You are on page 1of 12

In Review

The Neuropsychological Correlates of Borderline


Personality Disorder and Suicidal Behaviour

Jeannette LeGris, BN, MHSc, PhD Candidate1, Rob van Reekum, MD, FRCPC2

Objective: In subjects with borderline personality disorder (BPD), compared with subjects who attempted suicide, to
review neuropsychological (NP) function that may predispose to suicidal behaviour along a continuum of high and low
lethality.
Method: We undertook electronic searches of MEDLINE, PsycINFO, EMBASE, Biosos Reviews, and Cinhal. The
searches were restricted to English-language publications from 1985 onward. The search terms borderline personality
disorder, suicide, suicide attempt, self-harm behaviour, neuropsychological, executive function (EF), neurocognitive, and
neuropsychological function produced 29 neuropsychology studies involving BPD and 7 neuropsychology studies of
suicide attempters, regardless of psychiatric diagnosis.
Results: Of the BPD studies, 83% found NP impairment in one or more cognitive domains, irrespective of depression,
involving specific or generalized deficits linked to the dorsolateral prefrontal and orbitofrontal regions. The functions
most frequently reported (in 71% to 86% of BPD studies) are response-inhibitory processes affecting executive function
performance that requires speeded attention and visuomotor skills. Decision making and visual memory impairment are
also most frequently affected; 60% to 67% of BPD studies report attentional impairment, verbal memory impairment, and
visuospatial organizational impairment. Least affected processes in BPD appear to involve spatial working memory,
planning, and possibly, IQ. The similarities in NP deficits found in BPD and suicide-attempt studies involve decision
making and Trails performances. BPD studies, however, reflect more frequent impairment on the Stroop Test and
Wisconsin Card Sort Test performance than the suicide-attempt studies, whereas verbal fluency appears to be more
frequently impaired in those attempting suicide.
Conclusions: Impaired EF and disinhibitory processes, as indicated by verbal fluency, Trails, and Stroop performance,
primarily associated with dorsolateral prefrontal cortical regions may represent a dominant executive pathway to suicide
attempt. A primary motivational inhibitory pathway involving conflictual, affective, and reflexive decision-making
processes associated with orbitofrontal brain regions, in combination with significant cognitive rigidity, may influence the
repetitive expression of self-harm or low-lethality suicidal behaviour. The hypothesis of a specific trait-like cognitive
vulnerability for suicidal behaviour involving dysregulatory, disinhibiting pathways awaits confirmation.
(Can J Psychiatry 2006;51:131–142)
Information on funding and support and author affiliations appears at the end of the article.

Clinical Implications

· Deliberate and automatic neural processes, in combination with other risks, may yield unique pathways to suicide attempt and
self-harm.
· The notion of willful, deliberate self-harm behaviour in some patients may have to be reconsidered.
· Greater clinical recognition and accommodation of neurocognitive impairments in paitents with BPD warranted.

Limitations

· The paucity of comprehenisve NP studies of suicide makes comparisons with other populations preliminary and exploratory.
· The clinical significance of NP findings in suicidal patients awaits further clarification.
· The influence of ADHD–LD comorbidity on the NP performance of suicidal patients requires further understanding.

Can J Psychiatry, Vol 51, No 3, March 2006 W 131


The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry—In Review

Key Words: neuropsychological, suicide attempt, borderline connections. Recent investigations combining NP perfor-
personality, executive function mance with structural and functional neuroimaging have
localized the underlying neural pathways and biological
he NP study of suicide remains in its infancy. A growing mechanisms involved (9–17). While extensive NP research
T interest is emerging, however, in the neuropsychology of
BPD. BPD is associated with suicidal behaviour, and hence,
has been conducted on schizophrenia and ADHD, the role of
cognitive dysfunction in the causation of BPD, and suicidal
an understanding of the neuropsychology of BPD may lead to behaviour more generally, remains unclear (18,19).
a greater understanding of the neuropsychology of suicidal One in 10 patients with BPD will complete suicide; 75% will
behaviour. NP studies to date have provided inconsistent engage in self-injurious behaviour, a known risk factor for
results and (or) results that overlap with other clinical disor- suicide attempt (20–23). Because the clinical distinctions
ders. These developments raise questions about the specificity between self-harm and suicidal behaviour remain ambigu-
and stability of these NP deficits and lead one to wonder ous (24,25), the role of cognitive impairment as a potential
whether they are the causal organic antecedents to psychiatric risk factor for suicidal behaviour remains largely unknown.
disturbance (1,2), the result of environmental vulnerabili- Currently, few investigations exist examining the associa-
ties (3–5), some complex combination (6–8), or the result of tions between cognitive impairment and suicidal behaviour,
the illness itself. Longitudinal studies, as yet unavailable, may defined as self-directed injuries with at least some intent to
provide more definitive answers to these important causal die (26). Could dissociable cognitive processes associated
with self-harm and suicidal behaviour provide clinicians with
additional information to more reliably predict and manage
these behaviours? This review of NP function in BPD and in
suicide attempts regardless of psychiatric diagnosis examines
Abbreviations used in this article the role of cognitive dysfunction in the causation of suicidal
ADHD attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and self-harm behaviour.
ANT Attention Network Test
An examination of cognitive dysfunction in BPD, regardless
BDI Beck Depression Inventory
of the lethality of suicidal behaviour, and its comparison with
BPD borderline personality disorder cognitive dysfunction in suicide attempters in general, may
BWM backward masking yield distinct patterns of NP impairment. A greater under-
CD conduct disorder standing of suicidal and self-harm behaviour can inform sub-
COWAT Controlled Oral Word Association Test sequent research and clinical practice.
CPT Continuous Performance Test
EF executive function Search Methods and Results
fMRI functional magnetic resonance imaging We searched MEDLINE, PsycINFO, EMBASE, Cinhal, and
GNAT Go/No-go Association Task
Biosis Reviews for articles published in English from 1985
onward. Search terms were borderline personality disorder,
ID/ED Intra/Extra-Dimensional Set-Shift Task
neuropsychological, suicide, executive function,
IGT Iowa Gambling Task
neurocognitive assessment, neuropsychological function, and
LD learning disability
suicide attempt. Studies were included if they involved any
NART National Adult Reading Test number of commonly used standardized NP tests or batteries
NP neuropsychological in samples of BPD and suicidal populations. Manual searches
PFC prefrontal cortical of the authors’ reference lists produced additional relevant lit-
PPVT Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test erature. The search produced 29 NP investigations involving
PTSD posttraumatic stress disorder BPD as a primary or secondary comorbid disorder. Only 7
ROFC Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure test
published studies of NP function in suicide attempters were
located (27–33).
SD standard deviation
STROOP The Stroop Colour and Word Test
Overview of NP Studies of BPD
TBI traumatic brain injury
NP studies of BPD suffer from the methodological challenges
WAIS Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale
related to controlling for past or current alcohol or substance
WAIS-R WAIS-Revised abuse, comorbid anxiety, depression, ADHD, LD, and medi-
WCST Wisconsin Card Sort Test cation effects—all known to potentially affect neurocognitive
test performance (34). Generalized and domain-specific

