; “I excel at identifying
dition (t = 7.09, df = 84, p < .01). With supportive evidence opportunities”; “I am always looking for better ways to do
from sample 1, we proceeded to conduct our experimental things”; and “I love being a champion for my ideas, even
design using the second sample. against others’ opposition.” Coefficient alpha was .86.

Procedure and Measures (Sample 2)


Results and Discussion
The design was 2 × 2 full factorial between-subjects experi-
ment with random assignment where we manipulated reci- Hypothesis 1 predicted that when faced with a situation where
procity obligations and levels of accountability. Participants the norm of reciprocity creates stronger reciprocity obliga-
were randomly assigned to read one of four different vignettes tions, individuals are more likely to decide to perform OCB.
that described work situations where reciprocity obligations Results from both samples provided evidence to test this hy-
and levels of accountability were manipulated (see the pothesis. Across the three obligation intensity conditions in
Appendix). Consistent with the construct definitions of ac- sample 1, the likelihood of engaging in OCB significantly
countability (Frink & Klimoski, 1998; Hochwarter et al., varied (F = 37.12, df = 2, p < .01, η2 = .34), with the highest
2003), we manipulated this contextual factor by varying the likelihood of performing OCB in conditions with higher rec-
level of performance visibility and required justification of iprocity obligations (high obligation: M = 4.55, SD = .77;
one’s actions in the vignettes. equal obligation: M = 4.07, SD = .69; low obligation: M =
The dependent variable was again intentions to perform 3.20, SD = .99). Moreover, participants in the high obligation
OCB and the same 3-item scale was used. Participants also condition were significantly more likely to perform OCB than
those in either the equal or low conditions (t = 2.91, df = 88, p
2 < .01 and t = 8.56, df = 114, p < .01, respectively). Participants
We elected not to use a more explicit manipulation check, such as “did you
feel a sense of obligation to Tom (Avery),” in order to avoid creating a biasing in the equal obligation condition also indicated they were
demand characteristic that would too obviously queue respondents to “felt more likely to perform OCB compared to the low condition
obligations” and thus overtly convey our hypotheses possibly resulting in a (t = 4.25, df = 84, p < .01). Results from sample 2 replicated
significant threat to inference validity (i.e., reactivity). Our approach to this
manipulation check is also consistent with prior experimental work on felt these results. In sample 2, reciprocity obligations significantly
obligations (e.g., Korsgaard et al., 2010). related to individuals’ likelihood to engage in OCB (high
J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281 269

5
obligation: M = 4.44, SD = .51; low obligation: M = 3.00, SD
= .89; t = 16.27, df = 264, p < .01). Taken collectively, results 4.5
fully support Hypothesis 1 and indicate that reciprocity obli-
gations are causally antecedent to an individual’s intentions to 4
perform OCB.
Hypothesis 2 predicted that accountability would moderate 3.5 Low Accountability
High Accountability
the effects of reciprocity obligations on an individual’s deci-
3
sion to engage in OCB. Results from a two-way analysis of
variance supported this proposed interaction (see Table 1).
2.5
Simple effects analysis further indicated that reciprocity obli-
gations exerted significant effects across both accountability 2
conditions (high accountability: F(1, 266) = 188.26, p < .01; Low Obligation High Obligation
low accountability: F(1, 266) = 91.85, p < .01), with pairwise Fig. 1 Interaction between reciprocity obligations and accountability on
mean differences showing higher obligations associated with decisions to perform OCB (study 1)
higher intent to perform OCB (high accountability condition:
Δ mean = 1.69, 95% CI [1.49, 1.93]; low accountability con- higher intentions to perform OCB for individuals with low
dition: Δ mean = 1.18, 95% CI [1.42, .94]). Simple effects proactive personality (−1 SD) with a mean difference of
analysis also showed that accountability had significant ef- 1.71 (95% CI = 1.48, 1.94), as well as for individuals with
fects in the high obligation condition (F(1, 266) = 5.14, p < high proactive personality (+1 SD) with a mean difference of
.05), with a pairwise mean differences showing higher obliga- 1.26 (95% CI = 1.03, 1.49). The form of this moderation is
tion associated with higher intent to perform OCB (Δ mean = shown in Fig. 2 and indicates that the effects of reciprocity
.28, 95% CI [.04, .52]). However, the effects of reciprocity obligations are accentuated when individuals are high in pro-
obligations were equal in the low accountability condition active personality. These results fully support Hypothesis 3.
(F(1, 266) = 3.46, p > .05; Δ mean = .23, 95% CI [−.01, We additionally explored the possible combined effects of
.48]). Figure 1 displays this interaction, indicating that con- reciprocity obligations, accountability, and proactive person-
texts high in accountability amplify the effects of reciprocity ality on decisions to engage in OCB. Results from this post
obligations on decisions to engage in OCB. hoc exploratory analysis did not support a significant three-
Hypothesis 3 predicted that proactive personality moder- way interaction among these variables (SS = 1.13, F(1, 266) =
ates the effects of the obligation to reciprocate. Table 2 shows 2.62, p > .10).
the results relevant to this hypothesis from a two-way analysis In sum, the results from study 1 provide direct evidence for
of variance with a covariate. The interaction term between the norm of reciprocity proposition on individuals’ decisions
reciprocity obligations and proactive personality was signifi- to engage in OCB. Our findings indicate that when one’s
cant (p < .05). Note that we included terms for both account- obligation to reciprocate is increased due to previous contri-
ability and reciprocity obligations because our design was full butions from others in the workplace, the likelihood of offer-
factorial whereby each condition varied on both of these fea- ing OCB increases. These results contribute causal evidence
tures. A model without accountability terms corroborated the to the associations found in prior OCB research that has been
significant interaction between reciprocity obligations and cross-sectional in nature. Further, study 1 results demonstrate
proactive personality (SS = 2.67, F = 5.97, p < .05). that personal and situational attributes shape this main effect.
Pairwise mean differences from the model shown in Table 2
indicated that reciprocity obligations were associated with
Table 2 Effects of reciprocity obligations and proactive personality on
OCB in study 1
Table 1 Effects of reciprocity obligations and accountability on OCB in Source df SS F Partial η2
study 1
Reciprocity condition 1 144.82 335.57** .56
Source df SS F Partial η2
Accountability condition 1 .02 .04 .00
Reciprocity condition 1 136.97 271.48** .51 Proactive personality 1 14.66 33.96** .12
Accountability condition 1 .04 .06 .00 Reciprocity × accountability 1 5.09 11.78** .05
Reciprocity × accountability 1 4.28 8.49** .03 Reciprocity × proactive personality 1 3.21 7.43** .03
Error 262 132.19 Error 260 112.20

