You are on page 1of 13

Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Education for Chemical Engineers


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ece

An analysis of engineering students’ risk perception to support process


safety learning process
Rafael Amaya-Gómez a, *, Vivian Dumar b, Mauricio Sánchez-Silva c,
Maria Alejandra Torres-Cuello d, Alba Avila e, Felipe Muñoz f
a
Chemical Engineering Department, Universidad de los Andes, Cra 1E No. 19A-40, Bogotá, Colombia
b
Risk Management, The Bogotá Metro, Cra 9 No. 76-49, Bogotá, Colombia
c
Department of Civil & Environmental Engineering, Universidad de los Andes, Cra 1E No. 19A-40, Bogotá, Colombia
d
Administrative Department of Public Service, Cra 6 No. 12-62, Bogotá, Colombia
e
Electrical and Electronic Engineering Department: Microelectronic Center (CMUA), Universidad de los Andes, Cra 1E No. 19A-40, Bogotá, Colombia
f
Empresa Colombiana de Petróleos (ECOPETROL), Cra 7 No. 32-42, Bogotá, Colombia

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Multidisciplinary teams often undertake engineering projects beyond their original discipline involving different
Engineering kinds of risks. Risk perception is an inherent and embedded part of the decision-making process, which depends
Education on the personal background and instinctive attitudes or behaviors. Process safety and risk analysis training for
Process safety
engineers, and education for engineering students, provide valuable tools seeking safer workplaces; however,
Risk perception
Factor analysis
personnel’s risk perception is commonly neglected. This paper analyzes the risk perception and appetite of
undergraduate engineering students in Colombia based on a survey strategy and a weighted-approach following
a Factor Analysis. The survey considered financial, social, physical, and professional risks, and four main reasons
for risk-taking or risk-avoiding actions. The Factor Analysis allows us to classify the students tendency as risk-
averse o risk-prone, and propose didactic teaching planning using a modular toolbox for process safety educa­
tion, based on skills identified for junior engineers in Colombian Oil & Gas Industry. A total of 465 engineering
students from 12 Colombian universities completed the survey in 2016. The results suggest that risk perception
depends on the location, possible risk training, and accessible information. The obtained factors allow describing
the students’ overall risk profiles, which can serve as an input for refining the content and curriculum content of
current engineering programs regarding process safety. Improving the training of risk management in the en­
gineering curriculum will benefit upcoming multidisciplinary teams in high-risk industries.

1. Introduction and the need for a more inherent safer design. The pesticide plant
leakage in Bhopal (India) in 1985 highlighted the need for dispersion
Process Safety and risk management are in continuous evolution modeling and triggered the creation of the Center Chemical Process
partially because of the existence of more complex technologies, and Safety (CCPS). Other relevant examples include the BP Texas City ex­
emerging risks as those mentioned by the OECD (2003) produced by plosion (U.S.) in 2005, the T2 Laboratory Inc. runaway explosion (U.S.)
systemic changes in demography, environment, technology advances, or in 2007, the Imperial Sugar (U.S) explosion in 2008, the explosion of the
the socio-economic structure. Process safety is also evolving considering Deepwater Horizon offshore platform (Gulf of Mexico) in 2010, and the
the aftermath of relevant process accidents in different industries (Kerin, nuclear disaster of Fukushima (Japan) in 2011. These accidents trig­
2016; Mannan et al., 2016). Some examples include the Caprolactam gered the development of new technologies, new safety barriers, safety
Flixborough explosion (UK, 1974), which evidenced the need for culture, new hazards identification, and alternative risk assessments at
HAZOP analysis and adequate Management of Change (MOC). The different industrial sectors (Mannan et al., 2016).
Seveso dioxin leakage (Italy, 1976) promoted different legislation for In addition, industrial facilities handling hazardous materials are
the control and prevention of similar accidents (SEVESO UE Directive) subjected to unlikely threats but potentially catastrophic consequences,

* Corresponding author.
E-mail address: r.amaya29@uniandes.edu.co (R. Amaya-Gómez).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ece.2022.10.003
Received 29 July 2022; Received in revised form 6 October 2022; Accepted 18 October 2022
Available online 20 October 2022
1749-7728/© 2022 Institution of Chemical Engineers. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
R. Amaya-Gómez et al. Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

as in the case of Natech –Natural Hazard Triggering Technological process safety education, including books (e.g., Chemical Process Safety),
Disasters– events. For instance, climate change may favor the occur­ the Loss Prevention Bulletin (LPB), which provides lessons learned from
rence of failures due to landslides in mountainous regions or floodings in process safety case studies, and dedicated journal papers as in the case of
facilities near the coast (Cruz and Krausmann, 2013; Suarez-Paba et al., the recent Special Issue on Process Safety in Chemical Engineering Edu­
2020; Mesa-Gómez et al., 2020; He et al., 2022). These events imply cation and Training, focusing on the “what”, “how”, and “why” is
additional challenges from the traditional industrial point of view. In required process safety training and education.
this regard, authors like Reniers et al. (2018) proposed the EPIC From an academic education perspective, different alternatives have
framework, which is associated with Natech safety in Chemical Industry been implemented to raise students’ awareness of Process Safety’s
through Education (learning and training), Proactive risk minimization, importance. In this regard, some common teaching strategies in under­
Intensified inspection and analysis, and Cooperation. Process safety graduate programs include standalone courses that can be mandatory or
evolution requires continuous monitoring of hazards and conditions of elective in the curriculum, or by incorporating process safety related
safety barriers and engineering resilience procedures (Kerin, 2016; topics in core (mandatory) courses (Amaya-Gómez et al., 2019). Engi­
Planas et al., 2014; Mannan et al., 2016). Engineers need to be more neering programs usually cover the main Engineering and Physical
prepared for risk assessment and management, considering possible Sciences Research Council (EPSRC) taxonomy, and they follow accred­
catastrophic scenarios and how should be the emergency response. itation requirements and literature advice, which makes the design of
The preparation of engineers has its driving force in training and degree curricula an exercise under several constraints (Campbell and
early education. According to Mkpat et al. (2018), process safety educa­ Belton, 2016). For instance, to increase international competitiveness
tion has its basis in three main paths: (i) an academic route that involves and Research & Innovation leadership, some approaches have been
bachelor, master’s, or even doctoral formation; (ii) a professional route raised such as the Bologna Process (Ministerial Decree No. 509/99)
that includes internships and research from an industrial perspective; (Crosier and Parveva, 2013; Terry, 2007), which establishes a three year
and (iii) professional agencies training from a regulation perspective. Bachelor of Science (BSc), two-year Master of Science (MSc) and three
Gajek et al. (2022) assessed a couple of surveys developed by the EFCE year Doctor of Philosophy (Ph.D.). Therefore, one of the more attractive
Working Party on Loss Prevention and Safety Promotion for active in­ ways to include process safety is at core mandatory subjects such as
dustrial workers in Europe, members of professional associations, col­ Chemical Reaction Engineering or Process Plant Design. For instance,
lege universities, and professional institutions. This work searched for Perrin and Laurent (2008) presents this context in three French Engi­
the main formation for the different stakeholders in Europe in process neering Schools (Ecole Nationale Supérieure), where safety topics are
safety, indicating that the majority of training came from employers, contemplated at introductory laboratory courses in chemistry and
followed by college institutions, and some professional institutions. chemical engineering, and in core courses such as design project and
These results acknowledge that undergraduate process safety education industrial training. This situation is not limited to France, but also in
is critical, considering senior students will be part of different high-risk different countries in Europe have been incorporated process safety
industries, where different skills or attributes are desired for next engi­ topics in regular bachelor and master’s curricula as pointed out by
neers. For instance, the American Society for Engineering Education Kouwenhoven (2021) like in the case of the Politecnico di Milano (Italy)
(ASEE) recognized that a global engineer need to understand concepts in different short compulsory safety courses for those using laboratories.
related to Risk Management and Continuous Improvement. Also, on a Similar examples have also been reported in the United States by Dee
general workplace professional competences, it is desired the ability to et al. (2015) in the Massachusetts Institute of Technology (MIT) and
apply judgment in decision-making processes to manage risks effectively Pintar (1999) in the Michigan Technological University (MTU).
(ASEE, 2015). This overall preparation can be related to the “learning Overall, these strategies aim to inherently include process safety in
pyramid process” (Benintendi, 2016), where at the university tier, the the engineering program by teaching specific introductory related sub­
learning process begins with understanding some basic principles and by jects in several courses, which in turn prevents discrimination with the
applying some corresponding principles. At the professional tier, it is chemical engineering activities (Benintendi, 2016; Pintar, 1999). Some
necessary to use this knowledge in conjunction with field experience to of the topics contemplated in this regard include asset integrity and
support management decisions. reliability, process design, fire and explosion studies, hazard identifi­
In this regard, different resources to process safety education have cation and risk analysis, human factors, incident management, process
been promoted by different stakeholders (Amaya-Gómez et al., 2019; control, Process Safety Management, and Risk decision-making (Mkpat
Mkpat et al., 2018). From a professional perspective, currently there are et al., 2018). However, a suitable learning environment to be applied in
different recognized institutions such as AIChE (American Institute of a broader student population still need to be consolidated. According to
Chemical Engineers), CCPS (Center of Chemical Process Safety), IChemE Kouwenhoven (2021) a scarce amount of time in European programs are
(Institution of Chemical Engineers), ABET (Accreditation Board for En­ devoted for process safety (less than 8.3% in BSc and 12.5% in MSc
gineering and Technology, Inc.), or CSB (Chemical Safety Board) that programs), where the main subjects covered include Risk Identification
provide diverse teaching material for process safety education. For and Risk Classification. These topics are also highlighted by Gajek et al.
instance, SAChE (Safety and Chemical Engineering Education) program (2022) in the rating of importance among educators and industry with
is a collaborative effort between CCPS and engineering schools that elements associated with return of experience from past accidents,
distribute teaching materials and online courses for students, professors, human factors, SIS, HAZOP, and Fires and Explosions. There is another
and recent professionals to assure safer operations. CCPS develops topic apart from risk estimates and management that is relevant in
different engineers training courses as the widely used Process Safety high-risk industries: the personnel Risk Perception. Although it is asso­
Boot Camp intensive 4-day training that covers areas such as Risk-based ciated with hazards perception and their threats feelings, several works
Process Safety, Process Hazard Analysis, among others. The Chemical have discussed its importance towards safety attitudes and behaviors
Safety Board (CSB) promotes detailed lessons learned from root-case (Mearns and Flin, 1995; Rundmo, 1996; Arezes and Miguel, 2008).
analysis in previous incidents, which help identify safety barriers, Mearns and Flin (1995) describe Risk Perception as the study of beliefs,
analyze their failure rates, and determine the accident sequence. attitudes, judgments, and felling a person has regarding hazards and
IChemE has a variety of interesting resources that can be used for the risk-taking within a wider social and cultural context. Certainly, biased
education of process safety. Some of them contemplate training courses risk perceptions can cause misjudgments of potentially-hazardous
both online and on-site that cover main elements such as hazard iden­ sources. These misjudgments may represent risky behaviors, lack of
tification, HazOp, LOPA, consequence modeling, emergency planning, adequate actions towards the risk source, inappropriate safety measure
dust explosion, risk assessment, and process safety awareness. Also, decisions, common occupational accidents, and catastrophes (Rundmo,
IChemE offers plenty of publications that can be used in advance for 1996). Nevertheless, Gajek et al. (2022) surveys’ results indicate that