132 W Can J Psychiatry, Vol 51, No 3, March 2006


The Neuropsychological Correlates of Borderline Personality Disorder and Suicidal Behaviour

deficits were affirmed in 24 of 29 published studies located in In 1 of 2 reviews, O’Leary (59) reports on 4 comprehensive
this review, despite the wide variation in samples studied (see studies, including her own, that found impairment in simple
Note). Studies primarily included convenience samples of and logical verbal memory and visuospatial organization (40,
women aged 18 to 50 years, with mixed-sex samples repre- 41,44,60). All 4 confirm deficits in complex visual memory,
sented in 7 studies and all-male subjects represented in only 3 with 2 of the 4 also identifying lowered IQ (41,44). Because
investigations. To date, slightly more inpatients are repre- studying IQ in general remains controversial (61), many
sented than outpatients, with some investigators including investigators have not measured or reported on it. In a review
both. The controlled studies were matched for age, education, of 14 studies, Monarch and others note a lack of comprehen-
sex, and less frequently, IQ—all believed to affect NP func- sive NP investigations of BPD (42); these often involve a sin-
tion. Only 5 investigators (19,35–38) failed to find significant gle domain, with attentional tests appearing to lose favour to
NP impairment in BPD. specific EF measures. More refined hypotheses and tests with
greater localizing value may have led to these trends. As well,
First-Generation Studies: NP and BPD many attentional processes are believed to underly EF func-
tion (62). Findings of impulsivity, verbal memory, and
Psychopathology Correlates
visuospatial EF impairment were evident in Monarch’s
First-generation studies were exploratory for the presence or
review. She subsequently found WAIS Digit Symbol scores
absence of NP abnormalities in BPD (35,39–44) associated
(a measure of visuomotor speed, attention, and intelligence)
with personality and psychopathology measures. These
to be 3 to 5 SDs below historical comparison-group scores in
important studies made use of comprehensive NP batteries,
her own study. Recent investigations demonstrate variable
but they were characterized by very small samples with no
exclusion of subjects with current Axis I and II comorbidity;
current Axis I comorbidities. These early studies ranged from
however, representativeness is questionable because it is
those without a control group (35,42) to others with carefully
unlikely that a “pure” BPD condition exists (8,63). The exclu-
matched healthy control subjects (40,41,43,44). Only
sion criteria may explain the small samples studied.
Burgess (39) and van Reekum (43) made use of psychiatric
comparisons. Reports of general intelligence, attention, mem-
ory, and motor deficits were documented by some investiga- NP Findings in BPD
tors (39,41,43,44). EF was described as abstract thinking, BPD and General Intellectual Function
problem solving, and complex visual or verbal information
The WAIS-R is the most comprehensive test of general intel-
processing.
lect, whereas the NART (64) is widely used as a verbal esti-
mate of premorbid intellectual ability. The Digit Symbol,
Second-Generation Studies: Biological Block Design, or Picture Arrangement subtests of the WAIS
Correlates of EF in BPD also represent intelligence estimates. Three comprehensive
Building on first-generation findings, incorporating data WAIS assessments found subjects with BPD to be more intel-
drawn from populations having brain lesions and from lectually compromised than were control subjects (40,41,44);
healthy populations, and using modern neuroimaging tech- normal findings were reported by Cornelius, who had no con-
nology, second-generation investigators focused on selective trol group (35); and Driessen, who, after controlling for
attentional and (or) EF tests with known localizing value to depression with BDI scores, did not find any difference in
specific brain regions. These recent technological advances IQ (19). Using 4 WAIS subtests, Irle and others found signifi-
are strengthening support for the view that biological under- cant impairment in verbal IQ, and more severe performance
pinnings are implicated in the disorder (4,19,37,45). Greater impairment on the Block Design (4). Using IQ estimates,
conformity to standardized research diagnosis; use of healthy, Burgess (39) and Bergvall (56) also endorsed lowered verbal
psychiatric, and comorbid control subjects; larger samples; IQ in subjects with BPD. However, normal IQ in BPD was
use of convergent computerized and manual NP tests; and also reported (3,19,36,37,48,50,51). These inconsistencies
greater attention to relevant confounds generally characterize suggest that the full-scale WAIS may be more sensitive or that
these investigations (3,18,36,38,46–57). However, the inclu- the negative findings associated with IQ estimates may reflect
sion or exclusion of subjects with mild neurologic the classic heterogeneity of BPD. Overall, performance IQ
comorbidity (that is, with TBI, ADHD, LD, or epilepsy) con- seems to be more consistently affected than verbal IQ, which
tinues to vary across studies (5,19,37) and detracts from the may reflect the visuospatial impairment found in individuals
comparisons among samples. To date, few investigators have with BPD. Overall, 54% of studies found lower IQ in subjects
attempted to disentangle this significant confound (3, with BPD, suggesting impaired premorbid functioning. Many
5,18,43,47,51,58), which is related to both BPD diagnosis and researchers set an IQ level in their inclusion criteria and thus
EF dysfunction. might have erroneously eliminated those most affected.

Can J Psychiatry, Vol 51, No 3, March 2006 W 133


The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry—In Review

Attention and BPD subgroup but no visual or verbal memory differences among
Although lacking a clear definition, 2 aspects of attention— subjects in either organic and nonorganice groups (18). These
that is, automatic or reflexive processes and voluntary con- findings suggest that organicity does not account for all
trolled processes—are well established. Sustained attention, observed NP deficits. Depression and anxiety also had little
such as occurs in vigilance, and the notion of a time- effect on the test results. Attention and verbal fluency scores
limited capacity to remain vigilant are known to be affected by correctly predicted 71.3% of group membership.
depression, fatigue, ageing, and brain damage (34). Selective
Of 15 studies, 9 (60%) identified significant, sustained, selec-
attention or concentration involves focusing on relevant stim-
tive, or highly specific attentional impairment in BPD. Do
uli while ignoring irrelevant stimuli. Slowed processing
attentional difficulties further reflect the presence of
underlies many attentional disorders, which can have broad
dissociative phenomena commonly experienced by some
effects on all aspects of attention. Fifteen studies examined
patients during acts of self-injury? Parallel visuospatial and
selective and sustained attention by means of the CPT, ANT,
spatial reasoning, figural memory, immediate verbal memory,
computerized BWM test, or Digit Symbol modalities; of the
and selective and sustained attentional deficits have recently
15 studies, 60% found general or specific impairment (3,4,18,
been confirmed in a study of subjects suffering from deper-
40,42,47,49,53,54), with negative results also reported (19,
sonalization (66). However, these subjects performed nor-
38,41,44,45,65).
mally on the WCST, Trails Tests, IQ tests, and Stroop tests
Posner and others compared negative affect and effortful con- and thus mirrored only some of the NP deficits associated
trol in BPD patients and healthy and temperamentally with BPD.
matched control subjects, using a reaction-time task assessing
3 attentional networks (alerting, orienting, and conflict reso- Verbal Memory in BPD
lution) (53). Only effortful control was related to ANT con-
Memory impairment is not unique to BPD and, when seen in
flict scores, indicating a specific abnormality in BPD,
BPD, is often attributed to comorbid depression. Of 12 studies
irrespective of temperament, in the area of conflict resolution
reviewed, 8 (67%) found verbal learning and memory impair-
and, more generally, cognitive control. The conflict network
ment in comparisons with healthy (40,42,44,49) and psychiat-
is associated with the subcortical anterior cingulate gyrus,
ric (4,18,39,46) control subjects. To clarify the influence on
which normally develops between ages 2 and 7 years, a time
verbal memory of comorbid BPD in clinical depression, Kurtz
when many BPD patients report periods of abuse or neglect.
and Morey tested 20 patients with depression and comorbid
Paris and colleagues reported sustained attentional impair-
BPD, 20 patients with depression only, and 20 healthy control
ment in 41 school-aged children with similar symptoms
subjects (46). Despite similar depression severity, patients
(described by the authors as “borderline pathology of child-
with comorbid BPD showed poorer verbal recall and recogni-
hood”), compared with psychiatric control subjects (3). After
tion memory than did depression-only patients and healthy
controlling for ADHD and other comorbidities, children with
control subjects, suggesting that BPD is associated with cog-
BPD demonstrated poorer orientation to task, slower reaction
nitive deficits beyond those caused by depression. However,
time, inconsistent responses, and greater risk-taking.
in a similar 3-group comparison with patients having schizo-
Zelkowitz and others further compared the predictive effects
phrenia and comorbid BPD, Lysaker and others did not find
of attentional and EF deficits with histories of sexual abuse
any relation between comorbid borderline traits and neuro-
and violence in predicting BPD diagnosis (4). These authors
cognitive function (65). O’Leary’s analysis of medication-
found that environmental trauma explained 25% of the diag-
free BPD patients indicated significant impairment in imme-
nostic variance and that attention and abstract thinking defi-
diate, delayed, and distorted verbal recall, but memory
cits contributed 33%. Attentional impairment on Digit
improved with cueing (44). Monarch and others also vali-
Symbol and Digit Span tests, a measure of gross attention (4,
dated significant verbal memory impairment, using historical
39–42,44), has also been reported in BPD. In 30 women with
control subjects (42). Burgess used 6 subtests of memory in a
BPD and severe histories of sexual and physical abuse, Irle
sample of 27 BPD patients who were compared with control
and colleagues found marked selective attention to be
subjects having schizophrenia and control subjects with
associated with reduced hippocampal volume and exposure to
depression and found an association between deficits in atten-
traumatic stress (4).
tion and memory and self-injury in the BPD group (39). This
Travers and King studied a sample of 80 subjects with BPD, association was not found within the comparative schizophre-
of whom 66% were confirmed with organic impairments (that nia group who self-harmed. Subjects with BPD had more
is, a history of significant head injury, encephalitis, epilepsy, delayed and omission memory errors than either the schizo-
ADHD, or LD). They found greater attentional and EF prob- phrenia or depression groups. Depression was not correlated
lems and substance abuse histories in the BPD organic to self-harm in any of the clinical group comparisons. Irle and