Model R2 = .52 Model R2 = .59


**p < .01 **p < .01
270 J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281

5
A primary supposition throughout our previous rationale is
4.5
that performing OCB is an inherently social process steeped in
complex interpersonal exchanges (Organ et al., 2006). Thus, it
4 stands to reason that the extent to which an individual accu-
rately diagnoses and responds to reciprocal cues in the social
3.5 Low Proactive Personality environment is governed in part by that person’s interpersonal
High Proactive Personality
skill. In its broadest form, interpersonal skill involves “goal-
3
directed behaviors, including communication and
relationship-building competencies, employed in interperson-
2.5
al interaction episodes characterized by complex perceptual
2
and cognitive processes, dynamic verbal and non-verbal inter-
Low Obligation High Obligation action exchanges, diverse roles, motivations and expectan-
Fig. 2 Interaction between reciprocity obligations and proactive cies” (p. 81; Klein et al., 2006). More specifically, the Klein
personality on decisions to perform OCB (study 1) et al. (2006) interpersonal skill taxonomy suggests the uni-
verse of interpersonal skills is categorized in one of two broad
In contexts of high accountability where one’s performance is dimensions: communication-based or relationship-based
highly visible to others, our results suggest reciprocity obliga- skills. Communication-based interpersonal skills span written,
tions are even more likely to promote OCB. Individuals with oral, non-verbal, active listening, and assertive forms of com-
more proactive personalities also appear more likely to offer munication, whereas relationship-based skills span coopera-
OCB in general; yet, when presented with the obligation to tion, coordination, trust, intercultural sensitivity, service ori-
reciprocate, this dispositional propensity increases the likeli- entation, self-presentation, social influence, conflict resolu-
hood of OCB. tion, and negotiation.
Although the experimental results of study 1 enhance in- Individuals with strong interpersonal skill are more likely
ternal validity and inferences of causal ordering, such designs to be sensitive to social interactions and have increased
do raise questions about the generalizability of observed ef- knowledge of the norms that dictate socially appropriate be-
fects. We thus conducted a second study using a field sample havior (Riggio, 1986). Within social exchanges, these in-
to seek corroboration of the main effects for reciprocity obli- creased social sensitivities afford interpersonally skilled indi-
gations and the moderating effects of accountability and viduals the advantage of being able to accurately assess social
proactivity. We further extended this analysis by examining cues and determine the extent to which the norm of reciprocity
another individual difference variable, this time focusing on a exists in various work situations. In doing so, interpersonally
malleable attribute that is also relevant to social interactions skilled people often maintain high-quality interpersonal rela-
(interpersonal skill). tionships (Segrin & Taylor, 2007), as they fulfill their role in
upholding socially normative behavior.
Given the interpersonal nature of OCB, previous scholars
Study 2: The Interplay between Reciprocity have correspondingly proposed that proficiency related to co-
Obligations and Interpersonal Skill ordinating with others, social perceptiveness, perspective-tak-
ing, and conversational skills are likely to account for variation
Consistent with an interactionist approach, we have argued in OCB (Dudley & Cortina, 2008; Motowildo et al., 1997).
that both individual and situational factors shape social ex- Although empirical research testing these conjectures is quite
change relationships and, in particular, the positive effects of limited, there is some related evidence suggesting that some
reciprocity obligations on decisions to engage in OCB. In this facets of interpersonal skill promote increased cooperation
second study, we used a field sample to again test the moder- needed for task proficiency (Ferris et al., 2001; Morgeson
ating roles that accountability and proactive personality play et al., 2005) and facilitate more effective interpersonal interac-
in the relationships between reciprocity obligations and OCB. tions connected to job performance (Ozer, 2011). A study by
We further tested how individual differences in interpersonal Ellington et al. (2014) also found that interpersonal skill was
skill augment the links between reciprocity and OCB. We also positively associated with the frequency with which OCB is
applied a more generalized approach to reciprocity by using performed. Taken collectively, research on interpersonal skill
an equity-based (Adams, 1965) conceptualization of reciproc- and related evidence from OCB studies suggest that these attri-
ity obligations, taking into account the overall inputs and re- butes are likely to accentuate the effects of reciprocity obliga-
ceived benefits from one’s coworkers (versus help received or tions on OCB. The primary reason for this prediction is because
given on past projects as in study 1). Finally, we extended our interpersonally skilled individuals are more likely to accurately
criterion from decisions to perform OCB to managerial ratings interpret normative reciprocal cues and, thus, more effectively
of OCB. return OCB to fulfill these social exchange obligations.
J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281 271

& Hypothesis 4: The relationship between the obligation to scale (1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree): “I am held
reciprocate and OCB is amplified for individuals with very accountable for my actions at work”; “if things at work
higher interpersonal skill. do not go the way that they should, my boss will let me
know”; “my boss holds me accountable for all of my deci-
sions; my job requires that I often have to explain why I do
certain things at work”; “in the grand scheme of things, my job
Study 2: Method is very important”; and, “my co-workers, subordinates, and
bosses closely scrutinize my efforts at work.” Coefficient al-
Participants and Design pha for this measure was .82.

Participants for study 2 were 300 incumbents enrolled in a Interpersonal Skill This variable was operationalized with sev-
part-time Masters of Business Administration program for en items that were rated by participants’ coworkers. These
working professionals at a large university in the Midwest. ratings were collected as part of a developmental multisource
Mean age of the sample was 34.25 and 46% was female. feedback project and all ratings were anonymous to ensure
The average tenure in their current jobs was 3.28 years (SD candor and confidentiality on the part of the respondents.
= 2.44) and the average years of overall work experience was Items correspond to the broad dimensions of interpersonal
6.32 (SD = 3.71). The average tenure with their current man- skills represented by Klein et al. (2006). Coworkers were
ager was 2.49 (SD = 1.45). Data were collected using two asked to rate how descriptive seven interpersonal behaviors
surveys that were administered 5 weeks apart. The first survey were of the target individual using a 5-point scale (1 = not at
measured reciprocity, accountability, proactive personality, all descriptive to 5 = very descriptive). The interpersonal skill
and demographics. The second survey was a multisource in- items were as follows: “works well in team settings”; “builds
strument that measured OCB and interpersonal skill. rapport with others inside the organization”; “displays sincere
interest in others”; “shows empathy toward others”; “builds
Measures strong relationships with industry professionals outside his/
her own organization”; “knows a wide range of people who
Reciprocity Obligations We operationalized reciprocity obli- can get the job done.” ICC(1) was .54 and .92 for ICC(2),
gations using an equity-based approach similar to previous suggesting support for a rating target effect and the aggrega-
studies that have examined reciprocity in the workplace tion of coworkers’ ratings (LeBreton & Senter, 2008).
(e.g., Halbesleben & Wheeler, 2011; Smets et al., 2004; Van
Dierendonck et al., 2001). This variable was measured with a Proactive Personality This variable was assessed with the
4-item scale that asked, “overall, how much effort do you feel same 6-item scale used in study 1. The coefficient alpha in
you put into relationships with your coworkers,” “overall, this study was .81.
how much do you feel your coworkers put into relationships
with you,” “how much benefit do you feel you receive from OCB This variable was operationalized with supervisor ratings
the relationships that you have your coworkers,” and “how using an 8-item scale described by Dierdorff et al. (2012) and
much benefit do you feel your coworkers receive from their based on the summary dimensions of citizenship behavior
relationships with you.” Items were rated using a 5-point scale identified by Coleman and Borman (2000). Scale items were
from very little (1) to very much (5). To calculate reciprocity as follows: “takes advantage of developmental opportunities”;
obligations, a score was created by subtracting the ratio of “demonstrates initiative”; “shows strong commitment toward
coworker benefits to coworker inputs from the ratio of self- her/his work”; “endorses, supports, or defends organizational
perceived benefits to the self-perceived inputs. Perfectly equi- objectives”; “works with others to effectively resolve con-
table environments are indicated by scores of zero. Positive flicts”; “engages in behavior that benefits the organization as
values indicated participants were receiving relatively more a whole”; “manages group activities responsibly and effec-
benefits compared to the effort they invested in social relation- tively”; and, “helps other organizational members.”
ships at work (i.e., high reciprocity obligations), whereas neg- Coefficient alpha for this scale was .94.3
ative values indicated participants were receiving relatively
less benefits compared to the effort they invested (i.e., low 3
The OCB measure we used spans discretionary behavior that is directed at
reciprocity obligations). Coefficient alpha was .80.
different targets, such as the job, other individuals, or the organization. It could
be that reciprocity obligations are primarily linked to OCB that is strictly
Accountability This variable was operationalized with a 6- interpersonal in nature. We thus reexamined our data using only the three
item scale described by Rubin et al. (2013), which is based individual-focused items to measure our criterion. The level of support for
each of our predictions was unchanged. The statistical significance and direc-
on a longer measure by Hochwarter et al. (2003). The scale tions of individual parameter estimates in these tests were consistent with those
asked participants to rate the following items using a 5-point using the full OCB scale.
272 J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281