8
R. Amaya-Gómez et al. Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

only 12% of the respondents recognized this topic in their engineering construction and the other from the perception of those involved in
curriculum, which is also reflected in the lower rating importance for particular situations. An official definition of risk is “a measure of the
educators in process safety related topics, with a better rating only for probability and severity of adverse effects” (Lowrance, 1976). In other
permits to work, mechanical integrity and safety legislation. words, the risk is a calculation of how likely an incident is to occur and
In addition to personnel in high-risk facilities like offshore platforms given its occurrence, how severe the consequences would be, consid­
(Mearns and Flin, 1995), researchers have also focused on how students ering the uncertainty of an outcome (Campbell Institute, 2014). Based
perceive risks (Hussin and Wang, 2010; Munguia et al., 2016; Carlson, on the above, risk definition is grounded on three components: proba­
2015; Álvarez-Chávez et al., 2019). The objective is to understand their bility, severity, and perception/context.
attitudes and possible behaviors facing dangerous situations, consid­ On the other hand, risk perception is the study of people’s beliefs,
ering available safety measures. This information would help to support attitudes, judgments and feelings about hazards, danger and risk-taking,
a teaching planning that is dependent on the risk appetite. For instance, within a broader context of social and cultural values (Mearns and Flin,
Hussin and Wang (2010) evaluated the industrial safety perception of 1995; Douglas and Wildavsky, 1982; Vlek and Stallen, 1981; Weinstein,
postgraduate students in the University of Aberdeen (Scotland) about 1980). Thus, it is a highly personal decision-making process based on an
four high-risk industries; these are: Oil & Gas, Nuclear, Mining, and individual frame of reference developed over a lifetime (Brown, 2014).
Aviation. The results indicated that students were more influenced by However, ‘risks’ are not perceived but hazards or various features of
the severity of a potential accident than the probability of the event. decision problems, which lead to feelings of danger or safety (Mearns
Therefore, the nuclear and mining industries were perceived as less safe and Flin, 1995). According to the Campbell Institute (2014), there are
than the Oil & Gas and aviation, although students recognized that ex­ macro, intermediate, and micro factors that affect risk perception and
plosions and toxic gas leakages are likely accident events in Oil & Gas risk tolerance alike.
facilities. Besides, researchers from the University of Sonora in Mexico Macro factors are related to the structure or institutional character­
have assessed college students’ risk perception of hazards in chemistry istics of an organization, such as those issues related to the culture of
laboratories (Munguia et al., 2016; Álvarez-Chávez et al., 2019). For this safety and the level of safety leadership within an organization or
purpose, a Workers’ Risk Perception Dimensional Evaluation (EDRP-T) has community, given that these factors affect risk tolerance and perception
been implemented to characterize the students’ risk perception in of its members. For example, when management demonstrates a
chemistry and biology laboratories. Furthermore, Carlson (2015) has commitment to safety, employees’ perception of the safety management
studied the risk perception in 14 different classes of undergraduate system is positively influenced, resulting in less risk-taking behavior and
students at the North Carolina State University regarding toxic risk reduced injury rates (O’Toole, 2002). Another example of the organi­
perception. According to the author “the educational treatment was zational characteristics influencing risk perception is related to the or­
indicated as effective for changing risk perception for select activities ganization’s response to the occurrence of a given event. If an
related to toxicological hazards” (Carlson, 2015, pp. i). Based on the organization member is aware of the possible consequences or punish­
students’ risk perception, a teaching planning that focuses on risk ments for someone’s unsafe behavior, the likelihood of this member
analysis and risk evaluation could help the students’ formation in pro­ falling in unsafe behaviors will be lower. In addition to trusting that
cess safety and would enhance required abilities for junior engineers; for there will be consequences for behaving unsafely, workers need to have
instance, considering Risk Assessment Matrix (RAM) and probabilistic faith in the credibility of those communicating safety messages to take
sensitization. proper precautions when working (Fischhoff, 1995; Weyman and Kelly,
Risk perception has an important cultural component that poses 1999).
additional challenges in emerging countries like Colombia, where reli­ The intermediate factors are related to the community (peers) in
gious beliefs, social and economic conditions may drastically affect which an individual is involved. For that reason, it includes peer or
students’ attitudes and learning processes. This work seeks to assess community pressure elements, in which stress from peers both within
students’ risk perception based on a survey analysis. This survey con­ and outside the workplace can cause people to take risks that go against
siders financial, social, physical, and professional risks and different their better judgment. For example, a new individual in an organization
reasons why a student would undertake or avoid those risks. A Factor may take unnecessary risks or fall in unappropriated behavior seeking
Analysis taking into account a Generalized Degrees of Freedom (GDF) for peer acceptance. Finally, the micro factors are those related to the
was implemented to describe the student’s risk profile. These profiles individual’s knowledge regarding a situation. Those who are less
are used to support didactic teaching strategies in four topics of interest informed of a situation are less likely to take risks, while those with more
(i) Process Safety basic principles, (ii) introduction of Mechanical knowledge are more likely to have higher levels of risk tolerance
Integrity, (iii) Process Hazard Analysis techniques, and (iv) runaway (Campbell Institute, 2014).
reactions. These topics are considered by Amaya-Gómez et al. (2019) The Risk Management process can easily break down if diverse risk
based on the skills required for junior engineers from the Colombian Oil perceptions among team members are not recognized, considering their
Company (ECOPETROL). In this paper, we present our approach for different risk tolerance and acceptable levels. This situation turns more
Process Safety education and the results from the surveys from 465 complicated, considering how risk is perceived by stakeholders affected
students from 12 universities in Colombia. by risk management decisions, including company employees, share­
This paper is organized as follows: Section 2 describes what risk is holders, regulators, community neighbors, and the general public
and how it is perceived. Section 3 presents our proposed learning (Amyotte and McCutcheon, 2006). Considering the factors mentioned
approach based on an evaluation of students’ risk perception and above, one can differentiate the understanding of risk perception from
appetite; Section 4 presents the case study in Colombia. Section 5 con­ two separate points of view; one coming from an expert and the other
tains the results from the risk perception of the engineering students and coming from the public. Experts try to base their risk perceptions on
proposes the learning approach. Finally, Section 6 contains some research findings and statistical evidence (Paek and Hove, 2017). When
concluding remarks. experts judge risk, their response higlhly correlates with technical esti­
mates of annual fatalities or losses (Slovic, 1987). In other words, ‘ex­
2. What is risk and how it is perceived? perts’ often assess risk as to the expected value of the negative outcomes
(the consequences) of a decision (Weisenfeld and Ott, 2011).
To develop a curriculum for engineering students regarding process Conversely, the general public judgment of “risk” is related to subjective
safety, one must depart from the definition of two key concepts; risk and perceptions, intuitive judgments, and inferences made from media
risk perception. Although they may be used indistinctly in practice, they coverage and limited information (Paek and Hove, 2017), so they tend
constitute two different terms, one of which departs from a theoretical to differ from experts’ risk estimates (Slovic, 1987).