134 W Can J Psychiatry, Vol 51, No 3, March 2006


The Neuropsychological Correlates of Borderline Personality Disorder and Suicidal Behaviour

other also found logical verbal memory to be impaired (4), but social awareness, and response inhibition. Dinn argued that
stringent control for the effects of depression was not possi- medication should not have contributed so selectively to non-
ble. In most of the studies reviewed, verbal memory deficits in verbal skills while sparing verbal skills. Separate analyses
BPD appear to exist beyond those attributed to depression. controlling for depression, substance and alcohol abuse, and
These impairments may be secondary to executive dysfunc- anxiety did not alter the findings. Immediate and delayed
tion such as difficulties with attention, working memory, visual recall was also supported in a study of a small unmedi-
strategy formulation, and the inhibition of competing cated sample (41); however, BPD patients had greater learn-
recollections (62,67–69). ing disability, developmental delay, and lower full-scale IQ
than control subjects, despite similar years of education. In
Visual Memory in BPD summary, visual memory impairment reported in these and
other studies seems consistent with the clinical observations
Visual memory impairment is reported in 71% (10 of 14) of
of BPD patients’ perceptual distortions in many aspects of
BPD studies reviewed (4,18,40–44,49,52,60). In addition to
daily living. Reduced hippocampal volumes in BPD (4,19)
being more prevalent than verbal memory impairment, right
may contribute to greater visual memory impairment.
hemispheric visual memory appears to be more markedly
impaired in these samples (49). The ROFC requires subjects
to reproduce 18 elements of a complex geometric figure from Visuospatial Processing
memory, without warning and after a 1- and 30-minute delay. Visuospatial integrity has been tested in 17 BPD investiga-
The Rey Copy portion of the ROFC measures figural detail tions, of which 11 (65%) implicate impairment as measured
and the Recall component involves spatial memory for loca- by the Rey Copy, Block Design, Corsi, Embedded Figures,
tion. Seven studies found impairment in both Copy and Recall Digit Symbol, and Picture Arrangement tests. Efficient Rey
conditions (4,18,40,42,43,49,52), with Recall-only difficul- Copy depends on organization, intact visual information pro-
ties reported by O’Leary (44). cessing, and visual memory (61). Deficits for processing
visual information on the Rey Copy have been strongly asso-
Irle and others (4) found that 30 abused women who suffered ciated with BPD (18,40–44,49,52) and confirmed by other
from BPD with Axis I and II comorbidity had 11% smaller tests of visuospatial impairment (4,54,55). Visuospatial scan-
right parietal cortex and 17% smaller hippocampal volumes, ning deficits, may affect the timed performance on tests of
compared with control subjects. Greater trauma and marked EF (70).
deficits in immediate visual memory and visuospatial cogni-
tion were associated with reduced hippocampal size.
EF and BPD
Comorbid anorexia, current or past alcohol abuse, and depres-
Perhaps not surprisingly, as many as 86% (12 of 14) of
sion did not predict NP impairment or volumetric change.
reviewed studies confirm a degree of EF impairment in BPD.
While not differing on brain volumes, subjects with BPD and
EF involves insight, self-awareness, reflection, initiation,
comorbid PTSD performed more poorly on NP measures.
evaluation, and control of thought and behaviour. Interpreta-
There was no difference in brain volumes of subjects on anti-
tions of abnormal EF findings are challenging because these
depressants, benzodiazepines, or neuroleptics, compared with
tests frequently involve multidimensional components of
subjects not on these medications. Because 26 of the Irle
attention, memory, response inhibition–suppression, and
study’s 30 subjects met criteria for depression, the effects of
visuospatial ability. EF impairment is most frequently associ-
this comorbidity cannot be discounted; however, she suggests
ated with, but not limited to, dorsolateral PFC localization.
that BPD is a neurodevelopmental deficit of the right hemi-
While the interrelated functions of the frontal lobes associated
sphere with subtle NP impairment. This study lends support to
with EF remain poorly understood, visual performance abili-
Driessen’s earlier findings of hippocampal reductions in BPD
ties, most frequently affected in BPD samples, seem to be
irrespective of PTSD comorbidity (13). Dinn and others
well-supported to the frontal regions (67).
administered an extensive NP battery to 9 female medicated
in p atien ts w ith self-h arm, su icid e attemp t, an d EF: Tests of Speeded Visual-Motor Skills. Digit Symbol Cod-
schizoaffective and PTSD comorbidity (49). The same tests ing and Trail Making Test Parts A and B are widely used, non-
were administered to a second sample of 139 university stu- specific, timed measures of psychomotor speed, attention,
dents with BPD features. The students exhibited the same NP mental flexibility, visual conceptual, and visuomotor tracking
impairment as the patients, although the deficits were less pro- and are considered highly vulnerable to brain injury (61). Of
nounced. Contrary to Dinn’s initial hypotheses, nonverbal 7 BPD studies, 5 (71%) employing Trails tests, indicate
memory, nonverbal EF, and visuospatial ability associated impairment (18,42,43,49,50). Lezak suggests that slow Trails
with prefrontal regions were more strikingly implicated than performances at any age on one or both of Parts A and B does
the expected orbitofrontal functions measuring impulsivity, not differentiate whether the problem is one of motor slowing,

