You are on page 1of 20

Biol. Rev. (2018), pp. 000–000.

1
doi: 10.1111/brv.12462

What, where and when: spatial foraging


decisions in primates
Cinzia Trapanese1,2,3,4∗ , Hélène Meunier2,3† and Shelly Masi4†
1
École Doctorale Frontières du Vivant (FdV) – Programme Bettencourt, Centre de Recherches Interdisciplinaires, Tour Maine Montparnasse,
Paris 75015, France
2 Centre de Primatologie de l’Université de Strasbourg, Fort Foch, Niederhausbergen 67207, France
3
Faculté de psychologie Laboratoire de Neurosciences Cognitives et Adaptatives, UMR 7364, CNRS et Université de Strasbourg, Strasbourg
67000, France
4
Département Hommes et Environnements Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique/Muséum National d’Histoire Naturelle, University Paris
Diderot, Sorbonne Paris Cité, Musée de l’Homme, UMR 7206-CNRS/MNHN, Paris 75116, France

ABSTRACT

When exploiting the environment, animals have to discriminate, track, and integrate salient spatial cues to navigate
and identify goal sites. Actually, they have to know what can be found (e.g. what fruit), where (e.g. on which tree) and
when (in what season or moment of the year). This is very relevant for primate species as they often live in seasonal and
relatively unpredictable environments such as tropical forests. Here, we review and compare different approaches used
to investigate primate spatial foraging strategies: from direct observations of wild primates to predictions from statistical
simulations, including experimental approaches on both captive and wild primates, and experiments in captivity using
virtual reality technology. Within this framework, most of these studies converge to show that many primate species can
(i) remember the location of most of food resources well, and (ii) often seem to have a goal-oriented path towards spatially
permanent resources. Overall, primates likely use mental maps to plan different foraging strategies to enhance their
fitness. The majority of studies suggest that they may organise spatial information on food resources into topological
maps: they use landmarks to navigate and encode local spatial information with regard to direction and distance. Even
though these studies were able to show that primates can remember food quality (what) and its location (where), still very
little is known on how they incorporate the temporal knowledge of available food (when). Future studies should attempt
to increase our understanding of the potential of primates to learn temporal patterns and how both socio-ecological
differences among species and their cognitive abilities influence such behavioural strategies.

Key words: spatial foraging strategies, cognitive map, primates, decision rules, spatial memory.

CONTENTS
I. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2
II. Into the primate mind . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 3
(1) Random walks models: Lévy walks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
(2) The strategy of re-using the same routes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4
(3) Path integration . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
(4) Landmark navigation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
(5) Cognitive maps: acquisition, storage and organisation of information . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5
III. How to infer the mental processes underlying primate movements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
(1) Experiments in captivity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
(2) Manipulating or observing the natural environment: wild primates . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
(a) Field experiments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 7
(b) Direct observations of wild primates: indicators of intentionality and the change-point test (CPT) 9
(c) An alternative method: the Circuity Index . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13
(d) Quantitative statistical models based on direct observations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13

* Address for correspondence (Tel: +33 7 52 59 25 74; E-mail: cinzia.trapanese@cri-paris.org).


† Authors contributed equally to this work.

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


2 C. Trapanese and others

(e) Botanical or ecological knowledge: a complementary or an additional strategy? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 13


(f ) Planning ahead: integrating ecological variables to assess behaviour goals . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14
(3) Primate navigation in virtual reality . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 14
IV. General discussion and perspectives . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 15
V. Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 16
VI. Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17
VII. References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17

I. INTRODUCTION travel routes of similar length to the optimal path (Cramer


& Gallistel, 1997; Janson & Byrne, 2007). But is the strategy
Finding and locating food resources in wild habitats of visiting all available food locations always the most
can be cognitively demanding. For instance, animals convenient? As the environment can be unpredictable and
living in tropical forests have to cope with complex unstable, a variety of reasons could lead primates to miss
environments often characterised by relatively unpredictable out a feeding patch: to avoid environmental and physical
food availability. Indeed, since all rainforest trees do not impediments, to avoid predators or competitors, to feed on
always fruit each year, and because fruiting periods are another food that is abundant in a given season (e.g. better
often short (Chapman et al., 1999), frugivorous animals maturation state), or to use a more valuable food available in
have to know what tree produces fruits and when. What, the habitat. Cunningham & Janson (2007a) showed that wild
where and when an individual eats are likely to affect its saki monkeys (Pithecia pithecia) increased the overall distance
fitness, and, ultimately, species-level adaptations (Lambert travelled in order to visit preferred resources such as highly
& Rothman, 2015; Janmaat et al., 2016). When exploiting productive fruit trees and tree-water holes. This strategy
the environment, animals have to discriminate, track and allowed them to encounter the majority of the available fruit
integrate salient spatial cues to navigate and identify feeding trees even if they did not feed on them. This behaviour likely
sites. Animals receive also a barrage of sensory inputs, allows them to minimise intra-group feeding competition and
which help them to take efficient feeding decisions in to monitor continuously the state of resources throughout
relation to the spatio-temporal distribution of food (Dominy their home range (Cunningham & Janson, 2007a). Primates
et al., 2001). While foraging, individuals generally face usually visit more than one destination when foraging: they
three major challenges: (i) locating ephemeral, scattered thus use multi-destination routes (Gallistel & Cramer, 1996).
goal sites characterised by fluctuating annual and seasonal When individuals leave a patch, they are expected to move
patterns, (ii) competing with co-feeders (intra-group and to their next goal (e.g. sleeping site, feeding site, refuge area)
extra-group competition; Garber, Bicca-Marques & de O in a direct manner, minimising unnecessary travel in order
Azevedo-Lopes, 2009) and (iii) avoiding predators. to maximise their energy balance (Chapman & Chapman,
For animals, an efficient way to maximise energy intake is 2000). If the potential choices between food locations are
by planning foraging decisions. In this context, planning is already included in a known foraging sequence by the
defined as the ability to search in advance for the best strategy individual, route optimisation becomes analogous to the
to forage efficiently (e.g. where to go to find a given resource classical optimal foraging problem, the Traveling Salesman
and when). We can argue that planning implies intentionality, Problem (TSP). This is an experimental algorithmic problem
or the ability to formulate goals to proceed towards them with that has many applications: an individual has to choose
direct intent (Dooris, Kelley & Trainer, 2004; e.g. maximise an optimal route to visit many different locations before
energy intake while minimising costs). On the other hand, returning to the starting point. Since this represents a
we can also assume intentionality without planning, for combinatorial optimisation problem, it has been intensively
example the intentionality in the use of a behavioural tactic. studied from a computational standpoint as part of a
Optimisation of foraging strategies has been shown in many class of mathematical problems termed non-deterministic
animal taxa such as birds (Davies & Houston, 1981; Gill, polynomial-time hardness (NPhard; Lawler et al., 1985).
1988), bats (Lemke, 1984; Racey & Swift, 1985), rodents Solutions to TSPs become increasingly complex and
(Reid & Reid, 2005), primates (e.g. Di Fiore & Suarez, 2007; intractable as the number of destinations increases
Noser & Byrne, 2013) and even pollinating insects (Janzen, (MacGregor & Chu, 2011). Cramer & Gallistel (1997) tested
1971; Heinrich, 1976; Lihoreau, Chittka & Raine, 2011). the capacity to solve TSP-like problems in a group of captive
In traplining bumblebees (Bombus terrestris), for example, it vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops). To visit four feeding
has been suggested that they are able to trade off between stations, they optimised their route to return to the starting
the distance travelled and the prioritisation of high-reward point in 20 out of 26 trials. In a second experiment, four
sites when foraging (Lihoreau et al., 2011). So what are the baited locations were provided on one side of the enclosure,
strategies that animals – and primates in particular – use to and two more on the other side, with the nearest location in
optimise their energy balance? Some studies have shown that each group being equidistant from the starting point. Vervet
using a simple planning rule of travelling step-by-step from monkeys chose the side with the greater number of foraging
one destination to the next nearest can produce complex sites in all trials, showing the ability to plan their movements

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


Spatial foraging decisions in primates 3

at least three steps ahead (two further destinations beyond the principal methods used to investigate the cognitive mech-
nearest site; Cramer & Gallistel, 1997). The computational anisms underpinning decision making, memorisation, and
complexity required for such planned foraging raises the planning. We discuss those methods in order of increasing
question of whether in the wild primates can calculate experimental complexity, from behavioural observations in
the shortest path among multiple destinations (Lawler et al., the wild, through testing of theoretical expectations in exper-
1985; Gallistel & Cramer, 1996). Wild individuals (and most imental settings both in the wild and captivity, and finally to
primate species) are confronted more often with open-TSP a new approach based on virtual reality. In order to provide
or the so-called optimal Hamiltonian path problem (Garey useful insights and perspectives for future investigations, the
& Johnson, 1979). In the mathematical field of graph theory, limitations of each method are also discussed.
a Hamiltonian path visits each vertex of the graph exactly
once. Applying this to foraging routes, individuals visit each
destination only once and they do not need to return to the II. INTO THE PRIMATE MIND
starting point (Janson, 2014).
How does species socio-ecology shape the level of
Studies on spatial foraging decisions in primates aim to
cognitive sophistication in foraging strategies? Primates, and
investigate how primates encode, internally represent and
particularly anthropoids, have relatively large brains com-
integrate spatial, temporal and quantity information to plan
pared to other mammals (Clutton-Brock & Harvey, 1980). their feeding choices efficiently. The simplest strategy for
The Social Brain Hypothesis identifies social complexity as foraging would be to move randomly until a feeding resource
the main driver for primate cognitive complexity (Dunbar, is located. This would be particularly efficient for species with
1998) whereas many comparative studies have linked a a generalist diet living in a highly productive environment
larger brain size to a frugivorous diet (Clutton-Brock & containing abundant and readily available food (e.g.
Harvey, 1980; Milton, 1981; DeCasien, Williams & Higham, herbaceous vegetation; Garber, 1987). However, this strategy
2017). Various other hypotheses about the evolution of is unlikely to be effective for primates, given that many are
a large brain in the primate lineage have been proposed frugivorous and territorial, and live in large home ranges
[e.g. the cognitive-buffer hypothesis (Allman, McLaughlin with scattered and ephemeral fruit trees (Strier, 2007). In
& Hakeem, 1993), the Machiavellian intelligence hypothesis tropical forests, where the majority of primates live, fruits are
(Byrne & Whiten, 1988), the cultural intelligence hypothesis available only at certain times of the year, and fruiting trees
(Whiten & Van Schaik, 2007), the ecological intelligence are visited by many animals competing for this energy-rich
hypothesis (Milton, 1981)]. The debate between a social or resource (Marriott, Robinson & Karikari, 1981; Diaz-Perez,
ecological basis for cognitive complexity is now linked with Bautista & Villanueva, 2000; Houle, Chapman & Vickery,
a growing body of evidence showing that brain organisation 2007). According to Optimal Foraging Theory (Schoener,
may also relate to divergent ecological adaptations [Rosati, 1971; Stephens & Krebs, 1986), therefore, primates should
2017; the ecological intelligence hypothesis (Milton, 1981)]. optimise their travelling patterns. Their complex cognitive
This seems to be the case for gorillas where differences abilities allow them to minimise energy expenditure and
in life history and brain structure between the two species hence approach maximum foraging efficiency (Dall et al.,
correspond to their divergent ecological habits (Stoinski et al., 2005; Robbins & Hohmann, 2006). Detours would increase
2013; Barks et al., 2015): one species (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) is costs in terms of risk of starvation, predation, and increased
more frugivorous while the other (Gorilla beringei beringei) is energy expenditure. However, the relative costs will vary
more folivorous as a consequence of lower fruit availability in according to the feeding ecology of the species (Garber,
their mountain habitat (Masi, Cipolletta & Robbins, 2009). 1987). For example, folivorous primates’ resources are more
Recently, comparisons across several primate species found homogeneously distributed and are present throughout the
evidence suggesting a greater influence of ecological rather year (and are therefore easily accessible) than are resources of
than social variables on brain size (DeCasien et al., 2017). fruit specialists (e.g. Chapman, White & Wrangham, 1994;
After controlling for body size and phylogeny, brain size was Chapman, Wrangham & Chapman, 1995; Sayers, 2013;
better predicted by the diet than by multiple measures of Masi et al., 2015; Masi & Breuer, 2018).
sociality (DeCasien et al., 2017): frugivores had larger brains According to the concept of rule-based decision-making
than folivores. Investigating the spatial foraging strategies (Bugnyar & Heinrich, 2006), animals can use a ‘trial
of primates may thus help to shed light on the origin of the and error’ strategy and/or different forms of social
complex cognitive abilities of primates and on the roots of learning to create expectations (hypotheses) and tactics
the most complex planning capacities of humans. (rules) to find solutions (Laland, 2004). These capacities
Investigating spatial foraging decisions in primates require behavioural flexibility to generalise cause-and-effect
raises a number of challenges, including the methodological relationships from one context to another by recognising
approach used. For a better understanding of the approaches critical elements common to past and present situations
used to investigate primate spatial foraging strategies and (Watanabe & Huber, 2006). This would imply the ability
the underlying cognitive mechanisms, we here review (i) to recall which behavioural tactic was most successful
the existing literature to highlight the current foundations in the past and the capacity to apply that tactic to the
on which the theoretical framework is based, and (ii) the current problem (Watanabe & Huber, 2006). If a given