Results and Discussion Table 4 Main and interactive effects of reciprocity obligations on OCB
from study 2

Table 3 shows the means, standard deviations, and zero-order Predictor Step 1 Step 2 ΔR2
correlations among study 2 variables. Obligation intensity was
positively related to supervisor-rated OCB performance (r = β SE β SE
.25, p < .01), thus offering evidence of the generalizability of Reciprocity .11** .03 .12** .03
the effects found in the prior experimental study.
Accountability −.06* .03 −.06* .03
Accountability was negatively related to supervisor-rated
Proactive personality .12** .03 .13** .03
OCB (r = −.15, p < .05), whereas proactive personality and
Interpersonal skill .09** .03 .09** .03 .19**
interpersonal skills were positively related to supervisor-rated
Reciprocity × accountability .06* .03
OCB (r = .31 and .30, p < .01, respectively). Table 4 shows
Reciprocity × proactive personality .08* .04
multiple regression analysis results from study 2. Similar to
Reciprocity × interpersonal skill −.04 .04 .03**
study 1, we again expected a main effect of reciprocity obli-
gations on OCB and that this effect would be conditioned by N = 300
accountability and proactive personality. Results in Table 4 *p <.05
fully support these predictions, with a positive main effect for **p < .01
reciprocity obligations (β = .11, p < .01), and a significant
interaction term between reciprocity obligations and proactive
personality (β = .08, p < .05) as well as between reciprocity Although interpersonal skill failed to moderate the effects
obligations and accountability (β = .06, p < .05). The forms of of reciprocity obligations on OCB, both reciprocity obliga-
moderation are depicted in Figs. 3 and 4. Replicating study 1 tions and interpersonal skill showed main effects on OCB
results, these plots show that both accountability and proactive and the effects of reciprocity obligations were conditioned
personality amplify the effects of reciprocity obligations on by accountability. Because individual behavior like OCB oc-
performance of OCB. curs within a work context that has both social and asocial
Hypothesis 4 predicted individuals’ levels of interpersonal elements, it may be that accountability could play a role in
skill moderate the main effect of reciprocity obligations on the relationships among reciprocity obligations, interpersonal
OCB such that the relationship would be amplified for more skill, and OCB. To examine this possibility, we conducted a
interpersonally proficient individuals. Interpersonal skill post hoc analysis that explored a three-way interaction be-
exerted a significant and positive main effect on OCB (β = tween reciprocity obligations, interpersonal skill, and account-
.09, p < .01). However, the interaction term between interper- ability. Results from this exploratory analysis showed that this
sonal skill and reciprocity obligations was not statistically interaction was significant (β = −.06, p < .05, one-tailed). The
significant (β = −.04, p > .10) and thus failed to support form of this interaction is shown in Fig. 5. One noticeable
Hypothesis 4. We also examined a model with only an inter- trend suggested by this moderation is that, when reciprocity
action term for reciprocity obligations and interpersonal skill obligations increase, the likelihood of engaging in OCB gen-
(i.e., not interaction terms for proactivity and accountability). erally increases except when individuals possess low levels of
This model did not reveal a significant interaction between interpersonal skill and are in low-accountability contexts. In
reciprocity obligations and interpersonal skill (β = −.48, SE this situation, increased obligation to reciprocate diminishes
= .029, t = −1.64, 95% CI = −.11 to .01). the performance of OCB by these individuals.

Table 3 Means, standard deviations, and correlations for study 2


variables General Discussion
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4
Using a multi-method approach across two studies, we find
1. Reciprocity obligations .20 .72 convergent evidence to support reciprocity obligations as an
2. Accountability 3.16 .81 −.13* underlying mechanism affecting when individuals choose per-
3. Proactive personality 4.12 .80 .06 .04 form OCB. We further find that the effects of reciprocity ob-
4. Interpersonal skill 4.07 .85 .08 −.11* .43** ligations are conditioned by specific individual and situational
5. OCB 4.30 .52 .25** −.15** .31** .30** factors. Here, we examined the personal attributes of proactive
personality and interpersonal skill, as well as situational dif-
N = 300 ferences in accountability. We chose these variables to ac-
*p < .05 count for the fact that OCB is an inherently discretionary
**p < .01 and prosocial workplace behavior (Organ, 1988; Van Dyne
J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281 273

5.00
Low Accountability assumed to be antecedents in previous OCB scholarship, ev-
4.75 High Accountability idence for this supposition has been predominately correla-
4.50 tional in nature. Even a cursory examination of the host of
4.25 OCB studies that have invoked social exchange theory will
quickly reveal warnings of causal interpretation. Thus, results
4.00
from study 1, along with evidence from Korsgaard et al.
3.75 (2010), represent relatively rare empirical results that help to
3.50 affirm the fundamental antecedent effect of reciprocity obli-
3.25 gations on decisions to perform OCB.
Drawing on role theory and an interactional approach, re-
3.00
Low Reciprocity High Reciprocity sults from study 1 show that the effects of reciprocity obliga-
Fig. 3 Interaction between reciprocity obligations and accountability on tions on OCB do not exist in isolation, but rather are moder-
OCB (study 2) ated by individual and situational factors. We found that work
contexts characterized by high levels of accountability signif-
et al., 1995) and that reciprocity obligations are shaped by icantly moderated the relationship between high reciprocity
normative rules of social exchange arising in part from exter- obligations and a person’s decision to engage in OCB. This
nal cues (Emerson, 1976). suggests that when individuals work in environments where
Using an experimental vignette approach, results from our performance is highly visible and externally evaluated, their
first study empirically establish the primary link between rec- adherence to the norms of reciprocity are amplified. With
iprocity obligations and decisions to perform OCB. regard to individual differences, study 1 also demonstrates that
Participants in the high obligation condition chose to perform obligations to reciprocate with OCB are significantly stronger
OCB at significantly higher rates than those in the low obli- for individuals with high levels of proactive personality. This
gation conditions. These results suggest that when individuals finding is consistent with the propositions that proactive per-
receive help from others in the workplace, they will recipro- sonality augments reciprocal behavior and that proactive indi-
cate with OCB. Such a finding supports the widely held theory viduals are more likely to recognize and manage interactions
that individuals internalize the norm of reciprocity as a form of at work situations (Grant & Ashford, 2008).
moral acting or social responsibility (Blau, 1964; Perugini In study 2 using a field sample, we sought to replicate the
et al., 2003) and that this social norm obligates them to re- effects we observed for reciprocity obligations, accountability,
spond to others’ past behavior by “paying it back” with OCB. and proactive personality, as well as extend our examination
At first blush, these results may appear unsurprising. Yet, it is to an additional moderator that reflects a person’s interperson-
important to note that evidence confirming reciprocity obliga- al competence. The results again confirm the norm of reci-
tions as direct antecedents to OCB is both essential and much procity hypothesis and replicate the positive moderating roles
needed in the OCB literature. As Cropanzano and Mitchell of accountability and proactive personality in the relationships
(2005) noted in regard to the effects of social exchanges, between reciprocity obligations and OCB. With regard to in-
“one could plausibly argue that a series of contingent ex- terpersonal competence, we argued that such skills are funda-
changes alter the interpersonal relationship or that an interper- mental to reciprocal obligations and thus, high levels of inter-
sonal relationship alters the series of contingent exchanges” personal skill would amplify the relationship between reci-
(p. 888). That is, though social exchanges have long been procity and OCB. However, our results did not support this
predicted moderation. While interpersonal skill was positively
5.00 related to performing OCB, it failed to moderate the relation-
Low Proactive
Personality
ship between reciprocity obligations and OCB. One reason for
4.75
this finding could be that reciprocity obligations hold enough
4.50 High Proactive
Personality
influence to promote OCB regardless of an individual’s level
4.25 of interpersonal skill. That is, the effects of social exchanges
4.00
on OCB appear to be consistent across individuals even
though individuals may differ with regard to their effective-
3.75
ness at navigating interpersonal interactions at work. Another
3.50 reason could stem from our measurement of interpersonal
3.25 skill. Interpersonal skill represents a broad set of specific com-
munication and relationship-building skills. Although the
3.00
Low Reciprocity High Reciprocity items we used align with broad interpersonal skill dimensions,
Fig. 4 Interaction between reciprocity obligations and proactive the measure was not exhaustive and could introduce content
personality on OCB (study 2) deficiency. This deficiency may have lowered our ability to
274 J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281