Can J Psychiatry, Vol 51, No 3, March 2006 W 135


The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry—In Review

coordination, visual scanning, poor motivation, or conceptual administered. Three of the 4 WCST subtests were impaired,
confusion (61). While Trails performance can be affected by with no differences in spatial working memory shown
visuospatial impairment (71), the time to complete makes it between subjects and control subjects. Medication, emotional
sensitive to attentional and psychomotor difficulties, as also affect, depression, and anxiety did not alter the results.
measured by the Digit Symbol test. Digit Symbol impairment,
In a rare antecedent NP study of BPD, Paris and others stud-
also evident in 7 BPD investigations (35,40–42,47,59,60), is
ied 94 school-aged children in psychiatric day treatment; 41
believed to be unaffected by intellect, memory, or learning.
demonstrated similar symptoms (described as “borderline
Motor persistence, sustained attention, response speed, and
pathology of childhood”) (3). No sex differences were found
visuomotor coordination play important roles in a person’s
on any NP measures. IQ was in the normal range, and subjects
normal Digit Symbol performance (34). Poor EF may be the
did not differ from control subjects on neurologic soft signs or
result of significantly slowed psychomotor speed in
the ROFC Copy or Recall tests. Significant differences on all
combination with impaired attention (72).
scales of the WCST were found in children with BPD symp-
EF: Tests of Decision Making and Planning. Impaired antici- toms, who required more trials to complete and demonstrated
patory planning in BPD has been confirmed in some stud- more perseverative responses and errors and fewer conceptu-
ies (18,48,49,51) but not in others (36,37,56). We located alizations, compared with their psychiatric peers. Owing to
only 2 studies of decision making in BPD (48,51). In both, high CD comorbidity, CD was entered as covariate but did not
decision making and planning impairment were implicated. alter the findings. Paris suggests that NP impairment in chil-
Only 3 of 6 studies examining spatial working memory, dren with BPD symptoms mirrors that of adults with BPD.
believed to affect decision making, found impairment in BPD Preservative errors and delay in shift set on the WCST are
(49,51,54). Similarly, planning deficits have been inconsis- associated with dorsolateral PFC function (5,75).
tently reported in only 57% of the BPD samples reviewed.
EF: Response Inhibition. Of 14 BPD studies, 12 (86%) impli-
Bazanis hypothesized that self-damaging behaviour may
cate dysregulated control (stop–start) mechanisms involving
reflect an impairment in decision making and planning (48).
motor, attentional, and other impulsive cognitive pro-
He tested 42 selfharming BPD patients who were without
cesses (36–38,41,43,45,47,48,51,54–56). Investigators who
TBI, substance or alcohol abuse, or depression, nor were they
found no other NP impairment (36–38) discovered
receiving medication exceeding 300 mg chlorpromazine
dysregulation on the timed Stroop, which requires both atten-
equivalents. Of the sample, 98% were engaged in self-harm
tion and impulse control. The Stroop (76) measures the ease
and suicide attempt. This group was compared with 42
with which a person can shift his or her perceptual set to con-
matched nonclinical control subjects. Visual recognition
form to changing demands and suppress a habitual response in
memory and aggression measures were also administered.
favour of an unusual one. A markedly slowed naming
Only impairments in decision making and planning were
response when a colour word is printed in a different colour
noted. BPD patients took longer to decide, selected the most
has been attributed to a slowed conflict response, a failure of
unlikely outcomes, and placed earlier bets on whether their
response inhibition, impaired selective attention and (or) a
choices were correct, demonstrating disinhibited responses.
difficulty in ignoring distraction (61). Only 2 investigators
Planning deficits indicated longer deliberation times, more
report intact response-inhibition behaviours on the
attempts, and longer latency for first solution attempt. Bazanis
GNAT (38,49). Verbal fluency, a measure of response initia-
described these deficits as localized to the orbitofrontal and
tion, has been infrequently tested in only 5 BPD studies, with
dorsolateral frontal brain regions and speculated that a general
3 (60%) reporting impairment (18,49,59). While more flu-
aversion to delay might be an important feature of BPD.
ency studies in BPD are warranted, fluency deficits may con-
Dowson and others confirmed longer deliberation times on a
tribute to a lack of initiative and self-direction, impaired
similar computerized decision task (73).
problem solving, and the excessive interpersonal dependency
EF: Abstraction and Cognitive Flexibility. The WCST (74) commonly observed in patients with this disorder. Some BPD
assesses the ability to form abstract concepts and to shift and patients’ psychomotor regulation difficulties may further pre-
maintain cognitive set. It also measures perseveration and vent them from acting on any newly acquired insight. These
learning efficiency. Of 13 BPD studies, 8 (62%) reported dysregulated arousal and (or) inhibition responses may
impairment on either the WCST (3,5,43,45,49,50) or the explain the commonly experienced “disconnect” between
ID/ED (54,56). Lenzenweger and others tested 24 women knowing what is right to do and yet not being able to follow
with BPD on sustained attention, spatial working memory, through, despite intentions. Impulsive personality traits,
and EF—together known as “controlled information process- known to be high in BPD samples, do not consistently
ing” (45). Personality, anxiety, depression, and a computer- correlate with laboratory measures of response inhibition or
ized spatial working memory test, as well as the WCST, were attentional impulsivity (38,45,54,55).

136 W Can J Psychiatry, Vol 51, No 3, March 2006


The Neuropsychological Correlates of Borderline Personality Disorder and Suicidal Behaviour

EF: Spatial Working Memory. Only 3 of 6 BPD studies 60% to 67% of studies, appeared to involve visuospatial pro-
endorse difficulties with spatial working memory. Slower cessing, verbal memory, abstraction, cognitive flexibility, and
visual stimulus perception and working memory impairment verbal fluency. The NP functions least affected in BPD appear
were observed by Stevens in 22 women with BPD, in whom to involve spatial working memory, planning, and possibly,
impulsivity, dissociation, and negative affect did not influ- IQ. Could lower IQ and smaller hippocampi compromise
ence working memory performance (54). No NP scores were overall adaptation and coping and result in greater risk for the
associated with depression. After controlling for depression development of BPD, as has been suggested in individuals
and substance abuse, Dinn and others endorsed striking defi- with PTSD (77,78)? Decision-making and planning functions
cits in visual working memory, as well as slowed planning and may involve distinct processes, as inconsistent planning and
set shifting typically associated with the prefrontal regions of consistent decision-making impairment was evident, albeit in
the brain (49). Surprisingly, omission-only errors on the few BPD studies to date. Several investigators have implied a
GNAT suggested no orbitofrontal involvement. In contrast, level of discrete dysfunction in keeping with frontal function
Berlin and Rolls found intact spatial working memory perfor- known not to dramatically disrupt NP performance (34).
mance but deficient time perception in 19 self-harming
While NP tests do not necessarily affirm localized brain dys-
women with BPD, in whom a faster subjective sense of time
function, examining relations among several related tasks
was related to behavioural impulsivity (55). Dowson and oth-
with established sensitivity for a particular brain region pro-
ers also compared spatial working memory and decision mak-
vides evidence of the probable involvement of those struc-
ing in 19 adults with ADHD, 19 subjects with BPD, and 19
tures (79). This impairment in NP function may be clinically
nonclinical control subjects (51). Subjects with BPD demon-
significant, involving minimally one-half of all BPD samples
strated longer decision–deliberation times than both groups.
reviewed. With as many as 86% of BPD samples demonstrat-
No differences in spatial working memory were evident
ing a range of EF impairment, more thorough cognitive
among the groups. Since comorbidity as well as any shared
assessments are needed when planning for effective treatment
etiology between ADHD and BPD would have reduced the
and recovery.
differences in neurocognitive performance, Dowson con-
cluded that the results were substantial and significant. The
dysfunction of working memory in BPD, while not exten- NP Findings in Suicide Attempters
sively studied, may be the result of reduced PFC metabol- EF Correlates of Suicide Attempt
ism (11,14), but this requires further study. Early investigators hypothesized that cognitively rigid indi-
In the only NP study comparing the 3 clusters with personality viduals were more prone to suicidal behaviour (80,81) and
disorder on the basis of their EF, Besteiro-González and oth- poor interpersonal problem solving (82,83). This led investi-
ers found that cluster B personalities had better overall scores gators to selectively administer fluency and WCST measures.
on all measures except the Stroop (38). The cluster A group Surprisingly, we located only 7 NP studies of adult suicide
performed most poorly on attention and concept formation, attempters (see Note). However, learning disabilities in ado-
whereas reaction times on the Stroop were slower for the clus- lescents, involving psychomotor and visuospatial organiza-
ter B group. There were no differences among the clusters on tion impairments that affect interpersonal relationships, have
any pathophysiological or personality measures, adding fur- been associated with an increased risk of suicidal
ther weight to the observations of dysregulated control pro- behaviour (84,85).
cesses in BPD. Overall, 5 of 6 (83%) BPD samples showed Suicide and EF: Verbal Fluency
impairment on the Stroop.
Strong support exists for verbal fluency scores as an indicator
Summary of NP Correlates in BPD of frontal lobe dysfunction (86,87). Verbal fluency reflects
A range of cognitive deficits in BPD appear to exist beyond initiating processes and is reduced in patients suffering from
the influence of depression, prescribed medication, and possi- depression and suicidality despite the challenge, due to the
bly, substance abuse. Unfortunately, only 5 of 29 studies strin- depressive state, of separating a willed lack of effort from an
gently controlled for the presence of learning disability and unwilled reduced capacity to make an effort. In 9 inpatient
ADHD (3,18,43,47,48). The most frequently compromised men with affective and adjustment disorder, Bartfai and oth-
cognitive processes, as reported in up to 86% of BPD studies, ers found reduced intellectual reasoning and verbal and
involved dysregulated control mechanisms; these were fol- design fluency 3 weeks following a suicide attempt, but they
lowed by other EFs involving speeded attention, flexibility, found no impairment in problem solving, planning, or cogni-
and visuotracking abilities, as found in 71% of studies. Visual tive flexibility (28). Fast inspection on the Porteus Maze plan-
memory impairment was also evident in 71% of the BPD stud- ning task for the suicide-attempt group implicated
ies reviewed. Moderately compromised functions, evident in impulsivity. Abilities to generate alternative problem