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


4 C. Trapanese and others

tactic fails, the individual is expected to incorporate this provide faster dispersal, encounter a higher number of novel
information and try other tactics or decision rules to solve sites (Bartumeus et al., 2005) and have a reduced likelihood
the problem (Garber, 2000). Primates seem to be able to of intra-specific competition than Brownian walks. In Lévy
develop foraging strategies that integrate both social and walks the variance (i.e. second-order moment) diverges and
ecological information (Bicca-Marques & Garber, 2005). adds a super-diffusive feature, increasing their efficiency
Garber et al. (2009) suggested a set of ‘decision rules’ for compared to purely diffusive movements. Moreover, Lévy
primates: information is organised hierarchically with the walks accurately describe the pattern but not the underlying
most important information for that decision considered process, as they represent an efficient search strategy for
first. For example, species with a more-specialist diet will resources that are distributed in self-similar ways across
first evaluate ‘food type information’ (e.g. a given fruit various environmental scales [i.e. both searches at a reduced
type such as leaves or flowers), whereas species living in scale (which might be purely random) and searches at a high
large social groups will first prioritise ‘quantity information’ scale with goal-oriented movements (Benhamou, 2007)].
(e.g. a clumped food distribution would be preferred to a Many animals adopt Lévy walk search strategies when
more scattered one). Secondly, ‘social information’ such food (or prey) is rare and unpredictable (Humphries et al.,
as the presence/absence/identity of other group members 2010), e.g. amoebas (Acanthamoeba castellanii) (Levandowsky,
(or other groups or predators) and finally ‘spatial and White & Schuster, 1997), fruitflies (Drosophila melanogaster)
temporal information’ (when the food is present/ripe and (Reynolds & Frye, 2007), wandering albatrosses (Diomedea
where it is located) are considered. Based on previous exulans) (Viswanathan et al., 1996, 1999), jackals (Canis adustus)
experience the forager will use this information to develop (Atkinson et al., 2002), reindeer (Rangifer tarandus tarandus)
particular decision rules (Garber et al., 2009). Some sets of (Mårell, Ball & Hofgaard, 2002) and also hunter–gatherer
rules are likely to be relatively simple, integrating limited humans (Ju/’hoansi hunters and foragers; Brown, Liebovitch
amounts of social and ecological information, and represent & Glendon, 2007; Raichlen et al., 2014).
so-called ‘rules of thumb’ (e.g. for subordinate individuals: In non-human primates, Lévy walk patterns have been
the avoidance of any feeding site that is currently occupied observed in spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi; Boyer et al., 2006):
by more-dominant individuals; Bugnyar & Heinrich, 2006). their walks, consisting of a sum of independent and infrequent
Other rules are likely to be more complex since they integrate steps, seem to represent an optimal searching strategy to find
several types of context-specific information and thus require scarce resources. In bearded saki monkeys (Chiropotes spp.),
greater computational abilities. For example, if after a given waiting times were consistent with a Lévy pattern (i.e. many
period of time the quantity of food in a given patch has not low-quality patches and a few high-quality patches) while
fallen below a critical level, a satiated individual will tolerate their step lengths during foraging were more consistent
additional foragers (Garber et al., 2009). with a Brownian pattern (as resources had a more random
We now aim to reviewing the spatial and temporal distribution) (Shaffer, 2014). Moreover, the distribution of
foraging strategies described in primate species according to their turning angles indicated a high degree of directional
their different socio-ecology. For each strategy, we underline persistence between patches. Thus, the results suggest that
advantages and disadvantages in terms of optimising their they encode the locations of high-quality food patches
energy balance. and minimise travel time between them. They also feed
opportunistically from abundant and randomly distributed
(1) Random walks models: Lévy walks lower quality patches en route (Shaffer, 2014).
The activity budget of an animal will be affected by
(2) The strategy of re-using the same routes
resource availability and distribution (Jager et al., 2011).
To be able to exploit food resources that are scattered An alternative strategy to the random-walk is to re-use the
in space, an efficient strategy is to perform a random walk same routes. A clear definition of what really constitutes a
(Codling, Plank & Benhamou, 2008). Prominent examples ‘route’ has not yet been formulated since its length or use
of random walk strategies are Brownian walks and Lévy is species dependent and thus generalisations are difficult
walks that are conceptual tools for modelling animal search (Noser & Byrne, 2013). The term ‘route’ has been used in
paths (Codling et al., 2008). In these strategies both the literature referring to humans (e.g. Brunyé & Taylor, 2008),
direction and step length of the constituent moves are drawn large mammals (e.g. Call et al., 2008) and insects (e.g. Lent,
from a probability distribution (Codling et al., 2008). The Graham & Collett, 2009), but less frequently to non-human
movement patterns differ in the distribution of step lengths. primates. We can assimilate the concept of ‘route’ to that
In Brownian walks, step lengths l j are derived from an of ‘pathway’: it represents an established animal path or
exponential distribution and have a characteristic scale, movement, i.e. it represents the ‘fossilised’ traces of a
usually defined by the first and second moment (the mean movement path (Benhamou, 2010). Since it is essential that
and the variance, respectively) of the step length density the traces are fossilised, a path cannot be travelled only once
distribution P(l j ). In Lévy walks, step lengths are derived to become a route. It is also very difficult to define the length
from a power-law distribution (where many short steps are of a route precisely: for example, Di Fiore & Suarez (2007)
occasionally alternated with a long one) and they have no found that 95% of the locations visited by white-bellied spider
characteristic scale (Viswanathan et al., 1999). Lévy walks monkeys (Ateles belzebuth) over several months fell within

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


Spatial foraging decisions in primates 5

50 m of a repeatedly travelled route, whereas Sigg & Stolba cues (Rodrigo, 2002). In fact, the Aka hunter–gatherer
(1981) felt that a ‘buffer zone’ of 500 m was appropriate populations of the Central African Republic seem to use
to define a repeatedly travelled ‘street segment’ in a group path integration together with the positions of landmarks
of hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas). Moreover, routes (presence of honey bee nests on trees, or positions of logs or
are not necessarily used exclusively by one species, since particularly large old trees such as those of the species Lophira
they are paths that facilitate transfer (e.g. avoiding obstacles alata, Omphalocarpum elatum, Dialium polyanthum, Pachyelasma
such as dense vegetation, rivers, difficult terrain, etc.) and tessmanii, etc.; S. Masi, unpublished observations).
are not necessarily the shortest path to a destination, but
are convenient. Routes can be a clearly visible path through (4) Landmark navigation
the vegetation or they may be less well defined or hidden.
Milton (2000) hypothesised that route travelling is a common If visual cues are distant from the goal, and not directly
trait among primates; indeed re-use of the same paths by associated with it, it is necessary to remember the distance
wild primates occurs in distantly related species, such as and direction from the landmark to the goal, to calculate
proboscis monkeys (Nasalis larvatus) in Borneo (Boonratana, one’s position relative to the landmark, and to infer one’s
2000), howler monkeys (Alouatta spp.) in Nicaragua (Garber & position relative to the goal (Rodrigo, 2002). Similarly to
Jelinek, 2006) and in Barro Colorado Island Panama (Milton, path integration, such landmark navigation has been shown
2000), and eastern chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweifurthii) in in bees (Cartwright & Collett, 1982), other insects (Collett,
Uganda, that re-use parts of their routes at least twice a year 1992), and also in mammals (Collett, Cartwright & Smith,
(Bertolani, 2013). The travelling paths of Borneo orangutans 1986). A study in captive capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)
(Pongo pygmaeus) were described as arboreal ‘highways’ re-used showed that they can only learn simple associations between
by several individuals (MacKinnon, 1974). Data collected a goal and a local landmark; they were not able to consider
over 8 years on sympatric groups of spider monkeys (Ateles distances from all possible goals in order to choose the shortest
belzebuth) and woolly monkeys (Lagothrix poeppigii) (Di Fiore & one (Potì, 2000). However, captive cotton-top tamarins
Suarez, 2007) revealed that the animals consistently re-used (Saguinus oedipus oedipus) rely on the spatial relationship or
the same routes and that most feeding trees were within positions of multiple landmarks to locate hidden food items
25 m of a network of routes that ran mostly along ridges, in their environment (Dolins, 2009).
following the topography of the study site (Di Fiore & Suarez, Studies on spatial orientation in humans have shown that
2007). Using habitual paths that cross commonly used food the use of landmarks is dependent on the ecological context
patches and landmarks facilitates foraging and reduces the in which they live (Byrne, 1979; Thorndyke & Hayes-Roth,
cognitive effort necessary for spatial navigation (Bertolani, 1982). It seems that Western men travelling in a city
2013), but will have a cost in terms of the reduced likelihood follow landmarks and are unable to provide clear directions
of discovery of potential new food resources. when mapping their pathways (Byrne, 1979; Thorndyke &
Hayes-Roth, 1982); on the other hand, aboriginal people,
(3) Path integration who inhabit more open environments, are more accurate in
determining the direction of places that can be over 300 km
According to Gallistel (1990) animals may navigate by away (Lewis, 1976). Hunter–gatherer populations show also
combining two sorts of position vectors: egocentric vectors, sex-specific navigational strategies: while men prefer to use
which specify the locations of terrain features (landmarks) in an orientation strategy based on cardinal directions, women
a body-centred coordinate system, and geocentric vectors, prefer a landmark strategy to orientate themselves (e.g. Choi
which specify the position of the animal in an earth-centred & Silverman, 1996, 2003; Dabbs et al., 1998; Lawton &
coordinate system. The geocentric coordinate system is Kallai, 2002). De Lillo, Kirby & James (2014) studied the
created by dead reckoning, or path integration, and has ability of Western humans to keep track of a set of spatial
been studied in many animal taxa such as insects [bees locations while travelling. This ability depended on whether
and ants (Collett, 1996; Dyer, 1996; Wehner, Michel & the order in which the locations were explored was consistent
Antonsen, 1996)], rats (Benhamou, 1997; Maaswinkel & with certain organisation principles: short movements were
Whishaw, 1999) and other mammals (Séguinot, Cattet & associated with better memory recall and required less time
Benhamou, 1998; Etienne & Jeffery, 2004). Path integration to perform (De Lillo et al., 2014). These results indicate that,
is the process of continuously updating one’s own position in at least in Western populations, organisational factors (i.e.
space, by keeping track of, and integrating, all distances and spatial configuration, e.g. in clusters, in a matrix etc.) may
changes of direction. Path integration has been particularly play a large role in their spatial orientation ability.
well studied in insects, such as bees, ants, and spiders
(Wehner & Srinivasan, 2003). The mechanism is not precise,
(5) Cognitive maps: acquisition, storage and
however, because errors accumulate, both randomly and
organisation of information
systematically, and hence adjustments need to be made
regularly (Muller & Wehner, 1988). For this reason, path Linear travel paths to distant (out-of-sight) food resources
integration is unlikely to be the only orientation system used. have traditionally been used as evidence that animals had a
In both invertebrates and mammals, it is likely integrated goal when initiating travel (Garber, 1989; Janson & Di Bitetti,
with other mechanisms such as navigation based on visual 1997; Janson, 1998; Janmaat, Byrne & Zuberbulher, 2006a;

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


6 C. Trapanese and others

Valero & Byrne, 2007; Noser & Byrne, 2007a,b; Cunningham information provided by landmarks, thus distances are more
& Janson, 2007b; Luhrs et al., 2009; Normand & Boesch, approximate. In the case of the Euclidean map, animals do
2009). However, in response to a degree of unpredictability not tend to re-use pathways, but reach the same point from
in food availability, animals, including primates, will have different directions (Normand & Boesch, 2009). They use
a dynamic relationship with their habitat. They can use short-cuts that connect two resources which correspond to
straight-line routes but may deviate from them according changes in direction. A Euclidean mental map is considered
to the situation, either because of anthropogenic [e.g. to be the most flexible and therefore the most efficient
construction of a road, deforestation (Tutin, 1999; Hockings, way to navigate; it is also more efficient in less well-known
2007; Hockings, Anderson & Matzusawa, 2012); availability areas (Gallistel & Cramer, 1996). There is no clear evidence
of human crops (e.g. Krief et al., 2014)] or natural causes regarding the use of such maps in any non-human species.
[e.g. seasonality (Brockman & van Schaik, 2005); landscape In human children, some sort of Euclidian notion emerges
features (e.g. rivers); predation pressure; decision to exploit at around 6 or 7 years of age, allowing more complex
a different resource along the route (Cunningham & Janson, and flexible spatial operations (Piaget & Inhelder, 1956).
2007a)]. The different strategies used by primates thus involve However, its development has been shown to be a function
adjustments to the changing environment and depend on the of socioecological conditions (Parker, 1993; Wassmann &
species and their ecological niche. Dasen, 1998; Boesch, 2007), thus experiments in other
At the highest level of complexity, primates may use human societies may provide different results. Given that
abstract mental representations to acquire, store and decode some cognitive abilities of chimpanzees are comparable to
spatial, temporal and ecological information, thus relying those of 3-year-old children (Woodruf & Premack, 1979),
on what Gallistel & Cramer (1996) called a ‘global cognitive evidence for the use of Euclidean maps in chimpanzees in
map’. The concept of a cognitive map is not used consistently Taï, Ivory Coast (Pan troglodytes verus; Normand & Boesch,
in the existing literature; for example, Tolman (1948, p. 189) 2009) was surprising and follow-up studies are needed to
defined it as ‘an internal representation (or image) of external confirm this ability. The use of such sophisticated mental
environmental feature or landmark’, while O’Keefe & Nadel organisation in non-human primates is therefore still under
(1978, p. 86) defined it as ‘the representation of a group of debate. However, it has been suggested that a possible
places, some related to others by means of a set of rules advantage to chimpanzees of using Euclidean maps is to
of spatial transformation’. Modern researchers refer mainly increase their travel efficiency in the context of tool use
to two different types of cognitive maps (Garber, 2000): (Normand & Boesch, 2009). Tool use in these chimpanzees
a route-based map, also called a network or topological requires a precise knowledge of different stone hammer
map, and a Euclidean map, which is coordinate-based or locations (Boesch & Boesch, 1984) and an accurate tool
geometric. The term ‘mental map’ is often used to refer to selection (in terms of size and shape) that together imply
cognitive maps in general, however sometimes this term has an ability to anticipate future events (Sirianni, Mundry &
been erroneously restricted to Euclidean maps (e.g. Tolman, Boesch, 2015). Normand & Boesch (2009) analysed the
1948; O’Keefe & Nadel, 1978).We suggest that the term travel speed, directions, route used and the linearity index
‘route-based map’ (Poucet, 1993) is widely adopted for the (the direct distance between feeding sites divided by the
first type of map: a topological map in which animals use distance travelled) as a measure of goal-reaching efficiency
landmarks to navigate and to approximate local information of chimpanzees moving through a forest habitat (Valero &
about direction and distance (Asensio et al., 2011). They Byrne, 2007; Noser & Byrne, 2007a,b). Their results were
can use qualitative information about connections and more consistent with the use of a detailed Euclidean map
destinations to memorise a finite number of routes, a network, than a topological map. However, it remains possible that
which is then used repeatedly to reach the resources in chimpanzees could combine a Euclidean map with other
relation to their position. In this graph-structured network, navigation systems (e.g. landmark use, route-based map, etc.).
vertices (also called nodes) represent locations (landmarks) Recently, the presence of a Euclidean map in rats has been
that are well defined and well known (Schkolkopf & Mallot, supported by the discovery of neurons in the hippocampus
1995). The second type of cognitive map, we propose to refer that fire when the animal is in a particular place, regardless of
to as a Euclidean map (O’Keefe & Nadel, 1978) as this is used its field of view, and even in complete darkness (Eichenbaum,
most frequently in the literature, although ‘coordinate-based 2014; Eichenbaum & Cohen, 2014). This neuronal activity
map’ (Garber & Dolins, 2014) would perhaps be more could reveal the capacity to store specific spatial information
intuitive. A Euclidean map is a detailed geometrical map in the brain independently from visual cues. Moreover,
in which the individual has a quantitative representation Killian, Jutras & Buffalo (2012) and Killian, Potter & Buffalo
of the entire environment including the metric angles and (2015) showed that grid cells in the primate entorhinal
distances. The nervous system contains such information cortex represent and encode positions in visual space. This
as position vectors and can perform basic computations observation suggests that primates might be able to encode
with these vectors, such as rotating them and adding them a representation of their environment in order to memorise
(Gallistel & Cramer, 1996). Other simpler strategies (e.g. a valuable resources.
topological map) do not require such precise organisation Although efficient travel between out-of-sight resources
but they benefit from the additional visual and qualitative can be achieved by both a route-based map and Euclidean