Fig. 5 Three-way interaction 5.00 Low Interpersonal Skill-Low Accountability

between reciprocity obligations, High Interpersonal Skill-Low Accountability


4.75 Low Interpersonal Skill-High Accountability
accountability, and interpersonal
skill on OCB (study 2) High Interpersonal Skill-High Accountability
4.50

4.25

4.00

3.75

3.50

3.25
Low Reciprocity High Reciprocity

detect sufficient displays of interpersonal skill and contributed relationship to ensure the receipt of future benefits from an-
to the lack of moderation in our data. other person. This is an important distinction because a sub-
Another explanation for the lack of moderation by inter- stantial amount of OCB scholarship has been concerned with
personal skill could be that this personal attribute is “activat- the various sources of motivation that propel individuals to
ed” (Tett & Burnett, 2003) by other situational cues beyond perform OCB (e.g., impression management, prosocial
reciprocity obligations. We explored this possibility in a post values, concern for others). Some of this literature posits that
hoc analysis that revealed a more complex relationship (see people engage in OCB in a calculated, self-interested manner
Fig. 5). Results here suggest that reciprocity obligations, in- (Bolino, 1999), whereas the obligation to reciprocate has been
terpersonal skill, and accountability interact to shape how rec- characterized as an internalized, automatic response to social
iprocity affects the performance of OCB. In particular, when norms (Korsgaard et al., 2010). Our results may offer a bridge
obligations are high, strong interpersonal skill generally am- between these perspectives by suggesting that engagement in
plifies the effects of reciprocity on OCB regardless of levels of OCB often involves simultaneous considerations of both felt
accountability. Yet, we also find that accountability has a obligations and expectations of future benefits (i.e., paying
buffering effect such that in high accountability contexts, the back and paying forward) and, more importantly, these moti-
positive relationship between reciprocity obligations and vations are likely to vary across different contexts. For in-
OCB remains even for those individuals low in interpersonal stance, we find that under conditions of high obligation, high
skill. It is also interesting to note that the relationship between accountability contexts consistently amplify decisions to en-
interpersonal skill, accountability, and OCB also appears to gage in OCB. This suggests that individuals respond to both
turn particularly deleterious when those weak in interpersonal self-interest (i.e., enhancing performance in the eyes of others)
skill work in low-accountability contexts, where our results and feelings of social obligation (i.e., paying someone back)
indicate that these individuals perform less OCB even when to uphold the norm of reciprocity.
obligations to do so are high. This latter finding suggests ac- Our results also help to clarify the rather mixed findings in
countability may facilitate the recognition of social cues that previous literature examining relationships between account-
are inherent to social exchanges, especially for individuals that ability and OCB. Study 1 results suggest that decisions to
are the least skilled in this regard. In other words, accountabil- perform OCB are greater under conditions of high obligation
ity might compensate for lower interpersonal skill, at least in than low obligation regardless of the level of accountability,
terms of fulfilling norms of reciprocity related to OCB. although accountability does substantially accentuate the ef-
Although the results presented in Fig. 5 are suggestive of a fects of reciprocity obligations. It is interesting to note the
more complex relationship, these findings should be possible paradox between the essential definitional notion of
interpreted in light of the exploratory nature of this analysis, OCB as discretionary action and the supposition that account-
which require further consideration and replication in future ability reflects contextual pressure to regulate one’s behavior.
research. Our results reinforce OCB as volitional even when such dis-
cretionary behavior occurs in a context of high accountability.
Theoretical Implications This suggests that the environmental pressures concomitant
with accountability may more generally extend to expecta-
It is important to recognize that our studies are squarely fo- tions of an account-giving for any work behavior rather than
cused on the obligation to reciprocate (i.e., paying you back) specifically linked to OCB. Another plausible interpretation is
rather than what has been termed expected reciprocity or that this pattern shows feelings of accountability may be acti-
“paying me forward” (Korsgaard et al., 2010). Expected rec- vated by feelings of obligation. Such a supposition is consis-
iprocity describes when one contributes to an exchange tent with Hall et al. (2009) who noted that a phenomenological
J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281 275

framework of felt accountability is based on how people per- very little attention has been given to the role played by inter-
ceive and enact accountabilities, which includes: personal proficiency in previous OCB research, despite the
fact that this behavior entails a social process rich in complex
“…not only formal aspects of their jobs (what is found social exchanges. The evidence we find coupled with other
in their written job descriptions) but also informal as- OCB research (e.g., Ellington et al., 2014; Morgeson et al.,
pects of their work (what is not officially required but 2005) points to the value of incorporating a skill-based focus
what employees think that they need to do, given the on personal attributes to augment the heavy personality trait–
norms in the workplace)” (p. 383). based focus of prior OCB and social exchange theory schol-
arship (Chiaburu et al., 2011; Cropanzano & Mitchell, 2005).
Accountability, therefore, is more likely to be felt when The nuanced exploratory results we find showing that inter-
individuals perceive that the norm of reciprocity is in play personal skill, accountability, and reciprocity intertwine to
and, in the absence of such obligatory feelings, individuals predict OCB further highlight the importance of simulta-
may ignore or experience less accountability. Thus, previous neously attending to individual attributes and the contexts in
equivocal findings of both positive and negative relationships which individuals work, a point long echoed by an interac-
between accountability and OCB may be due in part to the fact tional perspective (see Terborg, 1981).
that an essential third variable (i.e., reciprocity obligations)
has not been directly measured. Implications for Practice
While certain task contexts, such as those high in account-
ability, increase the salience of social exchanges, individuals Our results point to several managerial implications for those
differ in how they tend to react to these environmental norms. interested in increasing OCB. Managerial interventions that
We proposed that proactive personality is one such individual foster prosocial norms of reciprocity, communicate the value
difference because it reflects the extent to which people seek of prosocial behavior, equip individuals with the skills needed
control, search the environment for opportunities, and take to recognize and perform OCB, and enhance personal ac-
action to affect change in their work situations. The significant countability are suggested by our findings. For example, or-
interaction between proactive personality and reciprocity ob- ganizational socialization tactics could be used to enhance
ligations supports this conjecture and is consistent with the prosocial values over self-interest (Van Maanen & Schein,
supposition that those high in proactive personality are more 1979) and such tactics have been found in meta-analytic re-
attuned to social cues in their work situations (Ashford & search to promote role clarity (Bauer et al., 2007). Increasing
Black, 1996). Interestingly, our findings also show that pro- role clarity serves to reduce the ambiguity around desired job
active personality seems to propel individuals toward OCB behavior, and ambiguity has been put forth as an important
across both high and low obligation conditions (see Fig. 2). situational antecedent to proactive and discretionary actions
Thus, while it is clear that high obligation results in more OCB (Grant & Ashford, 2008). In addition, work design efforts that
overall and that this effect is amplified by proactivity, it also seek to imbue jobs with relational characteristics such as task
appears that individuals with high proactive personality seem significance (Grant, 2007) can increase social connectedness
to be less hampered by whether or not others have offered and feelings of the importance of one’s work, both of which
citizenship. That is, congruent with the notion that proactivity enhance prosocial motivation and behavior (Grant, 2008).
is indicated by “anticipatory actions” and a “do what it takes” Because skills are malleable attributes (Lievens & Sackett,
disposition (Grant & Ashford, 2008), our results show that 2012), our results also suggest that training interventions that
proactive individuals tend to engage in OCB even in the ab- inculcate interpersonal competence in the form of interperson-
sence of strong norms of reciprocity (albeit at lower levels al skills are likely to foster OCB and build the capacity to
than when reciprocity obligations are high). Such a conclusion recognize social cues that underlie norms of reciprocity.
is further extended by the lack of evidence we found for a Results further suggest that creating feelings of account-
three-way interaction among accountability, proactive ability through explicit performance management techniques
personality, and reciprocity obligations, suggesting that (e.g., results-based goals, electronic performance monitoring)
proactive individuals may often engage in OCB irrespective without regard to social norms may not successfully facilitate
of situational constraints. Overall, the evidence we accumulate OCB. In fact, some research finds that increasing accountabil-
regarding the role of proactivity extends Korsgaard et al. ity produces diminishing returns on OCB (Hall & Ferris,
(2010) by suggesting that obligations to reciprocate may be 2011). Put differently, managers would be well advised to
amplified by other dispositions beyond a strictly prosocial trait use techniques that enhance feelings of felt obligations
such as the concern for others. through interventions such team charters, which establish ex-
Finally, our results from study 2 pertaining to the influence plicit social norms of cooperation and collaboration (Mathieu
of interpersonal skill offer unique contributions to both social & Rapp, 2009). Without fostering mutual feelings of obliga-
exchange theory and OCB scholarship. As we noted earlier, tion and norms of reciprocity, the benefits of accountability
276 J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281