Can J Psychiatry, Vol 51, No 3, March 2006 W 137


The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry—In Review

solutions and new ideas were decreased in suicidal patients, unaffected by negative, positive, or neutral words; however, a
independent of diagnoses and alcohol abuse. longer time to name the colour of suicide-related words indi-
cated a highly specific attentional bias toward suicide stimuli.
In an fMRI study of verbal fluency in unmedicated suicide
Severity of suicidal intent was positively correlated to this
attempers suffering from depression, Audenaert and others
attentional bias after control for depression, anxiety, and
found reduced blood perfusion in anatomically specific areas
hopelessness.
of the prefrontal cortex during letter and category fluency
tests, along with poorer overall word production (32). More Suicide Attempt and Decision Making
recently, low serotonin receptor binding was found in recent Using the IGT, Jollant and others examined decision making
suicide attempters, but interestingly, serotonin was even in violent and nonviolent suicide attempters, a control group
lower in patients who had deliberately self-harmed (88). In a of normal subjects, and a control group of formerly
comprehensive comparison by Keilp and others of high- and nonsuicidal subjects with depression (33). All subjects were
low-lethality suicide attempters (27), high-lethality subjects free of current Axis 1 disorder when tested. Both groups of
performed more poorly on all tests of EF than did clinical and suicide attempters scored lower than healthy control subjects
control groups; moreover, they were the only group to per- on decision making, whereas violent suicide attempters per-
form more poorly than healthy control subjects on tests of formed more poorly than control subjects with nonsuicidal
intellect, attention, and memory. High-lethality attempters depression. Lack of other differences between the 2 groups of
were differentiated by their EF, whereas depression patients attempters, or between the 2 control groups, suggested that
differed from the healthy control subjects on attention and decision-making impairment in high-lethality attempters was
memory. This suggests that executive impairment in suicide independent of affective disorder and possibly represented a
attempters exists beyond depression or depression severity. cognitive vulnerability for suicide. When substance abusers
High-lethality attempters were specifically discriminated by in both suicide-attempter groups and those on prescribed
letter fluency, selective reminding test, WCST failure to medication were removed from the analysis, similar findings
maintain set, and Trail A reaction time scores, suggestive of prevailed. Decision-making scores were unrelated to age,
significant EF impairment. This study added to the evidence education, intelligence, age at first attempt, number of
of fluency impairment associated with suicidal behaviour. attempts, or severity of intent. Decision making for all suicide
Keilp argues that diffuse brain damage resulting from a lethal attempters was positively correlated to affective lability (89),
attempt does not appear to be a confounder, as tests whereas decision making in nonviolent suicide attempters
representing diffuse brain injury did not show selective was associated with anger expression and hostility.
impairment (27).

Cognitive rigidity and other NP functions were comprehen- Comparison of NP Function in BPD and
sively evaluated by King in a sample of 57 elderly medicated Suicide Attempters (Table 1)
inpatients suffering from depression with and without suicide In a comparison of NP findings among BPD and suicide
attempts (31). The only difference between attempters and attempter samples, verbal fluency was more frequently found
nonattempters was that time and sequencing errors on Trail B to be compromised in suicide attempters than in subjects with
showed an increase among attempters that appeared to worsen BPD, while impairments in psychomotor performance (that
with age. Trails tests additionally assessed speed, visual scan- is, Trails A and B), and decision making were found equally in
ning, new learning, mental flexibility, sequencing, and con- both populations. Surprisingly, measures of abstract thinking
centration (41). Ellis and others also report no differences and cognitive flexibility were relatively intact in suicide
between 20 suicide attempters, and 27 psychiatric attempters (25%), whereas 62% of BPD studies indicated
nonattempters, with both groups scoring in the impaired range impairment according to the WCST. Subjects with BPD,
of standardized scores on several tests, despite similar depres- however, were more frequently found to be impaired on the
sion severity (29). Thirty-five percent of the suicide attempt- Stroop test, a measure of attention and response inhibition
ers and 44% of the nonattempters in this study functioned well with frequent reports of slowed reaction or response times.
below normal on the PPVT, WCST, Trail making, and finger While one must not assume that BPD samples encompass
tapping tests. In contrast to most BPD studies, King’s only those with self-harming behaviour, these divergent find-
finding (31), and those of Ellis and others (29), support the ings in the 2 populations may reflect unique cognitive pro-
influence of psychopathology on the NP results, independent cesses distinguished by performance on the COWAT, WCST,
of suicidal behaviour. Using a modified emotional Stroop test, ROFC, GNAT, and Stroop, as well as other inhibitory control
Becker compared 31 recent suicide attempters with 31 control processes that may be differentially engaged among those
subjects who suffered from anxiety and depression but who with and without a conscious intent to die. Behavioural
had not attempted suicide (30). Suicide attempters were dysregulation, implicated in 86% of BPD samples, involves a

138 W Can J Psychiatry, Vol 51, No 3, March 2006


The Neuropsychological Correlates of Borderline Personality Disorder and Suicidal Behaviour

Table 1 Overlap of EF impairment in BPD and suicide attempters

BPD studies Suicide-attempt studies

2/2 Decision making (100%) 1/1 Decision making (100%)


5/6 Stroop (83%) 2/3 Stroop (67%)
5/7 Trails (71%) 3/4 Trails (75%)
3/5 Verbal fluency (60%) 3/4 Verbal fluency (75%)
8/13 WCST (62%) 1/4 WCST (25%)

disruption of cognitive control associated with a general fail- modulated by the hippocampus and amygdala, may involve
ure to modify action, thought, and feeling needed to conform slowed or variable motor responses and difficulties in motor
to the social and intellectual requirements of a situation (90). control. Understandably, the integration of executive and
These inhibitory EF deficits may act as important cognitive motivational inhibition is closely linked to real-life
precursors to the self-harm behaviours seen frequently in behaviour (93).
BPD. Coolidge further suggests that these processes may be
These inhibitory models, activated by different regions of the
manifestations of genetically determined EF (91).
brain, can guide an understanding of the predominant path-
It appears that NP performance and suicidal behaviour may be ways that may lead to suicidal behaviour. One pathway to sui-
independent of clinical depression, as reflected in most, but cidal behaviour involving suicide attempt with intent may
not all suicide-attempt studies, consistent with the trends in involve both arousal and executive inhibition deficits. These
the BPD literature. The NP inconsistencies among high- and deficits are evidenced by low verbal fluency, slower informa-
low-lethality attempters may mirror the clinical ambiguity so tion processing on Trails and Stroop tests, and conflict scores
often associated with self-harm and suicidal behaviour. Deci- on decision tasks. A greater risk of acting on suicidal thoughts
sion making, found to be impaired in all suicide-attempt and may be the result of inabilities to express and repress an inap-
BPD studies (33,47,48,73), may represent a shared inhibitory propriate choice. Suicidal patients may demonstrate primarily
pathway leading to suicidal behaviour, but this requires arousal and executive inhibitory control deficits through
additional study. perseverative nonsuppression of deliberate thought and
action to end their lives. A compromised ability to generate
and verbalize more creative problem-oriented solutions may
Implications
lead to feelings of entrapment. Paradoxically, the decision to
Three models have been proposed to explain dysregulated
take one’s life may reduce anxiety.
behaviour, distinguished by the degree of anxiety or arousal
involved and by the temporal quality of the incentive (imme- Conversely, self-harm behaviour involving ambivalent or a
diate or distal) (92–95). Behavioural control also depends on lack of deliberate intent or forethought may involve another
the positive or negative nature of the incentive. Briefly, the pathway of motivational inhibition associated with an imme-
first model is an arousal model of disinhibition that involves diate delay aversion to intense negative affect. Inability to
reflexive or automatic alerting and attention needed to under- endure a temporary, albeit intense, emotional state may reflect
stand new information; underarousal leads to slowed informa- a propensity for immediate rewards and avoidance of anxiety
tion processing. A second model of executive inhibition with little regard for future consequences, irrespective of the
requires a deliberate, thoughtful suppression of a previously number of past occurrences. Reflexive and repetitive
learned inappropriate response to achieve a future goal. An self-harm behaviour, which for some provides a form of
action is suppressed and intentionally held in working mem- immediate emotional relief, may reflect these deficient
ory (executive) as new information is received. Anxiety or reward–punishment signals. This motivational dysregulation
fear is typically not activated in this case. The third, or motiva- may represent an important delay aversion pathway to
tional inhibition, model (96,97) proposes a “reactive” process self-harm. In BPD samples, impaired decision making involv-
to emotionally salient incentives, involving immediate pun- ing temporal rewards or punishments on risky, uncertain out-
ishment or reward to avoid high anxiety. Hypersensitivity to comes, combined with executive inhibition deficits (indicated
immediate rewards with an aversion to more distal future by higher Stroop and WCST scores), may interact to produce
rewards, as well as an active avoidance of delay (anxiety), even greater behavioural and emotional dysregulation, result-
drive these motivational processes. Motivation, believed to be ing in reactive, avoidant self-harm behaviour. The potential