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


Spatial foraging decisions in primates 7

map, the two types of map result in different predicted was available after a short interval, but not after a long
travelling patterns: in a Euclidean model, routes are not one. Recently, spatial memory skills have been studied
consistently re-used because each travelling event results from in four captive strepsirrhine primates that vary in level
a novel calculation aimed at finding the best path (Byrne, of frugivory (Rosati et al., 2014). The results showed that
1979). In addition, under a Euclidean model changes in species with the greatest ecological disparity exhibited the
direction occur at the start and end points of each travelling most variation in spatial memory skills: recall after a long
event, while under a route-based map hypothesis a significant delay, learning mechanisms supporting memory, and recall
proportion of changes in direction are expected to occur as of multiple locations in a complex environment (Rosati et al.,
a result of a reorientation process (Bertolani, 2013). 2014). The most frugivorous species, ruffed lemur (Varecia
sp.), consistently showed a more robust spatial memory than
the other species (ring-tailed lemur Lemur catta, mongoose
lemur Eulemur mongoz, sifaka Propithecus coquereli) across tasks,
III. HOW TO INFER THE MENTAL PROCESSES particularly in comparison with sifakas, the most folivorous
UNDERLYING PRIMATE MOVEMENTS species.
Other studies addressed only the when component, testing
(1) Experiments in captivity memory for food locations. Capuchin monkeys (Sapajus cay;
In comparison to studies in wild habitats, experiments on Tujague et al., 2015) showed long-term spatial memory
captive primates enable the control and manipulation of revealing a capacity to remember the positions of three
external variables and the spatio-temporal availability of accessible and non-accessible baited sites for periods up to
resources, thus allowing detailed investigations of primate 4 months. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) were also able to
spatial navigation and spatial memory. An extensive remember food locations well after limited exposure, 24 h
summary of spatial foraging studies on captive primates and 3 months (Mendes & Call, 2014). In an earlier study,
is provided in Table 1. Menzel (1973) established the ability of chimpanzees to
The food choice behaviour of titi monkeys (Callicebus remember the positions of 18 different feeding sites. Their
moloch) and squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) was examined in searching pattern approximated an optimum route and they
captivity using an eight-arm maze under various conditions rarely re-checked a site they had already visited. Other
of spatial and temporal food availability (Andrews, 1988). captive apes, such as western gorillas (Gorilla gorilla) and
Both species avoided revisiting the same feeding sites. Borneo orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) similarly remember
Moreover, under conditions in which the goals differed food location while avoiding revisitation of depleted sites
in food abundance, both species tended to select goals [even those visited by other individuals (MacDonald, Pang
that maximised energy intake, even though this behavioural & Gibeault, 1994; MacDonald & Agnes, 1999; Gibeault &
adjustment involved a certain delay. Captive yellow-nosed MacDonald, 2000)]. Although in general the use of captive
monkeys (Cercopithecus ascanius whitesidei) showed a similar animals allows tighter control of environmental and social
ability to remember feeding locations and also applied variables, it may not always be possible to relate the results to
the least-distance strategy (minimising the total distance natural animal behaviour, and different studies use different
travelled) (MacDonald & Wilkie, 1990). experimental designs. Moreover, captive-reared primates
Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) showed higher search might not have acquired appropriate experience to develop
efficiency in a cluster organisation of food than in a their navigation abilities compared to their free-living
matrix, using a strategy to reduce memory requirements counterparts (Menzel & Beck, 2000), since environments in
for solving the task (De Lillo et al., 1997); a similar strategy captivity lack the complexity and unpredictability of natural
was used by tree shrews (Tupaia belangeri) when tested with contexts. Therefore, studies investigating spatial strategies
the same experimental protocol (Bartolomucci, de Biurrun using captive animals have often limited ecological validity
& Eberhard, 2001). In a follow-up experiment, capuchin and generalisation to the behaviour of wild animals is
monkeys (Cebus apella) showed a higher search efficiency problematic (Dolins et al., 2014).
when the search space featured either a linear (in which
case they used methodical search trajectories from one end (2) Manipulating or observing the natural
to the other of the linear array) or circular arrangement of environment: wild primates
containers, as opposed to a matrix or cross arrangement (De
(a) Field experiments
Lillo et al., 1998). These results suggest that capuchins use
either the travel distance or the associated cognitive costs to During last two decades, researchers have increasingly
organise their searching behaviour (De Lillo et al., 1998). investigated spatial foraging navigation directly in the
Considering the three variables what, where and when, field using methods developed in captive settings. This
Hampton et al. (2005) showed that the working memory approach is of particular interest because while retaining the
of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) was restricted to the possibility of controlling and manipulating some variables,
what and where components but did not include when. The it allows measurements to be made in wild animals in their
macaques could not differentiate between short and long natural environment. However, only a limited number of
delays: they seemed unable to learn that a preferred food variables can be controlled. Table 2 provides a summary

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


8

Table 1. Spatial foraging studies based on experiments on captive primates. Landmarks and/or other cues: type of landmarks, cues or beacons used in the study; – indicates
that they were not present or not specified in the study

Number of Number of feeding Landmarks and/


Reference Species tested subjects stations used or other cues Conclusions
Menzel (1973) Chimpanzee 6 18 – Optimised route (‘least-distance’ strategy)
(Pan troglodytes) and avoided revisiting the same feeding
stations
Andrews (1988) Titi monkey (Callicebus moloch), 8 C. moloch and 8 S. 8 Potential landmarks: plastic Visited the most rewarding sites; revisiting of
squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus barriers, buildings, vacant the same sites was not observed
sciureus) field cages, trees and
agricultural fields
MacDonald & Wilkie (1990) Yellow-nosed monkeys 2 1st experiment: 4 – Minimised the total distance travelled
(Cercopithecus ascanius whitesidei) 2nd experiment: 8 (‘least-distance’ strategy)
MacDonald (1994) Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) 2 8 – To minimise foraging effort, used a counting
strategy during the food-search phase
Cramer & Gallistel (1997) Vervet monkey (Cercopithecus 4 1st experiment: 4 – Chose the optimal route and planned their
aetiops) 2nd experiment: 6 movements three steps ahead
De Lillo, Visalberghi & Capuchin monkey (Cebus apella) 4 9 in clusters and 9 in – Minimised distances more when searching in
Aversano (1997) matrix a cluster configuration than in a matrix
De Lillo et al. (1998) Capuchin monkey (Cebus apella) 4 9 in circle, 9 in matrix, 9 – Search efficiency higher when searching
in cross and 9 in line either in a linear or a circular

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


configurations arrangement of food containers
MacDonald & Agnes (1999) Orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) 3 8 – Minimised expended energy by reducing
travelled distance
Gibeault & MacDonald Gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) 6 16 – Able to remember not only the sites they
(2000) depleted but also those depleted by
foraging partners
Hampton, Hampstaed & Rhesus macaque (Macaca mulatta) 10 3 – Remembered food locations for up to 25 h;
Murray (2005) failed to learn that preferred food was
available only after a short delay and not
after a long one
Dolins (2009) Cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus 17 1st experiment: 3 Geometric shapes at each To locate hidden food they relied on spatial
oedipus oedipus) 2nd experiment: 4 feeding station relationships among multiple landmarks
rather than on an associative strategy
Mendes & Call (2014) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) 12 4 – Remembered food locations after limited
exposure (1–2 trials), after 24 h and after
a 3-month retention interval
Rosati, Rodriguez & Hare Ruffed lemur (Varecia sp.), 1st experiment: 44 1st experiment: 2 Objects of different shapes The most frugivorous species (ruffed lemur),
(2014) ring-tailed lemur (Lemur catta), 2nd experiment: 48 2nd experiment: 2 and 8 and colours at each feeding consistently showed more robust spatial
mongoose lemur (Eulemur 3rd experiment: 47 station, only in experiment memory than the other species, especially
mongoz), Coquerel’s sifaka 3 in comparison with the most folivorous
(Propithecus coque reli) species, the sifaka
Tujague, Janson & Lahitte Capuchin monkey (Sapajus cay) 16 6 Visual and olfactory cues Showed a long-term memory (4 months) of
(2015) used as guides or beacons sites with accessible and non-accessible
food
C. Trapanese and others
Spatial foraging decisions in primates 9

of experimental field studies on spatial foraging in wild investigate the mechanisms involved in foraging (Janson &
primates. Byrne, 2007).
To investigate spatial memory and route planning in
wild primates, researchers carry out foraging experiments (b) Direct observations of wild primates: indicators of intentionality
by placing a number of baited feeding sites in strategic and the change-point test (CPT)
positions in a small- or large-scale array. Field experiments
in sanguinus monkeys (Aotus nigriceps), tamarins (Saguinus To investigate the cognitive processes underlying travel paths
imperator imperator and S. fuscicollis weddelli) and titi monkeys in wild animals and the spatial planning of their foraging
(Callicebus cupreus) showed that these species relocate food behaviour, indicators of intentionality are used to understand
rewards based solely on spatial information (site predictabil- whether primates have a goal in mind (Janson & Byrne,
ity) and on visual cues (Bicca-Marques & Garber, 2004). 2007). The degree of directedness in relation to a feeding
Similarly, white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus capucinus) site is one such indicator: movements should be straighter
towards larger and more-productive sites, e.g. a fruit tree
could remember the spatial positions of 13 feeding platforms
(Milton, 1981) or water hole (Altmann, 1974). Changes
using mainly spatial information (local landmarks; Garber
in travel speed could also signify that animals know in
& Paciulli, 1997). Janson (1998, 2007) found that a group of
advance the position of a goal site [e.g. in yellow baboons
brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) moved significantly
Papio hamadryas cynocephalus (Pochron, 2001, 2005), sooty
more often towards closer platforms, and in straighter lines
mangabeys Cercocebus atys atys and grey-cheeked mangabeys
than expected by any random search model, integrating
Lophocebus albigena johnstoni (Janmaat et al., 2006a)]. Changes
information on spatial location, reward, and perhaps poten-
in travel direction may also be important: foraging routes
tial competition in their choice of multi-site foraging routes.
are more likely to deviate at meaningful, preferred or
The food rewards of taking a particular foraging route
productive sites (Pochron, 2005; Valero & Byrne, 2007)
will depend on the number of individuals sharing the food
and at significant landmarks (e.g. nodes of a trail system,
along the way together with the individual dominance rank rivers, etc.; Dolins & Mitchell, 2010; Noser & Byrne, 2010)
(Janson, 1985): subordinates generally run ahead to reach rather than between such locations (Dolins & Mitchell, 2010;
unused resources first (Janson, 2007). In a recent follow-up Noser & Byrne, 2010). Table 3 provides a summary of
field experiment, Janson (2016) presented an array of eight studies on spatial foraging in wild primates using analytical
feeding stations with a progressive increase in the abundance approaches.
of food rewards in proportion to the time lapsed since the pre- Byrne et al. (2009) proposed a statistical procedure for
vious visit. Comparison of their observations with simulated determining intentionality: the change-point test (CPT). This
movements derived from statistical models showed that the procedure, based on permutation, calculates retrospectively
monkeys could remember and integrate three characteristics if a change in direction from a recorded step n to n + 1
of the feeding site: distance to the site (where), the elapsed time was significantly more abrupt than expected on average
since the previous visit (when), the rate of food renewal of the when permutating points. Change-points are considered as
site (what). Another study on a group of wild vervet monkeys points where the animals re-orient themselves. A study on a
(Chlorocebus pygerythrus; Teichroeb, 2015) revealed that when community of wild chimpanzees revealed that the majority
alone, individuals take the shortest possible path to visit all of direction changes occurred clearly in relation to the
of the feeding sites (consistent with the convex hull strategy, network of trails in order to re-use routes (Bertolani, 2013).
i.e. the smallest convex set containing the points). However, Moreover, application of the CPT showed that chimpanzees
in a competitive situation, their strategy varied according use long-term spatial memory to monitor large fruit trees
to their dominance rank: a cluster strategy was adopted by by remembering the location of abundantly fruiting trees
dominants (i.e. moving towards clumped close resources) and across different fruiting seasons (Janmaat et al., 2013a). In
the nearest-neighbour rule by subordinates (i.e. choice of the addition, chimpanzees use their memory to anticipate and
closest site not yet visited). In another similar task (Teichroeb revisit high-value fruit trees from long distances (Ban, Boesch
& Aguado, 2016), vervet monkeys prioritised high-reward & Janmaat, 2014). The nutrient content and the abundance
food sites over low-reward sites, accessing food sites in order of fruit indeed exerted a significant impact on female
of proximity (nearest-neighbour rule) when alone, while in travel paths of western chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus;
the presence of competitors they prioritised high-reward Ban et al., 2016); females were more likely to change their
sites, sometimes by-passing low-reward sites. Vervet mon- travel direction to reach rarer trees providing fruit with a
keys therefore can integrate distance/reward information higher fat content (Ban et al., 2016). The arboreal brachiating
over multiple feeding stations, show spatial discounting and gibbons (Hylobates lar) show goal-oriented travel compared
can optimise their path in different ways according to social to a heuristic travel model based on straight travel paths
context. in random directions, and change direction primarily at
In general, studies on wild primates allow the maintenance preferred fruit sources (Asensio et al., 2011). Applying the
of a degree of complexity similar to that of natural conditions, CPT to the travel route of a group of wild baboons (Papio
thus providing insights into the selection pressures involved ursinus) revealed that their directional changes represented (i)
in determining species navigation abilities. When possible, locations where they decided to return to their sleeping site,
a combination of captive and wild experiments is ideal to and (ii) locations close to important landmarks (e.g. change