we find may not extend to performance effectiveness. Finally, appropriately assessed broad dimensions of interpersonal skill
when implementing any intervention to boost OCB, managers (communication and relationship-building skills), our predic-
must be aware of the potential trade-off that exists for overall tion relied on theoretical suppositions regarding socially rich
performance effectiveness, where several studies have shown interactions that require a heavy dose of social perceptiveness
that too much OCB can detract from task proficiency and social aptitude and could require additional measurement
(Ellington et al., 2014; Rapp et al., 2013; Rubin et al., 2013). to assess in full.
In examining individual differences, we focused on proac-
Limitations and Future Directions tive personality and interpersonal skill. Proactive personality
entails behavioral tendencies to show personal initiative to-
We focused on the obligation to reciprocate in conceptualizing ward one’s work situation in general, not just in a prosocial
the norm of reciprocity and how it operates on decisions to manner. Research has found personality traits related to
perform OCB. As noted earlier, social exchange theory also prosocial tendencies are associated with increased obligations
posits another type of regulatory expectation in the form of to reciprocate with OCB (e.g., other orientation; Korsgaard
expected reciprocity. Unlike the obligation to reciprocate et al., 2010), as well as increased performance of interpersonal
(paying you back), this expectation of “paying me forward” behavior (e.g., psychological collectivism; Dierdorff et al.,
has been proposed to involve different motivational states and 2011; Jackson et al., 2006). Future studies could examine
modes of reasoning that are more rational and self-interested how proactivity and these kinds of prosocial traits interrelate
in nature (Meglino & Korsgaard, 2006). Thus, whether the and incrementally predict feelings of obligation and OCB.
moderation effects we find for accountability, interpersonal Along these lines, our evidence for interpersonal skill suggests
skill, and proactive personality generalize to this type of reg- that attributes that capture a person’s capability to accurately
ulatory expectation is an issue for future research. We specu- perceive social information are valuable to explore. For exam-
late that such moderation is likely to generalize, as account- ple, cognitive complexity, which reflects the ability to differ-
ability heightens the visibility of social exchanges regardless entiate social information in a multidimensional manner
of the direction of reciprocity, proactive personality reflects (Bieri, 1955), seems particularly salient to recognizing norms
tendencies to act strategically and preemptively (i.e., rational- of reciprocity and the variety of social exchanges that pervade
ly) to ensure future benefits for oneself, and interpersonal skill the workplace.
indicates levels of individual proficiency to navigate the social Our focus was restricted to a specific facet of work context,
context within which one works. which is more proximal to a person’s work role than other
Another boundary of our research stems from our focus on broader facets that depict organizational characteristics. It
reciprocity obligations specifically connected to one’s co- may be that certain organizational cultures shape the effects
workers. While relationships with one’s coworkers are argu- we found for reciprocity obligations, proactive personality,
ably among the most proximal social aspects of work, other interpersonal skill, and accountability. For instance, cultures
sources of reciprocity obligation are possible. For example, emphasizing cooperation, teamwork, service orientation, in-
LMX research has found the quality of relationships with novation, or personal responsibility would seem especially
one’s supervisor is positively related to OCB (Ilies et al., relevant in this regard. Indeed, recent evidence reveals that
2007) and thus, future research could examine if the effects more “constructive configurations” of organizational values
we find similarly manifest when obligations are linked to (high levels of human relations, innovation, and market focus,
one’s supervisor. There are other features beyond reciprocity with a moderate level of bureaucracy) promote individual
obligations that can describe the nature of interpersonal inter- OCB through affective commitment and psychological em-
actions with one’s coworkers. For example, interdependence powerment (Marinova et al., 2019). Thus, future work that
reflects the degree to which one’s task performance is recip- integrates organization-level factors with individual reciproc-
rocally contingent on the performance of others (Dierdorff & ity and OCB seems warranted.
Morgeson, 2007) and is positively related to OCB (Bachrach Finally, though we conducted an experimental study to
et al., 2006). Research has also shown that the overall extent to promote causal interpretations and used vignettes that alluded
which coworkers engage in OCB in general shapes whether or to a “history” of reciprocity (or lack thereof), this study is best
not individuals perform OCB themselves (i.e., “OCB norms”; characterized as emphasizing a single exchange event. Our
Bommer et al., 2007). Such evidence points to the need for field study took a broad approach to capture the general levels
future research that integrates different sources of reciprocity of reciprocity obligations in the workplace. Although exam-
obligation as well as the varying nature of interpersonal inter- ining single exchange events is a concern for a large propor-
actions in which social exchanges take place. Similarly, our tion of research using social exchange theory, it is important to
measure of interpersonal skill drawn from a multisource in- recognize that social exchanges develop over time, and thus
strument may have been insufficient to fully capture the exert cyclical effects where relational benefits can be a result
breadth of interpersonal skill dimensions. Although the items or a resource for any given exchange. Our results should be
J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281 277