9
R. Amaya-Gómez et al. Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

However, experts and the general public share a component in which 3.1. Surveys structure and design process
several largely unconscious emotional processes shape risk perception.
The human brain is hard-wired to react quickly and defensively to A review of psychometrics and risk profiles was developed to
perceived threats (LeDoux, 2012). This situation includes physical establish the measurable parameter of this survey (i.e., the risk
threats, sights, sounds, smells, and even words or memories associated perception or appetite) to determine factors that significantly affect the
with fear or danger. Another largely unconscious process is the use of risk perception of a Colombian engineering student. For instance Slovic
mental shortcuts to quickly make sense of partial information (Tversky (1987), exposed a factor-analytic representation based on 81 hazards
and Kahneman, 1974). Third, different characteristics of a threat carry regarding a Dread and Unknown Risks to support the statement that
different weights in terms of how people perceive the risk involved. people’s risk perception and attitudes are determined not only by a sort
Finally, people tend to shape their views, so they match those in the statistics (e.g., expected number of fatalities, death probability or life
groups they most closely relate to, which is a concept known as cultural expectancy reduction), but also by different qualitative and quantitative
cognition (Kahan et al., 2011). characteristics. Moreover, Leiserowitz (2003) developed an in-depth
A misjudgment of risk may lead to inappropriate decisions, and un­ examination of the political and socio-cultural dimensions of public,
safe behaviors or human errors given that risk perception is a critical global warming risk perceptions, policy preferences, and individual
antecedent of at-risk behavior (Deloitte Touche Tohmatsu, 2017). Biases behaviors, using standard analyses of political identification and ideol­
in risk perception can cause misinterpretations of potential risk sources. ogy, and Cultural Theory-based approaches. It implemented an affective
When risks are misinterpreted, workers may undertake inappropriate image analysis to investigate the role of connotative meaning in risk
risks (Arezes and Miguel, 2008). A cognitive bias corresponds to the perception, decision making, and risk behavior, highlighting dominant
systematic error in judgment and decision-making common to all human emotional and rational factors. Indeed, risk perception is unique for
beings, which can emerge due to cognitive limitations, motivational every individual, and it depends on a set of psychological, physical, and
factors, or adaptations to natural environments (Wilke and Mata, 2012). social factors, and how they are considered in their mental processes.
Biases are the result of cognitive processes. According to Kahneman Our approach focuses on rational rather than emotional factors
(2011), cognitive processes can be divided into two conceptual systems: considering the difficulty of their measurement.
system one and system two that are used in decision making. System one Recall a decision under risk is a function of the how likely an event is
produces intuitive reactions and automated decisions. It is generated perceived, its consequences, and the psychological propensity of the
without much conscious effort and leads the available information individual to undertake the risk. Therefore, a risk decision-making
through a subconscious pattern recognition based on similar past situ­ process depends on the risk tolerance/acceptable level of the individ­
ations (Hogarth, 2001). System two, corresponds to a deliberate type of ual, how much risk the observer perceived, and the willingness to act at
thinking, involving focus, reason, and analysis. Although both systems that risk level. Interestingly, studies about risk tolerance genetics have
are sources of bias, it is common to find biases emanating from system suggested that from 20% to 63% is hereditary (Roszkowski and Davey,
one. Putting into practice the use of system two by directing 2010). Hereditary susceptibility diseases such as cancer have significant
decision-making processes through logic may help reduce their psychological consequences, although it rarely causes psychiatric
appearance. morbidity (Bratt et al., 2000). However, there are still some un­
Although the use of system two is recommended for reducing the certainties, including risk perception impacts and risk communication
likelihood of biases, it is more likely for a human being to use system interventions regarding genetic counseling (Edwards et al., 2008).
one. Sometimes, it may result in a compulsive decision-making process One of the fundamental elements of this survey was to consider
that might not be the most adequate but the easiest one. According to adventure tourism. According to Dickson and Dolnicar (2004), the
Perlman et al. (2014), the ability to identify hazards or risks is positively tourism industry’s commonly aim to reduce risk perceptions among
correlated with work experience and formal safety training. Someone tourists to be more attractive, but at the same time, there is a sub-sector
who has been trained may ground their decision-making processes on of the tourism industry of adventurers that are actively searching for
the technical knowledge and training they have received regarding risk high perceived risks. As it was quoted from Bernstein ”Nobody takes a
and risk perception. As pointed out by Renn (2008) and Slovic et al. risk in the expectation that it will fail”. Dickson and Dolnicar (2004)
(1980), the biases in risk perception would include: developed a literature review on risk in tourism consumer behavior to
propose an adventure tourism desired risk conceptualization. Moreover,
• Availability – events that people remember immediately are seen as they illustrate a distinction about absolute, real, and perceived risks
more likely. considering safety controls; and expose aspects that influenced risk
• Anchor effect–odds are estimated according to the plausibility of the perceptions.
contextual relations of cause and effect, not on the knowledge of Overall, aspects such as life experiences, genetics, social and cultural
statistical frequencies. surroundings can affect risk perception, developing a unique ”risk pro­
• Representation–there is a difference between personal experiences file”-a set of features that define attitudes and actions. It will be
and experiences that occurred to others. considered that there are reasons related to the individual risk profile to
• Cognitive divergence–unknown information is often ignored or motivate whether or not to undertake a particular risk, as previously
underestimated. reported in fundamental fraud theory (Abdullahi and Mansor, 2015).
This survey aims to measure the risk aversion/propensity of the
3. Proposed approach for learning process safety based on students that are going to receive training from the Process Safety
students’ risk perception modules described in Amaya-Gómez et al. (2019). Overall, this survey
has 54 questions with five possible choices using the following format:
A learning environment involve the complete physical, social, and
pedagogical context in which the learning is intended to occur (i) level of agreement of a set of assertions (12 questions).
(UNESCO, 2012). This concept includes the place of learning (e.g., (ii) frequency of implementation of a set of actions (10 questions).
classroom), pedagogical strategies including unanticipated events, and (iii) level of certainty based on their risk profile (17 questions).
the available resources provided to the learner. This work focuses on the (iv) 10 popular proverbs based on their risk profile.
pedagogical strategies and not all the issues related to the learning en­ (v) 5 simple closed Yes/No questions (used as validation).
vironments. In this regard, an approach for support the teaching plan­
ning based on students’ risk perception that is described below. Notably, each of these questions corresponds to different reasons and
types of risk that students are willing to assume or avoid, which are