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


10
Table 2. Field experiment studies on spatial foraging in wild primates. Landmarks and/or other cues: type of landmarks used in the study; – indicates not present or not
specified

Number of
Number of feeding Landmarks and/
Reference Species tested subjects stations used or other cues Conclusions
Garber & Paciulli Capuchin monkey (Cebus 10–12 13 Yellow blocks at each Used spatial information (local landmarks) to
(1997) capuchinus) feeding station remember feeding sites
Janson (1998) Capuchin monkey (Cebus 15 15 – Moved significantly more often towards the
apella) closest platforms in straighter lines than
expected by any random search model
Bicca-Marques & Night monkey (Aotus 3–4 A. nigriceps, 4–6 S. i. 8 Red light at each feeding All species relocated food rewards solely based
Garber (2004) nigriceps), tamarin, imperator, 4–5 S. f. station on spatial information; used visual cues to
(Saguinus imperator weddelli distinguish real food from sham food
imperator, S. fuscicollis 4–6 C. cupreus
weddelli), titi monkeys
(Callicebus cupreus)
Bicca-Marques & Tamarin (Saguinus imperator 16 4 Coloured blocks, In group-foraging context, integrated social
Garber (2005) and S. fuscicollis) red-and-yellow striped and ecological information in
posts at each feeding decision-making mechanisms
station
Janson (2007) Capuchin monkey (Cebus 41 3 – Preferred the closest site even if providing less

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


apella nigritus) food; to balance the energetic costs, they
chose a detour more often when the reward
was larger
Luhrs et al. (2009) Grey mouse lemur 8 7 – Using spatial cues, rapidly learnt a new spatial
(Microcebus murinus) arrangement of feeding stations; showed
high travel efficiency in directed movements
Teichroeb & Vervet monkey 24 7 Objects, beacons at each Located food above chance levels in the
Chapman (2014) (Chlorocebus pygerythrus) feeding station presence of goal-object cues and associative
cues; dominance of sight over smell when
foraging was found
Teichroeb (2015) Vervet monkey 21 6 – Individually: chose the shortest possible path in
(Chlorocebus pygerythrus) fewer than half of the trials (convex hull)
In social group: dominants used a cluster strategy
while subordinates used the
nearest-neighbour rule to reduce
competition
Janson (2016) Capuchin monkey (Cebus around 30∗ 8 – Feeding choices were consistent with dynamic
apella nigritus) memory for elapsed time specific to each site
Teichroeb & Aguado Vervet monkey 21 5 – Individually: accessed food sites in order of
(2016) (Chlorocebus pygerythrus) proximity (nearest-neighbour rule or a
convex hull)
In social group: prioritised high-reward sites
sometimes bypassing low-reward sites

Exact number of individuals not specified.
C. Trapanese and others
Table 3. Field studies on spatial foraging in wild primates according to the analytical approach used: (A) the Change Point Test, (B) the Circuity Index, (C) other statistical
models and/or comparison to computerised simulations, and (D) botanical or ecological knowledge. Evidenced mechanisms/knowledge: for A–C this column provides the
navigation mechanisms used by primates as suggested by the results of the study, for D this column indicates the ecological knowledge used by primates that was investigated
by the study

Mechanisms/
Number of knowledge evidenced
Reference Species study subjects by the study Conclusions
(A) The Change Point Test
Asensio et al. (2011) Gibbon (Hylobates lar) 11 groups, 3–6 Route-based map In a large-scale space their travel was goal oriented towards
individuals each most of the preferred resources
Noser & Byrne (2013) Baboon (Papio ursinus) 25 Route-based map Directional changes corresponded to locations where the
decision to return to their sleeping site was taken or near
important landmarks
Spatial foraging decisions in primates

Janmaat, Ban & Boesch (2013a) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) 5 Long-term spatial Used long-term spatial memory to monitor large fruit trees,
memory remembering this information also across fruiting seasons
Ban et al. (2016) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) 5 Spatial memory Changed travel direction at rare and fat-rich fruit trees
Joly & Zimmermann (2011) Mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus) 7 Spatial memory or Change-points coincided with out-of-sight keystone food
olfactory cues resources; travel paths were goal oriented; insufficient
evidence for a presence of a route-based map
(B) The Circuity Index
Garber & Hannon (1993) Moustached tamarin (Saguinus 1 mixed group∗ Spatial memory and Although likely to rely also on olfactory cues, used a detailed
mystax), saddleback tamarin olfactory navigation spatial map to minimise distance between scattered feeding
(Sanguinus fuscicollis) sites
Porter & Garber (2013) Weddell’s saddleback tamarin 5 Route-based map Navigated at large scales using a route-based spatial
(Sanguinus fuscicollis weddelli) representation
Garber & Porter (2013) Weddell’s saddleback tamarin 5 Route-based map Used route-based travel near to goal landmarks to relocate and
(Sanguinus fuscicollis weddelli) monitor food in small-scale space
Suarez et al. (2014) Spider monkey (Ateles belzebuth) 19 Route-based map Foraging and ranging behaviour was goal-oriented
(C) Other quantitative statistical models and/or comparison to computerised simulations
Boyer et al. (2006) Spider monkey (Ateles geoffroyi) 1 group∗ Lévy walk Probably use Lévy walks to find randomly distributed resources
Di Fiore & Suarez (2007) Spider monkey (Ateles belzebuth). 16–23 A. belzebuth, Route-based map Both species use landmarks to navigate and likely memorise a
woolly monkey (Lagothrix poeppigii) 18–25 L. poeppigii series of route segments to forage efficiently
Noser & Byrne (2007a) Baboon (Papio ursinus) 25 Route-based map Considering time and energy costs, lacked the ability to
compute Euclidean relations among locations; instead using
landmarks to navigate
Noser & Byrne (2007b) Baboon (Papio ursinus) 25 Route-based map Planned journey choosing among several out-of-sight resources;
approached food sites in an efficient, goal-directed way
Valero & Byrne (2007) Spider monkey (Ateles geoffroyi 20 Spatial memory Travelled in straight lines when moving between resources and
yucatanensis) likely planned their movements in advance
Erhart & Overdoff (2008) Lemurs (Propithecus edwardsi, Eulemur 10 P. edwardsi and 7 Route-based map Both species preferred nearest-neighbour food patches and P.
fulvus rufus) E. f. rufus edwardsi travelled in relatively straight lines
Normand & Boesch (2009) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) 15 Euclidean map (possibly) Travelled in a straight line to reach food resources from many
different directions; reduction in the distance travelled at the
periphery of the home range
Noser & Byrne (2010) Baboon (Papio ursinus) 25 Route-based map Cognitive maps either contained information on relatively few
trees or on a single route along which several trees were
situated
Janmaat & Chancellor (2010) Grey-cheeked mangabey (Lophocebus 20–28 Long-term spatial Foraging efficiency was influenced by spatial memory and
albigena johnstoni) memory knowledge acquired via long-term exposure

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


11
12

Table 3. Continued

Mechanisms/
Number of knowledge evidenced
Reference Species study subjects by the study Conclusions
Janmaat et al. (2014) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) 5 Future-oriented cognitive Left sleeping nests earlier when breakfasting on highly
mechanisms ephemeral fruits, especially when they were located at a
distance
Shaffer (2014) Bearded saki (Chiropotes sagulatus) 40–50 Lévy walk Step length consistent with a Brownian walk pattern; waiting
times consistent with a Lévy walk
Schreier & Grove (2014) Baboon (Papio hamadryas) 110 Route-based map ‘Tipping points’ (landmarks) used as transition between
movements in small-versus large-scale space
Hopkins (2016) Manteld howler monkey (Alouatta 18 Spatial and temporal Maximised food gain while minimising distance travelled
palliata) knowledge
(D) Botanical or ecological knowledge

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


Olupot, Waser, & Chapman (1997) Grey-cheeked mangabey (Lophocebus 15 Frugivorous bird calls Possibly use the calls of hornbills to locate fruiting trees
albigena)
Janmaat et al. (2006a) Sooty mangabey (Cercocebus atys atys), 95–100 C. a. atys and Spatial memory and tree Fruiting trees were approached significantly faster than trees
grey-cheeked mangabey 20–25 L. a. johnstoni phenology without fruit
(Lophocebus albigena johnstoni)
Janmaat, Byrne & Zuberbulher Grey-cheeked mangabey (Lophocebus 18–24 Weather conditions Took into account past weather conditions when searching for
(2006b) albigena johnstoni) food
Janmaat et al. (2012) Grey-cheeked mangabey (Lophocebus 20–27 Tree phenology Probably used fruiting synchrony to find available fruit
albigena)
Janmaat, Ban & Boesch (2013b) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) 5 Tree phenology Used fruiting synchrony and based their expectations of finding
fruit on a combination of different botanical knowledge (e.g.
categorisation of fruit species, rates of fruit discovery)
Janmaat et al. (2016) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) 3 groups∗ Botanical and ecological Remembered the quantity and frequency of fruit production
knowledge across years; flexibly planned revisiting time to feeding trees
via efficient travel


Exact number of individuals not specified.
C. Trapanese and others
Spatial foraging decisions in primates 13

of slope, roads) where they adjusted their direction (Noser & not randomly distributed (Joly & Zimmermann, 2007). This
Byrne, 2013). The travel to out-of-sight resources of solitary study provides, to our knowledge, the first evidence for
and nocturnal mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus) appeared to the relocation of stationary food resources in nature and
be planned ahead since their strategy was more efficient at thereby, for spatial memory in a strepsirrhine primate. A
detecting resources than a heuristic random travel model. In follow-up study on the same species revealed that, primarily
this case too, change-points coincided with food resources using spatial cues, mouse lemurs rapidly learn the spatial
(Joly & Zimmermann, 2011). However, the authors were arrangement of new feeding sites (Luhrs et al., 2009). They
unable to exclude fully the use of olfactory cues to locate showed high travel efficiency in directed movements and
resources despite the large distances at which the change of the presence of a detailed mental representation of their
direction occurred. surroundings. Route-based goal-oriented travel was also
observed in wild lemurs (Propithecus edwardsi and Eulemur fulvus
(c) An alternative method: the Circuity Index rufus; Erhart & Overdoff, 2008) and in wild tamarins (Saguinus
mystax and Sanguinus fuscicollis weddelli; Garber & Hannon,
Garber & Hannon (1993) used another method for 1993; Garber & Porter, 2013). The tamarins seem to pay
determining intentionality, the Circuity Index, or the actual attention to near-to-goal landmarks to relocate and monitor
distance travelled divided by the straight-line distance food availability in small-scale space and to minimise the
between sites. They studied the foraging patterns of travelling distance between scattered feeding sites. However,
moustached tamarins (Saguinus mystax) and saddleback to locate resources in small-scale space primates may also rely
tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis): the tamarins’ foraging routes on olfactory cues. By comparing the animals’ movements
were relatively direct despite discontinuities in the canopy, with different possible orientation mechanisms, it has been
detours due to physical impediments and the risk of meeting shown that baboons (Papio ursinus) plan their journeys among
predators. Later, Porter & Garber (2013) and Garber several out-of-sight resources in an efficient, goal-directed
& Porter (2013) used the Circuity Index to investigate way (Noser & Byrne, 2007a,b). These findings suggest that
movements between out-of-sight goals in saddleback baboons have a spatial mental representation of important
tamarins (Sanguinus fuscicollis weddelli): they tended to travel locations, i.e. possess a mental representation of several
in both small and large-scale space using a route-based alternative resources situated beyond their perceptual field,
map, and certain areas of the forest appeared to serve thus allowing them to reach each of them. However, they
as switch points. Both spider and woolly monkeys (Ateles appear to be strongly tied to a network of learned sequences
belzebuth and Lagothrix poeppigii; Di Fiore & Suarez, 2007) of landmarks and lack the ability to compute Euclidean
use preferred routes that seem to be associated with relations among sites (Noser & Byrne, 2007a,b), rather using
distinct topographic features (i.e. ridge-tops). In both a route-based map to find out-of-sight locations. Comparison
species, these environmental features may constitute easily between foraging movements of spider monkey (Ateles geoffroyi
recognised landmarks useful for spatial navigation via a yucatanensis) in the wild and computer-generated simulations
route-based map. revealed that they also can plan to reach more than one
resource site (Valero & Byrne, 2007; Suarez et al., 2014).
(d) Quantitative statistical models based on direct observations By testing several heuristic models of decision making
as possible alternative strategies (i.e. movement to core
The newest approach to the study of wild primate foraging home-range areas instead of individual trees; travel along
strategies relies on comparisons between observational a sensory gradient; movement along arboreal pathway
data from wild primates and predictions from quantitative networks without a pre-determined destination; straight-line
statistical models. These predictions are often derived from travel in a randomly chosen direction; random walks),
null models of random searches, including factors such Hopkins (2016) showed that mantled howler monkeys
as the distribution of distances moved, the distribution of (Alouatta palliata) maximise resource gain while minimising
the angles turned between movements (and possibly the distance travelled via the integration of spatial and temporal
interdependence between them), and the ‘tortuosity’, i.e. knowledge of food availability. For all these species, the
the convolutedness of the path, which is inversely related authors rejected the null hypothesis of random foraging
to the efficiency of the orientation mechanism involved walks but all seem to lack the capacity for vector operations
(see Sections II.3 and II.4) (Benhamou, 2004; Janson & characteristic of Euclidean maps. Instead they rather orien-
Byrne, 2007). This approach has revealed that several wild tate themselves using a limited number of well-established
primate species are likely to know where they are going, routes.
remember where their resources are, and can generally
optimise their travel (e.g. Janson, 1998; Pochron, 2001;
Janmaat et al., 2006a,b; Cunningham & Janson, 2007a; (e) Botanical or ecological knowledge: a complementary or an
additional strategy?
Noser & Byrne, 2013). These studies show that a topological
route-based model best explains how primates find a suitable In tropical forests, primates could complement their use of
route and orientate in their landscape. Grey mouse lemurs sensory cues with other foraging strategies to facilitate the
(Microcebus murinus) regularly revisit the same feeding sites, discovery of newly emerged fruits (Janmaat et al., 2013b).
and when leaving their nest, their departure directions are Primates may know where a food source is located, but use