Can J Psychiatry, Vol 51, No 3, March 2006 W 139


The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry—In Review

involvement of this motivational pathway, believed to local- Chronically Suicidal Patient: Evaluating the Clinical and Health
Services Impact of Dialectical Behaviour Therapy in Individuals
ize to the orbitofrontal regions with connections to the With Borderline Personality Disorder” with the Arthur Sommer
amydala and hippocampus (99–101), may challenge the Rotenberg Chair in Suicide Studies, St Michael’s Hospital,
notion of deliberate self-harm behaviour in BPD. These Toronto, Ontario.
hypotheses examining causal pathways to suicidal behaviour
require further examination and analysis but may provide a Acknowledgements
conceptual framework for subsequent study. We acknowledge the contributions of Paul Links, Arthur Sommer
Rotenberg Chair in Suicide Studies, and Professor, Dept of
Psychiatry, University of Toronto; and Rosemary Tannock, Senior
Conclusions Scientist, Brain and Behaviour Program, The Hospital for Sick
Cognitive impairment predominantly associated with, but not Children, and Associate Professor of Psychiatry, University of
restricted to, the right frontal hemisphere (visual dominance) Toronto.
exists in BPD. The specific source, severity, and outcome of
this impairment, however, remain unclear. Contrary to popu- References
lar belief, these deficits, particularly in BPD, do not appear to 1. Andrulonis PA, Vogel NG. Comparison of borderline personality subcategories
be primarily the result of comorbid depression or of its sever- to schizophrenic and affective disorders. Br J Psychiatry 1984;144:358 –63.
2. van Reekum R. Acquired and developmental brain dysfunction in borderline
ity. Whether BPD contributes to impairment beyond unrecog- personality disorder. Can J Psychiatry 1993;38(Suppl 1):S4 –S10.
nized comorbid ADHD or LD awaits further validation. One 3. Paris J, Zelkowitz P, Guzder J, Joseph S, Feldman R. Neuropsychological factors
associated with borderline pathology in children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc
cannot ignore the consistency of this impairment across multi- Psychiatry 1999;38:770 – 4.
ple domains, as understood by both first- and second- 4. Irle E, Lange C, Sachsse U. Reduced size and abnormal asymmetry of parietal
cortex in women with borderline personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry
generation investigators. However, reports of clinical 2005;57:173– 82.
significance are noticeably lacking in the studies to date. Fur- 5. Zelkowitz P, Paris J, Guzder J, Feldman R. Diatheses and stressors in borderline
pathology of childhood: the role of neuropsychological risk and
ther research is therefore needed to clarify the extent of NP trauma.[comment]. J Am Acad ChildAdolesc Psychiatry 2001;40:100 –5.
impairment that may contribute to the unique behavioural 6. Bradley R, Jenei J, Westen D. Etiology of borderline personality disorder:
disentangling the contributions of intercorrelated antecedents. J Nerv Ment Dis
manifestations of BPD or the dimensional nature of suicidal 2005;193(1):24 –31.
behaviour. While cognitive impairment in BPD has been 7. Mann JJ, Waternaux C, Haas GL, Malone KM. Toward a clinical model of
suicidal behavior in psychiatric patients. Am J Psychiatry 1999;156:181–9.
associated with the growing evidence of biological dysfunc- 8. Links PS, Heslegrave RJ, Mitton JE, van RR, Patrick J. Borderline personality
disorder and substance abuse: consequences of comorbidity. Can J Psychiatry
tion, the direction of causation remains inconclusive. Perhaps 1995;40(1):9–14.
most important, the outcome of this impairment on everyday 9. Boutros NN, Torello M, McGlashan TH. Electrophysiological aberrations in
borderline personality disorder: state of the evidence. J Neuropsychiatry Clin
function awaits confirmation. Neurosci 2003;15:145–54.
10. Tebartz van EL, Hesslinger B, Thiel T, Geiger E, Haegele K, Lemieux L, and
It is evident from this review that not all individuals with BPD others. Frontolimbic brain abnormalities in patients with borderline personality
suffer from impaired cognitive and behavioural EF, which disorder: a volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. Biol Psychiatry
2003;54:163–71.
challenges the notion of a single causal pathway model. Nigg 11. De la Fuente JM, Tugendhaft P, Mavroudakis N. Electroencephalographic
suggests that causal models do not have explanatory power abnormalities in borderline personality disorder. Psychiatr Res 1998;77:131–8.
12. Juengling FD, Schmahl C, Hesslinger B, Ebert D, Bremner JD, Gostomzyk J,
unless they capture the broad essence of the disorder as it is and others. Positron emission tomography in female patients with borderline
manifested by the individual patient (98). Self-harm behav- personality disorder. J Psychiatr Res 2003;37:109–15.
13. Driessen M, Beblo T, Mertens M, Piefke M, Rullkoetter N, Silva-Saavedra A,
iour, considered unique to BPD, warrants further understand- and others. Posttraumatic stress disorder and fMRI activation patterns of
traumatic memory in patients with borderline personality disorder. Biol
ing within an NP or cognitive framework. No matter how one Psychiatry 2004;55:603–11.
interprets these findings, one cannot ignore the converging 14. Goyer PF, Andreason PJ, Semple WE, Clayton AH, King AC, Compton-Toth
BA, and others. Positron-emission tomography and personality disorders.
patterns of NP impairment associated with BPD that may rep- Neuropsychopharmacology 1994;10(1):21–8.
resent a cognitive vulnerability for the development of the dis- 15. Meares R, Melkonian D, Gordon E, Williams L. Distinct pattern of P3a
event-related potential in borderline personality disorder. Neuroreport
order and the manifestation of suicidal behaviour. A trait-like 2005;16:289–93.
vulnerability for suicide involving dysregulatory, 16. Soloff PH, Meltzer CC, Becker C, Greer PJ, Kelly TM, Constantine D.
Impulsivity and prefrontal hypometabolism in borderline personality disorder.
disinhibiting processes is suggested by the available data but Psychiatr Res 2003;123:153–63.
awaits clarification and confirmation. 17. Lyoo IK, Han MH, Cho DY. A brain MRI study in subjects with borderline
personality disorder. J Affect Disord 1998;50:235–43.
18. Travers C, King R. An investigation of organic factors in the neuropsychological
functioning of patients with borderline personality disorder. J Personal Disord
Note 2005;19(1):1–18.
A detailed list of studies investigating neuropsychology in BPD is 19. Driessen M, Herrmann J, Stahl K, Zwaan M, Meier S, Hill, and others. Magnetic
available from the author. resonance imaging volumes of the hippocampus and the amygdala in women
with borderline personality disorder and early traumatization. Arch Gen
Psychiatry 2000;57:1115–22.
Funding and Support 20. Oquendo MA, Galfalvy H, Russo S, Ellis SP, Grunebaum MF, Burke A, and
others. Prospective study of clinical predictors of suicidal acts after a major
This review was supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health depressive episode in patients with major depressive disorder or bipolar disorder.
Research fellowship as part of the clinical trial “Hope for the Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:1433–41.