10
R. Amaya-Gómez et al. Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

presented diversely to evaluate students’ possible actions due to program, university, and location. This information allowed us to
particular risk exposure. In what follows, the conceptual dimensions of analyze risk perception based on their region and university in ease-
this survey are presented. implementation spreadsheets.
Four categories about risk reasons were defined to support the risk A weighted-approach using a Factor Analysis is proposed to evaluate
profile definition of undertaking or avoiding risky actions: students Risk Perception and Appetite in three phases (Fig. 1). This
factor analysis aims to describe the variance of the survey in few un­
• Preventive: Set of reasons to avoid risk situations of physical observable factor scores, which are linearly related to the questions
integrity, i.e., suffering pain, physical damage, or health deteriora­ results. First, data processing and factors selection are applied based on
tion. These reasons are related to people who prioritize their well- Generalized Degrees of Freedom (GDF). Then a factor evaluation, vali­
being and physical fullness senses. Indeed correspond to people dation, and description are implemented. Finally, students’ risk
who ponder their needs for safety and stability to those of adventure perception are described using these factors to support Learning Plan­
and surprise (based on physical and psychological aspects). They are ning. In what follows, these phases will be illustrated in detail.
individuals whose risk assessment factors are closely linked with the
protection of their surroundings. 3.2.1. Data preparation phase
• Adventurous: Set of reasons to implement risk decisions based on Initially, each survey question is evaluated from a direct/inverse
pleasure-seeking and well-being impressions. They are associated perspective, assigning a higher grade from 1 to 5 (Likert Scales) or 1/5
with taking any possible risk to pursue excitement sensations. Indeed (Yes/No Questions) to those that classify a student as risk prone.
correspond to the individual’s profile that ponders their adventure Moreover, a scaling and centering process is considered based on their
and surprise need rather than safety and stability. Their risk assess­ mean and standard deviation. The selection of the number of factors is
ment factors are closely linked with the satisfaction of their personal implemented using the approach proposed by Chen et al. (2010), based
needs and desires. on unbiased risk estimation and a data perturbation for estimating GDF.
• Inspirational: Set of reasons that lead to undertaking risks for ideals Let Y be the questionnaire results associated with a n × p, where n
purposes. They are related to situations that imply undertaking social represents the number of respondents and p the number of questions,
or physical integrity risks. They correspond to individuals profile and assume each question Yi are independent and identically distrib­
that follows their convictions, beliefs, knowledge, and creativity. uted, therefore the factor model can be described as follows (Chen et al.,
Indeed these individuals ponder their adventure and surprise need 2010):
rather than safety and stability, following their ideals. Their risk
Yi = α + A fi + ϵi (1)
evaluation factors are linked to the satisfaction and desires of the
need of their community. where α is associated with the expected value of Yi, A is a p × q matrix
• Rewarding: Set of conditional reasons that lead to assume or avoid a with the factor loadings, fi is a q-dimensional (q < p) normal distributed
given risk based on the corresponding reward. These reasons are column vector of the factors and ϵi is a p-dimensional error vector of each
associated with individual profiles that prioritize their expected factor (N(0,Γ)). According to Chen et al. (2010), several methods can be
benefits and ponder their needs either of adventure and surprise, or implemented to select the number of factors such as Factors Variance
safety and stability, depending on their greater profits. Their risk Proportion, Scree Test, Guttman-Kaiser (GK) rule, Akaike’s Information
evaluation factors depend almost exclusively on the expected reward Criterion (AIC), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), Bozdogan’s index
by assuming or avoiding a risk action. of Informational Complexity Criterion (ICOMP), or the Likelihood Ratio
Test. However, these methods perform correctly under certain condi­
Notably, any individual may include in their risk profile diverse tions. For instance, GK tends to select more factors for a large number of
types of reasons. For instance, an acrobat justifies not only Adventurous dimensions (questions in this case), LR depends on a significance level,
reasons but also Preventive and Rewarding reasons in order to assure and AIC tends to select more factors than BIC due to a lesser penalty.
their well-being and acrobatics willing to perform. Moreover, consid­ Finally, in this phase, questions with the lowest contribution on the
ering that there are several possible hazardous situations where a risk absolute squared sums loading of all the factors are removed.
perception can be evaluated, we restrict the risk reasons to the following
categories: 3.2.2. Factor evaluation and validation
The factors are evaluated following a Maximum Likelihood
• Professional: Situations where professional reputation is compro­
mised. It includes simple situations such as communication of ac­
quired knowledge to situations of professional ethics exposure.
• Financial: Situations where individuals evidence aversion or prone
taking risks leading to the materialization of financial liquidity or
solvency.
• Physical: Situations where individuals assess the risk of physical
damage (i.e., integrity or well-being).
• Social: Situations where individuals are exposed to risks associated
with their reputation or simply the risk of being mocked.

Based on the above, the survey questions were designed to address


every type of risk and reason equitably.

3.2. Risk perception evaluation and learning planning approach

This survey was delivered to all the engineering program students


who implemented the proposed modules in the spring semester 2016.
The data was collected from a Google Form discrete data survey,
following the characteristics mentioned above. Additionally, the re­
spondents were asked to provide information about their engineering Fig. 1. Proposed approach description.

11
R. Amaya-Gómez et al. Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

perspective as reported by Chen et al. (2010). In this regard, it can be Table 1


̂ = 1∑n Yi and the estimates A
implemented the estimate of α as α ̂ and Γ
̂ Modules learning planning strategies.
n i=1
from the max log-likelihood function (Chen et al., 2010). Case Didactic Strategy

− n[ ( )] -Module 1: Focus on accident consequences (e.g., human, environmental,


l(A, Γ) = plog(2π) + log|AA’ + Γ| + tr (AA’ + Γ)− 1 S (2) economic losses), accident sequence, and safety barriers. Moreover, exhibit
2
C1 factors that affect risk perception and the difference among volunteer risks (e.
∑ g., Parachute), non-volunteer risks (e.g., nuclear energy), unknown risks, and
where S = 1n ni=1 (Yi − Y)(Yi − Y)′ . For this purpose, the R-project
known risks.
function factanal was implemented that considers the number of factors -Module 2: Identify the mechanical integrity program, the Risk-Based
determined by GDF, Thomson’s Method for scores estimating (McDo­ Maintenance (RBM) approach, and the acceptance criteria for process
nald and Burr, 1967), and Varimax Kaiser factor rotation criterion equipment (based on RAGAGEP’s * ).
-Module 3: Focus on risk evaluation to aware students of acceptable and
(Kaiser, 1958). Information regarding Factor Analysis and their imple­
tolerable criteria and the available techniques for risk and hazard evaluation.
mentation on R can be found elsewhere (Everitt and Hothorn, 2011; -Module 4: Focus on equipment protection elements (e.g., relief valves,
Crawley, 2007; Thomas, 2016). rupture disks) and Runaway reactions Criticality Levels.
Our approach seeks to determine students’ risk perception as a latent -Module 1: Focus on safety Leading & Lagging Metrics, Safety Pyramid, Risk
variable (i.e., cannot be measured directly, but can be related to C2 Management, and GHS hazards classification. Moreover, expose an accident
measurable variables), following an exploratory factor analysis that in­ review and their principal consequences.
vestigates the relationship between measurable variables and factors -Module 2: Focus on failure definition & analysis, Risk-Based Maintenance,
Reliability-centered maintenance (RCM), Risk-Based Inspection (RBI), and
without making any previous assumption (Everitt and Hothorn, 2011).
Safety Integrity Level (SIL).
Consequently, these factors were described based on their loadings (i.e., -Module 3: Focus on Risk Analysis and their methods (i.e., quantitative,
the degree to which the factor causes the variable), plotting against one qualitative, semi-quantitative), Methods based on scenarios, Hazard Analysis
another. Finally, a validation approach was implemented based on five and Identification, and selection criteria for risk and hazard evaluation
techniques.
simple closed Yes/No questions. For this purpose, a grade of 3/5 was
-Module 4: Focus on factors that affect reactive system hazards, Semenov
used as a predefined threshold in every survey question, representing a Diagram (produced heat vs. removed heat), and temperatures of the reactive
theoretical risk-neutral student to compare aversion/propensity in a system.
sensitivity and specificity matrix.
-Module 1: Focused on the differences hazard-risk and safety-security-
C3 personal safety. Identify the accident sequence, safety barriers, safety metrics,
3.2.3. Risk profile description and strategic learning approach and factors that affect risk perception.
In the third phase, students’ risk perception are described based on -Module 2: Expose principal elements from PSM, Asset Management, failure
definition, and the elements of a Mechanical Integrity Program. Focus on the
the factors characterizations and statistic tools as histograms. This phase
relation with Process Safety and Reliability.
aims to determine underlying patterns about students’ risk perception, -Module 3: Focus on Risk Management. Expose principal elements from risk/
considering their university (public vs. private) and location. For that hazard evaluation and analysis. Develop HazOp exercises during class.
reason, each of the obtained factors is compared separately. Finally, -Module 4: Expose basic principles of runaway reactions, Semenov Diagram,
Learning planning didactic strategies are suggested depending on four and runaway reaction critically levels.
-Module 1: Expose an accident review, the accident sequence, safety metrics
risk perception results and the available educating modules (Table 1):
C4 & Safety Pyramid, Safety Barriers & Swiss Cheese Model, factors that affect
(Case 1) Predominant risk-averse students, (Case 2) Predominant risk- risk perception.
prone students, (Case 3) Predominant risk-neutral students, and (Case
-Module 2: Exhibit the common failures in process equipment, failure
4) Predominant factors associated with destiny. These cases were definition, deterioration, and reliability concepts such as failure probability,
determined based on the student’s risk perception results and a refer­ asset management, and mechanical integrity elements.
ence threshold. The last case focused on those students who firmly -Module 3: Focus on Risk Analysis and their methods (i.e., quantitative,
believe that accidents happen due to destiny or will of God and that qualitative, semi-quantitative), Methods based on scenarios, Hazard Analysis
and Identification, and selection criteria for risk and hazard evaluation
nothing could have been done to avoid them. The final discrimination
techniques.
aims to raise awareness among students in that accidents is often pre­ -Module 4: Focus on factors that affect reactive system hazards, Semenov
ceded by multiple incidents or near misses, as well as unsafe behaviors Diagram (produced heat vs. removed heat), and temperatures of the reactive
or insufficient operating disciplines. system.

*RAGAGEP: Recognized and generally accepted good engineering practices.