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


14 C. Trapanese and others

other sensory cues to define more narrowly what is and is other taxa. Taken together these studies reveal a cognitive
not edible (Dominy et al., 2001). Sensory acuity declines with mechanism, behavioural planning, by which large-brained
distance; thus, primates have developed a keen spatial and primates could buffer the effects of seasonal variability in food
temporal memory for food location and edibility (Garber, availability and inter-specific competition while foraging.
2000). Thus, some primates know where the resources are
(Garber, 1989; Garber & Paciulli, 1997; Janson, 2007), (3) Primate navigation in virtual reality
when to look for them (Janmaat et al., 2006a,b) and what or
how much is likely to be available (Cunningham & Janson, New technologies can now be used to investigate comparative
2007a,b). For example, studies on grey-cheeked mangabeys spatial cognitive abilities in non-human primates and
(Lophocebus albigena) suggested that they (i) may use the calls humans. A virtual reality software program simulates an
of hornbills to locate fruit (Olupot et al., 1997), (ii) seem to experimental situation that can vary in features, scale,
take into account past weather conditions when searching and complexity concurrently allowing precise control over
for food, and are more likely to approach trees with fruit at variables such as landmarks (e.g. De Lillo & James, 2012;
various stages of development than empty trees of the same Dolins et al., 2014). Changes to geometry of space could
species (Janmaat et al., 2006b), and (iii) seem to use cues allow more precise assessment of the subject’s attention to
related to synchronous fruiting during foraging (Janmaat various topographical features (De Lillo & James, 2012). The
et al., 2012). The latter strategy allows prediction of the use of virtual reality also allows researchers to obtain data
fruiting state of a large number of trees of the same species with large sample sizes relatively rapidly. Table 4 provides
without having to check specific trees (Janmaat et al., 2012). a summary of studies that used virtual reality to investigate
In a follow-up study, Janmaat et al. (2013b) found that navigation abilities in primates.
chimpanzees may use botanical skills to plan and search for Mushiake et al. (2000) carried out one of the first studies
food resources. Western chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of this kind. In a maze ‘path-finding’ computerised task, two
seem to combine knowledge of the synchronous emergence Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) chose the shortest path
of rainforest fruits with sensory cues, such as sight and smell. rather than approaching the target randomly. Moreover,
They base their expectations of finding fruit on a combina- when some paths in the maze were blocked, they chose a new
tion of knowledge that includes prior success rates of fruit shortest path, bypassing the obstacles. More recently, a study
discovery and categorisation of fruit species (Janmaat et al., comparing chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and human attention
2013b). The study suggested that chimpanzees possess some to visual–spatial features during navigation revealed that
ability to classify food trees, remembering their quantity both species discriminated efficiently between positive and
and frequency of fruit production across the year (Janmaat negative landmarks, and used relatively efficient navigation
et al., 2013a,b). They might also have a sophisticated ability to successfully localise goal sites (Dolins et al., 2014). In the
to plan revisiting of fruit trees flexibly, maximising travel most complex maze, human performance was less accurate
efficiency (Janmaat et al., 2014). Similarly, interviews with compared to that of one female chimpanzee. This result could
hunter–gatherer Aka people suggested that they rely on arise from a loss of capacity in humans due to our modern
botanical information to infer seasonal variables, including lifestyle, or may simply have arisen due to the very small size
the availability of fruit on other forest plant species (e.g. the of the sample (4 chimpanzees, 16 humans). However, the
presence of new leaves on certain tree species, for example results suggest that chimpanzees and humans do learn and
the young red leaves of Lophira alata, appear just before respond to virtual environments similarly. Interestingly, age
the beginning of the fruiting season; S. Masi, unpublished differences were found in the human subjects (Dolins et al.,
observations). 2014). An increasing data set from capuchin monkeys (Cebus
apella), baboons (Papio papio), and humans now indicates clear
species differences in the perception of the global spatial
(f ) Planning ahead: integrating ecological variables to assess behaviour
organisation of a collection of local elements in foraging tasks
goals
(e.g. Deruelle & Fagot, 1997, 1998; Fagot & Deruelle, 1997;
Janmaat et al. (2014) found that chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes De Lillo et al., 2005, 2011).
verus) leave their sleeping nests earlier when breakfasting In a computerised virtual task, capuchin monkeys (Cebus
on highly ephemeral fruits, especially when they are apella), but not rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta), showed a
located at a distance. In addition, females positioned their significant preference for a densely arranged array over a
sleeping nests in the direction of the breakfast sites of the sparsely arranged one (Parrish et al., 2017). The difference in
next day more often for ephemeral fruit than for widely terms of sensitivity to density was linked to a difference in the
available (predictable) fruit. Evidence of planning ability perceptual processing of multi-element arrays: capuchins
beyond immediate needs was also found in male Sumatran perceive individual items (i.e. local features) prior to
orangutans (Pongo abelii; van Schaik, Damerius & Isler, 2013). and sometimes more accurately than global configuration
The direction in which a male emits ‘long calls’ in the (Spinozzi, De Lillo & Truppa, 2003; De Lillo et al., 2005).
morning predicts his subsequent travel direction for many By allowing us to obtain a large data set rapidly, virtual
hours, and a new call indicates a change in his travel reality could lead to a better understanding of the cognitive
direction. The authors argue that the ability to plan in mechanisms underlying spatial foraging cognition in apes,
this way is likely to be adaptive for great apes, as well as monkeys and other animals.

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


Spatial foraging decisions in primates 15

Table 4. Studies on virtual reality navigation in primates. Number of stimuli: number of virtual goals or cues for navigation

Number of
Reference Species tested subjects Number of stimuli Conclusions
Mushiake et al. (2000) Japanese macaque 2 12 dots (goals) Chose the shortest route to the
(Macaca fuscata) targets
Dolins et al. (2014) Chimpanzee (Pan 4 P. troglodytes, 8 variable goal locations, Both species able to discriminate
troglodytes) and 16 H. sapiens directional cues between positive and negative
human (Homo sapiens) provided by positive Landmarks; in contrast to
and negative landmarks chimpanzees and adult humans,
performance in human children
decreased as maze complexity
and size increased
Parrish, James & Capuchin monkey 15 S. apella and 2 patches with equal Capuchins showed a density bias in
Beran (2017) (Sapajus apella), 7 M. mulatta number of dots (N = 12 discrimination of dense versus
rhesus macaque or 5) and 2 patches with sparse stimuli but macaques did
(Macaca mulatta) unequal number of dots not
(N = 5 versus 6 and
N = 10 versus 12)

IV. GENERAL DISCUSSION AND PERSPECTIVES of more parsimonious and testable explanations (Bertolani,
2013). Neither is there convincing evidence of complex rules
The available results from al the studies discussed above used to plan multi-step travel routes (Poucet, 1993; Gallistel
suggest that primates can remember the locations of food & Cramer, 1996; Janson, 2014). The most parsimonious
resources (e.g. Menzel, 1973; Janson, 1998, 2016; Pochron, hypothesis is the use of a less cognitively demanding
2001; Janmaat et al., 2006a). Studies also agree in showing route-based map by primates (e.g. Byrne, 2000; Di Fiore
that primate travel across spatially fixed resources is often & Suarez, 2007; Noser & Byrne, 2007a) which relies on
goal-oriented: routes taken between known resources often landmarks and a network of established routes.
approximate straight lines (e.g. Janson, 1998; Pochron, The use of statistical models to analyse field data on
2001; Di Fiore & Suarez, 2007; Valero & Byrne, 2007; animal movements has greatly increased our understanding
Cunningham & Janson, 2007a,b; Noser & Byrne, 2007a,b). of primates’ spatial movement strategies. However, by
Researchers often interpret primate foraging strategies in allowing investigation of only one behaviour at time, even
terms of enhancing fitness (Janson & Byrne, 2007): either this approach sometimes fails to identify the mechanisms
by minimising the distance travelled by visiting the closest involved precisely (Janson, 1998).
available resources (e.g. Garber, 1989; Janson, 1998), or Finally, spatial movement decisions may not necessitate
by minimising the energy expenditure in multi-destination the complex planning required by optimal solutions (such as
routes, opportunistically skipping some nearby resources in the Travelling Salesman Problem), but will always require
(Gallistel & Cramer, 1996; Cramer & Gallistel, 1997). processing of multiple sources of information to locate food.
Primates can choose to minimise the energy expenditure Wild primates face challenges involving seasonally variable
[e.g. avoiding hills, slopes, long-distance travel (Suarez, 2003; environments, and thus learn through exploration, encoding,
Di Fiore & Suarez, 2007)] or to reduce the computational recalling travel routes, orienting themselves, monitoring food
demands by using habitual travel routes (Di Fiore & Suarez, availability and finding the best strategy to minimise energy
2007; Noser & Byrne, 2007a,b). Monitoring food resources expenditure and maximise efficiency. Movements observed
(Janmaat et al., 2006a,b) allows some primates to return in frugivorous primates have been explained in terms of
to a patch only when it is fully recovered (Janson, 1990, the ‘gravity rule’ which assumes that every goal has an
1996), thus reducing the daily overlap in foraging areas attraction to the animal user proportional to the goal’s
(Di Bitetti, 2001). They can also minimise intra-group value at a given moment and that is inversely proportional
feeding competition by visiting food patches with the highest to the distance of the goal from the user (Sen & Smith,
availability of resources (Cunningham & Janson, 2007a). 1995). The goal’s value can differ over time. In other words,
While it is now accepted that primates possess a this is a combination of the different costs and benefits
‘mental map’ of their foraging areas, the type of mental for accessing the goal: they may depend on the forager’s
representations primates use while foraging and how state (e.g. physiological state or predation risk; Janson, 2014)
information about resources (i.e. what, where and when) is or the relative availability of other preferred food (Sen &
organised remain subject to debate. The Euclidean map Smith, 1995). We can hypothesise that long-lived species that
model is not supported conclusively in any animal (even inhabit relatively unpredictable environments will require a
in chimpanzees, despite claims by Normand & Boesch, higher capacity for learning, and thus that they will possess
2009). We await further investigation or the development more developed planning capacities (van Woerden et al.,