140 W Can J Psychiatry, Vol 51, No 3, March 2006


The Neuropsychological Correlates of Borderline Personality Disorder and Suicidal Behaviour

21. Soloff PH, Lynch KG, Kelly TM, Malone KM, Mann JJ. Characteristics of 50. Stein DJ, Hollander E, Cohen L, Frenkel M, Saoud JB, DeCaria, and others.
suicide attempts of patients with major depressive episode and borderline Neuropsychiatric impairment in impulsive personality disorders. Psychiatr Res
personality disorder: a comparative study. Am J Psychiatry 2000;157:601–8. 1993;48:257–66.
22. Brodsky BS, Malone KM, Ellis SP, Dulit RA, Mann JJ. Characteristics of 51. Dowson JH, McLean A, Bazanis E, Toone B, Young S, Robbins TW, and others.
borderline personality disorder associated with suicidal behavior. Am J Impaired spatial working memory in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity
Psychiatry 1997;154:1715–9. disorder: comparisons with performance in adults with borderline personality
23. Yen S, Shea MT, Sanislow CA, Grilo CM, Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, and disorder and in control subjects. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2004;110(1):45–54.
others. Borderline personality disorder criteria associated with prospectively 52. Harris CL, Dinn WM, Marcinkiewicz JA. Partial seizure-like symptoms in
observed suicidal behavior. Am J Psychiatry 2004;161:1296–8. borderline personality disorder. Epilepsy Behav 2002;3:433–8.
24. Townsend E, Hawton K, Altman DG, Arensman E, Gunnell D, Hazell P, and 53. Posner MI, Rothbart MK, Vizueta N, Levy KN, Evans DE, Thomas KM, and
others. The efficacy of problem-solving treatments after deliberate self-harm: others. Attentional mechanisms of borderline personality disorder. Proc Natl
meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials with respect to depression, Acad Sci U S A 2002;99:16366–70.
hopelessness and improvement in problems. Psychol Med 2001;31:979–88. 54. Stevens A, Burkhardt M, Hautzinger M, Schwarz J, Unckel C. Borderline
25. Joiner TE Jr, Brown JS, Wingate LR. The psychology and neurobiology of personality disorder: impaired visual perception and working memory. Psychiatr
suicidal behavior. Annu Rev Psychol 2005;56:287–314. Res 2004;125:257–67.
26. Mann JJ. Neurobiology of suicidal behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 2003;4:819–28. 55. Berlin HA, Rolls ET. Time perception, impulsivity, emotionality, and
27. Keilp JG, Sackeim HA, Brodsky BS, Oquendo MA, Malone KM, Mann JJ. personality in self-harming borderline personality disorder patients. J Personal
Neuropsychological dysfunction in depressed suicide attempters. Am J Disord 2004;18:358–78.
Psychiatry 2001;158:735–41. 56. Bergvall AH, Nilsson T, Hansen S. Exploring the link between character,
28. Bartfai A, Winborg IM, Nordstrom P, Asberg M. Suicidal behavior and personality disorder, and neuropsychological function. Eur Psychiatry
cognitive flexibility: design and verbal fluency after attempted suicide. Suicide 2003;18:334–44.
Life Threat Behav 1990;20:254–66. 57. Coolidge FL, Segal DL, Stewart S, Ellett JAC. Neuropsychological dysfunction
29. Ellis TE, Berg RA, Franzen MD. Neuropsychological performance and suicidal in children with borderline personaltity disorder features: a preliminary
behavior in adult psychiatric inpatients. Percept Mot Skills 1992;75:639–47. investigation. J Res Pers 2000;34:554–61.
30. Becker ES, Strohbach D, Rinck M. A specific attentional bias in suicide 58. Fossati A, Novella L, Donati D, Donini M, Maffei C. History of childhood
attempters. J Nerv Ment Dis 1999;187:730–5. attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and borderline personality
31. King DA, Conwell Y, Cox C, Henderson RE, Denning DG, Caine ED. disorder: a controlled study. Compr Psychiatry 2002;43:369–77.
A neuropsychological comparison of depressed suicide attempters and 59. O’Leary KM. Borderline personality disorder. Neuropsychological testing
nonattempters. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2000;12(1):64–70. results. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2000;23(1):41–60, vi.
32. Audenaert K, Goethals I, Van LK, Lahorte P, Brans B, Versijpt J, and others. 60. Carpenter C, Gold J, Fenton W. Neuropsychological testing results in borderline
SPECT neuropsychological activation procedure with the Verbal Fluency Test in patients. [poster 316]. Presented at the 146th Annual Meeting of the American
attempted suicide patients. Nucl Med Commun 2002;23:907–16. Psychiatric Association; 1993 May 25; San Francisco (CA).
33. Jollant F, Bellivier F, Leboyer M, Astruc B, Torres S, Verdier R, and others. 61. Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment. 3rd ed. New York (NY): Oxford
Impaired decision making in suicide attempters. Am J Psychiatry University Press; 1995.
2005;162:304–10. 62. Alexander MP, Stuss DT. Disorders of frontal lobe functioning. Semin Neurol
34. Darby D, Walsh K. Neuropsychology, a clinical approach. 5th ed. Toronto (ON): 2000;20:427–37.
Elsevier; 2005. 63. Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, McGlashan TH, Dyck IR, Stout RL, Bender DS, and
35. Cornelius JR, Soloff PH, George AWA, Schulz SC, Tarter R, Brenner RP, and others. Functional impairment in patients with schizotypal, borderline, avoidant,
others. An evaluation of the significance of selected neuropsychiatric or obsessive–compulsive personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry
abnormalities in the etiology of borderline personality disorder. J Personal 2002;159:276–83.
Disord 2005;3(1):19–25. 64. Nelson HE. The National Adult Reading Test (NART): test manual. Windsor
36. Sprock J, Rader TJ, Kendall JP, Yoder CY. Neuropsychological functioning in (UK): NFER; 1982.
patients with borderline personality disorder. J Clinical Psychol 65. Lysaker PH, Wickett AM, Lancaster RS, Davis LW. Neurocognitive deficits and
2000;56:1587–600. history of childhood abuse in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: associations
37. Kunert HJ, Druecke HW, Sass H, Herpertz SC. Frontal lobe dysfunctions in with Cluster B personality traits. Schizophr Res 2004;68(1):87–94.
borderline personality disorder? Neuropsychological findings. J Personal Disord 66. Guralnik O, Schmeidler J, Simeon D. Feeling unreal: cognitive processes in
2003;17:497–509. depersonalization. Am J Psychiatry 2000;157:103–9.
38. Besteiro-González JL, Lemos-Giráldez S, MuZiz J. Neuropsychological, 67. Stuss DT, Alexander MP. Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a
psychophysiological, and personality assessment of DSM-IV clusters of conceptual view. Psychol Res 2000;63:289–98.
personality disorders. European Journal of Psychological Assessment 68. Godefroy O. Frontal syndrome and disorders of executive functions. J Neurol
2004;20:99–105. 2003;250(1):1–6.
39. Burgess JW. Neurocognitive impairment in dramatic personalities: histrionic, 69. Cummings JL. Frontal-subcortical circuits and human behavior. Arch Neurol
narcissistic, borderline, and antisocial disorders. Psychiatr Res 1992;42:283–290. 1993;50:873–80.
40. Judd PD, Ruff RM. Neuropsychological dysfunction in borderline personality 70. Peterson BS, Skudlarksi J, Gatenby C, Zhang H, Anderson A, Gore J. An fMRI
disorder. J Personal Disord 1993;7:275–84. study of stroop word-color intererence: evidence for cingulate subregions
41. Swirsky-Sacchetti T, Gorton G, Samuel S, Sobel R, Genetta-Wadley A, Burleigh subserving multiple distributed attentional systems. Biol Psychiatry
B. Neuropsychological function in borderline personality disorder. J Clin 1999;45:1237–58.
Psychol 1993;49:385–96. 71. Vuontela V, Steenari MR, Carlson S, Koivisto J, Fjallberg M, Aronen ET.
42. Monarch ES, Saykin AJ, Flashman LA. Neuropsychological impairment in Audiospatial and visuospatial working memory in 6–13 year old school children.
borderline personality disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2004;27(1):67–82. Learn Mem 2003;10(1):74–81.
43. van Reekum R, Conway CA, Gansler D, White R, Bachman DL. 72. Zakzanis KK, Leach L, Kaplan E. On the nature and pattern of neurocognitive
Neurobehavioral study of borderline personality disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci function in major depressive disorder. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav
1993;18:121–9. Neurol 1998;11:111–9.
44. O’Leary KM, Brouwers P, Gardner DL, Cowdry RW. Neuropsychological 73. Dowson J, Bazanis E, Rogers R, Prevost A, Taylor P, Meux C, and others.
testing of patients with borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry Impulsivity in patients with borderline personality disorder. Compr Psychiatry
1991;148:106–11. 2004;45(1):29–36.
45. Lenzenweger MF, Clarkin JF, Fertuck EA, Kernberg OF. Executive 74. Heaton RK. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Lutz (FL): Psychological Assessment
neurocognitive functioning and neurobehavioral systems indicators in borderline Resources; 1981.
personality disorder: a preliminary study. J Personal Disord 2004;18:421–38. 75. Goldman-Rakic PS. Specification of higher cortical functions. J Head Trauma
46. Kurtz JE, Morey ML. Verbal memory dysfunction in depressed outpatients with Rehabil 1993;8:13–23.
and without borderline personality disorder. Journal of Psychopathology and 76. Golden J. Stroop Colour and Word Test. Chicago (IL): Stoelting; 1978.
Behavioural Assessment 1999;21:141–56. 77. Twamley EW, Hami S, Stein MB. Neuropsychological function in college
47. Dougherty DM, Bjork JM, Huckabee HC, Moeller FG, Swann AC. Laboratory students with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatr Res
measures of aggression and impulsivity in women with borderline personality 2004;126:265–74.
disorder. Psychiatr Res 1999;85:315–26. 78. Andreasen NC, Flaum M, Swayze V, O’Leary DS, Alliger R, Cohen G, and
48. Bazanis E, Rogers RD, Dowson JH, Taylor P, Meux C, Staley C, and others. others. Intelligence and brain structure in normal individuals. Am J Psychiatry
Neurocognitive deficits in decision-making and planning of patients with 1993;150:130–4.
DSM-III-R borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med 2002;32:1395–405. 79. Spinella Marcello. Neurobehavioural correlates of impulsivity: evidence of
49. Dinn WM, Harris CL, Aycicegi A, Greene PB, Kirkley SM, Reilly C. prefrontal involvement. Int J Neurosci 2004;114:95–104.
Neurocognitive function in borderline personality disorder. Prog 80. Neuringer C. Rigid thinking in suicidal individuals. J Consult Clin Psychol
Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2004;28:329–41. 1964;28:54–8.