4. Colombian case study
The educating modules were developed following both the SAChE
A total of 465 surveys were carried out in 12 universities from three recommendations, and a review of the skills required for junior engi­
Colombian Regions: Andean (77%), Caribbean (16%), and Pacific (7%). neers by ECOPETROL for the Oil & Gas industry. The four topics of in­
These universities were distributed as follows: 4 public and 4 private terest, which became the proposed educating modules, were Process
universities in the Andean Region; 1 public and 1 private university in Safety Basic Principles (Module 1), Introduction of Mechanical Integrity
the Caribbean Region; and 1 public university in the Pacific Region. (Module 2), Process Hazard Analysis Techniques (Module 3), and
Overall, 247 surveys were from students in public universities (53%) Runaway Reactions (Module 4).
and 218 surveys of private universities (47%). These universities were
selected given to their implementation of our educating modules 5. Results and discussions
described in Amaya-Gómez et al. (2019) in 2016.
The modules were designed to raise awareness about the importance 5.1. Data processing and factors selection
of process safety and profound relation with all the engineers. For this
purpose, pedagogical strategies from constructivist teaching methods The survey questions were initially classified as Direct/Inverse risk-
and situated learning characteristics were implemented. An alternating prone indicators, obtaining a ratio of 29/25. The questions related to
process is proposed using concepts and definitions, case analysis, sce­ Preventive risk reasons were classified as Inverse, and those to Adven­
nario exercises, among others. The primary objective was to establish an turous reasons were almost classified as Direct risk-prone indicators. All
easy communication between the corresponding professor and the stu­ the risk reasons were classified based on the proposed risk situations;
dents based on technical terms that favor their discussion and analysis thus, Adventurous Reasons were not classified straightforward. This
process. classification, along with the question identification, Risk Reasons, and

12
R. Amaya-Gómez et al. Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

Risk Type, is illustrated in Table 2. These questions were identified adequate significance-results, as in five or six factors.
based on the following order: 1–12 Level of agreement questions, 13–17
Closed Yes/No questions, 18–27 Frequency of implementation ques­
tions, 28–37 Popular Proverbs, and 39–54 Level of certainty questions 5.2. Factor description and validation
(Table 3).
This questionnaire was validated using pilots with both samples of Based on the factanal R-function, a four-factor analysis was imple­
risk-prone and risk-averse people (not included in the analysis) and the mented using a Varimax rotation criterion and Thomson’s score esti­
closed Yes/No questions for determining if the surveys measure the mating method. The Loadings factors plot was implemented against
intended risk perception (face validity). From a reliability point of view, each other, see Fig. 3. In what follows, each of these factors will be
the questionnaire was evaluated using Cronbach’s alpha using the Free explained in more detail.
Statistic Software of Wessa (2021), obtaining an overall result of 0.67
(confidence interval from 0.63 to 0.71), which according to Tsang et al. • Factor 1-Risk mitigation actions: This factor is characterized as a
(2017) indicate adequate internal consistency (i.e., consistency of the greater weight in items that indicate undertaking risks without
survey results). Finally, the construct validity was evaluated using the mitigation actions. Risk-averse students (lower scores) need to
Pearson correlation coefficients between each individual item and the guarantee the existence of mitigation elements for potential risk in a
sum of the total, obtaining relevant results with a direct correlation, given situation. Risk-prone students (higher scores) ponder possible
except for the questions U3 and U5. rewards they get by assuming a risk rather than their consequences.
Based on the students’ results, three questions were deleted (U24, Overall, based on the reasons to undertake risks, risk-prone students
U47, and U48) because they do not support the definition of the stu­ ponder expected rewards, and risk-averse students commonly focus
dents’ risk perception. Both risk-averse and risk-prone students tend to on planning actions. Besides, students with higher scores in this
avoid risk situations involving non-compliance with social coexistence factor could face a risky situation following experts recommenda­
norms, as illustrated in the percentage of students for these three choices tions, for instance, they would risk their professional projection
(99.36%, 93.14%, and 97.64%, respectively). Additionally, questions following an idea, as long as it comes from a professional with
U1, U22, U26, U30, and U32 were deleted due to their lower Squared prestige.
Absolute Sum of Loadings (Fig. 2). Note that the questions from the • Factor 2-Destiny/Divine Backup: In this factor, the higher scores
Moralist Evaluation and the Closed Yes/No questions were already are associated with items that represent destiny/fortune backup ac­
removed. Moreover, question U42 was not deleted considering expert tions. What motivates a risk-prone student to face a risk situation is
criteria to determine the student perception in Financial Savings against to believe in their destiny or fortune. Risk-averse students consider
bank loans. that they are not favored by fortune or destiny, and it is better to
The results of the Generalized Degrees of Freedom (GDF) approach avoid unnecessary risks.
showed that 5 or 6 factors analysis should be implemented to minimize • Factor 3-Planners: Risk-averse students in this factor (higher scores)
their corresponding risk (unbiased average MSE). In this work, we consider it essential to make rigorous and well-thought plans and
implemented a four-factor analysis considering the low variance remain them even though they do not offer the expected benefits. On
description with the last two factors and the risk difference of less than the contrary, risk-prone students (low scores) do not foresee nor
7% with the initial result. Moreover, these factors implemented a low follow predefined rules for risk prevention/mitigation. They
number of questions, which is not enough to evaluate students’ risk commonly follow the quote You Only Live Once, i.e., they ponder
perception. Table 4 shows the Chi-Square statistic results and p-Value pleasure of a given situation rather than their possible associated
hypothesis testing, indicating that four factors are sufficient to expose risks. Students with higher scores are concerned about improving
their development in several conditions (i.e., financial, professional,

Table 2
Risk perception survey-questions distribution.
ID Risk Reasons Risk Type Direct/Inverse ID Risk Reasons Risk Type Direct/Inverse

U1 Inspirational Social Direct U28 Adventurous Social Direct


U2 Inspirational Physical Direct U29 Adventurous Physical Inverse
U3 Preventive Social Inverse U30 Inspirational Social Direct
U4 Adventurous Professional Direct U31 Rewarding Physical Direct
U5 Inspirational Professional Inverse U32 Rewarding Social Inverse
U6 Adventurous Social Direct U33 Preventive Social Direct
U7 Inspirational Professional Direct U34 Adventurous Financial Inverse
U8 Inspirational Financial Direct U35 Adventurous Professional Direct
U9 Inspirational Professional Direct U36 Inspirational Social Direct
U10 Adventurous Financial Direct U37 Preventive Professional Inverse
U11 Inspirational Financial Direct U38 Adventurous Professional Inverse
U12 Rewarding Financial Direct U39 Preventive Professional Inverse
U13 Preventive Physical Inverse U40 Rewarding Financial Direct
U14 Preventive Financial Inverse U41 Preventive Financial Inverse
U15 Rewarding Social Direct U42 Preventive Financial Inverse
U16 Rewarding Social Direct U43 Preventive Physical Inverse
U17 Inspirational Physical Inverse U44 Rewarding Professional Inverse
U18 Preventive Physical Inverse U45 Preventive Social Inverse
U19 Adventurous Physical Direct U46 Inspirational Physical Direct
U20 Inspirational Social Direct U47 Preventive Professional Inverse
U21 Inspirational Professional Direct U48 Inspirational Financial Inverse
U22 Rewarding Professional Inverse U49 Rewarding Financial Inverse
U23 Preventive Professional Inverse U50 Rewarding Physical Direct
U24 Preventive Social Inverse U51 Adventurous Financial Direct
U25 Rewarding Professional Direct U52 Rewarding Physical Inverse
U26 Adventurous Social Direct U53 Rewarding Financial Direct
U27 Adventurous Social Inverse U54 Adventurous Physical Direct

13
R. Amaya-Gómez et al. Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

Table 3 Table 3 (continued )