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


16 C. Trapanese and others

2011). Similarly, more-terrestrial species are likely to require and although they exhibit intermediate performance on
more-sophisticated abilities for locating food than arboreal several foraging cognitive tasks, they possess exceptional
species, who may be able to assess ripe fruit availability and socio-cognitive skills such as following gaze direction (Sandel,
abundance more easily from the top of the canopy. MacLean & Hare, 2011) or making transitive inferences
There is a growing body of evidence that primates (MacLean, Merritt & Brannon, 2008). Thus, for lemurs
remember food locations (where) and food quality (what) but foraging cognition varies with ecology, while social cognition
there is still little information available concerning temporal varies with social complexity (Rosati, 2017). However, a
knowledge (when): are primates able to integrate spatial and similar conclusion may not be applicable to other primate
temporal decisions? The ability to recall specific events in species and concerning all social cognitive skills [e.g. Theory
the past, termed ‘episodic memory’, has been postulated to of Mind capacities (Call & Tomasello, 2008)]. Cognitive
be uniquely human (Tulving, 2005). However, some animals social skills can also be driven by ecological correlates rather
in captivity (e.g. birds, rats, chimpanzees; Allen & Fortin, than by social group size (e.g. Devaine et al., 2017). Indeed,
2013) behave as if they possess integrated memories of past cognition likely evolved in mosaic patterns, with different
events, remembering what happened, where and when (Crystal, evolutionary processes shaping different cognitive domains
2010), although others e.g. rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) (Rosati, 2017).
failed to perform these tasks (Hampton et al., 2005). To date, Many views on human uniqueness focus on the special
evidence for episodic memory in wild animals is scarce. In nature of human sociality, but it has been emphasised that
wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus), there is some humans also stand out among primates in terms of ecological
evidence for integrated memories of prior food patch use, niche (Hardesty, 1972). Large-brained foragers that need a
including where the patch is, how productive it is and high-energy diet to maintain their large and costly brains
when was last visited (Janson, 2016). Wild chimpanzees (Milton, 1981; Byrne, 1997) could gain a clear evolutionary
(Pan troglodytes verus) have been shown to use sophisticated advantage by using flexible planning to reduce unnecessary
botanical and ecological knowledge to locate and relocate energetic expenditure (Janmaat et al., 2014). This attribute
food (Janmaat et al., 2013a,b, 2014, 2016). may have been particularly important for hominoids living
Developing an integrative research framework of cognitive in highly seasonal habitats, probably experiencing similar
foraging in primates would enable us to relate the cognitive environmental challenges to those of modern primates
abilities of individuals directly to their foraging behaviour. (Milton, 1981; Chapman et al., 2005). This proposed benefit
Future studies should integrate experiments on captive of flexible planning skills encourages deeper investigation into
animals (to control and analyse different variables) with the temporal aspects of ecological complexity (Milton, 1981;
experiments on wild animals in which the complexity of Byrne, 1997) and of its role in brain-size evolution in hominid
the natural environment is maintained. Such integration and non-human primates (Zuberbühler & Janmaat, 2010).
of studies on one species should allow us to unravel the
cognitive mechanisms associated with when: knowing when
is the right time to revisit a resource and how they can V. CONCLUSIONS
infer that. Step-by-step controlled manipulation of what,
where and when – i.e. food quality, location and distribution, (1) The available evidence suggests that several primate
and temporal availability – is a crucial step to assess the species can memorise the locations of food resources. During
relative importance of those variables in primate foraging foraging, their ranges and trajectories depict goal-oriented
decisions. Moreover, to clarify goal-directed foraging abilities behaviours and the use of straight-line paths to minimise
a deeper investigation of the effects of socio-ecological and travelling distances between sites.
cognitive factors in different primate species is also crucial. (2) Spatial foraging strategies of primates have been seen in
In the same framework, across-species comparisons will terms of fitness-maximisation strategies. Locating ephemeral
allow us to investigate not only the influence of ecological and scattered resources such as ripe fruit in tropical forests
and evolutionary conditions on diet and foraging habits in may have triggered the evolution of the relatively large brain
primates, but also how the ecological framework has led size of primates.
to specific solutions at the level of behavioural strategies (3) There is growing evidence that primates use
and related cognitive capacities (Rosati, 2017). Rosati (2017) route-based maps when foraging. This map is a topological
underlined how both social and ecological factors can explain representation in which animals use landmarks and routes
variation in cognition, with each being relevant in distinct to navigate and approximate directions and distances. To
cognitive domains. For example, lemurs with more complex date, there is no clear evidence for the integration of more
diets, e.g. more frugivorous species, show more sophisticated sophisticated cognitive maps such as the Euclidean map in
memory and inhibitory control capacities in comparison primates or in humans.
to more folivorous species (MacLean et al., 2013; Rosati (4) Despite the growing body of literature showing that
et al., 2014). Patterns of variation across species are very primates remember food location and quality, very little
different in terms of social cognitive capacity: ring-tailed information is available on the extent of primate temporal
lemurs (Lemur catta) live in large groups with complex knowledge of food availability and further investigations are
dominance hierarchies (Sauther, Sussman, & Gould, 1999) needed.

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


Spatial foraging decisions in primates 17

VI. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Brown, C. T., Liebovitch, L. S. & Glendon, R. (2007). Lévy flights in Dobe
Ju’hoansi foraging patterns. Human Ecology 35, 129–138.
Brunyé, T. T. & Taylor, H. A. (2008). Working memory in developing and applying
The authors thank the Ethoikos foundation (Roberto mental models from spatial descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language 58, 701–729.
Bugnyar, T. & Heinrich, B. (2006). Pilfering ravens, Corvus corax, adjust their
Cozzolino in particular), the Muséum National d’Histoire behavior to social context and identity of competitors. Animal Cognition 9, 369–376.
Naturelle de Paris and the Centre de Primatologie de Byrne, R. W. (1979). Memory for urban geography. Quarterly Journal of Experimental
l’Université de Strasbourg for financial and institutional Psychology 31, 147–154.
Byrne, R. W. (1997). The technical intelligence hypothesis. An additional evolutionary
support. We also thank Benjamin Robira and Ornella stimulus to intelligence. In Machiavellian Intelligence II: Extensions and Evaluations (eds
Ferrario for their feedback on this review. We are very A. Whiten and R. W. Byrne), pp. 289–311. Cambridge University Press,
grateful for the thoughtful suggestions and comments of the Cambridge.
Byrne, R. W. (2000). How monkeys find their way: leadership, coordination, and
anonymous reviewers. cognitive maps of African baboons. In On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in
Groups (eds S. Boinski and P. A. Garber), pp. 491–518. The University of Chicago
Press, Chicago.
Byrne, R. W., Noser, R., Bates, L. A. & Jupp, P. E. (2009). How did they get here
VII. REFERENCES from there? Detecting changes of direction in terrestrial ranging. Animal Behaviour
77, 619–631.
Byrne, R. W. & Whiten, A. (1988). Machiavellian Intelligence. Clarendon Press, Oxford.
Allen, T. A. & Fortin, N. J. (2013). The evolution of episodic memory. Proceedings of Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2008). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30
the National Academy of Sciences United States of America 110, 379–386. years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12, 187–192.
Allman, J., McLaughlin, T. & Hakeem, A. (1993). Brain weight and life-span in Call, K. A., Ream, R. R., Johnson, D. S., Sterling, J. T. & Towell, R. G.
primate species. PNAS Neurobiology 90, 118–120. (2008). Foraging route tactics and site fidelity of adult female northern fur seals
Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour 49, (Callorhinus ursinus) around the Pribilof Islands. Deep-Sea Research Part II: Tropical Studies
227–267. in Oceanographics 55, 1883–1896.
Andrews, M. W. (1988). Selection of food sites by Cakebus moloch and Saimiri sciureus Cartwright, B. A. & Collett, T. S. (1982). How honey bees use landmarks to
under spatially and temporally varying food distribution. Learning and Motivation 19, guide their return to a food source. Nature 295, 560–564.
254–268. Chapman, C. A. & Chapman, L. J. (2000). Determinants of group size in social
Asensio, N., Brockelman, W. Z., Malaivijitnond, S. & Reichard, U. H. (2011). primates: the importance of travel costs. In On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel
Gibbon travel paths are goal oriented. Animal Cognition 14, 395–405. in Groups (eds S. Boinski and P. A. Garber), pp. 24–42. The University of Chicago
Atkinson, R. P. D., Rhodes, C. J., MacDonald, D. W. & Anderson, R. M. Press, Chicago.
(2002). Scale-free dynamics in the movement patterns of jackals. Oikos 98, 134–140. Chapman, C. A., Struhsaker, T. T., Zanne, A. E., Clark, C. J. & Poulsen, J. R.
Ban, S., Boesch, C. & Janmaat, K. R. L. (2014). Taï chimpanzees anticipate revisiting (2005). A long-term evaluation of fruiting phenology: importance of climate change.
high-valued fruit trees from further distances. Animal Cognition 17, 1353–1364. Journal of Tropical Ecology 21, 31–45.
Ban, S., Boesch, C., N’Guessan, A., N’Goran, E. K., Tako, A. & Janmaat, Chapman, C. A., White, F. J. & Wrangham, R. W. (1994). Party size in chimpanzees
K. R. L. (2016). Taï chimpanzees change their travel direction for rare feeding trees and bonobos: a reevaluation of theory based on two similarly forested sites. In
providing fatty fruits. Animal Behaviour 118, 135–147. Chimpanzee Cultures (eds R. W. Wrangham, W. C. McGrew, F. B. M. De Waal
Barks, S. K., Calhoun, M. E., Hopkins, W. D., Cranfield, M. R., Mudakikwa, and P. G. Heltne), pp. 41–57. Harvard University Press, Cambridge.
A., Stoinski, T. S., Patterson, G., Erwin, J. M., Hecht, E. E., Hof, P. R. & Chapman, C. A., Wrangham, R. W. & Chapman, L. J. (1995). Ecological constraints
Sherwood, C. C. (2015). Brain organization of gorillas reflects species differences on group-size analysis of spider monkey and chimpanzee subgroups. Behavioral Ecology
in ecology. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 156, 252–262. and Sociobiology 36, 59–70.
Bartolomucci, A., de Biurrun, G. & Eberhard, F. (2001). How tree shrews Chapman, C. A., Wrangham, R. W., Chapman, L. J., Kennard, D. K. & Zanne,
(Tupaia belangeri) perform in a searching task: evidence for strategy use. Journal of A. E. (1999). Fruit and flower phenology at two sites in Kibale national park,
Comparative Psychology 115, 344–350. Uganda. Journal of Tropical Ecology 15, 189–211.
Bartumeus, F., Da Luz, M. G. E., Viswanathan, G. M. & Catalan, J. Choi, J. & Silverman, I. (1996). Sexual dimorphism in spatial behaviors: applications
(2005). Animal search strategies: a quantitative random-walk analysis. Ecology 86, to route learning. Evolution & Cognition 2, 165–171.
3078–3087. Choi, J. & Silverman, I. (2003). Processes underlying sex differences in route-learning
Benhamou, S. (1997). Path integration by swimming rats. Animal Behaviour 54, strategies in children and adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences 34,
321–327. 1153–1166.
Benhamou, S. (2004). How to reliably estimate the tortuosity of an animal’s path: Clutton-Brock, T. H. & Harvey, P. H. (1980). Primates, brains and ecology.
straightness, sinuosity, or fractal dimension. Journal of Theoretical Biology 229, 209–220. Journal of Zoology 207, 151–169.
Benhamou, S. (2007). How many animals really do the Lévy walk? Ecology 88, Codling, E. A., Plank, M. J. & Benhamou, S. (2008). Random walk models in
1962–1969. biology. Journal of the Royal Society Interface 5, 813–834.
Benhamou, S. (2010). Orientation and navigation. In Encyclopedia of Behavioral Collett, T. S. (1992). Landmark learning and guidance in insects. Philosophical
Neuroscience (Volume 2, eds G. F. Koob, M. Lemoal and R. F. Thomson), Transactions Biological Sciences 337, 295–303.
pp. 497–503. Academic Press, Oxford. Collett, T. S. (1996). Insect navigation en route to the goal: multiple strategies for the
Bertolani, P. (2013). Ranging and travelling patterns of wild chimpanzees at Kibale, Uganda: use of landmarks. The Journal of Experimental Biology 199, 227–235.
a GIS approach. PhD Thesis: University of Cambridge. Collett, T. S., Cartwright, B. A. & Smith, B. A. (1986). Landmark learning and
Bicca-Marques, J. C. & Garber, P. A. (2004). Use of spatial, visual, and olfactory visuo-spatial memories in gerbils. Journal of Comparative Physiology 158, 835–851.
information during foraging in wild nocturnal and diurnal anthropoids: a field Cramer, A. E. & Gallistel, C. R. (1997). Vervet monkeys as travelling salesmen.
experiment comparing Aotus, Callicebus, and Saguinus. American Journal of Primatology Nature 387, 464–464.
62, 171–187. Crystal, J. D. (2010). Episodic-like memory in animals. Behavioural Brain Research 215,
Bicca-Marques, J. C. & Garber, P. A. (2005). Use of social and ecological 235–243.
information in tamarin foraging decisions. International Journal of Primatology 26, Cunningham, E. & Janson, C. (2007a). Integrating information about location and
1321–1344. value of resources by white faced-saki monkeys (Pithecia pithecia). Animal Cognition 10,
Boesch, C. (2007). Taking development and ecology seriously when comparing 293–304.
cognition: reply to Tomasello and Call. Journal of Comparative Psychology 122, 453–455. Cunningham, E. & Janson, C. (2007b). A socioecological perspective on primate
Boesch, C. & Boesch, H. (1984). Mental map in chimpanzees. Primates 25, 110–170. cognition, past and present. Animal Cognition 10, 273–281.
Boonratana, R. (2000). Ranging behavior of proboscis monkeys (Nasalis larvatus) in Dabbs, J. M., Chang, E. L., Strong, R. A. & Milun, R. (1998). Spatial ability,
the Lower Kinabatangan, Northern Borneo. International Journal of Primatology 21, navigation strategy, and geographic knowledge among men and women. Evolution
497–518. and Human Behavior 19, 89–98.
Boyer, D., Ramos-Fernandez, G., Miramontes, O., Mateos, J. L., Cocho, G., Dall, S. R., Giraldeau, L. A., Olsson, O., McNamara, J. M. & Stephens, D. W.
Larralde, H., Ramos, H. & Rojas, F. (2006). Scale-free foraging by primates (2005). Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends in Ecology
emerges from their interaction with a complex environment. Proceedings of the Royal & Evolution 20(4), 187–193.
Society B 273, 1743–1750. Davies, N. B. & Houston, A. I. (1981). Owners and satellites: the economics of
Brockman, D. K. & van Schaik, C. (2005). Seasonality in Primates. Cambridge territory defense in the pied wagtail, Motacilla alba. Journal of Animal Ecology 50,
University Press, Cambridge. 157–180.