considered in light of this limitation. Future longitudinal re- Equal Obligation Condition Tom has been at the company for
search is needed to understand how individual differences and about as long as you and has been working in your department
work contexts impinge upon exchange relationships and for about 2 years. You have had the chance to work with him
OCB. and other coworkers on several different projects during this
Although social exchange theory is widely invoked to ex- time. On these projects, it was a regular occurrence for Tom
plain various forms of prosocial behavior, empirical examina- and you to help each other on task assignments.
tions of the effects of reciprocity obligations on the perfor- You are now working once again with Tom and a few other
mance of OCB and the boundary conditions of these influ- coworkers on another project. Although everyone is working
ences are strikingly sparse. Not only do our results strongly on the same project, tasks are individually assigned to each
affirm the role that reciprocity plays in prompting OCB and person and are independently completed. This means that you
corroborate prior correlational studies, they extend this extant can finish your assignments on your own without waiting for
literature by explicating the situational and individual condi- input or help from others.
tions under which this foundational effect operates. The current project is about halfway through and you are
diligently working on your assignments. You notice that Tom
seems to be struggling to complete his project assignments.
Appendix 1. Study 1 Experimental Vignettes
(Sample 1) Appendix 2. Study 2 Experimental Vignettes
(Sample 2)
High Obligation Condition Tom has been at the company for
about as long as you and has been working in your department
for about 2 years. You have had the chance to work with him High Obligation, High Accountability Condition Avery has
and other coworkers on several different projects during this worked at the company for about as long as you and has been
time. On these projects, it was a regular occurrence for Tom to working in your department for about 2 years. You have had
help you out with tasks that were specifically assigned to you. the chance to work with Avery and other coworkers on several
In fact, it is safe to say that he has helped you more often than different projects during this time. On these projects, it was a
you have helped him. regular occurrence for Avery to help you out with tasks that
You are now working once again with Tom and a few other were specifically assigned to you. In fact, it is safe to say that
coworkers on another project. Although everyone is working Avery has helped you more often than you have helped
on the same project, tasks are individually assigned to each Avery.
person and are independently completed. This means that you You are now working once again with Avery and a few
can finish your assignments on your own without waiting for other coworkers on another project. Although everyone is
input or help from others. working on the same project, tasks are individually assigned
The current project is about halfway through and you are to each person and are independently completed. This means
diligently working on your assignments. You notice that Tom that you can finish your assignments on your own without
seems to be struggling to complete his project assignments. waiting for input or help from others.
Similar to previous projects, your department will again
Low Obligation Condition Tom has been at the company for follow a project management process that requires clear plan-
about as long as you and has been working in your department ning, execution, and evaluation of all task assignments. This
for about 2 years. You have had the chance to work with him means that the success and failures of your actions and deci-
and other coworkers on several different projects during this sions are highly visible to your boss and coworkers. Your boss
time. On these projects, it was a regular occurrence for you to relies on this information to evaluate your performance and
help Tom on tasks that were specifically assigned to him. In determine outcomes such as raises, bonuses, and promotions.
fact, it is safe to say that you have helped Tom more often than The current project is about halfway through and you are
he has helped you. diligently working on your assignments. You notice that
You are now working once again with Tom and a few other Avery seems to be struggling to complete assigned tasks.
coworkers on another project. Although everyone is working
on the same project, tasks are individually assigned to each High Obligation, Low Accountability Condition Avery has
person and are independently completed. This means that you worked at the company for about as long as you and has been
can finish your assignments on your own without waiting for working in your department for about 2 years. You have had
input or help from others. the chance to work with Avery and other coworkers on several
The current project is about halfway through and you are different projects during this time. On these projects, it was a
diligently working on your assignments. You notice that Tom regular occurrence for Avery to help you out with tasks that
seems to be struggling to complete his project assignments. were specifically assigned to you. In fact, it is safe to say that
278 J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281

Avery has helped you more often than you have helped to each person and are independently completed. This means
Avery. that you can finish your assignments on your own without
You are now working once again with Avery and a few waiting for input or help from others.
other coworkers on another project. Although everyone is Similar to previous projects, your department will again
working on the same project, tasks are individually assigned follow a project management process that allows individuals
to each person and are independently completed. This means to accomplish their own tasks as they see fit. This means that
that you can finish your assignments on your own without project work is very self-directed with little oversight or eval-
waiting for input or help from others. uation of your actions by your boss and coworkers.
Similar to previous projects, your department will again The current project is about halfway through and you are
follow a project management process that allows individuals diligently working on your assignments. You notice that
to accomplish their own tasks as they see fit. This means that Avery seems to be struggling to complete assigned tasks.
project work is very self-directed with little oversight or eval-
uation of your actions by your boss and coworkers.
The current project is about halfway through and you are References
diligently working on your assignments. You notice that
Avery seems to be struggling to complete assigned tasks. Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Advances in experimental and social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 267–
299). Academic Press.
Low Obligation, High Accountability Condition Avery has Aguinis, H., & Bradley, K. J. (2014). Best practice recommendations for
worked at the company for about as long as you and has been designing and implementing experimental vignette methodology
working in your department for about 2 years. You have had studies. Organizational Research Methods, 17(4), 351–371.
Ashford, S. J., & Black, J. S. (1996). Proactivity during organizational
the chance to work with Avery and other coworkers on several
entry: The role of desire for control. Journal of Applied Psychology,
different projects during this time. On these projects, it was a 81(2), 199–214.
regular occurrence for you to help complete tasks that were Ashford, S. J., & Cummings, L. L. (1985). Proactive feedback seeking:
specifically assigned to Avery. In fact, it is safe to say that you The instrumental use of the information environment. Journal of
have helped Avery more often than Avery has helped you. Occupational Psychology, 58(1), 67–79.
Bachrach, D. G., Powell, B. C., Bendoly, E., & Richey, R. G. (2006).
You are now working once again with Avery and a few Organizational citizenship behavior and performance evaluations:
other coworkers on another project. Although everyone is Exploring the impact of task interdependence. Journal of Applied
working on the same project, tasks are individually assigned Psychology, 91(1), 193–201.
to each person and are independently completed. This means Bateman, T. S., & Crant, J. M. (1993). The proactive component of
organizational behavior: A measure and correlates. Journal of
that you can finish your assignments on your own without Organizational Behavior, 14(2), 103–118.
waiting for input or help from others. Bauer, T. N., Bodner, T., Erdogan, B., Truxillo, D. M., & Tucker, J. S.
Similar to previous projects, your department will again (2007). Newcomer adjustment during organizational socialization:
follow a project management process that requires clear plan- A meta-analytic review of antecedents, outcomes, and methods.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(3), 707–721.
ning, execution, and evaluation of all task assignments. This Bergeron, D. M., Schroeder, T. D., & Martinez, H. A. (2014). Proactive
means that the success and failures of your actions and deci- personality at work: Seeing more to do and doing more? Journal of
sions are highly visible to your boss and coworkers. Your boss Business and Psychology, 29(1), 71–86.
relies on this information to evaluate your performance and Biddle, B. J. (1979). Role theory: Expectation, identities, and behaviors.
Academic Press.
determine outcomes such as raises, bonuses, and promotions.
Bieri, J. (1955). Cognitive complexity-simplicity and predictive behavior.
The current project is about halfway through and you are The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51(2), 263–268.
diligently working on your assignments. You notice that Blau, P. (1964). Exchange and power in social life. Wiley.
Avery seems to be struggling to complete assigned tasks. Bolino, M. C. (1999). Citizenship and impression management: Good
soldiers or good actors? Academy of Management Review, 24(1),
82–98.
Low Obligation, Low Accountability Condition Avery has Bolino, M. C., & Grant, A. M. (2016). The bright side of being prosocial
worked at the company for about as long as you and has been at work, and the dark side, too: A review and agenda for research on
working in your department for about 2 years. You have had other-oriented motives, behavior, and impact in organizations.
the chance to work with Avery and other coworkers on several Academy of Management Annals, 10, 599–670.
Bommer, W. H., Dierdorff, E. C., & Rubin, R. S. (2007). Does prevalence
different projects during this time. On these projects, it was a mitigate relevance? The moderating effect of group level OCB on
regular occurrence for you to help complete tasks that were employee performance. Academy of Management Journal, 50,
specifically assigned to Avery. In fact, it is safe to say that you 1481–1494.
have helped Avery more often than Avery has helped you. Bowling, N. A., Beehr, T. A., & Swader, W. M. (2005). Giving and
receiving social support at work: The roles of personality and reci-
You are now working once again with Avery and a few procity. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 67(3), 476–489.
other coworkers on another project. Although everyone is Chiaburu, D. S., Oh, I. S., Berry, C. M., Li, N., & Gardner, R. G. (2011).
working on the same project, tasks are individually assigned The five-factor model of personality traits and organizational
J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281 279