Implemented questionnaire. Question
ID Question
Question typology
ID Question
typology
I thoroughly read contracts, agreements, and policies for
The only reason to buy a piece of clothing or an accessory services and products purchased.
U1 is that it is to my liking, regardless of fashion trends, cost or 1 I prefer to save up to the full commercial value of a product
U42 5
if it is very flashy. I want to purchase rather than request a loan.
Life is very short, so I must take risks and seize every I demand that the package of the syringe be opened in
U2 1 U43 5
opportunity that comes my way. front of me.
The use of "hands-free" makes it safe to communicate while I set short-term personal goals to improve skills or facets of
U3 1 U44 5
driving a vehicle. my character.
I prefer to take a risk and try something without waiting to I check my emails for spelling, wording, and punctuation
U4 1 U45 5
make a plan. several times before I send them.
My career was chosen principally due to the estimated If I were playing a sport and came up with a unique stunt, I
U5 1 U46 5
economic projection. would not hesitate to perform it.
The past experiences of others do not influence my U47 I avoid breaking regulations to avoid sanctions. 5
U6 decisions since each person experiences their processes 1 U48 I pay my debts within the agreed time to do so. 5
differently. I prefer investing in Real State because it will increase its
U49 5
If my leader is an example for my team and comes up with value in the long term
U7 an idea that could put my profession at risk, I do not 1 I consume products that fascinate me even though I know
U50 5
hesitate to develop it. they are not suitable for my health.
I risk everything when I am sure of a good idea, even if it If I am on a winning streak in a game of chance, I definitely
U8 1 U51 5
might bankrupt me. increase the number of my bets.
Once I have failed to carry out an idea, I discard all U52 I am afraid that something unexpected will ruin my plans 5
U9 1
possibility of further attempts with similar ideas. If I buy insurance to protect my assets, I tend to relax in
U53 5
I would invest large sums of money in a raffle without caring for these.
U10 1
hesitation. I have no problem enjoying mechanical attractions in a
U54 5
I am willing to invest a large part of my assets in forming park where accidents have occurred before.
U11 1
my company and developing my ideas.
Q.Typology: 1-Level of agreement, 2-Closed questions, 3-Frequency of imple­
In exchange for an economic benefit, I am willing to
U12 1 mentation, 4-Popular proverb, 5-Level of certainty.
compromise my physical integrity.
Do you have a working flashlight, whistle, and emergency
U13 2
water supply at home? social) through predefined plans, whereas students with lower scores
Have you or your family purchased funeral or funeral
U14 2 assume risks of pursuing excitement sensations and pleasure-seeking
insurance for your family nucleus?
If you were aware of not having singing skills, would you instead of safety stability.
U15 2
participate in karaoke? • Factor 4-Return of Experiences (REX): In this factor, higher scores
U16
Would you perform a highly-risk surgery for purely
2 (Risk-averse students) are characterized to consider consequences in
cosmetic purposes? previous REX in their decision-making process to assume/avoid a
If you are planning a trip to help a vulnerable population
U17 and find out there is an outbreak of a contagious disease, 2
particular risk. They tend to prevent any change from their initial
would you give up the trip? plans because it can involve undesirable risks. Moreover, they
U18
I carry an umbrella in winter or when there is constant
3
consider others’ perception and usually follow the predefined norms,
rain. which may be determined by past experiences.
U19 Usually, I practice contact sports 3
If I come up with a funny note associated with the topic in
U20
the middle of a meeting, I express it without hesitation.
3 Students’ scores distribution from each factor can indicate reasons
U21 I use portions of text verbatim without citing the source. 3 that impulse them to assume risks. Based on these results, an overall risk-
U22
I get discouraged by not receiving feedback on the progress
3 prone and risk-averse description were obtained:
of my work on a project. Risk-prone students: They follow their ideals to undertake risks (so­
In case of an idea that I consider innovative, I spend time
U23
evaluating it before trying to implement it
3 cially, professionally, financially, or physically), and they believe
U24 I keep my commitments. 3 doubtless in their occurrences for a risk decision-making process
It is difficult for me to carry out activities that do not have regardless of others’ perception. Although Risk-prone individuals are
U25 3
retribution. also able to undertake risks for ideas from people they trust profes­
U26 I can delegate tasks easily 3
sionally. Moreover, they do not consider previous REX results weighting
U27 I worry about what other people might think of me. 3
U28 Who lives prudently lives sadly 4 reasons to assume/avoid risks. Notably, they are self-protected in risks
U29 Whoever loves danger will perish in it 4 that may affect their physical integrity, tending to increase their level of
Expecting life to treat you fairly because you are a good risk aversion, whereas, for professional, social, and financial risks, they
U30 person is like expecting a lion not to attack you because 4 do not take into account risk mitigation actions in their decision-making
you are a vegetarian
U31 Once a year does not hurt 4
process.
U32 You have to put your own oxygen mask on first 4 Risk-averse students: Although they consider their ideas an essential
U33 It is better to be alone than in bad company 4 factor in assuming/ avoiding risk situations, they tend to seek further
U34 Never sell the bear’s skin before one has killed the beast 4 information regarding their surroundings, e.g., results from similar sit­
You cannot swim for new horizons until you have the
U35 4 uations or expert judges/people close to them. It is evidenced that they
courage to lose sight of the shore
U36 Once too often for the experience 4 tend to be more supported in risk situations decision-making processes
U37 Prevention is better than formatting 4 mainly related to previous professional and financial REX recommen­
U38
I need to see immediate results from every activity I
5
dations. Indeed, they implement available risk mitigation actions to
undertake. prevent their consequences, which indicates the high importance of risk
I leave everything in writing to guarantee the fulfillment of
U39
agreements made.
5 management to decide whether or not to assume a particular risk.
If a trusted person tells me about a very profitable informal Students’ factors were classified as Risk-averse (RA) or Risk-prone
U40 5
business, I will definitely invest in that business. (RP) based on each question’s loadings and a discriminant threshold
U41 5 of 3/5 in every survey question. Those students whose product sum was
greater or equal to this threshold were classified as RP; otherwise, they

14
R. Amaya-Gómez et al. Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

Fig. 2. Squared sum of loadings for every question.

The 5 Closed Yes/No questions are outputs as Risk-averse action, or


Table 4
Risk-prone action (considering Direct/Inverse risk-prone indicators)
Sensitivity analysis of the number of factors.
were implemented to validate this proposed classification. The U13
Chi Square Chi Square question sought to determine if the student had basic emergency sup­
Factors p-Value Factors p-Value
Test Test
plies (e.g., flashlight, whistle, or food reservations). U14 determined if
the student is aware of the existence of any funeral insurance in their
119 01
1 1.61x10− 2083.65 11 1.14x10− 459.37
78 01
2 2.53x10− 1706.54 12 3.03x10− 407.95 family. U15 evaluated student willingness to participate in a music event
42 01
3 4.78x10− 1334.37 13 5.93x10− 357.99
4 1.29x10− 27
1136.77 14 7.81x10− 01
316.65
despite not having any skills. U16 determined if the student is willing to
5 1.28x10− 17
973.01 15 9.26x10− 01
274.71 do a high-risk esthetic surgery. Finally, U17 allowed identifying if the
6 1.80x10− 12
860.04 16 9.63x10− 01
242.81 student would participate in a volunteer despite an alert of a highly
09 01
7 4.68x10− 766.91 17 9.90x10− 208.68 dangerous contagious disease. Considering students, actions, and factors
06 01
8 4.19x10− 676.42 18 9.97x10− 180.41
04 01 classification, a sensitivity & specificity matrix was determined
9 4.16x10− 598.13 19 9.98x10− 156.35
10 7.39x10− 03
531.82 20 1.00 130.29 (Table 5).
As an example, questions U13 and U17 will be described. For the
sake of simplicity, for now on, the four scenarios RA-actions/RA-
were classified as RA. Interestingly, these factors provided information students, RP-actions/RA-students, RA-actions/RP-students, and RP-
about why students assume/avoid a certain risk, as it can be noticed in actions/RP-students will be denoted as R1, R2, R3, and R4, respec­
the differences among RP numbers from each factor (i.e., 16, 19, 405, tively. The results from the sensitivity & specificity matrix for the U13
and 272, respectively). question indicated that:

Fig. 3. Second and fourth factor loadings comparison.

Table 5
Students risk aversion/propensity sensitivity & specificity matrix.
Risk-averse action Risk-prone action
Factor
U13 U14 U15 U16 U17 U13 U14 U15 U16 U17

F1 175 318 167 416 309 274 131 282 33 140


F2 175 317 168 414 303 271 129 278 32 143
Risk averse students
F3 21 41 22 49 49 39 19 38 11 11
F4 74 134 85 173 139 119 59 108 20 54

Factor U13 U14 U15 U16 U17 U13 U14 U15 U16 U17

F1 7 12 5 15 9 9 4 11 1 7
F2 7 13 4 17 15 12 6 15 2 4 Risk averse students
F3 161 289 150 382 269 244 116 255 23 136
F4 108 196 87 258 179 164 76 185 14 93

15
R. Amaya-Gómez et al. Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

1. Students that are not favored by destiny and implement preventive/ differ significantly because the RP factors ratio classification is very
mitigative actions as emergency supplies. similar (4% F1, 4% F2, 90% F3, and 56% F4), even though the Andean
2. Students who are not planners usually take risk mitigation measures Region counts with most of the reporting universities. A risk perception
and REX recommendations, but they probably have not lived similar comparison per city is implemented depicted in Fig. 5 illustrates that
experiences; thus, it does not seem indispensable for them. cities as Cartagena, Bogotá, and Bucaramanga have the highest number
3. Students that although are not planners nor guided by past experi­ of risk-averse students (including Markedly Risk-averse). On the con­
ences, they are not favored by destiny, then they take actions for risk trary, Cali and Medellin are the cities with the highest number of risk-
mitigations as in this case. prone students (including Slightly risk-prone), as almost 60% of their
4. Students that are not planner nor guided by other experiences. students can be characterized as risk-prone. This figure also illustrates
that cities with industrial activities such as Cartagena and Bucaramanga
Similarly, for the U17 question, it was obtained: (considering their proximity to Barrancabermeja) are more risk-averse
than those in non-industrial cities such as Medellin. This result may
1. Students who would not participate as volunteers because they occur because people from these cities already include high-risk ele­
implement risk preventive/mitigative actions (also follow REX rec­ ments on their common risk perception based on their lived experiences,
ommendations), and they are not favored by destiny. as evidenced by the fourth factor regarding Return of Experience.
2. Students that would-be volunteers because although they employ Furthermore, students’ perception was assessed, taking into account
risk mitigation actions, they are not guided by past experiences from if the university was public or private (Fig. 6); through this analysis, we
others. found out that students from public universities tended to be more risk-
3. Students who would not cooperate on it, although they are not prone compared to students from private universities. The main reason
guided by others’ experience (indeed not planners), are not favored for this behavior was that they are less-planners and do not implement
by destiny, so risk prevention decisions are implemented. mitigation/prevention actions.
4. Students that are characterized for the quote You Only Live Once and Finally, based on these results and the proposed educating modules,
not by past experiences, they would cooperate as volunteers the following Learning Planning was suggested:
regardless of any alert.
(i) universities in which their students’ second factor were classified
Based on results in Table 5, similar descriptions can be obtained from as RP, should consider C4 Didactic Strategy;
the questions U14, U15, and U16. These results indicate that the ob­ (ii) universities from Bogotá, Cartagena and Bucaramanga should
tained factors allow describing students’ behavior in a risk decision- evaluate C1 or C3 Didactic Strategies; and
making process, although these descriptions depend on factors (iii) students from Medellin, Cali, and Barranquilla should contem­
interpretations. plate C2 or C3 Didactic Strategies, respecting to the professor’s
autonomy.