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


18 C. Trapanese and others

De Lillo, C., Aversano, M., Tuci, E. & Visalberghi, E. (1998). Spatial constraints Garber, P. A., Bicca-Marques, J. C. & de O Azevedo-Lopes, M. A. (2009).
and regulatory functions in monkeys’ (Cebus apella) search. Journal of Comparative In South American Primates, Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects (ed. R. H.
Psychology 112, 353–362. Tuttle). Springer Science+Business Media, University of Chicago, Illinois.
De Lillo, C. & James, F. C. (2012). Spatial working memory for clustered and linear Garber, P. A. & Dolins, F. L. (2014). Primate spatial strategies and cognition:
configurations of sites in a virtual reality foraging task. Cognitive Processes 13, 243–246. Introduction to this special issue. American Journal of Primatology 76, 393–398.
De Lillo, C., Kirby, M. & James, F. C. (2014). Spatial working memory in immersive Garber, P. A. & Hannon, B. (1993). Modelling monkeys: a comparison of
virtual reality foraging: path organization, traveling distance and search efficiency computer-generated and naturally occurring foraging patterns in two species of
in humans (Homo sapiens). American Journal of Primatology 76, 436–446. neotropical primates. International Journal of Primatology 14, 827–852.
De Lillo, C., Spinozzi, G., Palumbo, M. & Giustino, G. (2011). Attention Garber, P. A. & Jelinek, P. E. (2006). Travel patterns and spatial mapping in
allocation modulates the processing of hierarchical visual patterns: a comparative Nicaraguan mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata). In New Perspectives in the Study
analysis of capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and humans. Journal of Experimental of Mesoamerican Primates: Distribution, Ecology, Behavior, and Conservation (eds A. Estrada,
Psychology Animal Behavior Processes 37, 341–352. P. A. Garber, M. M. Pavelka and L. Luecke), pp. 287–310. Springer, New
De Lillo, C., Spinozzi, G., Truppa, V. & Naylor, D. M. (2005). A comparative York.
analysis of global and local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli in young children Garber, P. A. & Paciulli, L. M. (1997). Experimental field study of spatial memory
and monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology 119, 155–165. and learning in wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus capuchinus). Folia Primatologica 68,
De Lillo, C., Visalberghi, E. & Aversano, M. (1997). The organization of 236–253.
exhaustive searches in a patchy space by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Garber, P. A. & Porter, L. M. (2013). Navigating in small-scale space: the role of
Comparative Psychology 111, 82–90. landmarks and resource monitoring in understanding saddleback tamarin travel.
DeCasien, A. R., Williams, S. A. & Higham, J. P. (2017). Primate brain size is American Journal of Primatology 76, 447–459.
predicted by diet but not sociality. Nature, Ecology & Evolution 1, 0112. Garey, M. R. & Johnson, D. S. (1979). Computers and Intractability. Freeman, San
Deruelle, C. & Fagot, F. (1997). Hemispheric lateralisation and global precedence Francisco.
effects in the processing of visual stimuli by humans and baboons (Papio papio). Gibeault, S. & MacDonald, S. E. (2000). Spatial memory and foraging competition
Laterality 2, 233–246. in captive western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). Primates 41, 147–160.
Deruelle, C. & Fagot, F. (1998). Visual search for global/local stimulus features in Gill, F. B. (1988). Trapline foraging by hermit hummingbirds: competition for an
humans and baboons. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 5, 476–481. undefended, renewable resource. Ecology 69, 1933–1942.
Devaine, M., San-Galli, A., Trapanese, C., Bardino, G., Hano, C., Saint Hampton, R. R., Hampstaed, B. M. & Murray, E. A. (2005). Rhesus monkeys
Jalme, M., Bouret, S., Masi, S. & Daunizeau, J. (2017). Reading wild minds: a (Macaca mulatta) demonstrate robust memory for what and where, but not when, in
computational assay of theory of mind sophistication across seven primate species. an open-field test of memory. Learning and Motivation 36, 245–259.
PLoS Computational Biology 13, e1005833. Hardesty, D. L. (1972). The human ecological niche. American Anthropologist 74,
Di Bitetti, M. S. (2001). Home-range use by the tufted capuchin monkey (Cebus apella 458–466.
nigritus) in a subtropical rainforest of Argentina. Journal of Zoology 253, 33–45. Heinrich, B. (1976). Foraging specializations of individual bumble-bees. Ecological
Monographs 46, 105–128.
Di Fiore, A. & Suarez, S. A. (2007). Route-based travel and shared routes in
Hockings, K.J. (2007). Human-chimpanzee coexistence at Bossou, the Republic of Guinea: a
sympatric spider and woolly monkeys: cognitive and evolutionary implications.
chimpanzee perspective. PhD Thesis, University of Stirling.
Animal Cognition 10, 317–329.
Hockings, K. J., Anderson, J. R. & Matzusawa, T. (2012). Socioecological
Diaz-Perez, G. C., Bautista, S. & Villanueva, R. (2000). Quality changes in
adaptations by chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, inhabiting an anthropogenically
sapote mamey fruit during ripening and storage. Postharvest Biology and Technology 18,
impacted habitat. Animal Behaviour 83, 801–810.
67–73.
Hopkins, M. E. (2016). Mantled howler monkey spatial foraging decisions reflect
Dolins, F. L. (2009). Captive cotton-top tamarins’ (Saguinus oedipus oedipus) use of
spatial and temporal knowledge of resource distributions. Animal Cognition 19,
landmarks to localize hidden food items. American Journal of Primatology 71, 316–323.
387–403.
Dolins, F. L., Klimowiez, C., Kelley, J. & Menzel, C. R. (2014). Using virtual
Houle, A., Chapman, C. A. & Vickery, V. L. (2007). Intratree variation in fruit
reality to investigate comparative spatial cognitive abilities in chimpanzees and
production and implications for primate foraging. International Journal of Primatology
humans. American Journal of Primatology 76, 496–513.
28, 1197–1217.
Dolins, F. L. & Mitchell, R. W. (2010). Spatial Cognition, Spatial Perception: Mapping
Humphries, N. E., Queiroz, N., Dyer, J. R. M., Pade, N. G., Musyl, M. K.,
the Self and Space. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
Schaefer, K. M., Fuller, D. W., Brunnschweiler, J. M., Doyle, T. K.,
Dominy, N. J., Lucas, P. W., Osorio, D. & Yamashita, N. (2001). The sensory
Houghton, J. D. R., Hays, G. C., Jones, C. S., Noble, L. R., Wearmouth,
ecology of primate food perception. Evolutionary Anthropology 10, 171–186. V. J., Southall, E. J. & Sims, D. W. (2010). Environmental context explains
Dooris, M. J., Kelley, J. M. & Trainer, J. F. (2004). Strategic planning in higher Lévy and Brownian movement patterns of marine predators. Nature 465(7301),
education. New Directions for Institutional Research 123, 5–11. 1066–1069.
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology 6, Jager, M., Weissing, F. J., Herman, P. M. J., Nolet, B. A. & Van De Koppel, J.
178–190. (2011). Lévy walks evolve through interaction between movement and environmental
Dyer, F. C. (1996). Spatial memory and navigation by honeybees on the scale of the complexity. Science 332(6037), 1551–1553.
foraging range. The Journal of Experimental Biology 199, 147–154. Janmaat, K. R. L., Ban, S. D. & Boesch, C. (2013a). Chimpanzees use long-term
Eichenbaum, H. (2014). Time cells in the hippocampus: a new dimension for mapping spatial memory to monitor large fruit trees and remember feeding experiences across
memories. Nature Reviews - Neuroscience 15, 732–744. seasons. Animal Behaviour 86, 1183–1205.
Eichenbaum, H. & Cohen, N. J. (2014). Can we reconcile the declarative memory Janmaat, K. R. L., Ban, S. D. & Boesch, C. (2013b). Tai chimpanzees use botanical
and spatial navigation views on hippocampal function? Neuron 83, 764–770. skills to discover fruit: what we can learn from their mistakes. Animal Cognition 16,
Erhart, E. M. & Overdoff, D. J. (2008). Spatial memory during foraging in 851–860.
prosimian primates: Propithecus edwardsi and Eulemur fulvus rufus. Folia Primatologica 79, Janmaat, K. R. L., Boesch, C., Byrne, R., Chapman, C., Gonébi Bi, Z. B.,
185–196. Head, J. S., Robbins, M. M., Wrangham, R. W. & Polansky, L. (2016).
Etienne, A. S. & Jeffery, K. J. (2004). Path integration in mammals. Hippocampus 14, Spatio-temporal complexity of chimpanzee food: how cognitive adaptations can
180–192. counteract the ephemeral nature of ripe fruit. American Journal of Primatology 78,
Fagot, J. & Deruelle, C. (1997). Processing of global and local visual information 626–645.
and hemispheric specialisation in humans (Homo sapiens) and baboons (Papio papio). Janmaat, K. R. L., Byrne, R. V. & Zuberbulher, K. (2006a). Evidence for a spatial
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 23, 429–442. memory of fruiting states of rainforest trees in wild mangabeys. Animal Behaviour 72,
Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The Organization of Learning. MIT Press, Cambridge. 797–807.
Gallistel, C. R. & Cramer, A. E. (1996). Computations on metric maps in mammals: Janmaat, K. R. L., Byrne, R. V. & Zuberbulher, K. (2006b). Primates take
getting oriented and choosing a multidestination route. Journal of Experimental Biology weather into account when searching for fruits. Current Biology 16, 1232–1237.
199, 211–217. Janmaat, K. R. L. & Chancellor, R. L. (2010). Exploring new areas: how
Garber, P. (1987). Foraging strategies among living primates. Annual Review of important is long-term spatial memory for mangabey (Lophocebus albigena johnstonii)
Anthropology 16(1), 339–364. foraging efficiency? International Journal of Primatology 31, 863–886.
Garber, P. A. (1989). Role of spatial memory in primate foraging patterns: Saguinus Janmaat, K. R. L., Chapman, C. A., Meijer, R. & Zuberbulher, K. (2012). The
mystax and Saguinus fuscicollis. American Journal of Primatology 19, 203–216. use of fruiting synchrony by foraging mangabey monkeys: a ‘simple tool’ to find
Garber, P. A. (2000). Evidence for the use of spatial, temporal and social information fruit. Animal Cognition 15, 83–96.
by some primate foragers. In On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups (eds Janmaat, K. R. L., Polansky, L., Ban, S. D. & Boesch, C. (2014). Wild chimpanzees
S. Boinski and P. A. Garber), pp. 165–203. The University of Chicago Press, plan their breakfast time, type, and location. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
Chicago. 111, 16343–16348.