citizenship behaviors: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Grant, A. M., & Mayer, D. M. (2009). Good soldiers and good actors:
Psychology, 96(6), 1140–1166. prosocial and impression management motives as interactive predic-
Claes, R., Beheydt, C., & Lemmens, B. (2005). Unidimensionality of tors of affiliative citizenship behaviors. Journal of Applied
abbreviated proactive personality scales across cultures. Applied Psychology, 94(4), 900.
Psychology, 54(4), 476–489. Grant, A. M., Parker, S. K., & Collins, C. G. (2009). Getting credit for
Clark, K., & Sefton, M. (2001). The sequential prisoner’s dilemma: proactive behavior: Supervisor reactions depend on what you value
Evidence on reciprocation. The Economic Journal, 111(468), 51– and how you feel. Personnel Psychology, 62, 31–55.
68. Greguras, G. J., & Diefendorff, J. M. (2010). Why does proactive per-
Coleman, V. I., & Borman, W. C. (2000). Investigating the underlying sonality predict employee life satisfaction and work behaviors? A
structure of the citizenship performance domain. Human Resource field investigation of the mediating role of the self-concordance
Management Review, 10, 25–44. model. Personnel Psychology, 63(3), 539–560.
Crant, J. M. (2000). Proactive behavior in organizations. Journal of Halbesleben, J. R., & Wheeler, A. R. (2011). I owe you one: Coworker
Management, 26(3), 435–462. reciprocity as a moderator of the day-level exhaustion–performance
Cropanzano, R., & Mitchell, M. S. (2005). Social exchange theory: An relationship. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32(4), 608–626.
interdisciplinary review. Journal of Management, 31(6), 874–900. Hall, A. T., & Ferris, G. R. (2011). Accountability and extra-role behav-
Deckop, J. R., Cirka, C. C., & Andersson, L. M. (2003). Doing unto ior. Employee Responsibilities and Rights Journal, 23(2), 131–144.
others: The reciprocity of helping behavior in organizations. Hall, A. T., Royle, M. T., Brymer, R. A., Perrewé, P. L., Ferris, G. R., &
Journal of Business Ethics, 47(2), 101–113. Hochwarter, W. A. (2006). Relationship between felt accountability
Dienesch, R. M., & Liden, R. C. (1986). Leader–member exchange mod- as a stressor and strain reactions: The neutralizing role of autonomy
el of leadership: A critique and further development. Academy of across two studies. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 11,
Management Review, 11, 618–634. 87–99.
Dierdorff, E. C., & Morgeson, F. P. (2007). Consensus in work role Hall, A. T., Zinko, R., Perryman, A. A., & Ferris, G. R. (2009).
requirements: The influence of discrete occupational context on role Organizational citizenship behavior and reputation mediators in
expectations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92(5), 1228–1241. the relationships between accountability and job performance and
Dierdorff, E. C., Bell, S. T., & Belohlav, J. A. (2011). The power of “we”: satisfaction. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies, 15(4),
Effects of psychological collectivism on team performance over 381–392.
time. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(2), 247–262. Hall, A. T., Frink, D. D., & Buckley, M. R. (2017). An accountability
Dierdorff, E. C., Rubin, R. S., & Bachrach, D. G. (2012). Role expecta- account: A review and synthesis of the theoretical and empirical
tions as antecedents of citizenship and the moderating effects of research on felt accountability. Journal of Organizational
work context. Journal of Management, 38(2), 573–598. Behavior, 38, 204–224.
Dudley, N. M., & Cortina, J. M. (2008). Knowledge and skills that facil- Hattrup, K., & Jackson, S. E. (1996). Learning about individual differ-
itate the personal support dimension of citizenship. Journal of ences by taking situations seriously. In Individual Differences and
Applied Psychology, 93(6), 1249–1270. Behavior in Organizations (pp. 507–547). Jossey-Bass.
Eisenberger, R., Armeli, S., Rexwinkel, B., Lynch, P. D., & Rhoades, L. Hochwarter, W. A., Kacmar, C., & Ferris, G. R. (2003). Accountability at
(2001). Reciprocation of perceived organizational support. Journal work: An examination of antecedents and consequences. Annual
of Applied Psychology, 86(1), 42–51. Meeting of the Society of Industrial and Organizational Psychology.
Ellington, J. K., Dierdorff, E. C., & Rubin, R. S. (2014). Decelerating the Ilies, R., Nahrgang, J. D., & Morgeson, F. P. (2007). Leader-member
diminishing returns of citizenship on task performance: The role of exchange and citizenship behaviors: A meta-analysis. Journal of
social context and interpersonal skill. Journal of Applied Applied Psychology, 92(1), 269–277.
Psychology, 99(4), 748–758. Jackson, C. L., Colquitt, J. A., Wesson, M. J., & Zapata-Phelan, C. P.
Emerson, R. M. (1976). Social exchange theory. Annual Review of (2006). Psychological collectivism: A measurement validation and
Sociology, 2, 335–362. linkage to group member performance. Journal of Applied
Farh, J. L., Podsakoff, P. M., & Organ, D. W. (1990). Accounting for Psychology, 91(4), 884–899.
organizational citizenship behavior: Leader fairness and task scope Johns, G. (2006). The essential impact of context on organizational be-
versus satisfaction. Journal of Management, 16, 705–721. havior. Academy of Management Review, 31, 386–408.
Ferris, G. R., Witt, L. A., & Hochwarter, W. A. (2001). Interaction of Katz, D., & Kahn, R. (1978). The Social Psychology of Organizations
social skill and general mental ability on job performance and salary. (2nd ed.). John Wiley & Sons.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 86(6), 1075–1082. Klein, C., DeRouin, R. E., & Salas, E. (2006). Uncovering workplace
Frink, D. D., & Klimoski, R. J. (1998). Toward a theory of accountability interpersonal skills: A review, framework, and research agenda. In
in organizations and human resources management. In G. R. Ferris G. P. Hodgkinson & J. K. Ford (Eds.), International review of in-
(Ed.), Research in Personnel and Human Resources Management dustrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 80–126).
(Vol. 16, pp. 1–51). JAI Press. Wiley & Sons, Ltd..
Fuller, B., & Marler, L. E. (2009). Change driven by nature: A meta- Korsgaard, M. A., Meglino, B. M., Lester, S. W., & Jeong, S. S. (2010).
analytic review of the proactive personality literature. Journal of Paying you back or paying me forward: Understanding rewarded
Vocational Behavior, 75(3), 329–345. and unrewarded organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of
Gouldner, A. W. (1960). The norm of reciprocity: A preliminary state- Applied Psychology, 95(2), 277–290.
ment. American Sociological Review, 25, 161–178. LeBreton, J. M., & Senter, J. L. (2008). Answers to 20 questions about
Grant, A. M. (2007). Relational job design and the motivation to make a interrater reliability and interrater agreement. Organizational
prosocial difference. Academy of Management Review, 32(2), 393– Research Methods, 11(4), 815–852.
417. LePine, J. A., Erez, A., & Johnson, D. E. (2002). The nature and dimen-
Grant, A. M. (2008). Does intrinsic motivation fuel the prosocial fire? sionality of organizational citizenship behavior: a critical review and
Motivational synergy in predicting persistence, performance, and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(1), 52–65.
productivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(1), 48–58. Lester, S. W., Meglino, B. M., & Korsgaard, M. A. (2008). The role of
Grant, A. M., & Ashford, S. J. (2008). The dynamics of proactivity at other orientation in organizational citizenship behavior. Journal of
work. Research in Organizational Behavior, 28, 3–34. Organizational Behavior, 29(6), 829–841.
280 J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281