5.3. Risk perception description and learning planning approach According to the Health and Safety Executive (HSE), Safety Culture is
defined as the product of individual and group values, attitudes, com­
Based on the RA/RP discrimination mentioned above, a four-class petencies, and patterns of behavior that determine the commitment to,
classification was proposed to describe the level of propensity or aver­ and the style and proficiency of, an organization’s health and safety
sion of the engineering students, using the number of factors that were programs (HSE, 2002). It attempts to improve workplace safety,
classified as Risk-prone: (i) 0 factors/Markedly Risk-averse students, (ii) concentrating on technical issues and human errors. Indeed, their
1 factor/Risk-averse students, (iii) 2 factors/Slightly Risk-prone stu­ importance is related to the fact that management systems and their
dents, and (iv) 3 factors/Risk-prone students. Note that none of the associated procedures depend on actions from individuals and groups
students have four risk-prone factors; thus, they are not hypothetically for their successful implementation (CCPS, 2015). Process Safety has an
classified as Markedly risk-prone students. important cultural component that can affect Risk Perceptions, as we
A region comparison was developed based on the four-class classi­ have already illustrated in this work. We want to emphasize the need to
fication results in order to describe the risk perception of the students, establish a technical language that transcends any language, which is
obtaining the resulst shown in Fig. 4. Note that students from the An­ easy to communicate among the community, operators, and stake­
dean Region tend to be more risk-averse, whereas students from the holders around the world. This work proposes an alternative for the
Pacific Region more risk-prone. Interestingly, the histograms do not

Fig. 4. Risk perception results per region.

16
R. Amaya-Gómez et al. Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

Fig. 5. Risk perception results per city.

Fig. 6. Public vs. private universities risk perception results.

learning of Process Safety depending on the students’ risk perception, the past (Return of Experience). The risk-prone profile tends to follow
but further analysis is required to strengthen process safety culture in their ideals, tend not to consider previous lessons learned, and avoid risk
future engineers. It is worth mentioning that this work contemplated mitigation actions during the decision-making process. The risk-averse
desired skills for engineers in the Oil & Gas industry; however, these profile is characterized as seeking additional information to support
skills can also be required for any industrial sector handling hazardous decisions, considering the return of experience from previous incidents,
materials (e.g., chemical, food) with appropriate adjustments. Also, we and actions to mitigate possible consequences. This work implemented a
acknowledge that different sectors like aviation, mining, and nuclear discriminating threshold with a neutral loading of the survey to compare
can also be high-risk sectors as contemplated by Hussin and Wang the results from the students’ surveys, obtaining that overall, they tend
(2010) in the industrial safety perception of postgraduate students in the to be slightly risk-prone in a higher proportion. The results indicate that
University of Aberdeen (Scotland). the students near industrial refineries tend to be more risk-averse than
those in non-industrial locations.
6. Conclusions The results suggest didactic strategies based on the content of a
modular toolbox on (i) Process Safety basic principles, (ii) Mechanical
In recent years, many researchers have investigated different fea­ Integrity, (iii) Process Hazard Analysis (PHA) techniques, and (iv)
tures related to process safety teaching, including appropriate inclusion runaway reactions from a collaborative work between Los Andes Uni­
in the engineering curriculum (i.e., given accreditation constraints) or versity and the Colombian Oil company (ECOPETROL). These modules
which tools are valuable for the learning process. However, one of the follow the recommendations of process safety education from SAChE
topics perceived among educators as not as important is the student’s and skills required for recently graduated engineers by ECOPETROL in
risk perception (Gajek et al., 2022), although risk perception has been the Oil & Gas industry. The strategies are chosen depending on the
recognized as valuable in high-risk industries such as the offshore oil students’ risk perceptions, i.e., predominant risk-averse, risk-prone, or
industry (Rundmo, 1996), where decisions require managing risks risk-neutral, and if the students have a marked belief in accidents by
effectively. According to Rundmo (1996), there is a positive correlation destiny. Process Safety and Risk Management are elements that tran­
between risk perception and risk behaviors, which in turn, may affect scend any engineering; they are continuously evolving in the past years.
how risk is measured, and which safety measures are required. This work is limited for some particular reasons and contexts to un­
This work proposed an approach to support didactic strategies for dertake a given risky situation, but risk perception goes beyond them,
teaching process safety in engineering students based on the students’ personnel risk perception includes additional cultural and societal
risk perceptions. For this purpose, a survey with financial, social, contexts that were not assessed yet. This work aims to be an invitation
physical, and professional risks was contemplated, using preventive, for educators to incorporate risk perception in their planning and course
adventurous, inspirational, and rewarding criteria to undertake or avoid content; as remarked by Benintendi (2016) the university tier is the basis
these risks. The survey was applied to 465 engineering students from 12 of the learning pyramid process, which influences the future performance
universities in Colombia in the spring semester of 2016, and a Factor in the professional tier to support further managing decision-making
Analysis was implemented to identify respondents’ risk profile and processes.
tendency as risk-averse or risk-prone. The Factor Analysis described the
students’ profile based on how they perceived risk mitigation actions,
destiny (or divine backup), planning strategies, and lessons learned from