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


Spatial foraging decisions in primates 19

Janson, C. (1985). Aggressive competition and individual food consumption in wild MacLean, E., Merritt, D. J. & Brannon, E. M. (2008). Social complexity predicts
brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 18, 125–138. transitive reasoning in prosimian primates. Animal Behaviour 76, 479–486.
Janson, C. (1990). Social correlates of individual spatial choice in foraging brown MacLean, E., Sandel, A. A., Bray, J., Oldenkamp, R. E., Reddy, R. B. & Hare,
capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Animal Behaviour 38, 910–921. B. A. (2013). Group size predicts social but not nonsocial cognition in lemurs. PLoS
Janson, C. (1996). Toward an experimental socioecology of primates: examples from One 8, e66359.
Argentine brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus). In Adaptive Radiations of Mårell, A., Ball, J. P. & Hofgaard, A. (2002). Foraging and movement paths of
Neotropical Primates (eds M. A. Norkonk, A. L. Rosenberger and P. A. Garber), female reindeer: insights from fractal analysis, correlated random walks, and Lévy
pp. 309–325. Plenum, New York. flights. Canadian Journal of Zoology 80, 854–865.
Janson, C. (1998). Experimental evidence for spatial memory in foraging wild capuchin Marriott, J., Robinson, M. & Karikari, S. K. (1981). Starch and sugar
monkeys, Cebus apella. Animal Behaviour 55, 1229–1243. transformation during the ripening of plantains and bananas. Journal of the Science of
Janson, C. (2007). Experimental evidence for route integration and strategic planning Food and Agriculture 32, 1021–1026.
in wild capuchin monkeys. Animal Cognition 10, 341–356. Masi, S. & Breuer, T. (2018). Dialium seed coprophagy in wild western gorillas:
Janson, C. (2014). Death of the (traveling) salesman: primates do not show clear multiple nutritional benefits and toxicity reduction hypotheses. American Journal of
evidence of multi-step route planning. American Journal of Primatology 76, 410–420. Primatology 80, e22752.
Janson, C. (2016). Capuchins, space, time and memory: an experimental test of Masi, S., Cipolletta, C. & Robbins, M. M. (2009). Western lowland gorillas (Gorilla
what-where when memory in wild monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 283, gorilla gorilla) change their activity patterns in response to frugivory. American Journal
20161432. of Primatology 71, 91–100.
Janson, C. & Byrne, R. V. (2007). What wild primates know about resources-opening Masi, S., Mundry, R., Ortmann, S., Cipolletta, C., Boitani, L. & Robbins,
up the black box. Animal Cognition 10, 357–367. M. M. (2015). The influence of seasonal frugivory on nutrient and energy intake in
Janson, C. & Di Bitetti, M. S. (1997). Experimental analysis of food detection in wild western gorillas. PLoS One 10, 0129254.
capuchin monkeys: effects of distance, travel speed, and resource size. Behavioral Mendes, N. & Call, J. (2014). Chimpanzees form long-term memories for food
Ecology and Sociobiology 41, 17–24. locations after limited exposure. American Journal of Primatology 76, 485–495.
Janzen, D. H. (1971). Euglossine bees as long-distance pollinators of tropical plants. Menzel, C. R. & Beck, B. B. (2000). Homing and detour behavior in golden lion
Science 171, 203–205. tamarin social groups. In On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups (eds
Joly, M. & Zimmermann, E. (2007). First evidence for relocation of stationary food S. Boinski and P. A. Garber). University of Chicago Press, Chicago.
resources during foraging in a strepsirhine primate (Microcebus murinus). American Menzel, E. W. (1973). Chimpanzee spatial memory organization. Science 182,
Journal of Primatology 69, 1045–1052. 943–945.
Joly, M. & Zimmermann, E. (2011). Do solitary foraging nocturnal mammals plan Milton, K. (1981). Distribution patterns of tropical plant foods as an evolutionary
their routes? Biology Letters 7, 638–640. stimulus to primate mental development. American Anthropologist 83, 534–548.
Killian, N. J., Jutras, M. J. & Buffalo, E. A. (2012). A map of visual space in the Milton, K. (2000). Quo vadis? Tactics of food search and group movement in
primate entorhinal cortex. Nature 491, 761–764. primates and other animals. In On the Move: How and Why Animals Travel in Groups (eds
Killian, N. J., Potter, S. M. & Buffalo, E. A. (2015). Saccade direction encoding S. Boinski and P. A. Garber), pp. 375–417. The University of Chicago Press,
in the primate entorhinal cortex during visual exploration. Proceedings of the National
Chicago.
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112, 15743–15748.
Muller, M. & Wehner, R. (1988). Path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis.
Krief, S., Cibot, M., Bortolamiol, S., Seguya, A., Krief, J. M. & Masi, S.
PNAS 85(14), 5287–5290.
(2014). Wild chimpanzees on the edge: nocturnal activities in croplands. PLoS One 9,
Mushiake, H., Saito, N., Sakamoto, K., Sato, Y. & Tanji, J. (2000). Visually
e109925.
based path-planning by Japanese monkeys. Cognitive Brain Research 11, 165–169.
Laland, K. N. (2004). Social learning strategies. Animal Learning & Behavior 32, 4–14.
Normand, E. & Boesch, C. (2009). Sophisticated Euclidean maps in forest
Lambert, J. E. & Rothman, J. M. (2015). Fallback foods, optimal diets, and
chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 77, 1195–1201.
nutritional targets: primate responses to varying food availability and quality. Annual
Noser, R. & Byrne, R. W. (2007a). Mental maps in chacma baboons (Papio ursinus):
Review of Anthropology 44, 493–512.
using intergroup encounters as a natural experiment. Animal Cognition 10, 331–340.
Lawler, E. L., Lenstra, J. K., Rinooy Kan, A. H. G. & Schmoys, D. B. (1985).
Noser, R. & Byrne, R. W. (2007b). Travel routes and planning of visits to out-of-sight
The Traveling Salesman Problem: A Guided Tour of Combinatorial Optimization. Wiley,
resources in wild chacma baboons, Papio ursinus. Animal Behaviour 73, 257–266.
Chichester.
Noser, R. & Byrne, R. W. (2010). How do wild baboons (Papio ursinus) plan
Lawton, C. A. & Kallai, J. (2002). Gender differences in wayfinding strategies and
their routes? Travel among multiple high-quality food sources with inter-group
anxiety about wayfinding: a cross-cultural comparison. Sex Roles 47, 389–401.
Lemke, T. O. (1984). Foraging ecology of the long-nosed bat, Glossophaga soricina, with competition. Animal Cognition 13, 145–155.
respect to resource availability. Ecology 65, 538–548. Noser, R. & Byrne, R. W. (2013). Change point analysis of travel routes reveals
Lent, D. D., Graham, P. & Collett, T. S. (2009). A motor component to the novel insights into foraging strategies and cognitive maps of wild baboons. American
memories of habitual foraging routes in wood ants? Current Biology 19, 115–121. Journal of Primatology 76, 399–409.
Levandowsky, M., White, B. S. & Schuster, F. (1997). Random movements of O’Keefe, J. & Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford University
soil amoebas. Acta Protozoologica 36, 237–248. Press, Oxford.
Lewis, D. (1976). Oceania - observations on route finding and spatial orientation Olupot, W., Waser, P. M. & Chapman, C. A. (1997). Fruit finding by mangabeys
among the aboriginal peoples of the Western Desert region of Central Australia. (Lophocebus albigena): are monitoring of fig trees and use of sympatric frugivore calls
Wiley Online Library 46, 249–282. possible strategies? International Journal of Primatology 19, 339–353.
Lihoreau, M., Chittka, L. & Raine, E. N. (2011). Trade-off between travel distance Parker, S. T. (1993). Imitation and circular reactions as evolved mechanisms for
and prioritization of high-reward sites in traplining bumblebees. Functional Ecology cognitive construction. Human Development 36, 309–323.
25, 1284–1292. Parrish, A. E., James, B. T. & Beran, M. J. (2017). Exploring whether nonhuman
Luhrs, M. L., Dammhahn, M., Kappeler, P. M. & Fichtel, C. (2009). Spatial primates show a bias to overestimate dense quantities. Journal of Comparative Psychology
memory in the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus). Animal Cognition 12, 599–609. 131, 59–68.
Maaswinkel, H. & Whishaw, I. Q. (1999). Homing with locale, taxon, and Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1956). The Child’s Conception of Space. Routledge and Kegan
dead reckoning strategies by foraging rats: sensory hierarchy in spatial navigation. Paul, London.
Behavioural Brain Research 99, 143–152. Pochron, S. T. (2001). Can concurrent speed and directness of travel indicate
MacDonald, S. E. (1994). Gorillas’ (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) spatial memory in a foraging locational knowledge in free-ranging yellow baboons (Papio h. cynocephalus) of Ruaha
task. Journal of Comparative Psychology 108, 107–113. National Park, Tanzania. International Journal of Primatology 22, 773–785.
MacDonald, S. E. & Agnes, M. M. (1999). Orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) spatial Pochron, S. T. (2005). Does relative economic value of food elicit purposeful
memory and behavior in a foraging task. Journal of Comparative Psychology 113, encounter in the yellow baboons (Papio hamadryas cynocephalus) of Ruaha National
213–217. Park, Tanzania? Primates 46, 71–74.
MacDonald, S. E., Pang, J. C. & Gibeault, S. (1994). Marmoset (Callithrix jacchus Porter, L. M. & Garber, P. A. (2013). Foraging and spatial memory in wild
jacchus) spatial memory in a foraging task: win-stay versus win-shift strategies. Journal Weddell’s saddleback tamarins (Sanguinus fuscicollis weddelli) when moving between
of Comparative Psychology 108, 328–334. distant and out-of-sight goals. International Journal of Primatology 34, 30–48.
MacDonald, S. E. & Wilkie, D. M. (1990). Yellow-nosed monkeys’ (Cercopithecus Potì, P. (2000). Aspects of spatial cognition in capuchins (Cebus apella): frames of
ascanius whitesidei) spatial memory in a simulated foraging environment. Journal of reference and scale of space. Animal Cognition 3, 69–77.
Comparative Psychology 104, 382–387. Poucet, B. (1993). Spatial cognitive maps in animals: new hypotheses on their
MacGregor, J. N. & Chu, Y. (2011). Human performance on the travelling salesman structure and neural mechanisms. Psychological Review 100, 163–182.
and related problems: a review. The Journal of Problem Solving 3, 1–29. Racey, P. A. & Swift, S. M. (1985). Feeding ecology of Pipistrellus pipistrellus
MacKinnon, J. (1974). The behaviour and ecology of wild orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). (Chiroptera, Vespertilionidae) during pregnancy and lactation. I. Foraging behavior.
Animal Behaviour 22, 3–74. Journal of Animal Ecology 54, 205–215.

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society


20 C. Trapanese and others

Raichlen, D. A., Wood, B. M., Gordon, A. D., Mabulla, A. Z., Marlowe, Suarez, S. A., Karro, J., Kiper, J., Farler, D., Mcerloy, B., Rogers, B. C.,
F. W. & Pontzer, H. (2014). Evidence of Lévy walk foraging patterns in human Stockwell, B. & Young, T. (2014). A comparison of computer-generated and
hunter–gatherers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111, 728–733. naturally occurring foraging patterns in route-network-constrained spider monkeys.
Reid, R. A. & Reid, A. K. (2005). Route finding by rats in an open arena. Behavioural American Journal of Primatology 76, 460–471.
Processes 68, 51–67. Teichroeb, J. A. (2015). Vervet monkeys use paths consistent with context-specific
Reynolds, A. M. & Frye, M. A. (2007). Freeflight odor tracking in Drosophila is spatial movement heuristics. Ecology and Evolution 5, 4706–4716.
consistent with an optimal intermittent scalefree search. PLoS One 2, e354. Teichroeb, J. A. & Aguado, W. D. (2016). Foraging vervet monkeys optimize travel
Robbins, M. M. & Hohmann, G. (2006). Primate feeding ecology: an integrative distance when alone but prioritize high-reward food sites when in competition.
approach. In Feeding ecology in apes and other primates (eds G. Hohmann, M. M. Animal Behaviour 115, 1–10.
Robbins and C. Boesch). Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Teichroeb, J. A. & Chapman, C. A. (2014). Sensory information and associative
Rodrigo, T. (2002). Navigational strategies and models. Psicológica 23, 3–32. cues used in food detection by wild vervet monkeys. Animal Cognition 17, 517–528.
Rosati, A. (2017). Foraging cognition: reviving the ecological intelligence hypothesis. Thorndyke, P. W. & Hayes-Roth, B. (1982). Differences in spatial knowledge
Trends in Cognitive Sciences 21, 691–702. acquired from maps and navigation. Cognitive Psychology 14, 560–589.
Rosati, A., Rodriguez, K. & Hare, B. (2014). The ecology of spatial memory in Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. The Psychological Review 55,
four lemur species. Animal Cognition 17, 947–961. 189–208.
Sandel, A. A., MacLean, E. L. & Hare, B. (2011). Evidence from four lemur species Tujague, M. P., Janson, C. H. & Lahitte, H. B. (2015). Long-term spatial memory
that ringtailed lemur social cognition converges with that of haplorrhine primates. and learning set formation in captive capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus = Sapajus
Animal Behaviour 81, 925–931. cay). International Journal of Primatology 36, 1067–1085.
Sauther, M. L., Sussman, R. W. & Gould, L. (1999). The socioecology of Tulving, E. (2005). Episodic memory and autonoesis: uniquely human? In The
the ringtailed lemur: thirty-five years of research. Evolutionary Anthropology 8, Missing Link in Cognition: Origins of Self-Reflective Consciousness (eds H. Terrace and
120–132. J. Metcalfe), pp. 3–56. Oxford University Press, New York.
Sayers, K. (2013). On folivory, competition, and intelligence: generalism, Tutin, C. E. G. (1999). Fragmented living: behavioural ecology of primates in a forest
overgeneralizations, and models of primate evolution. Primates; Journal of Primatology fragment in the Lopé Reserve, Gabon. Primates 40, 249–265.
54, 111–124. Valero, A. & Byrne, R. W. (2007). Spider monkey ranging patterns in Mexican
Schkolkopf, B. & Mallot, H. A. (1995). View-based cognitive mapping and path subtropical forest: do travel routes reflect planning? Animal Cognition 10, 305–315.
planning. Adaptive Behavior 3, 311–348. van Schaik, C. P., Damerius, L. & Isler, K. (2013). Wild orangutan males plan
Schoener, T. (1971). Theory of feeding strategies. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics and communicate their travel direction one day in advance. PLoS One 8, e74896.
2, 369–404. van Woerden, J. T., Willems, E. P., van Schaik, C. P. & Isler, K. (2011). Large
Schreier, A. L. & Grove, M. (2014). Recurrent patterning in the daily foraging brains buffer energetic effects of seasonal habitats in catarrhine primates. Evolution
routes of hamadryas baboons (Papio hamadryas): spatial memory in large-scale versus 66, 191–199.
small-scale space. American Journal of Primatology 76, 421–435. Viswanathan, G. M., Afanasyev, V., Buldyrev, S. V., Murphy, E. J., Prince,
Séguinot, V., Cattet, J. & Benhamou, S. (1998). Path integration in dogs. Animal P. A. & Stanley, H. E. (1996). Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses.
Behaviour 55, 787–797. Nature 381, 413–415.
Sen, A. K. & Smith, T. E. (1995). Gravity Models of Spatial Interaction Behavior. Springer Viswanathan, G. M., Buldyrev, S. V., Havlin, S., Da Luz, M. G. E., Raposo,
Verlag, Berlin. E. P. & Stanley, H. E. (1999). Optimizing the success of random searches. Nature
Shaffer, C. A. (2014). Spatial foraging in free ranging bearded Sakis: traveling 401, 911–914.
salesmen or Lévy walkers? American Journal of Primatology 76, 472–484. Wassmann, J. & Dasen, P. (1998). Balinese spatial orientation: some empirical
Sigg, H. & Stolba, A. (1981). Home range and daily march in a hamadryas baboon evidence of moderate linguistic relativity. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute
troop. Folia Primatologica 36, 40–75. (N.S.) 4, 689–711.
Sirianni, G., Mundry, R. & Boesch, C. (2015). When to choose which tool: Watanabe, S. & Huber, L. (2006). Animal logics: decisions in the absence of human
multidimensional and conditional selection of nut-cracking hammers in wild language. Animal Cognition 9, 235–245.
chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 100, 152–165. Wehner, R., Michel, B. & Antonsen, P. (1996). Visual navigation in insects:
Spinozzi, G., De Lillo, C. & Truppa, V. (2003). Global and local processing coupling of egocentric and geocentric information. The Journal of Experimental Biology
of hierarchical visual stimuli in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of 199, 129–140.
Comparative Psychology 117, 15–23. Wehner, R. & Srinivasan, M. V. (2003). Path integration in insects. In The
Stephens, D. W. & Krebs, J. R. (1986). Foraging Theory. Princeton University Press, Neurobiology of Spatial Behaviour (ed. K. J. Jeffery), pp. 9–30. Oxford University
Princeton. Press, Oxford.
Stoinski, T. S., Perdue, B., Breuer, T. & Hoff, M. P. (2013). Variability in the Whiten, A. & Van Schaik, C. P. (2007). The evolution of animal ‘cultures’ and
developmental life history of the genus Gorilla. American Journal of Physical Anthropology social intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 362, 603–620.
152, 165–172. Woodruf, G. & Premack, D. (1979). Intentional communication in the chimpanzee:
Strier, K. B. (2007). Primate Behavioral Ecology, 3rd Edition (). Allyn & Bacon, Boston. the development of deception. Cognition 7, 333–362.
Suarez, S.A. (2003). Spatio-temporal foraging skills of white-bellied spider monkeys (Ateles Zuberbühler, K. & Janmaat, K. R. L. (2010). Foraging cognition in non-human
belzebuth belzebuth) in the Yasuni National Park, Ecuador. PhD Thesis: Department of primates. In Primate Neuroethology (eds M. L. Platt and A. A. Ghazanfar), pp.
Ecology and Evolution, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, 336 p. 64–83. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

(Received 11 December 2017; revised 17 August 2018; accepted 20 August 2018 )

Biological Reviews (2018) 000–000 © 2018 Cambridge Philosophical Society

You might also like