Liden, R. C., & Maslyn, J. M. (1998). Multidimensionality of leader– Ozer, M. (2011). A moderated mediation model of the relationship be-
member exchange: An empirical assessment through scale develop- tween organizational citizenship behaviors and job performance.
ment. Journal of Management, 24, 43–72. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(6), 1328–1336.
Lievens, F., & Sackett, P. R. (2012). The validity of interpersonal skills Parker, S. K. (1998). Enhancing role breadth self-efficacy: The roles of
assessment via situational judgment tests for predicting academic job enrichment and other organizational interventions. Journal of
success and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, Applied Psychology, 83, 835–852.
97(2), 460–468. Perugini, M., Gallucci, M., Presaghi, F., & Ercolani, A. P. (2003). The
Lynch, P. D., Eisenberger, R., & Armeli, S. (1999). Perceived organiza- personal norm of reciprocity. European Journal of Personality,
tional support: Inferior versus superior performance by wary em- 17(4), 251–283.
ployees. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84(4), 467–483. Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., & Bommer, W. H. (1996). A meta-
Marinova, S. V., Cao, X., & Park, H. (2019). Constructive organizational analysis of the relationships between Kerr and Jermier’s substitutes
values climate and organizational citizenship behaviors: A configu- for leadership and employee job attitudes, role perceptions, and
rational view. Journal of Management, 45, 2045–2071. performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 380–399.
Masterson, S. S., Lewis, K., Goldman, B. M., & Taylor, M. S. (2000). Podsakoff, P. M., Ahearne, M., & MacKenzie, S. B. (1997).
Integrating justice and social exchange: The differing effects of fair Organizational citizenship behavior and the quantity and quality of
procedures and treatment on work relationships. Academy of work group performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 262–
Management Journal, 43(4), 738–748. 270.
Rand, D. G. (2012). The promise of Mechanical Turk: How online labor
Mathieu, J. E., & Rapp, T. L. (2009). Laying the foundation for successful
markets can help theorists run behavioral experiments. Journal of
team performance trajectories: The roles of team charters and per-
Theoretical Biology, 299, 172–179.
formance strategies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(1), 90–103.
Rapp, A. A., Bachrach, D. G., & Rapp, T. L. (2013). The influence of
Meade, A. W., & Craig, S. B. (2012). Identifying careless responses in time management skill on the curvilinear relationship between or-
survey data. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 437–455. ganizational citizenship behavior and task performance. Journal of
Meglino, B. M., & Korsgaard, A. (2004). Considering rational self- Applied Psychology, 98(4), 668–677.
interest as a disposition: Organizational implications of other orien- Riggio, R. E. (1986). Assessment of basic social skills. Journal of
tation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89(6), 946–959. Personality and Social Psychology, 51(3), 649–660.
Meglino, B. M., & Korsgaard, M. A. (2006). Considering situational and Rioux, S. M., & Penner, L. A. (2001). The causes of organizational
dispositional approaches to rational self-interest: An extension and citizenship behavior: A motivational analysis. Journal of applied
response to De Dreu (2006). Journal of Applied Psychology, 91(6), Psychology, 86(6), 1306–1314.
1253–1259. Rubin, R. S., Dierdorff, E. C., & Bachrach, D. G. (2013). Boundaries of
Meyer, R. D., Dalal, R. S., & Hermida, R. (2010). A review and synthesis citizenship behavior: Curvilinearity and context in the citizenship
of situational strength in the organizational sciences. Journal of and task performance relationship. Personnel Psychology, 66(2),
Management, 36(1), 121–140. 377–406.
Mischel, W. (1977). The interaction of person and situation. In D. Segrin, C., & Taylor, M. (2007). Positive interpersonal relationships me-
Magnusson & N. Endler (Eds.), Personality at the crossroads: diate the association between social skills and psychological well-
Current issues in interactional psychology (pp. 333–352). Erlbaum. being. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(4), 637–646.
Mitchell, T. R., Hopper, H., Daniels, D., Falvy, J., & Ferris, G. R. (1998). Settoon, R. P., & Mossholder, K. W. (2002). Relationship quality and
Power, accountability, and inappropriate actions. Applied relationship context as antecedents of person-and task-focused inter-
Psychology: An International Review, 47, 497–517. personal citizenship behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology,
Morgeson, F. P., & Dierdorff, E. C. (2011). Work analysis: From tech- 87(2), 255–267.
nique to theory. In S. Zedeck (Ed.), APA handbook of industrial and Settoon, R. P., Bennett, N., & Liden, R. C. (1996). Social exchange in
organizational psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 3–41). APA. organizations: Perceived organizational support, leader–member ex-
Morgeson, F. P., Reider, M. H., & Campion, M. A. (2005). Selecting change, and employee reciprocity. Journal of Applied Psychology,
individuals in team settings: The importance of social skills, person- 81(3), 219–227.
ality characteristics, and teamwork knowledge. Personnel Smets, E., Visser, M. R., Oort, F. J., Schaufeli, W. B., & Haes, H. J.
Psychology, 58(3), 583–611. (2004). Perceived inequity: does it explain burnout among medical
Morrison, E. W., & Phelps, C. C. (1999). Taking charge at work: specialists? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(9), 1900–
extrarole efforts to initiate workplace change. Academy of 1918.
Management Journal, 42(4), 403–419. Smith, R. W., Kim, Y. J., & Carter, N. T. (2020). Does it matter where
you’re helpful? Organizational citizenship behavior from work and
Motowildo, S. J., Borman, W. C., & Schmit, M. J. (1997). A theory of
home. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology. Advance online
individual differences in task and contextual performance. Human
publication, 25, 450–468. https://doi.org/10.1037/ocp0000181.
Performance, 10(2), 71–83.
Spitzmuller, M., Sin, H. P., Howe, M., & Fatimah, S. (2015).
Organ, D. W. (1988). Organizational Citizenship Behavior. Lexington
Investigating the uniqueness and usefulness of proactive personality
Books.
in organizational research: A meta-analytic review. Human
Organ, D. W. (1990). The motivational basis of organizational citizenship Performance, 28, 351–379.
behavior. Research in Organizational Behavior, 12(1), 43–72. Terborg, J. R. (1981). Interactional psychology and research on human
Organ, D. W. (1997). Organizational citizenship behavior: It’s construct behavior in organizations. Academy of Management Review, 6(4),
clean-up time. Human Performance, 10(2), 85–97. 569–576.
Organ, D. W., Podsakoff, P. M., & MacKenzie, S. B. (2006). Tett, R. P., & Burnett, D. D. (2003). A personality trait-based
Organizational citizenship behavior: Its nature, antecedents, and interactionist model of job performance. Journal of Applied
consequences. Sage. Psychology, 88(3), 500–517.
Ostroff, C., & Schulte, M. (2007). Multiple perspectives of fit in organi- Thomas, J. P., Whitman, D. S., & Viswesvaran, C. (2010). Employee
zations across levels of analysis. In C. Ostroff & T. A. Judge (Eds.), proactivity in organizations: A comparative meta-analysis of emer-
Perspectives on Organizational Fit (pp. 3–70). Lawrence Erlbaum gent proactive constructs. Journal of Occupational and
Associates. Organizational Psychology, 83(2), 275–300.
J Bus Psychol (2022) 37:263–281 281

Van Dierendonck, D., Schaufeli, W. B., & Buunk, B. P. (2001). Burnout Wayne, S. J., Shore, L. M., & Liden, R. C. (1997). Perceived organiza-
and inequity among human service professionals: A longitudinal tional support and leader-member exchange: A social exchange per-
study. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 6(1), 43–52. spective. Academy of Management Journal, 40(1), 82–111.
Van Dyne, L., Graham, J. W., & Dienesch, R. M. (1994). Organizational Zellars, K. L., & Tepper, B. J. (2003). Beyond social exchange: New
citizenship behavior: Construct redefinition, measurement, and val- directions for organizational citizenship behavior theory and re-
idation. Academy of Management Journal, 37(4), 765–802. search. Research in Personnel and Human Resources
Van Dyne, L., Cummings, L. L., & McLean-Parks, J. (1995). Extra-role Management, 22, 395–424.
behaviors: In pursuit of construct and definitional clarity. In L. L.
Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic-
behavior (Vol. 17, pp. 215–285). JAI Press.
tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Van Maanen, J., & Schein, E. H. (1979). Toward a theory of organiza-
tional socialization. In B. M. Staw & L. L. Cummings (Eds.),
Research in organizational behavior (pp. 209–264). JAI Press.

You might also like