17
R. Amaya-Gómez et al. Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

Declaration of Competing Interest He, Z., Chen, C., Weng, W., 2022. Multi-hazard risk assessment in process industries:
state-of-the-Art. J. Loss Prev. Process Ind. 76, 104672.
Hogarth, R., 2001. Educating Intuition. University of Chicago Press.
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial HSE, 2002. Safety Culture: A Review of the Literature, HSL/2002/25. Technical Report
interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence Health and Safety Executive.
the work reported in this paper. Hussin, M., Wang, B., 2010. Industrial safety perception among post-graduate
engineering students. Knowl. Based Syst. 23, 769–771.
Kahan, D., Jenkins-Smith, H., Braman, D., 2011. Cultural cognition of scientific
Acknowledgments consensus. J. Risk Res. 14, 147–174.
Kahneman, D., 2011. Thinking, Fast and Slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Kaiser, H., 1958. The varimax criterion for analytic rotation in factor analysis.
R. Amaya-Gómez thanks the National Department of Science, Psychometrika 23, 187–200.
Technology and Innovation of Colombia for the Ph.D. scholarship Kerin, T., 2016. The evolution of process safety standards and legislation following
(COLCIENCIAS Grant No. 727, 2015). landmark events-what have we learnt? Process Saf. Prog. 35, 165–170.
Kouwenhoven, P., 2021. Process safety education: a comparative study. Educ. Chem.
The authors would like thank all the students that completed the Eng. 36, 128–142.
survey that let us present these results. Also, the authors are special LeDoux, J., 2012. Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron 73, 653–676.
thankful for the discussion and help during the design and validation Leiserowitz, A., 2003. Global Warming in the American Mind: The Roles of Affect,
Imagery, and Worldviews in Risk Perception, Policy Preferences and Behavior (Ph.D.
process of Nicolás Villalba and Ana María Hernández. thesis).
Lowrance, W., 1976. Of Acceptable Risk: Science and the Determination of Safety. W.
References Kaufmann.
Mannan, M., Reyes-Valdes, O., Jain, P., Tamim, N., Ahammad, M., 2016. The evolution
of process safety: current status and future direction. Annu. Rev. Chem. Biomol. Eng.
Abdullahi, R., Mansor, N., 2015. Fraud triangle theory and fraud diamond theory.
7, 135–162. PMID: 26979411.
understanding the convergent and divergent for future research. Int. J. Acad. Res.
McDonald, R., Burr, E.J., 1967. A comparison of four methods of constructing factor
Account. Financ. Manag. Sci. 5, 38–45.
scores. Psychometrika 32, 381–401.
Álvarez-Chávez, C., Marín, L., Pérez-Gamez, K., Portell, M., Velázquez, L.,
Mearns, K., Flin, R., 1995. Risk perception and attitudes to safety by personnel in the
MuñozOsuna, F., 2019. Assessing college students’ risk perceptions of hazards in
offshore oil and gas industry: a review. J. Loss Prev. Process Ind. 8, 299–305.
chemistry laboratories. J. Chem. Educ. 96, 2120–2131.
Mesa-Gómez, A., Casal, J., Muñoz, F., 2020. Risk analysis in Natech events: state of the
Amaya-Gómez, R., Dumar, V., Sánchez-Silva, M., Romero, R., Arbeláez, C., Muñoz, F.,
art. J. Loss Prev. Process Ind. 64, 104071.
2019. Process safety part of the engineering education DNA. Educ. Chem. Eng.
Mkpat, E., Reniers, G., Cozzani, V., 2018. Process safety education: a literature review.
Amyotte, P., McCutcheon, D., 2006. Risk management-an area of knowledge for all
J. Loss Prev. Process Ind. 54, 18–27.
engineers. Technical Report.Discussion Paper Prepared for Canadian Council of
Munguia, N., Álvarez, C., Pérez, K., Esquer, J., 2016. Students’ awareness of chemical
Professional Engineers.
risk on a Mexican Campus: the University of Sonora case study. Cent. East. Eur. J.
Arezes, P., Miguel, A., 2008. Risk perception and safety behaviour: a study in an
Manag. Econ. 179–188 (p.).
occupational environment. Saf. Sci. 46, 900–907. Occupational Safety and Risk at
O’Toole, M., 2002. The relationship between employees’ perceptions of safety and
{ESREL} 2006.
organizational culture. J. Saf. Res. 33, 231–243.
ASEE, 2015. The Attributes of a Global Engineer Project. 〈http://www.gedcouncil.org/p
OECD, 2003. Emerging Risks in the 21st Century. An Agenda for action. Technical Report
ublications/attributes-global-engineer-project〉.
Orgnisation for Economic Co-operation and Development.
Benintendi, R., 2016. The bridge link between university and industry: a key factor for
Paek, H.-J., Hove, T., 2017. Risk Perceptions and Risk Characteristics. Technical Report
achieving high performance in process safety. Educ. Chem. Eng. 15, 23–32.
Oxford University Press.
Bratt, O., Damber, J.-E., Emanuelsson, M., Kristoffersson, U., Lundgren, R., Bratt, H., H
Perlman, A., Sacks, R., Barak, R., 2014. Hazard recognition and risk perception in
Grönberg, H., 2000. Risk perception, screening practice and interest in genetic
construction. Saf. Sci. 64, 22–31.
testing among unaffected men in families with hereditary prostate cancer. Eur. J.
Perrin, L., Laurent, A., 2008. Current situation and future implementation of safety
Cancer 36, 235–241.
curricula for chemical engineering education in France. Educ. Chem. Eng. 3,
Brown, V., 2014. Risk perception-it’s personal. Environ. Health Perspect. 122,
e84–e91.
A276–279.
Pintar, A., 1999. Teaching chemical process safety: a separate course versus integration
Campbell, G., Belton, D., 2016. Setting up new chemical engineering degree
into existing courses. In Proceedings of the ASEE Annual Conference. Charlotte,
programmes: exercises in design and retrofit within constraints. Educ. Chem. Eng.
North Caroline. ISSN: 2153-5965 (4.479.1–4.479.10).
17, 1–13.
Planas, E., Arnaldos, J., Darbra, R., Muñoz, M., Pastor, E., Vílchez, J., 2014. Historical
Campbell Institute, 2014. Risk Perception: Theories, Strategies, and next steps. Technical
evolution of process safety and major-accident hazards prevention in Spain.
Report National Safety Council.
Contribution of the pioneer Joaquim Casal. J. Loss Prev. Process Ind. 28, 109–117
Carlson, D., 2015. A Risk Perception Analysis: Toxicology Education, Its Effect on
(European Process Safety Pioneers).
Quantitative Judgments of Risk, and the Influence of Demographic Variables
Reniers, G., Khakzad, N., Cozzani, V., Khan, F., 2018. The impact of nature on chemical
(Master’s thesis). Raleigh, NC, United States.
industrial facilities: Dealing with challenges for creating resilient chemical industrial
CCPS, 2015. Safety Culture: “What Is At Stake”. Technical Report Center for Chemical
parks. J. Loss Prev. Process Ind. 56, 378–385.
Process Safety.
Renn, O., 2008. Risk Governance: Coping with Uncertainty in a Complex World.
Chen, Y., Huang, H., Tu, I., 2010. A new approach for selecting the number of factors.
Earthscan.
Comput. Stat. Data Anal. 54, 2990–2998.
Roszkowski, M., Davey, G., 2010. Risk perception and risk tolerance changes attributable
Crawley, M., 2007. The R Book. Wiley.
to the 2008 economic crisis: a subtle but critical difference. J. Financ. Serv. Prof. 64,
Crosier, D., Parveva, T., 2013. The Bologna Process: Its Impact in Europe and Beyond.
42–53.
Technical Report United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization.
Rundmo, T., 1996. Associations between risk perception and safety. Saf. Sci. 24,
Cruz, A., Krausmann, E., 2013. Vulnerability of the oil and gas sector to climate change
197–209.
and extreme weather events. Clim. Chang. 121, 41–53.
Slovic, P., 1987. Perception of risk. Science 236, 280–285.
Dee, S., Cox, B., Ogle, R., 2015. Process safety in the classroom: the current state of
Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., Lichtenstein, S., 1980. Facts and fears: understanding perceived
chemical engineering programs at US universities. Process Saf. Prog. 34, 316–319.
risk. In: Schwing, R., Albers, W.A. (Eds.), Societal Risk Assessment. Springer, Boston,
Deloitte Touche Tohmatsu, 2017. From Risk Perception to Safe Behaviour. 〈http://www.
pp. 181–216.
deloitte.com.au/media/docs/au_Deloitte_from_risk_perception_to_safe_behaviour.
Suarez-Paba, M., Cruz, A., Muñoz, F., 2020. Emerging Natech risk management in
pdf〉.
Colombia: a survey of governmental organizations. Saf. Sci. 128, 104777.
Dickson, T., Dolnicar, S., 2004. No risk, no fun: The role of perceived risk in adventure
Terry, L., 2007. The Bologna process and its implication for U.S. legal education. J. Leg.
tourism. In: Proceedings of the 13th International Research Conference of the
Educ. 57, 237–252.
Council of Australian University Tourism and Hospitality Education CAUTHE.
Thomas, E., 2016. Factor Analysis. 〈http://web.stanford.edu/class/psych253/tutorials/
Lincoln, New Zealand.
FactorAnalysis.html〉.
Douglas, M., Wildavsky, A., 1982. Risk and Culture: An Essay on the Selection of
Tsang, S., Royse, C., Terkawi, A., 2017. Guidelines for developing, translating, and
Technological and Environmental Dangers. University of California Press.
validating a questionnaire in perioperative and pain medicine. Saudi J. Anesth. 11,
Edwards, A., Gray, J., Clarke, A., Dundon, J., Elwyn, G., Gaff, C., Hood, K., Iredale, R.,
S80–S89.
Sivell, S., Shaw, C., Thornton, H., 2008. Interventions to improve risk
Tversky, A., Kahneman, D., 1974. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases.
communication in clinical genetics: Systematic review. Patient Educ. Couns. 71,
Science 185, 1124–1131.
4–25.
UNESCO, 2012. A Place to Learn: Lessons from Research on Learning Environments.
Everitt, B., Hothorn, T., 2011. An Introduction to Applied Multivariate Analysis with R
Technical Report United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization.
Use R! Springer New York.
Vlek, C., Stallen, P.-J., 1981. Judging risks and benefits in the small and in the large.
Fischhoff, B., 1995. Risk perception and communication unplugged: twenty years of
Organ. Behav. Hum. Perform. 28, 235–271.
process. Risk Anal. 15, 137–145.
Weinstein, N., 1980. Unrealistic optimism about future life events. J. Personal. Soc.
Gajek, A., Fabiano, B., Laurent, A., Jensen, N., 2022. Process safety education of future
Psychol. 39, 806–820.
employee 4.0 in Industry 4.0. J. Loss Prev. Process Ind. 75, 104691.

18
R. Amaya-Gómez et al. Education for Chemical Engineers 42 (2023) 7–19

Weisenfeld, U., Ott, I., 2011. Academic discipline and risk perception of technologies: an Weyman, W., Kelly, C., 1999. Risk Perception and Risk Communication: A Review of
empirical study. Res. Policy 40, 487–499. Literature Volume CRR 148. Health and Safety Executive.
Wessa, P., 2021. Cronbach Alpha (v1.0.6) Free Statistics Software (v1.2.1), Office for Wilke, A., Mata, R., 2012. Cognitive bias. In: Ramachandran, V. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of
Research Development and Education. 〈https://www.wessa.net/rwasp_cronbach. Human Behavior, second ed. Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 531–535.
wasp/〉.

19

You might also like