Can J Psychiatry, Vol 51, No 3, March 2006 W 141


The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry—In Review

81. Duberstein PR, Conwell Y, Caine ED. Age difference in the personality 95. Castellanos FX, Tannock R. Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity
characteristics of suicide completers and preliminary findings from a disorder: the search for endophenotyes. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002;3:617–27.
psychological autopsy study. Psychiatry 1994;57:213–24.
96. Gray JA. The neuropsychology of anxiety; an enquiry into the functions of the
82. Schotte DE, Clum GA. Problem-solving skills in suicidal psychiatric patients.
septo-hippocampal system. New York (NY): Oxford University Press; 1982.
J Consult Clinl Psychol 1987;55(1):49–54.
83. Pollock LR, Williams JM. Problem-solving in suicide attempters. Psychol Med 97. Kagan J, Reznick JC, Snidman N. The physiology and psychology of behavioral
2004;34:163–7. inhibition in children. Child Dev 1987;58:1459–73.
84. Rourke BP, Young GC, Leenaars AA. A childhood learning diability that
predisposes those afflicted to adolescent and adult depression and suicide risk. 98. Nigg JT, Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Sonuga-Barke EJ. Causal heterogeneity in
J Learn Disabil 1989;22:181–5. attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: do we need neuropsychologically
85. McBride HS, Siegel L. Learning disabilities and adolescent suicide. J Learn impaired subtypes?. Biol Psychiatry 2005;57:1224–30.
Disabil 1997;30:652–9. 99. Rolls ET. The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain Cogn
86. Pendleton MG, Heaton RK, Lehman R, Hulihan D. Diagnostic utility of the 2004;55(1):11–29.
Thurstone word fluency test in neurological evaluations. J Clin Neuropsychol
1982;4:307–17. 100. Bechara A, Damasio H, Damasio A. Role of the amygdala in decision-making.
87. Miller E. Verbal fluency as a function of a measure of verbal intelligence and in The New York Academy of Science 2003;985:356–69.
relation to different types of cereral pathology. Br J Clin Psychol 1984;23:53–7. 101. Dias R, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Dissociable forms of inhibitory control
88. Audenaert K, Peremans K, Goethals I, Otte A, Dierckx R, van Heeringen C. within prefrontal cortex with an analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test:
Functional imaging of the suicidal brain. Nucl Med Commun 2005;26:391–3. restriction to novel situations and independence from “on-line” processing.
89. Harvey PD, Greenberg BR, Serper MR. The Affective Lability Scale: J Neurosci 1997;17:9285–97.
development, reliability and validity. J Clin Psychol 1989;45:786–93.
90. Sonuga-Barke EJ. The dual pathway model of AD/HD: an elaboration of
neuro-developmental characteristics. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2003;27:593–604.
91. Coolidge FL, Thede LL, Jang KL. Are personality disorders psychological Manuscript received and accepted November 2005.
manifestations of executive function deficits? Bivariate heritability evidence 1
Fellow, The Arthur Sommer Rotenberg Chair in Suicide Studies, St
from a twin study. Behav Genet 2004;34(1):75–84. Michael’s Hospital, Toronto, Ontario; Assistant Professor, Faculty of
92. Barkley RA. The executive functions and self-regulation: an evolutionary Health Sciences, School of Nursing, McMaster University, Hamilton,
neuropsychological perspective. Neuropsychol Rev 2001;11(1):1–29.
Ontario.
93. Nigg JT. Neuropsychologic theory and findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity 2
disorder: the state of the field and salient challenges for the coming decade. Biol
Assistant Professor, Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto,
Psychiatry 2005;57:1424–35. Toronto, Ontario; Baycrest Centre for Geriatric Care, Toronto Ontario.
94. Sonuga-Barke EJ. Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Address for correspondence: J LeGris, Faculty of Health Sciences, Rm
from common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biol 2J12, McMaster University, 120 Main street West, Hamilton, ON L8N 3Z5
Psychiatry 2005;57:1231–8. legrisj@smh.toronto.on.ca; legrisj@mcmaster.ca

Résumé : Les corrélats neuropsychologiques du trouble de la personnalité limite et du comportement


suicidaire
Objectif : Chez des sujets souffrant du trouble de la personnalité limite (TPL) comparés à des sujets ayant tenté de se
suicider, examiner la fonction neuropsychologique (NP) qui peut prédisposer au comportement suicidaire sur un
continuum de létalité élevée et faible.
Méthode : Nous avons effectué des recherches électroniques dans MEDLINE, PsycINFO, EMBASE, Biosos Reviews
et Cinhal. Les recherches étaient limitées aux publications en anglais de 1985 à aujourd’hui. Les termes de recherche
trouble de la personnalité limite, tentative de suicide, neuropsychologique, fonction exécutive, neurocognitif, et fonction
neuropsychologique ont produit 29 études neuropsychologiques incluant le TPL, et 7 études neuropsychologiques de
personnes ayant tenté de se suicider, sans égard au diagnostic psychiatrique.
Résultats : Sur les études du TPL, 83 % constataient une déficience NP dans un ou plusieurs domaines cognitifs,
indépendamment de la dépression, faisant appel à des déficits spécifiques ou généralisés liés aux régions préfrontale
dorsolatérale et orbitofrontale. Les fonctions les plus fréquemment déclarées (dans 71 % à 86 % des études du TPL) sont
les processus inhibiteurs de réponse affectant le rendement de la fonction exécutive qui exige une attention accélérée, la
souplesse cognitive, le traitement visuospatial, et le processus décisionnel; 60 % à 66 % des études du TPL rapportent
une déficience de l’attention, une déficience verbale et une déficience de la mémoire non verbale. Les processus les
moins affectés par le TPL semblent être la mémoire de travail spatiale, la planification et possiblement, le QI. Les
similitudes des déficits NP dans les études du TPL et des tentatives de suicide sont le processus décisionnel et les
rendements au Trail Making Test (TMT). Les résultats de la fluidité verbale, du test Stroop et du test Wisconsin
nécessitent d’autres comparaisons.
Conclusions : Les fonctions exécutives partagées, et les processus désinhibiteurs d’éveil qu’elles apportent, ainsi que la
fluidité verbale, les rendements au TMT et possiblement au Stroop associés aux régions du cortex préfrontal dorsolatéral
peuvent représenter une voie commune vers la tentative de suicide. Une voie inhibitrice motivationnelle prédominante
impliquant les processus décisionnels associés aux régions orbitofrontales peut influencer l’expression d’actes
autodestructeurs ou un comportement suicidaire de faible létalité. L’hypothèse d’une vulnérabilité cognitive semblable à
un trait pour un comportement suicidaire impliquant des voies dysrégulatrices, désinhibitrices n’est pas confirmée.

142 W Can J Psychiatry, Vol 51, No 3, March 2006

You might also like